The Cognitive Neuroscience of Lucid Dreaming: Try Out and Tell Us What You Think.
The Cognitive Neuroscience of Lucid Dreaming: Try Out and Tell Us What You Think.
Try out PMC Labs and tell us what you think. Learn More.
Copyright notice
Publisher's Disclaimer
Abstract
Lucid dreaming refers to the phenomenon of becoming aware of the fact that
one is dreaming during ongoing sleep. Despite having been physiologically
validated for decades, the neurobiology of lucid dreaming is still incompletely
characterized. Here we review the neuroscientific literature on lucid dreaming,
including electroencephalographic, neuroimaging, brain lesion,
pharmacological and brain stimulation studies. Electroencephalographic studies
of lucid dreaming are mostly underpowered and show mixed results.
Neuroimaging data is scant but preliminary results suggest that prefrontal and
parietal regions are involved in lucid dreaming. A focus of research is also to
develop methods to induce lucid dreams. Combining training in mental set with
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 1/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
1. Introduction
Becoming aware that one is dreaming while dreaming, what is today referred to
as lucid dreaming, has been known about since antiquity. In Western literature,
it may have first been mentioned by Aristotle in the fourth century BCE in the
treatise On dreams of his Parva Naturali, in which he states: “often when one
is asleep, there is something in consciousness which declares that what then
presents itself is but a dream” (Aristotle, 1941, p. 624). Likewise, in Eastern
cultures, particularly of the south Asian subcontinent, reports of individuals
engaging in practices to cultivate awareness of dream and sleep states date
back millennia (LaBerge, 2003; Norbu and Katz, 1992; Wallace and Hodel,
2012). These include meditative practices specifically designed to “apprehend
the dream state” (Padmasambhava, 1998, p. 156).
Despite the fact that such personal accounts of lucid dreams have been
described for centuries, the topic faced skepticism from some scientists and
philosophers (e.g., Malcolm, 1959), in part due to the lack of objective
evidence for the phenomenon. This began to change in the late 1970s and early
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 2/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
Through this technique, which has since become the gold standard, reports of
lucid dreams could be objectively verified by the presence of distinct volitional
eye movement patterns as recorded in the electrooculogram (EOG) during
polysomnography-verified sleep (Figure 1). The most common version of the
eye signaling technique asks participants to signal when they realize they are
dreaming by rapidly looking all the way to the left then all the way to the right
two times consecutively then back to center in the dream without pausing
(referred to as left-right-left-right eye signals, abbreviated as LRLR). As can be
seen in Figure 1, the LRLR signal is readily discernable in the horizontal EOG,
which exhibits a distinctive shape containing four consecutive full-scale eye
movements that have larger amplitude compared to typical REMs. As we
describe in detail below, lucid dreams can be validated by this method through
the convergence between reports obtained after awakening of becoming lucid
and making the eye movement signals during the dream, accompanied by the
objective eye movement signals recorded in the EOG with concurrent
polysomonographic evidence of REM sleep.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 3/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
Fig 1.
Lucid REM sleep eye movement signaling paradigm.
The eye signaling technique also offers a way of objectively contrasting lucid
REM sleep to baseline non-lucid REM sleep, providing a method to investigate
the changes in brain activity associated with lucid dreaming. Furthermore,
lucid dreamers can not only signal to indicate that they are aware that they are
dreaming, but they can also make the eye movement signals to time-stamp the
start and end of experimental tasks performed during lucid dreams (LaBerge,
1990). By providing objective temporal markers, this technique has opened up
a new method for studying the psychophysiology of REM sleep, allowing, for
example, investigations into the neural correlates of dreamed behaviors (e.g.,
Dresler et al., 2011; Erlacher, Schredl and LaBerge, 2003; LaBerge, 1990;
Oudiette et al., 2018). Lucid dreaming thus provides a way to establish precise
psychophysiological correlations between the contents of consciousness during
sleep and physiological measures, as well as enables experimental control over
the content of dreams, and therefore provides a potentially highly useful
experimental methodology.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 4/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
While neuroscientific studies on lucid dreaming have been performed since the
late 1970s, the topic has received increasing attention in recent years due to its
relevance to the emerging neuroscience of consciousness. In this article, we
review the existing literature on the neuroscience of lucid dreaming, including
electrophysiological, neuroimaging, brain lesion, pharmacological and brain
stimulation studies. Additionally, we review recent studies that illustrate how
lucid dreaming can be used as a methodology in the cognitive neuroscience of
consciousness. Finally, we present strategies to measure lucid dreaming both
physiologically and with questionnaires, and discuss procedures to investigate
lucid dreaming in the sleep laboratory.
These findings also raise the further question of whether lucid REM sleep is
associated with localized activation of specific brain regions or changes in
specific frequencies of neural oscillations compared to non-lucid REM sleep.
In this section, we will review EEG studies that have attempted to address this
question. As will be discussed below, while these studies represent important
first steps toward measuring the electrophysiological changes associated with
lucid dreaming, all of them have interpretive issues and most suffer from low
statistical power. As a result, there is considerable discrepancy among findings.
Below we group and discuss results based on the regional EEG band power
changes reported to be associated with lucid REM sleep dreaming.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 5/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
Subsequent research has not supported the hypothesis that lucid REM sleep is
associated with increased alpha activity. For example, in a follow-up study
Tyson, Ogilvie and Hunt (1984) found that only pre-lucid but not lucid dreams
significantly differed in alpha activity compared to non-lucid REM sleep. In
another study of eight frequent lucid dreamers using a similar experimental
design, Ogilvie, Hunt, Kushniruk and Newman (1983) observed no difference
in the number of lucid dreams following awakenings from periods of high or
low alpha activity. Furthermore, in a replication attempt of the original case
report that observed an increase in alpha during lucid REM sleep (Ogilvie et
al., 1978), LaBerge and colleagues found no significant differences in alpha
power at the same central channel (C3) from a single subject (LaBerge, 1988b).
Finally, a later follow-up case study analyzed EEG spectral power in five lucid
REM sleep epochs compared to non-lucid REM sleep periods, and observed no
differences in alpha power (Ogilvie, Vieira and Small, 1991). Together this
evidence does not support a reliable association between alpha power and lucid
dreaming. However, the limited spatial coverage of EEG montages in these
studies (in several cases consisting of only one EEG channel) makes it unclear
to what extent these results can be generalized to other brain areas.
2.1.2. Parietal beta Holzinger, LaBerge and Levitan (2006) examined EEG
spectral changes during lucid REM sleep in a group of eleven participants who
reported prior experience with lucid dreaming. Six out of the eleven
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 6/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
Voss et al. (2009) corrected for eye movement artifacts using regression of the
EOG signal (Gratton, Coles and Donchin, 1983) and by computing current
source densities (CSD) in addition to scalp potentials. However, regression-
based correction procedures are insufficient to remove the SP artifact, and
while the CSD derivation attenuates the SP artifact at posterior channels, it is
not sufficient to remove it at anterior scalp locations (Keren et al., 2010; Yuval-
Greenberg and Deouell, 2009). As Keren et al. (2010) state: “In conclusion,
SCD [CSD] seems to be effective in attenuating the SP effect at posterior sites.
However at sites anterior to Cz and closer to the orbits efficacy gradually
decreases, preserving the temporal and spectral signature of the SP and its
amplitude relative to baseline.” (p. 2258). The influence of the SP artifact on
gamma band power in the comparison of lucid to non-lucid REM sleep is
particularly relevant given that, as noted above, lucid REM sleep has been
associated with increased phasic activation and higher eye movement density
(e.g., LaBerge, 1990).
It is important to note that the need to account for the SP artifact does not
preclude a potential association between increased frontal gamma power and
lucid REM sleep. Indeed, given the link between gamma band power, local
field potentials (LFPs) and the blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signal
(Lachaux et al., 2007; Nir et al., 2007), if the transition from non-lucid to lucid
REM sleep involves activation or recruitment of additional frontal brain
regions (a plausible hypothesis, also in light of the findings of Dresler et al.
(2012); see Neuroimaging of lucid dreaming below), regional increases in
gamma power might be predicted. However, the spatial topography and
frequency localization of any such effects are likely to be strongly influenced
by the correction and removal of the SP artifact.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 8/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
A limitation of the Dodet et al. (2014) study is that EEG signals were only
evaluated at six electrodes, and only in frontal and central scalp regions (Fp1,
Fp2, F7, F8, C3, C4). Parietal electrodes were not included in the EEG
montage, and occipital channels reportedly could not be evaluated due to noise.
Thus, it is possible that local changes in EEG spectra in posterior regions, such
as parietal or occipital areas, were missed due to the limited electrode montage.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 9/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
The finding of lower delta activity during lucid REM sleep is in line with
previous observations that lucid dreams tend to occur during periods of
increased cortical activation (LaBerge, 1990). Specifically, slow waves,
reflected by delta (~0.5-4 Hz) power in the EEG, are associated with neuronal
down states (“off’ periods) in which neurons are hyperpolarized (Steriade,
Timofeev and Grenier, 2001). Decreased delta power (EEG activation)
therefore reflects recovery of neural activity. While the bi-stability between
“on” and “ off’ periods is a central feature of non-REM sleep, slow wave
activity has also been observed in REM sleep (Baird et al., 2018b; Funk,
Honjoh, Rodriguez, Cirelli and Tononi, 2016). Neuronal down states have also
been linked to the loss of consciousness during both anesthesia and sleep
(Purdon et al., 2013; Tononi and Massimini, 2008), which is hypothesized to be
related to the breakdown of causal interactions between bi-stable neurons
(Pigorini et al., 2015; Tononi, Boly, Massimini and Koch, 2016). Therefore,
one potential explanation is that this finding reflects reduced bi-stability and
increased causal interactions between cortical neurons in these areas during
lucid REM sleep. Notably, reduced delta power in posterior cortex has been
found to be associated with dreaming as opposed to dreamless sleep in both
REM and NREM sleep (Siclari et al., 2017). An intriguing speculation based
on these results is therefore that this reduction in delta power also extends to
frontal regions during lucid REM sleep dreaming. However, it remains to be
seen whether these findings can be replicated and whether the results
generalize to non-clinical populations.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 10/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
Another factor that might contribute to the discrepant findings is the fact that
lucid dreaming can be achieved and executed in different ways. For example,
the observed changes in the EEG during lucid REM sleep might depend in part
on the degree of vividness, working memory, emotional tone, self-
consciousness, attention and insight, which could vary across individuals as
well as specific dreams. Relatedly, different subjective experiences and
contents during lucid dreams plausibly have their own neurobiological
substrates (Mota-Rolim, Erlacher, Tort, Araujo and Ribeiro, 2010), just as in
non-lucid dreams (Siclari et al., 2017). Changes in brain activity during lucidity
may also partly depend on how experienced the lucid dreamer is. For instance,
lucid dreams of less experienced individuals may often be more ephemeral and
involve less control over dream content, while more experienced lucid
dreamers may be more likely to have longer and more stable lucid dreams, as
well as the capacity to exert greater amounts of control. This might lead to a
more distinct signal in the EEG for experienced lucid dreamers on the one
hand, but also presumably less neural activity related to the effort needed to
maintain the state (neural efficiency). In line with this, Dodet et al. (2014)
suggested that the mental effort needed to achieve and sustain lucidity might be
reduced in narcoleptic patients, who may access the lucid REM sleep state with
less effort.
These comments should not be taken to indicate that there is not a consistent
neurobiology of lucid dreaming. However, it does suggest that analysis of the
EEG spectral changes associated with lucid dreaming used in previous studies
may need to be optimized for detecting more subtle and localized effects. All
studies reviewed above have measured the average power over a given spectral
band and region over comparably long time intervals. However, it is possible
that lucid dreaming is associated with spectral changes that can only be
detected by a better time resolved analysis, such as time-frequency analysis,
that may be overlooked by averaging over large windows in time or frequency
space.
Larger sample sizes are needed in future studies to achieve adequate statistical
power. One way to approach this would be to undertake more extensive
population screening for high-frequency lucid dreamers. For example, through
mass surveys, thousands of potential participants could be screened and the top
few percent reporting the highest lucid dream frequency could be selected for
training before undergoing sleep laboratory recordings. As we discuss below,
new techniques for lucid dream induction also have the potential to enable
efficient collection of larger datasets.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 12/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 13/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
Stumbrys and Erlacher (2012) reported two potential cases of lucid dreams
during NREM with eye signaling. However, due to the study protocol, the
experimenters could only collect dream reports the following morning. In the
first case, the participant reported the next morning making an eye movement
signal in a lucid dream early in the night, but given the long amount of time
between the report and the signal there is uncertainty whether the report
corresponds to the observed signal during NREM sleep. Furthermore, while the
two 30-second polysomnography epochs for sleep scoring preceding the eye
signal appear to be unambiguous N2 sleep, the EEG dynamic shifts during the
30-second epoch containing the eye-signal to lower amplitude activity without
apparent spindles or K-complexes. Without knowing the stage of the epochs
following the eye signal, which was not reported in the study, it is possible that
the eye signal occurred in a transitional sleep stage. In the second case reported
by Stumbrys and Erlacher (2012), the eye signals occurred with some signs of
arousal and the participant had no memory of executing the eye signals the
following morning. These data therefore provide ambiguous evidence for
signal-verified lucid dreams during NREM sleep.
In three further case reports of eye movement signaled NREM lucid dreams,
one case was reported to occur in N1 and two cases in N2 visually scored sleep
stages (Mota-Rolim et al., 2015). The first case was scored as N1, since an
increase in theta (4-7 Hz) and a decrease in alpha (7-14 Hz) power was
observed in more than half of the 30 second scoring epoch, meeting the AASM
criteria for classification of stage N1 sleep. The other two cases were scored as
occurring during N2 episodes since they had spindles and K-complexes in the
30 second scoring epoch with the eye signals. These data thus replicate signal-
verification of N1 lucid dreams and provide preliminary evidence for signal-
verified N2 lucid dreams.
Together, these results suggest that although most lucid dreams occur during
REM sleep, they can also occur during NREM sleep. However, additional
studies providing objective evidence of NREM lucid dreams confirmed by eye-
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 14/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
MRI scanner. Compared to non-lucid REM sleep, lucid REM sleep showed
increased fMRI BOLD signal in a number of cortical regions, including the
superior frontal gyrus, aPFC, medial and lateral parietal cortex, inferior/middle
temporal gyri and occipital cortex (Figure 2a). Interestingly, several of these
regions, particularly the parietal regions and frontal pole, are areas that, as
noted above, consistently show reduced regional cerebral blood flow during
non-lucid REM sleep compared to wakefulness (Nir and Tononi, 2010).
Figure 2.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 16/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
Dresler et al. (2012) additionally observed increased BOLD signal during lucid
dreaming in the bilateral precuneus and inferior parietal lobules (angular and
supramarginal gyri). As noted above, parietal cortex and the precuneus in
particular has been implicated in self-referential processing, episodic memory,
and the experience of agency (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006), mirroring the
increase of these cognitive capabilities during lucid dreaming. Finally,
activation increases during lucid dreaming were also found in some occipital
and inferior temporal regions, which are part of the ventral stream of visual
processing involved in conscious visual perception (Rees et al., 2002). While
these activations may seem puzzling at first sight, as non-lucid dreams are also
characterized by vivid dream imagery, they are in line with reports that lucid
dreams can be associated with increased visual vividness and clarity of the
dream scene (e.g., Green, 1968).
There are several limitations to the study by Dresler et al. (2012) that are
important to note. First, as mentioned above, the findings are based on
observations from a single subject and caution is therefore warranted in
generalizing from the results. Currently no group-level fMRI study of lucid
dreaming has been conducted, and such a study, along with systematic
replications, will be needed before firm conclusions can be drawn. Another
limitation is that, as described below (see Section 7.1), the participant was
performing a task during the lucid REM sleep segment (repeated eye signaling
and hand clenching). While Dresler and colleagues accounted for task
activation via nuisance regression of the left and right fist clenching task, it is
possible that some of the activations observed still partially reflect task
execution and maintenance of attention/task-set rather than lucidity per se. One
way to address this in future studies would be to contrast periods of lucid REM
sleep when the participant is not performing an explicit task to non-lucid REM
sleep, i.e., a “no-task, within-state paradigm” (Siclari et al., 2013).
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 17/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
A limitation of the study by Filevich et al. (2015) is that participants were not
frequent lucid dreamers per se, but rather subjects from a database who scored
higher relative to other participants on the scale. Lucid dream frequency for the
two groups was not reported in the study, thus it remains unclear to what extent
the “high lucidity” group experienced frequent lucid dreams in absolute terms.
Furthermore, the composite measure of dreaming used to distinguish the two
groups measured not only frequency of lucid dreams but also different
dimensions of dream content. While several of these content dimensions have
been found to be higher in lucid dreams (Voss, Schermelleh-Engel, Windt,
Frenzel and Hobson, 2013), it is likely that several of these dimensions also co-
vary more generally with dream recall and/or cognitive content in dreams
unrelated to lucidity. As a consequence, as the authors note, some of the results
could have been partly influenced by differences in dreaming “styles”, content
or dream recall.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 18/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
bilateral angular gyrus, bilateral middle temporal gyrus and right inferior
frontal gyrus (Figure 2b). The frequent lucid dream group also showed
decreased functional connectivity between left aPFC and bilateral insula.
Whole-brain graph-theoretic analysis revealed that left aPFC had increased
node degree and strength in the frequent lucid dream group compared to the
control group. However, in contrast to the findings of Filevich et al. (2015), no
significant differences in gray matter density were observed between groups in
either a whole-brain analysis or an aPFC region-of-interest analysis.
Given the link to metacognition, it has also been suggested that lucid dreaming
may be linked to large-scale networks that regulate executive control processes,
in particular the frontoparietal control network (Dresler et al., 2015;
Spoormaker et al., 2010). To address this question, Baird et al. (2018a)
additionally evaluated the association between frequent lucid dreaming and
connectivity within established large-scale brain networks, including the
frontoparietal control network. Consistent with a link between the
frontoparietal control network and lucid dreaming, Baird and colleagues found
that frequent lucid dreamers had increased functional connectivity between
aPFC and a network of regions that showed the greatest overlap with a
frontoparietal control sub-network (Dixon et al., 2018; Yeo et al., 2011).
However, neither connectivity within the frontoparietal control network
broadly defined through meta-analysis (or within or between any other large-
scale networks), nor connectivity within frontoparietal control sub-networks, as
defined through parcellation of resting-state networks, showed significant
differences between the lucid dream group and the control group. The authors
speculate that this could be attributed to both the partial overlap of the regions
that showed increased aPFC connectivity in lucid dreamers with the
frontoparietal control network. However, it is important to keep in mind that
while this study did not find a significant difference in resting-state
connectivity within the frontoparietal network in frequent lucid dreamers, it
remains an open question whether lucid REM sleep dreams show increased
connectivity within the frontoparietal control network compared to non-lucid
REM sleep dreams.
to have frequent lucid dreams. However, while the convergence between these
two preliminary studies is encouraging, the paucity of neuroimaging data on
this question limits strong conclusions at the current time.
In line with these remarks, it has been suggested that not only regional
activation of frontoparietal brain areas but also connectivity between these
regions could be important for lucidity to emerge during REM sleep
(Spoormaker et al., 2010). Indeed, while activation in these frontal and parietal
regions has been linked to key functions associated with lucid dreaming,
including metacognition, as discussed above, regional activations and
metabolic increases in these regions have also been observed during states of
global unconsciousness and subliminal information processing. For instance,
subliminally presented no-go stimuli during a response inhibition task activate
frontoparietal cortices in the absence of awareness (van Gaal, Ridderinkhof,
Scholte and Lamme, 2010). Moreover, loss of consciousness during the tonic
phase of generalized tonic-clonic seizures is associated with a transient
increase rather than decrease in metabolism in frontoparietal cortex
(Blumenfeld, 2008; Engel, Kuhl and Phelps, 1982).
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 20/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
connectivity from frontal to parietal regions (Boly et al., 2011; Boly et al.,
2012; Lee et al., 2013). These findings converge with theoretical work and
computational modeling (Tononi, 2011), which has suggested a link between
consciousness and effective connectivity within a neural architecture, or the
capacity of a set of neural elements to exert causal influence over other neural
groups in a system. At present, it is unclear whether this reduction of top-down
frontoparietal connectivity is linked to changes in global brain activity,
alterations in primary consciousness (i.e., subjective, phenomenal states of
seeing, hearing or feeling), or whether it could relate to self-awareness (i.e.,
explicit conscious awareness of oneself and one’s state). A common
interpretation of these results is that top-down frontoparietal connectivity is a
marker of global loss of consciousness, including primary consciousness (e.g.,
Mashour, 2014). However, given that the reduction in top-down connectivity
has also been observed under ketamine (Lee et al., 2013), during which patients
often report vivid dream-like experiences, it is plausible that the reduction of
frontoparietal connectivity could instead indicate loss of self-awareness.
Lucid dreams have not previously been reported following either unilateral or
bilateral thalamic stroke. However, loss of neurons in the anterior and
dorsomedian thalamic nuclei that occurs in familial fatal insomnia is associated
with loss of nocturnal sleep as well as oneiric ‘intrusions’ during wakefulness
(Montagna, 2005; Raggi, Cosentino, Lanuzza and Ferri, 2010). Furthermore,
hypersomnia and irregular sleep are frequently reported following paramedian
thalamic stroke (Hermann et al., 2008; Luigetti et al., 2011). These clinical
features could be manifestations of disruption of the intralaminar and midline
thalamic nuclei located in the mediodorsal thalamus, which are part of the
brain’s arousal network (Van der Werf, Witter and Groenewegen, 2002). Thus,
one possibility is that lucid dreams in these patients could have partly resulted
from increased or abnormal functioning of the brain’s arousal systems during
sleep, which is consistent with the reported increase in nocturnal awakenings.
Both patients also reported that their dreams contained highly anxious and
emotional content, which could reflect abnormal connectivity between these
thalamic nuclei and limbic structures with which they are densely connected
(Van der Werf et al., 2002). The highly emotional or disturbing content may
have also contributed to lucid dreaming, as these types of experiences could
induce individuals to question whether the explanation for such surprising or
frightening experiences is that they are dreaming (LaBerge and Rheingold,
1990).
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 22/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
A main target of research is to develop methods to make the lucid dream state
more accessible. Indeed, reliable techniques to induce lucid dreams are needed
for it to be effectively used in both clinical and scientific applications.
Evidence suggests that lucid dreaming is a learnable skill (LaBerge, 1980a)
that can be developed by training with various induction strategies (LaBerge
and Rheingold, 1990; Price and Cohen, 1988; Stumbrys, Erlacher, Schadlich
and Schredl, 2012). These include training in prospective memory techniques
(LaBerge and Rheingold, 1990), which can be further aided by application of
external sensory cues (LaBerge and Levitan, 1995; LaBerge, Levitan, Rich and
Dement, 1988; LaBerge, Owens, Nagel and Dement, 1981b) and/or
interrupting sleep with short periods of wakefulness (Aspy, Delfabbro, Proeve
and Mohr, 2017; LaBerge, 1980a; LaBerge, Phillips and Levitan, 1994;
Stumbrys et al., 2012). Within cognitive neuroscience, studies have evaluated
pharmacological as well as non-invasive brain stimulation approaches to lucid
dream induction. In this section, we review studies that have taken a
pharmacological approach to lucid dream induction and in the next section we
review electrical brain stimulation studies.
Following on these results and additional pilot research, LaBerge, LaMarca and
Baird (2018b) conducted a double blind, placebo-controlled study in a large
group of participants (N=121) with high dream recall and an interest in lucid
dreaming. The first goal of the study was to quantify the size and reliability of
the effect of AChEI on lucid dreaming. The second goal was to test the
effectiveness of an integrated lucid dream induction protocol that combined
cholinergic stimulation with other methods, including sleep interruption and
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 23/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 24/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 25/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 26/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 27/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 28/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
The authors reported they were able to induce lucid dreams with a success rate
of 58% with 25 Hz stimulation and 77% with 40 Hz stimulation. However, it is
important to note how lucid dreams were classified in this study: instead of
assessment of lucid dreams with eye signaling, self-report, or through statistical
analysis of judges’ ratings of dream reports, as in Stumbrys et al. (2013b),
dreams were assumed to be lucid if subjects reported “elevated ratings (>mean
+ 2 s.e.) on either or both of the LuCiD scale factors insight and dissociation”.
While dissociation scores (i.e., “seeing oneself from the outside” or a “3rd
person perspective”) have been found to be increased in lucid dreams before
(Voss et al., 2013), dissociation in the sense of adopting a 3rd person
perspective has never been considered a defining feature of lucid dreams per se
(e.g., DeGracia and LaBerge, 2000; Gackenbach and LaBerge, 1988; Green,
1968; LaBerge, 1985, 1990). It is therefore controversial to classify dreams as
lucid based solely on higher ratings of dissociation.
dream contains insight) end of the scale spectrum in absolute values. Overall,
therefore, the results of this study appear to be comparable to the tDCS finding
of Stumbrys et al. (2013b) in that prefrontal tACS gamma band stimulation
induced numerical increases in some measures of dream content, but does not
appear to have lead to increases in the number of lucid dreams when defined in
the traditional sense of being aware of dreaming while dreaming.
Studies of electrical brain stimulation have also thus far only tested a small part
of the parameter space and there are many other stimulation protocols that
appear to be worth evaluating. For example, given the neuroimaging results
reviewed above, synchronous frontoparietal tACS—in which synchrony is
increased between frontal and parietal regions by simultaneous stimulation of
these regions (e.g., Violante et al., 2017)—is another stimulation method that
would be interesting to examine. Finally, as noted above, these methods are
likely to maximize their effect when participants are trained in at least the
minimal mental set for lucid dream induction, which was not done in either the
study by Voss et al. (2014) or Stumbrys et al. (2013b).
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 30/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 31/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
Contrasting left vs. right fist clenches, increased BOLD signal in the
contralateral sensorimotor cortex was observed in both lucid dreams of the
fMRI experiment, as well as in waking and imagination conditions. Compared
to executed hand clenches during wakefulness, however, BOLD signal
increases during dreaming in contralateral sensorimotor cortex were more
localized, which could indicate either weaker activation or more focal
activation of hand areas only. BOLD signal fluctuations during dreaming were
approximately 50% of those observed for executed hand clenches during
wakefulness. Correspondingly, the NIRS data showed increased oxygenated
hemoglobin and decreased deoxygenated hemoglobin in the contralateral
sensorimotor cortex during dreamed hand clenching. This hemodynamic
response was also observed in the supplementary motor area, which is involved
in the timing, monitoring and preparation of movements (Goldberg, 1985).
This differed with the fMRI data in which no significant differences in the
supplementary motor area were found. Interestingly, during dreamed hand
clenching, hemodynamic responses were smaller in the sensorimotor cortex but
of similar amplitude in the supplementary motor area when compared to overt
motor performance during wakefulness.
Overall, these data suggest that the pattern of brain activation observed during
dreamed motor execution overlaps with motor execution during wakefulness.
However, given the different patterns of activation, the data may also suggest
that the interactions between the supplementary motor area, somatosensory and
sensorimotor cortex differs between REM sleep dreaming and waking. The
authors suggest that the reduced activation in sensorimotor cortex could be due
in part to the lack of sensory feedback as a result of REM sleep atonia.
However, given that this study consisted of two case reports—one for each
imaging modality—the data should be interpreted cautiously. Clarification of
the neural correlates of dreamed motor activity, as well as clarification of any
differences in this network compared to overt motor performance, awaits
larger-scale group-level studies. Nevertheless, this experiment provides a
proof-of-concept that neuroimaging of specific dream content can be
accomplished using lucid dreaming as a methodology.
The results revealed that intentional slow tracking of visual motion (of both
circles and lines) during lucid REM sleep dreams results in SPEMs. Pursuit eye
movements in REM sleep dreams did not significantly differ from pursuit
during waking perception, and both were characterized by high pursuit ratios
and low saccade rates. In contrast, tracking in imagination was characterized by
low pursuit with frequent saccadic intrusions. A Bayesian classification model
that included pursuit ratio and saccade rate discriminated both REM sleep
dreaming and perception from imagination with greater than 98% accuracy.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 34/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
(for a review, see Arnulf, 2011). By demonstrating that individuals can trace
circles and lines with their gaze while in EEG-verified REM sleep, which can
be recorded with EOG, the data provide unique evidence that shifts in the
perceived gaze direction in dreams give rise to the appropriate corresponding
eye movements. This is consistent with the view that a subset of eye
movements during REM sleep are linked to the direction of subjective gaze
during dreams. Lastly, the results also provide a novel source of data for a
central research question on the topic of smooth pursuit in humans and non-
human primates that dates back at least forty years. Specifically, an enduring
question has been whether a physical stimulus and/or retinal image motion is
necessary to drive the neural circuitry of smooth pursuit (Spering and
Montagnini, 2011). By demonstrating that sustained SPEMs can be elicited in
the absence of visual input to the cortex, as is the case during REM sleep, the
findings provide strong evidence that neither a physical motion stimulus nor
readout of retinal image motion are necessary for SPEMs.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 35/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
A case study (Zadra and Pihl, 1997), and one small, controlled pilot-study
(Spoormaker and Van Den Bout, 2006) have found that lucid dreaming therapy
was effective in reducing nightmare frequency. Also a study of 32 patients who
suffer from frequent nightmares found a slight advantage of lucid dreaming as
add-on to Gestalt therapy compared to the latter alone (Holzinger et al., 2015).
In contrast, a larger online study did not find any additional effect of lucid
dreaming therapy as an add-on to other cognitive-behavioral techniques, such
as imagery rehearsal therapy (Lancee, Van Den Bout and Spoormaker, 2010),
although low power and high dropout rates (>70%) limited the scope of the
conclusions. Controlled experiments with larger sample sizes are needed to
further evaluate the potential usefulness of lucid dreaming for the treatment of
recurring nightmares and related disorders, as well as to examine the neural
mechanisms of these interactions.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 36/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
the insight into the current dream state, however it is currently unclear if an
increased nightmare frequency causes an increased lucid dreaming frequency
in narcolepsy, or if the strongly increased frequency of REM sleep in
narcolepsy affects nightmares and lucid dreaming independently. In particular
sleep onset REM (SOREM) episodes appear to prompt lucid dreams (Dodet et
al., 2014), which is in line with the dream lucidity-enhancing effects of the
early morning “Wake-Back-to-Bed” procedure in healthy subjects. Of note,
patients with narcolepsy make a clear distinction between their experiences of
lucid dreaming and sleep paralysis (Dodet et al., 2014), and the increased lucid
dreaming frequency in this patient group does not appear to be associated with
medication use (Rak et al., 2014). It has been speculated whether, potentially as
a result of a considerably increased lucid dreaming experience, narcoleptic
patients might exhibit different neurophysiological correlates of lucid dreaming
(Dodet et al., 2014).
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 37/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 38/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
instance, while some experimenters instruct participants to move their eyes left
and right (e.g., Dresler et al., 2012), in other cases participants have been
instructed to “scan the horizon” from left to right (Dodet et al., 2014).
Differences in eye signaling instructions could partly account for the varying
degrees of effectiveness in objectively identifying the eye signals across
studies. For example, Dodet et al. (2014) reported that while three control
participants and twelve narcoleptic patients reported making the eye signal in
overnight EEG recordings, none of these could be objectively identified, and
only about half of reported signals could be unambiguously identified from nap
recordings. While it is possible that some of these instances represent cases
where participants misremembered or misreported a lucid dream, it is likely
that this high rate of ambiguous eye movement signals is partly attributable to
the specific instructions that were given. Similarly, while the exact instructions
provided to participants was not described, Voss et al. (2009) stated that they
were unable to obtain reliable eye signals using the standard signaling method
and therefore attempted to develop a novel signaling method employing two
sets of eye-signals separated by a pause. Again, suboptimal results could
plausibly be due to the instructions provided and the way the eye signals were
executed by participants in the study.
One reason for the confusion and diverse instructions given for the eye
signaling methodology is that a standardized set of instructions for making the
signals has never been published. Here we report a simple set of instructions
adapted from LaBerge et al. (2018a) that has been reported to yield nearly
100% correspondence between subjective reports of eye signals and objective
EOG recordings. The instruction is as follows:
When making an eye movement signal, we would like you to look all the
way to the left then all the way to the right two times consecutively, as if
you are looking at each of your ears. Specifically, we would like you to
look at your left ear, then your right ear, then your left ear, then your right
ear, and then finally back to center. Make the eye movements without
moving your head, and make the full left-right-left-right-center motion as
one rapid continuous movement without pausing.
opportunity to view the signals and receive feedback from the experimenter.
Additionally, participants practice making the eye signals in any lucid dreams
they have at home in the weeks leading up to the sleep laboratory visit, in order
to gain experience with executing the signals.
All three questionnaires provide measures for evaluating how some content
dimensions might differ across lucid and non-lucid dreams. However, as
mentioned, questionnaires such as these are not sufficient to objectively
establish whether a participant had a lucid dream. It is worth noting that only
the DLQ in its first question (“I was aware that I was dreaming”) directly
queries whether the dreamer was lucid, and the MACE was never intended for
this purpose. Most studies using the DLQ and LuCiD questionnaires have
collected a dream report from participants in addition to the questionnaire
responses. We would like to emphasize the importance of this point: Instead of
relying on the inference of lucidity solely from questionnaire measures of
dream content, accurate assessment of lucidity can be facilitated by, in addition
to using the eye signaling method, collecting a full dream report from
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 40/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
A limitation of the above methods is that they do not measure variation in the
length or degree of lucid dreams. Indeed, lucid dreams can range from a mere
fleeting thought about the fact that one is dreaming followed by an immediate
loss of lucidity or awakening, to extended lucid dreams in which an individual
is able to engage in sequences of actions (e.g., LaBerge and Rheingold, 1990).
Distinguishing between these different “levels” or subtypes of lucid dreams
will likely be valuable to understanding observed differences (or lack of
differences) in brain structural or functional measures associated with lucid
dream frequency. Along these lines, several recent questionnaires have taken
first steps to measure individual differences in specific characteristics of lucid
dreaming. For example, the Lucid Dreaming Skills Questionnaire (LUSK)
measures participants’ frequency of several different but inter-related aspects
of awareness and control in lucid dreams (Schredl, Rieger and Göritz, 2018).
Another questionnaire, the Frequency and Intensity Lucid Dreaming
Questionnaire (FILD) queries participants regarding the duration of their lucid
dreams and various aspects of dream control, as well as the frequency with
which they deliberately attempt to induce lucid dreams (Aviram and Soffer-
Dudek, 2018). In addition to providing data for individual differences studies,
such questionnaires could also potentially be useful in selecting participants for
sleep laboratory experiments of lucid dreaming, for example by selecting
participants who report high levels of dream control in addition to frequent
lucid dreams (in order to select participants who are more likely to be able to
effectively make the eye signals or engage in experimental tasks during lucid
dreams).
For all the above methods, important steps need to be taken to minimize
measurement error, particularly to ensure that participants have a clear
understanding of the meaning of lucid dreaming (Snyder and Gackenbach,
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 42/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
10. Conclusion
Despite having been physiologically validated for approximately four decades,
the neurobiology of lucid dreaming is still incompletely characterized. Most
studies conducted to date have relied on small sample sizes, which limits the
generalizability of the findings. Not surprisingly, the results of such
underpowered studies are not consistent: almost every EEG study reports
changes in spectral power in a different frequency band or brain area.
Neuroimaging data on lucid dreaming is even sparser. Currently, there is only
one fMRI study contrasting lucid and non-lucid REM sleep and it is a case
study. Nevertheless, the results of this study converge with MRI studies that
have evaluated individual differences in lucid dreaming frequency. Together,
this preliminary evidence suggests that regions of anterior prefrontal, parietal
and temporal cortex are involved in lucid dreaming. The involvement of these
brain regions in metacognitive processes during the waking state is also in line
with these findings.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 44/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
might lead to new strategies for lucid dream induction, together with continued
research on stimulating lucid dreams with visual or auditory cues (LaBerge and
Levitan, 1995; LaBerge et al., 1988; LaBerge et al., 1981b).
In conclusion, additional studies with larger sample sizes, for example large-
scale group-level high-density EEG, MEG, and concurrent EEG/fMRI studies,
will be important next steps toward characterizing the neural functional
changes associated with lucid dreaming. For now, a more detailed
understanding of the neurobiological basis of lucid dreaming remains an open
question for ongoing research. Lucid dreaming shows promise as a useful
methodology for psychophysiological studies of REM sleep, with potential
applications in both clinical and basic research domains. Perhaps the largest
potential of research on lucid dreaming is that it provides a unique method to
investigate the neurobiology of consciousness, which remains one of the largest
lacunas in scientific knowledge.
Highlights
Acknowledgements
We thank Stephen LaBerge for helpful discussion.
Funding
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been
accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early
version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and
review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note
that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the
content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Competing interests
References
21. Braun AR, Balkin TJ, Wesensten NJ, Gwadry F, Carson RE, Varga M,
Baldwin P, Belenky G, Herscovitch P, 1998. Dissociated pattern of
activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid eye
movement sleep. Science
279, 91–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
22. Brylowski A, Levitan L, LaBerge S, 1989. H-reflex suppression and
autonomic activation during lucid REM sleep: A case study. Sleep
12,
374–378. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
23. Cavallero C, Cicogna P, Natale V, Occhionero M, Zito A, 1992. Slow
wave sleep dreaming. Sleep
15, 562–566. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
24. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR, 2006. The precuneus: A review of its
functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain
129, 564–583.
[PubMed] [Google Scholar]
25. Christoff K, Ream JM, Geddes L, Gabrieli JD, 2003. Evaluating self-
generated information: Anterior prefrontal contributions to human
cognition. Behav. Neurosci
117, 1161–1168. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
26. Cuadra G, Summers K, Giacobini E, 1994. Cholinesterase inhibitor effects
on neurotransmitters in rat cortex in vivo. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther
270,
277–284. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
27. Dane J, Van de Caslte R, 1984. A comparison of waking instruction and
posthypnotic suggestion for lucid dream induction. Lucidity Letter
3.
[Google Scholar]
28. David AS, 1990. Insight and psychosis. The British Journal of Psychiatry
156, 798–808. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
29. DeGracia DJ, LaBerge S, 2000. Varieties of lucid dreaming experience,
in: Kunzendorf RG, Wallace B (Eds.), Individual differences in conscious
experience. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp. 269–307. [Google Scholar]
30. Dement W, Wolpert EA, 1958. The relation of eye movements, body
motility, and external stimuli to dream content. J. Exp. Psychol
55, 543–
552. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
31. Dixon ML, De La Vega A, Mills C, Andrews-Hanna J, Spreng RN, Cole
MW, Christoff K, 2018. Heterogeneity within the frontoparietal control
network and its relationship to the default and dorsal attention networks.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci, E1598–E1607. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[Google Scholar]
32. Dodet P, Chavez M, Leu-Semenescu S, Golmard J, Arnulf I, 2014. Lucid
dreaming in narcolepsy. Sleep
38, 487–497. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[Google Scholar]
33. Donner TH, Siegel M, 2011. A framework for local cortical oscillation
patterns. Trends Cog. Sci
15, 191–199. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
34. Dresler M, Eibl L, Fischer CF, Wehrle R, Spoormaker VI, Steiger A,
Czisch M, Pawlowski M, 2014. Volitional components of consciousness
vary across wakefulness, dreaming and lucid dreaming. Front. Psychol
4,
987. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 48/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 51/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
76. LaBerge S, 1988a. Lucid dreaming in western literature, in: LaBerge JGS
(Ed.), Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain. Plenum Press, New York, NY, pp.
11–26. [Google Scholar]
77. LaBerge S, 1988b. The psychophysiology of lucid dreaming, in:
Gackenbach J, LaBerge S (Eds.), Conscious mind, sleeping brain:
Perspectives on lucid dreaming. Plenum Press, New York, NY, pp. 135–
153. [Google Scholar]
78. LaBerge S, 1990. Lucid dreaming: Psychophysiological studies of
consciousness during REM sleep, in: Bootzin RR, Kihlstrom JF, Schacter
DL (Eds.), Sleep and Cognition. American Psychological Association,
Washington DC, pp. 109–126. [Google Scholar]
79. LaBerge S, 2001. The paradox and promise of lucid dreaming
Research
update: Cholinergic stimulation of lucid dreaming; voluntary control of
auditory perception during REM lucid dreams, International Association
for the Study of Dreams, Berkeley. [Google Scholar]
80. LaBerge S, 2003. Lucid dreaming and the yoga of the dream state: A
psychophysiological perspective, in: Wallace BA (Ed.), Buddhism &
science: Breaking new ground. Columbia University Press, New York,
NY, pp. 233–258. [Google Scholar]
81. LaBerge S, 2010. Signal-verfied lucid dreaming proves that REM sleep
can support reflective consciousness. Int. J. Dream Res
3, 26–27.
[Google Scholar]
82. LaBerge S, 2015. Lucid dreaming: Metaconsciousness during paradoxical
sleep, in: Glucksman MKM (Ed.), Dream research: Contributions to
clinical practice. Routledge, New York, NY, pp. 198–214.
[Google Scholar]
83. LaBerge S, Baird B, Zimbardo PG, 2018a. Smooth tracking of visual
targets distinguishes lucid REM sleep dreaming and waking perception
from imagination. Nat. Comm
9, 3298. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[Google Scholar]
84. LaBerge S, Dement WC, 1982. Voluntary control of respiration during
REM sleep. Sleep Res. 11, 107. [Google Scholar]
85. LaBerge S, Greenleaf W, Kedzierski B, 1983. Physiological responses to
dreamed sexual activity during lucid REM sleep. Psychophysiology, 454–
455. [Google Scholar]
86. LaBerge S, LaMarca K, Baird B, 2018b. Pre-sleep treatment with
galantamine stimulates lucid dreaming: A double-blind, placebo-
controlled, crossover study. PLoS ONE
13, e0201246. [PMC free article]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar]
87. LaBerge S, Levitan L, 1995. Validity established of DreamLight cues for
eliciting lucid dreaming. Dreaming
5, 159–168. [Google Scholar]
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 52/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 53/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 54/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 55/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 56/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 57/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
151. Schredl M, Rieger J, Göritz AS, 2018. Measuring lucid dreaming skills: A
new questionnaire (LUSK). Int. J. Dream Res [Google Scholar]
152. Siclari F, Baird B, Perogamvros L, Bernardi G, LaRocque JJ, Riedner B,
Boly M, Postle BR, Tononi G, 2017. The neural correlates of dreaming.
Nat. Neurosci
20, 872–878. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
[Google Scholar]
153. Siclari F, LaRocque JJ, Postle BR, Tononi G, 2013. Assessing sleep
consciousness within subjects using a serial awakening paradigm. Front.
Psychol
4. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
154. Snyder TJ, Gackenbach J, 1988. Individual differences associated with
lucid dreaming, in: LaBerge S, Gackenbach J (Eds.), Conscious mind,
sleeping brain. Plenum, New York, NY, pp. 221–259. [Google Scholar]
155. Sparrow G, Hurd R, Carlson R, Molina A, 2018. Exploring the effects of
galantamine paired with meditation and dream reliving on recalled
dreams: Toward an integrated protocol for lucid dream induction and
nightmare resolution. Conscious. Cogn
63, 74–88. [PubMed]
[Google Scholar]
156. Sparrow GS, Thurston M, Carlson R, 2013. Dream reliving and
meditation as a way to enhance reflectiveness and constructive
engagement in dreams. Int. J. Dream Res
6, 84–93. [Google Scholar]
157. Spering M, Montagnini A, 2011. Do we track what we see? Common
versus independent processing for motion perception and smooth pursuit
eye movements: A review. Vision Res. 51, 836–852. [PubMed]
[Google Scholar]
158. Spoormaker VI, Czisch M, Dresler M, 2010. Lucid and non-lucid
dreaming: Thinking in networks. Int. J. Dream Res
3, 49–51.
[Google Scholar]
159. Spoormaker VI, Van Den Bout J, 2006. Lucid dreaming treatment for
nightmares: A pilot study. Psychother. Psychosom
75, 389–394. [PubMed]
[Google Scholar]
160. Steriade M, Timofeev I, Grenier F, 2001. Natural waking and sleep states:
A view from inside neocortical neurons. J. Neurophysiol
85, 1969–1985.
[PubMed] [Google Scholar]
161. Stickgold R, Malia A, Fosse R, Hobson JA, 2001. Brain-mind states: I.
Longitudinal field study of sleep/wake factors influencing mentation
report length. Sleep
24, 171–179. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
162. Stumbrys T, Erlacher D, 2012. Lucid dreaming during NREM sleep: Two
case reports. Int. J. Dream Res
5, 151–155. [Google Scholar]
163. Stumbrys T, Erlacher D, Malinowski P, 2015. Meta-awareness during day
and night: The relationship between mindfulness and lucid dreaming.
Imagin. Cogn. Pers
34, 415–433. [Google Scholar]
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 58/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 59/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
178. Vogel GW, 1975. A review of REM sleep deprivation. Arch. Gen.
Psychiatry
32, 749–761. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
179. Voss U, Holzmann R, Hobson A, Paulus W, Koppehele-Gossel J, Klimke
A, Nitsche MA, 2014. Induction of self awareness in dreams through
frontal low current stimulation of gamma activity. Nat. Neurosci
17, 810–
812. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
180. Voss U, Holzmann R, Tuin I, Hobson JA, 2009. Lucid dreaming: A state
of consciousness with features of both waking and non-lucid dreaming.
Sleep
32, 1191–1200. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
181. Voss U, Schermelleh-Engel K, Windt J, Frenzel C, Hobson A, 2013.
Measuring consciousness in dreams: The lucidity and consciousness in
dreams scale. Conscious. Cogn
22, 8–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
182. Wagner AD, Shannon BJ, Kahn I, Buckner RL, 2005. Parietal lobe
contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cog. Sci
9, 445–453.
[PubMed] [Google Scholar]
183. Wallace BA, 2013. Buddhism and science: Breaking new ground.
Columbia University Press. [Google Scholar]
184. Wallace BA, Hodel B, 2012. Dreaming yourself awake: Lucid dreaming
and Tibetan dream yoga for insight and transformation. Shambhala
Publications, Boston, MA. [Google Scholar]
185. Windt JM, 2010. The immersive spatiotemporal hallucination model of
dreaming. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci
9, 295–316. [Google Scholar]
186. Windt JM, 2015. Dreaming: A conceptual framework for philosophy of
mind and empirical research. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
[Google Scholar]
187. Windt JM, Metzinger T, 2007. The philosophy of dreaming and self-
consciousness: What happens to the experiential subject during the dream
state?, in: Barrett D, McNamara P (Eds.), The new science of dreaming:
Cultural and theoretical perspectives. Praeger; , Westport, CT, pp. 193–
247. [Google Scholar]
188. Windt JM, Nielsen T, Thompson E, 2016. Does consciousness disappear
in dreamless sleep? Trends Cog. Sci
20, 871–882. [PubMed]
[Google Scholar]
189. Yeo B, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead
M, Roffman JL, Smoller JW, Zöllei L, Polimeni JR, 2011. The
organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional
connectivity. J. Neurophysiol
106, 1125–1165. [PMC free article]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar]
190. Yuschak T, 2006. Advanced lucid dreaming: The power of supplements.
Lulu Enterprises, Raleigh, NC. [Google Scholar]
191. Yuval-Greenberg S, Deouell LY, 2009. The broadband-transient induced
gamma-band response in scalp EEG reflects the execution of saccades.
Brain Topogr. 22, 3–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 60/61
18/7/2021 The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6451677/ 61/61