0% found this document useful (0 votes)
196 views

Rat Control Guidance

This document provides a complete guide to best practices for controlling rats with rodenticides. It contains information on rat biology, selecting the appropriate rodenticide and bait, how to carry out treatments, and how to monitor success. The guidelines are intended to help amateur and professional pest controllers maximize rat control efforts while minimizing risks to other animals. It provides comprehensive advice based on extensive research, including how to anticipate rat behavior and tailor control methods to specific infestations.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
196 views

Rat Control Guidance

This document provides a complete guide to best practices for controlling rats with rodenticides. It contains information on rat biology, selecting the appropriate rodenticide and bait, how to carry out treatments, and how to monitor success. The guidelines are intended to help amateur and professional pest controllers maximize rat control efforts while minimizing risks to other animals. It provides comprehensive advice based on extensive research, including how to anticipate rat behavior and tailor control methods to specific infestations.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 67

THE CONTROL OF RATS WITH RODENTICIDES: A COMPLETE

GUIDE TO BEST PRACTICE

These guidelines contain information on rat control based on extensive R&D


carried out by CSL over many years. They should be useful to anyone,
amateur or professional, who needs to control rats with rodenticides,
particularly in rural areas and especially on farms. The information
presented is comprehensive and includes some aspects of rat biology and
control that experienced pest controllers may want to skip.

November 2002

1
INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................................3

1. Where rats are likely to occur ........................................................................6

2. Aspects of rat biology relevant to control ....................................................7

3. Selecting the right rodenticide ....................................................................12

4. Selecting the best bait to use ......................................................................16

5. Carrying out the treatment ...........................................................................19

5.1 Surveying the site 19


5.2 Applying the bait 20
5.3 How many bait points? 23
5.4 How much bait per point? 25
5.5 How often should baits be inspected? 28
5.6 Monitoring the treatment 31
6. Is the treatment working? Signs of success ..............................................33

7. Is the treatment working? Indicators of poor control................................35

7.1 No take at all/small, sporadic takes 35


Symptoms 35
Possible causes 35
Remedies 37
7.2 Good takes initially, partial control, then poor bait takes 39
Symptoms 39
Possible causes 40
Remedies 41
7.3 Good takes continuing 42
Symptoms 42
Possible causes 42
Remedies 46
8. Long-term management of rat problems ....................................................49

GLOSSARY........................................................................................................53

2
INTRODUCTION

Rattus norvegicus, generally known as the


Brown, Common, Norway or Sewer rat

There has been a considerable amount of research on how to

control populations of the Common or Norway rat (Rattus norvegicus)

with rodenticides and anticoagulant rodenticides in particular. Following

their introduction over 50 years ago, anticoagulants revolutionised the

practice of rodent control leading to far more effective control than there

had ever been. Subsequent research helped devise the best methods of

applying these products and no doubt led to the practice of rodent

control being viewed all too often as a routine procedure. Despite the

advances, rodent problems have not gone away, partly because rats are

highly adaptable and often refuse to eat the bait or seem to eat it

endlessly without suffering any ill effects. In such circumstances,

techniques other than rodenticide baiting may have to be used to break

3
the cycle of increasing the potency of the rodenticide to counter the

‘resistance’ of the rodents.

Rodenticide use may be avoided altogether if action is taken to

prevent rat infestations developing in the first place. Potential food

sources and harbourage for rats should be removed wherever

practicable, or protected by physical barriers if it is not. Similarly, once

infestations have been eliminated, there should be opportunities to alter

conditions sufficiently to prevent the problem arising again. The

availability and ease of use of rodenticide products should not be used

as an excuse to avoid tidying up a site. If after all considerations,

rodenticides seem the best option, then these guidelines should help to

maximise control while minimising risks to other animals.

To ensure a high level of control, it is essential to anticipate how

rats are going to react to baits, containers and traps, so as to increase

the likelihood of attracting the animals to them. For example, highly

palatable baits do not always draw rats away from what may appear to

be unappetising food, such as rotting grain. In their natural habitats,

social pressures, fear of predators, or simply not recognising the bait as

a food item may stop some rats from approaching it. The solution may

need to be tailored to that specific population.

4
Although rodenticides remain the most frequently used tools for

control, they are hazardous to other animals and public concern about

the risks of children, pets, livestock and wildlife eating the bait or in

some cases eating poisoned rodents has inevitably brought their use

under more scrutiny. Nevertheless, concern about safety has to be

balanced against the consequences of not controlling rats, such as

damage to structures, contamination of food supplies and increased

risks of contracting rodent-borne disease.

Given that rodenticides are at present an economical means of

tackling infestations, professional and amateur pest controllers should

be aware of what is currently considered to be the best advice available

on their use. This includes getting the most out of existing products and

techniques, as well as offering solutions to the most common problems

encountered during rodent control operations.

This guide is intended to provide information on the control of rats

and to complement the leaflet from DEFRA RDS (formerly MAFF FRCA)

on Rats (WM4), the Agriculture Information Sheet No 31 from HSE on

Safe use of rodenticides on farms and holdings, the DEFRA (MAFF)

booklet Code of Practice for the prevention of rodent infestations in

poultry flocks and the HGCA booklet Rodent control in agriculture –

a guide.

5
1. WHERE RATS ARE LIKELY TO OCCUR

Rats survive best where the supply of food, water and

harbourage is uninterrupted. Under these conditions, they can breed all

year round and substantial populations can develop. Farm buildings

often supply suitable harbourages, with an abundance of food present,

either seasonally or permanently. Other favourable habitats include

refuse tips and sewer systems.

Strips of maize or kale grown as cover for pheasants provide food for rats long
after the main crop has been harvested. Water-filled ditches around fields are
ideal habitats for rats.

Hedgerows and ditches bordering arable fields also offer

permanent harbourage, but the food supply may be limited to summer

and autumn as crops ripen and seeds or fruits develop. Rats will also

take advantage of grain and cover crops, such as kale, left for game-

birds both before and during the shooting season. During spring and

autumn, recently sown seed may be dug up for food. Thus, rats living

along field margins may be able to find sufficient food to support them

throughout the year. They may, however, move into farm buildings and

6
grain stores in early winter if food becomes short, only to move back to

the fields later when all the stores are emptied. In contrast, livestock

farms, particularly intensive rearing units, may maintain a year-round

supply of animal feeds that rats can exploit. Hence, there is no need for

rats to move out in spring and an infestation may persist throughout the

summer.

In urban areas, other than sewers, rat colonies tend to be much

smaller than those on farms and may be found in gardens, parks,

warehouses, factories and restaurants. Some of these surface

infestations, particularly in central urban areas where sewer systems are

older, may originate from movements of rats through defective sewers.

2. ASPECTS OF RAT BIOLOGY RELEVANT TO CONTROL

Rats tend to be very wary of new objects (e.g. bait


containers) that appear in their home range
particularly if all other objects are rarely moved.

7
The most well known characteristic attributed to rats is their

avoidance of new objects (neophobia). Bait containers, unfamiliar

foods, traps and even totally harmless objects may be avoided for days

or weeks, particularly if there is no incentive (e.g. hunger, seeking

shelter) for the animals to investigate them. However, the response to

new objects is not consistent in all situations and the context in which an

object first appears may determine the speed with which it is

approached. Thus, in rapidly changing environments, such as a rubbish

tip face where fresh rubbish is continually deposited, neophobic

responses may be short-lived and perhaps do not occur at all.

At the other extreme, an environment such as a grain store may

be undisturbed for many weeks, so that rats may become very familiar

with its layout. Consequently, new objects may be easily distinguished

from the background, leading to active avoidance. Sometimes the

response is mixed with rats apparently more willing to approach

containers in some parts of a site and less willing elsewhere. Around

livestock units the constant movement of animals and turnover of food

stocks maintain a degree of habitat instability that makes bait containers

less readily distinguishable from other new objects.

Rats tend to avoid a new bait container for far longer than they

will avoid a new food. If bait is placed directly on the ground, it may be

8
approached relatively quickly, but it will not be readily eaten until the rats

are sure that it is safe to eat. They will eat small quantities at first, which,

if followed by unpleasant symptoms, will deter them from eating any

more. Hence, the best rodenticides should not produce symptoms of

poisoning within the first 24 hours, unless a lethal dose is likely to have

been ingested by the time symptoms occur. Anticoagulants are poisons

that are slow-acting and normally, by the time symptoms appear, a

sufficient dose has been consumed. Other rodenticides, such as

calciferol, act relatively quickly and if the rat recovers from the illness,

any future encounters with the same formulation, or even the same

bait base, or the poison in a different base, are likely to be avoided. This

learned aversion (sometimes called ‘bait shyness’) may extend to the

type of bait container used as well as the location of the bait point and

may last for the lifetime of the affected animals. The use of plain bait for

a period before laying the poison bait (i.e. prebaiting) often increases

bait consumption and consequently effectiveness, but it is seldom

completely successful at eradicating infestations.

Although neophobia is commonly cited as a cause of bait or trap

avoidance, it is not the only reason. Social pressures within a rat

population may prevent some individuals from approaching baits, but

the way they operate in the field is largely unknown. Competition

between rats over access to bait may explain why treatments with

9
potent anticoagulants are not noticeably shorter than those with the less

toxic compounds, given that less bait has to be eaten in order to receive

a lethal dose. Some individuals may only get to the bait once others

have succumbed to the effects of the poison.

Perhaps because rats are naturally antagonistic towards each

other, they tend to make several brief visits to a food source in order to

obtain a meal and avoid confrontations. At each visit they pick up a

mouthful and then often take it somewhere else to eat. Small particles,

such as grains of wheat, may be consumed entirely after being

transported, but larger items may be partially eaten only and the

remains abandoned. Transferring food is therefore normal for a rat, but

such behaviour poses a risk to other animals if the abandoned food is

poison bait. Placing bait directly into a rat’s burrow may make feeding

‘safer’ for the occupant, but unfortunately it does not stop visitors

‘stealing’ bait particles. (See below: S.4 –Selecting the best bait to

use)

The majority of commercially available rodenticide products to

control rats contain one of several anticoagulants. Anticoagulants have

had a good track record with regard to safety and efficacy compared

with many earlier rodenticides (e.g. zinc phosphide, red squill (the

former soon to be withdrawn from sale, the latter no longer approved)).

10
To overcome resistance to the first anticoagulants, such as warfarin,

more potent compounds with the same mode of action (‘second-

generation’ anticoagulants) were developed, although rats have also

now developed resistance to some of these. Resistance is an inherited

trait, so that through a process of selection, successive treatments with

anticoagulants may lead to populations that are not controlled at all.

How quickly this can occur is not fully understood. Partly, it depends on

the proportion of resistant survivors after each treatment and

subsequently how many breed successfully with other resistant rats.

Furthermore, resistance to the ‘second-generation’ anticoagulants, such

as difenacoum and bromadiolone seems to be a matter of degree with

some individuals more or less resistant than others. If most rats in an

infestation are highly resistant to one or both of them, then very little

control may be expected. Where the majority have a lesser degree of

resistance, the outcome is less certain and may depend on the rate at

which individual rats consume bait.

It is a fundamental biological principle that reliance on one class

of pesticides (i.e. anticoagulant rodenticides) will exert a continuing

selection pressure on rat populations towards increased resistance.

Using more potent (‘second-generation’) compounds may, temporarily,

remove the practical problems associated with control failure but is likely

to increase the selection pressure and eventually select for higher

11
degrees of resistance. This may be avoided if steps are taken to

eliminate resistant survivors by means other than the use of

anticoagulant rodenticides.

Resistance for some rats comes at a price, in that they require

above average amounts of dietary vitamin K1 in order to maintain

normal blood clotting function. They may obtain this from several

sources, but of concern to the pest controller should be any proprietary

animal feed that contains relatively large quantities of the vitamin

(particularly pig and poultry food), as rats may be able to get the

antidote at virtually the same time as getting the poison!

3. SELECTING THE RIGHT RODENTICIDE

Despite the problem of anticoagulant resistance, nearly all

currently registered rodenticide products contain an anticoagulant. This

group of poisons is commonly divided into ‘first-‘ and ‘second-

generation’ compounds, largely distinguished by the greater potency of

the latter, which were developed to overcome rodents resistant to the

former.

A list of rodenticides and formulations approved for use to control rats

is given below. For anticoagulants, a further distinction is sometimes

made between those that can cause mortality after only one feed and

12
those that require several feeds over perhaps 2 or more days. Broadly,

rats will eat about 10% of their body weight each day and to obtain a

diet containing all the essential nutrients for a healthy life, they will

probably have more than one source of food. Thus, where rats only

take part of their daily requirement from the bait point, it is an advantage

if that part contains a lethal dose of poison. Brodifacoum and

flocoumafen are typically described as ‘single-feed’ anticoagulants,

meaning that rats can receive a fatal dose in one feed, not that, in

practice, they always will. However, the symptoms of anticoagulant

poisoning normally take 2 or more days to appear, so that many rats

may consume more bait than is needed to kill them, increasing the

potential threat to predators and scavengers. This risk of secondary

poisoning means that products containing these active ingredients are

restricted to indoor sites only, such as within enclosed buildings or in

sewers. During treatments with any anticoagulant rodenticide, livestock,

pets, wildlife and humans must be considered at risk and bait should be

placed in such a way that non-targets cannot gain access. An antidote,

vitamin K1, is readily available to treat cases of accidental poisoning.

Rodenticides and formulations currently available for rat control

13
Type of rodenticide Active ingredient Formulations

Warfarin Loose grain

First-generation Loose grain, Contact


Coumatetralyl powder
anticoagulant
Chlorophacinone Liquid concentrate

Loose grain, Pellets, Wax


Difenacoum blocks, Place packs

Second-generation Loose grain, Contact


Bromadiolone powder, Pellets, Wax
anticoagulant
blocks, Place packs

Brodifacoum Loose grain, Pellets, Wax


blocks, Place packs

Flocoumafen Wax blocks

Non-anticoagulant Calciferol Liquid concentrate,


Pellets

Fumigant Aluminium phosphide Tablets

Note: Manufacturers may vary the availability of active ingredients and


formulations. Products containing the first-generation anticoagulant,
diphacinone, may also be available.

The choice of which anticoagulant to choose depends on the

likelihood that rats resistant to it are present. The full extent of

anticoagulant resistance in the UK is not known, but populations wholly

resistant to particular compounds have been found. However, it is more

likely that when ‘resistant’ populations are encountered, some rats will

be susceptible, so that a degree of control is usually possible in the

short term, but repeated use of the same active ingredient will increase

the proportion of resistant rats in the population and subsequent control

14
attempts will be prolonged or may fail. Hence, it is difficult to recommend

a particular anticoagulant, unless a sample of the population is tested

first. Always choosing the least potent anticoagulant first may waste

much time and money before discovering that it isn’t working.

Nevertheless this is likely to pose a lower risk to humans, pets, livestock

and wildlife. The aim is to strike the right balance between efficacy and

safety. If resistance is anticipated, difenacoum or bromadiolone should

be selected instead, but rats with a degree of resistance to at least one

of these rodenticides have been found across much of central southern

England, in parts of East Wales and the West Midlands and in the

counties of Norfolk, Humberside, North Yorkshire, Kent and Sussex.

However, only in a small area of NW Berkshire or Humberside is it likely

that treatments with difenacoum or bromadiolone will fail completely due

to resistance. Elsewhere, provided rats consume sufficient bait, some

degree of control can be expected. Brodifacoum and flocoumafen

should be fully effective against most, if not all, rat populations across

the whole country, but the restrictions on their use mean that they

cannot be used to control rats living outdoors.

If for some reason anticoagulants are not available or cannot be

used or significant resistance is known to be present, then rodenticide

formulations based on calciferol are an alternative option. These are

able to kill both anticoagulant-resistant and non-resistant rats, but

15
particular care is required in their use. They are toxic to other animals

and antidotes are not readily available. As they produce symptoms

quicker than anticoagulants, those rats not killed after their first or

second feed are likely to develop bait aversions. To minimise that effect,

it is advisable to prebait for at least several days with plain bait, so

helping to increase subsequent takes of poisoned bait. Care has to be

taken that prebaiting does not also encourage non-target animals to eat

the poison bait. It is usually not worth keeping the poison bait down for

longer than 5-7 days with calciferol, because any rats not poisoned by

then are unlikely to be controlled – the product label should give advice

on this matter.

4. SELECTING THE BEST BAIT TO USE

Rodenticide baits are formulated as loose-grain, pellets, wax

blocks and place-packs. All are cereal-based and manufacturers’

ready-made formulations may contain additives such as flavourings and

mould inhibitors. Whether rats will eat a bait or not depends both on

what they are used to eating and on their individual preferences. Within

a wild rat population, baits highly palatable to some individuals may not

appeal to others. It is not practicable to test a range of bait bases before

a treatment begins and with ready-made baits there is no alternative but

to use the product supplied by the manufacturer.

16
If it is possible to purchase a rodenticide concentrate, this should

be mixed with food that the rats are currently eating (provided that food

does not contain large amounts of vitamin K1). Alternatively, use

processed cereals, such as oatmeal and cut wheat, for making up baits.

In general, finely-ground cereals, which tend to be dry and ‘dusty’ are

less acceptable to rats than coarser-grained ones. Adding corn oil and

sugar, which may also mask the taste of a rodenticide, can enhance bait

palatability. Wheat grains, made succulent by soaking in water

overnight, are particularly attractive to rats. However, damp wheat

rapidly dries out in hot weather and may either sprout or go mouldy,

necessitating frequent bait replacement. Damp wheat is more suitable

for mixing with dry concentrates than liquid (oil) ones. (Note: It is a

breach of the approval conditions of a ready-to-use product to add

attractants, flavour-enhancers, etc. in order to ‘improve’ a bait’s

palatability.)

The cereals used for baits must be free from insects and mites so

that these pests are not spread to the sites that are being treated. A

mite infestation in the bait will also make it unpalatable to rats.

17
Rat removing a pellet from a box: rats will remove any kind of food
from any location where they don’t want to stay for more than a
few seconds. Preventing such transfer does not always persuade
them to eat food on the spot; they may ignore it instead.

The efficiency of a rodenticide treatment is greatly enhanced if

rats start to eat the bait as soon as it is laid. Although rats appear to eat

anything they fancy, they are essentially seedeaters, hence grain-sized

bait particles are ideal. Larger food items, such as blocks, may be less

readily recognised as being edible and may cause a delay in bait

uptake. Moreover, as such items take some time to eat, rats may prefer

to eat them somewhere else, rather than risk attack from other rats or

predators. Thus, wax blocks and place-packs (sachets) are

particularly prone to being carried away. To counteract bait transfer,

commercially available bait boxes may have a rod inside them for

holding blocks to encourage a rat to eat them in situ, but this is not

always successful. Some rats, unable to remove a secured block will

18
break it into smaller pieces and carry each piece away separately or

ignore it altogether. Rats may also carry away whole-wheat baits.

If, when bait points are inspected, any containers seem to be

unusually clean, with no crumbs of bait remaining and few smear marks

on them, then it should be suspected that the bait has been transferred

elsewhere. Wood mice (Apodemus sylvaticus) are also prone to hoard

food and may carry away rodenticide baits: they may be present in and

around farm buildings alongside a light infestation of rats. If bait is being

moved or hoarded, a search for it should be carried out, so that it can be

picked up before other animals find it.

5. CARRYING OUT THE TREATMENT

5.1 Surveying the site

Typical signs of rat presence such as burrows, runs and fresh

droppings define the limit of an infestation. The density of fresh signs

broadly indicates the density of rats. Around farmsteads, these may be

distributed inside and outside buildings and over adjacent ground. If field

boundaries, particularly hedgerows and ditches, radiate out from

buildings they too may be infested. Occasionally, infestations may

appear to extend continuously along connecting hedgerows, although

19
the frequency of fresh signs may vary considerably. In these

circumstances, it may be impracticable to treat the entire infestation.

Moreover, rats can move long distances in one night (500 m is common,

but 3 km or more is possible), and it may not be apparent that rats from

distant colonies are visiting the main population. Also, such visits may

not occur every night. Thus, even if it is not cost-effective to treat

outlying infestations, their existence should be noted, because they may

help to explain an unusually prolonged treatment.

As part of the preparation for a rodenticide treatment, it is

worthwhile drawing an accurate sketch map of the site. When the

treatment begins the location of bait points can be plotted on the map as

well as indicating them with a chalk mark on walls, fence posts, or on

the cover over the point. It might also be useful to mark on the map the

distribution of rat signs and any obvious food sources such as stored

grain and animal feed. To assess the progress of a treatment, simple

techniques can be used, such as weekly counts of the number of active

burrows or the presence/absence of fresh footprints on patches of

smoothed mud or sand laid at key points around a site.

5.2 Applying the bait

Rats are more likely to eat the bait sooner if it is laid beside runs,

in holes, along walls and in sheltered places, but it is not safe just to lay

20
rodenticide bait on the ground without some kind of cover to stop other

animals eating it. The HSE leaflet Safe use of rodenticides on farms

and holdings gives good advice on suitable bait containers and the use

of naturally occurring objects to protect the bait.

Two types of bait box: the one on the left has a lockable lid and high
internal baffles to prevent children, pets and wild animals bigger than rats from
reaching the bait – such containers are often described as ‘tamper-resistant’.
The box on the right has a low baffle and is less resistant to non-target
interference, but is probably more ‘rat-friendly’.

Rats typically visit bait containers for a few seconds only, time

enough to eat a grain of wheat or pick up a bait particle to take away.

They may stay longer if other (friendly) rats are with them, so that bait

boxes that allow ‘communal’ feeding may be better than ones with high

internal baffles that rarely allow more than one rat to feed at a time,

although such designs do discourage non-target animals from reaching

the bait. Whatever container is used, it will be very difficult to prevent

animals smaller than rats from entering.

21
Regardless of container type, if the rats are not already familiar

with it, they may avoid it initially, which will delay bait uptake and

possibly extend treatment time. It is therefore worthwhile setting out the

bait containers several days in advance of laying the bait so that the

animals become familiar with their presence. As a guide, trays should be

laid at least 3-5 days and boxes at least 5-7 days before the treatment

begins. If despite such actions, the majority of rats take little or no bait

over the first 2 weeks, then such treatments will probably make slow

progress and baiting tactics should therefore be changed immediately

(see Section 7.1). Otherwise, the long bait exposure periods will put

non-target animals at potentially greater risk.

Alternatively, on sites that seem prone to re-infestation, empty

boxes or other types of container can be positioned after an infestation

has been eradicated and before a new one appears. Immigrant rats are

unlikely to distinguish containers from any other object and thus should

not specifically avoid them. Baiting can then begin as soon as rat signs

appear. It is of course essential to site the containers in places that

invading rats are likely to colonise and to check them regularly to make

sure that access has not been blocked by growing vegetation. The

presence of rats in and around the containers can be monitored with

22
tracking patches made of fine sand or smoothed mud to record

footprints or indicator bait blocks that show tooth marks.

5.3 How many bait points?

Bait points should be sited close to fresh rat signs, such as by the

side of, or on, well-used runs, near active burrows or gaps in vegetation.

If rats are running along the inside or outside walls of a building, the

containers should be placed so that their entrances are adjacent to the

wall to allow rats running close to it to easily enter them. It is not

advisable to site points immediately next to piles of stored grain or other

edible commodities, as the baits may then be in direct competition with

the animals’ usual food source, as well as presenting a possible

contamination risk. Points should be sited so that they intercept rats on

their way to their normal feeding location. Suitable places for baits

should be where rats might nest such as junk piles, dung heaps, under

pallet stacks, beneath discarded machinery and rubbish tips. Bait points

can also be laid around the bottom of straw stacks, along field

boundaries (ditches, hedgerows) and around ponds.

23
Bait containers will normally be spaced several metres
apart, but they can be closer together if rat activity locally is
intense.

In environments where food and harbourage are unevenly

distributed, it is likely that the density of rats, and hence bait points, will

also be distributed irregularly. As a rough guide, an average density of 2

points per 10 x 10 m square or 1 point every 5 m should be sufficient to

treat most infestations, but in places where rats appear to be very

numerous, baits can be laid as close as 1 m apart. Increasing the

number of bait points above this average density does not appear to

increase the speed or degree of control, but conversely the density

cannot be reduced indefinitely without any effect on the treatment

outcome. The location of the individual points and the willingness of rats

that find them to take the bait are probably of more importance.

24
If too few points are laid, this will only become apparent after the

treatment has started. If more than one quarter of the points with a bait

take are ‘complete’ (i.e. all the bait has been eaten by rats) at one

inspection visit, more points should be added. On the other hand, it

would be a mistake to remove too early baits that have not been

touched, as rats need time to find them and overcome any hesitancy

about approaching them. Generally during anticoagulant treatments,

errors of bait placement can be rectified with some loss of efficiency, but

not overall effectiveness. This is not necessarily true for treatments with

fast-acting rodenticides that require rats to be conditioned to feed at a

bait point in order that they eat sufficient of the poison bait.

5.4 How much bait per point?

The amount of bait to place at each point depends on the

replenishment schedule, the toxicity of the rodenticide and the

infestation size. Guidance will normally be present on the product label.

With the less toxic anticoagulants such as warfarin, coumatetralyl or

chlorophacinone, the aim is to maintain an uninterrupted supply of bait

as individual rats may need to feed on it continuously for 3 or 4 or more

days. Relatively large quantities (200-400 g) should therefore be laid at

each point initially, particularly if the site is one where rats are likely to

be less wary of containers (e.g. on a refuse tip, some livestock farms).

25
When inspecting the baits, the quantity of bait should be doubled

where complete takes occur. Other bait points with partial takes

should be topped up to maintain a surplus. With the more toxic

anticoagulants, such as difenacoum and bromadiolone, quantities of bait

can be smaller at first, (e.g. 100 g), particularly if all the rats are known

or are likely to be susceptible. Initial bait size may be even smaller (50

g) if the most potent anticoagulants, brodifacoum and flocoumafen, are

used against indoor infestations. The important point is that rats should

be able to find sufficient bait whenever they want to, so that they ingest

a lethal dose as soon as possible. This may mean that baits are

replenished more frequently at first in order to keep the amount of bait in

line with the rats’ needs. Moreover, by matching bait quantity to the

numbers of rats willing to eat it, there will be less surplus bait available

should non-target animals accidentally get access to it.

As rats start to die, replenishments should become less frequent

and simultaneously the amount of bait per point can be reduced. If rats

appear reluctant to eat any bait, as they may do around a grain store,

the size of baits can be reduced to a token amount (say, 25 g) or

alternatively the bait replaced with plain food until such time as the rats

start to take it (see below).

26
Treatments with calciferol produce better results if effective

prebaiting with unpoisoned (plain) bait is undertaken first. This poison

can be used without a prebait, but the outcome is less certain. At all bait

points, it is best to lay plain food of the same type that will be mixed

with the poison (or of similar type to the ready-to-use bait) until as many

rats as possible are eating it. The delay in finding these new food

sources and then the suspicious nature of the animals towards them

means that takes build up slowly and only as the rats’ confidence grows

does the total bait take begin to stabilise. The initial size of prebaits can

be any amount, but 100-200 g is usually sufficient. Thereafter, a surplus

should be maintained by ‘doubling up’ all ‘complete’ takes. For large rat

populations, prebaiting may need to continue for up to 3 weeks,

although smaller ones may need only 3-5 days. The plain bait should be

checked daily, especially if the rats are readily attracted to it. If the rats

appear reluctant, then less frequent checks can be made until the takes

start building up. The rats may be judged to be feeding freely when

about the same total amount of bait has been eaten for 2-3 consecutive

days. Then all the prebait can be replaced with poison bait at all points

at which it was eaten. Those rats accustomed to feeding on the prebait

should eat the poison bait on the first night.

The quantity of calciferol bait at each point should be equal to

that on the last night of prebaiting. It is a false economy to cut the

27
prebaiting period short as this may increase the proportion of rats that

are sublethally poisoned and hence likely to become bait shy. If non-

target animals visit the same bait points as rats, those points should be

moved or provided with more protection. If non-target activity still

persists, poison bait should not be placed at the affected points.

5.5 How often should baits be inspected?

The frequency of bait inspections largely depends on the rats’

response, but also the likelihood of non-targets taking bait or eating

poisoned rats. During anticoagulant treatments, some or all of the rats

may ignore the bait completely or eat it without hesitation. During the

first week of baiting, at least two visits may be required to see how the

rats are reacting.

If the rate of bait uptake is slow and control is not urgently

required, a visit once a week thereafter may be sufficient to check that

the baits are still fresh, search for dead rats and ensure that other

animals have not found the bait.

If bait uptake is rapid, the first rat dead or dying of anticoagulant

poisoning may appear after about 4 days and then others in the

population may succumb over the following days and continue doing so

for several days after the last take of bait. While rats often die

28
underground, many do not and thus a search for bodies must be carried

out and any carcasses found should be disposed of safely to prevent

predators and scavengers eating them. Hence, during the first 2 weeks,

5 or more visits may be necessary not only to maintain a constant

supply of bait, but also to recover dead or dying rats. If all is well and

bait take and rat signs are clearly decreasing, 1-2 visits per week may

suffice thereafter, unless there are concerns about the safety of the

baits, in which case extra visits should be made.

Anticoagulant treatments can take as little as 2-3 weeks to be

effective, but those lasting 5 weeks should not be regarded as

abnormally long, particularly if the infestation is heavy. When a rat

population appears to be responding well and the number of animals is

clearly declining, bait points that have not had a take for 2-3 days may

be removed.

Calciferol baits can be left down for about 5-7 days, but should

be checked daily. Rats may die 4-10 days after consuming a lethal

dose, thus searching for bodies may need to continue beyond the end of

the treatment. As soon as takes cease at individual points, all bait

remaining in them should be removed.

When control is achieved, the options are:

1. to remove all remaining bait and containers;

29
2. to leave some containers in place and bait them with plain food

(e.g. indicator blocks/pellets), which should be monitored

regularly to check for signs of reinvasion;

3. following an anticoagulant treatment only, to leave some

containers with rodenticide in them for a further 6-8 weeks to

provide a source of bait should rapid immigration to the site

seem likely (these semi-permanent baits should be adequately

protected and further visits made to the site to monitor for signs

of reinfestation);

4. to establish permanent bait stations that contain an

anticoagulant formulation at all times (the number of stations

will usually be less than the number required to treat an active

infestation). Note: the non-anticoagulants are not suitable

for permanent baiting.

Option 4, permanent baiting using a few well-constructed, well-

sited bait boxes each containing a small quantity (say, 50 g or less) of

bait, is a preventative measure to control the occasional intruding rat in

order to stop a new infestation arising, but carries some risks if care is

not taken. When no rats are present, permanent baits should not be

placed around a site in the numbers and of a size needed to control an

active infestation in the expectation that they will prevent a rat

population becoming established at some time in the future. Other

30
animals, particularly small mammals and small birds, may enter bait

stations if there are no rats to deter them. Both they and the predators

that feed on them may subsequently be poisoned. Permanent baiting is

also ill advised if reinvasion is more or less constant and anticoagulant

resistance is suspected or known to be in the area. Such baiting

potentially maintains the selection pressure favouring resistant rats and

in time may seriously jeopardise the effectiveness of future

anticoagulant treatments. Also, any rats that are poisoned may be a

hazard for predators and scavengers if dead bodies are not found and

removed. To minimise the risk of non-target poisoning, permanent baits

should be well protected and frequently inspected, especially if rats and

other wild animals are likely to be short of alternative food. For example,

birds such as finches and hedge sparrows are prone to enter bait boxes

during frosty weather. In other circumstances, less frequent inspections

may suffice. In general, permanent baits outdoors will probably require

checking more often than those placed indoors, provided the risk of non-

target animals entering treated buildings is low.

5.6 Monitoring the treatment

If a treatment fails, it is necessary to ascertain why. The more

information that can be collected during the course of a treatment, the

easier it will be to diagnose the problem. The minimum information

should be:

31
1. a map of the site showing all the bait points;

2. the name of the product being used or the active ingredient and

the bait base;

3. the date the treatment started;

4. the quantity of bait laid at each point;

5. the dates of subsequent bait inspections and the condition of

each bait (i.e. not touched, completely eaten, partially eaten,

non-target interference).

Additionally, if it can be done cost-effectively, an independent

assessment of rat numbers before and after 2-3 weeks of treatment

would also be useful. It is good practice to monitor treatments

regardless of the final outcome as well-kept records

1. indicate the rate of progress;

2. help to optimise the amount of bait laid;

3. verify the need to change, if necessary, the treatment strategy;

4. indicate when dead rats are likely to appear and therefore the

need to search for them.

Good records also provide information in cases where accidental

poisoning has occurred and to indicate what actions were taken in

response. In the long-term, sufficiently detailed records may help to spot

a gradual loss of efficacy perhaps through increasing prevalence of

32
resistance. Before that loss becomes serious, alternative methods of

control should be implemented.

6. IS THE TREATMENT WORKING? SIGNS OF SUCCESS

Rats die, on average, 5-7 days after eating a lethal dose of

anticoagulant and then often underground, so there may be little visible

evidence of success during the first week. By the end of the second

week, however, there should be clear signs that the treatment is being

effective. With rat populations that are relatively easy to attract to bait

points, the peak consumption of bait and the maximum number of bait

points visited often occur within the first week or soon after. Thereafter,

there should be a steady decline in takes and points visited. A small rise

in takes during, say, the third, fourth or fifth weeks should not be

regarded as unusual, provided the general trend is downward. The signs

of an effective treatment are:

1. >50% of ‘active’ points have a take within the first 2 weeks of

the treatment (‘active’ refers to bait points that have been

‘found’ by rats i.e. those on or near fresh rat signs;

2. there are no ‘complete’ takes (i.e. all bait eaten) at each

inspection visit, or that the number of such takes is decreasing;

3. during the first two weeks each ‘active’ point has a take >50 g

over 2-3 days;

33
4. most fresh rat droppings are coloured by the warning dye that

is incorporated into commercial products;

5. dead rats appear, but if not, fresh rat signs are diminishing.

When rats eat poison baits, their droppings may be coloured by the warning

dye that manufacturers add to their products. A BLUE dye is common, but

other dyes (e.g. RED) may also be used. The difference between coloured

and normal droppings can clearly be seen in the picture.

After treatment with calciferol, success is apparent if:

1. most of the bait has been eaten at all points with a good

prebait take on the first or second night (baits may be eaten

over another 2-3 nights);

2. dead rats appear within a week;

3. within two weeks following the first application of poison bait,

fresh signs are difficult to find.

34
7. IS THE TREATMENT WORKING? INDICATORS OF POOR
CONTROL

7.1 No take at all/small, sporadic takes

Symptoms

a) Few, if any, ‘active’ points (i.e. baits with fresh rat signs near them),

but infestation persists.

b) Less than 50% of the ‘active’ bait points have a take, especially

during the first 2 weeks.

c) The amount eaten at each point is insignificant (<50 g over 2-3 days)

in relation to the number of rats as indicated by the density of signs.

d) Fresh droppings show no signs of dye.

[Note: a – c will also apply to the period of prebaiting before a zinc

phosphide or calciferol treatment.]

Possible causes

1. Errors in bait placement

• Too many points have been located in places that rats visit

infrequently or where rats feel too exposed to attack.

• Bait points have been placed just off well-used runs so that

they tend to be inadvertently missed rather than actively

35
avoided. If an infestation is light, fresh signs may be difficult to

find and the best place for a point is not immediately obvious.

• Bait containers have been put in place on the first day of the

treatment instead of several days in advance.

• Rats have been moving in inaccessible places such as in

hollow walls, above false ceilings, in drains or in narrow ‘dead’

spaces inside and outside buildings.

2. Neophobia towards container/novel food

• Rats have found the bait points, but then actively avoided them

as seen by the appearance of a ‘bypass’ (trampled soil/flattened

grass/smear marks) around each container. Without space to

divert, the run has been abandoned. Prone to occur on sites

with little disturbance and abundant resources.

3. Unattractive formulation

• The formulation is unpalatable or not attractive enough to divert

rats from their usual food. Tracks indicate that rats have visited

bait stations, leaving bait undisturbed, walked over or spread

about over the floor of the container.

• Baits have been placed too close to the rats’ normal food

source, which the animals visit first to feed on and then are not

hungry enough to want any bait.

36
• Rats used to feeding on loose grain have not recognised

rodenticide baits as edible if presented as wax blocks or sealed

inside a plastic bag.

Remedies

Depending on the urgency with which the infestation has to be

controlled:

• Re-site bait containers as necessary to correct misplacements,

but quicker results will be achieved by transferring bait from

containers to active rat burrows. Use a loose-grain (e.g. whole

wheat) or pelleted formulation in preference to other types and

lightly block the burrows with grass or hay/straw or cover the

entrances with material such as corrugated sheets or wooden

boards. Do not stamp on the entrance as this may cover the

bait with soil.

37
Placing bait inside active burrows is the most effective means of
encouraging rats to consume lethal amounts, but avoid overfilling or
blocking the entrance with heavy objects as the burrow may be
abandoned before any bait is eaten.

• If active burrows are difficult to find, do not exist, or for safety

reasons bait must only be presented in suitable containers,

change the formulation to loose grain or pellets, which rats are

more likely to recognise as edible. If either of those is already

being used and control is imperative, trapping or fumigating rat

burrows with aluminium phosphide may be the only options.

To make a significant impact with traps in a short time, some

skill is required. Fumigation (gassing) is only appropriate

where rat burrows are located away from buildings and should

only be carried out by fully trained operators.

• If the only option is to persist with the treatment and after 2

weeks no bait has been eaten, either reduce the quantity of

poison bait to a token amount, say 25 g, or alternatively replace

it with plain bait. This reduces the risk of other animals being

accidentally poisoned while waiting for rats to start taking bait.

This is especially relevant to outdoor bait points. When rats

eventually start to feed, increase the amount of poison bait or

switch back to the original formulation.

38
• Destabilising the habitat by moving objects around can

sometimes increase bait uptake at sites where little changes

from day to day. For example, stacks of bales that rats live in

can be dismantled, or bulk grain or animal feeds moved around

in a storage facility. Such a drastic change in the rats’

environment may only need to be done once, provided that it

immediately produces substantial bait takes.

• If there has been a poor response to the prebait prior to a

calciferol treatment, there is little point in applying the poison

bait. Doing so will kill only a few rats and may induce bait

shyness in any rats consuming the bait. Continue with the

prebaiting, changing the method of baiting as described above,

but if this is not feasible and anticoagulants cannot be used,

trapping and/or gassing may be the only practicable option.

7.2 Good takes initially, partial control, then poor bait takes

Symptoms

a) During anticoagulant treatments, baits found by rats have good takes

that may rise during the first 1-2 weeks before falling away quickly to

insignificant levels. Thereafter, small takes continue sporadically at

some points. Inspection of the baiting records may show that

39
particular points, or several adjacent points, have had very little bait

taken since the treatment began.

b) During a calciferol treatment (and after an effective prebaiting

period), bait takes drop rapidly after 1-2 days reaching near zero by

the end of the first week.

c) Dead rats are found and some areas become clear of infestation.

Elsewhere, fresh signs of rats are still present.

Possible causes

1. Neophobia, unattractive baits

• The reasons given under 1 above also apply, but the effects

of neophobia or unattractive bait vary across the infested area.

Typically, on farms with both grain stores and livestock-rearing

areas, rats can be more difficult to attract to bait in the former,

but much easier in the latter.

2. Bait aversion

• Some rats have become bait shy, because they felt ill within a

few hours of eating a small quantity of bait and linked their

symptoms to it (learned aversion). Bait aversion is especially

likely during treatments with relatively fast-acting poisons such

as calciferol. It does not normally occur with anticoagulants,

although the possibility is not entirely ruled out.

40
• If an anticoagulant formulation is being used, a more probable

explanation is poor acceptance to some rats in the target

population. Wide variation between individuals in their

acceptance of a single type of bait is common among wild rats.

Also, different anticoagulants are not all equally palatable to

rats.

Remedies

• If the survivors of a partially successful calciferol treatment have

developed an aversion, change not only the active ingredient

and bait base, but also the container and the location of the bait

point. A follow-up anticoagulant treatment is probably the most

practicable option to eliminate the remaining rats provided they

are susceptible. Baiting active burrows, if possible, is preferable

to setting out new stations, as this will eliminate any container

neophobia. Trapping may be quicker if only a few animals are

left, but may be the only option if the rats are anticoagulant

resistant.

• Survivors of an anticoagulant treatment are unlikely to have

developed an aversion due to sublethal poisoning. Thus it is not

necessary to change the containers or the location of baits. Try

another bait: if difenacoum in cut wheat seems unacceptable,

41
use bromadiolone on whole wheat, or vice versa. If that does

not work, bait active burrows, covering them as necessary. If the

burrow baits are rejected, the rats may have unusual taste

preferences or they may indeed have developed an aversion.

Trapping or gassing may then be the only option.

7.3 Good takes continuing

Symptoms

a) Persistent (i.e. >2 weeks) partial or complete takes at specific points.

b) Several adjacent bait containers empty at each inspection visit.

c) Bait takes cease at specific points, but then recur after 7 days.

d) After 2 weeks rat signs not diminishing, very few dead rats seen.

e) Most fresh rat droppings are coloured by the dye in the rodenticide

bait.

Possible causes

1. Under-baiting (a, b)

• The commonest cause of a poor treatment is too little bait at too

few points in relation to the number of rats. The first rats to

encounter the bait consume it completely, leaving nothing for

those that come later. Should they die, other rats may find

sufficient bait and start to feed. The treatment may still succeed

but only slowly. Alternatively, most rats may make many short

42
visits to the bait points consuming a small quantity each time, so

that by the time the bait runs out, the majority have not acquired

a lethal dose. In this case, few, if any, rats may die.

2. Bait transfer (hoarding) (d)

• Individual rats carry the bait away, grain by grain, block by

block. Generally, the larger the bait particle, the more likely it will

be carried off. Wax blocks and place packs secured inside a

container may be broken up into smaller places or split open

and then carried away piece by piece. Transferred bait may not

be eaten, but simply cached somewhere. Refilling the container

merely provides the stimulus for the particular rat(s) involved to

carry bait away again. On inspection, the container seems

relatively clean, with no fragments of bait remaining.

3. Other animals eating the bait (d)

• Mice typically leave plenty of droppings in a bait container that

they visit and they also eat certain foods in ways distinct from

rats. Thus individual grains of whole wheat eaten by mice have

an ‘apple-core’ appearance, whereas grains eaten by rats look

like ‘banana ends’. Where wax-block baits have been partially

eaten, the size of the incisor tooth marks distinguishes rats from

mice.

43
• Small birds (e.g. chaffinches, sparrows) tend to scatter loose-

grain bait over the bottom of the container and may also leave

droppings. Larger birds, particularly those with long necks such

as chickens and pheasants, may be able to reach inside boxes

that do not have baffles to stop them. If the birds cannot reach

all the bait, the bait remaining may show a characteristic ‘V’

shape (loose-grain bait only) indicating the limit to which they

were able to stretch their necks.

• If bait points show signs of considerable disturbance, such as

missing cover or upturned or crushed containers, then cattle,

sheep, pigs or dogs may have been responsible. Where this

happens, foxes and badgers may gain access to bait.

4. Reinvasion (c)

Recolonisation of cleared areas may be very rapid (i.e. within a

few days) if rats are close (i.e. <200 m) to the site. Baiting

records will show consecutive takes at specific points followed

by 2-3 inspection visits when no takes are recorded. Then takes

begin again from those same points.

Rats from more distant colonies (even a neighbouring farm) may

move into a site during the course of a treatment. The rate of

reinvasion from such places may be very low and difficult to

44
detect. The only indication will be that fresh signs suddenly

appear in areas thought to be rat free.

5. Resistance (a, b, d, e)

• If none of above apply, then resistance should be suspected.

• Resistance to calciferol has never been recorded and therefore

is most unlikely.

• During anticoagulant treatments, if the infestation is light, or the

number of resistant animals is relatively small, the pattern of bait

takes will show persistent takes of >25 g/day at specific points.

Against heavy infestations with a large number of resistant rats,

frequent ‘doubling up’ of bait may result in large quantities (>1

kg) at some points.

• Treatments with warfarin or other first-generation anticoagulants

against populations containing mostly warfarin-resistant rats will

kill very few rats. Similarly, very few will die if most are highly

resistant to difenacoum/bromadiolone and

difenacoum/bromadiolone formulations are being used. Effective

control may be possible with difenacoum/bromadiolone if rats

with only a lower degree of resistance are present, provided that

the amounts of bait they consume are adequate.

• Populations containing high proportions of resistant rats seldom

appear suddenly. More likely, a succession of partially

45
successful treatments leaves an increasing proportion of

resistant individuals each time. At some point the number of

resistant rats rises to a level where the reduced effectiveness of

treatments becomes obvious. If good records of previous

treatments are maintained, early detection of resistance may be

possible before serious control problems emerge.

Remedies

• Under-baiting is easily rectified. Simply increase the density of

bait points. The best baiting tactic is a relatively large number of

bait points each with a relatively small amount of bait, rather

than a small number of points each with a large amount. More

bait stations increases the chance of intercepting rats as they

move around their habitat, particularly if rats have many options

on which way to go.

• Bait transfer (and hoarding) may be difficult to stop, especially

if the rats that do it are not eating the bait they carry away, but

merely leaving it somewhere. Securing wax blocks or place

packs inside a container may partially solve the problem, but

could result in no take at all or the bait broken up and then

carried away piece by piece. Reducing the particle size of the

bait may make transfer less likely. Thus, replace wax

46
blocks/place packs with whole wheat (i.e. split open place

packs) and whole wheat with cut wheat or oatmeal.

• If other animals are eating the bait, immediate action is

required, as most are susceptible to the rodenticides currently

available. It will be difficult to exclude animals smaller than rats.

Takes by small birds are more likely during prolonged cold

spells (freezing conditions) when food is difficult to find and less

likely during the breeding season. Changing the type of

container may deter birds prone to enter one particular design.

Wax-block formulations are less attractive to birds and, it

appears, to rats also and can be used instead of loose-grain

bait. Mice and voles may eat any type of bait at any time,

although the latter may do so infrequently. Protection for baits

disturbed by larger animals may need considerable reinforcing

to prevent further interference. Ultimately, bait points may have

to be removed rather than re-sited if other animals persist in

visiting them.

• Rapid recolonisation (reinvasion) should be prevented by

extending the treated area in any direction where rats are found.

Thus, in addition to farm buildings, nearby infested hedgerows

and ditches should be included. Bait to the limit of the visible rat

47
signs along the field margins. If some rats are coming from more

distant places and are visiting a site for the first time or visit

irregularly, visible signs of their travels may be difficult to spot.

As farm rats may travel 500 m or more in one night, the source

of these animals should be found and treated. Large numbers of

rats are often found on rubbish tips between fields, around

pheasant feeders and in any place where a food supply exists. If

that supply is suddenly removed, the rats will move away. On a

large scale, this is likely to happen in the post-harvest period

when fields are ploughed up and any grain left by the combines

is buried.

• A potential downside of treating a wider area to prevent

reinvasion is that this could lead to an increase in selection

pressure towards anticoagulant resistance. A control strategy

that allows some susceptible individuals (i.e. those outside the

treated area) to survive may prevent later infestations being

entirely composed of resistant individuals. Thus, after a partially

successful treatment, any invading susceptible rats could help

dilute the resistant proportion of the residual population. By

deliberately allowing some rats to survive, the potential benefit

of reduced selection pressure should be weighed against the

risk of disease transmission from immigrant rats.

48
• Resistant rats should be eliminated without applying further

selection pressure. Preferably, such elimination should be

attempted using non-anticoagulant rodenticides, gassing or

physical control methods e.g. trapping. If anticoagulants have to

be used, bromadiolone or difenacoum should kill rats resistant

to warfarin and other first-generation anticoagulants. Both are

available for outdoor use, but some warfarin-resistant rats are

resistant to those also. However, few rat populations are

completely resistant to difenacoum or bromadiolone treatment,

thus some control will be possible. The key is to encourage rats

to feed well on the bait by adopting baiting methods that

maximise bait uptake. Nevertheless, the most resistant animals

may survive and to prevent an even greater resistance problem

developing in the future, all available control options should be

utilised. This includes the use of brodifacoum and flocoumafen,

but only inside buildings with limited access for rats to the

outside.

8. LONG-TERM MANAGEMENT OF RAT PROBLEMS

49
The end of a treatment is the appropriate time to carry out or

make recommendations about hygiene and proofing to prevent

reinfestation. Poor hygiene practices often provide rats with essential

food and shelter. Removing these vital resources is one of the most

basic preventative measures. Proofing is the structural modification of

buildings to prevent rats getting in or to limit their spread once inside.

The DEFRA (formerly MAFF) Technical Bulletin No 12, ‘Proofing of

Buildings against Rats and Mice’ gives more details.

Some specific recommendations are:

• Fill holes with concrete or cement mortar (small rats can

squeeze through a 10 mm crack).

• Fix kicking plates to doors.

• Repair defective drains.

• On farms, cap breeze-block walls to prevent rats getting inside

them.

• Remove food at night put out for pets and birds during the day.

• Use dustbins with well-fitting lids and do not overfill them.

• Store food in rat-proof containers or in rat-proof storerooms.

• Prevent rubbish accumulating and cut back vegetation on

rough ground to deny rats shelter.

50
Non-poisonous bait blocks can be used
to indicate the presence of rats

If after all practicable hygiene and proofing measures have been

taken, a site still seems prone to reinfestation, permanent bait stations

can be installed to intercept incoming rats. Such stations should

preferably contain non-toxic indicator baits instead of anticoagulant

formulations because of the risk of selecting for anticoagulant resistance

and accidentally poisoning non-target animals. When rats are detected,

the indicator bait can be replaced with a rodenticide product. Section

5.5 gives advice on permanent baiting with a rodenticide.

Rodenticide treatments should be seen as short-term solutions

for the control of rat infestations and for that purpose they are generally

efficient, provided care is taken to minimise risks to other animals.

However, should anticoagulant resistance (particularly to second-

generation compounds) become more widespread, such treatments

may quickly lose their cost-effectiveness. To delay, or perhaps prevent

51
that happening, it is essential to minimise the selection pressure for

resistance. In practice, this means using non-anticoagulant methods

(zinc phosphide/calciferol treatments, gassing burrows away from

buildings, trapping, physical exclusion, habitat modification) as much

as possible once resistance is detected or suspected. Even if resistance

does not yet present a practical control problem in an area, it may be

prudent to alternate non-anticoagulant methods with anticoagulant

treatments. No new rodenticides are likely to be introduced for some

considerable time. Moreover, it is important to eliminate by any

legitimate means any surviving rats from an anticoagulant treatment.

The ease with which rodenticides can be used should not deter

occupiers from determining why infestations arose in the first place and

then taking appropriate remedial action. Resistance and risks to non-

targets mean that long-term solutions to keeping rat numbers down

should rely less and less on rodenticides. Put simply, prevention is

better than rodenticide cure.

52
GLOSSARY
Aluminium phosphide: see Fumigation

Anticoagulants: Discovered by chance during an investigation

into a haemorrhagic disease in cattle in the 1930s. A synthetic derivative

of the causative chemical agent was named warfarin and was

introduced as a rodenticide in the UK around 1950. Anticoagulants

prevent the manufacture of clotting agents inside the body, leading to a

fatal haemorrhage in 4-10 days. Several other anticoagulants have been

developed in the intervening years mainly in response to the

development of resistance to warfarin, but all have the same mode of

action.

Bait container: Available commercially as trays, tunnels, or

boxes made of moulded plastic or metal, the latter material less

preferred by rats. Boxes are frequently marketed with the description

‘tamper-resistant’ and have internal baffles to prevent animals larger

than rats reaching the bait. Home-made containers (wooden boxes,

drain pipes) can be just as effective as bait dispensers, but may need

extra cover or more frequent inspections to minimise the risk of

accidental poisoning.

53
Bait shyness: Typically results in little or no bait being eaten.

The term is sometimes used to refer to avoidance of bait because of its

novelty to rats and at other times to the avoidance of bait following an

unpleasant experience (illness). The latter condition is more

appropriately called a ‘learned aversion’.

Bait transfer: The removal of bait particles by rats from a supply

source to any other location. The supply source may be a protected

container and such behaviour may circumvent attempts by pest

controllers to prevent bait being eaten by other animals, particularly if

the individuals that carry bait away then abandon particles in the open.

Brodifacoum: One of the most potent second-generation

anticoagulants introduced commercially in 1979. Rats do not normally

die quicker from brodifacoum poisoning than from warfarin poisoning,

but a lethal dose can be ingested after one feed. Nevertheless, rats

showing some degree of resistance to brodifacoum have been found,

although this is not thought to be of practical importance.

Bromadiolone: A second-generation anticoagulant that became

available commercially in the UK after 1980. One important attribute

appears to be that it does not markedly reduce the palatability of baits in

contrast to other anticoagulants that clearly do so. Rats highly resistant

54
to the compound have been discovered in Berkshire and Yorkshire to

the extent that virtually no control of some populations has been

possible.

Calciferol: A non-anticoagulant rodenticide used in the form

ergocalciferol (vitamin D2) and more recently cholecalciferol (vitamin D3)

to kill resistant rats and mice. The mode of action is to stimulate the

absorption of calcium from the intestines and to mobilise calcium from

bones causing calcification of soft tissue, particularly in the major

arteries and kidneys. Calciferol is also toxic to other mammals and birds

and there is some evidence that secondary poisoning may be possible.

Chlorophacinone: A first-generation anticoagulant, it belongs to

a different chemical group, the indane-diones, to warfarin and

coumatetralyl. No more or less effective than warfarin against

susceptible rats and largely ineffective against warfarin-resistant

animals.

Complete take: A commonly used term to indicate that all bait

has been eaten by rats between two inspection visits. Can be denoted

by ‘C’ on record sheets. Too many complete takes during the first week

of an anticoagulant treatment almost certainly suggests that too few

baits have been laid.

55
Concentrate: A rodenticide dissolved in an edible oil or mixed

dry with a finely ground cereal that requires further dilution in order to

obtain the most effective (and approved) concentration of poison.

Concentrates are useful when rats cannot be attracted to ready-to-use

formulations, allowing a bait base that they are already eating or one

more attractive to be used instead. Damp baits are often very attractive

to rats and whole wheat soaked in cold water for a few hours has proved

very successful. A manufacturer’s agreement is sometimes needed

before users can be supplied with concentrates.

Coumatetralyl: A first-generation anticoagulant, introduced a few

years after warfarin and appeared to be reasonably effective against

warfarin-resistant rats. However, initial optimism was soon dispelled

when rats resistant to both warfarin and coumatetralyl were discovered.

One advantage of coumatetralyl over warfarin is that at concentrations

over 0.025% it does not markedly reduce the palatability of bait.

Difenacoum: The first second-generation anticoagulant to be

introduced (1974). It proved to be particularly effective against some

warfarin-resistant rats, but not all of them. Almost immediately, rats

resistant to it were discovered in Hampshire in populations where the

average prevalence of warfarin resistance was 85%. Resistance to

56
second-generation compounds is not uniform within or between

populations, with some individuals/populations succumbing after

ingesting doses that are only slightly larger than that normally lethal to

susceptible animals, while others can feed for several days on nothing

but rodenticide bait and survive.

Doubling up: A term referring to the replenishment of bait

following an inspection that finds no bait remaining and signs that it was

all eaten by rats. For example, if 200g has been eaten, replenish the

container with 400g, 500g with 1 kg.

Fast-acting: A description of the speed of the rodenticidal

process often leading to death within 24 hours. Likely to cause rats to

die on the surface rather than underground, so that a successful

treatment will be immediately apparent. A disadvantage of fast-acting

poisons is that unless they are species-specific, there may be no time to

administer an antidote, even if one is available, if accidents occur.

First-generation (anticoagulant): A term used to describe the

many compounds that were introduced before 1970, of which only

warfarin, coumatetralyl and chlorophacinone currently remain registered

for use in the UK. Chlorophacinone belongs to a different chemical

57
group (indane-diones) to all other anticoagulants (hydroxycoumarins),

but the mode of action appears to be the same.

Flocoumafen: A highly potent second-generation anticoagulant,

with the same restrictions on its use as brodifacoum (indoors only). The

resistance to it that has been discovered appears, as with brodifacoum,

to be of no practical importance. Thus, both brodifacoum and

flocoumafen should be effective against rats that are resistant to other

second-generation compounds, but it is difficult to see how they can be

used effectively indoors to control rats that live mainly outdoors.

Formulation: The active ingredient mixed with an edible base

attractive to rats. The base may be a cereal such as whole or cut wheat,

either loose or ground and compressed into a pellet or moulded into a

wax block. Many commercial formulations are ready to use, but

occasionally rats ignore them preferring their normal food supply. In

such cases, if a manufacturer sells a concentrate, then this can be

mixed with the same food that the rats are eating.

Fumigation: An alternative means of controlling rats in situations

where other rodenticides cannot be used. The technique covers ad hoc

gassing of rat burrows to the highly specialised treatment of

commodities stored in silos, ships and warehouses. To control rats in

58
burrows phosphine gas fills tunnel systems when a pellet or tablet of

aluminium phosphide is placed well inside each entrance, which is then

sealed. Moisture present in the atmosphere or soil causes the gas to

evolve.

Habitat modification: Altering features of the environment to

make them unattractive to rats. One technique is to keep cover, such as

vegetation, cut short enough to increase the vulnerability of rats to

predator attack. Removing discarded machinery/building

materials/empty storage cases etc. can achieve the same effect. As rats

often nest outdoors, but may find their food indoors, the aim is to make

foraging trips more risky. The consequence should be that predators will

take more rats or that rats will move away to find better cover. Either

way population density is reduced. It is important that any modification

of a habitat does not disadvantage other species.

Harbourage: Virtually anything that provides rats with shelter.

Typically around farm buildings this can be dung heaps, junk piles, pallet

stacks, straw bales as well as undisturbed ground between buildings

where the soil is easy to excavate. Rats may also exploit hollows walls

and false ceilings and often dig under buildings if the foundations are

shallow. It will rarely be possible to remove all sources of harbourage.

59
Rats can find shelter in ‘natural’ features such as along hedgerows,

ditches, streams and riverbanks or under tree roots.

Indicator bait: Non-toxic bait in the form of a block or pellet that

can be laid at strategic points around a site to give early warning of

rodent activity before an infestation becomes established. The blocks

have central fixing holes so that if desired they can be secured in place

to prevent rats carrying them away. In such cases, rat activity is likely to

be revealed by incisor marks found on partially gnawed blocks.

Learned aversion: see Bait shyness

Loose-grain: Description of a formulation type, usually based in

commercial products on cereals that are either whole or coarsely

ground. Baits based on food types (especially in relation to particle size,

texture and taste) that are ‘natural’ to rats should always be acceptable,

so that failure to eat them is due to some other factor. A practical

disadvantage of loose-grain baits is that they are easily dispersed into

the environment if containers are disturbed and any spillage is

consequently not easily retrieved.

Neophobia: Fear of new objects, even those that are completely

harmless. It’s been argued that intensive control of rats with baits and

60
traps over many decades has led to the selection of individuals in some

areas that appear to be abnormally suspicious and are therefore

particularly difficult to eliminate. However, this is more likely a

misinterpretation of events in that rats apparently ‘avoiding’ baits do so

because alternative food is more easily accessible. Extreme neophobia,

as a heritable trait, would seem to be a poor survival strategy to cope

with frequently changing conditions.

Partial take: A convenient description for a quantity of bait eaten

by rats at a bait point. Often denoted by ‘P’ on record sheets to

distinguish it from ‘NT’ (no take) or ‘C’ (complete take) or ‘D’ (disturbed).

It may be useful to indicate how much bait has been eaten for each

partial take with a fractional estimate e.g. ⅓ or ⅔.

Pellet: A type of formulation made by mixing finely divided

cereals with a rodenticide concentrate and then forcing the resulting

mixture through a die under pressure to bind the constituents together.

Rodenticide in the extruded pellet should be evenly dispersed, which

may improve the acceptability of baits containing an intrinsically

unpalatable poison. Such a poison, when mixed with loose cereal

grains, normally adheres to the surface of individual particles (vegetable

oil is commonly used as a sticker) and thus might be more readily

detected by rats.

61
Permanent baiting: Maintaining quantities of rodenticide bait

(usually an anticoagulant formulation) at one or more locations around a

site at all times whether or not rats are present. Permanent baiting may

occur (by default) if uneaten bait is not retrieved at the end of a

treatment, or if a treatment aim of total eradication is never achieved.

Physical exclusion: The simplest form of exclusion is often to

keep the door shut! Barriers such a rodent-proof doors and fences have

rarely been considered cost-effective. Makeshift barriers can be used to

protect temporary stocks of small-volume commodities. Alternative

means of excluding rodents have included chemical, ultrasonic and

electromagnetic repellents, but none have so far proved to be reliably

effective.

Place-pack: Essentially, loose-grain bait sealed inside a plastic

bag (sachet). Like wax blocks, easy to use, but unless secured in place

may be carried away by rats. Also, wild rats do not seem to readily open

sealed sachets, which means the bait inside them may not be eaten for

some time. In such circumstances it may be better to use a different

formulation or empty the contents out.

62
Prebaiting: A technique introduced to encourage rats to eat

lethal doses of fast-acting rodenticides. Prebaiting seeks to persuade

individual rats to eat larger and larger quantities at each visit, as well as

attracting increasing numbers of rats away from their usual food source.

Against large populations these two processes can take at least 3

weeks to complete, which is indicated by approximately the same

amount of bait (in total) being eaten by rats each day for 3 days.

Resistance: A trait found in rats (and other rodent pests)

enabling them to survive doses of poison that would normally be

expected to kill. An important aspect is that the characteristic can be

passed to the next generation. Animals may develop an increased

tolerance to a poison through repeated sub-lethal dosing, but such

acquired tolerance would not be heritable. Resistance to the

anticoagulant warfarin was first discovered within 10 years of its

commercial introduction as a rodenticide and has been found in widely

separated parts of the UK. This suggests that it has arisen

independently and not by the movement of rats across the country.

While resistance is an all-or-none characteristic, its detection in the

animal is subject to errors of measurement. Consequently, claims about

the practical significance of resistance, especially to second-generation

anticoagulants, should be supported by more than one confirmatory

63
test. In the UK, resistance to the non-anticoagulant, calciferol, has never

been found.

Secondary poisoning: A risk associated with rodenticide

treatments, when predators, such as foxes, stoats and weasels, and

scavengers, such as red kites, eat rats or other small animals that have

eaten poison bait and then they become casualties as well. While

primary poisoning (non-target animals eating the bait directly) can be

prevented by adequate protection of bait points, the risk of secondary

poisoning can be reduced by regularly searching for and disposing of

dead or dying rats/mice/small birds.

Second-generation (anticoagulant): Introduced primarily to

counter resistance to first-generation anticoagulants that appeared not

only in the UK, but also in other European countries and the USA. They

retain the same mode of action as the previous compounds and are

considerably more toxic not only to rats, but also to other species.

Selection pressure: Increased by reliance on only one means to

control rats, decreased by alternating the use of one method with

another and removed entirely by using a different control method every

time. In relation to anticoagulant resistance, repeated or permanent use

of anticoagulants eliminates all susceptible rats leaving only resistant

64
animals to breed amongst themselves. To take advantage of the

detrimental effects that some rats face by being resistant,

anticoagulants should not be used again until the resistant strain has

died out. Recent research suggests that for some resistant rats, this

period may be several years long.

Single-feed: A feature of rodenticide formulations that are so

toxic to the target species that a lethal dose can be ingested after one

feed. The term has tended to replace ‘acute’ that was used to describe

non-anticoagulant, fast-acting rodenticides. Poisons that have to be

ingested over 2 or more days were described as ‘chronic’, a term now

commonly replaced with ‘multi-feed’. Even warfarin will kill some rats

after one feed, but the majority of a population will only succumb after 2

or more feeds. Equally, resistant rats may need more than one feed to

succumb to a ‘single-feed’ anticoagulant bait, as will susceptible rats

that only eat very small amounts when visiting bait points.

Slow-acting: A description of the rodenticidal process, in which

symptoms of poisoning appear a long time after a rat has fed on bait, so

that it is unable to associate its illness with a particular food type or its

taste. A characteristic of anticoagulants that has undoubtedly been

responsible for their success, as it minimises the likelihood of learned

aversions developing.

65
Vitamin K: The antidote to anticoagulant poisoning. Natural

foods containing vitamin K1 and manufactured animal feeds enriched

with vitamin K3 help resistant rats to thrive in the absence of

anticoagulants. Some strains of resistant rat require additional vitamin K

in order to maintain normal blood clotting activity. Vitamin K3 may also

protect resistant rats from small doses of second-generation

anticoagulants.

Warfarin: One of the first hydroxycoumarin anticoagulants to be

synthesised and marketed as a rodenticide to control commensal

rodents. Its use has declined in recent years, as products containing

second-generation anticoagulants are preferred by users. It is a feature

of warfarin and other similar compounds that a single lethal dose is often

several times greater than the sum of 4-5 daily doses that also cause

death.

Warning dye: An additive in rodenticide formulations, often a

food-grade colouring agent to discourage children, in particular, from

eating poison bait by mistake. An additional technique used by

manufacturers to increase safety is to add low concentrations of a

bittering agent, such as denatonium benzoate, so that people eating bait

accidentally will be repelled by the bitter taste.

66
Wax block: A type of formulation in which whole, finely- or

coarsely- ground cereals are mixed with a quantity of paraffin wax and

then cast or compressed to form a block that may weigh 15-225g

depending on the manufacturer. Blocks are often brick-shaped with

multiple gnawing edges and have a central hole for securing them in

place, such as inside a box. An early use of block baits was to anchor

them in manholes to control sewer rats, since coarse cereal baits could

easily be washed away. However, the results were variable, not least

because it was suspected that rats did not always recognise block baits

as food. Nevertheless, block formulations are easy to use, relatively

weather-resistant and seem to be less attractive to birds, thus offering

added safety to rodenticide treatments.

67

You might also like