Understanding Attachment
Understanding Attachment
ATTACHMENT
This page intentionally left blank
UNDERSTANDING
ATTACHMENT
PARENTING, CHILD CARE, AND
EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
JEAN MERCER
Mercer, Jean.
Understanding attachment: parenting, child care, and emotional development /
Jean Mercer,
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 0-275-98217-3 (alk. paper)
1. Attachment behavior. I. Title.
BF575.A86M47 2006
155.4'18—dc22 2005019272
10 98765432
For Andrew—
Preface ix
1. What Is Attachment? The Study of Emotional Ties 1
2. Babies, History, War, and Politics: Early Work on the
Attachment Concept 13
3. The Growth of Attachment Theory: Connecting Ideas
through Research 33
4. Attachment, Age, and Change: Emotional Ties from
Birth to Parenthood 55
5. Families, Experiences, and Outcomes: What Difference
Does Attachment History Make? 79
6. Attachment, Mental Health, and Psychotherapy:
Emotional Ties and Emotional Disturbance 103
7. What Everybody Knows: Popular Views of Attachment 127
8. N e w Directions: Parents, Children, and
Attachment Concepts 149
Notes 173
Bibliography 181
Index 189
This page intentionally left blank
Preface
I
first thought about writing this book while I was sitting in a court-
room. I was observing the trial of two unlicensed therapists who had
managed to kill a child they were treating and who had done so partly
because they did not understand the facts about attachment (part of this
story is told in Chapter 7 of this book). Expert witness after expert witness
testified, and the word "attachment" was used many times—but no one
said what it meant. I looked at the jury to see if they looked confused. They
didn't seem to be having any problem. I waited for the judge to ask them
if they understood the term, for the prosecutor—for anybody at all—to
inquire whether the jury understood this important issue. No one did. No
member of the jury asked for a definition, either.
During a break, I sidled up to one of the prosecutors and asked her
whether anyone was going to tell the jury what attachment was. She smiled
pleasantly and murmured something, but her thoughts were obviously on
other issues, such as conviction. I minded my own business, then, because
I thought what the therapists had done was seriously wrong, and I wanted
them to be convicted, too. If the prosecutor could manage without having
the jury understand what attachment was, that was fine with me. (They
were convicted and are serving sixteen-year sentences, by the way.)
Nevertheless, I was puzzled. "Attachment" is referred to and discussed
daily in the twenty-first-century United States. As you will see in this book,
legal decisions about children are made on the basis of this concept. We
seem to be living in the "Age of Attachment" rather than the Age of
X Preface
they are really quite right. Although an infant is a member of the human
species, he or she is not human in the full, mature, complete sense—the
sense that involves being a member of a group and caring about others. It
is the development of attachment that makes the baby a real human being
and prepares him or her to take his or her rightful place among other hu-
mans. How this happens to almost every individual is the real subject of
this book.
Our own family experiences cannot be the only source for understand-
ing emotional ties, but they can help us flesh out the sometimes dry bones
of systematic research. I have tried to include here some examples from my
own family life. In addition to personal experience, we can benefit from
the fact that attachment, separation, and loss have been the subjects of songs
and stories for generations. Literary treatments range from Biblical descrip-
tions of the prodigal son and the judgment of Solomon to modern forms—
the orphan status of Donald Duck's and Mickey Mouse's nephews, the
adoption of Superman, the lost father of Luke Skywalker. Orphanhood and
the lost or estranged parent have traditionally received the most emphasis;
as the blues song tells us: "Motherless children / Have a hard time / When
they mother is dead." More recently, the focus has been on family relation-
ships that are present but only partially satisfactory, as The Simpsons shows
us. The modern concern seems to be conveyed by Philip Larkin's poem,
"They you up, your mum and dad. / They do not mean to, but they
do." Artistic and literary descriptions of attachment can be vivid and
compelling, but they are not in themselves sources of information because
they are restatements of our own beliefs, and that is why we like them.
It may be valuable while reading to think from time to time of great writ-
ers' depictions of "disorder and early sorrow," or even of the mother of
James James Morrison Morrison Wetherby George Dupree, who went
downtown without consulting her three-year-old and, A. A. Milne tells us,
was never heard of since.
The hardest part about studying attachment may be abandoning old as-
sumptions and starting from scratch in our understanding of family emo-
tional connections. I hope this book will help readers accomplish this
important task and come to a new comprehension of a basic part of being
human, the source of some of our deepest pleasure as well as of our great-
est emotional agonies, without which we would not belong to the family
of man.
This page intentionally left blank
chapter 1
What Is Attachment?
The Study of
Emotional Ties
A nurse remarks, "A lot of my time at that job went into helping
mothers bond with their babies."
A mother tells her husband, "I like this day care center because they
keep changing Tommy's caregivers. I don't want him to get attached
to them."
A character in a detective novel comments, "He was ready to murder—
he never had any deep attachment after his mother."
A grandmother advises, "He just has that blanket because he's insecure.
You should take it away from him."
Another grandmother says, "We had never seen our grandson, but we
read him a story and we bonded right away."
A newspaper article describes a military interrogator as moving closer
to a prisoner "in order to bond with him."
2 Understanding Attachment
What Is Bonding?
So far, we have talked about attachment, love, and liking, and about the
connections between these feelings and thoughts about other people. What
happened to bonding? Why is it not mentioned? This term is frequently
used in people's descriptions of preferences for friends and relatives. "The
mother and the baby bonded." "We went fishing and did a little male
bonding." "My sister says her cat is unfriendly, but it bonded with me."
Television programs, newspapers, even courtroom discussions speak of
bonding as if the term is clearly understood.
The word bonding was originally used to refer to a father or mother's sud-
den development of positive, protective feelings toward a baby born to them
or a very young adopted infant.3 When people spoke of bonding in that
sense, they usually meant to imply certain ideas: (1) that bonding involved
the feelings and behavior of adults toward babies, not of babies toward adults;
6 Understanding Attachment
(2) that bonding involved a dramatic, irreversible shift in the adult's emo-
tional life; and (3) that bonding was needed for the child's sake because it
enabled the adult to do the hard work involved in early parenthood.
As time went on, however, there were some questions asked about these
assumptions, and the term bonding became popularized and used more fre-
quently with less and specific meaning. Today, you will often hear people
talk of bonding and attachment as if they were a single process. Nursing pro-
fessionals sometimes use bonding as a name for the things they do to foster a
good relationship between a new baby and mother—things like encouraging
skin-to-skin contact or showing the mother things the baby can do.
Whatever meaning bonding once had seems to have been lost, as the
term has been used more and more loosely and carelessly. This book will
not use the word "bonding" without the accompaniment of a clear defini-
tion, and then only with respect to a very narrow range of human relation-
ships. It might be good for readers to think twice before using a word that
communicates so many different things to different people. Although at-
tachment has its own confusions, it still maintains a common enough mean-
ing to be useful—and we really have no substitute that carries all the
meanings of attachment emotions, attachment behaviors, and internal
working models that grow out of early relationships.
Child Care
When children are cared for in groups outside their family homes, their
experiences can play an important role in their development. Regulations
governing day care centers should include applications of what we know
about attachment. For example, the numbers of children cared for by indi-
vidual day care staff members, staff turnover, and the ways that caregivers
interact with children are all related to attachment issues and can help or
hinder early emotional development.
Parents choosing day care arrangements need help in understanding the
role these experiences have in determining their children's development.
Motivated and educated parents are a critical force in bringing about regu-
lations that produce higher quality child care. A survey done by the orga-
nization Zero to Three 4 showed that many U.S. parents believe it is a good
thing for young children to have several different caregivers. Parents with
this misunderstanding may opt for child care arrangements that are poten-
tially harmful.
Foster Care
Foster care programs are an essential factor in the lives of thousands of
children whose parents are not able to care for them effectively. These
children may be placed in foster care because they have been neglected or
abused, or because the parents are unable to provide adequate housing or
other necessities. The parents may be young, poorly educated, or mentally
or physically handicapped, and drugs or alcohol may have played a part in
the family situation.
Although it might seem logical to expect that children in foster care
would welcome and appreciate a home without neglect or abuse, in fact,
these children suffer from being separated from their parents, and they need
sympathetic responses to their reactions to loss. The age at which foster
placement occurs is an important factor here. Foster parents need to be able
to work with children reacting to separation, and as we will see later, they
may need to be chosen for personality characteristics that enable them to
provide a good emotional environment for children.
Foster care programs need to take into account attachment issues, such
as the need for familiarity, consistency, and continuity of care. Frequent
8 Understanding Attachment
Kinship Care
For some children, a substitute for placement in the foster care system is
kinship care. When the parents are not successful in providing an appropri-
ate home, a child may be placed with a family member, not informally, but
with the approval and supervision of the state's child protective services
agency. Attachment is an issue in kinship care just as it is in other foster care
placements, unless the child has had a longstanding familiarity with the
family member. As we will see, the genetic relationship is no substitute for
the experience of a familiar relationship, and movement in and out of
kinship care needs to be planned as carefully as any other form of care.
Adoption
Of all practical situations, adoption is the one that seems to have the
greatest relevance to attachment. When adoption occurs, children are
asked to undergo and adjust to separation from familiar people, then to
develop a new attachment for the adoptive family. Adoptive parents do
not just want to care for and rear an adopted child; they want to create a
new family relationship and new emotional ties between themselves and
the child.
How well the new connections work, and what the adoptive parents
need to do, depends strongly on the age of the child at the time of adop-
tion. As we will see, applying the abstract concept of attachment to real-
world situations requires attention to age differences. Adoptive parents
need to understand how a child's emotional ties change with development
and how the family needs to handle children in different circumstances.
Even if they have raised other children successfully, adoptive parents may
need help and training in order to do well with adoptees.
A highly demanding situation for adoptive families involves foreign
adoptions. Children adopted from other countries can be difficult to com-
municate with because of different language backgrounds and what may
have been serious experiences of neglect and abuse. They may have had
little opportunity to form attachments in the past, and they may also have
physical and mental problems that interfere with emotional connections.
Adoptive families who are trying to cope with all these problems can benefit
What Is Attachment? 9
Handicapping Conditions
Whether they are adopted or not, children with certain handicapping
conditions may offer a challenge in terms of emotional development, as
well as mental or physical growth. As we will see later, young children
cement their emotional ties with their parents by communicating their in-
terest in familiar people. They do so with the sounds they make, through
eye contact, by following, and by expressions of pleasure or displeasure.
Children with handicaps may not be able to communicate in the emotional
ways their parents expect. For example, a visually impaired child may not
look at the parents' eyes or facial expression and respond to it as a normally
sighted child would, and the parents may interpret this as a lack of emo-
tional connection. Parents of children with handicaps may also need to
make a practical application of information about attachment.
Children with handicapping conditions or serious illnesses may spend
significant periods of time in hospitals, receiving treatment or recovering
from it. Information about attachment can help clarify their emotional
needs during these periods, as well as the ways in which medical staff and
parents can be most supportive.
further life changes. It is also important for attorneys and judges to be aware
of the effects of custody decisions on children's emotional lives.
School Issues
Although most readers will think of child care as a definite factor in early
emotional development, many will assume that by school age, children are
no longer vulnerable to separation or affected by other attachment issues.
However, separation and loss can have an effect on children's schoolwork,
play, and friendships, and schools can apply information about attachment in
ways that might reduce the impact of divorce or changes in living situations.
Such applications could be especially helpful for children in foster care.
Psychotherapy
Forms of psychotherapy that stress attachment are used in treatment of
both children and adults. It is possible, although not certain, that some
emotional problems are a direct result of difficulties in early attachment. It
is evident that diagnosis and treatment of emotional disturbance must in-
corporate the appropriate application of well-established principles of at-
tachment, or they risk doing harm rather than good.
H
uman beings have observed emotional development as long as
history has been recorded, and have paid a great deal of attention
to mothers' love for their children. The story of the judgment of
Solomon, like other tales, stresses the love of a mother for her child—in
this case so strong that the mother would rather lose the child and know it
to be well than keep it and have it harmed. Traditional stories of this type
gave less consideration to a child's preference for a familiar caregiver, but
our forebears did have some concerns about children's feelings.
M O T H E R LOVE
The mother's powerful preference and concern for her child were
sometimes considered to be an instinctive response, a biological charac-
teristic that humans shared with animals. (The difficulty of getting a
ewe to accept an orphan lamb when her own has died was well known,
for example.) At the same time it was recognized that an adoptive
mother could develop a strong emotional connection to a baby, who
would return her love. It was hard to argue that maternal love was
based on some biological response, built in and already present at the
time a baby was born. It was often assumed that the experience of
breastfeeding caused maternal affection to develop, and, although our
culture has largely forgotten the practice, adoptive nursing is possible for
humans.
14 Understanding Attachment
BABY LOVE
T h e idea that breastfeeding played a role in emotional preference was as-
sociated with the practice of wet nursing. T h r o u g h the eighteenth and n i n e -
teenth centuries, and even the early 1900s, it was not u n c o m m o n for a
family to hire a wet nurse—a lactating w o m a n w h o was paid to breastfeed an
infant. Some wet nurses lived with the family, and some took the child for
long periods of time to their o w n homes, possibly at a considerable distance.
This arrangement might have been made because the biological m o t h e r was
sick or had died; it might have allowed the m o t h e r to carry on earning
m o n e y with skilled work; it could have been based on the wish to allow the
m o t h e r to start another pregnancy quickly; or it might have resulted simply
from the wish for a fashionable figure and free social life.1 Discussion of wet
nursing usually stressed the possible dangers of a nurse with unwholesome
milk, or the possibility that undesirable traits could be taken in with the milk.
T h e r e was little concern about effects on the mother-child relationship.
This strategy was effective; the child was allowed to spend another year
with the nurse, and a gradual separation at that time left h i m in good health.
T h e obvious cause of the children's preference for their w e t nurses was
the experience of breastfeeding, the w e t nurse's primary responsibility. It
Babies, History, War, and Politics 15
was generally ignored that wet nurses also bathed, dressed, played with,
comforted, and even slept with their charges, all possible sources of emo-
tional connection. This view has not changed greatly, and in fact the aver-
age person in the United States is convinced that breastfeeding is desirable
because it creates mutual love, although, as we will see, there is little evi-
dence to support this idea.
FORMAL THEORIES O F A T T A C H M E N T
So far, our discussion on the history of the attachment concept has dealt
with informal, unsophisticated ideas shared by average parents and their
medical advisers. By the end of the nineteenth century, however, some
writers began to develop formal concepts of human emotional develop-
ment that included attachment processes.
Sigmund Freud
Freud based his thinking about attachment on the belief that feeding cre-
ates the child's emotional preference. Though development of the concept
6 Understanding Attachment
eventually surpassed this early idea, the foundation of attachment for Freud
was what our grandmothers called "cupboard love."
To discuss what Freud thought on a given matter is not as simple as it
may appear. Freud's ideas developed and changed in the context of a long
and eventful professional life. His conceptualization of the events of the
first years of life did not change greatly, however, even when some of his
ideas about human motivation altered.
To understand Freud's view of what we now call attachment, we need
to examine his ideas about the nature of infant personality as stated in about
1916. The newborn was seen as being driven by a combined need for food
and desire for the pleasure of sucking. Initially, in this view, the young
child's interests are narcissistic and focused on the self. Even when at the
breast, the baby concentrates on its own bodily sensations and gratifica-
tions. Nursing mothers take advantage of this by cleaning the baby's ears or
cutting nails while there is no resistance because the child is absorbed in
sucking. Gradually, though, that outside object, the breast, comes to be of
interest and is gratifying in and of itself. The mother becomes a love object,
not just a useful contraption for presenting the breast. Staying near the
mother then means security and happiness, and the threat of separation
becomes a cause for anxiety.
In order to disconnect the pleasure of feeding from the experience of
play and comfort, we would have to look for cases where one adult fed
the baby and another played and comforted it. In Freud's day, as in our
own, this would have been an uncommon situation. A baby may have
more than one caregiver, but each one generally provides a full spectrum
of care when on duty. Freud's view of feeding as a necessary cause of the
child's emotional preference emerged from the confusion of social and
biological factors (just as had earlier been the case when people thought
about wet nurses). The related fact that caregivers and feeders were almost
exclusively female led to the assumption that the mother was the focus of
the child's early emotional life. In addition, Freud stressed biological
drives like hunger because of their obvious connection with medicine
and the natural sciences—fields with respectable foundations of evidence
that reached far beyond what existed at the time for social and behavioral
studies.
Ian Suttie
Freud's many followers, and some of his opponents, maintained that
the emotional preference of the child for the familiar caregiver arose from
the gratification of being fed. However, this belief plays little or no role
in the modern concept of attachment.
Babies, History, War, and Politics 17
In fact, late in Freud's life at least one thinker was already abandoning the
assumption that childhood emotional preference was founded on feeding
experiences. Dr. Ian Suttie argued that the child's need for the mother's
love was a primary necessity in itself and did not have to be connected with
the satisfaction of hunger. 5 Suttie was far more inclined than Freud to ex-
amine social and cultural factors and to note that not all groups of people
have the same childhood experiences, but, like Freud, he was not greatly
concerned with the smaller details of the child's development of emotional
preferences for people. Suttie's early death prevented him from elaborating
on these ideas.
The Ethologists
While Freud and Suttie were considering early emotional development
in humans, a quite different group of thinkers was approaching some re-
lated problems by studying the behavior and development of animals. The
German researcher Konrad Lorenz was a leader among them.
Lorenz worked in a discipline that attempted to explain behavior primar-
ily in terms of hereditary factors, with little emphasis on the role of learn-
ing. Focusing primarily on animal behavior, ethologists had little interest in
the effect of cultural differences on the individual. Their mission was to
describe behavioral characteristics of animal species, but there eventually
were some attempts to apply ethological concepts to human beings.6
Ethological theory had at least as strong an influence on modern ideas
about attachment as Freudian theory did and, thus, needs to be examined.
Before the twentieth century began, there had been many studies of ani-
mals using a natural history approach, but ethologists brought to their tasks
a clear theoretical system that went beyond simple observation. Certain
assumptions guided their work:
like an eye blink, but with complicated actions, such as a mother mouse
caring for her offspring by following them and returning them to the
nest if they crawled out. Behaviors like this have many steps and must
be flexible and elaborate, guiding the mouse to follow her pups in dif-
ferent directions or to sniff them out behind an obstacle.
3. Ethologists assumed that inherited behaviors were species-specific.
Members of the species who are capable carry out these species-specific
behaviors (for example, a male opossum does not carry young in a
pouch, of course). There might be instinctive behaviors found only
among males or only among females of the species, and similarly there
might be instinctive behaviors that occurred only at a certain point in
development, not in older or in younger individuals.
4. Ethologists assumed that instinctive behaviors had some real connec-
tion to survival of the species. The types of behavior most likely to
involve instinct would be eating, drinking, defense, courtship, mating,
and care of the young; behaviors like intelligent learning and problem-
solving were much less likely to be instinctive. Of course, the most
important aspects of instinct from the perspective of this book are those
related to attachment issues: possible instinctive reactions of the young
to their caregivers, reactions that could mesh with the adults' instinc-
tive care behaviors in ways that facilitate survival of the young and of
the species.
5. Ethologists assumed that instinctive behaviors were usually directed
toward something in the environment and contributed to survival.
Generally, the objects that receive the animal's instinctive behavior
also release the behavior by providing the kinds of stimuli that cause
the behavior to begin. If the stimuli are the wrong ones, a normal in-
stinctive behavior may not occur. A baby animal that is sick or mal-
formed may not release its mother's instinctive caregiving behavior,
and a mother who behaves inappropriately may not release the baby's
instinctive responses to her.
To say that a behavior is instinctive does not mean that it will
occur in its perfect form under all circumstances. Nor does it mean
that the outcome of instinctive behavior is always appropriate. A
suspended, stuffed head of a female will release the male mating
behavior in some bird species. The males approach, position them-
selves, and copulate with her nonexistent body just as they would
with an intact live female.
6. The idea that instinctive behaviors are connected to their objects opens
the door to an important ethological idea about learning. By defini-
tion, instinctive behaviors are unlearned, but an animal might need to
learn something about the objects toward which instinctive acts can be
Babies, History, War, and Politics 9
directed. Hunting and eating prey may be instinctive, but a wolf that
encounters a porcupine learns rapidly that a tender rabbit is a better
target than a painfully prickly beast. The ability to learn about the
potential objects of instinctive behavior is especially important for adults'
care of the young and for the young animal's appropriate response.
A female sheep, for example, quickly learns the smell of her own
lamb and will care only for that one, butting away any wandering or-
phan that tries to nurse. This devotion to her own lamb helps ensure
its survival, which would be endangered had the mother not learned to
recognize her own. Similarly, lambs, calves, and the young of other
species quickly learn to follow their own mothers, not just any female
of their species (most of whom would not care for the little one).
These animals' instinctive behaviors have to be combined with indi-
vidual learning for the best survival results, so the parallel with human
attachment is obvious.
IMPRINTING
Ethologists were especially intrigued by a rapid type of learning they
called imprinting. This type of learning was most obvious in young birds,
such as ducklings, whose early and later life experiences alter the object of
their instinctive behavior.
Young ducklings have the innate, unlearned behavior, in the first few
days after hatching, of following a moving object. In their natural life in the
wild, that object is most likely to be the mother duck, who has made her
nest and laid her eggs in a relatively solitary place. In other settings, how-
ever, the ducklings will follow other moving objects that happen to be
present: other kinds of birds, cats, dogs, people, or even toy trains. The
instinctive following seems to be released by the movement the duckling
sees, not by the kind of object that is moving.
Within a few days after this hatching and following, however, the young
duckling stops following just any moving object. It now restricts its follow-
ing to the kind of object that it initially saw and followed soon after it
hatched, whether this object is its own mother, a human caretaker, or even
something dangerous like a fox. The duckling is now considered to be
imprinted on the object it followed earlier and only that or similar objects
can release the instinctive following reaction.
Imprinting is, by definition, a form of learning because it involves a
change in behavior that depends on experience. It is an unusual type of
learning, though, because it only happens quickly and effectively during a
specific time period. (Ordinary learning happens in roughly the same way
at most times during an individual's life.) Imprinting has a different speed
20 Understanding Attachment
and pattern than other kinds of learning. To think of it another way, im-
printing is based not just on experience but also on an interaction between
the two basic mechanisms of development: experience and maturation.
Human attachment also involves both experience and maturation.
CRITICAL PERIODS
The time during which imprinting works best is called a critical or sensitive
period. Time is essential to the idea of imprinting, because an experience at
a particular time has an effect on development that it would not have had
earlier and will not have later. It is in this window of time that critical and
special processes appear to occur. Many critical periods happen early in
development, a point that we will need to keep in mind when we examine
modern thinking about attachment.
If you have been around farm animals, it probably does not surprise you
at all that ducks follow moving things, even things that are not at all duck-
like. What may be more surprising is that the duck's imprinting experiences
during an early critical period have a long-term effect on the duck, even
after it has passed the early following stage. As the imprinted duck becomes
mature and gets ready to mate, its preference for a partner is determined not
just by ordinary duck beauty and charm, but by the effects of the imprint-
ing experience. The duck remains extremely resistant to change and now
courts another duck, or a human being, or a toy train, the choice depend-
ing on the early following experience. Zookeepers must be very cautious
when hand-rearing birds that imprint, or they will find it impossible to
breed them as adults. Bird caretakers have been known to use a hand pup-
pet to attract the young bird's interest, as unrequited love for the zookeeper
may later cause the bird to reject mates of its own species.
THE FOUNDATIONS OF
A T T A C H M E N T T H E O R Y A N D W O R L D EVENTS
By the 1930s, much of Freudian theory and ethological theory were al-
ready in place. It would have been possible to blend these two views and
to produce the theoretical approach to attachment that emerged in the
1950s and later, but this blending did not occur.
World events in the 1930s and 1940s simultaneously postponed theo-
retical advances and piqued interest in early emotional life. The commit-
ment to "total war" during World War II delayed advances in theory.
However, due to circumstances resulting from the war, large groups of
young children were separated from familiar caregivers. This had occurred
for individuals in the past, but reactions could not readily be recorded.
Babies, History, War, and Politics 21
Given the numbers of children involved during World War II, the possibil-
ity of observations on early emotional development increased, and research
interest grew.
Even before the war began, social and medical changes had already begun
to call attention to the effects of separation. Until well into the twentieth
century, young children who needed intensive medical treatment fell into
two categories: if poor, they were likely to die untreated; if affluent, they
received surgical or medical treatment in their own homes. By the 1930s,
advances in medical techniques and an increased awareness of infection
made it more likely that children would be hospitalized, but until the ad-
vent of antibiotics, fear of infection set severe limits on visits by parents.
During this period, large numbers of children experienced not only fear
and pain but also abrupt separation from their parents. One man who was
a child during this time recalls his tonsillectomy at age three. His mother
brought him to the hospital, handed him to a nurse, and then left, returning
for him as instructed ten days later. He did not speak again for a year after
this event.
John Bowlby
During and following World War II, a new and important formulation
of ideas about emotional development was offered by the theorist whose
name is invariably linked with this topic: John Bowlby.10 "Attachment the-
ory" and "Bowlby's attachment theory" are almost synonymous terms to-
day, although, as we will see in Chapter 8, another form of attachment
theory, not identical with Bowlby's work, is beginning to emerge.
Bowlby's theory of attachment involved ideas about human develop-
ment that were somewhat unusual for their time, although not completely
unique. He believed that the emotional attachments of infants and toddlers
to their caregivers were based on social interactions, not on physical grati-
fications, and that they were built into human beings as a result of adapta-
tion during early evolution. He saw a developmental timetable for
attachment emotions and behaviors that was determined by an inherited
human nature, not by learning. Most importantly—and, from the modern
viewpoint, most questionably—Bowlby considered early attachment expe-
riences to have a powerful effect on personality development. He was par-
ticularly interested in the impact of attachment history on mental health
and on criminal behavior. As a leader in the international discussion of
child development, Bowlby was in a position to stress this view, and a
world that had so recently seen thousands of children separated from their
parents was ready to listen carefully. It was within the context of Bowlby's
thoughts that most of the events about to be described occurred.
At night, small children have only their mutual comforting to rely on,
since it may take quite a while for the single night worker to hear a
child who wakes up crying.... Even when the night watch is finally
summoned by the child's anxious cry, the person who comes when
26 Understanding Attachment
Romanian Adoptees
Under the rule of the dictator Nicolae Ceausescu, the small, poor coun-
try of Romania developed a frighteningly large population of children who
existed in institutions. (Existed is the correct word; to say they were cared
for is stretching the meaning of that term.) From 1965 until 1989, Ceausescu
and his party enforced social regulations intended to double the population
and provide plenty of cheap labor. Women were not allowed access to
birth control or abortion until they had had four or five children.
Because of the country's extreme poverty, Romanian families were, in
many cases, unable to care for the children they bore. Orphanages were
established to "warehouse" children from poor families, and it became so-
cially acceptable to place children in these institutions. A large proportion
of the orphanage children came from Romany or Gypsy families, an ethnic
group considered inferior and undesirable. Children with birth defects and
problems, such as fetal alcohol syndrome, were especially likely candidates
for orphanages.
Conditions in the orphanages were almost unimaginable. Children re-
ceived little care, often being tied into cribs and given food in bottles until
a late age. There were few toys, little opportunity for free movement, and
almost no social stimulation or responsiveness from adults. Medical care
was minimal, and special needs children received no treatment for their
problems. Malnutrition was common, as were skull deformities caused by
lying in the same position for long periods. Many had hearing problems as
a result of untreated infections. There is little doubt that physical abuse, as
well as neglect, occurred at the hands of attendants and older children.
Considerable numbers of the children were HIV-positive. Because of the
institutional conditions and their pre-existing problems, many of the or-
phans showed severe developmental delays, failing to walk or talk until well
after the expected age.
In 1989, at the end of the Ceausescu regime, there were over 150,000
children in these Romanian institutions. Sadly, the changes in Romania's
social practices under that regime persisted after the fall of the dictator, and
abandonment of children by poor families has continued.
Beginning shortly after Ceausescu's fall, Western families began to adopt
children from Romanian orphanages, and they have continued to do so
until very recently. In 2004 Romania formulated a policy prohibiting many
foreign adoptions.
28 Understanding Attachment
being left in places where they were likely to be found, or if they were sick,
at hospitals.
Adoption of Chinese orphans by Westerners began in about 1994 and
increased in frequency at a rapid rate. Girls are generally seen as manage-
able, quiet, and affectionate, and Westerners stereotypically think of Asians
as cooperative and intelligent. In addition, these children are babies, mak-
ing them even more desirable.
the quasi-experimental type of study done by the ERA Study Team, yielded
such a mass of puzzling variables that it is difficult to determine cause and
effect or to know whether a child's condition was a result of the orphanage
experience, abandonment, a combination of the two, or the orphan's treat-
ment following adoption. This work involves multiple questions and looks
at the immediate result of the children's orphanage experiences and the
children's ability to recover when placed with adoptive families, making
the resulting data exceedingly complex.
One built-in problem with this type of study is the factor of spontaneous
developmental change. However dreadful the conditions, a child who sur-
vives continues to develop, although not necessarily at a normal rate or
along a normal trajectory. Changes in adopted children are to some extent
simply the result of continuing maturation, though the nurturing provided
by the adoptive family also plays a role. Recovery from a harmful experi-
ence may largely be a matter of resilience—one's natural ability to get back
on a normal developmental pathway. All studies of child development have
to deal with this factor, as did the Romanian orphan research.
The research on the Romanian orphans had other problems. It was dif-
ficult to know what the conditions had been before the children were in-
stitutionalized. Family poverty, maternal malnutrition, maternal depression,
prenatal exposure to alcohol or drugs, problems at the time of birth, genetic
issues—these could all increase a child's vulnerability to deprivation and
lessen the child's response to better care following adoption. Although or-
phanage conditions were uniformly abominable, another difficulty had to
do with knowing an individual child's experiences. Something that clearly
made a difference was the length of time the child spent in the institution.
Children who were very appealing might have received a bit more atten-
tion, and even the experience of being with slightly older children could
have been beneficial (depending on the other children's characteristics).
The results of the ERA Study Team's work are far from simple. Some
of the children, in fact, recovered very well from their experiences after
several years of adoptive life. Others did well medically, but showed prob-
lems in their emotional and cognitive development. Importantly, there
was support for the idea that separation is only one of many important fac-
tors in development and may have a critical impact if followed up by poor
parenting.16
Do the statements in the last paragraph apply to children in U.S. foster
homes as well as to Eastern European orphans? It would be fair to say that
attachment theory has not yet integrated the ERA findings. It may be that
the conclusion above applies when a child has been exposed to many risk
factors, but not otherwise. However, most children in the United States
today do not experience separation except in the context of serious family
Babies, History, War, and Politics 31
problems and troubled lives, and the ERA Team's conclusion may well
apply to ordinary foster care children, as well as to children dramatically
rescued from appalling institutional conditions.
CONCLUSION
The understanding of attachment has grown gradually over the centu-
ries, but theory development and research have progressed most rapidly in
the twentieth and twenty-first centuries. Early psychoanalytic thought fo-
cused on the relationship between mother and child, theorizing the con-
nection to be based largely on the child's physical needs. Later theories
considered the possibility that social needs and behaviors are innate human
tendencies rather than responses based on hunger and discomfort. Evidence
from studies of children separated from their parents during World War II
suggests that attachment and separation played essential roles in emotional
development, and John Bowlby based an entire theory of attachment on
this and later information. In the 1990s, new research was conducted when
large groups of children were adopted from foreign countries where they
had been institutionalized. This study suggests that separation alone does
not play as important a role in emotional development as Bowlby thought,
but theories of attachment have yet to respond to this work.
This page intentionally left blank
chapter 3
The Growth of
Attachment Theory:
Connecting Ideas
through Research
T
he end of World War II allowed thinkers, who had for years been
busy with national wartime concerns, the leisure and time to consider
attachment. As we saw in the previous chapter, Britain had become
the center of psychoanalytic thought as the Nazis destroyed German intel-
lectual life. Ethological theory also found a British home with the immigra-
tion of the Dutch ethologist Nikolaas Tinbergen, winner of the Nobel Prize
for Medicine, and his colleague Konrad Lorenz. In postwar Britain, psycho-
analytic and ethological ideas came together, as did the material to which they
could be applied: observations of children separated from familiar people.
children the "usual behavior was replaced by something that could vary from
extreme friendliness to any human partner combined with anxious avoidance
of inanimate objects, to a generalized anxiety expressed in blood-curdling
screams which could go on indefinitely."1 The implications for these chil-
dren's emotional development were worrisome, but the research itself pro-
vided one of many steps in the formulation of a valid theory of attachment.
The new studies were also shaped by an idea shared by scientists in
a variety of fields. For some years before the war, a sophisticated view
of scientific work had been emerging, and in the form of operationism,
this approach to knowledge influenced the investigation of attachment.
Operationism takes the stance that communication about scientific knowl-
edge is primarily communication about measurement. If I want to tell
someone about an observation I have made, I must recount what opera-
tions—that is, measurements—I used in order to keep track of what I saw.
Only if two observers share the same operations do they know whether
they have seen the same thing, and only then can one check his or her
observations against the other's. If observers of little boys and little girls'
attachment behavior use different operations and different definitions, they
are very likely to report that they find sex differences—even though the
children may actually be very similar, if measured with the same ruler. The
social sciences are particularly in need of operational definitions, and this
was a real issue for the early developers of attachment concepts as they
struggled to decide whether a child demonstrated attachment or not.
As theories about attachment were formulated, they were also guided by
another concept connected with operationism: the idea of falsifiability. This
term sounds like it means "fudging the data," but it actually is a way for
researchers to keep themselves honest. Falsifiability is the possibility that
expected research results will not come out the way a researcher thought
they would, and research must be designed in ways that allow this to hap-
pen. A researcher abandons falsifiability if he or she throws out all the data
that do not support the hypothesis—as sometimes happens in the study of
parapsychology, for example.
When researchers stress falsifiability, they are allowing their developing
theory to be guided by systematic empirical evidence. A theory whose
predictions are not always supported by evidence must be modified, and
modification is what modern theorists of early emotional development
have attempted to do as they study attachment.
T W O THINKERS
Out of the foundations of time and place described above came two
people whose work forms the essential basis of modern theories of
The Growth of Attachment Theory 35
attachment: John Bowlby and Mary Salter Ainsworth. Both Bowlby and
Ainsworth had unusual qualifications in research as well as in clinical
training. Both stressed excellent observation as the foundation of knowl-
edge about child development, prefiguring the mantra of developmental-
ist Sally Provence: "Don't just do something; stand there and watch."
Bowlby's and Ainsworth's lives and thought have been beautifully de-
scribed by Jeremy Holmes and Inge Bretherton, 2 and much of the mate-
rial below has been drawn from that work.
but this recent change does not erase the characteristics that evolved
long ago.
4. Separation anxiety is an indication that attachment has occurred and
therefore a normal and desirable event in emotional development.
Ordinarily, the child's distress is soon relieved by reunion with the
caregiver. When separation from familiar people is abrupt and long
lasting, however, the child who has achieved attachment is not only
anxious, but can be expected to go through predictable stages of griev-
ing and emotional reorganization. Although the steps of infant grief are
intense, worrisome, and demanding of adult support, they are never-
theless a normal and necessary response to serious separations. Neither
separation anxiety, nor grief folio wing serious loss, would be expected
until after there had been a focus on a familiar caregiver, and as we saw
above, this does not happen until six months at the earliest.
5. Although Bowlby did not emphasize this point, it is important to real-
ize that attachment emotions and attachment behavior are age-related,
with upper as well as lower limits. Just as the characteristics we have
discussed are not displayed before six months, at the earliest, they do
not occur in the same form after, perhaps, three years of age. The at-
tachment behaviors Bowlby initially described are typical of the late
infant period, the toddler stage, and the early preschool years. Emotional
ties alter and modulate with age, and so do related feelings and actions,
as we will see in a later chapter.
6. In later work, Bowlby suggested that attachment behaviors and emo-
tions are connected with the individual's internal working model of
the social world. This suggestion offered a way for attachment to
change developmentally as the individual passed the age period in
which instinctual factors played the strongest role. An internal working
model is a set of memories, emotions, and thoughts that determines a
person's expectations and attitudes, and that consequently shapes be-
havior. Attachment-related behaviors like preferring certain people,
maintaining near or far physical distance from those attachment fig-
ures, seeking help from people, and displaying distress upon separation
or loss are determined by this internal working model.
Of course, attachment-related behaviors are not necessarily directed
toward a consistent set of people throughout a lifetime, nor are the
behaviors themselves always the same. These facts show us that the
internal working model changes with age and experience. For example,
a baby girl may crawl rapidly to her mother and signal to be picked up
when frightened by a loud noise. Thirty years later, she may look
around to locate her husband when jostled in a crowd of people,
or may move to protect her own baby, ignoring potential threats to
40 Understanding Attachment
showed another. (Then, as now, it was not completely clear whether babies
influenced mothers, mothers influenced babies, or each affected the other.)
Ainsworth's Baltimore research, a model demonstration of a new ap-
proach, involved the study of connected patterns of behavior, rather than
old-fashioned frequency counts of events like crying.11 More complicated
and difficult to do than the older style of research, Ainsworth's work was
also far more capable of displaying the complexities of developmental
change. By demonstrating these complexities, Ainsworth made a critical
contribution to attachment theory as Bowlby was elaborating it.
Showing how mothers' actions guided their children's attachment was
valuable in itself, but it also demonstrated the importance of the internal
working model of social relationships. Repeated acts by the mothers trig-
gered responses in the babies, but these involved more than the primitive,
instinctual level of action suggested by the ethologists. These repeated
events let babies learn what they might expect from other human beings.
They established a pattern of expectations and attitudes that started with the
earliest relationships and would be generalized and applied to new people.
For example, the expectations learned from a mother would also be ex-
tended to a child care provider and later to a kindergarten teacher, and so
on. Importantly, the internal working model could change and develop as
the child grew and had new experiences. Early instinctual behaviors did not
change with experiences, but the internal working model of attachment
could continue to alter, eventually giving rise to the individual's attitude
toward his or her own child. Lest this description of the internal working
model sound too cool and intellectual to have anything to do with attach-
ment, let us recall that the model includes emotion and motivation based
on the powerful early feelings of the toddler seeking his or her mother's
comfort and fleeing the stranger's approach.
The Strange Situation: A Behavior Sample. Ainsworth was now interested
in two new measurement problems. Visiting families in Uganda and in
Baltimore had been enormously useful, but the collected information was
very difficult to analyze. Families can be quite different. How can we com-
pare them to each other? And, if the mother's behavior is so important,
how can we possibly tease it out of the complications of real-life situations
where mother, father, children, culture, home, and accident determine
how people act?
Ainsworth wanted to establish some way of describing infant behavior
which would allow her to compare babies, or families, or cultural groups
without being confused by background details. She was especially inter-
ested in how infants behaved in high or low stress situations, but how
could she create or reduce stress in a family, even temporarily? She needed
a standardized test, one in which the situation could be controlled, all
The Growth of Attachment Theory 43
mothers and children had the same experiences, and trained observers
could collect the same information each time. Only then would it be pos-
sible to make comparisons or even to find norms and understand what
were frequent and what were unusual behavior patterns.
The result of Ainsworth's efforts was a test known as the Strange Situa-
tion. This measurement device has been the basis of thousands of research
studies. Indeed, we may wonder whether the topic of attachment would
ever have achieved its present eminence without the Strange Situation as a
means of exploration, and as an answer to the question: how in the world
can we compare these babies?
The Strange Situation. Like many other psychological tests, the Strange
Situation involves a narrow sample of information drawn from a wide range
of measurable factors. First, it is intended to test babies at about twelve months
of age (although some researchers have not paid much attention to this). All
the evidence shows that the great majority of babies of this age show attach-
ment behaviors, readily and intensely. This is the best time to catch behav-
ioral evidence of the child's emotional connection to a caregiver.
Second, the Strange Situation chooses a simple, common, mildly threat-
ening situation, and keeps it very brief. Though an effort is made to limit
unhappiness, the intention, of course, is to cause enough distress to the
child to elicit attachment behavior. This approach produces stress at the
level a child might feel when the mother steps away briefly when shop-
ping, but not nearly at the level of attachment behavior that would occur
when, for example, a parent and child are both terrified by a ferocious dog,
a parent runs to help another child who is hurt and screaming, or there
is domestic violence.
The Strange Situation: Wlwt Happens, What Is Measured. The Strange Situa-
tion is intended to assess the toddler's responses to a brief separation from
the mother, followed by a reunion. It takes about twenty-two minutes
altogether and requires a room with certain features and an adult who is a
stranger to the child. There are eight steps or episodes in the Strange
Situation procedure.
Episode 1: The mother and baby enter the room with an observer who
shows the mother where she can put the baby down and then where she
can sit.
Episode 2: The mother puts the baby down near some toys. The mother
does not start playing with the toys, but she may respond if the baby
starts, for example, to bring a toy, put it in her lap, or hand it to her. If
the baby has not started playing after two minutes, the mother may take
the baby to the toys. (For completeness, we should note that by this age
44 Understanding Attachment
normal babies can crawl or even walk alone; they can pick up toys, put
them down, or throw them; they understand some words and may speak
a little. Although they do not play in very complex, planned ways, they
are interested in toys; they like to pick them up, mouth them, throw
them, or bang them against things. If they are walking, they often like to
carry things, especially big things.)
Episode 3: A stranger comes into the room, greets the mother and baby,
and sits quietly near the mother for one minute. During the next minute,
the stranger talks with the mother. Then for a third minute, the stranger
gets down on the floor and tries to play with the baby. The mother then
leaves the room quietly. The baby generally notices that she is leaving.
(This introduces, in a mild form, the stress factor that interested
Ainsworth.)
Episode 4: The mother is still absent. The stranger sits on her chair, but
responds to the baby, if the baby tries to start play. The stranger offers
comfort, if the baby shows distress. The mother stays out for three min-
utes, if the baby is not upset, but if there is distress, the mother returns
and the stranger leaves.
Episode 5: The mother calls the baby's name from outside the door and
then comes in. If the baby needs comfort, the mother offers it and tries to
get play started; if not, she sits on her chair and responds, but does not start
play. After three minutes, she leaves, saying, "Bye-bye, I'll be back soon."
Episode 6: The baby stays alone for three minutes. If there is distress, the
stranger comes back.
Episode 7: The stranger offers comfort, if the baby is distressed, or else
stays seated on the chair for three minutes. If the baby is too upset, the
mother returns before the three minutes have elapsed.
Episode 8: The mother returns, the stranger leaves, and the mother
spends three minutes, as she did in the first episode.
OTHER MEASURES O F A T T A C H M E N T
Fascinating and productive as the Strange Situation is, it is not the final
step in measuring attachment patterns. As human beings get older, they
continue to have emotional attachments to others, but the ways they show
these attachments change with age. Even preschoolers placed in a situation
like the Strange Situation do not behave in exactly the same ways as twelve-
month-olds. By adulthood, people still have emotional bonds and impor-
tant internal working models of social relationships; these are expressed in
mature ways, not like those of an infant. (We would be very worried about
an adult who cried when a loved one had been away for three minutes.)
Individuals who are past the toddler stage follow different rules, and they
cannot be tested in the same manner.
There has been work in assessment of attachment in preschool children,
using techniques such as asking children to finish incomplete stories.
Assessment of school-age children has proved more difficult, and the
eminent child psychiatrist Charles Zeanah has expressed doubt that any-
one is presently capable of managing this. One approach for preschool and
school-age children has been the use of a Q-sort technique in which
observers decide which type of attachment is characteristic of a given
child.14
More recently, the attention of researchers has turned toward the as-
sessment of adult attachment relationships, including connections with
parents, marriage and friendships, and attitudes toward children. Again, this
kind of measurement is not going to resemble the Strange Situation. Adults
do not show the same responses to separation as toddlers do, though there
are some similarities, such as behaving differently when around strangers
or feeling ambivalent when reunited with a person who has made us feel
deserted.
The study of attachment relationships in adults focuses on the internal
working model of social relations, which has presumably changed, devel-
oped, and become far more elaborate as the years have passed. We may be
able to get at that model more effectively by talking about relationships,
rather than by looking at overt behavior. The Adult Attachment Interview
is a way to do this.13
The Adult Attachment Interview uses open-ended questions about
childhood experiences to get at the adult's internal working model. The
specific experiences reported are meaningful. Stories of a family cooperating
happily suggest a very different working model than do tales of bleak years
in an orphanage or an abusive foster home. But analysis of the Adult
Attachment Interview goes beyond the stories that are told to the way the
interviewee tells them. Do the stories give an organized, coherent picture
The Growth of Attachment Theory 47
of the individual's thoughts about family history? Are there separate stories
that do not form a consistent pattern? Does the individual speak in clear,
descriptive sentences, or hesitate, trail off, begin again? Does the inter-
viewee claim that he or she does not remember much about childhood
family relations and dismiss them as a matter of little interest?
As was the case for the Strange Situation, responses to the Adult
Attachment Interview can be placed into a few categories. Adults are con-
sidered to be in the autonomous-secure group, if they recount an organized
story of childhood memories—whether or not they find the memories
pleasing or satisfying. The term preoccupied is used to categorize adults who
have many, contradictory childhood memories, but no way of organizing
them into a meaningful whole. Finally, those who state that they have few
or no memories of early family relations are classified as showing a dismissing
pattern. Adults' responses to the AAI are quite reliable, that is, researchers
usually find similar results each time, and these results do not seem to be
related to other memory or intelligence factors.16
ATTACHMENT THEORY A N D
MEASUREMENT: LINGERING QUESTIONS
Our historical account has shown how attachment theory has grown
over the last fifty years or more, but there are some broad issues and impli-
cations that still need consideration. The very idea of emotional attach-
ment, as Bowlby described it, is attractive to some and almost repulsive to
others. Some responses seem almost parallel to the early rejection of Freud's
view of infantile sexuality. College students are often negative about the
idea of attachment, perhaps fearing that the autonomy desirable at their
present age would be impossible if attachment had already occurred. Others
who are newly introduced to the attachment concept become thoroughly
confused. They try to puzzle through Bowlby's theory and find it disturb-
ing that they cannot reduce this complex system to one or two simple
foundations. To close, we need to deal with some general questions about
attachment theory, as it exists today.
Is Attachment Good?
The short-term advantages of an infant's attachment are evident. In early
childhood, the major behavior linked to attachment involves staying near a
familiar adult, especially in a strange place, or when anything unusual is
happening. The wish to stay near acts as an invisible playpen and has enor-
mous safety value for creepers and toddlers. Unexpected events or strange
people send the child scurrying to the caregiver's side. Although adults al-
ways need to monitor young children carefully, normal attachment behav-
ior means that the adult does not have to spend all of his or her time
retrieving a wandering child.
Whether attachment is desirable over the longer term is a question fre-
quently asked by young parents. Parents in the United States place a high
value on independence and are concerned with the idea that attachment in
early childhood might culminate in a weak, dependent adult personality.
The idea that parents should be sensitive and responsive to infant's signals,
and should let the child stay close, arouses in many American parents the
fear that the child will be spoiled, demanding, selfish, cranky, and undisci-
plined. American parents often prefer the idea that their children will grow
up into independent, self-sufficient, friendly people. They may be con-
cerned that long-lasting emotional ties will somehow make these character-
istics impossible to achieve. Some have even expressed the wish that their
day care center frequently change children's caregivers to prevent attach-
ment,17 or they have rejected breastfeeding because they feel it may cause a
baby to be too attached to the mother.
As we will see in later chapters, however, there are probably no grounds
for the concern that attachment will cause harm, especially in the form of
clinginess and dependency in later life. On the contrary, there is a good
deal of evidence that secure attachment provides an excellent foundation
for personality and social development. A securely attached child has a
better chance to be both independent and friendly than one who has had
poor early relationships. Adults who have had good early attachment
50 Understanding Attachment
CONCLUSION
Our growing understanding of attachment has involved the interweav-
ing of measurement techniques and theoretical changes. Modern demands
for systematic evidence encouraged the formulation of measurement pro-
cedures that made it possible to test aspects of attachment. The data pro-
duced by those techniques was fed back into theory and stimulated new
ways of thinking. Attachment theory developed in step with attachment
measurement, but at the same time was influenced by and instrumental in
the growth of evolutionary psychology.
This page intentionally left blank
chapter 4
Attachment, Age,
and Change: Emotional
Ties from Birth to
Parenthood
O
ur struggles in the last chapter with the definition of attachment
made it clear that this term has different meanings w h e n applied
to people of different ages. Attachment is not a thing inside a
h u m a n being or a p e r m a n e n t change that is exactly like a duck's imprint-
ing, but a w h o l e pattern of behaviors, feelings, and ideas that change grad-
ually as the individual develops. T h e great importance of this pattern is that
it continues to influence our social relationships t h r o u g h o u t life.
T h e study of developmental changes in attachment gives us essential in-
formation that helps our understanding of groups (adopted children, for
example) and of individuals (such as children w h o seem unusually clingy
and concerned with their parents). R e n e Spitz, writing m o r e than half a
century ago, prefigured this statement w h e n he urged
way as she used to do. Both people have changed very gradually as a result
of the many occasions when they have affected each other, and the child,
of course, has also changed as a result of maturation.
Attachment develops as a transactional process between the child and a
caregiver. Over time, caregivers change their behavior patterns and emo-
tional responses, just as children do. However, the research information we
have about attachment is primarily about the child's contribution to the
process. To look at both partners at once is exceedingly difficult. To what-
ever extent it is possible, this chapter will examine both sides of the trans-
actional process, though most often it will be necessary to concentrate on
one side or the other. As Donald Winnicott so famously declared, "There
is no such thing as a baby"—a baby alone, that is, and we need to remem-
ber this at all times.3
T H E EARLY M O N T H S
From a time soon after birth, human babies are responsive to human be-
ings. They imitate facial expressions before they are a day old, and they
watch faces and show special interest in people's eyes. It is clear they rec-
ognize human beings as something different from inanimate objects. Many
of a young infant's actions are attractive to adults, especially the social smile
that appears at six to eight weeks in response to another person. When we
adults see a baby respond to us like this, we become interested and gratified,
and we want more of that interaction. Such attractive baby responses may
be called engagement behaviors, because they help us become more and more
emotionally involved with the baby. Although the baby's engagement be-
haviors make us feel special, however, they do not actually mean that the
baby is concerned with us as individuals.
Early Fearlessness
During the first six months after birth, human beings do not show the
preference for familiar people or the fear of unfamiliarity most often associ-
ated with attachment. There is plenty of evidence that little babies can tell
the difference between familiar and unfamiliar adults, but little proof that
they actually prefer one to the other. (An adult who is familiar to the baby,
however, is also an adult to whom the baby is familiar, and it may be that
a person who knows the baby well can comfort or play with the baby more
effectively than a stranger can.)
When abruptly separated from familiar people, young babies show little
disturbance in sleep or eating. They do not seem frightened of strangers,
and this is not surprising because they are not frightened of the dark, loud
noises, big dogs, or anything else on the usual list of childhood fears. Small
58 Understanding Attachment
babies may well respond with distress to a new caregiver who is clumsy or
fails to understand the babies' signals, but this is not the same thing as being
afraid.
Dyadic Self-Regulation
The youngest babies are characteristically more concerned with their
internal processes than they are with the people around them. Infants of
a few months may be very responsive to our social overtures at times—
because of their own needs and interests and because we just happen to do
something that gets their attention—but their responses occur on their own
schedule. Most of the time, they ignore us, cry, or fall asleep. If they are
crying, they may need a great deal of help calming down, and they may not
reach a happy, quiet, alert state until they have again gone through a sleep
cycle.
One of a parent's major jobs during this period is soothing or comforting
the baby—a task as important in its own way as feeding and cleaning.
Skillful, prompt comforting helps a baby establish a first step in emotional
development, a step on which attachment probably depends.
This first step is called dyadic self-regulation, and it is the ability the baby
has to be soothed and to recover from distress with the help of a caregiver.
The baby begins to attain calm when in a dyad, or couple relationship, with
another person who is helping. The ability to calm down without this help
is not achieved until much later and is perhaps never totally mastered, even
in adulthood. Most adults still need and appreciate sympathetic comforting
when we are seriously upset.
It is critically important to understand that an infant of less than six
months does not have much ability to calm down alone from real distur-
bance, such as a painful experience. He or she may cry frantically and even-
tually fall into an exhausted sleep, but this is not the same as attaining the
quiet, alert state that permits play and learning. After three or four months,
however, most caregivers have figured out ways of offering effective com-
fort, such as holding the baby in a certain position, rocking with a particu-
lar rhythm, or offering a pacifier. The babies also have discovered some
understanding of things that make them feel better; they settle into posi-
tions of comfort fairly easily, even beginning to calm as soon as help is of-
fered. When they are older, they may imitate a caregiver's strategy by
singing to themselves or climbing into the rocking chair on their own.
Dyadic self-regulation is a major step toward attachment, because it helps
the baby experience much more quiet, alert time than it did before. These
quiet periods are a time for looking at and listening to people, and as they
get longer, new transactional processes take place. Not only is the baby
Attachment, Age, and Change 59
quiet and interested in other people, caregivers, who are pleased that they
can make the baby happy, are more inclined than ever to be sociable and
communicative. From about the age of two months, the baby's smiles re-
ward the caregiver, and within a few more months, the baby's laughter
follows the caregiver's playful approach.
Caregivers who have learned good comforting tricks now begin a period
of courtship, working hard to get babies to show pleasure. They concen-
trate on social play. Even during feeding or diapering, the adult's energy
goes into keeping the baby attentive and happy.
In the next months, the baby's pleasure and interest become more and
more evident to sensitive caregivers, and the caregiver finds more jokes and
simple games that please the adult and child. Strangers can manage to make
the baby laugh, too, but nothing is really as good as the familiar play routine
developed by the play-partners through transactional processes.
EIGHT M O N T H S T O T W O YEARS
The toddler period is the stage of life most clearly associated with attach-
ment behavior and emotion. As children of this age become more capable of
moving and communicating, they reveal new concerns about separation.
conflict between attachment and the need for autonomy, and they express
their feelings through resistance and negativism. Temper tantrums, so typi-
cal of this period of development, are most often triggered in attachment-
related situations—a mother who is shopping refuses to pick up the toddler,
for example, and the child responds by throwing himself on the floor, and
thrashing and screaming. An examination of this situation shows us impor-
tant attachment issues at work: the child is in a strange place, possibly near
strange people, and he seeks to be near the mother in reaction to this expe-
rience. She, however, does not cooperate, and intent on her shopping,
does not even offer the eye contact and friendly facial expression that might
help the child tolerate this level of distress. The child's loss of emotional
control shows the intensity of his or her fear of separation in a threatening
situation.
From about eight months to two years or so, babies show a very impor-
tant form of attachment behavior, in addition to the ones just described.
This is a much more subtle behavior than stranger and separation avoid-
ance, and can be observed only in certain situations, so parents often do not
notice or remember it. Secure base behavior is an essential indication of
attachment, however.
Children show secure base behavior when in a strange place or situation
with a familiar caregiver. The child essentially uses the adult as a base from
which to make exploratory forays. He or she stays near the adult for a
while, and then moves away to explore the interesting, but worrisome new
environment. At first the baby moves only a small distance; then he comes
back to the adult, perhaps climbing into the lap briefly for what appears to
be an emotional refueling. Then another sortie is made, and another, until
the toddler has managed to explore the situation thoroughly. As the explo-
ration proceeds, the child may not even return all the way to the adult, but
may look back, make eye contact, and find the adult's happy, unfrightened
expression enough reassurance for the time being. The adult will often of-
fer emotional support over a distance, by talking to the child, and the child
may reply with pre verbal jargon or with actual words.
Secure base behavior in the presence of a familiar caregiver is much differ-
ent from the frightened, depressed stillness of a toddler left alone or with a
strange adult in a strange place. When we see the baby can use a person as a
secure base, we understand that person plays an important role in the child's
internal working model of social relationships. An adult who can serve as a
secure base is an adult to whom the child has formed an attachment.
Secure base behavior shows us how children's emotional lives support
their ability to learn from the world through exploration. Even when they
are not exploring, however, infants and toddlers show us there are connec-
tions between their emotional attachment to adults, and their interests in
62 Understanding Attachment
objects and in communication. Toward the end of the first year, babies work
hard to accomplish joint attention.4 They catch the eye of a familiar adult
and move their own gaze, signaling the adult to look at something interest-
ing; the two look back and forth at the object and then at each other, enjoy-
ing their shared experience just as two adults do. Interesting things seem
more approachable when a familiar adult is present; they are more fun too.
Children's displays of stranger anxiety and separation anxiety are often
distressing to adult caregivers, especially in modern family life, where ma-
ternal employment and non-maternal child care are frequent and econom-
ically necessary. The disadvantages of these childhood feelings and behaviors
are obvious, and their advantages are hard to see. If we try to see life from
the perspective of our remote ancestors, however, it may be more evident
that such attachment behaviors have a very positive side. In prehistoric
times, a wandering child might easily have been killed or stolen by a neigh-
boring band of adults, and those who feared the unfamiliar were more
likely to survive. Children who preferred to stay near adults would also
have had more chances to learn social rules by watching adult behavior. In
the ancient past (and possibly today as well), adults may have done a better
job of child care when they were aware that an unattended child might
begin to cry loudly, disturbing adults, and potentially attracting the atten-
tion of enemies or predators.
Secure base behavior does not cause the same obvious problems for
adults as stranger and separation anxiety may; its advantages may also be less
easy to see. But the ability to use a familiar person as a secure base makes it
possible for the child to learn and explore, and, at the same time, stay safe.
This is a more desirable outcome for human beings than safety alone. One
modern difficulty connected with secure base behavior involves the toddler's
need for familiar people, without whose presence even the best-designed
preschool education may be valueless. Young children who are cared for in
their own homes almost automatically have a secure base. Young children
in child care outside the home may have no familiar caregivers unless spe-
cial efforts are made to encourage good relationships with consistent child
care staff. The secure base issue makes excellent early childhood care a
highly labor-intensive enterprise.
These have to do with the child's short- and long-term reactions to abrupt,
lengthy separations from familiar people. These reactions, aspects of what
Rene Spitz called hospitalisnV are fortunately not part of every child's early
family life, though they do occur for far too many toddlers.
A toddler who has achieved attachment to one or a few caregivers will
respond with serious distress to a separation that lasts for more than a day or
two. Far from being too young to be affected by the loss of a parent, these
children are at their most vulnerable age. Their responses to loss are com-
pletely parallel to the grief and mourning response of a bereaved adult. (In
fact, the idea of stages of grief, so popular today, initially came out of work
with orphaned young children.) Playing, learning, and other developmen-
tal processes may be put on hold for a period of months, and physical health
too may be affected.
In the nature of things, of course, a young child may suffer an abrupt
separation lasting days or weeks for reasons that are not very serious: the
parents may have decided that their marriage needed a second honeymoon,
or a new baby may have been born. A reunion with the parents eventually
occurs, but children under two years do not understand what is happening
and cannot always accept it with grace. The child's behavior during the
separation shows distress, and when the parents return, they may be ig-
nored, greeted coolly, or even hit or pushed away by the angry toddler.
The child's angry or aloof behavior soon gives way to intense concern that
the parents will leave again, and there may be weeks or months of clingi-
ness, anxiety, and sleep problems before the child's worries resolve.
In some cases, of course, there has been a serious reason for the separa-
tion, and there will be no reunion. The child's distress is great and pro-
longed. Given an emotionally supportive caregiver, young children may
recover completely from a serious loss and are then able to form a new at-
tachment. In the course of recovery, however, the child will show deep
unhappiness, crying, eating and sleep disturbances, failure to play, and, of-
ten, a regression from recently achieved goals, such as toilet training. This
profound disturbance gradually gives way to a depressed withdrawal and a
lack of interest. If the lost person returns, the child shows no recognition.
Finally, a new attachment and a new interest in life can occur. (For some
children, such as the mutilated orphans of the Congo, there is no sympa-
thetic caregiver to offer a new life, and the experience of loss becomes the
central organizing feature of life.)
touch briefly on two common ideas at odds with the material we are dis-
cussing in this section. First, we need to realize that the experience of adop-
tion does not necessarily cause problems of attachment, or of general
development. It is important to understand that children adopted early in
their first year proceed through the same developmental steps with their
adoptive parents as non-adopted children do with their biological parents.
Because attachment is based on social experiences that adoptive parents
usually provide, there is no reason that the early adoption of children should
have negative effects on attachment. (Later adoption, of course, may in-
volve a different set of social experiences, especially if it occurs after the
point when attachment is usually evident.)
A second common misconception has to do with breastfeeding. Breast-
feeding, or any other type of feeding, has in itself no known effect on the
child's internal working model of social relations (nor, as we will see later
in this chapter, is it known to influence the mother's feelings toward the
child). Ordinarily, of course, the person who feeds the baby most is also the
one most likely to play and interact socially in ways that connect to attach-
ment, playful communications being a real part of the feeding interac-
tion. Breastfeeding mothers usually have a bit more leisure or flexibility
of schedule, they are healthy themselves, and they have healthy babies.
These factors encourage both breastfeeding and attachment, whereas sick
mothers with sick babies and overwhelmingly demanding schedules are
more likely to have problems with nursing and attachment. We might see
better attachment in breastfed babies than in some bottle-fed babies, but the
feeding method would not be the direct cause of good or poor attachment
development.
T H E PRESCHOOL PERIOD
As we have seen, the social behavior of the four- or five-month-old
gives way to much different attachment-related behavior by the end of the
first year. It appears that this first stage of attachment forms the core of the
internal working model of social relations, which will continue to develop
for many years. The intense, anxious emotional concern the toddler shows
for the caregiver will change again and again, until in his own late adult-
hood, the grown-up, former toddler becomes the concerned caregiver for
elderly parents as well as for children or grandchildren. To prepare for this
series of changes, the individual has to stop expecting to be the one who is
nurtured. He or she has to become capable of negotiating or compromising
with respect to other people's needs and of developing close relationships
with new people. Without such developmental changes, the individual
would remain a big, self-centered baby.
Attachment, Age, and Change 65
The preschool period features a child's first steps away from the emo-
tional egocentrism of the toddler. Preschool children in most cultures are
likely to experience more separations from their parents than babies do.
If they were breastfed, they have generally been weaned by this age.
Most are at least beginning to handle their own toilet needs. They walk
and talk well. Sometimes their own interests in play and in their peers
draw preschoolers away from their parents' sides. But the parents too seek
separation. A mother may have a new baby to care for, and her work at
home or away may require her to arrange that someone else care for her
preschooler.
Like older human beings, preschoolers do not mind an unpleasant event
like separation so much, if they can control when and how it occurs.
Preschool children may protest vigorously against separation from their
parents, but they also seek separation themselves, or accept it calmly after
some negotiation has occurred. For instance, the preschooler may agree to
be left at the day care center when she has received the number of kisses
equal to the time on the clock, when the coat and lunch have been put in
the cubby, or when the parent reads one story.
Separations and reunions are a fact of life for most preschool children,
but their effect on the internal working model depends on the child's ex-
perience of predictability and control. Ideally, the preschool child's internal
working model of social relations will involve a trust in compromise with
others, and will thus move the child past the belief that safety and security
exist only in the presence of a familiar caregiver. Experiences of caring ne-
gotiation are an important way to begin to trust compromise. Without
these developments, the later experience of school and independent play
with peers can only be frightening and difficult.
Parents have their own goals for their preschool children's development,
and these involve compromise too. Most parents want the child to develop
greater independence, but at the same time they want family rules to be
obeyed. Though a few parents want to exercise complete authority over
the child, most are committed to the development of a goal-corrected partner-
ship, a relationship in which both the parent's and the child's needs will be
considered, using mutual communication and planning as negotiating tools.
An important issue at this time is the parent's skill at communication; care-
ful communication is a major way to help the preschool child maintain a
sense of security when the parent is absent, and, as well, it is an essential
model for the child to follow.
Negotiation, bargaining, and compromise are thus aspects of the pre-
schooler's attachment relationships. They bring about a step toward matu-
rity in the internal working model. We can recognize attachment at work
at this stage of life when the child attempts to negotiate separation from
66 Understanding Attachment
SCHOOL-AGE CHILDREN
By the elementary school years, children generally have a social circle
outside the immediate family. School, teachers, neighborhood friends,
clubs, and religious groups all form part of the child's internal working
Attachment, Age, and Change 67
model of social relations. Children of this age may leave home for periods
of time, for a sleepover birthday party, a lengthy hospital treatment, or even
to go to boarding school. They may experience periods of loneliness and
homesickness, but on the whole, they are ready to include new situations
and relationships in their internal working models.
Although friendships with age-mates are an important aspect of attach-
ment for school-age children, parents remain at the core of the child's
emotional life. As was true of preschoolers, schoolchildren's behavior may
not reveal this fact unless there is a threat, or in cases of illness or injury,
when they desire the security of nearness to the parent. Talking on the
phone or even daydreaming about a reunion may be enough to help the
separated child—although we should note that having to use these expedi-
ents takes the child's energy away from work or play. The internal working
model of attachment still designates the parents as the ones who make you
feel safe—this is the case even when a parent has been unpleasant, unreli-
able, or overtly abusive.6
For most parents and children, the elementary school period is one of
lessened negative emotion. Both adults and children have improved nego-
tiating techniques, and parents are less likely than before to assume that the
child's wishes are just silly whims. Parents also have more confidence that
yielding to a child will solve a problem rather than create incessant de-
mands. Children's improved command of language enables them to com-
municate in situations where they once would have dissolved into tears and
had tantrums. The attachment relationship with the parent still involves
negative as well as positive emotion, but this is likely to be expressed with
sulking, dawdling, and forgetting rather than with direct anger.
Parent and school-age child are generally continuing to work toward a
goal-corrected partnership. In order to manage this, both must recognize
how the child's changing needs and abilities affect their relationship and
their ways of making decisions. If a boy of twelve says he wants music les-
sons and promises to practice, for example, a parent will generally assume
that he knows what he is talking about, even though the same promise made
at age six was not kept. The parent gradually encourages the child's initiative
and autonomy, thus moving toward a more mature, flexible relationship
between parent and child. As the parent systematically modulates the roles
each plays in the attachment relationship, he or she also helps the child build
flexibility into his internal working model of all social relationships.
T H E ADOLESCENT YEARS
For all families, the management of attachment concerns in adolescence
can have a critical impact on the teenager's later success in life. A fine bal-
ance between independence and emotional connection needs to be main-
tained, while the family serves as a secure base for the teenager's first
exploration of the real world.
Events in the teenager's modification of attachment to parents are in
many ways parallel to those of toddlers. This does not imply that adoles-
cents are babyish, or so incompetent that adults must manage their lives,
but like toddlers, they are in the process of finding ways to use their family
attachments as support for coping with new tasks. One of the tasks for
which they need family support is, paradoxically, emotional separation from
the parents. The aspects of this confusing situation include ambivalence,
Attachment, Age, and Change 69
PARENTHOOD
Mothers love their children because of their natural instinct, don't they?
Or do they? The idea of maternal instinct is one way of explaining the
change in their model of social relationships that most women experience
after a child is born. Like many uses of the instinct idea, however, this ex-
planation becomes less satisfactory when it is examined carefully. Neither
mothers nor fathers know automatically how to care for a newborn baby.
Different cultures have different infant-care practices, a fact that suggests
infant care is not instinctive for humans. If it were, everyone would behave
in exactly the same way.
Because the word instinct does not seem to describe human parental
behavior very well, investigators in this field at one point turned to "bond-
ing" as a substitute. In an earlier chapter of this book, we referred to the
term bonding as a description of an emotional connection between human
beings, and we noted that this was a nontechnical use. Technically, the
word bonding is used only to describe the development of an adult's posi-
tive feelings toward a child. There is no question that the feelings we call
bonding can be extraordinarily powerful; they preoccupy and motivate
Attachment, Age, and Change 71
the staff pediatrician during his rounds, the delay could be as much as
twenty-four hours.) When the baby was finally brought to the mother, she
might feel timid about unwrapping her child and bothering the nurses; the
visit might be brief, and usually there was little privacy.
Researchers working on bonding were concerned that these hospital
policies might be interfering with bonding, or at least that the conditions
were less than ideal. They proposed that mothers and babies be given a
period of early contact within a short time after the birth. The father was
to be included, if possible, and he and the mother were to be given privacy
and encouragement to undress and examine their baby. When parents had
this experience, they were usually engrossed with the baby and thrilled
with the sense of doing something very important.
The evidence from the reports of the parents was not enough to prove
any real advantages from early contact. Did the parents' early contact create
a bond that helped them care for the baby, who in turn developed particu-
larly well? This was the kind of question that is essential but so difficult to
answer experimentally. How was it even possible to create a situation where
one group of parents had early contact, but another comparison group did
not? And what about ethical concerns? What if it should prove that early
contact actually did some harm to the family, rather than giving the antici-
pated benefit?
Fortunately, a situation allowing research did arise, as one hospital
changed its policy from delayed to early contact for healthy babies and
their mothers. A comparison was made between the development of the
delayed-contact babies, born in the last week of the old policy, and of the
first week's group of early-contact babies. The babies' development was
followed over several years, and significant advantages were reported for
the early-contact group. 8
The researchers proposed the following chain of events: (1) Early contact
brought mother and baby together when the mother was most ready to
bond, soon after giving birth. (2) The newborn was at that time more alert
and responsive than the baby would have been the next day, and its appear-
ance and behavior affected the mother emotionally, causing her to form a
bond to her particular baby. (3) Bonding was an internal emotional and
cognitive change that made the mother more aware of, and more con-
cerned with the baby's needs, so she became more sensitive, responsive,
and receptive to the baby's signals than she would otherwise have been.
(4) The mother's increased sensitivity helped her do a better job caring for
the baby. (5) As a result, the baby's condition was improved; bonding led
to good child health and development.
The idea that early contact could accomplish so much was eagerly
welcomed. Programs to encourage early contact were established, and
Attachment, Age, and Change 73
hospital policies altered to allow this. The idea was generally accepted that
bonding—and therefore good development—depended on early contact.
(Even today, nurses sometimes speak about bonding when they are simply
helping mothers with early contact and encouraging mother-child interac-
tion.) Some parents were concerned and distressed about bonding, when
health problems delayed contact with their newborn.
Factors in the parent also influence his or her emotional response to the
baby. Fear and grief seem to interfere seriously with bonding. A parent who
is mourning the death, during the previous year, of a loved one may have
difficulty feeling an emotional connection to a newborn. (This applies to
grief about an earlier miscarriage or stillbirth, too, and the death of one twin
is especially problematic.) Anxiety about a baby's health seems to trigger a
sort of premature mourning that interferes with bonding. Maternal depres-
sion lasting for more than a few months causes problems with the infant's
emotional development, putting the relationship more and more at risk.
Situational factors can also distort bonding. A difficult, unwanted, or
frightening pregnancy leaves the mother less prepared than she should be
for beginning a new relationship. If a mother can see her sick newborn only
through a nursery window, she may feel that the child is not her own.
Most new parents bring to their situation some factors that could inter-
fere with bonding. Many do not experience their first contact with the
baby as a thrilling moment of falling in love. But days or weeks of interac-
tion with the baby cause an emotional reorganization which changes the
internal working model, and results in powerful positive emotions and a
deep preoccupation with the child. Although early parenthood can involve
fury and frustration, most parental feelings are strongly positive, enabling
them to provide enough sensitive, responsive care to support the child's
good development.
Do the hormonal changes of childbirth really cause bonding, or at least
make it more likely to occur? Probably hormones play only a small role, if
any. Fathers and adoptive mothers, who have no dramatic hormone
changes, show bonding to the same degree as biological mothers. We need
to remember that the hormonal changes of pregnancy and childbirth are
also accompanied by important experiences that can occur in the absence
of pregnancy. A biological mother has normally known of her pregnancy
for six months or more and has had plenty of time to focus her thoughts
and feelings on the person she expects her baby to be. This experience—
which is shared by fathers and adoptive mothers—may do as much or more
to prepare her for bonding as biological events do. A pregnant woman also
experiences much social guidance, as other people tell her of the ups and
downs of birth and motherhood, treating her as a prospective mother and
helping her prepare to be one. Fathers may receive this type of guidance
mainly from their wives, their own parents, and intimate friends. Adoptive
mothers may receive it through the adoption agency, but they do receive
it just as pregnant women do.
A persistent myth about bonding is the belief that breastfeeding causes it
to occur. (As we saw earlier, it is also common to assume, and equally inac-
curate, that breastfeeding causes attachment.) People who have this belief
Attachment, Age, and Change 75
often offer an unlikely picture of a mother gazing into a very young baby's
eyes as the child nurses. This very old idea certainly goes back at least to
Shakespeare's time as we see from Lady Macbeth's line, "I have given suck,
and know how tender 'tis to love the baby that milks me." In the early
twentieth century, just as Freud was suggesting the gratification of needs
was a source of attachment, one writer proposed that lactation is the cause
of maternal love.9
Of course, it is not practical or ethical to try to test the role of breastfeed-
ing in an experimental fashion. In order to do so we would have to make
sure that certain mothers breastfed and that others did not, a most difficult
task, as well as one involving potential dangers for some children. What we
know about the effects of breastfeeding comes from our observations of
self-selected mothers who have chosen breast or bottle-feeding on their
own, or at least without any researcher's advice. However, observations of
breastfeeding and bottle-feeding mothers give us no reason to think that
one group loves their babies any less than the other. Adoptive mothers,
who rarely breastfeed in this country, ordinarily experience bonding, and
their children have excellent developmental outcomes if they have been
adopted early in their lives.10 If breastfeeding were necessary for bonding,
the extreme popularity of bottle-feeding in the 1940s and 1950s should
have created catastrophic developmental problems, and this did not occur.
In addition, the necessity of breastfeeding would mean that no father could
ever bond to his child.
It may well be that mothers who have bonding problems may also be less
likely to breastfeed. It is also the case that a sick newborn, especially one
with facial birth defects, may be too weak to breastfeed, and bonding prob-
lems may occur. However, what we are seeing in these cases is the effect of
other factors on both bonding and feeding, not of feeding on bonding, or
vice versa.
T H E STOCKHOLM SYNDROME
A rare and little-understood form of adult attachment is sometimes called
the Stockholm Syndrome after the city in which it was first clearly de-
scribed. This syndrome occurs in situations where an individual, usually a
woman, is threatened or held hostage in a case of genuine danger. The
hostage-taker negotiates for the woman's safety with the authorities, using
the captive's life as a bargaining chip. Rather than hating and fearing the
captor, as might seem logical, the hostage falls in love and does not want to
be rescued or separated from the captor.
The situation occurs so infrequently that it has hardly been studied in de-
tail, but the characteristic emotional changes seem to have much in common
76 Understanding Attachment
REVIEW A N D SUMMARY
Attachment and bonding both have to do with attitudes and beliefs about
specific people. Neither could develop in a social vacuum, for each requires
the participation of at least two people. We focus on the feelings of the
child when studying attachment and those of the adult when studying
bonding, but it would be ideal if we could look simultaneously at the steps
of both partners in their interpersonal dances. Perhaps we can manage a
little of this if we simply examine which events are happening at which
given time.
Shortly after a baby's birth, the parents are interested in and attracted to
the baby, and they respond happily if they can feed their child or get the
baby to look at them. The baby cannot do a great deal, but does sometimes
look at faces and eyes, and may even imitate facial expressions, giving the
parents a sense of gratification. As parents bond, they become more preoc-
cupied with the baby, and more sensitive and responsive to signals. This
helps to comfort and soothe the baby, who becomes gradually a little more
able to regulate his own emotions, enabling him to spend more time quiet,
alert, and responsive to adults. The parents are aware of this change and are
encouraged to try more and more playful communications. The parents
now strongly prefer their baby to other babies and know how to play with
and soothe her, but the baby does not yet particularly prefer the parents.
By about eight months, the child becomes capable of fearfulness and
begins to stay close to the parents, especially if there is any threatening or
unfamiliar event going on. Parents respond to this new behavior with
mixed emotions and often with some anxiety. They change their behavior
toward the child in anticipation of the fear and distress the child may ex-
press. Some parents are supportive and comforting, others punitive about
the beginning of attachment behavior.
During the later toddler and preschool period, child and parent begin to
move apart emotionally and socially. The child can tolerate more separa-
tion, but wants to negotiate it in order to keep some sense of control.
Parents at this point may have reason to encourage separation; a new baby
Attachment, Age, and Change 77
Families, Experiences,
and Outcomes: What
Difference Does
Attachment History
Make?
A
startling news story in early 2004 revealed a strange family history
and raised questions about the effects of experiences on children's
emotional lives. At a children's birthday party, a young mother
named Luz Cuevas encountered a former friend with a six-year-old girl
who was identified as the friend's daughter. This was not particularly un-
usual, but Ms. Cuevas immediately wondered whether this child might pos-
sibly be her own daughter, Delimar, who had disappeared as a week-old
infant, apparently having been consumed in a blazing house fire. Unnoticed,
Ms. Cuevas obtained a clipping of the child's hair and managed to get a DNA
analysis. The result? It was Ms. Cuevas' own child, who must have been
kidnapped as a tiny baby by the woman claiming to be her mother, the fire
having been set deliberately to cover the kidnapping. When these facts
were discovered, Delimar was taken by the state child protective service
agency and eventually returned to Ms. Cuevas.
Delimar, thus, experienced two abrupt separations: the first at one week
old and the second at six years of age. What would we expect to be the
results of these experiences? Chapter 4 has already answered this question
with respect to the earlier separation. As devastating as the event was to her
mother, the infant Delimar would have had no difficulty with the change
of caregiver at one week old, as long as she was treated well.
What about the separation at age six though? This question opens up
many issues. Are there short-term or long-term consequences of relation-
ships and of losses at different ages? How do early experiences influence the
80 Understanding Attachment
What
In asking about the influence of attachment events, we might be inquir-
ing about serious psychopathology. In Bowlby's early study of the forty-
four boys,1 attachment was thought to be a major factor determining
antisocial behavior. (Bowlby's later work did not stress this idea as much as
his earlier writings.) Some writers have speculated on whether attachment
problems might be factors in autism or even adult serial murder. This ques-
tion is a complex one and will be addressed in Chapter 6. At this point, we
can say that attachment alone is probably not the cause of major emotional
disturbances of the types mentioned above.
For our present purposes, the influence of attachment experiences may
be best defined as the extent to which attachment events shape an individ-
ual's emotional and behavioral reactions to other people—the unique char-
acteristics that we refer to as personality. Such influences could change from
one stage of life to another, but for our purposes, they would only include
feelings and behaviors within a normal range. The focus is on individuality—
normal personality differences—and on the minor emotional disturbances
that may be troublesome but do not interfere severely with education,
work, marriage, and parenthood. Because attachment involves an internal
working model of social relationships, we tend to look for the effect of at-
tachment experiences on relationships, rather than on, say, memory or ath-
letic prowess.
When
Why
What leads us to ask this kind of question in the first place? Why should
attachment events have a real impact, when some other early experiences
do not? Part of the answer involves the enormous significance of emotional
relationships and social behavior in human lives. From childhood and be-
yond, our success and happiness are linked to emotional connections with
others. The preschooler who fights other children and resists his teacher will
be punished, deprived, removed from the group, and prevented from hav-
ing many satisfying experiences. The schoolchild who is sullen or quarrel-
some is not chosen to go on a special trip or be in the school play. Although
some adults may achieve professional success through intellectual abilities,
most of us need to use good social skills and cultivate personal relationships
to be successful at work and home.
Fortunately, we can pursue the question of attachment and personality
because we have the tools to do so. Chapter 3 described techniques for as-
sessment of attachment in children and in older people. No one would
claim that our tests of attachment are perfect, but they have been used for
many years by large numbers of researchers, both to collect new informa-
tion and to make comparisons with much older work.
Tests like the Strange Situation have shown us the continuity or stability
of the measurements we make of attachment behavior. This is an essential
factor in our understanding of the links between early attachment experi-
ences and any later impact on personality. To a considerable extent, we can
expect that a twelve-month-old, categorized as securely attached using the
82 Understanding Attachment
Strange Situation, will show related, similar characteristics years later; an-
other child, classified as insecure-avoidant will develop along different, but
equally predictable lines.3 When they are years older, the children will not
behave in just the same ways they did as toddlers, of course, because all
their ways of acting will have matured; however, the differences that once
existed between the one-year-olds will resemble the differences that now
exist between the five-year-olds.
This continuity or stability of attachment behaviors tells us that some
characteristics were already established at an early age; this means it is pos-
sible that the characteristics were actually caused by early attachment expe-
riences. Another fact that suggests that personality may be shaped by early
attachment experiences is that some children show different attachment
behaviors toward their mothers and than they do their fathers.4 These differ-
ences imply that attachment is not altogether in the child, but is instead in
the relationship, and thus arises, at least in part, from early experiences.
C O N F O U N D E D VARIABLES:
CONFUSED A N D C O N F U S I N G D A T A
A word of warning is in order before we discuss attachment experiences
and personality development. It is extremely difficult to sort out causes and
effects in this matter. There are some individual personality characteristics that
clearly do not come out of attachment experiences. These traits, called tem-
peramental or constitutional differences, apparently result from genetic or
other biological factors. It may be all too easy to confuse these with the child's
actual attachment emotions, behavior, or even, memories and thoughts.
Take, for example, a temperamental quality called "intensity of reac-
tion." Infants and toddlers show individual differences in their emotional
expressiveness. Some cry loudly and vigorously when unhappy, yet are
equally vehement in their expressions of pleasure, laughing and shouting
when tickled or chased. On the other hand, young children who are more
mild in their reactions may frown or look sad instead of crying, and smile
or chuckle instead of shrieking when amused.
These temperamental differences affect attachment in two ways. First,
adults may notice a child's concerns about separation when they are so
loudly and vociferously expressed, but may pay less attention to the milder
signals from the less intense child. Second, having noticed the child's unique
behaviors, the adults may provide different types of experiences to a child
who was already different. Temperament can cause children to have differ-
ent kinds of social experiences.
A parent of an intense toddler may plan to handle separations carefully in
public so unfavorable attention is not attracted by the child's cries. Some
Families, Experiences, and Outcomes 83
parents may try to sneak out, when leaving the child with a sitter or at a day
care center, in an attempt to avoid hearing the child's loud distress. Others
may decide that care outside the home is impossible for their child, who
seems so terribly upset when left. Still others may punish the crying child
for disobedience or willfulness.
Parents of milder toddlers may feel relieved and proud of their child's
independence when little distress about separation is displayed. They may
go ahead with day care arrangements and concern themselves very little
about the moment of parting. On the other hand, some parents of mildly
reacting toddlers may be disturbed by their child's relative calm, and they
may fear that he or she has not formed an attachment.
The important point here is that children's different emotional expres-
siveness—their mild or intense reaction to separation—can cause the par-
ents to handle separations differently. The child's attachment experiences
are directly affected by the parent's actions, and indirectly affected by the
child's own temperament. Which contributes most to the child's develop-
ing personality characteristics? It is extremely difficult to tell.
A final source of confusion is connected with the question of the impor-
tance of early experience. Is the child's individuality shaped primarily by
early experience, or is it gradually molded over years of growing up? To
answer this, we would have to look at children whose infant and toddler
years were spent in one sort of emotional situation, and who then went to
quite different households. There are, of course, some children who are
adopted by new families or who live in foster families for years; with any
luck, these children go from poor emotional situations to good ones. We
do not see many who have reversed this order, so we do not have very
good material for examining the two potential formative periods. And most
children in industrialized countries live in the same families from birth
through adolescence. For the average child, chances are that the basic en-
vironment of infancy and of later years will be the same. The parents who
encouraged attachment in infancy with sensitivity and responsiveness will
continue to provide appropriately sensitive, responsive care for their older
children. If we look at the average ten-year-old, then, we have no way of
knowing whether her personality was completely determined at age two,
or whether the ensuing years did the real work of making her who she is.
SHAPING A T T A C H M E N T A N D
I N D I V I D U A L I T Y : INFANTS A N D TODDLERS
Whether we are considering early experiences or early displays of attach-
ment emotions and behaviors, much of what we know about attachment
focuses on early life. The first two years of life have received the most
84 Understanding Attachment
T h e y may fear losing the child and they may try to avoid conflict with that
child, a difficult task with respect to the negotiation of separation w e dis-
cussed in an earlier chapter.
W h e n grief and fear are overwhelming, mothers' attitudes, feelings, and
behavior are powerfully affected. A fascinating and disturbing account of
this is given in Death Without Weeping.6 N a n c y Scheper-Hughes describes
the dire poverty in Brazil and its impact on mothers' caregiving. H u n g r y
and sick themselves, the p o o r Brazilian mothers suffer many emotional and
material losses. After giving birth, they may identify a child as one w h o does
not want to live. T h e y fail to feed or care for the baby, w h o eventually dies
of malnutrition and disease. This decision w o u l d certainly mean these m o t h -
ers have failed to bond, if w e could define such a category at all. H o w e v e r ,
w h e n a baby survives and becomes stronger, the mother's interest increases.
Such mothers then describe the children as attached. O n e interviewee said,
The little one never lets me out of her sight. [She] hangs onto my skirts,
and she cries whenever I leave the house. I can't take a step without
her. What kind of children are these, so afraid to move without their
mama? So I yell at them to toughen them up. 7
insecure attachment relations. They may avoid the mother when she returns
from an absence, or may go to her but not be able to be comforted by her.
Most studies of mothers' behavior and children's attachment status have
focused on the small number of toddlers whose behavior is in the D
category—disorganized or disoriented reactions to the mother's return
from a brief absence. These children have often experienced many changes
in caregiver or inconsistent relationships with their parents. Parents who
are depressed or who habitually use drugs and alcohol are particularly likely
to create inconsistent, unpredictable experiences for their children. Parents
who themselves experienced early losses, or were physically or sexually
abused, may also lack sensitivity or responsiveness to their children.
In addition to the past experiences that may cause D-category behaviors,
it is possible that a parent's behavior at the time of the reunion serves as a
direct trigger for disorganized/disoriented-child responses. When children
respond with fear or freezing on a parent's return, their actions may be
cued by the parent's own facial expressions. The parent may appear fright-
ened and thus be frightening to the child, and may also show sudden, un-
predictable behavioral changes, like abruptly moving a hand across the child's
face.9 A "natural" source of comfort and security for a toddler, the parent,
also acts in this situation as a source of fear. The child looks at the parent's
face for information about the parent's emotional state and finds there is
indeed a reason to be frightened. Freezing or collapsing may be a logical
response to a puzzle where no solution is really right.
forced both mothers and fathers to separate from their young children,
causing distress, anxiety, and possible changes in attachment. For many
other infants and toddlers, marital disagreements, separations, and finally
divorce may disrupt attachments to parents.
Such separations certainly have short-term effects on many young chil-
dren between six months and two years of age. Far from being too young to
notice, they are strongly affected because they are too young to understand
anything that is happening. Anxiety and clinginess are common responses to
family separations. Sleep problems, depression, and temporary behavior
changes are also common in these cases, there being an obvious parallel to
the child's reaction to the loss of a familiar person through death.
Paradoxically, infants and toddlers often show what appears to be in-
creased distress when they have a visit with a parent who has left the house-
hold. For instance, an eighteen-month-old may have settled a bit (meaning
he may be sleeping better and appearing less disturbed) in the month
following his father's departure from the marital home. Then the father
reappears for an afternoon visit, and the child responds with renewed sleep-
lessness, clinginess, and restlessness. The custodial parent may read this as a
signal that visits had better be avoided, or may even believe that the visitor
treated the child abusively—a belief that may culminate in a charge of
sexual abuse so frequent today.
Handling this situation is a delicate task, especially in light of the emo-
tional distress of both parents and its negative impact on their coping skills.
It is difficult for people in this situation to see beyond the present discom-
fort to the long-term goal of maintaining a positive relationship between
the child and each of the parents. Nevertheless, it is clear that most parent-
child relationships have real value for the child's development, and that
such relationships are preserved by staying the course through the early
years of separation.
To maintain attachment to an often-absent parent, infants and toddlers
need visits that are as lengthy and as frequent as it is possible to make them.
These visits may—indeed, often will—be followed by increased expression of
distress by the child, but the distress is caused by the absence, not by the visit.
In these circumstances, tantrums or other displays of emotion should not be
punished, but accepted as what they are, a reaction to family disruption.
Ideally, the conditions for visiting should be as familiar as possible to the
child. Allowing the absent parent to come to the young child's home is the
best approach, allowing the child to deal with the situation in an environ-
ment of maximal security and familiarity. However, such home visits may
be agonizing for both parents, reminding them of matters they want to
forget. If the child shows distress, it may be extremely hard for the custodial
parent to stand back and let the other parent work to mend the relationship.
Families, Experiences, and Outcomes 91
child who experiences parental separation will usually live for years in a
household shaped by that separation. The circumstances may include fi-
nancial uncertainty, the need to move to a new house, distress and depres-
sion for the custodial and perhaps the non-custodial parent, and possible
new relationships with stepparents and stepsiblings, all in addition to the
continuing difficulty of balancing a visiting relationship.
It is possible for parents and children to maintain strong, developmen-
tally appropriate attachment relationships, in spite of early marital separa-
tion, and for children in these circumstances to continue on a trajectory of
positive personality development. However, this desirable outcome re-
quires care, energy, and a certain amount of good luck, and may occur a
good deal less frequently than we would like.
SHAPING A T T A C H M E N T A N D
PERSONALITY: T H E PRESCHOOL YEARS
As we discussed in an earlier chapter, preschool children show their
emotional attachment in different ways from those typical of toddlers.
Preschoolers often initiate separation themselves, and they can accept it
well, just as they can be appropriately sociable with friendly strangers. In
cases where they are disturbed about separation, preschoolers may show
their feelings by squabbling with the mother when she returns.
The child who does this seems to be trying to re-establish important
aspects of the relationship with the returned mother rather than just try-
ing to be close to her, as a toddler would do. The internal working model
of the relationship is in need of adjustment, and the child is seeking to
readjust it.
The preschool child's existing attachment relationships seem related to
other aspects of mood and behavior. Secure attachment in the past seems to
accompany the preschooler's willingness to comply with rules and her
trusting acceptance of adults as legitimate authorities. Such children also
tend to cooperate with their peers and do not need a lot of control by adult
caregivers. Less securely attached preschool children can be deliberately
disobedient, and may behave provocatively in order to get the attention of
an unmindful parent.12 For example, in one family where the father was
physically ill and weak, the preschool children responded to his withdrawal
by acting up until he took off his belt to punish them.
Both serious loss and everyday stress have potential effects on children of
preschool age. Separation from familiar people and adoption or foster care
placement at this age can lead to long-term sadness and irritability, although
these are less likely when the new family is effective in helping the child's
adjustment. Even holidays, birthday parties, and vacation trips can trigger
Families, Experiences, and Outcomes 93
from the early attachment relationship between parent and child, a rela-
tionship that has its parallels in other relationships, such as those between
teacher and pupil, older and younger person, or a more powerful person
and a weaker one. First-graders are well known for their tendency to call
their teacher "Mommy" and address their mothers as "Mrs. Brown" by
accident. This does not mean that schoolchildren of any age think the
teacher is their mother, but it does show the child is considering the paral-
lel aspects of the relationships. The child is thinking about how teacher-
pupil relationships can be expected to work, given the extent to which they
resemble parent-child relationships. The internal working model of rela-
tionships, developed in the early years, may thus help determine how the
child acts toward teachers, who in turn will behave in ways that influence
the child's attitudes toward school.
For good or ill, however, parents remain at the core of a school-age
child's emotional life. How this works varies from family to family, class to
class, and culture to culture, but it is generally true that parents are at its
center, even for children who go to boarding school or who leave home
for other reasons.
The nature of the child's attachment to the adult continues to shape his
or her relationship. Of course, school-age children display much less attach-
ment behavior toward their parents than they did when they were younger,
and they may seem to take the parents for granted much of the time. When
the parents go on a trip or the child goes to sleep-away camp, however, the
child may show concern about the separation. For example, one ten-year-
old went to Girl Scout camp for the first time, when her parents were leav-
ing for a long-planned cruise; she cried every day, and finally disclosed her
terror that there would be a shipwreck and her parents would drown.
When there is an apparent threat, the child tries to feel more secure by
getting closer to the parent, talking to them on the phone, or even just day-
dreaming about a reunion. Under these circumstances, it becomes harder
for the child to do schoolwork or even play constructively. It does not
matter whether the parent is an unpleasant, unreliable, or even abusive
person; the child still turns to him or her when feeling threatened. 13 The
desire for contact with the parent can be set off by threats other than
separation, too: being bullied on the playground, getting sick at school, or
being punished in the classroom. The child's internal working model still
includes the idea that relief from anxiety can be obtained through contact
with the parent.
School-age children regularly experience separations from teachers who
have become familiar, either when the teacher leaves the school or when
the child passes to a new grade level. Because American families tend to be
geographically mobile, many children experience separation from teachers,
Families, Experiences, and Outcomes 95
schools, and classmates as the family moves away. The practice of having
the child's home address determine which school is attended is another
common source of separation experiences, for even a move to a new house
may put the child in a new school district. Most difficult for the school-age
child is the family disruption following divorce. It often means a new house
and a new school, the transition coming at a time when the child is least
able to tolerate it.
Just as the school-age child is continuing to construct new mental catego-
ries and to learn new problem-solving skills, he or she is also constructing
new ways of thinking and acting toward parents and similar adults. In the
course of doing so, school-age children move toward behaviors and emo-
tions that allow some autonomy, as well as gratifying the wish to cooperate
and be on good terms with the parent. A school-age child may comply with
a parent's requests incompletely, but just enough so the parent is not actively
angry or punitive. (For example, the child puts her clean clothes in the dresser,
but leaves one sock hanging out or in the laundry basket.) Although such
behavior is annoying to parents, its goal is not actually to irritate them, but to
build toward a future relationship in which the child achieves greater inde-
pendence and the parent relinquishes responsibility and authority. Whether
the parent is relaxed and tolerant, or intrusive and authoritarian, about these
matters will help shape the child's understanding of later social relationships.
Sibling Relationships
As children get older, their relationships with siblings also add to the
internal working model of relationships, which is becoming more compli-
cated. Attitudes and relationships of school-age children with siblings are
determined by complex interactions among many factors. Power differ-
ences are almost always present (even between twins) because there are
differences in age, specific skills, or relationships with parents. In some fam-
ilies or cultures, the child belonging to the more favored sex has power in
spite of youth or small size. The internal working models of sibling rela-
tionships have not been studied much, but they probably have considerable
influence over other intimate relationships in later life. Especially when
children are near in age, sibling relationships seem to share the emotionality
of parent-child attachment relationships with an even greater display of
anger and frustration at times.
Although school-age children are able to do some negotiating in their
relationships with siblings and peers, they still need parents to provide some
scaffolding, or support, allowing the children to bring their social skills to
bear on emotional conflicts with other children. Because neither child in
the relationship is a very mature negotiator, they cannot do as well together
96 Understanding Attachment
as each could do with an adult. Conflicts and rivalry between siblings can be
terribly provoking to parents, but it is likely that with parental help, chil-
dren can use these conflicts to understand more about social interactions.
ADOLESCENCE: EXPERIENCES
SHAPE T H E INTERNAL W O R K I N G M O D E L
Most adolescents and their parents have to work out ways of negotiating
separation after they have long shared a close relationship that evolved from
early attachment ties. Usually parents have developed some family patterns
that worked when their children were younger. Now, the dramatic changes
of physical maturation make some of the old patterns ineffective or even
destructive. Parents have to do a great deal of fine-tuning to maintain the
relationship while the teenager is changing so rapidly. The adolescent, too,
has to alter his or her internal working model of relationships, no longer
can a teenager think of the emotional connection with the parents as a
younger child does.
The way a family works together to change relationships depends on
their history together, and as well, on their present circumstances. For
Families, Experiences, and Outcomes 99
example, in one study, girls who saw their mothers as affectionate and ac-
cepting were more capable of expressing their emotions than girls with less
accepting mothers. 22 The girls who could explain their feelings well used
their skill to help others to understand them. Thus, they could contribute
in a mature way to the reworking of family relationships, as well as using
the ability for a realistic remodeling of their internal working models.
We discussed earlier how important it is for preschoolers and parents to
participate in negotiation of separation. At that point in development, of
course, it is the parents who have reason to want to separate; the children
are the ones who generally like the parents near. During adolescence, when
negotiation of separation comes to the fore again, it is the teenager who
wants to move toward independence, while most parents cherish the child's
last years spent in the heart of the family. Ideally, negotiation can produce
a gradual separation; unfortunately some parents demand that the adoles-
cent remain a child to the point where he or she seizes independence pre-
maturely. In each situation, there is a different impact on the teenager's
internal working model of social relationships. A preferable model would
include the idea that people are able to work together, trusting in a mutu-
ally agreeable outcome. A less preferable view is that the support and care
of others comes only with complete compliance and dependence. Just as
parents of toddlers tend to use the time-out method of discipline—raising
more separation concerns—it is common for parents of teenagers to use
grounding, which increases the adolescent's anxiety about independence.
Negotiation of rules with adolescents can be a very effective parenting
technique. However, it requires time, energy, patience, and acceptance of
the idea that rules should change as a teenager matures. Parents who nego-
tiate also need to admit their side of the negotiation usually involves their
own needs, rather than some absolute standard of behavior. This admission
may be humiliating for some parents, who may feel as if they have revealed
a personal secret to the adolescent. For example, a recently divorced mother
may condemn a teenager's dating as disgraceful, rather than disclose her
anxiety about her own sexuality.
Negotiation of separation may be most difficult when the family history
has positioned the teenager as a hurried child.23 This term has been applied to
a child who has become parentalized into a parent's friend and confidante
following divorce. A divorced mother, for example, may tell her job and
relationship troubles to a preteen child, asking the child's opinion and ad-
vice. The child may experience adult-like responsibility for household tasks
and adult-like authority for decision-making about clothing, companions,
bedtime, and so on. When the hurried child arrives at adolescence, how-
ever, the parent becomes frightened by the autonomy the child has been
exercising. The possible dangers to an inexperienced adolescent become
00 Understanding Attachment
apparent to the adult, who knows how other people may interpret sophis-
ticated clothing on a thirteen-year-old girl or the freely chosen video of a
thirteen-year-old boy. The parent tries to reduce the teenager's power be-
low what it used to be, although the teenager knows that the teen years are
supposed to be a time of increased independence. Negotiation of rules will
not be possible for this family unless parent and child can explore the mis-
takes of the past, which will probably require the help of counseling.
SUMMARY A N D CONCLUSION
Differences in early attachment experiences apparently help to shape
personality differences, especially those that have to do with social and
emotional relationships. Rather than a direct connection between infant
experiences and adult personality, however, we see a long series of gradual
steps toward development of the adult internal working model of social
relationships. These steps involve experiences with other people, but they
also depend on maturation of the individual's intellectual and emotional
abilities. Six-year-old Delimar Cuevas, whom we met at the beginning of
this chapter, would have responded very differently to her change of homes
than a twelve-month-old. Many children and adolescents experience fam-
ily situations that can distort the development of ideas about social relation-
ships, but they can benefit from positive guidance by adults who are aware
of the attachment issues characteristic of particular age groups. By adult-
hood, internal working models of social relationships should be complex
and many-faceted, allowing for appropriate social and emotional relation-
ships and behaviors with a variety of people, including infants.
This page intentionally left blank
chapter 6
Attachment,
Mental Health,
and Psychotherapy:
Emotional Ties and
Emotional Disturbance
D
oes attachment have any connection to serious mental illness?
Many people have a vague idea that it does, but they are not very
clear on the details. One child care provider at a training confer-
ence questioned a speaker who had alluded to attachment: "Isn't there a
disease about that? Stranger anxiety or something?" Another member of
the audience, a nurse, spoke of a fifteen-month-old who was equally
friendly to all adults: "I just thought he was really nice and well-adjusted."
Talking about older children, people may describe a child as too clingy or
demanding or as a Mommy's boy. There may be general comments about
adults, concerning attachment issues or attachment problems.
This chapter will focus on connections between attachment and mental
health, focusing on the part attachment concepts play in psychotherapy and
the use of psychotherapy for attachment problems. Though there are many
unanswered questions in this area, the attachment concept gives us a useful
approach to the understanding of mental health.
these different age groups also have their own forms of mental illness, which
are displayed in age-typical ways.
An important consideration for infant mental health is the fact that the
attachment behaviors and feelings that would indicate emotional distur-
bance in an adult may be quite normal and healthy in a child. The child
care provider's question about stranger anxiety being a disease shows how
infant behavior may look when we mistakenly use an adult context. Anxiety
about unfamiliar people and places is a normal part of infant development,
as is the need for a secure-base person to make exploration possible. These
anxieties should almost never be considered indications of infant emotional
disturbance. On the contrary, as we will see later, it is the absence of anxi-
ety that might be an indication of mental illness.
DO ATTACHMENT
EXPERIENCES CAUSE M E N T A L ILLNESS?
Attachment is a robust process, and most children make a clear emotional
attachment to familiar caregivers. For a sizeable minority of the world's
children, however, attachment-related experiences do not follow a smooth
path. In industrialized countries, children may have caregivers who are de-
pressed or physically ill and who cannot provide the sensitive, responsive
care that is ideal. Domestic violence, alcohol, or drug use may lead to a
child being placed in foster care or adopted by a new, unfamiliar caregiver.
In the developing countries, the death of a mother from AIDS, or in civil
or other warfare, may leave a child who receives a minimum of care from
virtual strangers. Do these experiences cause actual mental illness? It is
Attachment, Mental Health, and Psychotherapy 107
the mother of her own, early emotional ties and losses. She may then actu-
ally be frightened when she looks at the child, and she may want to get away
from, what is in her mind, a frightening situation. The child looks at the
mother's face and then looks around, trying to figure out what is so fright-
ening to her—he sees that he must be a source of her fear. This is in itself
disturbing to the child, but in addition, the mother cannot offer effective
comfort, because her fear is increased when she is close to the child. The
child has frequent experiences of alarm, but, in the end, he must cope with
them alone, even though his impulse is to rush to the mother for security.
Is Disorganized /Disoriented Attachment a Mental Illness? The Disorganized/
Disoriented classification does not seem to match our definition of a mental
illness (although it is unusual behavior and does involve emotional distur-
bance in the form of frightened behavior). It does not involve problems of
development as serious as some seen in twelve-month-olds, such as autism.
The Disorganized/Disoriented child still manages to communicate with
others, to develop language, and to play. If the child has attachment rela-
tionships other than the one with the mother, they may be more normal, as
long as the behavior of the other adults is more normal than the mother's.
Children with Disorganized/Disoriented attachment do have the poten-
tial to develop later disturbances of social interactions, both with peers and
adults, especially if they have few opportunities for early interactions that
are emotionally healthy. No one would deem this status a sign of good
mental health or positive future development. However, as so often hap-
pens, it is hard to know whether later problems result directly from the
early attachment status itself, from the fact that these children often have
been maltreated, or from the experience of having to live with a trauma-
tized and anxious parent.
The DSM criteria for R A D do not make it clear how serious the
symptoms are, although many of them seem related to the disturbed be-
havior categorized as Disorganized/Disoriented attachment. It is hard to
know whether excessive familiarity means conversing eagerly with, or
climbing into the lap, of a friendly stranger. And how unfamiliar is a
relative stranger—is it the child's mother's friend who comes unexpect-
edly to pick him up at the day care center, a new child care provider, or
the mailman?
The RAD criteria also assume knowledge of the child's early social ex-
periences, which may be difficult or impossible to know accurately. The
child in question may have been abandoned, without any identifying infor-
mation, or, equally problematically, the story told by a noncustodial parent
or child care provider might be seriously biased.
Finally, the R A D criteria are confusing because they do not define de-
velopmentally appropriate behaviors, consider cross-cultural issues, or deal
with the effects of context, such as the behavior of the unfamiliar person or
characteristics of the environment. Because the criteria require more than a
brief observation of the child's behavior, decisions may be subject to dis-
torted reports by adult caregivers. It is possible that these factors will pro-
duce a diagnosis of RAD when some other diagnostic category would be
more appropriate.
According to one commentator, empirical studies of Reactive Attach-
ment Disorder are quite rare, and there have been difficulties with the
definition of the syndrome.16 The DSM description is unusual in that it
includes the background causes and attachment history of the child, as well
as his present symptoms. Most DSM categories focus on criteria related to
the individual's present situation.
ICD-10. Attachment disorders are also listed in the International
Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, commonly called
ICD-10. 17 ICD-10 is the European version of DSM. ICD-10 lists two
separate disorders: Reactive Attachment Disorder of Childhood and
Disinhibited Attachment Disorder of Childhood, and these are parallel to
the two types of syndrome combined in DSM. One difference between the
classifications is that ICD-10 does not include the background and history
as criteria for the disorders.
Alternative Ideas: Five Types of Attachment Disorders. The well-known child
psychiatrist Charles Zeanah and his colleagues have suggested five possible
outcomes of poor early attachment experiences.18 These categories can be
used with children from age one to four or five. The criteria are based on
behavior alone, with no attempt to depend on the child's early life history.
In addition, the category can be assigned even if the child shows the behav-
ior with only one adult caregiver—an important point, because as Zeanah
12 Understanding Attachment
has shown, children can have very different relationships and behaviors
with different familiar adults, as well as with strangers.
Zeanah's list of possible attachment disorders begins with Type I:
Nonattached Attachment Disorder. In this condition, the child develops no
preference for a familiar person and appears detached and uninterested in
people because he or she has had no opportunity to form attachments.
As we have seen elsewhere in this book, infants do not need a great deal
of help forming an attachment; they do need the chance to experience
the responsiveness of a small number of attentive people and to have
time to become familiar with them. Children with a Type I problem
may have been in institutions or had many changes of caregiver, and
perhaps were also badly neglected. They are able to form attachments
but have not had the chance. Their internal working models of social
relations do not link comfort or security with the nearness of familiar
people.
Boarder babies, who are sometimes cared for in hospitals for months,
their mothers having abandoned them, typically have lives that can lead to
a Type I disorder. They spend a good deal of time alone, with staff mem-
bers occasionally popping in, getting the baby's attention, and leaving again.
With this kind of social experience, the baby develops a quick, superficial
social reaction; he or she does not persist in interactions or seem upset
when the interactions stop abruptly. (Hospitals sometimes post notices in
these babies' rooms, requesting people to come in for fifteen minutes or
not at all.)
Zeanah and his colleague Alicia Lieberman gave a case description of a
child who fit the Type I category. He was twenty months old at the time
and had been with the same foster family for nine months. He was aggres-
sive and he bit himself and others, but he would stay for hours in the place
where he was put down. He did not reach for toys, preferring to stare at
spinning objects—often considered a symptom of autism. When moved
to a new living situation where he was favored, nurtured, and given
much attention, however, he improved greatly and began to show a prefer-
ence for one of the staff members. Apparently he had needed the much-
increased, responsive and interested interaction.
Zeanah and his colleagues have also proposed the diagnostic category
Type II: Indiscriminate Attachment Disorder, which resembles the disinhibited
type of Reactive Attachment Disorder described in DSM-IV. This group
of children will go to anyone, are overly friendly, and do not show the
usual secure base behavior, or run to a preferred caregiver when fright-
ened. Any adult seems to be equally comforting. (We should note the
potential dangers here, as these children are easily approached by molest-
ers.) Children who show these behaviors are likely to have been in
Attachment, Mental Health, and Psychotherapy 1 13
He looked soberly at his mother when she opened the door, but he
did not greet her [He] walked in silently and stood in her living
room. The natural mother told George to sit down, and he did so
immediately George did not move from where he sat down
initially unless his mother directed him otherwise He watched
her constantly, maintaining a vigilant and anxious expression
He cringed slightly when she spoke to him, but he always complied
immediately with her directives.19
W h i l e this was going on, George's brother, w h o was visiting from another
foster h o m e , ran around and vocalized.
T h e difficulty George was having clearly emerged from the relationship
he had with his m o t h e r — h e did n o t behave this way with other adults—
nor had his m o t h e r developed the same relationship with the other child.
T y p e IV: Aggressive Attachment Disorder is another possible o u t c o m e of
p o o r attachment experiences proposed by Zeanah. This category involves
a persistent, pervasive level of anger and aggressive behavior toward the
parent, but can also include symptoms of anxiety, such as sleep disturbance.
T h e child may hurt himself (by head-banging, for example), may have
lengthy tantrums, and may hit, bite, or shout, "I hate you!" These children
have often been involved in, or seen, domestic violence. T h e y may have
received inconsistent care and unpredictable physical punishment.
Zeanah and Lieberman suggested one m o r e type of attachment disorder
called T y p e V: Role-Reversed Attachment Disorder. In this pattern, the child
behaves parentally toward the caregiver, b o t h nurturing and acting bossy or
punitive.
As an example, Zeanah described t w e n t y - f o u r - m o n t h - o l d Beth, w h o
was brought in for an examination because of sleep problems. H e r m o t h e r
was depressed and cried during the interview.
Beth played with [toys] only in the first 20 minutes, ... when her
mother seemed more animated. Her mother looked sadder and began
to cry as she described her frustration and hopelessness Beth walked
over and signaled to be picked up. She remained in her mother's
lap Beth began to stroke her mother's hair gently. She continued
caressing her mother's hair and back intermittently for about 20
minutes. At times, however, she pulled her mother's hair vigorously
and provocatively.20
begin with disobedience and cruelty to animals and then develop into serial
killing, a characteristic expected of all adopted children who are seen as
suffering from attachment disorders.
The AT belief system emphasizes the idea that complete control over
children's actions is the only way to eradicate the intense aggressiveness
expected to result from a poor attachment history. All adopted children are
considered to be full of rage and to direct their anger first at younger chil-
dren and pets, and later at their adoptive parents. A treatment intended to
neutralize rage is advocated by this group and will be discussed later in this
chapter.
Again, there is no systematic evidence to support this belief system. It is
clear from both the DSM criteria for Reactive Attachment Disorder and
from Zeanah's categories that aggressiveness is not a hallmark of disorders
of attachment, although it may be present in some cases.
Assessing Attachment Disorders in Older Children. The descriptions of at-
tachment disorders given above show a good deal of variation. There does
not seem to be a single, invariable form of attachment disorder even among
preschool children. Although poor attachment experiences have a real po-
tential for causing poor emotional outcomes, their consequences can be
different for children in different situations, and can be influenced by indi-
viduals' basic temperamental characteristics. By the time children reach
school age, they have developed along individual lines to such an extent
that early attachment experiences are only one factor among many that
determine emotion and behavior. Assessments of Reactive Attachment
Disorder at that point may not be possible, as leaders in this area of research
have stated.
Teachers and parents need to be cautious about claims that Reactive
Attachment Disorder can be diagnosed by means of checklists or through
observations by mental health counselors. One group of counselors, for
example, has suggested that they can diagnose R A D in three to seven
controlled five-minute observations, but the evidence presented is quite
questionable.21
ATTACHMENT, DISORDERS OF
ATTACHMENT, AND PSYCHOTHERAPY
Serious disorders of attachment need treatment. Not only do they cause
unnecessary stress and fear in early life, but as development progresses, they
distort the child's experiences and beliefs about other people. This section
will examine therapeutic interventions that seem to help compensate for
a poor early attachment history. In addition, this section will consider the
fact that even people with mild attachment problems may benefit from
Attachment, Mental Health, and Psychotherapy 1 17
Types of Intervention
Treatment of emotional problems today most c o m m o n l y emphasizes the
family system and assumes that every family m e m b e r influences each of
the others. Therapeutic interventions with children are especially family-
oriented. Children are not sent to be fixed, nor are children the sole focus
of intervention, even w h e n the child's behavior and feelings are the reasons
the family has sought treatment. W h e n the child's difficulties seem to be
relationship-connected, all the family's relationships need to be considered,
and m o r e than one may need serious w o r k .
20 Understanding Attachment
angry." After watching the video with the clinician, Ms. V. responded very
differently and far more accurately: "I think he was scared Oh, my God!
That was a scary moment for him. He probably thought that I was leaving
and wouldn't come back."39 Though the video feedback intervention was an
experimental one, and the families all received more conventional psycho-
therapy, this treatment may prove to be an effective way to help traumatized
mothers become appropriately sensitive to their children's communications.
CONCLUSION
When attachment experiences are poor, individuals' internal working
models of relationships seem to develop along distorted lines. The early
models are based on a few primary relationships, and if those are all diffi-
cult, the child's beliefs and expectations about relationships will also be
problematic. Because relationship quality depends on both child and adult,
even a child with one very poor relationship may benefit from other, better
relationships. A very few children have no consistent relationships, but
even these children have models of how human beings interact, because
they have experienced some minimum care and companionship.
Children with poor experiences, involving interactions with inconsis-
tent, unresponsive adults, begin on a developmental trajectory or pathway
in which they have less and less opportunity to develop healthy expecta-
tions and good social skills. An adopted child, for instance, may turn away
from the parent when hurt; if the parent accepts this as meaning the child
does not like, or want, care, the child's belief that people are not a source
of nurturance will be further confirmed. When the child's development has
been affected repeatedly by such experiences, he or she may become so
negative in his or her emotional responses and behavior toward others that
the question of mental illness will arise.
The term attachment disorder describes an emotional disturbance that is
the outcome of poor early social and emotional experiences. However,
complete agreement among psychologists and psychiatrists does not exist
on the behaviors and feelings associated with disorders of attachment, and
a number of possible types have been described. Aggression does not seem
to be a major feature of attachment disorders.
Because attachment disorders arise from experience, treatment focuses
on experiences that can change the individual's understanding of the self
and of relationships with other people. The most effective treatments in-
volve improvement of communication between adult and child; they re-
quire the adult to increase his or her sensitivity and ability to respond to the
child's communications, especially those about fear of separation.
chapter 7
What Everybody
Knows: Popular
Views of Attachment
A
s the first chapters of this book have shown, the concept of at-
tachment is complex, and it remains the subject of much discus-
sion in professional child development and psychotherapy circles.
This fact does not prevent simplified views of emotional development from
becoming popularized. Indeed, the idea of attachment is broadly accepted
in the United States, although not necessarily with the definition we have
used in this book. Decisions and practices that claim to be based on attach-
ment are readily accepted by the media and the public, even though attach-
ment has a variety of definitions. In our Age of Attachment, statements
about emotional ties may go unquestioned.
The present chapter will examine three major popular ways of thinking
that use the term attachment, but fall outside the realm of professional psy-
chology and psychotherapy. Two of these belief systems involve parenting
practices and treatment of children. The third involves ideas that have been
developed in the courts and have strongly affected child custody and visita-
tion decisions.
ATTACHMENT PARENTING
New parents seeking advice about childrearing may come across books
and Internet sites that counsel the use of attachment parenting, an approach
developed by a physician named Dr. William Sears and his wife.] Attachment
parenting stresses the need for parental sensitivity and responsiveness.
28 Understanding Attachment
enabling the child to develop trust in others and positive self-esteem. The
child will learn to "bond to persons, not things," have "meaningful attach-
ments with peers," and, in adulthood, achieve a "deep intimacy with a
mate." Other positive outcomes are said to be better discipline, a healthy
conscience, and less need for punishment.
Not surprisingly, there is no systematic evidence basis for attachment-
parenting claims. The assumption that more sensitive mothers guide babies
to better development is well substantiated, but the other statements are
really impossible to test in any controlled way. The attachment parenting
recommendations described below are particularly difficult to confirm
through systematic investigation.
family bed with enough space for both parents and several children.
Attachment parenting advocates do not insist on co-sleeping, but they do
approve it as one of many sleeping arrangements. Concerns that a baby may
be suffocated or lain upon by a parent are rejected in light of evidence that
child deaths of this type are usually associated with a parent's alcohol or
drug intoxication. In addition, some research has shown that young babies'
breathing is regulated by physical contact with adults. Attachment parent-
ing advocates see co-sleeping as advantageous both to the parents, who can
reconnect emotionally with the child at night, and to toddlers, who may
otherwise experience separation anxiety.
childrearing practices, they have not been particularly involved with the
matters we are discussing here.) Readers who look at attachment parenting
web sites will see links to groups described with names such as "Christian
attachment parenting" or "gentle Christian mothers." These groups have
enthusiastically accepted attachment parenting principles as the positive,
child-centered guidelines that lead to the development of close family ties
and the rejection of domestic violence in any form.
The fundamentalist Christian interest in attachment parenting has undoubt-
edly been intensified by the adoption of an alternative-parenting program by
some church groups in the United States. This approach, one that strongly re-
jects attachment-parenting principles, is sometimes referred to as Baby wise, or
Ezzo, parenting. Its guidelines are intensely adult-centered and its techniques
are parent-directed. For example, breastfeeding at the baby's signal (a cardinal
rule of attachment parenting) is replaced with a parent-directed feeding sched-
ule for breastfed, as well as bottle-fed infants. Mothers are instructed to ignore
their emotional impulses to respond to their babies' cues, and instead follow a
recommended program. Rather than babywearing, the Ezzo approach coun-
sels planned playpen time as an important way to develop independence. This
philosophy's primary goal is the child's obedience to parental authority.2
It is possible that attachment parenting would not have grown as it has
without the obvious contrast presented by the Babywise group and the ap-
peals the two programs have made to people who share many, but evi-
dently not all, of its values.
A T T A C H M E N T THERAPY:
A TREATMENT A N D A CHILDREARING PHILOSOPHY
In spite of some differences in vocabulary, the connection between attach-
ment parenting and Bowlby's attachment theory is incompletely realized, but
it is genuine. In this section, however, we will discuss another popular belief
system. Though it claims connection with Bowlby's theory and shares some
definitions and terms, it is almost diametrically opposed in its basic assump-
tions and goals. This system involves a complete system of parenting princi-
ples as well as an intervention intended to be therapeutic. It is commonly
referred to as Attachment Therapy (AT), but attachment parenting and AT
have no connection whatsoever other than the use of the word attachment.
each time for a n u m b e r of minutes equal to the child's age in years. This does
not seem like a difficult demand until one realizes that the clock does not start
until the child is completely compliant, which may be a matter of hours.
O b e d i e n c e is also exacted t h r o u g h physical means, such as locks and
alarms on doors. Children may not have night-lights in their rooms, even
w h e n extremely anxious. Food is a major form of manipulation, and a child
w h o does not comply or does not show gratitude for a meal may go for
weeks on a diet of cold oatmeal or peanut butter sandwiches. It is notable
that school attendance is t h o u g h t of as a privilege rather than a right, so
children in A T treatment may have little or no break from these conditions
for periods of m o n t h s .
T h o m a s and other A T advocates are particularly concerned that children
must not have information about what is happening to them. T h e y do not
k n o w w h e n , or even if, they will see their parents again, or w h e t h e r they
will be allowed to eat with the family, go to school, or go o n a trip. Adults
must be in complete authority, according to the A T view, and possession
of knowledge is a part of authority.
O n e mental health practitioner, in an area w h e r e A T parenting was m o r e
c o m m o n than A T therapy, gave the following description of what she had
observed:
[A] child in foster care refers to the man living with her mother as 'dad'
and her biological father as 'father' ... and [is] given extra chores for telling
'falsehoods' about who she is related to When the child continues to
refer to the men in her mother's life as dad or father, plus slips up and calls
the foster mother 'mom', drinking 8 ounces ofvinegar while 'strong sitting'
is added to encourage connection between the 'lower attachment centers'
of the brain and 'higher reasoning,' thus eliminating 'false attachments.' 6
A T T A C H M E N T CONCEPTS A N D T H E LAW
Legal and judicial thinking today takes into account some ideas about
attachment. Because it takes a long time for new psychological or psychiat-
ric information to be incorporated into law, the ideas that tend to be used
are actually a matter of popular or traditional belief, rather than cutting-
edge research or clinical work. In this section, we will look at some com-
mon legal applications of attachment concepts, and we will see how
congruent these are with current work on early emotional development.
Laws govern many aspects of human relationships, especially those deal-
ing with property, such as marriage, inheritance, and parental rights and
obligations. Until late in the twentieth century, however, neither American
nor British law concerned itself much with the emotional aspects of rela-
tionships. Family law was focused on possession of property, titles, or status,
and the ways these could pass from one person to another. Illegitimate
children had no rights of inheritance, unless the father specifically said so in
his will, and they could not use the family name or inherit a title.
Parental rights followed the same pattern. Until the later part of the
nineteenth century, divorced fathers had a complete right to custody of
their children, just as all family property belonged to men. When formal
adoption laws were established, they too were based on property law.
What Everybody Knows 141
If one set of parents had a right to the custody of a child, another person
could not also have that right, no matter what might be the complexity of
emotional connections.
Family law began to recognize some non-property issues in the late
nineteenth and early twentieth centuries, with the advent of the "tender
years doctrine." This guideline suggested that infants and young children
would be best in their mothers' custody, because their mothers naturally
knew more about caring for them. The tender years doctrine was not con-
cerned with the child's and the mother's mutual affection, or with the pos-
sible effect of separation on the child.
The door opened to legal consideration of relationships with the first
discussion of the principle of the "best interest of the child."9 This principle
suggested that decisions about child custody should consider a variety of
factors that could potentially affect the child, not just the parents' property
rights to custody. These factors included the speed and method of decision-
making, as well as other concerns, but from the point of view of this book
the relationship between the child and one or more adults was the most
important factor. The principle of the best interest of the child brought the
concept of attachment into the courtroom, and attachment soon became a
focus of family law.
professionals pointed this out, concern for this issue seems to have been
absent in court.
In the 1993 case of Baby Jessica, the public's attention was captured by
the distress of a toddler and her adoptive parents as they became caught up
in a net of contradictory state laws and human errors. Jessica's unmarried
biological mother had named the wrong man as the baby's birthfather. The
actual biological father was unaware of the baby's birth and was never asked
to voluntarily give up his parental rights—an essential step for a clear adop-
tion process. Jessica was adopted and reared for two years by a couple in a
state that neighbored the one where she was born, the two states having
different laws about paternal rights in adoption.
When Jessica's birthfather became aware of the child's existence, he un-
dertook legal proceedings to assert his parental rights and take custody of
the child, whom he had actually never seen. The legal battle again con-
cerned the opposing rights of the birthfather and the adoptive parents, with
little or no concern about the impact of a decision on Jessica. The birth-
father won, and the adoption was disrupted. Jessica was taken screaming
from her adoptive mother's arms and given to the birthfather.
Cases like these, together with the progress of the best-interest principle,
have encouraged the consideration of the child's emotional needs as one of
the factors in child custody decisions. But, as we shall see, legal decisions
have made a rather unsystematic use of this factor.
to a second husband; he had adopted the children. She became seriously ill
and died in 1987; the children spent m u c h of her illness with their maternal
grandparents, but because of a quarrel saw little of t h e m after the funeral.
T h e adoptive father remarried and the children lived with him, his n e w
wife, and her children. T h e maternal grandparents complained that the fa-
ther was unfit, and they petitioned for m o r e visitation on the grounds of
their relationship with the children. T h e court refused and defined a psy-
chological parent—in this case the adoptive father—as one whose removal
from the child's life w o u l d cause the child severe psychological harm. T h e
decision c o m m e n t e d , " [ N ] o t even the most liberal reading of the [grand-
parents'] pleadings w o u l d support the conclusion that they stand in a
parent-child relationship with their grandchildren."
T h e Zack v. Fiebert decision emphasized the importance of the psycho-
logical parent and treated biological kinship as a relatively trivial factor, thus
affirming the serious importance of attachment issues. H o w e v e r , the logic of
the decision assumed that attachment concerns w o u l d be the same for y o u n g
teenagers as they are for infants and toddlers. Whereas Sees v. Baber assumed
that a toddler was too y o u n g to be badly affected by separation, Zack v.
Fiebert assumed that teenagers were still y o u n g e n o u g h — i n each case, a c o n -
clusion contrary to what is k n o w n about attachment and separation.
B.F. v. DYFS, 1997. T h e m o t h e r in this case placed her y o u n g child (age
unstated) in temporary foster care and eighteen m o n t h s later contacted the
N e w Jersey Division of Y o u t h and Family Services to request reunion. T h e
child was in a pre-adoptive h o m e , and the Division decided to bring an
action for her guardianship, preparatory to adoption by the family. H o w e v e r ,
it did not do so for another nine months, at w h i c h time the child had b e e n
separated from the biological m o t h e r for twenty-seven months. T h e m o t h e r
was not allowed to visit during this time.
T h e court returned the little girl to her biological mother's custody, o n
the basis of the following reasoning:
[B]ecause the psychological bonding between [the child] and her pre-
adoptive family during the period following the [Division of Youth
and Family Services'] refusal to allow visitation resulted from the
improper denial of visitation rights, consideration of that bonding by
the trial court would similarly have been improper.
M. J. B., a same-sex couple, had shared parenting from the time the chil-
dren had been born to one of them until the women separated when the
twins were two years old. The biological mother had custody of the chil-
dren, and her former partner requested visitation rights on the grounds of
having bonded with the children. This was refused by the trial judge, who
said: "While there is evidence ... of the plaintiff enjoying a bonded rela-
tionship with the children, the plaintiff has failed to establish the relation-
ship to have risen to the level of in toco parentis." This judge apparently
considered the possibility of degrees of attachment or psychological parent-
hood, an idea that had not appeared in decisions before.
The attorney representing the biological mother argued that the three-
year-old children were too young to be affected by separation. "The fact that
the plaintiff is a parental figure to the children and has 'bonded' with them is
not a sufficient basis [T]hese are very young children ... and children
form bonds with people such as grandparents, babysitters, [and] nannies."
The Appeals Court did permit visitation by the former partner and re-
ferred back to the Zack decision, focusing on the history of the adult-child
relationship rather than on an assessment of the children's attachment be-
haviors. "Psychological parenthood is a finding based upon the role the
person historically played in the child's life. Neither optimistic not pessi-
mistic predictions of future harm that would result from ending that role
can logically define the role itself."
The discussion of V. C. v. M.J. B., thus, included the beliefs that attach-
ments can be rank-ordered, that three-year-olds are too young to have real
attachments, and that the nature of an adult-child relationship is understood
on the basis of past events only. Relationship concepts and developmental
changes played confused roles in this decision.
Watkins v. Nelson, 1999. In this case, a seventeen-year-old, unmarried
mother was killed in a car accident twelve days after the birth of her child. The
maternal grandparents cared for the baby, asked to be appointed her guardians,
and contested the paternity claims of the biological father, who visited regu-
larly. When the baby was nineteen months old, a trial judge gave custody to
the grandparents, citing the principle of the best interest of the child.
The child, who had some physical problems, was said to be "develop-
mentally unable" to show attachment. The grandparents were given cus-
tody not on the basis of psychological parenthood but because of their
advocacy and involvement with her treatment and their experience with
learning disabilities. On appeal, a dissenting opinion argued:
custody may, for a while, disturb [the child's] peace of mind ... [but]
that will soon pass, considering her extreme youth.
This set of arguments again revealed a very mixed bag of beliefs about
attachment and separation. Psychological parenthood was indeed seen as
one of many important factors, and the long-term relationship with the
father was stressed. But once again there was a failure to recognize that the
extreme youth of the child made her more, rather than less, vulnerable to
changes in relationships.
CONCLUSION
The concept of emotional attachment is not limited to professionals in
child development and mental health work, but is part of popular thinking.
Unfortunately, nonprofessional use of attachment concepts has not neces-
sarily followed research findings or advances in theory. Popular advice
about early emotional development often distorts important principles, or
it asserts recommendations that have no basis in evidence. Such recom-
mendations given to adoptive parents have resulted in problems, including
serious injuries and some deaths.
Judges and attorneys who deal with child abuse cases generally hear argu-
ments related to popular views of attachment rather than to research or
established theory. These views may involve unsubstantiated statements
about Reactive Attachment Disorder and necessary treatment.
Over the last thirty years, judges and attorneys dealing with adoption and
child custody cases have begun to consider the role of attachment and
separation issues in determining the well-being of the child. Although these
factors are receiving far more consideration than in the distant past, they are
still applied in inconsistent and contradictory ways. The developmental
course of attachment seems to be imperfectly understood in the legal
world.
chapter 8
New Directions:
Parents, Children, and
Attachment Concepts
T
his book has examined a wealth of observations and research di-
rected toward understanding attachment. It may be hard to imagine
that there is still much work to be done in this area. Looking at
recent publications, however, it is plain that researchers are beginning to
take some very new approaches to the study of emotional development and
family relationships.
This final chapter will begin by summarizing the facts that have emerged
from John Bowlby's original work. We will then consider the possibility
that Bowlby's views may no longer fit the attachment experiences of the
average child the way we used to think they did. If children's experiences
in their families have altered since Bowlby's time, it may be necessary to
think of attachment in a different way, ideally a way that would work for
many different family patterns. The chapter will conclude with an exami-
nation of some new ideas that show promise for an increased understanding
of emotional and social life.
A N S W E R I N G BASIC QUESTIONS A B O U T A T T A C H M E N T
With fifty years of research and clinical work to draw on, we can sum-
marize what is presently known about attachment. A good way to do this
is to answer a set of questions posed by the well-known attachment re-
searcher Everett Waters. The questions are a useful way to share the basic
facts about attachment and other aspects of development. 1
150 Understanding Attachment
What Develops?
What characteristics of a person's emotional life change with age? Emo-
tions, behaviors, and thoughts about other people all change as attachment
develops. The newborn baby has engagement behaviors that attract and in-
terest caregivers, but it is not until about eight months of age that we see
children trying to stay near familiar people, expressing distress about separa-
tion, or showing pleasure and relief when reunited with a familiar person. In
the next year or so, immediate, emotional responses to separation become
less intense, and behavior begins to include negotiation of separation. During
the school years, children's attachment to their parents presents as an affec-
tionate partnership. During adolescence, other people, especially romantic
partners, begin to be the focus of emotion and behavior. In adulthood, indi-
viduals show intense emotion and characteristic behavior toward their own
children. The whole process seems to be related to the development of an
internal working model of social relationships—a set of feelings, behaviors,
beliefs, and expectations about interactions with other human beings.
experiences with a sensitive, responsive adult are the foundation for secure
attachment. The continued development of the internal working model of
social relationships occurs because of an increasing variety and complexity
of social interactions, helped along by language communication and by
maturing thought patterns.
Experience is such an important mechanism for attachment that psycho-
therapy or a new family situation can help to alter the internal working
model years after the initial attachment experiences have occurred. Some
adults manage to pull themselves up by their own bootstraps and to alter
their own thinking toward a more positive and secure view of social rela-
tions. Some, however, are so strongly affected by their families' past histo-
ries of attachment and separation, it is difficult for them to change.
A C H A N G I N G W O R L D : CULTURAL A N D SOCIAL
ALTERATIONS T H A T C A N INFLUENCE A T T A C H M E N T
A wealth of information about attachment seems to exist today. But will
it be true in a few years? Can we expect changes in our understanding of
attachment? Can we expect events that might actually change the average
child's emotional development?
Of course, changes in our thinking and new research are always possible—
and to be hoped for. But it is also possible that the way people live may
alter, and with it, children's emotional experiences and development.
Mixing Cultures
Attachment theory and research have up until now been based largely on
the habits and beliefs of Western European and North American people.
Although there has been serious cross-cultural research, this research was
formulated on the basis of Western beliefs about the importance of attach-
ment experiences—as indeed any research must be formulated on some
foundation of beliefs and values.
As movement from one cultural setting to another becomes more fre-
quent, and as non-Westerners more often live in the United States and
European countries, the cultural contrasts among beliefs about attachment
become more evident. In a recent case, a Chinese couple, Shaoqiang and
Quin Luo He, who lived in Memphis, learned that a judge had terminated
their parental rights to their five-year-old daughter, Anna Mae. Mr. and
Mrs. He had initially asked to place Anna Mae in temporary foster care at
her birth, a time when they were beset by a variety of legal and financial
troubles. When Anna Mae was three months old, they agreed to temporary
custody by the foster parents. The Hes visited Anna Mae almost every
week for the next two years. They then asked to have her returned. They
got into an altercation with the foster parents, were ordered to stay away,
and did so for several months. The couple's compliance, far from creating
the good will they desired, was interpreted as abandonment.
The American and European view of this situation assumes that legal
parenthood of a child can be decided on the basis of the parents' actions,
and that placement of a child for care by another person or family is on the
verge of abandonment. For Chinese families, a more likely assumption is
that young children can be cared for others, sometimes for years, without
breaking the parent-child connection. For Chinese children in families that
New Directions 153
have immigrated to the United States, being sent away may be a normative
experience, an experience which many other people have and which the
individual regards as an acceptable, though uncomfortable part of life. As
the separated child grows up, he or she adds this understanding to the in-
ternal working model of social relations; non-parental care does not mean
rejection or lack of parental love. This internal working model is, of course,
different from what might be found in a highly assimilated Chinese family
in the United States.
People of American culture tend to believe in some prenatal or genetic
connection between mother and child, but they simultaneously accept the
idea that parents earn their rights by showing that they care about the child.
This genetic assumption stresses that knowledge of the real father and the
real mother are important, even when these people may be thought to have
forfeited their right to the child. The belief that an adopted child longs for
the chance for reunion has produced laws that allow for open adoption and
for the availability of information to adoptees when they reach adulthood.
Another type of legal change has been the gradual alteration in the cultural
view of pregnancy outside marriage. Whereas women once feared un-
wanted pregnancy, birth, and adoption as potential sources of great social
disapproval, precluding later marriage, there is far less secrecy today. There
seems to be a social movement to revise our ideas about the parent-child
relationship, including the belief that the early relationship is permanent
and the assumption that more than one kind of attachment can exist.
It is difficult to know whether changing or differing beliefs about attach-
ment lead to different practices. If they do, however, the eventual outcome
may be a change in common attachment patterns and internal working
models of social relationships.
it may be that the different perspective can itself influence how attach-
ment develops.
Adoptive Families. At the time when the Supreme Court legalized abor-
tion with the Roe v. Wade decision, it was thought by some that there
might soon be fewer children to adopt. There has been some truth to this
prediction, but, as so often happens, the story has not played out exactly as
expected.
In the United States, it is not unusual for a married woman to be un-
happy when she finds out she is pregnant. Some give birth and some abort,
but virtually none surrender the children for adoption. In the past, an un-
planned pregnancy in an unmarried woman, especially a teenager, was of-
ten terminated by abortion, but adoption was also an option. About half
the girls aborted, when the procedure was illegal; 45 percent had the baby
adopted; only 5 percent kept custody of the child, usually with the help of
their parents. Today, about half of pregnant, unmarried girls abort; 45
percent retain custody of the baby, and 5 percent agree to adoption.
Does this mean that there are now many fewer children in need of
adoptive homes? No, unfortunately, this is not exactly the case. But it does
mean there are few young infants available for adoption in the United
States. The attempts of young, poorly educated mothers to care for their
children too often end in neglect and abuse, followed by later foster care
placement. Time passes, the mother makes more efforts, there is more
trouble, and eventually termination of parental rights occurs. Now the chil-
dren are free for adoption—but they are three, four, five years, or older,
bear the scars of their chaotic emotional lives, and often have special medi-
cal and educational needs. To create good emotional relationships in these
later adoptions is much more difficult than to deal with a young baby, who
often has charming engagement behaviors and readily responds to affection.
It is no wonder that many late-adopting parents are preoccupied with at-
tachment issues.
In the wake of these social changes, a foreign adoption industry has
grown up. There are many expenses and problems associated with foreign
adoption, certainly, but foreign-adopting parents have no worries about
whether a mother's parental rights have been properly terminated or
whether a birthfather will suddenly appear, as happened in the Baby Jessica
case described in Chapter 7. Nevertheless, parents who adopt foreign chil-
dren may be struck by the artificiality of the situation, and they may won-
der how they can possibly become a real family.
The social changes outlined in this section have powerful implica-
tions for people's ideas about attachment. These changes may well encour-
age the view that attachment is a job to be done, rather than the natural
consequence of growing social interactions and experience between parent
New Directions 157
and child. Parents adopting under difficult circumstances may even fear
that attachment will not take place and that the consequences may be
terrible.
Infant Day Care and the Day Care Wars. Concerns about attachment have
been raised by the fact that many children enter day care while still young
infants. It is reasonable to ask whether spending many hours each week
away from their parents can negatively affect children's attachment. To put
this issue in context, however, we should note that non-parental care is a
real part of the history of our species. Human parents love their babies, but
they have always wanted some respite from taking care of them. In any
traditional culture, it makes perfect sense that a young, healthy, capable
woman should spend her time in the fields, at the market, or doing weav-
ing or whatever she can to care for her family's needs; baby-minding may
be done by little girls or the elderly aunt who can't walk far anyway.
Babies have thriven in that sort of non-parental care and have done so
for most of human history. Why, then, are we concerned about day care
arrangements? The fact is, of course, they are very different from that tra-
ditional care pattern.
Babies cared for by little girls or by elderly aunts or grandmothers had
intimate contacts with their mothers many times a day. They had to, be-
cause in most traditional cultures, breastfeeding went on until the age of
eighteen months or two years. The point here is not the breastfeeding itself,
but the occurrence of real mutual attention between mother and child at
frequent intervals. Nursing babies in such societies would also have slept
with their mothers and had frequent interactions during the night.
Traditional non-parental care also gave the baby the same familiar care-
giver every day. A particular child nurse or older relative had the job, and
she was expected to know the baby well and to be able to respond to all its
needs, except breastfeeding. Child nurses were admonished not to let the
baby cry, and they developed ways of soothing, comforting, and engaging
a fretful baby. An important aspect of traditional child care, generally, was
the existence of one caregiver per infant or toddler.
Modern infant day care stands in some contrast to traditional non-parental
care. Although many day care centers would be willing to cooperate with
breastfeeding mothers, few mothers have time to make the journey from
their workplace, nurse the baby in an unhurried and interactive way, and
return to work. Neither, of course, do bottle-feeding mothers have this
chance for frequent workday interactions with their babies. Frequent
mother-baby contact during the working day is just not an option in the
modern infant day care pattern.
Whether or not an infant in day care has a consistent caregiver de-
pends on circumstances. When an infant is in family day care—a more or
158 Understanding Attachment
Relationship-Based Practice
Current work with parents and children emphasizes the need to form
and support good emotional relationships. This includes relationships be-
tween parents and children, those between day care providers and children,
and the relationships of parent educators and mental health professionals to
parents, children, and caregivers.
Cultural Issues in Working with Relationships. Awareness of cultural fac-
tors and the role they play in internal working models of relationships is a
major factor in working with families. A simple example involves the use
of eye contact—mutual gaze—as a method of emotional communication.
Americans of European background often seek or even demand eye con-
tact with a child, as a means of play and affection, but also as a way to exert
authority and demand obedience. "Look at me, Jimmy" is a way to over-
come the child's resistance and get compliance, and is frequently used by
white mothers, teachers, and day care providers. In non-European cultures,
however, the child who maintains eye contact, when spoken to, is showing
resistance and disrespect for the adult; a lowered gaze is considered respect-
ful and compliant. Culturally determined ways to send emotional messages
are gradually built into an internal working model of social relationships.
The way mental health professionals work with parents may be defined
in terms of a cultural view of relationships. Among Latino families, for ex-
ample, there is an expectation ofpersonalismo* This term refers to the value
of warm and individualized attention to others, with some informality and
openness, but without being overly familiar. Personalismo requires helping
professionals to give respect to parents' beliefs and assumptions, rather than
pressing upon the family the messages garnered from child development
research. One such belief among Latino families is that family members
should be interdependent, an idea that contrasts with the mainstream or
standard American goal of making babies independent. 9
Child Care and Relationships. The improvement of relationships for in-
fants and toddlers in day care has been a major thrust of child development
advocates in recent years. A national initiative, the Better Baby Care
Campaign,10 has focused on a number of improvements in the child care
standards as they relate to attachment. These include specialized training for
infant and toddler caregivers, with a stress on child development and family
relationships.
Other groups, such as the National Association for the Education of
Young Children (NAEYC), Zero to Three, and the World Association for
Infant Mental Health, are trying to bring about changes in child care and
produce a more attachment-friendly setting. They are working against the
common assumption that infant day care is the same thing as preschool,
New Directions 161
for the child wants them even when in mother's lap.) A common day care
practice in the 1970s and 1980s was to prohibit such objects in the day care
center, or to insist that the child share them—an idea about as acceptable to
toddlers as the proposal to adults that they share a spouse with a friend.
Attachment-friendly care encourages the use of attachment objects, recog-
nizing that these provide a way for children to handle separation and also,
the exciting stimulation of the group.
Recognition of secure base behavior in the day care center is a way to
support both emotional and cognitive development. The toddler's explora-
tion of new people and events works much better when a familiar person
is available as a secure base to return to for emotional refueling. The secure-
base person needs not just to be available, but to cooperate in the task. She
needs to be responsive when the exploring toddler comes back, but also
attentive enough to return eye contact from a distance, or to speak to a
child who is looking back anxiously. A good secure-base person does not
regard the child as clingy or whiny, but recognizes the emotional signifi-
cance of checking-in with a familiar adult.
The handling of transitions is another factor relevant to attachment. The
arrival of a child at the day care center, going to sleep and awakening, and
being picked up by a parent at day's end are all transitions which relate to
separation and reunion concerns. These times need to be handled carefully
and with attention to the child's communications. Arrival and departure are
especially problematic times because parent and child affect each other with
their emotional communications about their situation. The parent may fear
a scene, if the child does not readily accept separation, and may be equally
concerned with the child's distress, worries about other parents' disap-
proval, and anxiety about being late to work. Or, paradoxically, the parent
may be concerned that a child who separates quietly is not sufficiently at-
tached. In either case, the child is liable to be affected by the parent's invol-
untary communication of fear and sadness.
In the late afternoon, many children will express anger about separation,
often by ignoring the returning parent and resisting what the parent tries to
do. Parents in turn feel anxiety, weariness, and irritation at the complexity
of what should be a simple process. Day care centers can help by providing
plenty of time for transitions and giving emotional support for parents and
children who have trouble with separation and reunion. At the time of ar-
rival, especially, a caregiver needs to greet the baby and help to make the
entry a gradual one.
Finally, attachment-friendly day care centers need policies that support
families' emotional lives and their connections with their children.
Exhausted and emotionally drained, young working parents often turn to
their child's caregiver for advice and nurturing, needs that are not the
New Directions 63
caregiver's real job, nor anything she has usually been trained to deal with.
Day care staffs need help recognizing and understanding parents' needs, but
at the same time they need to set limits on the demands made of them. The
combination of responsiveness and limit setting needs to be done at a man-
agerial level and in a way that provides helpful guidelines for parents. The
increasing interest of day care centers in these relationship issues is a real,
but very gradual change.
demanding attention, and so on, before exploring again. After that occa-
sion, the parent would probably become more attentive, watching the
child, being available to return eye contact when the child looks back, and
perhaps speaking to the child. Together, the two would work out how far
away the child might go with comfort to both partners, and how much at-
tention the parent needs to pay in order to support the child's exploration.
After several more park visits, the transactional process would result in ef-
fective secure base behaviors, which involve seeking on the child's part and
cooperating on the parent's. (If, on the other hand, the parent was insensi-
tive to the child's signals, the result might be a degree of risk-taking behav-
ior sufficient to grab the parent's attention.)
Both systems theory and the transactional process are useful ways to ap-
proach the complexity of attachment. Unfortunately, it is quite difficult to
apply these ideas to the design of research on emotional development, or to
interpret research that involves so many factors. In any case, these ways of
thinking have helped us to get past some oversimplified perspectives on
attachment.
Attachment and Theory of Mind. A fascinating and potentially fruitful new
way of looking at attachment links the process with the development of
Theory ofMind.]:> Theory of Mind (TOM) is also referred to as mentaliza-
tion or Reflective Function. The term Theory of Mind describes the ability
of all normal human adults to read minds—not literally, of course, but to
make excellent guesses about what another person knows, wants, be-
lieves, hopes, and intends. We use T O M when we assume that all these
human bodies around us, a lot like ours, have inside them the capacity to
know and experience the world just as we do. In order to use T O M well,
however, we must also understand that another person's knowledge, de-
sires, intentions, and subjective experience are probably not identical to
our own.
Communication with others depends on T O M . If we were to assume
that speech was just mechanical, reflexive noise making, unconnected with
a mind, we could not understand another person's intentions or make our
own clear. When a guest refuses a second helping of mashed potatoes, our
capacity to use T O M enables us to decide with some degree of accuracy
whether to press the offer or let it drop, or even whether to take offense at
the implication that our cooking might not be appetizing.
Of course, we as adults all know that we can do T O M tasks, even though
we do not always do them very well. What does T O M have to do with
attachment or any part of early emotional development? The connection
comes in the course of early development and involves complicated steps.
The description below is a highly simplified summary of a large body of
research and theory, linking parents' abilities to mentalize their emotional
66 Understanding Attachment
who pays attention to the child, quickly and accurately mirroring the
child's expression. The responsive caregiver quickly notices the child's
gaze and joins in joint attention, looking as instructed and exchanging a
smile and glance of pleasure with the child. The caregiver talks to the child
and responds to and joins in pretend play, showing the affectionate, playful
sociability that seems to be a foundation of attachment. At the same time,
the caregiver is providing the language that helps to describe emotional
states ("Is that a scary noise?"), and some practice in understanding inten-
tion and belief. The child may come to think about intention because the
caregiver acts as if it is important. The caregiver's response to the child's
intentions also provides quick comfort and a return to security. A familiar
caregiver will understand the child's signals best and therefore be the best
at comforting the child and providing a secure base.
Development of T O M is essential for negotiation, a significant step in
the attachment process. To negotiate effectively, the child must recognize
that the parent's intentions, desires, and beliefs may be different from the
child's, and this means that to some degree, the child must be capable of
reflecting on his or her own mental state. Negotiation involves attempts to
move toward another's position without abandoning too much of one's
own, and thus requires knowledge of both people's positions, which are
based partly on T O M . Similarly, the development of the goal-corrected
partnership involves progressive change in each partner's assessment of the
other's knowledge and wishes. Parents can and do model this aspect of
T O M by offering compromise, explaining their thinking, and admitting
that they have had a false belief, if this is the case.
Finally, T O M seems to be necessary for development of an effective
internal working model of social relations. Mature T O M involves ways of
thinking both about the self and about other people. These capacities en-
able the individual to be aware of how his own intentions and beliefs are
like those of other people and how they are different. It also enables the
child to understand how others may read him, thus predicting safety, secu-
rity, hostility, nurturing, or rejection by others. Such predictions can in-
clude how others will act and feel about themselves and how one will act
and feel toward someone else. Importantly, mature T O M allows the un-
derstanding of individual differences and needs, a step that creates a differ-
ent position in the internal working model for the vulnerable baby than for
the adult enemy.
1. Newborn infants are little, if at all, affected by separation from the bio-
logical mother. The mother may be distressed by the loss, but there is
no need to be concerned about a direct effect on the child.
2. Attachment develops as a result of consistent, sensitive, responsive care,
especially in the first two years. For the best developmental outcomes,
foster care and nonparental day care need to be planned with this foun-
dation. Consistency may be the bugaboo of small minds, but is the
essential factor in the social environment of small children.
3. From about six to eight months and on, abrupt long-term separations
are disturbing to the child and interfere with sleeping, eating, playing,
and learning. A child in this age group is old enough to be affected by
170 Understanding Attachment
CHAPTER 1
1. Jerrold M. Post, Leaders and Their Followers in a Dangerous World (Ithaca,
NY: Cornell University Press, 2004); E. Bumiller, "Was a Tyrant Prefigured by
Baby Saddam?" New York Times, May 15, 2004, B9.
2. Margaret Mahler, "Rapprochement Subphase of the Separation-Individuation
Process," in Rapprochement: Tlie Critical Subphase of Separation-Individuation, ed. R. Lax,
S. Bach, and J. Burland (New York: Aronson, 1980).
3. Marshall H. Klaus, R. Jerauld, N. Kreger, W. McAlpme, M. Steffa, and
J. H. Kennell, "Maternal Attachment: Importance of the First Post-Partum Days,"
New England Journal of Medicine 286 (1972): 460-463.
4. Daniel Yankelovitch Group for Zero to Three and Civitas, "What Grown-
ups Understand about Child Development: A Benchmark Study" (Washington,
DC: Zero to Three, 2000).
5. Jack P. Shonkoff, "Science, Policy, and Practice: Three Cultures in Search
of a Shared Mission," Child Development 7\ (2000): 181-187.
CHAPTER 2
1. Valerie Fildes, Wet Nursing: A History from Antiquity to the Present (New
York: Blackwell, 1988).
2. Ibid., 115, 117.
3. R. de Saussure, "J. B. Felix Descuret," trans. R. de Saussure, Psychoanalytic
Study of the Child 2 (1940): 420.
174 Notes
4. Philip Slater, "Child Rearing During the Early National Period" (PhD
diss., University of California, Berkeley, 1970).
5. Ian Suttie, The Origins of Love and Hate (London: Penguin, 1935).
6. Irenaus Eibl-Eibesfeldt, Ethology: The Biology of Behavior (New York: Holt
Rinehart Winston, 1970).
7. K. M. Wolf, "Evacuation of Children in Wartime," Psychoanalytic Study of
the Child! (1945): 389.
8. Anna Freud and Dorothy T. Burlingham, War and Children (New York:
Medical War Books, 1943).
9. Anna Freud and Dorothy T. Burlingham, Infants Without Families: Reports
on the Hampstead Nurseries, 1935-1945, vol. 3, Writings of Anna Freud (New York:
International Universities Press, 1973).
10. John Bowlby, Attachment (New York: Basic, 1982).
11. Bruno Bettelheim, The Children of the Dream (Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press, 1969); S. Diamond, "Kibbutz and Shtetl: The History of an
Idea," Social Problems 2 (1957): 71-99; and M. E. Spiro, Children of the Kibbutz
(Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1958).
12. Bettelheim, Children of the Dream, 33.
13. Ibid., 108.
14. K.Johnson, H. Banghan, and W. Liyao, "Infant Abandonment and Adop-
tion in China," Population and Development Review 24, no. 3 (1998): 469-510.
15. Michael Rutter, J. Kreppner, J. O'Connor, and English and Romanian
Adoptees (ERA) Study Team, "Risk and Resilience Following Profound Early
Global Deprivation," British Journal of Psychiatry 179 (2001): 97-103.
16. Michael Rutter, "Nature, Nurture, and Development: From Evangelism
through Science Toward Policy and Practice," Child Development 73 (2002): 1-21.
CHAPTER 3
1. Rene Spitz, "Hospitalism: An Inquiry into the Genesis of Psychiatric Con-
ditions in Early Childhood," Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 1 (1945): 53-74.
2. Jeremy Holmes, John Bowlby and Attachment Theory (London: Routledge,
1993); Inge Bretherton, "The Origins of Attachment Theory: John Bowlby and
Mary Ainsworth," Developmental Psychology 28 (1992): 759-775.
3. Melanie Klein, Contributions to Psychoanalysis, 1921-1945 (London: Hogarth,
1948).
4. Bretherton, "Origins of Attachment Theory."
5. John Bowlby, "Forty-Four Juvenile Thieves: Their Characters and Home
Life," International Journal ofPsychoanalysis 25 (1944): 19-52, 107-127.
6. James Robertson and Joyce Robertson, Nine Days in a Residential Nursery
(Van Nuys, CA: Child Development Media, 1953) video.
7. Harry Harlow, "The Nature of Love," American Psychologist 13 (1959):
573-585.
8. John Bowlby, "The Nature of a Child's Tie to his Mother," International
Journal of Psychoanalysis 39 (1958): 350-433; John Bowlby, "Separation Anxiety,"
Notes 175
CHAPTER 4
1. Rene Spitz, "Relevancy of Direct Infant Observation," Psychoanalytic Study
of the Child 5 (1950): 66-73.
2. W. Ernest Freud, "Prenatal Attachment and Bonding," in Course of Life,
vol. 1, Infancy, ed. S. I. Greenspan and G. H. Pollock (Madison, CT: International
Universities Press, 1989), 467-484.
3. Donald Winnicott, "Transitional Objects and Transitional Phenomena," in
Collected Papers: Through Pediatrics to Psychoanalysis, ed. D. Winnicott (Middlesex,
England: Penguin, 1953), 1-25.
4. L. B. Adamson and C. L. Russell, "Emotion Regulation in the Emer-
gence of Joint Attention," in Early Social Cognition, ed. P. Rochat (Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum, 1999), 281-297.
5. Rene Spitz, "Hospitalism: An Inquiry into the Genesis of Psychiatric Con-
ditions in Early Childhood," Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 1(1945): 53-74.
176 Notes
CHAPTER 5
1. Bowlby, "Forty-Four Juvenile Thieves," 19-52, 107-127.
2. Jerome Kagan, Three Seductive Ideas (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press, 2000).
3. M. A. Easterbrooks and W. A. Goldberg, "Security of Toddler-Parent
Attachment," in Attachment in the Preschool Years, ed. M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti,
and M. Cummings (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1990), 221-244.
4. Mary Main and D. Weston, "The Independence of Infant-Mother and
Infant-Father Relationships: Security of Attachment Characterizes Relationships,
Not Infants," Child Development 52 (1981): 932-940.
5. Vivian B. Shapiro, Janet R. Shapiro, and Isabel Paret, Complex Adoption and
Assisted Reproductive Technology (New York: Guilford, 2001).
6. Nancy Scheper-Hughes, Death Without Weeping (Berkeley, CA: University
of California Press, 1992).
7. Ibid., 471.
8. Marinus van IJzendoorn, "Adult Attachment Representations, Parental
Responsiveness, and Infant Attachment: A Meta-Analysis on the Predictive
Validity of the Adult Attachment Interview," Psychological Bulletin 117 (1995):
387-403.
9. Mary Main and E. Hesse, "Parents' Unresolved Traumatic Experiences
Are Related to Infants' Insecure-Disorganized/Disoriented Attachment Status: Is
Frightened or Frightening Behavior the Linking Mechanism?" in Attachment in the
Preschool Years, ed. M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, and M. Cummings (Chicago:
University of Chicago Press, 1990), 161-184.
10. Robertson and Robertson, Nine Days in a Residential Nursery.
11. Jay Belsky and M. J. Rovine, "Nonmaternal Care in the First Year of Life
and the Security of Infant-Parent Attachment," Child Development 59 (1988): 157-167;
Jay Belsky and J. M. Braungart, "Are Insecure-Avoidant Infants with Extensive
Notes 177
CHAPTER 6
1. American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 4th ed. (Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1994).
2. Rutter, "Nature, Nurture, and Development," 1—21.
3. Mary Main and J. Solomon, "Discovery of an Insecure-Disorganized/Dis-
oriented Attachment Pattern," in Affective Development in Infancy, ed. T. B. Brazelton
and M. Yogman (Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 1986), 136.
4. Ibid., 137.
5. Ibid., 138.
6. Ibid.
78 Notes
7. Ibid., 139.
8. Main and Hesse, "Parents' Unresolved Traumatic Experiences," 161-184.
9. Ibid., 175.
10. Ibid.
11. Ibid., 176.
12. Ibid.
13. John Bowlby, M. Ainsworth, M. Boston, and D. Rosenblith, "The Effects
of Mother-Child Separation: A Follow-Up Study," British Journal of Medical Psy-
chology 29 (1956): 211-247.
14. American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual.
15. Rochelle F. Hanson and E. G. Spratt, "Reactive Attachment Disorder in
Children: What We Know About the Disorder and Implications for Treatment,"
Child Maltreatment 5, no. 2 (2000): 137-145.
16. Frederick R. Volkmar, "Reactive Attachment Disorder," in DSM-IVSource-
book, ed. T. A. Widiger, A. J. Frances, H. A. Pincus, R. Ross et al. (Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1997).
17. World Health Organization, ICD-10 (Geneva, Switzerland: W H O , 1992).
18. Charles H. Zeanah, O. Mammen, and A. F. Lieberman, "Disorders of At-
tachment," in Handbook of Infant Mental Health, ed. C. H. Zeanah (New York:
Guilford, 1993), 332-349.
19. Ibid., 343.
20. Ibid., 344.
21. CarlJ. Sheperis, R. A. Dogget, N. E. Hoda, T. Blanchard, E. L. Renfro-
Michael, S. H. Holdiness, and R. Schlagheck, "The Development of an Assessment
Protocol for Reactive Attachment Disorder," Journal of Mental Health Counseling
25, no. 4 (2003): 291-310.
22. Constance G. Dalenberg, Countertransference and the Treatment of Trauma
(Washington DC: American Psychological Association, 2000).
23. Mary Dozier, K. C. Stovall, K. Albus, and B. Bates, "Assessment for Infants
in Foster Care: The Role of the Caregiver's State of Mind," Child Development 72
(2001): 1467-1477.
24. Alicia F. Lieberman, "The Treatment of Attachment Disorders in Infancy
and Early Childhood: Reflections from Clinical Intervention in Later Adopted
Foster Care Children," Attachment and Human Development 5, no. 3 (2003): 279-282.
25. Ibid., 281.
26. Ibid., 282.
27. Ibid.
28. Mary Dozier, "Attachment-Based Treatment for Vulnerable Children,"
Attachment and Human Development 5, no. 3 (2003): 253-257.
29. Ibid., 255.
30. Ibid., 256.
31. Stanley I. Greenspan, Infancy and Early Childhood (Madison, CT: Interna-
tional Universities Press, 1992).
32. Jean Mercer, L. Sarner, and L. Rosa, Attachment Therapy On Trial (Westport,
CT: Praeger, 2003).
Notes 79
33. Ronald S. Federici, Help for the Hopeless Child (Alexandria, VA: Ronald S.
Federici and Associates, 2003).
34. Vivian B. Shapiro, Janet R. Shapiro, and Isabel Paret, Complex Adoption and
Assisted Reproductive Technology (New York: Guilford, 2001).
35. Daniel S. Schechter, "How Post-Traumatic Stress Affects Mothers' Per-
ceptions of their Babies: Brief Video Feedback Intervention Makes a Difference,"
Zero to Three 24, no. 3 (2004): 43-49.
36. Ibid., 43.
37. Ibid.
38. Ibid., 47.
39. Ibid., 48.
CHAPTER 7
1. J. Sears, M. Sears, R. Sears, and W. Sears, The Baby Book: Everything You
Need to Know About Your Baby From Birth to Age Two (New York: Little, Brown,
2003).
2. Gary Ezzo and R. Bucknam, On Becoming Babywise: Parenting Your Pretod-
dler, Five to Fifteen Months (Sisters, O R : Multnomah Books, 1995).
3. Robert W. Zaslow and M. Menta, The Psychology of the Z-Process (San Jose,
CA: San Jose State University Press, 1975).
4. Mercer et al., Attachment Therapy On Trial.
5. Nancy Thomas, "Parenting Children with Attachment Disorders," in
Handbook of Attachment Interventions, ed. T. M. Levy (San Diego, CA: Academic,
2000), 67-111.
6. The mental health practitioner who gave this description has given permis-
sion for its use, but has asked not to be named.
7. Elizabeth Randolph, Manual for the Randolph Attachment Disorder Question-
naire (Evergreen, C O : The Attachment Center Press, 2000).
8. Jirina Prekop, Haettest du mich festgehalten (Muenchen: Koesel, 1991).
9. J. Goldstein, A. Solnit, S. Goldstein, and A. Freud, The Best Interest of the
Child: The Least Detrimental Alternative (New York: Free Press, 1996).
10. American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual.
11. D. E. Arredondo and L. P. Edwards, "Attachment, Bonding, and Recipro-
cal Connectedness," Journal of the Center for Families, Children, and the Courts 146
(2000): 109-127.
CHAPTER 8
1. Everett Waters, K. Kondo-Ikemura, G. Posada, andj. Richters, "Learning
to Love: Mechanisms and Milestones" in Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology,
vol. 23, Self-Processes and Development, ed. M. Gunnar and L. Sroufe (Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum, 1991), 217-255.
2. C. L. Bokhorst, M. J. Bakermans-Kranenburg, R. M. Pasco Fearon,
M. van IJzendoorn, P. Fonagy, and C. Schuengel, "The Importance of Shared
180 Notes
Goldstein, J., A. Solnit, S. Goldstein, and A. Freud. The Best Interest of the Child:
The Least Detrimental Alternative. New York: Free Press, 1996.
Greenspan, S. I. Infancy and Early Childhood. Madison, CT: International Universi-
ties Press, 1993.
Hanson, R. F., and E. G. Spratt. "Reactive Attachment Disorder in Children:
What We Know About the Disorder and Implications for Treatment."
Child Maltreatment 5 (2000), no. 2: 137-145.
Harlow, Harry. "The Nature of Love." American Psychologist 13 (1959): 5 7 3 -
685.
Hart, A. "Chinese Parents Not Tricked, Judge Says in Custody Case." New York
Times, May 13, 2004, sec. A.
Hetherington, E. M., M. Bridges, and G. Insabella. "What Matters? What Does
Not? Five Perspectives on the Association Between Marital Transitions and
Children's Adjustment." American Psychologist 53 (1998): 167-184.
Holmes, Jeremy. John Bowlby and Attachment Theory. London: Routledge, 1993.
Huth-Bocks, A. C , A. A. Levendosky, G. A. Bogat, and A. von Eye. "The Impact
of Maternal Characteristics and Contextual Variables on Infant-Mother
Attachment." Child Development 75 (2004): 480-496.
Johnson, K., H. Banghan, and W. Liyao. "Infant Abandonment and Adoption in
China." Population and Development Review, no. 3 (1998): 469—510.
Kagan, Jerome. Three Seductive Ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press,
2000.
Kennell, J. H., and M. H. Klaus. "Caring for the Parents of a Stillborn or an Infant
Who Dies." In Parent-Infant Bonding, edited by M. H. Klaus and J. H.
Kennell. St. Louis: Mosby, 1982.
Klaus, M. H., R. Jerauld, N. Kreger, W. McAlpme, M. Steffa, andj. H. Kennell.
"Maternal Attachment: Importance of the First Post-Partum Days." New
England Journal of Medicine 286 (1972): 460-463.
Klein, Melanie. Contributions to Psychoanalysis, 1921—1945. London: Hogarth,
1948.
Leon, I. "Adoption Losses: Naturally Occurring or Socially Constructed?" Child
Development 73 (2002): 652-663.
Levy, David M. "The Concept of Maternal Overprotection." In Parenthood: Its
Psychology and Psychopathology, edited by E. J. Anthony and T. Benedek.
Boston: Little, Brown, 1970.
Lieberman, Alicia F. "The Treatment of Attachment Disorders in Infancy and
Early Childhood: Reflections from Clinical Intervention in Later Adopted
Foster Care Children." Attachment and Human Development 5, no. 3 (2003):
279-282.
Lurie-Hurvitz, E. "A Champion for Babies: An Interview with Joan Lombardi."
Zero to Three 21, no. 6 (2001): 40-44.
Mahler, Margaret. "Rapprochement Subphase of the Separation-Individuation
Process." In Rapprochement: The Critical Subphase of Separation-Individuation,
edited by R. Lax, S. Bach, and J. Burland. New York: Aronson, 1980.
Bibliography 85