L11 PHPC2017 EM, Bias, Causal Inference 2020
L11 PHPC2017 EM, Bias, Causal Inference 2020
Effect modification
Understand effect modification
Distinguish between confounding and effect
modification
Error & bias
Understand selection bias and information bias
Identify sources of bias in different study designs
Causal inference
Understand the basic principles of making causal
inference in epidemiology
Review: definition of confounding
A situation in which the effect of exposure or association
between exposure and outcome is distorted by the
presence of another variable (confounder)
The observed effect of exposure on the outcome is not
purel the effect of exposure itself, but a mixture of the
effects of exposure and other factors
Review: definition of confounder
11
Hypothetical data of effect modification
● Stratified analysis
● Multiple regression (NOT required at this stage)
● Need clinical/biologic argument for Effect
Modification, rather than relying solely on
numbers/statistics
16
Effect modification: stratified analysis
1 Calculate crude OR
● Stratified analysis
● Multiple regression (NOT required at this stage)
● Need clinical/biologic argument for Effect
Modification, rather than relying solely on
numbers/statistics
19
Confounding vs. effect modification
Confounding Interaction/Effect Modification
3 factors involved: exposure, 3 factors involved: exposure,
outcome, confounder outcome, effect modifier
Homogeneous strata-specific RRs Heterogeneous strata-specific RRs
& adjusted RR crude RR
A spurious relationship that we A useful knowledge for
don’t want understanding the mechanism
(e.g. a drug is more effective in
males than females)
Could be minimized with proper Cannot be removed (as a fact of
study design (e.g. matching, nature)
restriction, randomization)
Analysis method: stratified Analysis method: stratification,
analysis, standardization, multiple multiple regression
regression
20
Contents
22
Error
23
Error
Smpasitef
24
Bias
Systematic error occurs in the design or
implementation of an epidemiological study
that results in an incorrect estimate of the
association between exposure and outcome
Types:
1. Selection bias
2. Information bias
3. Confounding bias
25
Selection bias
Any error that arises in the process of identifying
(selecting) the population for study or analysis
Leads to systematic differences in characteristics
between those selected for study (or analysis) and
those not
The prevalence of a health status, or the relation
between exposure and disease, is different for those
who participate and those who should be theoretically
eligible for study, including those who do not
participate
26
Selection bias: examples
27
Selection bias: examples
29
Selection bias: examples
o Survival/survivorship bias
Only the subjects that survived a process are included in the
analysis.
A case-control study investigating stroke that only
enrolled prevalent stroke cases (who were diagnosed years
ago). Those who died shortly after diagnosis were excluded.
The selected sample, therefore, included less severe cases, but
not fatal cases, thus not representative of the target
population (all stroke cases).
Prevalence-incidence bias (Neyman bias)
May get wrong conclusion, e.g., age vs. stroke
30
Selection bias: examples
⑦ Loss-to-follow-up: people who are staying in a
cohort study / clinical trial are not the same as
those who left (migration, refuse to continue
because of worsening health condition, death)
If you are looking at the death rate in this group of
people and included in the analysis only those
staying in the cohort (may be not representative of
the original target population), then the death rate
you obtained could be biased.
Similar to healthy worker effect and non-response
bias in a way
31
Selection bias: how to reduce it?
32
Information bias (measurement error)
33
Information bias: examples
① Interviewer bias
● Problem: difference in soliciting, recording, interpreting
information from subjects
● e.g., in a case-control study, interviewers ask the case group
about previous exposure history in a leading or more in-depth
way, as compared with the control group
● Possible solutions
1) Blind exposure status of subjects in prospective studies,
including both cohort studies and RCTs, when collecting
outcome data
2) Blind case/control status of subjects when measuring
exposure history in case control studies
3) Mask study hypothesis
4) Use standardized, closed-form questionnaires
34
Information bias: examples
Recall Bias
②
Information accurac depends on respondents
memory
E.g., in case-control study, cases recall and/or report
previous exposure differently from controls
(differential)
Possible solutions
If better recall of cases is due to search for cause of
their disease, then use diseased (hospital) controls with
similar potential for recall
Rely on historical/ objective records rather than ask
subjects to recall
35
Information bias: examples
⑦ Detection Bias
Cohort studies and RCT
Systematic difference in following, obtaining, and
ascertaining outcome information between
comparison (exposed and non-exposed) groups
e.g., potential outcomes are identified through
thorough medical examination in the exposed
group, but by self-reporting in the control group
Possible solution: use standardized procedures
for outcome ascertainments for all subjects
36
Information bias: consequence
37
Information bias: consequence
Differential misclassification
Degree of misclassification is NOT the same in
study and comparison group
E.g., more in-depth and detailed interview in the
cancer cases group to obtain their exposure history
than in the control group
The association can be biased in any direction, over-
estimated or under-estimated
38
Bias: difficulty in dealing with it
39
Contents
1. Temporal order
2. Association: RR/OR 1
However, associated factors may not be causally related!
Causal relation: criteria
Two students, who do not know each other, always enter and
leave a lecture hall at the same time: (1) They are associated;
(2) But the are not the cause of the other s presence; (3) It
is because there is a regular class in the lecture hall.
46
Causal relation: criteria
1. Temporal order
2. Association: RR/OR 1
3. Consequential change: the association between the
exposure and outcome is really caused by the
exposure, rather than by chance (random error) or
bias (systematic error)
Causal relation: criteria
o
Hill s Criteria for Causal Inference
1. Temporality (temporal order)
2. Strength of association (RR, OR, RD, etc)
3. Biological gradient (dose-response relationship)
4. Consistency (repeatability)
5. Experimental evidence (RCT evidence)
6. Plausibility (explicable by known knowledge)
7. Coherence (biological/theoretical/factual/statistical)
8. Specificity (in the cause/effect, suitable for infectious disease)
9. Analogy (experience of similar events)
Strength of association
Is there an association?
How strong is the association? Stronger
associations are less likely to be entirely due to bias
51
Biological gradient
Dose-response relationship
Cigarette smoking and risk of cancer
Blood pressure and risk of stroke
Incidence of
stroke (/100,000)
52
Biological gradient
Level of exposure
53
Consistency
Consistent findings observed by different persons in
different places with different samples strengthens
the likelihood of an effect
There could be no chance for a second study, e.g.,
atomic bomb vs. cancer in Nagasaki and Hiroshima
of Japan
Other studies could be severely flawed and thus not
reliable
54
Experimental evidence
55
Causal inference is subjective process
Joey Yang
Division of Epidemiology
JC School of Public Health and Primary Care
The Chinese University of Hong Kong