2013 Ardila Development of Metacognitive and Emotional Executive Function in Children1
2013 Ardila Development of Metacognitive and Emotional Executive Function in Children1
181]
On: 06 February 2013, At: 03:59
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
To cite this article: Alfredo Ardila (2013): Development of Metacognitive and Emotional Executive Functions in Children,
Applied Neuropsychology: Child, DOI:10.1080/21622965.2013.748388
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to
anyone is expressly forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents
will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should
be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims,
proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in
connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD, 0: 1–6, 2013
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 2162-2965 print/2162-2973 online
DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2013.748388
It has been proposed that two major components of executive functions can be
distinguished: (1) one related to complex cognition (metacognition, such as planning,
problem solving, etc.); (2) the other related to coordinating and controlling emotional
Downloaded by [65.9.206.181] at 03:59 06 February 2013
behavior. Contemporary neuroimaging techniques have demonstrated that there are two
distinct functional-anatomical networks within the prefrontal cortex: one associated with
cognitive control and the other associated with value based decision making—each
related to specific frontal-lobe areas. Metacognitive (but not emotional) executive
functions have been demonstrated to be correlated with general intellectual level
(intelligence). Research has shown that emotional executive functions (such as attention
control) develop earlier in life (during the 1st year), before the development of
metacognitive executive functions (such as planning and verbal fluency), which develop
around the age of 3 and are correlated with the development of a grammatical language.
different components. As a matter of fact, most of the such as solving the Tower of Hanoi (Fincham, Carter,
disturbances reported in Phineas Gage (and in many van Veen, Stenger, & Anderson, 2002), the Controlled
cases of prefrontal syndromes) refer to behavioral/emo- Word Association Test (letter fluency; Baldo, Schwartz,
tional disturbances, or more exactly, disturbances in Wilkins, & Dronkers, 2006), working memory (Yoon,
coordinating cognition and emotion/motivation. Harlow Hoffman, & D’Esposito, 2007), and solving the Wisconsin
(1848, 1868) described severe emotional changes in Card-Sorting Test (Lie, Specht, Marshall, & Fink, 2006).
Phineas Gage but emphasized that no intellectual impair-
ments were observed; he clearly pointed out that neither
Emotional/Motivational Executive Functions
his memory nor his intelligence were compromised by the
brain lesion. Harlow (1848) stated that the “equilibrium Emotional/motivational executive functions are responsi-
or balances so to speak, between his intellectual faculties ble for coordinating cognition and emotion, or the ability
and animal propensities seem to have been destroyed. He to fulfill basic impulses following socially acceptable
is fitful, irreverent, indulging in gross profanity (which strategies. The ventromedial areas of the prefrontal
was not previously his custom), manifesting little or no cortex are involved in the expression and control of emo-
deference for his fellows, and impatient of restraint or tional and instinctual behaviors (Fuster, 1997, 2002).
advice when it conflicts with his desires” (p. 389). It should This function is related with so-called “inhibitory con-
be emphasized that the frontal damage in Phineas Gage trol” of behavior (Miller & Wang, 2006). Clinical evi-
involved the orbital and medial aspects, but not the pre- dence (e.g., Luria, 1969; Stuss & Knight, 2002) and
frontal dorsolateral area (Damasio, Grabowski, Frank, experimental research (e.g., Leung & Cai, 2007; Medalla,
Downloaded by [65.9.206.181] at 03:59 06 February 2013
Galaburda, & Damasio, 1994; Ratiu & Talos, 2004; see Lera, Feinberg, & Barbas, 2007) suggest that the neural
Figure 1). substrate for this inhibitory function resides mainly in the
Consequently, it can be assumed that there are two dif- medial and orbital portions of the prefrontal cortex.
ferent, but closely related types of executive function These two types of executive functions (“metacogni-
(e.g., Ardila, 2008; Fuster, 2002; Happaney, Zelazo, & tive” and “emotional/ motivational”) depend on rela-
Stuss, 2004). tively different prefrontal areas, and as a matter of fact,
two major variants in the prefrontal syndrome are fre-
quently distinguished, one mostly impairing cognition
Metacognitive Executive Functions
(or rather cognitive control—that is, “metacognition”)
Metacognitive executive functions include problem solv- and the other mostly impairing behavior.
ing, abstracting, planning, strategy development and
implementation, and working memory—abilities mostly Dorsolateral Syndrome. Cummings (1993) indicated
related to the dorsolateral area of the prefrontal cortex that the dorsolateral circuit is the most important in exec-
(e.g., Stuss & Knight, 2002). The dorsolateral prefrontal utive functioning. Cortical–subcortical connections have
cortex has been observed to participate in diverse plan- been suggested (Koziol & Budding, 2009). The most
ning, abstracting, problem-solving, and working-memory noted deficit is an inability to organize a behavioral
tasks. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, dor- response to novel or complex stimuli. Symptoms are on a
solateral prefrontal activation has been found in tasks continuum and reflect the capacity to shift cognitive sets,
engage existing strategies, and organize information to
meet changing environmental demands. Various research-
ers, including Luria (1969), have noted perseveration,
stimulus-bound behavior, echopraxia, and echolalia.
Lateralization has been noted in executive dysfunction
(Goldberg, 2001). The ventral and dorsal portions of the
prefrontal cortex are believed to interact in the mainte-
nance of rational and “nonrisky” decision making (Manes
et al., 2002). According to Fuster (1997, 2002), the most
general executive function of the lateral prefrontal cortex
is the temporal organization of goal-directed actions in
the domains of behavior, cognition, and language.
patients are unable to respond to social cues. Of note, it of age. This means that the brain maturation and the
was observed by Laiacona and colleagues (1989) that associated cognitive abilities do not grow in a steady and
these patients have no difficulty with card-sorting tasks. continuous way, but there are certain periods when prog-
Eslinger and Damasio (1985) coined the term “acquired ress is more notorious.
sociopathy” to describe deregulation that couples both Regardless of the difficulties in measuring executive
lack of insight and remorse regarding these behaviors. functions in children (Hughes & Graham, 2002), several
The orbitofrontal cortex appears to be linked studies have analyzed the early development of executive
predominantly to limbic and basal forebrain sites. Medial functions. Some executive functions that have been ana-
frontal-lobe damage causes apathy or abulia (a severe lyzed through ontogeny are: attentional control, plan-
form of apathy). Acute bilateral lesions in the medial- ning, set shifting, and verbal fluency (see Jurado &
frontal area can cause akinetic mutism, in which the Rosselli, 2007, for a review). Jurado and Rosselli empha-
individual is awake and has self-awareness but does not size that the first executive function to emerge, by the
initiate behaviors (Ross & Stewart, 1981). According to child’s 1st year, is the ability to inhibit overlearned behav-
Fuster (1997, 2002), the ventromedial areas of the ior, resulting in increased attentional control; however, its
prefrontal cortex are involved in expression and control greatest development is some time later (around 6 to 10
of emotional and instinctual behaviors. years of age). Other executive abilities, for instance plan-
It is evident that the two prefrontal syndromes can ning and set shifting, develop later, around age 3 with sig-
have rather different clinical expressions (metacognitive nificant improvement after age 7. Verbal fluency (a lan-
and emotional/motivational) depending upon the specific guage-mediated executive function) is the last to emerge
Downloaded by [65.9.206.181] at 03:59 06 February 2013
location of the damage. For Phineas Gage, the distur- and is significantly influenced by environmental factors.
bance was observed in controlling emotions and instinc- All executive functions, nonetheless, continue to improve
tual behaviors; it was associated with damage at the orbi- until adolescence.
tofrontal and medial-frontal areas. The dorsolateral Hongwanishkul et al. (2005) assessed different execu-
prefrontal cortex was not damaged, and cognition was tive functions in 3- to 5-year-old children using two puta-
unimpaired. tive measures of cool (metacognitive) executive functions
There is robust evidence to assume that two different (Self-Ordered Pointing and Dimensional Change Card
prefrontal systems support these two executive function Sort) and two putative measures of hot (emotional) exec-
subtypes. For instance, Gläscher et al. (2012) used voxel- utive functions (Children’s Gambling Task and Delay of
based lesion symptom mapping (i.e., method and soft- Gratification). Findings confirmed that performance on
ware for analyzing relationships between behavioral defi- both types of tasks develops during the preschool period.
cits in neurological populations and lesion sites associated However, the measures of hot (emotional) and cool
with those deficits) in 344 individuals with focal brain (metacognitive) executive functions showed different pat-
lesions, including 165 involving prefrontal pathology; a terns of relations to each other and to measures of gen-
comprehensive neuropsychological test battery was eral intellectual function and temperament: Whereas cool
administered to all participants. It was demonstrated that (metacognitive) executive functions significantly corre-
there are two distinct functional-anatomical networks lated with general intellectual ability (“intelligence”), hot
within the prefrontal cortex: (1) one associated with cog- (emotional) executive functions were not related to gen-
nitive control (that is, “metacognitive executive func- eral intellectual functioning (verbal mental age and per-
tions,” including response inhibition, conflict monitor- formance mental age).
ing, and switching), and supported by the dorsolateral Matute et al. (2008) analyzed the influence of age on
prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex; and (2) a the performance of a planning and organization task
second functional-anatomical network associated with (two skills included in the metacognitive executive func-
value-based decision making (“emotional/motivational tions), using the so-called “Mexican pyramid” (a test
executive functions”), which included the orbitofrontal, similar in principle to the Tower of London Test; Shallice,
ventromedial, and frontopolar cortex. 1982). Its purpose was to measure problem-solving abil-
ity, and it was taken from the Evaluación Neuropsicológica
Infantil (Child Neuropsychological Assessment [ENI];
EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS IN CHILDREN Matute, Rosselli, Ardila, & Ostrosky-Solís, 2007); 239
school-age children from México and Colombia, distrib-
Executive functions rapidly develop through childhood; uted into six age groups, were selected. Five measures
this development is clearly correlated with the maturation were analyzed: number of correct designs, number of
of the frontal lobes (Anderson, Anderson, Northam, movements per design, execution time in the correct
Jacobs, & Catroppa, 2001). Different growth periods have designs, number of correct designs built with the mini-
been proposed: the period from birth to 2 years of age; mum of movements, and its execution time. A significant
from 7 to 9 years of age; and a finally, from 16 to 19 years effect of age on the five measures was found. Comparison
4 ARDILA
Downloaded by [65.9.206.181] at 03:59 06 February 2013
FIGURE 2 Scores in four selected executive tests (1 = 5 to 6 years; 2 = 7 to 8 years; 3 = 9 to 10 years; 4 = 11 to 12 years; 5 = 13 to 14 years; adapted
from Ardila et al., 2005).
between groups showed that younger children (5 to 6 children were requested to find commonalities between
years old) had a smaller number of correct designs, while pairs of words (concept formation). Eight pairs of
children aged 5 to 8 years old needed more movements. words were presented, one at a time. Abstract general-
Execution time showed a greater number of differences izations or categorizations (i.e., “A dog and a cat are
between groups. According to the authors, the results animals”) were scored with 2 points; specific and
supported the notion that the development of planning descriptive answers (i.e., “A dog and the cat have four
and organization is fast in the 1st years of school age and legs”) were scored with 1 point, and incorrect answers
slows down in adolescence. received a 0. The maximum score was 16. (4) Matrices
Ardila, Rosselli, Matute, and Guajardo (2005) was a multiple-choice subtest consisting of a series of
reported the developmental pattern of different execu- visual pattern- matching and analogy problems pictured
tive functions between the ages of 5 to 6 years old and in nonrepresentational designs. It required the child to
13 to 14 years old, using 315 participants taken from conceptualize spatial and design relations such as in
public schools (156 boys and 159 girls) and divided into Raven’s Progressive Matrices (Raven, Court, & Raven,
five age ranges (5 to 6 years old, 7 to 8 years old, 9 to 10 1976). One point is given to each option correctly
years old, 11 to 12 years old, and 13 to 14 years old). selected by the child. The maximum score is 8. Figure 2
The scores in several executive function subtests taken presents the development of the scores in these four
for the ENI (Matute et al., 2007) were analyzed. The selected executive function tests.
results in four selected metacognitive executive func- Improvement between the first (5 to 6 years old) and
tions are presented here: (1) For Semantic Verbal the last age range (13 to 14 years old) was observed in the
Fluency, children were instructed to name all animals four tests, but it was highest for the Phonemic Verbal
they could in 1 minute. The score was the total number Fluency Test (the score among the oldest children was
of animals named correctly. (2) For Phonemic Verbal about 7 times greater than among the younger children),
Fluency, children were instructed to say as many words whereas in the other three tests, the score in the oldest
starting with M as they could in 1 minute. The score was group was only about 3 times greater than in the younger
the total number of correct words. (3) For Similarities, group.
DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 5
tional executive functions) are correlated with the Fuster, J. M. (2002). Frontal lobe and cognitive development. Journal of
child’s general intellectual level (intelligence). Neurocytology, 31, 373–385.
4. Contemporary research has shown that emotional Gläscher, J., Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Bechara, A., Rudrauf, D.,
Calamia, M., . . . Tranel, D. (2012). Lesion mapping of cognitive con-
executive functions (such as attentional control)
trol and value-based decision making in the prefrontal cortex. Pro-
developed earlier in life (during the 1st year), ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
before the development of metacognitive execu- America, 109(36), 14681–14686.
tive functions (such as planning and verbal flu- Goldberg, E. (2001). The executive brain. New York, NY: Oxford Uni-
ency), which develop around the age of 3, corre- versity Press.
Happaney, K., Zelazo, P. D., & Stuss, D. T. (2004). Development of
late with the development of grammatical
orbitofrontal function: Current themes and future directions. Brain
language (Ardila, 2009). A similar sequence has and Cognition, 55, 1–10.
been suggested for the historical evolution of Harlow, J. M. (1848). Passage of an iron rod through the head. Boston
executive functions (see Ardila, 2008, for a review). Medical and Surgical Journal, 39, 389–393.
In this regard, ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny. Harlow, J. M. (1868). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through
the head. Publications of the Massachusetts Medical Society, 2, 327–347.
Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S. C., & Zelazo, P. D.
(2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young chil-
REFERENCES dren: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 28, 617–644.
Anderson, V., Anderson, P., Northam, E., Jacobs, R., & Catroppa, C. Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in
(2001). Development of executive functions through late childhood childhood: Problems and solutions? Child and Adolescent Mental
and adolescence in an Australian sample. Developmental Neuropsy- Health, 7(3), 131–142.
chology, 20(1), 385–406. Jurado, M. B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive
Ardila, A. (2008). On the evolutionary origins of executive functions. functions: A review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology
Brain and Cognition, 68, 92–99. Review, 17, 213–233.
Ardila, A. (2009). Origins of the language: Correlation between brain Koziol, L. F., & Budding, D. (2009). Subcortical structures and cogni-
evolution and language development. In S. M. Platek & T. K. tion: Implications for neuropsychological assessment. New York, NY:
Shackelford (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary cognitive neuroscience Springer Publications.
(pp. 153–174). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Laiacona, M., De Santis, A., Barbarotto, R., Basso, A., Spagnoli, D., &
Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., & Guajardo, S. (2005). The influ- Capitani, E. (1989). Neuropsychological follow-up of patients oper-
ence of the parents’ educational level and development of executive ated for aneurysms of anterior communicating artery. Cortex, 25,
functions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(1), 539–560. 261–273.
Baldo, J. V., Schwartz, S., Wilkins, D., & Dronkers, N. F. (2006). Role Leung, H. C., & Cai, W. (2007). Common and differential ventrolat-
of frontal versus temporal cortex in verbal fluency as revealed by eral prefrontal activity during inhibition of hand and eye movements.
voxel-based lesion symptom mapping. Journal of the International Journal of Neurosciences, 27, 9893–9900.
Neuropsychological Society, 12, 896–900. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., & Hannay, J. (2004).
Barbas, H. (2006). Organization of the principal pathways of prefrontal Neuropsychological assessment. New York, NY: Oxford.
lateral, medial, and orbitofrontal cortices primates and implications Lie, C. H., Specht, K., Marshall, J. C., & Fink, G. R. (2006). Using
for their collaborative interaction in executive functions. In J. Risberg fMRI to decompose the neural processes underlying the Wisconsin
& J. Grafman (Eds.), The frontal lobes. Development, function and Card Sorting Test. Neuroimage, 30, 1038–1049.
pathology (pp. 21–68). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Luria, A. R. (1969). Frontal lobe syndromes. In P. J. Vinken & G. W.
Beer, J. S., John, O. P., Scabini, D., & Knight, R. T. (2006). Orbito- Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology (Vol. 2, pp. 725–757).
frontal cortex and social behavior: Integrating self-monitoring and Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North Holland.
6 ARDILA
Manes, F., Sahakian, B., Clark, L., Rogers, R., Antoun, N., Aitken, M., positive and negative mood on executive functions. Neuropsycholo-
& Robbins T. (2002). Decision-making processes following damage to gia, 45, 617–629.
the prefrontal cortex. Brain, 125, 624–639. Ratiu, P., & Talos, I. F. (2004). The tale of Phineas Gage, digitally
Matute, E., Ardila, A., Rosselli, O., & Ostrosky-Solís, F. (2007). Evaluación remastered. New England Journal of Medicine, 351, e21.
neuropsicológica infantil (ENI) [Child neuropsychology assessment]. Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1976). Manual for Raven’s Pro-
México: Manual Moderno/UNAM/Universidad de Guadalajara. gressive Matrices. London, UK: Lewis.
Matute, E., Chamorro, Y., Inozemtseva, O., Barrios, O., Rosselli, M., Ross, E. D., & Stewart, R. M. (1981). Akinetic mutism from hypotha-
& Ardila, A. (2008). Efecto de la edad en una tarea de planeación y lamic damage: Successful treatment with dopamine agonists. Neurol-
organización (‘Pirámide de México’) en escolares [The effect of age ogy, 31, 1435–1439.
in a planning and arranging task (‘Mexican Pyramid’) among school Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophi-
children]. Revista de Neurología, 47(2), 61–70. cal Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 298,
Medalla, M., Lera, P., Feinberg, M., & Barbas, H. (2007). Specificity in 199–209.
inhibitory systems associated with prefrontal pathways to temporal Stuss, D. T., & Knight, R. T. (2002). Principles of frontal lobe function.
cortex in primates. Cerebral Cortex, 17(Suppl. 1), i136–i50. New York NY: Oxford University Press.
Miller, P., & Wang, X. J. (2006). Inhibitory control by an integral feed- Yoon, J. H., Hoffman, J. N., & D’Esposito, M. (2007). Segregation of
back signal in prefrontal cortex: A model of discrimination between function in the lateral prefrontal cortex during visual object working
sequential stimuli. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences memory. Brain Research, 1184, 217–225.
USA, 103, 201–206. Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and
Mitchell, R. L., & Phillips, L. H. (2007). The psychological, neuro- atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Handbook of childhood
chemical and functional neuroanatomical mediators of the effects of cognitive development (pp. 445–469). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Downloaded by [65.9.206.181] at 03:59 06 February 2013