Assessing Children Learning
Assessing Children Learning
Children’s
Learning
Second Edition
www.fultonpublishers.co.uk
Note: The right of the author to be identified as the author of this work has been
asserted by him in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
ISBN 1–84312–040–2
Acknowledgements iv
6 Understanding Ourselves 92
Conclusion 175
Bibliography 197
iii
Acknowledgements
I am very grateful to the teachers who generously allowed me to quote from
their observations and case studies:
Jenny Colls Shelagh MacDonald
Maggie Ellis Jennifer Pozzani
Geoff Fisher Margaret Prosser
Sheila Gapp Maxine Purdy
Ann Lawson Mary Rosenberg
Ann Le Gassick Michael Tennant
Thank you for helping me to learn about children’s learning.
CHAPTER 1
1
Assessing Children’s Learning
crosses it out tidily. He places his answers on the line or in the box as
instructed, though he often adds some more digits in other empty spaces,
as if he interpreted a space as an invitation to write. (See questions 19
and 22 in figure 1.1.) He has learned to copy numbers and letters neatly
and accurately, even though this is not what is being asked of him. (See
questions 5 and 6 shown in figure 1.2.)
Figure 1.1
2
Learning from Jason
Figure 1.2
Figure 1.3
3
Assessing Children’s Learning
Figure 1.4
weight of the parcel on the left, if the scales balance (figure 1.4). He has
responded by writing four digits in the space provided. I am certain that
he has not calculated – or miscalculated – that 9182g + 50g = 500g, or that
500g – 50g = 9182g. He has simply written some numbers in the
appropriate place for an answer. He uses the same approach in question
7, question 9, and question 17 (figure 1.5).
Figure 1.5
4
Learning from Jason
Question 7 is a simple division problem; question 9 asks how much
more money is needed to buy the football, and in question 17, the pupils
are required to give the age of a child born on 8 April 1970 on 8 April
1983. Another child in Jason’s class interpreted this question as a
subtraction problem; working from left to right he seems to have
thought to himself: ‘8 from 8 is 0, put down 0); April from April is 0, put
down 0; 1983 from 1970’ – here the answer trails away as the pupil
realises he cannot complete the problem.
Jason frequently writes the number 8, and it is tempting to speculate
about the reason for this. (See questions 21, 23 for example.) Is it a
satisfying number for him to write? Has he recently mastered the art of
forming it with a single stroke? Or is it possible that he has noticed the
front cover of his test booklet (figure 1.7), and that he has seized on this
bold and impressive numeral as a possible clue to what is being asked of
him?
Figure 1.6
Figure 1.7
5
Assessing Children’s Learning
Figure 1.8
But sometimes, it seems, there may be other reasons for his responses;
in question 28, for example. It seems just possible here that Jason has
read off the length of the rod on the ruler as nearly 8, and interpreted the
half-centimetre mark on the ruler as figure 1. How long is the rod? It
could be that Jason said to himself ‘8, and back a bit to this 1 here. 8.
Eight. One.’ Why has he written it twice? To fill this allocated space?
More convincing evidence that Jason attends to at least part of the
content of the question, as he sees it (not as the tester sees it), can be seen
6
Learning from Jason
Figure 1.9
in question 34 (figure 1.10). What time does this clock show? Did Jason
reflect ‘There is the 9, there is the 1; perhaps an 8 for good measure . . . ’?
It is at least a possibility.
Figure 1.10
7
Assessing Children’s Learning
Figure 1.11
rest of his response is more interesting; I suggest that Jason heard the
word ‘clock’ as a significant instruction and, remembering previous class-
work on clocks, responded with a sketch of a clock, on which he then
duly marked the 8.
In these last two examples, I believe there is substantial evidence that,
against what must be, for him, inconceivable odds, Jason is struggling to
make sense of the test, and what his headteacher is asking him to do. His
mathematical understanding is still too scanty to be of much use to him, but
he uses all the other clues he can get. This is, I think, a remarkable achieve-
ment, and a tribute to Jason’s persistence, to his longing for meaning.
It is also possible to recognise Jason’s self-restraint, and his passivity,
during what was presumably an uncomfortable experience. He cannot
have been feeling relaxed, confident, sure of himself and certain of
success, during the test. Or does he not even know what it is he does not
know? It seems more reasonable to assume that he does know, very well,
that mathematics, and the mathematical tasks his teacher gives him,
make no kind of sense at all. And yet he is prepared to sit and comply, as
far as he is able, with a string of incomprehensible instructions.
Jason may not have learned much mathematics but he has learned
some important rules about being a pupil. Mary Willes’ study (1983) of
children entering a reception class charts the inexorable process by
which children – spontaneous, curious, independent – are transformed
into pupils, who know the rules. Her pessimistic summary of the task of
the pupil seems to describe Jason’s condition all too accurately:
. . . finding out what the teacher wants, and doing it, constitute the primary
duty of a pupil.
Willes (1983) p.138
8
Learning from Jason
Jason has learned not to resist and rebel when his teachers ask him to
do the impossible. Throughout the test he sat at his desk and did as he
was told, as far as he was able. It is only in my fantasy that Jason
overturns his desk and hurls it at his headteacher, screaming defiance,
demanding his rights as a pupil, as a child, as a human being.
When I was first shown Jason’s test booklet, the emotional impact on
me was very strong. As a result, it has taken me some time to see beneath
the surface features of this piece of primary practice to the larger, more
abstract issues it exemplifies. Jason’s test performance does certainly
illustrate inevitable weaknesses in formal group testing, in particular the
way in which such tests cannot tell us anything about the processes of
pupils’ thinking, but only whether pupils arrive at the unique right
answer. But there is more to be said. In Jason we see a child who, like all
other children, is capable of learning. He has been learning, during his
three years in school, but much of it is not what his teachers intended.
We have little evidence from this test of his learning in the cognitive
domain, but we can see how much he has learned about the social
conventions of the school – how to keep his pencil sharp, how to stay in
his seat, how to take a test, how to be a pupil. In the affective domain, we
can see how Jason has learned not to express dissatisfaction or disquiet
when meaningless demands are made on him. And yet we can also see
signs – small perhaps, but significant – that, in the limited ways left open
to him, Jason is still struggling to make sense of what goes on around
him in the puzzling world of school.
None of this has come about on purpose; his teachers have not
maliciously plotted to keep Jason in the dark, to break his will, to stupefy
him with bewildering questions, or to do anything but their best by him.
And yet things have gone seriously wrong for Jason. Jason’s obedience,
his compliance with his teachers’ demands, his search for meaning in a
largely meaningless world, force us to ask some difficult questions: what
is it that has gone wrong? How? And why? I believe that the wrong lies
not in his teachers’ intentions, not even in their teaching, but in what has
been happening to Jason’s learning. Jason stands as an example, and an
awful warning, of what can happen to all children whose learning is not,
for whatever reason, the prime concern and central focus of their
teachers’ attention.
Like all children, Jason has the right to an education in which his
learning is seen as of paramount importance – not his difficulties, or his
limitations, or his disadvantages, not his successes or failures – but his
9
Assessing Children’s Learning
10
Learning from Jason
about a more effective approach. I am arguing here, and throughout this
book, that we will never achieve effective assessment in schools and
classrooms if we conceptualise the task as a matter of making pragmatic
decisions about formats, formal testing procedures and record-keeping.
Trying to understand the place of assessment in education makes moral
and philosophical demands on our thinking. The practice of effective
assessment requires a thorough understanding and acceptance of the
concepts of rights, responsibility and power, lying at the heart of our
work as teachers. In searching for ways to make our assessment practices
more effective, we are committing ourselves to recognising children’s
rights, shouldering our responsibilities towards them, and striving to use
our power wisely and well.
11
CHAPTER 2
Looking at Learning:
Introductory
In thinking about Jason’s test booklet over the years, I have come to see
that the key issues in assessment, the most challenging and difficult
ones, are moral and philosophical, rather than organisational and
pedagogical. Questions of what, when, where and how to assess are of
secondary importance beside the more searching question of ‘Why
assess?’ And ‘Why assess?’ implies ‘Why educate?’ Effective assessment
can only be based on a thorough understanding-of our purposes in
teaching and of our aspirations for our pupils.
In the chapters that follow I discuss the concepts of rights,
responsibilities and power, the concepts introduced in Chapter 1 through
the analysis of Jason’s test booklet, with reference to two main areas of
concern: the interests of children, and the choices made by teachers. The
moral imperative for teachers’ choices to be made in the interests of
children is the starting point for an exploration of the choices that
teachers make in the practice of assessment – the choices they do make,
and the choices they could and should make. Put at its simplest, the task
of the teacher is to work for the interests of children, seeking to
understand and articulate those interests, and to serve them well. The
right of all children to a worthwhile education imposes on teachers the
awesome responsibility of understanding and providing the essential
components of such an education. This responsibility entails being able
to describe, explain and justify a set of beliefs about what constitutes
‘worthwhileness’ in primary education, and about what parts of
children’s learning and development are the teacher’s proper concern.
So, for example, my own system of beliefs and values has led me to take
a wide view of teachers’ responsibilities. Teachers are responsible, I
believe, for attending to children’s spiritual and moral growth, as well as
12
Looking at Learning: Introductory
to their physical well-being, to the essential differences between children
as well as to what they hold in common, to children’s rights to care and
loving attention as well as to carefully planned programmes of study, to
their right to be treated with honesty, trust and respect, as human beings,
rather than just as pupils.
One of the most challenging parts of this immense undertaking is to
find ways of assessing children’s learning that do honour to their rights
and interests, and that enhance the worthwhileness of their educational
experiences. The choices teachers make in assessing children’s learning
must be subject to this one central, inescapable principle: that children’s
interests are paramount. Assessment is a process that must enrich their
lives, their learning and development. Assessment must work for
children.
In recent years, especially since the implementation of the 1988
Education Reform Act, the term ‘assessment’ has come to suggest an
objective, mechanical process of measurement. It suggests checklists,
precision, explicit criteria, incontrovertible facts and figures. In this
book, I will be using the word in a different sense.
When we work with children, when we play and experiment and talk
with them, when we watch them and everything they do, we are
witnessing a fascinating and inspiring process: we are seeing them learn.
As we think about what we see, and try to understand it, we have
embarked on the process that in this book I am calling ‘assessment’. I am
using the term to describe the ways in which, in our everyday practice,
we observe children’s learning, strive to understand it, and then put our
understanding to good use.
In assessment, we can appreciate and understand what children learn;
we can recognise their achievements, and their individuality, the
differences between them. We can use our assessments to shape and
enrich our curriculum, our interactions, our provision as a whole; we can
use our assessments as a way of identifying what children will be able to
learn next, so that we can support and extend that learning. Assessment
is part of our daily practice in striving for quality. This book is based on
the view that effective assessment is a process in which our
understanding of children’s learning, acquired through observation and
reflection, can be used to evaluate and enrich the curriculum we offer.
The acts of the teacher in assessment – the responsibility to see, to
understand and to put this understanding to good use – are discussed
in detail in the chapters that follow. Teachers’ choices, the things they
13
Assessing Children’s Learning
actually do, will be examined in the light of the thinking that informs
those choices. The emphasis will be on the concepts and values that
teachers draw on in assessment, rather than on the physical trappings
of their practice, the ticksheets or record formats. Concepts such as
happiness, progress, improvement and comparison may be present
implicitly, as part of the mental framework that supports teachers’
assessment practices, but these concepts are rarely examined critically,
in the routines of every day, to establish the part they play in making
education more worthwhile for children. Effective assessment – clear
seeing, rich understanding, respectful application – will be advanced
by a full appreciation of the value-base from which teachers’ choices
are made.
It is sometimes convenient to think of the process of assessment as a
simple three-stage process; the assessor collects evidence, makes
judgements on the basis of that evidence, and certain events then follow.
This model can be crudely represented as in figure 2.1. In this book each
of these stages in the process will be discussed in turn, and later chapters
will explore the relationship between the stages, represented by the
arrows in the diagram. This plan has the advantage of tidiness, but the
disadvantage of suggesting that assessment is a neat and tidy process; it
is, of course, no such thing. Assessment is essentially provisional, partial,
tentative, exploratory and, inevitably, incomplete. The tension between
this necessary incompleteness, and the desire and pursuit of the whole,
is one of the reasons why teachers approach the task of assessment with
justifiable trepidation. Their trepidation is not allayed by the official
insistence, from the top of the educational system, that assessment, as
defined by a succession of education acts since 1988, should be both
complete and comprehensive. Teachers know better.
Figure 2.1
There are other tensions. One of these can be seen in the distinction
between the expressed purpose of a particular act of assessment, and
14
Looking at Learning: Introductory
what the assessors can, if they choose, learn from it. The avowed
intention of the assessment may have a narrow focus; there may be a
precise question about an individual child to be answered, or the
outcomes of a new approach to some specific curriculum area to be
ascertained, but the evidence obtained in the process of assessment can
always be reread and reinterpreted to tell us more than we thought we
were asking. Every enquiry into children’s learning, however narrowly
framed, will always also have something to say about teaching, about the
teacher, and about the whole curriculum, of which the teacher’s teaching
and the learner’s learning are only two constituent parts. Assessment is
a process of asking questions about learning that usher in both wider and
more deeply reaching (and often more discomfiting) questions about the
purposes and outcomes of education. The questions we ask in
assessment are members of the extensive set of evaluative questions that
we ask when we concern ourselves with the effectiveness and
worthwhileness of education as a whole. In a sense, however small a
piece of the classroom world we think we are looking at, we are always
giving ourselves the opportunity to look critically, or even, sometimes,
appreciatively, at a much bigger picture as well.
The case-study material that is used in this book to illustrate and
explain the central arguments is no exception to this general rule. Each
example has something to teach us about the present and the particular;
but its significance does not end there. Each example also has the power,
if we choose to respond, to stimulate a more abstract and principled
debate. The dictated stories of five-year-old children presented in Chapter
3, for example, illustrate, on one level, the extensive range of imaginative
purposes for which these children are using language. But at another
level, the material raises questions about the relationship between
fantasy and reality in the thinking of young children. These stories
suggest the possibility of reconsidering the common practice, in the
curriculum of many infant classrooms, of selecting topics and activities
because of their familiarity to the children, and their direct relevance to
their everyday experience. Similarly, the case-study of James, a child who
suffered great distress at parting from his mother each morning, can be
seen in two different ways. First, it can be seen to illustrate the possibility
of monitoring a child’s emotional development, as part of the process of
assessing children’s learning in all its forms. And, secondly, it can be seen
as a challenge to the received wisdom that maintains that it is the
business of primary school teachers to ensure their pupils’ happiness.
15
Assessing Children’s Learning
The story of James suggests that perhaps children have the right to be
unhappy, and to express their pain and grief. The great educator Korczak
(whose work is discussed in Chapter 6) included this right in his
idiosyncratic but thought-provoking list of Children’s Rights, p. 356: ‘The
child has the right to respect for his grief. (Even though it be for the loss
of a pebble.)’ (Lifton 1989).
In the same way, developmental psychologists, such as Margaret
Donaldson, whose work is briefly described in Chapter 4, have made it
their business to study children’s growing cognitive grasp of certain
carefully structured problems. But as we shall see, their work has more
to offer us: they have revealed unexpected connections between the
context of the task and the children’s cognitive and linguistic
performance. Their work suggests that teachers will do well to examine
carefully the context in which their assessments are made, as part of the
act of understanding, and not confine their attention simply to what the
children say and do.
In schools and classrooms, whenever we set about assessing children’s
learning, the possibility of these deeper questions, this broader vision, is
always there for us to recognise or not, as we choose. Even the statutorily
prescribed format of the standard assessment tasks, to be administered
at the end of each key stage, may have stimulated some teachers to
question their use of group work, or of peer support, or their policy on
invented spellings and the use of dictionaries. I am not suggesting that
administering a standard assessment task is the most effective way of
raising questions about appropriate primary practice; but I do maintain
that starting to ask questions about children’s learning, however precise
or trivial they may seem, is always the start of something big. It may even
be the beginning of a life-long enquiry into teaching, learning and the
curriculum. Small wonder that teachers are apprehensive about the long-
term and short-term effects of their practice in assessment on themselves
and on their pupils.
This justifiable apprehension was, of course, enormously increased by
the requirements of the 1988 Education Reform Act, which, for the first
time, imposed on teachers a programme of statutory procedures to be
followed in assessment. Teachers were not slow to realise that, as Blenkin
and Kelly (1992) so powerfully argue, ‘the central purpose of this
programme is administrative and political rather than educational’
(p.23). However, in spite of almost universal professional apprehension,
expressed throughout the years since 1988, I remain cautiously confident
16
Looking at Learning: Introductory
that practising teachers will not readily relinquish the educational
purposes of their own enquiries into teaching and learning.
A further tension for teachers can be discerned in the incontrovertible
fact that the willingness to embark, however tentatively, on such an
enquiry, does not itself guarantee clear sightedness and impartiality, a
rounded picture of real life, of teaching and learning as it really is. Like
trains at a level crossing, where one train sometimes conceals another
from the waiting motorist, the complexity of classroom events may
sometimes mean that careful scrutiny by the teacher of one part of the
scene may blot out an awareness of other equally important elements of
the picture. In particular it seems likely that teachers who are primarily
interested in their teaching, in their strategies, their goals and their lesson
plans, may pay less attention to the extent of the possible gap between
their intentions, and the actual outcomes, in terms of children’s learning.
Knowing what one set out to do, and looking for evidence that one has
done it, may not help one to see what has in fact resulted from one’s
good, even exemplary, intentions. Unintended learning is not, in any
event, easy to recognise; when it runs counter to the teacher’s intentions,
it may become virtually invisible.
Looking at learning, it is becoming clear, is not to be undertaken
lightly. The very conditions of classroom life, the complexity of
classroom events, the interconnectedness of context and curriculum, of
teaching and learning, all make it harder for us to look at learning as
clearly as we would wish. One of the great pioneers of early childhood
education, Susan Isaacs, whose work is discussed more fully in Chapter
4, has an important lesson to teach us here. The experimental school in
Cambridge, the Malting House, where for four years Isaacs and her
colleagues studied children’s learning intently and passionately, was
unusual in a number of ways. Isaacs describes the conditions in which
her observations took place as ‘relatively free’. Evelyn Lawrence, who
taught at the school, wrote of it:
There is no fixed curriculum. The children do what appeals to them at the
moment. The work of the educator is to select his material, and at times
indirectly to suggest activities, so that the child will of his own accord do
things which are useful for his work. Lately one or two of the older children
have drawn up rough outlines of their day’s work . . . No child would be
forced to keep to his programme if he seriously wanted to depart from it at
any time . . . There is, with all the children, much more active movement than
one finds in most schools. In fine weather they are out in the garden for most
17
Assessing Children’s Learning
Isaacs’ own words bear out this description; she refers to ‘an all-round
lessening of the degree of inhibition of the children’s impulses’ (Isaacs
1930, p.12). There were fewer checks on the verbal expression of the
children’s views and feelings than is common, and a correspondingly
‘greater dramatic vividness of their social and imaginative and
intellectual life as a whole’. The materials provided for the children, too,
led them to be much more generally active than children in ordinary
classroom conditions. More active and more genuinely exploratory: the
accounts of the children’s investigations into the workings of the Bunsen
burners with which their classroom was fitted make primary teachers
today turn pale with fright. But the emphasis at the Malting House on
the children’s freely chosen and self-directed activities was not in any
sense a licence for chaos and disorder. On the contrary:
This greater activity in all directions, originated, developed and sustained by
the children themselves, was a definite part of our educational aim. And it
not only led the children to show us their inner minds with far less reserve
and fear than in ordinary circumstances, but through the richer, more varied
and more immediate experience of the social and physical worlds which it
brought them, it also stimulated and diversified their actual responses. There
was, in other words, more for us to see; and we could see it more plainly.
Isaacs (op. cit.) p.12
18
Looking at Learning: Introductory
torrent of questions when looking for the first time at a large picture of
a railway station, with a train crossing a set of points and various signals.
Phineas (3;11) began to ask Mrs I. questions about the picture. ‘Why aren’t
there any coal trucks? Why is it darker there for? Why is it darker in there?
I saw a shadow. There is a fire in there. Why is there a fire in there? And
there are two men in there. Why are the men there for? Why couldn’t you
see it (the engine) going on the picture for?’ (He asked this exactly the same
way three times repeated.) ‘What are the railway lines for? What could the
puffer do without railway lines? Why aren’t there no railway lines
here?’(Pointing to the margin.) ‘Why couldn’t you see the next side for? Why
are those funny signals for? What is this round thing for?’ (Pointing to the
boiler.) ‘What are round boxes for?’ (Mrs I. was called away then; but he
went on talking to Priscilla.) ‘What is that man looking at, Priscilla? What are
the two colours for?’ (pointing to the signal flags.) Then Priscilla went away,
and he went to do something else.
Isaacs (op. cit.) p.148
19
Assessing Children’s Learning
freedom – for the children, for their choices, for ourselves, for our
pedagogy and for our assessment practices – that freedom is always, in
every school and classroom, substantially curtailed in the interests of
everyone else in the institution. There is a constant tension between
what might be appropriate for one pupil and what is appropriate for a
great many; between the needs of the individual and the group; between
the needs of one group or one class, and the whole body of pupils. The
sheer physical limitations that have, we believe, to be imposed on our
pupils’ movements to keep them safe from harm, make it impossible for
us to recreate Isaacs’ working conditions, and the intellectual and
emotional freedom of the 20 children at the Malting House school. But
in spite of all these constraints we can, still, I believe, seriously consider
the possibility of bringing our assessment practices up to the mark of
Isaacs’ breadth and clarity of vision: ‘There was more for us to see, and
we could see it more plainly’ – does not Isaacs’ achievement set us a goal
worth striving for?
Primary teachers commit themselves daily to a life of bewildering
complexity and uncertainty; not surprisingly, the inescapable
ambiguities, tensions and dilemmas of schools and classrooms awaken in
us unfulfilled longings for certainty. When we set about looking at
learning, we yearn for the single, simple point of view, for an end to
confusion, and a platform of verifiable facts on which to stand. Not
surprisingly, we are continually disappointed, but it may be that, in the
long term, this disappointment will serve us better than we think.
Erich Fromm’s The Fear of Freedom (1942) is a psychological analysis
of the ills of ‘modern’ European and American society. Fromm describes
the difference between irrational and rational doubt. Irrational doubt is
rooted in the isolation and powerlessness of the individual and a
negative attitude towards life. It is ‘one of the basic problems of modern
man’ and has dangerous consequences, especially ‘the compulsive quest
for certainty. . . (which) is rooted in the need to conquer the unbearable
doubt’ (p.66). Freedom from doubt, gained by seeking refuge in certainty,
is designated by Fromm as ‘negative freedom . . . freedom from’. Positive
freedom is associated with rational doubt, ‘which is rooted in the
freedom of thinking and which dares to question established views’.
Positive freedom, the freedom to . . . act spontaneously, to think for
ourselves, the freedom to realise ourselves, does not eliminate rational
doubt. The person, adult or child, teacher or pupil, who experiences
positive freedom is free to question, to learn, to welcome uncertainty as
20
Looking at Learning: Introductory
an inevitable part of growth towards proper understanding of a
‘meaningful world’. Fromm’s thinking has relevance for us today. He
argues that there are many adults in society who are committed, in their
search for understanding, to establishing unassailable bodies of fact, to
finding a formula that promises absolute certainty. These adults, he
concludes, are doomed to negative freedom, to a state of repressed
anxiety and overwhelming powerlessness.
This description suggests certain parallels with the lives and
philosophies of teachers. In our daily lives we are faced with the
possibility of embracing both rational doubt and positive freedom; it is
equally possible that we will be tempted to join ‘the compulsive quest for
certainty’ and so will achieve only negative freedom. In our assessment
practice, as we look at learning and try to understand it, this alternative
may be a very real danger. I am arguing that certainty is rarely achieved
in the process of looking at learning. However carefully and
questioningly we may look there is always more to see. Given what we
know about the effect of context on children’s learning, children’s
language, children’s behaviour, there is always more we need to know
about the context in which we choose to look at learning. Given what we
know about other aspects of the picture – for example, the emotional
dimension of learning, and the unintended outcomes of the hidden
curriculum – there is always more to know about the whole picture, both
its parts and its wholeness.
There is a dramatic example of how, once teachers start looking
closely, they learn to see more – and to see it more clearly – in Margaret
Meek’s Achieving Literacy (1983). This is a searching account of the
experiences of six teachers working with adolescent non-readers. These
teachers made audio-recordings of their lessons with individual pupils,
and met together as a group to share and discuss their observations. The
audio tapes of the lessons were rich sources of evidence; the group
analysed and re-analysed teacher intervention, pupil response and
teacher/pupil interaction. But there was still more to see. One lesson was
videotaped, and the group watched the video more than once. They saw
Jamie, aged 12, who had come for an individual reading lesson with the
teacher, Judith Graham. She describes the scene:
The table before us is mostly stacked with Jamie’s books, the two books he
has chosen to read, his writing notebooks, my record book, pen and paper. A
second copy of The Magic Finger is also there. We sit beside each other,
which seems friendly. Whilst Jamie initially turns over the pages himself and
21
Assessing Children’s Learning
presses down the centre of the book, I relieve him of this chore later and hold
down the corner of the page as well. When I ask him to write, I thoughtfully
produce a pen which I later discreetly remove when he uses it to point at
words he reads.
None of this would be evident on an audio tape. Even on the video tape
with the sound on one is primarily focusing on the spoken interchanges. But
look again at the scene as I have described it. The orderly table in front of us
may in fact exclude and intimidate Jamie. Certainly the idea of choice
becomes notional as you perceive me ‘tidying away’ the options I appear so
generously to have offered. And my extra copy of The Magic Finger – I never
open it, seeming to need to follow the same print as Jamie. Is the message
one of comforting cosiness or calculated control? The sitting alongside
where I can oversee the book at all times could also be construed as less
trusting than sitting opposite where I could be an audience. Jamie’s reading
aloud would then have a real purpose. And leaning back in my chair so that
I am slightly out of Jamie’s range of vision – does this indicate a wish to
minimize the intensity or am I in fact controlling from behind? Does Jamie
feel the unseen pressure behind him? My arm resting along the back of his
chair may further confuse the message.
Jamie’s behaviour with the pages of the book looks promising. We see him
flicking through, attempting to piece a story together, using the pictures. As
he starts to read he holds the top right hand corner in readiness to turn the
page and he enthusiastically rubs along the centre fold. Once the page is
turned his reading speed increases noticeably as he senses the progress he is
making. Why then do I deprive him later of this aspect of becoming a
reader? By the end of the lesson I am doing it all for him thereby increasing
his dependence, robbing him of simple competences that may make all the
difference to how he feels about himself as a reader. And as an aspiring
writer, Jamie surely has his own pen. By pressing mine on him and later
withdrawing it, albeit discreetly, I again reduce his autonomy and maintain
him in the position of a child who submits but who probably does not
ignore.
Meek (1983) p.126
This teacher and her colleagues had worked together for many hours,
supporting each other in the process of trying to understand their pupils’
learning. They were becoming highly skilled in analysis, reflection and
evaluation. They were aware of their pupils’ rights to respectful teaching,
and how those rights had been eroded by the practices that had allowed
them to enter the secondary school as non-readers. They were aware of
their massive responsibilities to these painfully fragile learners. And yet,
watching this videotape, they suddenly seemed to see, for the first time,
a new possibility. They realised how aspects of the physical interaction
22
Looking at Learning: Introductory
between teacher and pupil may ‘transmit as powerful a message as
anything spoken, and may critically impede or dramatically advance
progress towards the goal of independent and meaningful reading’. This
uncomfortable but revealing insight was not an outcome of their lack of
interest or concentrated attention; just the reverse. It was brought about
by their very willingness to look again, to look harder and more
searchingly. However attentively, however intently we look at learning,
there is always more to see, more to try to understand.
23
CHAPTER 3
Looking at Learning:
What Is There to See?
24
in mind, Michael Armstrong spent the school year 1976–77 in a primary
25
Assessing Children’s Learning
Paul has now finally solved the problem of how to keep a square shape rigid,
having failed to solve it by weaving. He ingeniously placed two pins in each
joint, thus effectively preventing the struts from rotating around the joint.
What a brilliant idea! In terms of technology it was both simpler and more
economical on materials than the method I had suggested. He had
successfully transformed the nature of the problem and its solution. (p.128)
26
These conclusions are not vague, optimistic generalisations about
Ann Lawson collected the books that Alison wrote over the next few
weeks, and noted that her dictated stories were not only longer, but more
imaginative, more unpredictable, more adventurous, than the stories she
wrote down for herself. In a tape-recorded interview, Ann Lawson tried
to investigate this discrepancy.
27
Assessing Children’s Learning
This teacher is very clear that her study of one child’s learning, closely
observed, and recorded in detail, has much to tell her about all children’s
learning, about their motivation, the challenges they set themselves, about
their attitudes, and most importantly for her, about their own
understanding of their learning. She knows that her own professional
learning has been immensely worthwhile, but even at the end of this case-
study, the question: ‘What is there to see?’ is for her, only a beginning.
Finally, having catalogued pages and pages of notes and observations I
would have imagined I knew almost everything about Alison. On the
contrary, I feel in some ways I know very little, and I wish I was able to do
my observations all over again, as I would be looking for even more detail.
28
Looking at Learning: What Is There to See?
of observations, including Margaret’s work with a maximum-minimum
thermometer. He chose this topic because during the class investigation of
‘weather’ Margaret’s responsibility was to record the daily temperatures,
maximum and minimum, for the class ‘weather station’. Michael Tennant
began his one-to-one session with Margaret by asking her to draw the
thermometer from memory (figure 3.1).
Figure 3.1
29
Assessing Children’s Learning
This picture, drawn simply from memory in the peace and quiet of the
library, and the subsequent discussion, reveal how little Margaret
understood about the instrument and yet how much she had within her,
given the opportunity, to tease out the essentials of the instrument and its
relation to those areas of her own ‘real existence’ like hot and cold that were
really important.
Centigrade and Fahrenheit caused some difficulty. They do for many of
us. It is a pity that the instrument had both. But her ability to set them out
on the picture and then merely concentrate on the Centigrade scale showed
a capacity for rejecting irrelevance.
She was aware of the function of the floats, and knew that the bottom of
the one on the left hand side marked a ‘minimum’ temperature, whilst that
on the right marked a ‘maximum’ temperature.
She was confused about the materials inside the thermometer. The floats
might ‘be a little bit of mercury’. Mercury was ‘a rubber liquid stuff inside the
tube’ (Margaret’s own words). These are not bad attempts at explaining
materials, and it is surely possible to use many a piece of equipment without
understanding its internal construction.
Margaret drew the magnet under the thermometer, and explained in
detail how this was used to reset the thermometer each day.
The scale itself caused great difficulty. She was being asked to think of a
scale with negative numbers when she had clearly not explored this notion.
Her attempt at a +/– scale in five degrees was it seems to me, a great feat of
perception, albeit revealing her failure to understand the basic way in which
the instrument operated. Nevertheless, when I asked her to talk to me about
her drawing, she began to see for herself the inadequacies of her model. She
totally failed to see the problem of 0° C being the same as 20° C. But then we
were in November and not yet into temperatures below 0° C. When she came
to read off the temperatures she read them off as 17° C max, and 131/2° C min.
She began to see the problem. But the solution was beyond her. In fact if one
examines her drawing one can see that the two sides of her thermometer are
really independent. The mercury does not go round the tube at the bottom.
She seemed to see the instrument as two separate pieces of equipment in
one. It had two different functions, maximum and minimum, and she had
failed to make the link between them. She had not yet explored the fact that
the mercury effectively moved around the tube from right to left and from
left to right so that when it went down on the left it appeared to go up on
the right. This of course meant that the scale on the left had to be the reverse
to the scale on the right. Nor was Margaret able to see that the instrument
could give us a reading of the present temperature.
30
tube) and a gap in her understanding. The drawing stands as an eloquent
Figure 3.2
31
Assessing Children’s Learning
Given another drawing, Margaret correctly read off both the present
temperature, and the minimum temperature recorded. On blank
drawings of the thermometer, Margaret coloured in the mercury to show
temperatures of –10° C and 25° C, and triumphantly, in one drawing, a
present temperature of 10° C, a minimum of 5° C and a maximum of 15°
C. One diagram continued to baffle her: a diagram of the thermometer
with the right-hand side calibration omitted (figure 3.3). It is as if she has
grasped that she is dealing with, in a sense, four different scales (plus and
minus for both maximum and minimum temperatures) but has not seen
how these four form a continuous series of numbers. There is still a gap
in her understanding. But Michael Tennant is more excited by the
thinking that has been going on, rather than concerned about the one
missing piece from the jigsaw.
Figure 3.3
The most exciting aspect of that hour or so with Margaret was the level of
our conversation. By talking about the instrument and drawings she came, I
believe, to a definition of gaps in her own knowledge. Her excitement at
wanting to control the instrument, to get it to move up and down at different
temperatures, showed some awakening knowledge of what the instrument
was doing and what it could tell us. She predicted that the maximum
32
temperature should come at midday and the minimum temperature at
33
Assessing Children’s Learning
school demands for success. When Margaret comes to school there is just too
much which has to be left outside beyond the school gate. In this, Margaret
is not alone.
34
Geoff Fisher expanded on these figures:
He goes on to comment that, if these days are typical, Kim would spend
just over one and a half hours a week queuing to see her teacher, and
Michael nearly two and a half hours!
What did this teacher learn about learning from this study?
The whole study has made me much more aware of the importance of the
learning which is taking place outside of the skills needed to produce
‘accepted’ work. These other aspects of learning include: How do I need to
behave if I want to fit in? What do I need to do to gain recognition? How
can I get by doing the least amount of work? What will please the teacher?
35
Assessing Children’s Learning
The different types of stories she has heard, from parents, peers, radio
and television, in music, poetry and prose, are some of the past
influences on her present story-telling. The content and characters are
various and the themes divergent. Massive forces – kings and
thunderstorms – cohabit with the intimate and particular – her friends,
her teacher, butterflies and frogs.
Sharon’s stories range less widely; they are variations on a single
theme:
The witch was hungry. She had nothing to eat.
The witch went out. She saw a pencil. She was very hungry so she ate it.
A little witch lady had nothing to eat, so she went to the hairdresser to put
her hair on. Then she ate her hair.
The shoes were so hungry they went to work and ate all the shoes.
A little small boy was very hungry. So he bought some cake and ate it. Then
he bought a book and ate the book too.
A little lady was very hungry. So she went to work to make a lot of money
and then she ate the money.
36
A little lady went out to work. She stole the governor’s hair and ate it.
I did not then, and would not now, attempt to read lessons in Sharon’s
personal psychology from these stories. What I did, and do, recognise is
a child working on a theme that is important to her; I found that it was
possible to respect Sharon’s interests without demanding that she reveal
her private meanings. Whatever the words meant to her secret self, the
‘eating’ metaphor served, for the time, her expressive purposes well
enough; I am proud to recall that I never tried to encourage her away
from ‘eating’ stories (‘Wouldn’t it be a good idea to do a different kind of
story today?’).
Darren’s stories are eclectic in content, but structurally they have a
common element; they are all two-part inventions.
The monster broke out of his cage and ran away because he saw a leopard
coming after him.
A little old lady and a little old man are standing in the rain because they
want a bath and they haven’t got a bath in their house.
A wolf ate an elephant because he was hungry and had no food.
The crocodile was going to eat the boat but Charlton jumped in the water
and killed the crocodile.
Captain Scarlet has gone to the pictures. He wanted to sleep in the cinema.
A man is on a boat which is sinking. Mighty Mouse is trying to save him.
Each of these six stories is like a necklace with just two beads; they are
stories composed of mini-stories. In the first, a monster story and a
leopard story are connected by ‘because’; the same link is used for the
indoors story and the outdoors story in the second. In the fifth example,
the stories of Captain Scarlet awake and Captain Scarlet asleep are linked
by a connection of personal intention. Heroism and bravery are
celebrated in the fourth story (Charlton was a conspicuous member of
the class, soon to be excluded by the headteacher for anti-social
behaviour, but a hero to his peers); intrepid human effort stops the first
story in mid-flow and redirects the action: now the devouring crocodile
is itself destroyed. Given what Darren already knows about stories and
how to tell them, it will not be long before he learns to accommodate
three or more elements into his creations, and to widen still further the
range of connections that he uses. The necklaces he strings will be longer,
with beads and patterns of infinite variety.
37
Assessing Children’s Learning
38
however, blind us to the possibility of looking at tiny fragments of
Giles, who reads The Beano in a literary way and has interesting hobbies
wrote:
Hallowe’en, incandescent, rats, dogs, barn, Beano (and insisted on adding
‘catapult’).
Alice is beautiful and precious, one of those children who seems to be full
of light. She wrote:
china, crystal, drop, light, shine, blossom.
39
Assessing Children’s Learning
Figure 3.5
The teacher treasured these pieces of writing and at the end of the
summer term, included them in the portfolios of work that the children
took with them into the next class. The receiving teacher was delighted
with these miniatures, and the insights they offered into the children’s
thinking. The canvas was small, certainly, but the pictures of learning
were glowing, intense, individual.
The studies of children’s learning that have been presented so far have
focused on children’s intellectual abilities; but teachers in primary
classrooms take a wider view. When we survey children’s learning, when
we attempt to establish the extent of the evidence at our disposal, we
cannot afford to exclude the emotional domain. Without doing violence
to our understanding of children as human beings, we cannot ignore
either the emotional turbulence of their lives, or the emotional
development that runs alongside the physical, social and cognitive
changes that we see taking place in our schools and classrooms.
Successive national initiatives and government policies may temporarily
distract us from these aspects of children’s learning, but we will be wise
not to neglect them for very long. There are useful historical precedents;
Susan Isaacs (1930), as we shall see in the next chapter, includes in her
discussion of biological learning a consideration of the emotional states
of cruelty and tenderness. Margaret Lowenfeld, a child psychiatrist,
writing only a few years later, records in Play in Childhood (1935) the
intensely emotional states to be observed in children’s play.
Lowenfeld describes one form of play in which children give
satisfaction to their feelings of hostility and resentment and she draws
attention to the adult counterpart of this play: the satirical form in
literature. When children create satirical forms, they are expressing
40
emotions of resentment, anger, malice and rage, feelings that are
41
Assessing Children’s Learning
James was in the beginning of his third year in the infant school, aged 6,
when he made the drawing shown below (figure 3.6). James was an only
child of a mother who was in her early forties when James started school,
rather older than the majority of other mothers with children in that class.
James’ father was an officer in the Merchant Navy and away from home for
up to six months at a time. James and his mother were particularly close to
one another and both of them found the parting at the beginning of each
school day very painful. During James’ first year in school, his mother would
stay beside him in the classroom for half an hour or so most mornings, but
as James moved up the school, she tried to reduce the time spent in this way,
though she was careful not to cause James any additional distress.
By the beginning of the third year in school James and his mother had
worked out a ritual for parting from each other that seemed to satisfy them
both. The route they took to school led down a narrow road through some
trees to a spot where the school drive branched off to the right, leading uphill
again to the school front door, about 50 yards away. James and his mother
stopped at this spot and kissed each other goodbye. James bolted up the
drive to the front door, while his mother waited on the same spot. From the
doorstep of the school, James waved and blew kisses, which were warmly
returned. His mother then started walking uphill, and as soon as she walked
away, James came into school and flew down the corridor to his classroom.
He stood on the classroom windowsill (luckily a low one) and waited until
he could see his mother coming back into sight through the trees, as she
walked homewards up the hill behind the school. When she saw him at the
window, a second round of waves and kisses ensued, and then, satisfied,
James was ready to join the class.
One misty November morning, James’ class teacher was unexpectedly
late, and I was standing in for her. I set out on one table some drawing
materials – white chalk and charcoal and grey paper – with, frankly, no
purpose except that of engaging the children more or less profitably until I
had time to prepare myself properly for a morning session with them. James
went straight to this table and settled to work. In about 20 minutes he
produced this drawing, which is, I believe, extremely beautiful. It is certainly
remarkably evocative of the damp moist mist that was curling around the
trees outside the classroom window. Fragments of the same mist seemed to
have crept into the school corridor, and the windows of the classroom were
covered with chilly condensation. I asked James the title of his picture:
‘Waving Goodbye to my Mother’. This should, of course, have been obvious,
but James did not remark on my obtuseness.
When I had time to gather my wits, look more carefully at his drawing,
and discuss it with colleagues (James’ present and previous teachers and the
non-teaching assistant who knew the family well), the importance of what he
had done began to become clear. James had drawn himself from the outside,
from the other side of the classroom window, looking in through the mist
42
Looking at Learning: What Is There to See?
Image Not Available
and condensation. He had drawn what his mother saw, as she waved
goodbye to him for the last time that morning. But he had also drawn an
emotional portrait of himself – as a child who is both distressed and
courageous, a child who hates parting but who has learned to say goodbye, a
child who is near to tears but remains serene and composed – a very realistic
self portrait. And the picture seems to represent not just this self, but James’
awareness of the characteristics of this self, as something to be proud of.
43
Assessing Children’s Learning
convinced that it was the best way to help James to learn to part more
easily from his mother. But this picture seemed to offer us evidence that
James’ emotional development was proceeding apace. Before the age of
seven, he had learned not only to understand his own – mixed – feelings
he had also learned to represent those feelings. He had learned to
represent them not only from his own point of view but from his
mother’s. This picture seems to say: ‘When my mother looks at me, she
sees her brave sad boy.’ The picture records a profound understanding of
the painful emotions that James experienced every morning on the way
to school.
It is not very often that children present their teachers with such
unsolicited testimony of their learning in the affective domain. This
makes it all the more important for teachers not to overlook this aspect
of life in schools and classrooms. When we ask what might constitute
evidence of learning, we must be certain not to exclude the affective and
the emotional. Whenever we ask ourselves: ‘What is there to see?’ or
‘What am I looking for?’ we must be certain to include children’s
emotional learning. When we reflect on our responsibility to try to see
everything, we must not forget the children’s right to respect for their
emotional powers, as well as their intellectual ones.
44
CHAPTER 4
Looking at Learning:
Learning to See
13.7.25: Some of the children called out that the rabbit was dying. They
found it in the summerhouse, hardly able to move. They were very sorry and
talked much about it. They shut it up in the hutch and gave it warm milk.
14.7.25: The rabbit had died in the night. Dan found it and said, ‘It’s dead –
its tummy does not move up and down now.’ Paul said, ‘My daddy says that
45
Assessing Children’s Learning
if we put it into water, it will get alive again.’ Mrs I. said, ‘Shall we do so and
see?’ They put it into a bath of water. Some of them said, ‘It is alive.’ Duncan
said, ‘If it floats, it’s dead, and if it sinks it’s alive.’ It floated on the surface.
One of them said, ‘It’s alive, because it’s moving.’ This was a circular
movement, due to the currents in the water. Mrs I. therefore put in a small
stick which also moved round and round, and they agreed that the stick was
not alive. They then suggested that they should bury the rabbit, and all
helped to dig a hole and bury it.
15.7.25: Frank and Duncan talked of digging the rabbit up but Frank said,
‘It’s not there – it’s gone up to the sky.’ They began to dig but tired of it and
ran off to something else. Later they came back and dug again. Duncan,
however, said, ‘Don’t bother – it’s gone – it’s up in the sky,’ and gave up
digging. Mrs I. therefore said, ‘Shall we see if it’s there?’ and also dug. They
found the rabbit, and were very interested to see it still there.
Isaacs (op. cit.) pp.182–3
46
about a week, he returned to his usual self again. This has coincided with the
I have once, and only once, used this extract as a piece of discussion
material in my in-service work with practising teachers. The ensuing
uproar was so passionate that there was simply no possibility of the
group of teachers learning anything about learning from the extract, or
from discussion of the extract. Indeed, when teachers are invited to
discuss a much less controversial (according to me!) extract – the
description of Frank and Duncan attempting to dig up the dead rabbit
47
Assessing Children’s Learning
48
A group of psychologists working with Margaret Donaldson (1978) in
49
Assessing Children’s Learning
50
of the world of shared experience, just will not do. There is early opportunity
51
Assessing Children’s Learning
The category, ‘Not Knowing What To Do’, that emerges from comments
like this seems to be a powerful one for these children. Another form of
inaction Barrett labelled ‘not being involved’, building on elements in
children’s responses such as ‘I don’t want to’, ‘I can’t’, ‘They are not
looking’, ‘They don’t want to listen’. A third category ‘worrying’ arose
from comments on a photograph of a child sitting at a table with an
exercise book, sucking a pencil.
Girl can’t do it. Perhaps she’s drawing a ship.
One’s putting her pencil in her mouth because she’s thinking what to draw.
I like drawing mummy, daddy and me in a house with curtains.
A boy doesn’t know what to do. He is sucking his pencil. He cannot do his
work. He must tell his teacher.
I don’t know how to do it. I don’t know how to paint or mix the colours
properly.
He’s thinking about his mum and dad at home.
I didn’t like to write when I came to school. I couldn’t make a snail. I couldn’t
draw a picture. It was too hard. I was too little. I feel miserable when I can’t
do it.
I’m frightened I might get it wrong.
Barrett (op. cit.) p.82
52
of classroom life, and the ways in which children respond to the
53
Assessing Children’s Learning
day by taxi, one of only three children out of a school population of 215
who attended the school from this small community.) Sheila Gapp, who
was Linda’s class teacher, carried out timed observations of Linda in a
variety of contexts – in small self-chosen groups, in larger teacher-
directed groups, in the whole class group, in the playground, working
individually, and during extra-curricular activities. She investigated
Linda’s perception of herself, as a person and as a learner, and collated
Linda’s chosen friends’ perceptions of her.
Her observations added up to a very agreeable surprise: ‘My
observations have led me to rethink my judgements. I have seen Linda
regularly participating in group work, being actively involved in seeking
outcomes and sharing experiences. Her contributions are highly valued
by the other group members, who turn to her frequently for assistance
and support.’ This teacher discovered that her first impressions of Linda’s
difficulty in ‘learning to live in a crowd’ were not supported by the
evidence she collected in other contexts. When each of Linda’s regular
working environments was closely observed, a fuller picture could be
seen. This teacher concluded: ‘It is not until you observe intently that you
find patterns of events and behaviour which might have remained
unnoticed.’
I have suggested that, as part of their daily life in classrooms, teachers
set themselves the grossly unrealistic task of trying to see and
understand everything. Inevitably we fall short. Inevitably we see more
of some sorts of learning than of others, more of some children and less
of others, more of one aspect of the curriculum and less of others that are
not, currently, at the front of our minds. Inevitably, we see more of what
we expect to see, of what we know is there, than of the unexpected and
unintended. Occasionally we do get a glimpse of children’s unintended
learning, and although the experience may be uncomfortable, it may also
be salutory in forcing us to remember that teachers’ good intentions do
not always translate directly into desirable learning outcomes. An
observation I recorded of a four-year-old boy, Luigi, is a useful example of
how teachers can learn from the unexpected.
Figure 4.1 shows a page of writing produced by Luigi on his fourth
afternoon in the four year old unit of a primary school. He chose to work at
the table where the writing materials were provided, selected paper, a pencil
and a ruler, and drew a line across the page, about two-thirds down. He
began to write on the left hand side of the page and completed his writing
on the right hand side with a full stop. He added the two crosses at the top
54
Looking at Learning: Learning to See
Image Not Available
left hand corner and prepared to leave the table. I asked him to explain what
he had done: he identified his name pointing to the two crosses (‘Luigi
Giardini’), and his writing (pointing to the marks across the lower half of the
page). He did not respond to my question about what the writing said. I
asked him about the blank expanse of paper: ‘That’s for my drawing.’ ‘And
are you going to do a drawing?’ ‘Oh, I can’t draw.’
In four afternoons, Luigi has learned a good deal about school routines
and expectations. He has also learned (though not necessarily at school)
some of the important things he needs to know about writing. But he has
also learned something much less worthwhile: that there are classroom
expectations that he believes he cannot meet (‘I can’t draw’). It goes
without saying that his teachers had not set out to teach him this; and it
was fortunate for all concerned that the observer’s question revealed
Luigi’s estimate of his own capabilities. His teachers were, as a result,
made aware of their responsibility for preventing Luigi’s perception of
himself as a child who ‘can’t draw’ from taking deeper root. Unintended
learning is not often so easily discerned, and so our central responsibility
55
Assessing Children’s Learning
56
John appears to ‘own’ the game. He has set up the ironing board as a shop
57
Assessing Children’s Learning
58
revealed, skills ‘more usually attributed by language researchers to
59
Assessing Children’s Learning
60
the bow to the violin
61
Assessing Children’s Learning
Children from the reception class came to discuss tortoises one day;
three tortoises had visited them that very morning and they could think
of nothing else. In this discussion, they used a strategy I deliberately
encouraged – that of describing by the use of negative concepts. For
example, tortoises can run, but they can’t:
run fast
ride a bike
drive a car
climb trees
eat sweets
have a belly button
wear clothes
At this point a child contributed ‘They can’t come to school’. He was
immediately corrected by a chorus of contradiction, but stood his
ground, restructuring his thinking in a remarkable way, I believe, for a
five-year-old: ‘They do come to school, but they don’t learn anything.’
A discussion on the topic of ‘saying goodbye’ is a good example of how
these children were able to move from the specific details of their life in
the here and now, to more general propositions, and, indeed to a
metaphorical representation of the same idea. Contributions included:
saying goodbye . . .
● to Laura, because she’s going on holiday
● to Mummies because they go home or to work
● When you’ve been on a holiday – to where you’re staying (followed
immediately by. . . )
● Lucy said goodbye to France
● Andrew said goodbye to Wales
● Lyndsey said goodbye to the ferry
● On the telephone – so they know you’ve finished
● Say goodbye to your dinner when you’ve eaten it
● Goodbye day – hello night – goodbye night – hello day
● Goodbye Friday, hello Saturday
● Goodbye sleep, when you wake up
Here are children working towards the real essence of ‘goodbye’ – a word
that conveys not just a parting of persons, but an ending, a cessation of
62
a particular relationship and, by extension, the beginning of a new
You can’t lose a sewing machine You can find a needle with a
You can’t buy a packet of magnet
sewing machines You need lots of needles
Won’t be much room in the On holiday, take a needle
car if you take it on holiday You don’t need a cupboard
You can store things inside for it
a sewing machine
The last words of the discussion came down in favour of the needle –
‘needles were invented first, they are smaller and much simpler’.
Once I began to appreciate the linguistic strengths revealed in these
discussions, I encouraged the children to pick abstract topics – not just
‘things they could see’. ‘Silence’ was a fruitful topic, as was ‘movement’.
‘Things that do not move’ gave the children some difficulty. They had
already listed, under moving things, cars, eyes, people, all the different
63
Assessing Children’s Learning
64
understanding; we were really discussing forms of protection against
65
CHAPTER 5
Ways of Seeing:
Trying to Understand
66
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
suffused with sympathies and antipathies. We are enriched by our own
reading and study, so that we have come to understand the conceptual
frameworks of others, as well as building and learning about our own.
Both the early observers of children’s lives, Margaret Lowenfeld and
Susan Isaacs, whose work I have already drawn on, introduce their
accounts with severe warnings about the impossibility of accuracy and
impartiality. Lowenfeld (1935) cites four distinct sources of error which
arise from the essential structure of the observer’s mind:
(1) The mind cannot grasp that which is wholly unfamiliar to it . . .
(2) The mind is more apt to see that which it has already noticed . . .
(3) The mind is unable to see that which it has not been trained to accept . . .
(4) The mind unconsciously distorts what it hears and sees, according to its
own prejudices. (p.35)
As an instance of the last type of error, Lowenfeld describes how the
personal feelings of the observer become active in the impersonal work
of ‘watching a roomful of children at play. . . An adult is apt to project on
to children ideas he has already formed in childhood, and to see children
not as they are, but as he wishes them to be’ (p.36).
But Lowenfeld does not take fright at her own warnings of possible
misunderstandings. She goes on to use her observations, scoured, as she
hopes, from error and prejudice, to build frameworks of understanding. For
example, Lowenfeld’s description of the satirical form of children’s play,
quoted in Chapter 3, is part of a broader classification of play: play as social
realism, play as romance, and play as satire (p.135). If we follow her
example, and accept the provisional nature of the frameworks we construct,
we will not be falling into irretrievable error, but cautiously and carefully
working within the limits of our human capacity to understand.
The work of Vivian Gussin Paley (1981), an American kindergarten
teacher, provides many instructive examples of the educator seeing
children ‘as (s)he wishes them to be’. Or rather, seeing them at first in this
way; Paley records her detailed evidence and sets it against her wished-
for interpretation. Time and again, the educator’s perspective, the
educator’s intentions, do not match the evidence. Time and again, Paley
is forced to reconsider, to reconstruct the framework within which she
makes meaning. In the following incident, the five-year-old children in
Paley’s kindergarten class have planted lima beans in individual milk
cartons, but after three weeks no green shoots have appeared. Wally
discovers that his beans have disappeared.
67
Assessing Children’s Learning
‘They’re gone!’ he yelled, bringing me his carton. ‘Gone! I looked through the
whole dirt!’
‘Can I look in mine?’ asked Rose.
‘You might as well,’ I answered. ‘They don’t seem to be coming up.’
There was a rush to the planting table. Everyone began digging into cartons
or dumping their contents on the newspaper-covered table.
Andy: Where are the beans?
Wally: They’re invisible.
Andy: Impossible. They came from a store. Someone took them out.
Teacher: Who?
Andy: A robber.
Eddie: When it was dark a criminal took them.
Teacher: Why would he do it?
Jill: Maybe someone came in and said, ‘Oh, there’s nothing growing.
We must take some of them out.’
Eddie: I think a robber broke in and said, ‘They don’t need to plant
those beans.’
Teacher: Why would a robber want them?
Wally: To sell them.
Andy: Or cook them.
Ellen: No, maybe to fool people with. See, he could plant them in his
garden and when flowers came up people would think he’s nice.
Teacher: If I were a robber I’d take the record player.
Eddie: Not if you wanted to plant seeds.
Paley (1981) pp.57–8
Here we see two ways of understanding the world at work. The children
account for the missing beans with imaginary robbers; the teacher is
intent on the logical connections, as she sees them, between a real robber
and what he would be likely to steal. The children do not consider that
the robber’s imaginary theft needs any further explanation: stealing is
what robbers do. If there are beans to steal, and a robber about, then the
robber will certainly steal them. Trying to understand her pupils’
thinking, Paley talks to another kindergarten class about the decaying
Hallowe’en pumpkin that they are observing.
Teacher: Why does your pumpkin look like this?
Tim: It’s full of mold.
Carter: It’s moldy. Plants are growing inside.
Julia: Little vines.
William: They make the top fall in.
Kevin: Dead plants and animals get mold.
Tim: Old pumpkins get moldy.
Julia: It’s going to become dust.
68
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
Teacher: How does that happen?
Julia: It’ll get so dry you won’t even see it.
Teacher: By the way, we have a problem in class. We planted lima beans
and after a long time nothing came up. We looked in the dirt to
see if any roots had grown and we couldn’t find the beans. They
were gone.
Kevin: Was the window open? The wind blew them away.
Teacher: They were deep down in the dirt.
Candy: A squirrel could have took them.
Teacher: We didn’t see a squirrel in the room.
Candy: It could have hid somewhere.
Teacher: Our windows are locked at night. How could the squirrel have
gotten back out?
William: He could scratch a hole in the window.
Kevin: Or in the door.
Carter: Maybe a robber stepped in. They can get in windows very easily.
Teacher: Why would he want the beans?
Carter: For his garden.
Julia: Or to cook them. Somebody has a key to your window, I think.
There was no further talk of squirrels once the robber theory was suggested.
I was so surprised by this change of opinion after talking about a rotting
pumpkin that I presented my question to the third kindergarten. One child
said a bird might be the culprit, another suspected worms. However, when a
third mentioned robbers, everyone immediately agreed that the beans had
been removed by a human intruder to plant, eat, or sell.
Paley (op. cit.) pp.59–60
69
Assessing Children’s Learning
Unlike the adults from whom they learned what robbers do, the children
see no reason to be frightened of robbers:
The robber feared by adults bears no relationship to the one created by the
children. Their robber is so busy stealing lima beans he has no time to cause
harm. (p.62)
70
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
they thought her prodigiously stupid and for the first two or three weeks
were continually bringing some fresh report of it into the drawing room.
‘Dear mamma, only think, my cousin cannot put the map of Europe together
– or my cousin cannot tell the principal rivers in Russia – or she never heard
of Asia Minor or she does not know the differences between water-colours
and crayons! – How strange! – Did you ever hear anything so stupid?’
‘My dear,’ their considerate aunt would reply; ‘it is very bad, but you must
not expect every body to be as forward and quick at learning as yourself.’
‘But, aunt, she is really so very ignorant! – Do you know, we asked her last
night, which way she would go to get to Ireland; and she said, should cross
to the Isle of Wight, She thinks of nothing but the Isle of Wight, and she calls
it the Island, as if there were no other island in the world. I am sure I should
have been ashamed of myself, if I had not known better long before I was so
old as she is. I cannot remember the time when I did not know a great deal
that she has not the least notion of yet. How long ago it is, aunt, since we
used to repeat the chronological order of the kings of England, with the dates
of their accession, and most of the principal events of their reigns!’
‘Yes,’ added the other; ‘and of the Roman emperors as low as Severus;
besides a great deal of the Heathen Mythology, and all the Metals, Semi-
Metals, Planets, and distinguished philosophers.’
71
Assessing Children’s Learning
72
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
configurations as he had been with spheres. At 2:7:9, while on the project
bus, Jock shouted to his mother twice on the journey, ‘look’. Once it was
‘scaffolding’ and once it was ‘a fence’. On that same day he rolled out some
long pieces of clay and called them ‘spider’s legs’ and, later, ‘stripes’. This is
an example of ‘fitting’ form (parallel lines) onto suitable content.
Jock (2:7:14) would not be parted from a tennis racquet. He hugged it and
gazed at it alternatively. Two days later he pointed to a grid configuration
and said, ‘Windmill’. He followed this with a painting of four vertical lines
named ‘Horsey, Man, Sheep and Tree’. Jock (2:7:28) carefully inspected a
hammock. Jock (2:8:0) made a model. He called it ‘tiger’, pointed to the nails
and called them ‘stripes’.
Athey (1990) p.93
73
Assessing Children’s Learning
(1) Kate was dressing up and wrapped first a sari and then a large shawl
around her.
(2) She went into the home corner and pulled the ironing board across the
gap, ‘I’m here now – it’s private’.
(3) She took a wicker basket and went around the nursery collecting objects
such as shells, nails, screws, small boxes and paper bags. She felt each
object, apparently exploring texture and shape. On reflection, the adult
who made this observation realized that each object that Kate selected
was either a form of container or, in the case of nails and screws, ‘went
into’ something else.
(4) Kate was talking on the telephone, the line went dead. ‘It’s the inside
that’s broken I think,’ she said.
These notes show a consistent thread of thinking over the two days, a
thread running through four separate activities. Kate was exploring
‘enveloping and containing’ with all the experiences available to her. In the
process she was collecting according to clear criteria and categorizing
objects, using her senses. She was defining space, hypothesizing and using
language to express her thinking.
Drummond and Nutbrown (1992) pp.94–5
74
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
makes an observation, and finds it to be correct or incorrect by comparison
with the teacher’s (or the currently accepted) version. He learns by
discovering disparities between his result and that obtained by more
experienced and skilful persons. In the discussion technique of teaching, the
student learns by comparing his observation with those of ten or so of his
peers. He compares not only the results, but how the results were arrived at
and in doing this the range of factors taken into consideration is much wider
than is usual in didactic teaching. What the student learns, it is hoped, is not
only how to make a more correct response when he is confronted with a
similar problem, but more generally to gain firmer control of his behaviour
by understanding better his own ways of working.
Abercrombie (1969) p.19
This radiologist relied on a schema (buttons are worn outside, not inside
the body) that misled him for a while. Only when he was forced to
reconsider could his perception become more open and reflective.
75
Assessing Children’s Learning
76
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
literature, from Plato, through Rousseau, to Dewey, and especially in
Dewey. However, Egan argues that some of Dewey’s propositions have
been debased and misinterpreted. For example, when Dewey writes ‘it is
a cardinal precept of the newer school of education that the beginning of
instruction shall be made with the experience learners already have’, the
term ‘experience’ has come to be seen ‘largely in terms of the everyday
practical world of children’s lives’ (Egan op. cit., p.190). The focus of
educators’ attention has been ‘the mundane and practical world in which
children live. What has been lost is the ability to see that world as the
child sees it, transfigured by fantasy’ (p.20).
Egan maintains that this is a serious loss; a prominent part of
children’s mental lives is fantasy, a wholesome and important activity,
and yet research programmes into children’s cognition and development
have tended to ignore this element of fantasy. Egan suggests that Piaget,
too, is partly to blame, in that his interest in the rational and logico-
mathematical operations of young children blocked from view what
Egan calls ‘the wild energy’ of their thinking.
This early focus of Piaget’s work discouraged other energetic and evident
features of young children’s thinking. Their romance and fantasy were
considered merely contaminants to his attempt to chart the growth of what
he calls intelligence. The child who cannot, on the one side, conserve liquid
quantity may, on the other, lead a vivid intellectual life brimming with
knights, dragons, witches and star warriors. It would be needlessly bold to
prejudge which is more important to future intellectual growth. (p.23)
77
Assessing Children’s Learning
calmly, asking only a few questions – ‘What does she need? Did anyone see
it happen?’ Intense activity and discussion ensued; within moments four
children arrived as an ambulance team, riding in an imaginary ambulance,
in response to a phone call from one of the original group. Two other
children, transformed into a doctor and a police officer, appeared in response
to further phone calls. The doctor palpated Shazia’s belly (as she lay on an
improvised bed): ‘I can feel the bullet wound. It’s very serious.’ Witnesses
joined the play, pressing close to the police officer to give their version of the
incident: ‘I saw his bushy tail.’ ‘I saw his face.’ The police officer wrote each
contribution down on a sheet of paper on a clipboard (in ‘imaginary’
writing). One of the ambulance team was pessimistic: ‘I tried that medicine
and all the tablets Emily gave me and she still won’t wake up. She’s dead. For
ever.’ Another child agreed: ‘Her heart’s broken and she’ll never wake up.’
And a third: ‘I felt her heart and it’s broken.’ Fortunately, at this point, Shazia
revived and rejoined the play, which by now involved ten or twelve children,
improvising and embellishing the continuing drama. Half an hour later, at
the end of the session, the whole class came together to sit on the carpet and
recall the main events of the morning. Shazia’s narrow escape was heatedly
discussed, and thought taken for the future. One child remarked: ‘If the wolf
comes this afternoon I think I’d better call the woodcutter.’ Another child
responded to this contribution by leaving the group to fetch the Yellow
Pages. After a few minutes she told the class, ‘His number’s 202’. Further
suggestions included ‘We could call Robin Hood, because he’s got arrows.’ In
the afternoon session I observed Shazia designing a ‘Wanted’ poster, which
incorporated a vivid portrait of the wolf – ‘His ears went like this . . . he was
standing behind the tree . . . He was really really thin.’
78
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
In Egan’s view, Shazia and her friends were not, in their play, running
away from the proper business of schooling, the acquisition of the basic
skills. On the contrary, the curriculum they were forging for themselves
in their fantasy play is close to the curriculum he advocates for all young
children:
Our early curriculum then, is to be made up of important content that is rich
in meaning for children. Its meaning will derive from its being articulated
on concepts they know from their experience – love/hate, fear/security,
good/bad, courage/cowardice and so on – and our curriculum concern will be
to get at what is of human importance to our social and cultural lives. (p.199)
79
Assessing Children’s Learning
sustain and care for one another. Their fantasy play is both a rehearsal
and a celebration of this learning.
When Egan watches children playing with smurfs, he sees them
engaged by the unrelenting conflict between binary opposites; when
Athey looks at children’s mark-making, she sees patterns of vertical and
circular schemas. When Lowenfeld observes children’s play, using the
‘world cabinet’, she distinguishes three different emotional functions
served by miniature world play. These three authors, who see the world
so differently, are powerful illustrations of the human desire to make
sense of our experiences, by creating patterns and structures of meaning.
When teachers assess children’s learning in schools and classrooms, we
too draw on internalised patterns of understanding, seeing perhaps, as
confirmed Piagetians, signs of specific cognitive stages, or, like Michael
Armstrong ‘knowledge as appropriation’. We make sense of children’s
learning by setting what we see and hear up against our working models
of children’s learning, and noting the areas of congruence and disparity.
But not all forms of assessment are based on clear and coherent
models of children’s learning; and where there is no underlying
understanding of the process of learning the outcomes of the assessment
will be relatively trivial, telling us little of any purpose. Ted Hughes calls
stories ‘little factories of understanding’; if we are to understand
children’s learning, we must tell ourselves stories about that learning,
stories that build on what we already know and that explore what we are
coming to realise. On the other hand, if we have no narrative or
explanatory framework for observations of children’s learning, there will
be no stories to tell, and no ‘factories of understanding’. We will be left
with the unvarnished facts, or a string of numbers, from which we will
learn nothing.
Until 1997, when baseline assessment on entry to the reception class
became a statutory requirement, teachers were free to design their own
entry assessment schedules. Throughout the 1990s, until new
requirements came into force, I set myself the curious task of collecting
examples of these, some of which are, as we shall see, grossly
inappropriate in a whole variety of ways. I was shown one such schedule
in its trialling version, which was to be used during the children’s first
term in the reception class. It comprised a list of attributes and areas of
development, ranging from toilet-training to creativity and cooperation,
with a six-point rating scale for each attribute (for example: Interest 1 2
3 4 5 6; Enthusiasm 1 2 3 4 5 6; Concentration 1 2 3 4 5 6). Teachers were
80
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
expected to assign a number between one and six to each child for each
item on the schedule: no written criteria or descriptions were given.
Teachers are here being invited to reduce the complexity of each pupil’s
individuality, and the richness of each child’s learning, to a meaningless
numerical scale. There is no underpinning representation of the process
of learning; as a result there is no possibility of using the schedule to
understand a child’s learning.
I would like to think that, by the end of the trialling period, the
reception class teachers using this schedule realised that it would do little
to enhance their understanding, or their pupils’ learning. However a
paper by Barry Bensley and Stuart Kilby (1992) reports the trialling and
subsequent adoption of a similar procedure in two Lincolnshire primary
schools. The baseline profile described in their paper was designed and
developed by the staff of these two schools, with perfectly honourable
intentions. But the end-products, particularly in the three-point scale
format preferred by one school, give me grave cause for concern.
Children are to be rated, for example, on
Sociability (a) loner/avoids others (1)
(b) normal (2)
(c) friendly/enjoys/seeks company (3)
81
Assessing Children’s Learning
Alphabet/reading skills
(a) no knowledge (1)
(b) knowledge of letters out of sequence (2)
(c) phonetic alphabet ‘parrot fashion’ (3)
(d) recognises isolated letters (4)
(e) reads simple words (5)
82
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
programme. This curriculum has been developed on Piagetian principles
with the concept of ‘active learning’ at its core (Hohmann et al. 1979).
The curriculum is conceptualised not in terms of future events, aims and
objectives that might be realised, or targets that might be achieved, but
in the present tense, the here and now of each child’s experience. The key
experiences shown on the record sheet represent nine areas of learning
that together constitute the young child’s developing cognitive and
affective powers. Every child’s learning can, it is claimed, be fostered by
the provision of resources and activities in each of these nine areas of
experience.
The High/Scope classroom or nursery is laid out in clearly defined
areas, each of which is well provided with materials selected with ‘key
experiences’ in mind. The educators’ role during the ‘work time’ period of
each High/Scope session is to observe and support children’s free and
unconstrained involvement in these activities, using the key experiences
as a framework for recognising significant evidence of each child’s
learning. They are not concerned with monitoring a child’s progress
along a preordained line from one to six, or from one to three; they are
alert to richer possibilities.
On this particular example of the High/Scope record keeping system
(figure 5.1), we can see how the educators have contributed anecdotal
evidence of Jinnie’s learning under the headings of the key experiences.
They are not constrained in their observations by the subject areas of the
National Curriculum, nor by the six areas of learning set out in the
Curriculum Guidance for the Foundation Stage (QCA 2000), but are free
to see the whole variety of ways in which Jinnie is learning to represent
her experiences to herself, to make sense of the world, its patterns and its
generalities, and to communicate her understanding to others. These
educators are recording learning in action, rather than performance on a
teacher-designed task, or achievement against a preconceived standard.
They are interested in seeing, and recording, how Jinnie puts her
learning to use, in her play, and in her relations with others. She has
learned, for example, to distinguish construction and mosaic blocks by
shape and colour; but her educators do not simply record her knowledge
of these attributes. They note that she uses the knowledge in a variety of
ways: to make comparisons, connections and patterns.
These detailed anecdotal records of children’s active learning are used in
a number of different ways. First and foremost, they constitute a
framework for coming to understand each child’s learning; but they serve
83
Assessing Children’s Learning
84
Pam Lafferty 1992 HIGH/SCOPE EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH FOUNDATION
High/Scope Endorsed Trainer Child Anecdotal Record (C.A.R.) (Condensed Sheet)
Child’s Name: Jinnie Birth Date: 16/9/86 (Remember to date all entries)
LANGUAGE REPRESENTATION CLASSIFICATION SERIATION NUMBER
3/9/91:- Said “Go Jo Flo 2/9/91:- Playing with the 7/10/91:- Stacked up the 11/9/91:- Remarked that 18/9/91:- Said “a few
Blow – they sound plastic animals and making brick piles into separate the new fruit bowl was means not a lot”
the same” the appropriate sounds for colours – red, blue, heavier than the old one
horse, cow and pig green and yellow 18/11/91:- Cut dough into
5/9/91:- J was sitting 27/9/1 :- Comparing four pieces and said “I’ve
looking at a story book 24/9/91:- Nailed two 12/11/91:- Said “My brushes said “Mine’s made four cakes”
and telling a story from pieces of wood together boots are red and yours larger and fatter than
the pictures in a cross shape and said are blue” yours” 6/12/91:- Counted 7
it was an aeroplane penguins accurately on a
3/12/91:- Whilst sitting 6/12/91:- Looking at the 17/12/91:- Chose a friend’s jumper
on a toilet seat said that 8/11/91:- Drew a robin words “Jack” and “Jake” number of triangle shapes
she was sitting on an with minute details and said that the two names from the box and
“O” coloured it in accurately were nearly the same, arranged them in order
with red just a little bit different of increasing size
SPACE TIME MOVEMENT SOCIAL/EMOTIONAL
23/9/91:- Talking to another 8/10/91:- When the tidy-up 12/9/91:- Was walking around 4/10/91:- On coming from the
child about a hat said “You sound was made, J began to put the nursery on all fours swaying garden to the inside, stopped in
need the ribbons at the back, things from the floor into their from side to side being an the doorway to leave muddy
not the side” correct baskets elephant wellingtons outside
30/10/91:- Folded a piece of card 28/11/91:- Said “If you want to 21/10/91:- For the first time 29/10/91:- Took a friend into the
in half to form a tunnel and then find me later, I’ll be in the Book managed to use her legs bathroom and used a cotton
walked plastic animals through it Area” to make the swing go wool ball to wipe mud from his
and used the word “through” knee
29/11/91:- Looked at a list of 13/12/91:- Used scissors to cut
9/12/91:- Noticed a triangular names on the board and said a “fringe” along the side of a 12/12/91:- At snack time said
patch of light on the carpet and “It will be Matthew’s turn to piece of paper “G only likes bananas – please
found a triangle shape to fit open the door tomorrow” save one for him”
exactly on top of it
Figure 5.1 High/Scope Child Anecdotal Record
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
other purposes too. They are used for planning provision that supports and
extends that learning, as the basis for staff discussion about each child, and
as the starting point for talking to parents about their children’s learning.
In this chapter I have described a number of different approaches to
that part of the assessment process in which educators try to understand
children’s learning. What these approaches have in common is a focus on
learning as it happens, on the process, rather than on the performance,
or the products of the learner. Athey, Paley, Egan, and the High-Scope
educators are all concerned with the internal, dynamic thinking
processes that they discern as they look at children and try to understand
them. The products of children’s learning – their dramas, or block
constructions, or dictated stories – contribute to the process of
assessment only in so far as they provide concrete evidence of the
abstract, mental processes that are these authors’ main concern.
The daily assessment practice of primary teachers in crowded
classrooms is much less likely to sustain this emphasis on process rather
than on product. In classrooms, children’s thinking is tantalisingly
transient, fast-moving, elusive; busy teachers understandably turn to
products as evidence of teaching and learning. Inevitably, once children
have learned to write independently, and to complete written
mathematical tasks on their own, there is a tendency to regard written
work as good enough evidence of what children are learning. However
the judgements that can be made on the basis of this kind of evidence are
unlikely to be as worthwhile – for teacher or for pupil – as judgements
that draw on other sources of evidence as well. Teachers who investigate,
for example, children’s mathematical thinking, by listening to their
pupils’ thinking aloud, explaining their calculations, very quickly
discover that apparent errors in the written record of those calculations
are often the result of systematic mathematical reasoning, which has
been mistakenly applied. Patrick Easen describes a number of children
who appear to be experiencing difficulty in learning mathematics. Dean,
for example, aged 12, writes on his paper:
6591
– 2697
——
4106
‘Wrong yes, but a freak answer no . . . Dean always subtracts the smaller
number from the larger number. He has an intuitive sense that this is
what subtraction is all about.’ (Easen 1987, p.28.)
85
Assessing Children’s Learning
Developing this point, Easen describes Paul (aged nine) who had
trouble with multiplication. Asked to multiply 148 by six he gave the
answer as 632. In asking him to talk through the calculation his teacher
learned, first, that he worked from left to right, and secondly that he
could multiply (he knew the products of 6 x 4 and 6 x 8), but thirdly that
his system for representing intermediate calculations sometimes led him
astray. In his final calculation he adds 8 (from the 48 derived from 6 x 8)
to 24 (from 6 x 4) to achieve the 32 of his written answer. With spoken
numbers Paul is operating well; with written numbers his invented,
systematic procedures let him down. But his errors are not random. No
more are the errors of Andrew, a seven-year-old boy who was the subject
of a case-study by Maggie Ellis, an Advanced Diploma student at the
Cambridge Institute of Education. In a tape recorded session, she asked
him to complete the written sum:
11+
7
He wrote 29 as the answer. She asked: ‘Would you tell me how you did it?’
A: I just mm – well I got to 1 or 1 um, 1 um, add them two together and that
makes 2. I put 2 down there, counted another 7, then, then add it to 2 – 9.
His difficulties with subtraction (on the same sheet he has written:
46 –
21
—
10)
are much harder to explain. But after some time using Lego bricks, one
possible explanation emerged.
To work out 13 – 5 he set out his Lego bricks in two rows like this:
■ ■ ■ ■ ■ ■ ■ ■ ■
■ ■ ■ ■
A: Then which one shall I take away, that one or that one? (points to each
row in turn).
M: What do you mean, Andrew?
A: Take away, take away 5.
M: Yes, take away 5.
A: Well, this one ’cos this is the longest (points to top row of bricks).
M: How many are left?
86
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
A: (He writes 8 as the answer.)
87
Assessing Children’s Learning
88
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
teacher indicates, however unwittingly, that their first response was
incorrect. We have no chance of learning about children’s learning if our
questions focus on their performance in a highly predictable question
and answer routine. We must not underestimate children’s ability to
divine the required answer without any mental activity corresponding to
the learning we believe we are assessing.
Equally, we must not pretend to ourselves that all children are
prepared to play the question and answer game according to the
teacher’s rules. There are plenty of free-thinking young children willing
and able to sabotage our careful interrogations with fantastic
interruptions of their own. My observation of another Science Standard
Assessment Task in the trialling period in 1991, which was designed to
assess knowledge and understanding of life processes, provides an
illustration. A group of children had been asked to record, by drawing,
‘the main stages of their life-cycle,’ showing themselves at the present
time, at babyhood, as they grow up, and having grown old.
Sheila is seven years and two months and has drawn three very schematic
human figures, and has labelled them one, four and seven. As she draws and
colours, she keeps up a cheerful running commentary. ‘Now I’ve got to do my
eyes. My eyes are brown. Very delicate to colour isn’t it?’ (writes) ‘Look that
makes a hundred and forty-seven doesn’t it? I’m four there. Now I’m
colouring this one. When I was at playgroup, I used not to let the kids go
down the slide. Now I’m going to do when I’m big . . . ’ (draws figure with a
much larger head) ‘. . . That’s how big I’m going to be . . . I’m bald there! How
old will I be when I grow up? I’m going to be 30.’ I asked Sheila if she knew
anyone bald. ‘Yes, my Donny, he’s got a little bald hair . . . Now I’m going to be
80!’ (starts a new figure). ‘My Donny is 82, nearly 83.’ So far, so normal. But
at this point, as far as the teacher was concerned, the assessment process
went off the rails. Sheila began to embellish her drawing: ‘Here’s my ears!
I’m going to be a rabbit when I grow up!!’ (excited giggling to herself).
‘There’s my big legs and my shorter arms. I’ve been to Disneyland . . . Look
when I’m 80 . . . This is my big tummy. My brother’s ten. He’s at a middle
school. I’m going to be eight on Feb 7th.’ (Adds feet, prompted by the teacher,
to the matchstick legs. Pauses, and draws four feet on each leg.)
89
Assessing Children’s Learning
required by the adult world’ but is not yet ‘committed to its burden of
rigid consistency’ (Paley 1981, p.81). We might also consider the
possibility that Sheila is using the words ‘going to be’ in two different
senses at one and the same time. Like any good surrealist, she is not
alarmed by ambiguity and contradiction, and so is quite prepared to use
the same form of words to refer to biological ageing (‘I’m going to be
eight on Feb 7th’) and to her own imaginative inner world (‘I’m going to
be a rabbit’). What a child is ‘going to be’ is not, in the eyes of the child,
simply a matter of physical maturation, but also a question for the
imaginative will. (Today, perhaps, or tomorrow, I’m going to be a rabbit,
a pirate, a robber, a princess. Or perhaps I’m going to be shot by a wolf.)
But Sheila’s teacher had been instructed to attend to her scientific, not
her fantastic, thinking, and she was, understandably, uncertain how to
record this child’s level of achievement. The Teacher’s Handbook she was
following did not allow for rabbit’s ears or four pairs of feet. The
Handbook assumed that children’s responses would either meet the
specified criteria or not. There is no place, in this assessment task, for the
flight of fantasy with which Sheila enlivened an unpromising exercise, a
task which was, as we have seen, well within her understanding. Sheila’s
non-conformity illustrates the folly of setting our sights too narrowly. We
cannot control the openness with which children may respond to the
deliberately closed questions we set them.
These examples from the trialling period of the first statutory
assessments of six- and seven-year-olds demonstrate the difficulties, and
the dangers, of product-focused assessment. The dangers of the whole
range of statutory assessment procedures, as defined in the 1988
Education Reform Act, were promptly and meticulously defined by
Blenkin and Kelly (1992). Time has passed, and many primary teachers
have come to see Key Stage One and Key Stage Two SATs as taken-for-
granted events in the school year: stressful, often and sometimes
superfluous, but an inescapable fact of life. This familiarity, however,
should not blind us to the weaknesses of the whole SATs approach. We
can, if we choose, hold on to our earlier conviction that assessment that
focuses on the learner, and the process of learning, rather than on the
product or the performance, while it may not satisify the demands of the
QCA for precision and accountability, will pay other dividends. The
judgements that teachers make in assessing learning are a vital
expression of their responsibilities to the learners in their care. To restrict
the scope of such judgements is to restrict the possibility of acting with
90
Ways of Seeing: Trying to Understand
full responsibility. Teachers have the right to make informed judgements
about learning and about learners. They therefore have a responsibility
to construct and develop frameworks of understanding within which to
make these judgements. Pupils have the right to expect their teachers to
act wisely, in the pupils’ interests, as a result of the judgements they
make. Assessment practices that do not recognise these rights and
responsibilities are not in the interests of pupils. In effective assessment,
the teacher’s ability to see and understand, the teacher’s acts of making
meaning, are all at the service of the pupils, not of the national bodies to
whom primary teachers are now held accountable.
91
CHAPTER 6
Understanding Ourselves
92
Understanding Ourselves
remains unclouded while theirs is partial, confused, incomplete.’ But a
more parsimonious explanation would refer to the hidden value systems
of all those who disagree.
For example, when an adult sees a baby sucking a dummy, a variety of
responses is possible. When I show a photograph of a baby sucking a
dummy to a group of early years educators and ask them what they see,
typical responses include:
● a baby who is being comforted
● a baby who is neglected
● a baby who is spoiled
● a baby whose language development may be impaired
None of these interpretations is necessarily right or wrong; all of these
judgements tell us something about the speakers’ values, and give us
some insight into their expectations of very young children.
During the development and trialling of the multi-disciplinary pack
Making Assessment Work (Drummond et al. 1992) the authors showed
photographs of children in a variety of circumstances to groups of early
years educators in order to stimulate an enquiry into the value systems
that inform our impressionistic judgements. The photograph shown
below (figure 6.1) evoked strongly differentiated responses. For example,
two of my own colleagues commented variously:
Figure 6.1
93
Assessing Children’s Learning
94
Understanding Ourselves
Hiroki disrupted the group by rolling on the cards and putting them in
his mouth. And so the day goes on until:
during the free playground period that ends the day, Hiroki played gently
with a toddler and more roughly with some of the older boys. He was finally
picked up shortly before 6.00 by his father, making him one of the last
children to go home.
Tobin et al. (1989) p.21
95
Assessing Children’s Learning
what it feels like to hit someone and hurt them, and to be hit and be hurt,
that they learn to control this urge to fight, that they learn the dangers of
fighting and get it out of their system. (p.33)
The authors report that most Japanese educators with whom they
discussed this issue agreed that:
fighting is natural and has a place in the informal pre-school curriculum. For
example, Assistant Principal Kumagai of Senzan Yochien told us ‘as the year
progresses we put fewer and fewer toys out during free play time, to give
children additional opportunities to learn to share and to deal with the
conflicts which arise’. (p.33)
96
Understanding Ourselves
understanding ourselves. If we want to increase our understanding of
children’s learning, we will have to take into account the connection,
however deeply buried it may be, between the people we are and the
judgements we make.
Madeleine Grumet, the American feminist curriculum theorist, puts
the case even more strongly; she emphasises the closeness of the
connection between the people we are and the curriculum we teach. In
her memorable phrase: ‘we are the curriculum’ (1981). Fictional
stereotypes of teachers endorse Grumet’s argument. Mr Gradgrind is the
curriculum, in Grumet’s phrase, for Sissy Jupe and Bitzer in Dickens’
Hard Times. The novel opens with his famous declaration:
‘Now, what I want is, Facts. Teach these boys and girls nothing but Facts.
Facts alone are wanted in life. Plant nothing else, and root out everything
else. You can only form the minds of reasoning animals upon Facts: nothing
else will ever be of any service to them.’
The identity between person and curriculum is so close that the man’s
very name, Gradgrind, has become synonymous with a knowledge-heavy,
assessment-centred curriculum, and with the ‘empty vessel’ theory of
children’s learning. In Gradgrind’s school, the master surveys ‘the
inclined plane of little vessels then and there arranged in order, ready to
have imperial gallons of facts poured into them until they were full to the
brim.’ Mr Gradgrind’s own children are part of the system too.
There were five young Gradgrinds and they. . . had been lectured at from
their tenderest years; coursed like little hares. Almost as soon as they could
run alone, they had been made to run to the lecture-room. The first object
with which they had an association, or of which they had a remembrance,
was a large blackboard with a dry Ogre chalking ghastly white figures on it.
Not that they knew, by name or nature, anything about an Ogre. Fact
forbid! I only use the word to express a monster in a lecturing castle, with
Heaven knows how many heads manipulated into one, taking childhood
captive, and dragging it into gloomy statistical dens by the hair.
No little Gradgrind had ever seen a face in the moon; it was up in the
moon before it could speak distinctly. No little Gradgrind had ever learnt the
silly jingle, Twinkle, twinkle, little star; how I wonder what you are! No little
Gradgrind had ever known wonder on the subject, each little Gradgrind
having at five years old dissected the Great Bear like a Professor Owen, and
driven Charles’s Wain like a locomotive engine-driver. No Gradgrind had
ever associated a cow in a field with that famous cow with the crumpled
horn who tossed the dog who worried the cat who killed the rat who ate the
malt, or with that yet more famous cow who swallowed Tom Thumb: it had
97
Assessing Children’s Learning
never heard of those celebrities, and had only been introduced to a cow as a
graminivorous ruminating quadruped with several stomachs.
When Mr Gradgrind finds his two eldest children, Louisa and Tom,
peeping in at the back of the circus tent, his lofty astonishment is
complete: ‘I should as soon have expected to find my children reading
poetry.’ The child-like Gradgrind knows nothing of circuses or poetry.
And yet,
Mr Gradgrind, though hard enough, was by no means so rough a man as Mr
Bounderby. His character was not unkind, all things considered; it might
have been a very kind one indeed, if he had only made some round mistake
in the arithmetic that balanced it, years ago.
This description starts with the person; through the person of Dr Strong
we are shown the curriculum of his school. It is the man’s values, rather
than his pedagogy or his syllabus, that we are invited to admire.
Meanwhile, at Blimber’s Academy, poor little Paul Dombey is on the
receiving end of a different set of values: the ancient and classical.
They comprised a little English, and a deal of Latin – names of things,
declensions of articles and substantives, exercises thereon, and preliminary
rules – a trifle of orthography, a glance at ancient history, a wink or two at
98
Understanding Ourselves
modern ditto, a few tables, two or three weights and measures, and a little
general information. When poor Paul had spelt out number two, he found he
had no idea of number one; fragments whereof afterwards obtruded
themselves into number three, which slided into number four, which grafted
itself on to number two. So that whether twenty Romuluses made a Remus,
or hic haec was troy weight, or a verb always agreed with an ancient Briton,
or three times four was Taurus a bull, were open questions with him.
‘Oh, Dombey, Dombey!’ said Miss Blimber, ‘this is very shocking. ‘
Miss Blimber expressed her opinions on the subject of Paul’s uninstructed
state with a gloomy delight, as if she had expected this result, and were glad
to find that they must be in constant communication. Paul withdrew with
the top task, as he was told, and laboured away at it, down below; sometimes
remembering every word of it, and sometimes forgetting it all, and
everything else besides: until at last he ventured up stairs again to repeat the
lesson, when it was nearly all driven out of his head before he began, by Miss
Blimber’s shutting up the book, and saying, ‘Go on, Dombey!’
It was hard work, resuming his studies, soon after dinner; and he felt
giddy and confused and drowsy and dull. But all the other young gentlemen
had similar sensations, and were obliged to resume their studies too, if there
were any comfort in that. It was a wonder that the great clock in the hall
never said, ‘Gentlemen, we will now resume our studies,’ for that phrase was
often enough repeated in its neighbourhood. The studies went round like a
mighty wheel, and the young gentlemen were always stretched upon it.
That wheel is of human design, and there are human hands turning it;
the energy that propels it is derived from human values.
In order to understand learning, we build theories about learning; we
try to delineate the characteristic features of our pupils’ learning in a way
that helps us to make sense of the bewildering complexities of the
classroom. I am arguing here that we need to do more: that we need to
think about teachers as well as learners. To understand learning, we must
also try to understand teaching; to understand a particular child’s
learning, we must try to understand that child’s teacher; we must try to
understand ourselves, the people we are, and the values we hold most
precious.
Most primary teachers of my generation, and many of those who
started teaching in the 1970s and early 1980s, would include in a
description of themselves, the people they are, some reference to the
Plowden Report (CACE 1967). Suddenly revived from the history books
in the winter and spring of 1991–2, when a fierce debate about primary
education raged in the popular (and the professional) press, the Plowden
Report appeared to have created two distinct types, indeed stereotypes,
99
Assessing Children’s Learning
100
Understanding Ourselves
it mean to be a ‘Plowden teacher’, in terms of beliefs about children,
about learning, about the purposes of education?
Witnesses to the Plowden Committee were asked about their
perception of the aims of primary education; there was ‘a wide general
measure of agreement’ but, the Report continues: ‘general statements
of aims, even by those engaged in teaching, tend to be little more than
expressions of benevolent aspiration’ with only a tenuous relationship
to specific educational practices (497). The Committee adopted instead
‘a pragmatic approach to the purposes of education’ (501). Their first
conclusion, in a paragraph that was first famous and later notorious,
as the epitome of Plowdenism, was that schools must transmit values.
The values that the Plowden Report is advocating are set out plainly
enough:
505. A school is not merely a teaching shop, it must transmit values and
attitudes. It is a community in which children learn to live first and foremost
as children and not as future adults. In family life children learn to live with
people of all ages. The school sets out deliberately to devise the right
environment for children, to allow them to be themselves and to develop in
the way and at the pace appropriate to them. It tries to equalise
opportunities and to compensate for handicaps. It lays special stress on
individual discovery, on first hand experience and on opportunities for
creative work. It insists that knowledge does not fall into neatly separate
compartments and that work and play are not opposite but complementary.
A child brought up in such an atmosphere at all stages of his education has
some hope of becoming a balanced and mature adult and of being able to
live in, to contribute to, and to look critically at the society of which he forms
a part. Not all primary schools correspond to this picture, but it does
represent a general and quickening trend.
CACE (1967)
101
Assessing Children’s Learning
And we see again an emphasis on values, on the belief system that guides
the teacher in wise decision-making in the interests of children: ‘our
whole conception of . . . the teaching of the infant school depends upon the
teacher; it will be successful in so far as she has faith in the underlying
principles, and confidence in her ability to interpret them’ (p.146).
Like Plowden, the Hadow Report acknowledges its debt to earlier
educators, and to established practice and ‘normal procedure’ in many
schools. But it makes one important claim to innovation: ‘What we do
102
Understanding Ourselves
desire to see is the acceptance of a different set of values from that which
has been usual in the past’ (p.123). Here is an explicit recognition of the
value-infested nature of educational debate and development. The shared
values of the Hadow Committee can be traced back through Froebel and
Montessori to Pestalozzi and Robert Owen; equally, the influence of
Susan Isaacs, with her interest in children’s intellectual and emotional
powers, is everywhere apparent. But a set of values does not amount to
an orthodoxy, let alone a stifling orthodoxy.
The Hadow inheritance, handed down through Plowden, is still a
powerful if unacknowledged influence on teachers and teachers’
thinking. But just as Plowden identified the tenuous relationship
between rhetoric and reality (‘It was interesting that some of the
headteachers who were considered by HM Inspectors to be most
successful in practice were least able to formulate their aims clearly and
convincingly’ (p.497)), so other educators have described the gap
between teachers’ beliefs and their ability to embody those beliefs in
well-constructed models of teaching and learning.
Brian Simon, for example, argues that the pedagogic romanticism of
Plowden has had far-reaching and damaging consequences:
By focusing on the individual child (‘at the heart of the educational process
lies the child’), and in developing the analysis from this point, the Plowden
Committee created a situation from which it was impossible to derive an
effective pedagogy (or effective pedagogical means). If each child is unique,
and each requires a specific pedagogical approach appropriate to him or her
and to no other, the construction of an all-embracing pedagogy, or general
principles of teaching, becomes an impossibility. And indeed research has
shown that primary school teachers who have taken the priority of
individualisation to heart find it difficult to do more than ensure that each
child is in fact engaged on the series of tasks which the teacher sets up for
the child.
Simon (1985) p.98
103
Assessing Children’s Learning
104
Understanding Ourselves
Teachers who do not, themselves, ask – and answer – ‘Why?’ questions,
will not be able to lead parents towards a full understanding of their
purposes. This is not to say that parents do not postulate their own
explanations:
Many Asian mothers were baffled by the purpose of all the equipment
except for books, although sometimes they produced ingenious explanations
for them: ‘Sand – all English people like the seaside; as it is so far away, they
bring some to school, to remind them of the beach.’
Hughes et al. (1980) p.193
105
Assessing Children’s Learning
classroom, by this criterion, is one in which things are learned every day
which the teacher did not previously know. (p.37)
106
Understanding Ourselves
work of teachers in classrooms, and emphasises both the mastery and
aspirations of the practising artist . . .
if my words are inadequate, look at the sketchbook of a good artist, a play in
rehearsal, a jazz quartet working together. That, I am arguing, is what good
teaching is like. (p.97)
This learning by the teacher, the teacher who is also artist and
researcher, is, for Stenhouse, at the heart of effective education. The ‘good
classroom’ is where the teacher learns things every day: teachers in such
classrooms learn about children, about children’s learning, and, equally
importantly, about themselves.
Another, very different, educator resembles Stenhouse in his interest in
teachers’ self-knowledge. Bruno Bettelheim was, for many years, the
Director of the Orthogenic School in Chicago. In this school for severely
emotionally disturbed children, which was opened in 1944, the staff
adopted a mainly psychoanalytic approach, and Bettelheim (1950)
explicitly acknowledges their corporate debt ‘to the writings of Sigmund
and Anna Freud, John Dewey, August Aichhorn, and all those other great
educators from Comenius and Pestalozzi to the Buhlers, Montessori and
Piaget . . . ’ But, as he goes on to say, it is not simply that these educators
have important things to say about children: they also speak directly to
the teachers themselves. Their greatness lies in the ways ‘they have helped
us to understand children and above all to understand ourselves’ (p.22).
For Bettelheim, the task of learning to work with the emotionally
disturbed children at the School, is a complex one. Staff members have ‘to
learn to become part of a purposeful unity and also what may seem still
more difficult – to preserve, at the same time, their personal uniqueness.
In a word, they must learn to become more themselves . . . ’ (p.22).
For teachers on the staff of the Orthogenic School, this growing self-
knowledge includes learning some attitudes that are very different from
those of many other adults in contemporary society. For example, the
107
Assessing Children’s Learning
staff of the School allow the children the freedom to waste and destroy
food and other materials, and they approve of infantile activities that are
supposedly below the child’s age level (such as playing with stuffed
animals and thumb sucking). They believe that children’s ‘emotional
needs take precedence over material considerations’ (p.41). The core
value that these teachers express is the centrality of emotion in personal
growth and development; their own emotional growth and increasing
integration is a precondition of the children’s slowly regained confidence
and security. In Bettelheim’s thinking, as for Stenhouse, teachers’ growth
and development runs alongside children’s growth and development.
Similar values are strong in the important work of Janusz Korczak, the
Jewish-Polish doctor who introduced progressive orphanages, designed
as just communities, into Poland in the years before the second world
war. He is chiefly remembered today for his final act of sacrifice, leading
the children in his care out of the Warsaw ghetto and onto the trains
carrying them to the extermination camp at Treblinka. But his life’s work
with children has more to teach us than the catastrophic horror of his
death. Korczak’s life was committed to the children he cared for and
educated in a number of different settlements and institutions. He cared
for their physical welfare, and, much like Margaret MacMillan in the
slums of Bradford and Deptford a generation earlier, he clothed and fed
and deloused the children, bringing them from a state of physical
destitution to comparative healthiness. But his chief concern was with
their moral welfare. In 1910 he abandoned his career as a paediatrician
at the Children’s Hospital in Warsaw to work for the Orphans Aid
Society, who were just embarking on a project to set up an orphanage for
Jewish children in a poor working-class neighbourhood of Warsaw.
Korczak worked with architects and philanthropists to design and build
the orphanage, which he saw as a children’s republic.
The underlying philosophy of the children’s republic was: children are
not the people of tomorrow, but people today. They are entitled to be taken
seriously. They have a right to be treated by adults with tenderness and
respect, as equals, not as masters and slaves. They should be allowed to grow
into whoever they were meant to be: the ‘unknown person’ inside each of
them is the hope for the future.
. . . In the process of working together, they would learn consideration and
fair play, and develop a sense of responsibility toward others, which they
would carry with them into the adult world. In helping his orphans to
respect others, a first step toward gaining self-respect, Korczak was a pioneer
in what we now call ‘moral education’. He was concerned not with teaching
108
Understanding Ourselves
children their ABCs – they would go to public school for that – but with the
grammar of ethics.
Lifton (1989) p.62
109
Assessing Children’s Learning
110
Understanding Ourselves
for adult teachers, understanding children. And this may be a good deal
more problematic than we would wish.
The slogans of the child-centred tradition, and the famous (or
infamous) one-liner from the Plowden Report (‘at the heart of the process
lies the child . . . ’) may have lulled us into thinking that the concept of ‘the
child’ is self-evidently simple, that when we talk about ‘the child’ or
‘children’, we have no more to do than see what there is to see, and say
what there is to say. But there is, as I argued earlier, always more to see,
always more to learn.
Valerie Walkerdine (1984) writes convincingly of the damage done to
teachers’ thinking by the reified concept of ‘the child’, as if there was only
one, an amalgam of Piagetian stage-theory, Froebelian growth and
spontaneity, and a dash of noble savagery. This universal, all-purpose
‘child’ is, she argues, a ‘powerful but impossible fiction’. She urges us to
clear out of our conceptual cupboards all the constructions that
educationalists have based on this fiction.
It is not just the singular ‘child’ who is a problematic figure in our
thinking. The concept ‘children’, too, we can recognise as a cultural
artefact, which at any given historical – or educational – period, has
particular meanings and implications. Our perceptions of children, of
child-like children, are coloured by our biographies, by our schooling, by
our religious, psychological and political beliefs, by the attitudes of those
around us, by the professional demands made on us in schools and
classrooms, by the hidden curriculum of contemporary schooling.
A rose may be a rose, but children are not children. Children are a
heterogeneous crowd of unique individuals, onto whom we project our
understanding of what it is to be four – or seven – or 11 years old.
Teachers’ perceptions of their pupils are shaped, first of all, by some
simple contingencies of time and place. When I started teaching, four-
year-old children were, first and foremost, of pre-statutory age: very few
of them were to be found in infant or primary schools. Some were in
nursery schools and classes: self-evidently pre-school children, in pre-
school education, which is based on play, self-directed activity and
independent choice. By the mid-1980s, most four-year-olds, for a variety of
reasons, were admitted to primary or infant schools at some point during
the academic year in which they turned five. They were then known as the
reception class, then formally as Year R, and from 1988, while still of non-
statutory age, they began their education within the provisions and
structures of the National Curriculum.
111
Assessing Children’s Learning
112
Understanding Ourselves
psychological processes take place. Their new paradigm emphasises that
childhood is socially constructed; biological immaturity is indeed a
universal and natural feature of human groups, but ways of
understanding childhood and children vary from place to place, from
time to time, from culture to culture.
In Visions of Infancy (1989) Ben Bradley sets out to ‘probe the
foundations of child psychology by looking at what scientists say about
babies’. He attempts to account for differences between various scientific
studies of infancy (such as Darwin’s and Freud’s, as well as Piaget’s and
Chomsky’s) by the personal and historical conditions of those who
conduct them. One of the most arresting sections of the book is an
illustrated description of the large centrally heated box, with a picture
window, in which the behaviourist Skinner placed his second daughter
for much of her childhood. There are comparable surprises in a series of
papers, edited by Alison James and Alan Prout, Constructing and
Reconstructing Childhood (1990), that range from Disneyland to the
ghetto streets of Asuncion, the capital city of Paraguay, in search of new
insights into the culturally specific sets of ideas and practices that
constitute working theories of childhood.
These papers by professional psychologists and sociologists may seem
remote from the interests of practising primary teachers. An account of
children’s involvement in the labour force in Norwegian village
communities, or a study of children’s daily life as patients on a terminal
cancer ward are, at first sight, unlikely to be the stimulus for a teacher’s
critical enquiry into children learning in a British classroom. The
arresting images of children around which some of these authors weave
their arguments may even serve to reinforce a taken-for-granted sense of
normality in our professional perceptions of the children whom we
educate. The unforgettable photograph of Skinner’s daughter in the ‘Heir
Conditioner’ box will certainly astonish early years educators in Britain
today; surely, no-one in their senses would ever consider housing a naked
two-year-old in a centrally heated box. But this justifiable astonishment,
reinforced by resistance to Skinner’s forceful advocacy of his own
invention, may prevent us feeling a comparable astonishment at any of
our own practices in early childhood care and education.
I am arguing that when, as teachers, we look at children’s learning and
seek to understand it, our understanding of children is an important part
of the picture. A description of the normal child, or the ideal child, or the
child-like child, is not usually made explicit in the process of assessment.
113
Assessing Children’s Learning
114
Understanding Ourselves
affecting each child and all children, their learning and their
development, for good or ill. We may be able more completely to
understand this process if we examine our beliefs about children, as well
as our beliefs about effective pedagogy.
A poem by Vladimir Nabokov, ‘An Evening of Russian Poetry’, depicts
the poet lecturing to an audience of schoolgirls, who madden him with
their insensitive questions.
Why do you speak of words,
When all we want is knowledge, nicely browned?
Nabokov (1977)
115
Assessing Children’s Learning
Amenability
1. Wishes to please and is most willing.
2. Helpful and cooperative fairly often.
3. Over anxious to please.
4. Frequently uncooperative and unresponsive.
5. Refuses actively to cooperate.
116
Understanding Ourselves
Application
1. Active, applies himself reasonably well.
2. Willing but needs constant prodding.
3. Easily distracted by surroundings and events.
4. Cannot apply himself – restless, overactive.
5. Inactive, slow, ineffective, helpless.
DEVELOPING SKILLS
1 2 3 4 5
7. Gross Motor Movement
Clumsy, uncoordinated, Developing coordination, Well coordinated, agile,
Tends to fall over or no apparent problems. responds quickly.
bump into things.
8. Fine Coordination and
Manipulation
Experiences great
difficulty in manipulation Appropriate competence. Dextrous, highly competent.
– small objects, pencils.
9. Drawing
Very limited development Recognizable. Usually Very well executed, great
Drawing or scribble self-explanatory. attention to detail, little
barely recognizable. explanation needed.
Would need explanation.
APPROACH TO LEARNING
14. Attention and
Concentration
Very short attention span. Can settle to a set task Works well on set task for
Unable to concentrate, for a short period. a long span. Not easily
easily distracted. distracted.
15. Organization
Disorganized, rarely Can organize self with Is capable of organizing
completes set task, some additional self with no additional
confused by expectations. assistance. assistance.
117
Assessing Children’s Learning
16. Involvement
Avoids learning. Few or Willing to become Keen, enthusiastic, with
no interests. involved in most activities. wide range of interests.
17. Type of Response
Little or no response Works steadily, acceptable Responds quickly, and
without supervision. progress being made. accurately, enjoys
demonstrating new skills.
Since it seems unlikely that the teachers who wrote these descriptions
were at heart anything but well-intentioned towards their prospective
pupils, the best way of explaining the negative values that are here being
projected onto some children is by invoking these teachers’ own
experiences as children, and as pupils, which may have been very
damaging. They themselves may not have been treated respectfully, with
trust and loving attention. We need not, however, in sympathising with
them, go on to adopt their approach.
Assessment practices, I am arguing throughout this book, are based on
the personal value systems of the teachers who make the assessments. I
have given some examples here that reveal aspects of teachers’ value
systems that are not in the interests of children. A value system that
rejects young children’s tears as immature is not respectful of children’s
grief. A value system that interprets a child’s lack of interest in an
assigned task as evidence of possible learning difficulties is mistrustful
of children’s appetite and capacity for learning. In effective assessment,
which works for children, we draw on a coherent set of values that are
truly respectful and trustful. We may need, in the process, to struggle to
understand our own past experiences, and if necessary, outlive them, in
the interests of the children we work with today. If our well-intentioned
assessment procedures reveal, on close inspection, an element of
rejection, disrespect or retribution, we have work to do in reaffirming the
basic human virtues that our adult lives are intended to express. The
moral dimension of assessment is inescapable, however nicely browned
the knowledge we are choosing to assess.
118
CHAPTER 7
Trying to Understand:
Making it Work
119
Assessing Children’s Learning
With this distinction in mind, we can see how defining and designing
assessment procedures with purposes in mind, as the TGAT Report did,
so helpfully, at the time, may prevent us from seeing the undesirable
outcomes of particular practices. For example, the statutory assessments
that are now a taken-for-granted event in the educational histories of six-
and seven-year-old children, have, we are repeatedly assured, entirely
beneficial and laudable purposes. Anxiety about statutory forms of
assessment at the end of the Key Stage One should not be allayed by
these assurances, because it is the outcomes of the procedures that are
potentially damaging. Statutory assessment, including the requirement
to ascribe pupils’ achievement to numerical levels, is intended to serve all
four of the purposes proposed by TGAT. But there may be other
outcomes. Assigning young children to one of three (or four) levels of
achievement has had, as we have now seen, the very undesirable
outcome of creating a new category of underachievement: the ‘Level One
Child’. The purpose of compiling numerical data on every child’s
performance on each attainment target may be to achieve accuracy and
objectivity (which may well prove to be illusory); but the outcome may
be a rigid stratification of pupils based on the string of numbers they
carry round with them in their record folders and that go forward to be
used in league tables, and in setting targets for future years.
Kenneth Clarke, Secretary of State for Education and Science at the
time, went so far as to identify this possibility as a welcome outcome of
National Curriculum assessment, and to recommend it as a desirable
practice, with undisputed benefits.
For many schools, there must be scope for organising their teaching in
classes grouped more closely with their attainments in the subjects of the
National Curriculum. We have introduced a clear framework of assessment
and monitoring such performance. We must use the benefits which that
framework provides.
DES (1991b) para 36
120
deficiencies in the school’s curriculum. To judge assessment practices by
121
Assessing Children’s Learning
Child: The elephant is the biggest animal and the mouse is the smallest.
Father: The mouse isn’t the smallest.
Child: No . . . it’s the snail.
Father: There are still smaller ones.
Child: The flea is of course the smallest.
Father: There are still smaller ones but you don’t know them. They live in
water.
Child: I know them but I don’t know what they are called.
If this conversation took place in class, the teacher might decide to ask
questions on where elephants live, how a snail protects itself or what kind of
food a mouse eats. These questions may interest the child and they are worth
asking, but they have a ‘hit-or-miss’ quality. The responses the father gave
show that he is aware of the prevailing concern of the child, which is size. A
size continuum is an invariant cognitive structure that links individual
objects in the world with each other. Schemas and concepts facilitate a
cognitive organization of disparate content.
Athey (1990) pp. 42–3.
The task for the educator attuned to children’s cognitive schemas then,
is to use the knowledge gained through observation and monitoring of
children’s spontaneous activity to good effect. Athey predicts that a
curriculum for young children that was shaped in this way would be
greatly more stimulating and challenging than the arbitrary content-led
curriculum of the traditional nursery teacher, who selects topics and
themes for their surface features or for their association with the time of
122
year. Tadpoles, fire-stations, pancakes and hedgehogs are all typical items
123
Assessing Children’s Learning
Without exception, the children complained about how stupid the stories in
their basic readers had been, and said how much they had hated having to
read them. With venom they spoke of ‘all those sweet little kids in the
stories,’ furious that the stories assumed they were so simple-minded as to
believe that children were like that . . . When asked why they had not
expressed their opinions about these readers before, the answer was that
nobody was interested in their true thoughts on this matter; everybody
wanted only to hear that they liked the stories. One mature fourth-grade girl
remarked: ‘In none of the stories does anybody ever say his true opinion, so
how could we?’ (pp.14–16)
124
inadequate father, this had reminded the girl of the ways in which her own
125
Assessing Children’s Learning
The authors’ case is a compelling one. They have shown us that, when we
choose, if we listen carefully, and take children seriously, we can learn
from the children themselves what it is important for us to know about
their learning. If the outcomes of assessment are to include an enhanced
curriculum and improved learning opportunities for all children, we
would be foolish to exclude their voices from our selection of evidence
on which to work. What children can tell us about their learning and
about the provision we make for that learning has an important part to
play in making assessment work.
Bettelheim’s interest in children’s emotional involvement in their
reading material is an important reminder that cognitive growth is only
one aspect of learning. Athey’s preoccupation with cognitive form should
not persuade us that cognitive form is all there is. Other authors
emphasise the connectedness of intellect and emotion, of thought and
feeling. Winnicott (1964), for example, writing of children’s play, and of
how children enrich themselves through their play, gradually enlarging
‘their capacity to see the richness of the externally real world’, identifies
the emotions associated with play. It is easy to see that children play for
pleasure but, Winnicott argues, it is harder to accept that children play to
master anxiety: ‘Anxiety is always a factor in a child’s play, and often it is
a major factor’ (p.144). This insight suggests that the emotional set
associated with learning is not one of unadulterated pleasure, but
includes some element of dissatisfaction, of internal disturbance. Piaget
used the phrase ‘cognitive dissonance’ to describe the internal state that
drives the processes of accommodation and assimilation; we can safely
assume that cognitive dissonance has a corresponding emotional effect
on those who experience it.
Building on Athey’s work, a small-scale study by Janet Shaw (1992)
suggests that a description of young children’s cognitive schemas,
identified through close observations of their patterns of thought and
activity, is incomplete if it excludes the emotional and symbolic
functions of children’s behaviour. Shaw’s account of young children’s
learning is multi-layered, showing how the outer form of children’s
activity, their words and actions, can best be understood as expressions
of cognitive, symbolic and emotional concerns, and not as simply one of
these, taken singly, in isolation. Shaw concludes that all cognitive activity
126
in young children has associated emotional form, and that young
127
Assessing Children’s Learning
Because one job you have to do is decide what we should learn next
and you have to do that by looking at our work and seeing what
we’re ready for next time.
Summative
How good we are at certain things.
So you know what people are good at.
If I’m good at reading.
Diagnostic
Like in maths, if I can’t do it, you know you have to show me again
how to do it.
Evaluative
So you can choose our project for next term.
So you can see if we need more certain types of books and things.
Informative
So you can let our parents know we are doing all right.
So you can tell our parents if were not getting on or if we’re
naughty.
to save embarrassment;
so people’s feelings aren’t hurt;
it might be unkind to tell someone they aren’t very good at something if
they think they are;
supposing it’s someone who’s a slow learner. Think how they would feel
if you said so.
What is remarkable in this discussion, is not that these eight- and nine-
year-olds are aware of the possible emotional impact of assessment; it is
that they regard that impact as inevitably negative. There are no
references to the motivating power of positive feedback, or any
recognition of the possibility that the assessment of achievement might
128
enhance children’s feelings of success and self-esteem. Their perception
This discussion surprised and alarmed the teacher. It seemed that for
more than half the class:
issues of identity are so blurred, that they must lack any reference point from
which to begin to think through, to evaluate and eventually define, their own
role in learning.
129
Assessing Children’s Learning
Prosser’s study, though small in scale, alerted her to ways in which her
children seemed to be losing out on possibilities for learning about
themselves; she was forced to consider what effect her assessments were
having on the children themselves, and their views of themselves:
The kind of image conflict apparent in Chris was later mirrored in other
conversations with individual children. Whatever the foundations for such
conflicts, however, the fact remains that more than half the class are beset by
confusions over self-image and identity which must sit very uncomfortably
on small shoulders . . . For all these children, such issues may impede or
inhibit learning.
She also noted that the children seemed to believe very firmly that
assessment was something that was done to them, by an adult. The
earlier discussions had showed how they thought this assessment was
best not revealed, and half the class had admitted that the nature of the
teacher’s assessments was unknown to them. To redress the balance in
favour of pupil involvement in assessment, she invited the class to devise
a self-assessment schedule, to which they gave the title, ‘What makes me,
ME?’ As they worked in small groups to determine the categories for this
schedule, Prosser was encouraged by the children’s motivation, interest
and awareness of the issues. But once individual pupils began to
complete the schedule in its agreed format, she was disagreeably
surprised to observe the pupils’ anxiety about whether they had fulfilled
the task ‘correctly’, asking the teacher several times if what they had
written down was ‘right’ or ‘true’.
My feelings were dramatically underlined by the following exchange:
David: Shouldn’t you write down on this if you agree with what we’ve
written?
MP: I don’t think so. It’s what you think that matters, not what I think.
David: But we might be wrong.
David has been learning, throughout his schooling, that whatever this
particular eccentric teacher may say about the importance of the pupil’s
point of view, it is, in the end, the teacher who distinguishes right
answers from wrong. In self-assessment, as in any classroom inquiry, the
130
pupil must defer to the teacher. Assessment practices that reinforce this
131
Assessing Children’s Learning
132
Once upon a time there was a little boy called Johnny and all he would do
Both Max and Daniel are confident that it is their favourite activity, and
Max knows why:
133
Assessing Children’s Learning
You can put a lot of life into drawings if you try. Then it feels as though it’s
really happening, if you get into drawing, it feels as though it’s really
happening.
Like Shazia’s play, and Katey’s writing, Max’s drawing is the domain
where his imagination makes things ‘really’ happen. School and home
are real too, of course, but when ‘you get into drawing, it’s really
happening’. Both Max and Daniel use writing and drawing as parts of the
same process, of building an imaginative world of their own.
Max: I draw my pictures before I start writing.
Daniel: Me too.
Max: So then I can write about my picture.
MR: Does it help you to write stories?
Max &
Daniel: Yes.
MR: Do you think you could write a story without drawings?
Daniel: I think I could – probably.
Max: I don’t think I could do it properly, without a drawing by the side.
134
writing, or Max and Daniel were forbidden to draw when they should be
135
Assessing Children’s Learning
136
Trying to Understand: Making it Work
Image Not Available
137
Assessing Children’s Learning
tickets and you (the teacher) must tell me how to write it. I want to write ‘You
can go on the slide’. (There has been a lot of ticket making /invitation writing
going on recently in child-initiated play activities.) The teacher writes out the
words at G’s dictation and G copies it (3) forming all the letters correctly.
Another child approaches – ‘Can I go on the slide?’
G: ‘I doing tickets. Wait! . . . ’ Another child tries to use the slide, ‘Wait Raji! . . .
Levi wait a minute!’
R tries again. G: ‘Raji! wait a minute . . . When I say “ready” everyone can go.
People can give the ticket back and more people can go on.’ She has finished
copying four tickets. She organises a ticket for each child waiting and stands
by the slide. G: ‘We’re ready.’ S, L, B and R get in line.
G: ‘Ticket. Slide down.’ She gives a ticket to M: ‘What does it say?’
G: ‘You can go on the slide.’
Unfortunately the play came to abrupt halt here with the appearance of a
tray of biscuits!
In this observation we can see how the teacher’s support for Guljeet’s
urgent purposes (writing the tickets) enabled her, in Vygotsky’s words ‘to
do with assistance today (what) she will be able to do by herself
tomorrow’ (p.87). The teacher did not conclude from her observation that
Guljeet could not write independently; on the contrary, she made a very
positive judgement about Guljeet’s developing mastery of the writing
system, including her ability to use adult support to achieve her goals.
She recognised in Guljeet’s dependence upon the adult writer what
Vygotsky calls the ‘buds’ or ‘flowers’, rather than the ‘fruits’ of
development. Her disposition to look to the future of Guljeet’s learning,
and not just at what she had or had not already learned, was firmly in
Guljeet’s interests.
Furthermore, this case-study illustrates how the adults who worked in
Guljeet’s classroom built on what they saw, using their understanding of
children’s learning to shape a curriculum that offered both food and
exercise to their developing powers. Children learning to write
independently were given many and varied opportunities to write
purposively, with support from others. Children learning to represent the
world around them in a variety of ways were given rich experiences of
different forms of representation. Children working for sustained
periods, with concentration and intensity on a single activity, were given
time to see their projects through to completion. Assessment worked for
the children in this classroom.
Making assessment work is a project that occupies teachers in a
number of different contexts: within the classroom, within the school as
138
a whole, within the parent/teacher partnership, within the process of
139
Assessing Children’s Learning
have been known to stress the importance of starting with a clean slate,
making up their own minds, taking it with a pinch of salt, and allowing
for exaggeration. They are likely to refer to their previous experience of
‘that’ nursery, ‘those’ children, and ‘some’ teachers.
Incidentally, there is some evidence that this is not a problem confined
to England and Wales; the report by HMI of practice in a small group of
French schools (DES 1991a) described the use of the Dossier Scholaire, a
cumulative record of each pupil’s achievements. Two conditions are
attached to its use: first, the parents of the pupils are not allowed to see
the Dossier, and, secondly, the receiving teacher is not allowed to read it
until after Christmas, that is, until after one-third of the school year has
already passed. The assessments recorded in the Dossier, then, can only
work for pupils in the remaining two-thirds of the school year. When the
receiving teacher does see the records, after Christmas, the impact and
relevance of what was written, at the very least, five months previously,
is likely to be minimal.
Practice in this country is not yet centrally regulated in this way, nor
perhaps so arbitrarily and absolutely fixed. But there are similarities
between the French approach and the English primary teacher who
insists on making up his or her own mind before studying pupils’ records
and assessments. The common factor is professional mistrust. The
assessments of other teachers, other educators, are treated with caution,
even with suspicion. The assumption seems to be that the judgements of
the receiving teachers, meeting a group of pupils for the first time, are
likely to be more accurate and better informed than the judgements of
teachers who have worked with these pupils over the previous academic
year. It is very difficult to see how such an assumption can be justified.
Assessment cannot be made to work for the benefit of pupils if
teachers mistrust one another’s judgements. There is little hope of
achieving continuity for each child’s learning if there is a break in
professional confidence at points of transfer, from year to year, or from
school to school. Assessment will only work for pupils when mistrust
gives way to trust, and when teachers treat each other’s judgements with
respect, confident that they can all contribute to a shared understanding
of children’s learning. Underlying effective practice in the transfer of
assessments and records will be a relationship between teachers based
on trust and respect. But these attitudes cannot be set in place at the
stroke of a pen on a policy document, or by an imperative from the
senior management team.
140
Trust and respect for each other’s judgements will only develop when
141
CHAPTER 8
142
before and the one after; the successive terms bring their own rituals, but
143
Assessing Children’s Learning
Practice can be reduced to the sum of its parts: the provision, the
timetable, the programmes of study, the traditions and expectations of a
particular time and place. The inertia-bound classroom is deeply
resistant to change and development. The ideals and aspirations of the
teachers who work there are somehow split off from the daily cycle of
events. We can represent life in such a classroom, even more simply, as
the relationship between three worlds:
Figure 8.2
144
accordingly, will spend every minute of his or her school week – week
PLAN – OR – REVIEW
looking ahead or looking back
good intentions or what actually happened?
‘wouldn’t-it-be-lovely’ or well, was it?
wishful thinking or factual descriptions
aspirations or analysis
theories or evidence
hope-for-the-best or real understanding
Figure 8.3
145
Assessing Children’s Learning
understand what and how our pupils learn, and then put that
understanding to good use.
There is still another element to be added to this analysis. I have
suggested that classroom practice should be driven by critical review as
much as by optimistic planning; but reviewing and planning cannot
reliably generate effective practice unless they are rooted in and
informed by principles, principles that have themselves been shaped by
our most deeply held values and beliefs. The extent to which we can
articulate our values, our sense of what we believe to be good and
worthwhile, have a direct bearing on the cycle of classroom events. Our
values can lead us, if we take the trouble to explore them and understand
them, to formulate the principles by which our classroom practices are to
be both planned and reviewed. Our on-the-spot choices and
instantaneous decisions during the hurly-burly of classroom events, can
be informed, if we so desire, by a framework of principles that express
our deepest understanding of the purposes of education. A
representation of this possibility is shown in figure 8.4.
Figure 8.4
146
This diagram represents a teacher’s core values, a teacher’s sense of
147
Assessing Children’s Learning
Figure 8.5
148
But Why?
But gradually the group began to use their stream of why questions to
force them to articulate their implicit understanding:
But Why?
But why do we say children are different at different times?
Because one-off assessments can be very misleading.
But Why?
But why do we think these assessments are misleading?
Because children are affected by changes in external factors – relationships,
environment, resources, everything around them.
But Why?
Because children are a product of their constantly changing environment.
And so . . . everything that educators do has an effect on children and their
learning.
And so . . . Assessment is part of everything that educators do . . .
And so . . . Assessment must work for the benefit of children.
149
Assessing Children’s Learning
example, they worked hard to uncover the core values that gave life to
and sustained their principles. The principle of parental involvement in
assessment, for example, was discussed at length, in an attempt to
discover why it was held to be so important; the concepts of partnership,
dependence and interdependence were examined. By the end of the
discussion a firm link had been established between what these teachers
believed to be centrally good in human relations, and their desire to
express the core values of respect and trust in the process of involving
parents in the practice of assessment. During the discussion the teachers
clarified and articulated the connection between their values and their
principles.
At a subsequent session of the same in-service course, the teachers and
nursery nurses were invited to describe one particular aspect of their
assessment practice. They brought with them to the session a variety of
formats and examples of their work: portfolios, tick-sheets, reading
records, annotated collections of children’s drawings, end-of-year reports,
and so on. They talked for some time about the ways in which they used
these formats, giving practical details and noting the benefits and
limitations of each practice. But the discussion came to an abrupt halt
when we tried to discern the principles that underlay the practices. After
a brief but awful silence, the group began to discuss, rather disconsolately,
their sobering awareness that there was no connection to be made
between the principles they had discussed so attentively the week before,
and the practices that were occupying them now. It was as if, like Mother
Hubbard, they had suddenly found ‘the cupboard was bare’.
After further discussion, and some mutual reassurance, this group of
educators came to see the urgency of establishing connections between
their inner and outer worlds. It was not that these educators lived
without values, or acted in unprincipled ways; the cupboard was not,
after all, as bare as Mother Hubbard found it. But they did appreciate that
they had neglected to establish and maintain a living link between belief
and practice, between ideal and reality, between aspiration and action.
They saw the inadequacy of a model of teaching and learning that is
confined to the practical domain, where practice and outcomes are split
from the educator’s idealism and conviction. They recognised the need to
reaffirm their belief in themselves as people with the power to think, as
well as people with the power to do.
At the heart of effective assessment, at the heart of worthwhile
teaching and a proper understanding of learning, is the power of teachers
150
to think, not just about pedagogical issues, but as moral beings. The
151
Assessing Children’s Learning
VALUE POLITICAL
What practices do I most Which practices do
value and believe in? others most/least
approve of?
CONCEPTUAL
EMPIRICAL
What is practice?
Which practices can What are its essential
be shown to be most elements?
effective in promoting
learning? PRAGMATIC
Which practices work
best (or do not work)
for me?
This model seems to me to miss the full meaning of the term ‘good’, and
to ignore its power to define and qualify the four other dimensions that
Alexander identifies. For example, the pragmatic consideration, ‘Which
practices work best for me?’begs the question of what it means for a practice
152
to ‘work’, let alone ‘work best’. The criteria by which such a judgement might
153
Assessing Children’s Learning
154
effective assessment. The values that underlay this principle were
155
Assessing Children’s Learning
156
CHAPTER 9
Rights, Responsibilities
and Power
157
Assessing Children’s Learning
colleagues had signed on for. The range of our responsibilities grew too,
as new areas of professional expectation opened up to us. Curriculum
developments over the last 30 years have massively increased the scope
of teachers’ responsibilities. Working with parents, working with
bilingual pupils, the introduction of micro-technology, the development
of multicultural and antiracist principles, the growth of health and sex
education, and developments in oracy and literacy: these concerns are
only a few examples of the steadily increasing extent of primary
teachers’ responsibilities.
During in-service courses, when the concept of responsibility is being
discussed, I often ask teachers and other educators to identify some
elements in the complexity of their work with children and families for
which they are not responsible; many of them find this very difficult.
They are so much more accustomed to listing the vast range of
responsibilities that they do shoulder, daily, that the suggestion that there
are areas of experience outside their personal responsibility is, for some,
extraordinary, and, for a few, quite unacceptable. One educator wrote:
‘The minute I say that something isn’t my responsibility, I begin to feel
guilty about it,’ and another commented, as the group discussed their
written replies, ‘You do feel that you are responsible for everything, even
though you know it’s impossible.’
It is, of course, entirely appropriate that teachers and other educators
should appreciate the full extent of their professional responsibility. But,
ironically, such an appreciation may sometimes act dysfunctionally,
making us less effective in meeting the responsibilities of which we are
so painfully aware. Some years ago a primary headteacher represented
this possibility to me very vividly; as a member of a group of teachers
studying for an Advanced Diploma in Educational Studies, she had been
invited to contribute to a group discussion by bringing in a photograph
or symbolic representation of some aspect of ‘being a teacher’. She
brought a full page spread from a tabloid newspaper, a dramatic aerial
photograph showing the ferry boat ‘The Herald of Free Enterprise’ lying
capsized in Zeebrugge harbour. This tragedy, still fresh in people’s
minds with its appalling loss of life and horrifying accounts of the
scenes on board, had, naturally, affected everyone in the group,
especially those who had recently travelled by ferry with their families.
But the connection with the world of the primary school seemed
obscure. We asked the headteacher to explain; her moving description
of her thinking in selecting this image centred on the concept of
158
Rights, Responsibilities and Power
responsibility, on the absolute and inexorable nature of the teacher’s
responsibility, which was, she suggested, ultimately quite as much a
responsibility for life and death as the ferry boat captain’s. She was not,
she explained, thinking only of the physical safety of the pupils in a
primary school, although teachers do, inevitably, take this part of their
responsibilities very seriously. The headteacher told the group that she
was, in a sense, haunted by her feelings of responsibility for the spiritual
safety of the pupils, and by her self-imposed commitment to their
educational well-being, and to every aspect of their human growth and
development.
Her views were listened to attentively and respectfully; no-one
attempted to persuade her that she was exaggerating. The group of
teachers seemed to accept the gravity of her insight, and to endorse her
expression of the seriousness of their chosen profession. The unexpected
image, so powerful a reminder of the fragility of human life and the
ubiquity of human error and failure, startled this group of teachers into
a fresh sense of the extent of their responsibilities. But as the discussion
continued, we became aware of the possible – even probable – cost of
this gain in understanding. We identified another imperative, which
would be needed to complement the concept of responsibility: the need
to affirm our ability to meet our responsibilities to the height of our
professional powers. If an awakened sense of responsibility were to
cause an increase in feelings of incompetence and powerlessness, our last
state would be worse than our first.
Looking back at my notes of this discussion after an interval of five
years, I saw a missed opportunity; I wished I had gone on to discuss
more openly with the group their feelings about this way of
conceptualising their power. Responsibility levies an emotional toll as
well as a professional one.
In subsequent work with early years educators from a variety of
settings, I have made up for that missed opportunity. Using a discussion
activity from the pack Making Assessment Work (Drummond, Rouse &
Pugh 1992), I have explored with teachers, headteachers and other
educators the cluster of concepts that concern us here: rights,
responsibilities and power. In an activity called Judge and Jury we
explore the distance, in terms of power, between the assessor and the
assessed, and the rights and responsibilities associated with this
relationship. The crude analogy, suggested by the title of the activity,
between the judge on the bench and the teacher in the act of assessment,
159
Assessing Children’s Learning
Figure 9.1
160
Rights, Responsibilities and Power
support and enable children. A smaller number of more emphatic replies
refer to the responsibility to empower, enhance, stimulate, stretch and
‘open doors’. The theme of the future is continued in reference to
children’s potential which is to be tapped into, developed, and extended.
Children’s emotional development is to be attended to, particularly their
self-confidence, which must be increased. A small minority of replies
refer to happiness as an important and desirable outcome of the teacher’s
professional responsibility (‘to make school happy’, ‘to give them a happy
start’). A larger minority focus on learning and opportunities for
learning, but a substantial majority do not include this concept in their
replies. The words ‘development’, ‘capabilities’, ‘abilities’ and ‘progress’,
which are frequently mentioned, can in a sense be read as substitutes or
synonyms for the word ‘learning’, but it is worth remarking that over 80
per cent of this sample do not immediately respond to a question about
their responsibility by referring to children’s learning.
This curious absence is reminiscent of the primary headteacher’s
remarks, so mercilessly recorded by Sharp and Green (1975) in Education
and Social Control:
We are here to look after the children – it’s their welfare that concerns me
and I’d put that as a priority. I don’t see the prime aim of the school as a
learning institution although I realize that I’m paid to do this . . . and I have
an obligation to see that my children become literate and numerate . . . but I
feel that the child himself, his well-being and his welfare must be our first
concern. (p.49)
161
Assessing Children’s Learning
task seem less overwhelming. Recognising that some children, like Jason,
though undoubtedly learning, do not learn what was intended by their
benevolent teachers, might sharpen our appreciation of our
responsibility to monitor the quality of each individual child’s learning,
asking ourselves persistently – ‘Is this learning worthwhile? In what
ways? How do I know?’ The concepts of quality and worthwhileness
were not mentioned by the teachers and educators in my sample, in their
responses to item no. 6 (figure 9.1), ‘As a teacher/educator, I have a
responsibility to . . . ’
The argument of this whole book has been that the act of assessment
requires us to see, to understand, and to apply our understanding of each
child’s learning. If we could be as acutely aware of specific, individual,
particular responsibilities as the respondents I have quoted seem to be of
vaster, more shadowy enterprises, the practice of assessment might,
perhaps, become more effective; perhaps teachers might feel more
confident and secure in their undertakings, and even communicate some
of their confidence and security to pupils like Jason. As it is, the feelings
that are described by my respondents as being part of their sense of
responsibility are serious and sombre. A very few find their
responsibilities exciting (but also daunting, according to one, and
panicky, according to another). The most positive response includes the
words ‘fulfilled, proud, happy and worried’; this worry is nearly
universal, in all its degrees, from a milder ‘anxiety’ and ‘concern’ to
feelings of ‘being overwhelmed’ by panic and fright.
Teachers’ feelings are strong on this issue, there is no doubt, and their
words are an encouraging reminder of the emotional strengths that
teachers bring to their work. Feelings of pride and humility, of
importance and inadequacy, feeling privileged and overawed, thoughtful,
reflective and pressurised, creative and doubtful, full of awe and wary:
these contradictions and tensions characterise the working lives of
teachers, and constitute, I would argue, part of the case for teachers’
emotional lives to be treated respectfully and seriously, throughout their
careers, by the whole educational community.
The responses to the fifth sentence, revealing teachers’ views of power,
are, for me, much less predictable, and give me even greater cause for
concern. These replies are overwhelmingly negative: there are references
to Hitler, Saddam Hussein (the 1991 Gulf War had just ended when I was
working with one group of educators), dictators, big brother, an overlord,
bureaucracies, armed forces, authority, oppression, inequality and
162
Rights, Responsibilities and Power
hierarchy. One teacher wrote: ‘The word power makes me think of
hierarchy’ with a footnote ‘. . . of which I am no way near the top.’ Another
writes: ‘Freedom taken from another individual, the victim of the power.’
A student teacher on teaching practice recorded: ‘Stone walls and
vulnerability.’ One headteacher wrote: ‘The word power makes me think
of “somebody else”,’ and another, ‘imposition, unreality, doom, force,
drive/driven, all enveloping.’
These responses are deeply disconcerting. If we as teachers are to act
responsibly, putting into practice our commitment, our moral obligation
to the interests of children, to children’s learning and development, then
I believe we must recognise and accept our power as teachers. We do
have the power to educate, for a better world, the children in our schools;
to deny this power is, by extension, to deny our real responsibilities to
children. We cannot fulfil our widest educational purposes, or even our
small-scale short-term projects for individual children, if we refuse to
accept that we do have the power to act in the interests of children.
What might be the reasons for teachers’ apparent readiness to disclaim
their own power? A student teacher does seem to me to be entitled to a
certain feeling of powerlessness, and hence, in her word, ‘vulnerability’.
But why a headteacher should use the words, ‘doom’, ‘force’ and
‘imposition’, is less immediately apparent. Hours of discussion, and
repeated readings of the completed sentences on the activity sheets, have
persuaded me that one of the reasons may be that these same teachers
have an undeveloped sense of their rights. Replies to the eighth and ninth
sentences have provided the evidence for this contention.
Some teachers openly and entirely disclaim the concept: ‘I do not feel
I have any rights.’ A headteacher wrote: ‘. . . I have the right to nothing. I
am paid to make decisions.’ Others are more caustic: ‘. . . I have the right
to make children jump through hoops.’ Many replies make minimal
claims. For example:
I have the right to do my best.
I have the right to make a fuss.
I have the right to spend the day with children.
I have the right to protest about gaps.
I have the right to change my mind.
I have the right to complain about too many children.
I have the right to make suggestions.
I have the right to some free time.
I have the right to a life of my own.
163
Assessing Children’s Learning
164
Rights, Responsibilities and Power
This sentence-completion data has helped me to learn about the
emotions that surround and invade the three key concepts of rights,
responsibilities and power. The data has also, perhaps more importantly,
illuminated for me the way in which these three concepts might be made
to work harmoniously together. These teachers’ responses have
suggested the interconnectedness of the concepts I was exploring,
indicating that a weakness, or lack of awareness in one area may have
effects on teachers’ thinking in another. In effective assessment, it seems
to me, educators are fully aware of their responsibilities, their rights, and
their power. This awareness includes an understanding of the educator’s
paramount responsibility for children’s learning, of the educator’s right
to act in ways that will bring worthwhile learning about, and the
educator’s power to act wisely and lovingly in the interests of children.
In the effective practice of assessment responsible educators
acknowledge their right to use their power for the benefit of children.
Our responsibilities towards children and our right to work in their
interests involve us in an extensive network of professional relationships
outside the classroom. In these relationships too the concepts of rights
and responsibilities play an important part. In the process of assessment,
parents and professional colleagues have rights and responsibilities that
are worth considering in more detail.
I argued earlier that the various purposes of assessment can more
usefully be discussed in terms of a variety of outcomes, in terms of the
actual impact of assessment practice on curriculum, on children, on
children’s learning, on groups and individual pupils, on parents, and on
teachers themselves. Some examples of the outcomes of formative
assessment were discussed in earlier chapters, where we saw how our
knowledge of children’s learning, acquired through observation and
reflection, could be used to enrich and extend children’s opportunities
for learning. The outcomes of summative assessment, carried out at
points of transfer of pupils, from teacher to teacher, or from school to
school, are a more problematic area of enquiry. In trying to discern these
outcomes, we are asking: what effect does one teacher’s assessment have
on another teacher’s thinking? On another teacher’s teaching? On
another teacher’s curriculum? We are exploring the relationship between
two sets of professional judgements – the judgements of those who make
the summary, and the judgements of those who receive it and work to
put it to use. What part do teachers’ rights and responsibilities play in
this relationship?
165
Assessing Children’s Learning
166
Rights, Responsibilities and Power
that teachers must commit themselves to swallowing unthinkingly every
last word uttered by each and every one of their colleagues. They have,
in order to earn the trust of others in their turn, the responsibility to
examine the assessments they receive, both judgements and evidence,
critically and sensitively. But their approach must be based on an
assumption of good intent, and of professional expertise, in others as
well as in themselves. Teachers’ mistrust seems to presuppose that other
teachers are out to deceive and mislead them. Does not this mistrust also
suggest a weakness in our confidence in our own professional abilities?
How can we justify the suggestion that our critical powers are so weak
that they might be damaged by the inadequate and ill-informed
judgements of others? Have we confused trust with gullibility? Do we
mistrust trust? Are we projecting anxiety about our own fallibility onto
the actions of others? Knowing my own professional weaknesses, do I
hesitate to rely on the strengths of others?
For every practising teacher, these are not rhetorical questions, but
practical ones, involving flesh and blood human beings, not the generic,
anonymous ‘teacher’ of the educational book or paper. They are questions
that can only be answered at the personal and individual level by the
persons involved. The possibility of mutual trust in each other’s
professional responsibility can only become a reality after practical steps
have been taken by the teachers concerned. In schools, or clusters of
schools, where communication between the teachers of different year
groups is minimal, this will be a tall order. Furthermore, improved
communication is unlikely to begin with a full-scale investigation into
the concepts I am discussing here. The unspoken questions ‘Can we trust
you?’ ‘Will you trust us?’ may well be the motivating themes of any
encounter between teachers who do not yet work together in teamship,
but they are probably inappropriate starting points for the enterprise of
building up trust and professional respect. On the other hand, practices
already in place may play a part in effectively minimising the chances of
improved, more trusting communication. The French system of a ‘dossier
scholaire’ for each pupil (DES 1991a), referred to in Chapter 7, which
remains, literally, a closed book for one-third of each academic year, is a
salutory reminder of the power of silence and non-communication to
limit the process of review and development. Until all teachers are ready,
willing and able enthusiastically to create opportunities for working
together on issues in assessment, they will not be fulfilling their
professional responsibilities to one another or to their pupils.
167
Assessing Children’s Learning
The right to trust and be trusted is not confined to teachers. Parents too
have rights in the practice of assessment, and the most effective
procedures that teachers and other educators have developed in recent
years have recognised these rights. Traditionally, for many years,
assessment has been the province of teachers; they made the assessments,
wrote the records, held the records and made the decisions about how to
communicate which parts of what they contained and to whom. But this
monopoly has been broken. The trail-blazing ILEA Primary Language
Record was designed as a collaborative exercise in recording achievement.
The development of the child’s language – or languages – in the home is
seen as just as important as progress in school, and so the contribution of
parents is an essential part of the recording process (Barrs et al. 1988). The
very first section of the record form is headed ‘Record of discussion
between child’s parent(s) and class teacher’. This discussion takes place at
the beginning of each academic year. The parents’ perspectives are treated
as valuable starting points for the teachers, a point at which they can begin
to learn about their pupils, a point at which parents contribute
significantly to teachers’ understanding. The final section of the record is
also designated for parents’ use; here they reflect on what they have read
on the completed record and here they are invited to comment, not only
on their child’s progress, but also on the teacher’s account of that progress.
The right of parents to contribute to the assessment process is fully
recognised in this record format; parents are given the responsibility of
contributing, and are assured that their contributions will be respectfully
and trustingly received.
In early years settings, in playgroups and daycare, as well as in nursery
schools and classes, there has been great interest in the developmental
record All About Me (Wolfendale 1990). This is a small booklet, in which
parents and educators together record anecdotal evidence that adds up to
a vivid personal picture of many aspects of a child’s development.
Parents use the All About Me booklet to note down and record, from time
to time, aspects of their child’s development and progress in seven
different areas: language; playing and learning; doing things for myself;
physical development; health and habits; other people and how I
behave; moods and feelings. It acts as a record for the family, and as a
basis for discussion with each child’s educators in nursery, family centre
or playgroup. All About Me is based on an appreciation of each child’s
uniqueness; it does not require parents to tick boxes or answer closed
Yes/No questions about their children, but to make comments in their
168
Rights, Responsibilities and Power
own words under each different heading. The record does not lay down
compulsory developmental milestones; it accepts that children develop
at different ages and stages, and so there is plenty of space to record each
child’s individuality, rather than simply noting the age of reaching
predetermined targets.
As parents and teachers come closer together, expressing the principle
of respect in the act of trusting one another and one another’s
judgements, new understandings become possible. The Sheffield Early
Literacy Development Project (Hannon et al. 1991) reports that when
educators work with parents, describing and discussing the children’s
experiences at home, the outcomes are beneficial for all concerned. The
parents involved in this project came to appreciate more fully the extent
of their children’s early literacy development. The educators involved
gained insights into the children’s activities at home that enriched their
workplace observations and tentative assessments.
A full-scale development project is not, of course, necessary to
stimulate teachers’ awareness of the powerful contribution that parents
can make to the practice of assessment. Ann Le Gassick, a nursery
teacher on an early years in-service course, reported how she was
discussing a particular child’s learning with the child’s mother. In the
course of the discussion, the teacher commented that the child never
used drawing materials in the nursery. The mother replied that he made
many drawings at home and offered to show them to the teacher, who
was pleased to accept the offer. She was not, however, prepared for the
excitement of seeing the drawing (figure 9.2) that the mother produced
next day, nor for the further excitement of talking about the picture with
the child, who described and explained in detail every element of this
amazingly complex representation. A whole new area of this child’s
learning had been opened up to the teacher by this casual conversation,
which, she reported, had also transformed her understanding of the
importance of involving parents in the assessment practice. Parents’
rights to contribute can be fostered and protected by individuals as well
as by institutions or authority-wide policies and practices.
Children too have rights in the assessment process, rights that go
further than the right to be heard, to have a voice. The development of
records of achievement, based on the continuous involvement of pupils
in their own assessment, was briefly officially endorsed by the School
Examinations and Assessment Council (SEAC 1991) for all primary
schools. There were then dozens of local initiatives, many cited in the
169
Assessing Children’s Learning
170
Figure 9.2
Rights, Responsibilities and Power
SEAC publication, which had in common the practice of giving the pupil
a greater or lesser degree of control over what is recorded in the
cumulative record.
But, as Stansbury (1992) promptly warned us, the label ‘Record of
Achievement’ may be used to obscure the actual purpose of some forms
of reporting. Some formats, including the National Record of
Achievement, define success in terms of school subjects. The use of a
standard format, as advocated over the last few years by some local
authorities, can have the effect of enabling comparisons to be made more
efficiently between the more and less successful pupils on standard
measures. The idea of a single set of standards, now enshrined in the
targets and levels of the National Curriculum, is gravely at odds with the
diversity of human beings; records of achievement are, in themselves, no
guarantee that the differences between pupils will be respected and
valued. It is worth remembering the conclusion reached by the Hadow
Report (Board of Education 1933), in discussing the outcomes of a pupil’s
two or three years in the infant school:
In none of this should a uniform standard to be reached by all children be
expected. The infant school has no business with uniform standards of
attainment. (para 105)
Records of achievement that are truly respectful of pupils’ rights will not
be concerned with matching every pupil up against the same numerical
scale; they will take account of each pupil’s right to be different, to be a
uniquely individual member of an equitable society.
Children’s rights as persons to be assessed extend to the ways in which
the assessment is to be carried out. The rights of bilingual children, for
example, and of children who are learning English in school as a second
or third language, have to be respected. There were early claims (Gregory
& Kelly 1992) that this respect would not be readily compatible with
National Curriculum assessment procedures, as they were first
introduced. Bilingual children have been shown to develop different
linguistic skills from monolingual children, and to show more flexibility
and freedom in their use of language. These linguistic strengths are not
tapped by language tests designed for monolingual children, on which
bilingual children often make more grammatical mistakes than their
monolingual peers. Recent small-scale studies suggest this pessimism
was not ill-founded. Rose Elgar, a member of a group of primary teachers
researching the impact of the National Literacy Strategy (Sheahan 2003),
171
Assessing Children’s Learning
This test was compulsory. Under the 1988 Education Reform Act, the
Secretary of State had acquired the power to compel teachers to assess
spelling in this way. As a consequence, the test was used in classrooms
where the teaching of spelling had been approached in a completely
different way, a way that emphasised collaboration and research.
Thousands of children in this country had been taught (and I hope still
are) that when faced with a question of spelling, there are several
172
Rights, Responsibilities and Power
appropriate strategies available to them: they may decide to ask a friend,
to discuss and confer, or to use reference material. This is hardly
surprising, since these are the strategies used by fully literate, mature
adults, when they are writing in contexts where correct spelling is
important. But in this test, these strategies were forbidden. This test of
spelling did not match the curriculum of spelling experienced by many
children. Children who had learned to use dictionaries with confidence
and accuracy, and who had learned to share their own knowledge with
their peers, had that learning denied by this test. Their rights as learners,
to have their learning respected, were denied. Their right to be assessed
in a way that matched their curriculum was denied. The teacher’s
responsibility to match the method of assessment to the curriculum in all
respects was denied.
It is, in my view, an insufferable burden for teachers to swallow these
denials, and many others like them, that have become a normal part of
the annual SATs procedures. Teachers are not blind to the damage that is
done to childen’s rights, and to their own powers as educators, but the
juggernaut of statutory testing is not easily resisted. There are plentiful
and persuasive arguments in favour of compliance with the testing
regime, not least the long-term benefits of raised standards for all, an
outcome universally ascribed to the continued use of testing. It remains
to be seen for how long professional obedience, rather than civil
disobedience, will rule the day. Maybe one day soon we will argue, as a
matter of urgency, that our responsibilities to children’s learning take
precedence over our statutory responsibilities to assess in ways that are
none of our choosing.
On the other hand, we may not have to wait for much longer to see
some of the lasting effects on pupils of being assessed at level one at age
seven, or level three at age 11: the vast literature from 30 years or more
of research on students perceived and labelled as low achievers already
suggests that the outcomes of the current regime will be anything but
benign. For just one example, let us return to the pupils in Meek’s
challenging study (1983) of non-reading adolescents (briefly discussed in
Chapter 2), who are perfectly and appallingly frank about their
estimations of themselves as learners.
One of the teachers in Meek’s group, Judith Graham, worked with a
pupil, Trevor, aged 12.6 at the start of the study. By the end of his infant
schooling he was reported to be ‘backward’; five years on ‘Judith chose
Trevor (for this study) because he was the classic underachiever. He
173
Assessing Children’s Learning
should have learned to read, but had mysteriously failed.’ Trevor spent
one tutorial session a week with Judith, stormy and emotional periods for
both of them. During a lesson at the beginning of the second year, Trevor
put up great resistance to reading at all. Judith Graham’s account
continues: ‘I suggest that he is “giving up” which stings Trevor into the
accusation, “it’s a year now. Nothing’s happened.”’ Trevor’s outrage and
despair are movingly described; his teacher can hardly muster the energy
to ‘start the long haul of re-establishing Trevor’s image of himself’. Even
in the act of doing so, she doubts her own judgement. ‘By taking on so
seriously – even somewhat emotionally – the task of building up Trevor’s
confidence, do I betray my own fear that I will not be able to teach him
to read?’ (p.158). Trevor, too, knows the meaning of fear. Three years into
the study, Judith records Trevor’s comments on his impending exams:
‘What’s the point of my coming in to do them? I can’t even read the paper,
and even if I can, I can’t write quickly enough to answer the questions.’. . . I
say ‘These exams are five weeks away. Anything can happen in five weeks.’
Then Trevor ‘Oh yes miss, who are you kidding? I’ve been coming for three
and a half years with you’. . . (p.216)
Three and a half years of failure in special reading lessons, on top of six
years of failure in the primary school, have broken Trevor’s spirit.
Assessment practices that contribute, however minutely, to a learner’s
sense of personal failure cannot be justified. All pupils are learners; their
rights to learn, and to feel that their teachers trust and respect their
learning, are paramount. The teachers’ responsibilities are plain.
However constrained our assessment practices may be by Acts of
Parliament beyond our control, we still have the right to use our
remaining powers to act wisely and lovingly in the interests of our
pupils.
174
Conclusion
In this book, I have identified three crucial questions that teachers and
other educators must, I believe, ask themselves as they set about
assessing children’s learning. These questions are: when we look at
learning,
● what is there to see?
● how best can we understand what we see?
● how can we put our understanding to good use?
I have tried not to suggest that there is only one possible set of
answers to these questions. We would be deluding ourselves if we
thought that these questions could ever be answered once and for all, or
that assessment is a practice that can ever be perfected. But this lack of
finality, this imperfection in our practice, should not cause us to feel
shame or despair. To recognise that we cannot achieve absolute
perfection in assessment is a first step towards recognising other
characteristics of the process.
Throughout the book, I have tried to illustrate these characteristics by
drawing on the assessment practice of other educators, past and present.
Children’s learning is so complex and various that the task of trying to
understand it is necessarily complex too. The task entails trying to see
and understand the whole, as well as the minutest parts; it requires us to
appreciate the past, and analyse the present, as well as envisage and
welcome the future; it obliges us to look for and attend to differences as
well as similarities, individuals as well as groups, the unexpected as well
as the intended outcome, absence as well as presence. It demands a broad
vision and a narrow focus.
175
Assessing Children’s Learning
But I believe that this inspiring phrase, ‘the loving use of power’, can
illuminate, for teachers, some more optimistic possibilities. I believe that
a full understanding of what it might mean, for teachers, parents and
children, if we learned to use our power to educate lovingly (as well as
effectively or efficiently) would transform our practice in assessment.
For all the pomp and circumstance of official policies and national
initiatives over the last 15 years, for all the ferocity of the debate about
assessment, and the passion of the arguments on either side, it would be
foolish to suggest that the teaching profession has been facing an issue of
recent invention, a new and unlooked for addition to their heavy load of
responsibilities. Of course, assessment is no such thing. Teachers are not
dumb beasts of burden; they have always been interested in learning and
struggled to make sense of it. Trying to understand learning with intense
professional commitment is nothing new. And in our cautious – and
anxious – and, I hope, courageous responses to demands on us that are
new, we will do well to turn back to earlier educationists, working within
a very different set of values from those of today’s legislators. Their work
may help us to be clearer and more articulate about the values that
permeate our chosen practices, and what we must do to live up to them.
Earlier in the book I quoted extensively from Susan Isaacs, not just
because of her influential place in the history of British primary
education, but also because of her influence on my own thinking. Her
work has played an important part in my learning about learning; from
176
Conclusion
her I have learned that there is always more to see, and I have tried,
following her example, to see it more clearly. Another great educator, to
whom I turn at moments of doubt and difficulty in these uncertain
times, is Edmond Holmes. Remembered by some today as the stern and
inspiring uncle of the irrepressible Gerard Holmes, author of The Idiot
Teacher (1952), his professional position was, at his retirement, HM Chief
Inspector of Elementary Schools. His most important book, published in
1911, What Is and What Might Be told, in his nephew’s words:
of how this experienced official, late in life, had come to realise that the
whole system of teaching as practised in the schools of England was
stultifying and repressive and destructive of the natural, spiritual and mental
powers latent in young children. And it told of a real school where an
altogether different and supremely successful method was practised by an
enlightened woman.
Holmes (1952) pp.20–21
In Edmond Holmes’ own words, the central theme of the book is no less
challengingly described:
Does elementary education, as at present conducted, tend to foster the
growth of the child’s faculties? . . . the answer to the question, so far at least
as thousands of schools are concerned, must be an emphatic No . . . The
education given in thousands of our elementary schools is in the highest
degree anti-educational.
Holmes (1911) pp.143–4
But there are exceptions. The first half of the book – ‘What Is’ – is devoted
to a scrupulous analysis of the ills of the current state of elementary
education. The second half – ‘What Might Be’ – describes one of these
exceptions, a village school of 120 pupils that Holmes names Utopia, and
its headteacher, whom he calls Egeria. It is nevertheless a real school:
in a very real village, which can be reached, as all other villages can, by rail
and road . . . I have paid (this) school many visits, and it has taken me many
months of thought to get to what I believe to be the bed-rock of (the
headteacher’s) philosophy of education.
Holmes (1911) p.154
177
Assessing Children’s Learning
178
Conclusion
(1) The communicative instinct – the
}
desire to talk and listen, which
develops into the desire to read These may be grouped
and write. together as the
sympathetic instincts – in
(2) The dramatic instinct – the desire and through which the
to play at make-believe, to child grows in the
imagine, to pretend, to identify direction of love.
one’s life with others.
}
to draw, paint and model, which
grows into a restless desire to These may be grouped
express and delight in a together as the aesthetic
perception of visible beauty. instincts – in and through
(4) The musical instinct – the desire which the child grows in
to dance and sing, to move and to the direction of beauty.
express oneself with rhythm and
grace.
}
desire to know the why of things,
to understand how effects are
produced, to discover new facts These may be grouped
and pass on, if possible, to their together as the scientific
causes. instincts – in and through
which the child grows in
(6) The constructive instinct – the direction of truth.
desire to synthesise, to build
things up, to put one’s knowledge
of the world to a practical use.
179
Afterword to the
Second Edition
In preparing this second edition I have made some minor changes to the
original text of the book, but I have kept these to a minimum because, to
be blunt, I have not changed my mind about any of the issues that I
invite my readers to think about. I have seriously reduced the number of
references to the 1988 Education Reform Act, still fresh in my mind at
the time of writing. The years have passed and the provisions of the ERA
have become part of history: for a whole generation of young teachers,
they are now ancient history. There are many teachers and other
educators working with children today who have themselves
experienced, as pupils, the best and the worst of the National Curriculum
and the statutory forms of testing that accompanied its introduction.
I have adjusted some references to extinct official bodies such as SEAC
(the School Examinations and Assessment Council) and NCC (the
National Curriculum Council) and included an acknowledgement of the
work of the current successor to the role, the QCA (the Qualifications and
Curriculum Authority). I have referred to the establishment (in England)
of the Foundation Stage of education, for three- to five-year-olds, and to
the requirement to assess every child’s learning at the end of this stage
with the Foundation Stage Profile. But I have not engaged in a detailed
critical review of the QCA Guidance for the Foundation Stage, or the
content and procedures of the Foundation Stage Profile, as I plan to do
this soon elsewhere. I have not given an account of recent policy
initiatives affecting primary and pre-schools, nor of new accountability
procedures such as Ofsted inspections: this book is not a history of
contemporary education, but an attempt to establish some enduring
principles in assessing children’s learning that are not subject to the
contingencies of short-lived national pressures and policies.
180
Finally, I have not watered down my extreme anxiety about
Frameworks of understanding
In Chapter 5 of this book, I described some of the ways in which teachers
and other educators have approached the work of trying to understand
children’s learning. I illustrated a range of different interpretations,
including those of some contemporary writers: Chris Athey, Vivian
Gussin Paley, Michael Armstrong, Kieran Egan, and some from the past:
Susan Isaacs and Margaret Lowenfeld, for example. I suggested that it is
unlikely that any one single model of learning will enable us to
understand everything we see, and that we will do better to draw on a
variety of frameworks in our search for meaning. Since the first edition
of this book, the choices at our disposal have been greatly enriched by the
work of the Assessment Reform Group, whose important and influential
publications have made a substantial contribution to our current
understanding of the power and purposes of assessment. The first of
these, Inside the Black Box (Black & Wiliam 1998), presented
overwhelming evidence, firstly that improving the practice of formative
assessment raises standards, secondly that there is room for
improvement, and thirdly that we already know a great deal about the
ways in which assessment can be made more effective. Black and Wiliam
went on to argue that if teachers were to implement these ideas in their
everyday practice they would need considerable support in the form of
‘living examples of implementation’. They sketched out an ambitious
programme for development, which would enable teachers to
reconstruct their current approaches to assessment in the interests of
learning: assessment for learning rather than assessment of learning was
to be the driving principle of this development work.
In a later publication (Black et al. 2002), the authors report on the
progress of their programme. They describe the innovative practices in
assessment that have been developed in selected secondary and primary
181
Assessing Children’s Learning
schools and the evidence that this work did raise standards. This
classroom work is described under four headings: questioning, feedback
through marking, peer and self-assessment, and the formative use of
summative tests. In all four of these areas, Black and his colleagues claim,
teachers became more effective as they redefined their role in learning,
relinquishing the delivery-recipient relationship of expert teacher/
passive learner, and repositioning themselves with their pupils as
‘partners in pursuit of a shared goal’. For the teachers whose work is
reported here, assessment for learning has become a dominant
responsibility, a key principle that extends to the whole of their teaching.
Further development work continues, under the auspices of the ESRC
Teaching and Learning Research Project, in the form of the Learning How
to Learn project, which is extending the earlier work on formative
assessment into a model of learning how to learn for both teachers and
pupils: this in time may offer teachers a powerful framework for
examining and understanding both their own practice and their
children’s learning.
One striking aspect of the work of this research and development
programme is that it is taking place in an educational milieu where the
prevailing trends seem to be set in a very different direction. My own
observations (over the last ten years) of children in the early years of
school, suggest that the process described by Willes in 1983, by which
active, enquiring and exploratory children quickly become submissive
and obedient pupils, is still a lived reality for most children. As funding
arrangements for four-year-olds have changed, many more children now
enter primary school at the beginning of the year in which they turn five.
But these children of below statutory age are, on the whole,
conceptualised as pupils, and subject to stringent classroom control. As a
substitute for my self-imposed task of collecting baseline assessment
schedules, now rendered inoperable by the imposition of the
standardised Foundation Stage Profile, I have started to record the lists
of classroom rules that appear on the walls of Key Stage One and
Foundation Stage classrooms. These do nothing to suggest that the
principle of children as ‘active learners who take responsibility for and
manage their own learning’ (Black et al. 2002, p.21) has any currency in
the wider world, outside the ‘Assessment for Learning’ project schools.
For example, in one classroom for children turning five I observed a list
that read:
182
Afterword to the Second Edition
In Octopus class we:
183
Assessing Children’s Learning
These aspirations are summarised in the form of five strands that run
through the document:
● belonging
● well-being
● exploration
● communication
● contribution
From the four principles and the five strands, each setting and centre for
young children weaves its own curriculum programme: the Maori words
184
of the title, Te Wha-riki, refer to a traditional woven floor mat, with an
185
Assessing Children’s Learning
186
Languages of Children’ that documents the approach; the publications of
187
Assessing Children’s Learning
188
understand and ‘read’ what is going on in practice . . . The awareness that we
This description, it seems to me, comes very close to the position for
which I argue in Chapter 8 of this book, in which the principles by which
teachers try to live, in terms of teaching and assessing learning, are
themselves the fruit of clear thinking and critical questioning of the
teachers’ core values and beliefs. The moral and philosophical work of
assessment, as I have tried to present it in these pages, is beautifully
exemplified in the Reggio educators’ work of documentation.
189
Assessing Children’s Learning
190
fails to connect, the teacher can choose how to interpret it: perhaps the
191
Assessing Children’s Learning
For what does education do to foster the growth of the child? If the child is
to grow, he must do the business of growing by and for himself. He must
himself digest and assimilate the food that is provided for him. He must
himself exercise all his organs and faculties . . . In other words, he must be
allowed to live and work in an atmosphere of freedom.
Now freedom is the last thing that education, as we know it in this and
other ‘civilised’ countries, allows to the child. At every turn it closes in upon
him with dogmatic pressure and constraint. From morning to evening, from
day to day, from year to year, it does, or tries to do, for him most of the things
which he ought to do for himself – his reasoning, his thinking, his imagining,
his admiring, his sympathising, his willing, his purposing, his planning, his
solving of problems, his mastering of difficulties, his controlling his passions
and impulses, his bearing himself aright in his dealings with others. So
complete is its distrust of the child’s nature, that it will allow him to do nothing
for himself which it can do, or even pretend to do for him; and it thus develops
into an elaborate system for paralysing activity.
Fisher (1913) pp. xxii–iii
The argument Holmes is making here is the one with which I close this
Afterword. If we choose to distrust children and their learning, we
cannot act in their interests. If we choose to clamp down on their
freedom as active and spontaneous learners, they will not learn for
themselves: they will become passive, obedient pupils; ‘learning by
swallowing’ is Holmes’ scathing, derisive description of the wrong kind
of learning. But if children are offered trust and freedom, and invited to
live and learn in a reciprocal relationship with their educators, as we
192
have seen them do in New Zealand and Reggio in the preceding pages,
Returning to Jason
Holmes’ argument, now over 80 years-old, that children deserve, at the
very least, both trust and freedom, calls for a contemporary illustration.
As a conclusion to this edition, it seems appropriate to return to the child
whose learning, or lack of it, stimulated my long-standing interest in the
process of assessment and the tangle of rights, responsibilities and power
that I see surrounding it. In Chapter 1 I analysed Jason’s response to a
formal mathematics test; here is Jason’s work on an informal classroom
spelling test, administered some months later.
193
Assessing Children’s Learning
First let us consider what this test tells us about Jason and his learning
and the choices his teachers seem to be making. The first instruction he
is given is, apparently, to ‘write down the numbers 1–10’, which he does.
But see how even in this simple direction there is room for ambiguity,
confusion and, for Jason, getting it wrong and having to start again,
writing the numbers down the page, in a vertical line, not just ‘writing
them down’. Now for the spellings themselves: in the first, Jason gets
the first letter right, and in the next two the first and last: a small degree
of achievement admittedly, but perhaps a promising sign that the
mysterious process of transforming isolated spoken words into a
sequence of written symbols is beginning to make sense to Jason. The
fourth and fifth spellings both have two letters correct, and the fourth,
being a two-letter word, receives the teacher’s blessing and a
tremendous tick. At the sixth spelling, Jason falters on his previous form
and only achieves one correct letter; looking back up the list at the
pattern so far, one may speculate whether Jason is using the letter ‘e’ as
an all purpose vowel . . . maybe knowledge of the local accent would
enable us to check out this possibility. However, at the seventh spelling,
Jason goes completely off the rails as he hears the teacher utter the word
‘seven’, to indicate the seventh spelling, and then ‘ten’. There is an
almost audible crashing of mental gears as Jason fails to overcome the
confusion that ensues. If this is a spelling test, he may well be asking
himself, how on earth can I spell a number? And, in any case, I’ve
already written ‘10’ (twice). It is no wonder to me that the eighth
spelling bears no resemblance to the target word ‘hat’, though I am not
confident in my own reading of what Jason has written. But then Jason
recovers himself; two letters are correct in the ninth word and all three
in the final one, though with two reversals (three, if you include the
reversal of the digits in 10 – is Jason left-handed perhaps?). But never
mind the reversals, Jason is now back in pupil mode, responding to
instructions as best he can, even when, as so often in his life as a pupil,
it all makes very little sense to him. With a score of 1/10 boldly marked
on his test paper, Jason moves on to face the next task, the next
challenge, with a very negative evaluation of his worth as a speller made
public, for all to see. This will not be the first time Jason has received
such a low mark for his performance as a pupil: his teacher is unlikely
to be surprised by it, and we can be fairly confident in assuming that
she or he will use it to predict Jason’s unsatisfactory and
undistinguished future career as a learner.
194
Now let us imagine a different scenario, where Jason’s future is shaped
195
Assessing Children’s Learning
April 2003
196
Bibliography
197
Assessing Children’s Learning
198
Bibliography
Drummond, M. J. and Nutbrown, C. (1992) ‘Observing and assessing
young children’ in Pugh, G. (ed.) Contemporary Issues in the Early
Years. London: Paul Chapman Publishing in association with the
National Children’s Bureau.
Drummond, M. J., Rouse, D. and Pugh, G. (1992) Making Assessment
Work: Values and Principles in Assessing Young Children’s Learning.
Nottingham: NES Arnold in association with the National Children’s
Bureau.
Early Years Curriculum Group (1989) Early Childhood Education: The
Early Years Curriculum and the National Curriculum. Stoke-on-Trent:
Trentham Books.
Easen, P. (1987) ‘All at sixes and sevens: the difficulties of learning
mathematics’ in Booth, T., Potts, P. and Swann, W. (eds) Preventing
Difficulties in Learning. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Edwards, C., Gandini, L. and Forman, G. (eds) (1994) The Hundred
Languages of Children: The Reggio Emilia Approach to Early Childhood
Education. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Co.
Edwards, C., Gandini, L. and Forman, G. (eds) (1998) The Hundred
Languages of Children: The Reggio Emilia Approach – Advanced
Reflections. Norwood NJ: Ablex Publishing Co.
Egan, K. (1988) Primary Understanding. London: Routledge.
Fisher, D. C. (1913) A Montessori Mother. London: Constable.
Fisher, D. C. (1924) The Home-Maker. New York: Harcourt Brace
Jovanovich, reissued (1999) London: Persephone Books.
Fromm, E. (1942) The Fear of Freedom. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Gandini, L. and Edwards, C. P. (eds) (2001) Bambini: The Italian Approach
to Infant Toddler Care. New York: Teachers College Press.
Gardner, D. E. M. (1969) Susan Isaacs: The First Biography. London:
Methuen.
Gregory, E. and Kelly, C. (1992) ‘Bilingualism and assessment’ in Blenkin,
G. M. and Kelly, A. V. op. cit.
Grumet, M. (1981) ‘Restitution and Reconstruction of Educational
Experience: an Autobiographical Method for Curriculum Theory’ in
Lawn, M. and Barton, L. (eds) Rethinking Curriculum Studies. London:
Croom Helm.
Gura, P. (ed.) (1997) Reflections on Early Education and Care Inspired by
Visits to Reggio Emilia Italy. London: British Association for Early
Childhood Education.
Hannon, P., Weinberger, J. and Nutbrown, C. (1991) ‘A study of work with
199
Assessing Children’s Learning
200
Ministry of Education (1996) Te Wha-riki Early Childhood Curriculum.
Bibliography
Wellington, NZ: Learning Media.
Nabokov, V. (1977) ‘An evening of Russian poetry’ in The Portable
Nabokov. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books.
NFER (1984) Mathematics 8. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
Nicholls, R. (ed.) (1986) Rumpus Schema Extra. Cleveland Teachers in
Education (LEA).
Nutbrown, C. (1987) ‘A case-study of the development and
implementation of a nursery curriculum based on schematic theory’.
Unpublished BEd dissertation, Sheffield City Polytechnic.
Paley, V. G. (1981) Wally’s Stories. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University
Press.
Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. (1956) The Child’s Conception of Space. London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Pollard, A. and Tann, S. (1987) Reflective Teaching in the Primary School.
London: Cassell.
Powys, J. C. (1974) Letters to Nicholas Ross. London: The Village Press.
Prisk, T. (1987) ‘Letting them get on with it: a study of unsupervised
group talk in an infant school’ in Pollard, A. (ed.) Children and Their
Primary Schools. London: Falmer.
Pugh, G. and De’Ath, E. (1989) Working Towards Partnership in the Early
Years. London: National Children’s Bureau.
Qualifications and Curriculum Authority (2000) Curriculum Guidance for
the Foundation Stage. London: Qualifications and Curriculum
Authority.
Richards, M. P. M. (ed.) (1974) The Integration of a Child into a Social
World. London: Cambridge University Press.
Richards, M. P. M. and Light, P. (eds) (1986) Children of Social Worlds:
Development in a Social Context. Cambridge: Polity Press.
School Examinations and Assessment Council (1991) Records of
Achievement in Primary Schools. London: HMSO.
Sharp, R. and Green, A. (1975) Education and Social Control. London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Shaw, J. (1992) ‘An investigation of parents’ conceptual development in
the context of dialogue with a community teacher’. Unpublished PhD
thesis, University of Newcastle-upon-Tyne.
Sheahan A. (ed.) (2003) Teachers Understanding Literacy Improvements in
Primary Schools. Cambridge: Faculty of Education, University of
Cambridge.
201
Assessing Children’s Learning
202