Rat Control Guidance
Rat Control Guidance
November 2002
1
INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................................3
GLOSSARY........................................................................................................53
2
INTRODUCTION
practice of rodent control leading to far more effective control than there
had ever been. Subsequent research helped devise the best methods of
control being viewed all too often as a routine procedure. Despite the
advances, rodent problems have not gone away, partly because rats are
highly adaptable and often refuse to eat the bait or seem to eat it
3
the cycle of increasing the potency of the rodenticide to counter the
rodenticides seem the best option, then these guidelines should help to
palatable baits do not always draw rats away from what may appear to
a food item may stop some rats from approaching it. The solution may
4
Although rodenticides remain the most frequently used tools for
control, they are hazardous to other animals and public concern about
the risks of children, pets, livestock and wildlife eating the bait or in
some cases eating poisoned rodents has inevitably brought their use
on their use. This includes getting the most out of existing products and
and to complement the leaflet from DEFRA RDS (formerly MAFF FRCA)
a guide.
5
1. WHERE RATS ARE LIKELY TO OCCUR
Strips of maize or kale grown as cover for pheasants provide food for rats long
after the main crop has been harvested. Water-filled ditches around fields are
ideal habitats for rats.
and autumn as crops ripen and seeds or fruits develop. Rats will also
take advantage of grain and cover crops, such as kale, left for game-
birds both before and during the shooting season. During spring and
autumn, recently sown seed may be dug up for food. Thus, rats living
along field margins may be able to find sufficient food to support them
throughout the year. They may, however, move into farm buildings and
6
grain stores in early winter if food becomes short, only to move back to
the fields later when all the stores are emptied. In contrast, livestock
supply of animal feeds that rats can exploit. Hence, there is no need for
rats to move out in spring and an infestation may persist throughout the
summer.
7
The most well known characteristic attributed to rats is their
foods, traps and even totally harmless objects may be avoided for days
new objects is not consistent in all situations and the context in which an
be undisturbed for many weeks, so that rats may become very familiar
Rats tend to avoid a new bait container for far longer than they
will avoid a new food. If bait is placed directly on the ground, it may be
8
approached relatively quickly, but it will not be readily eaten until the rats
are sure that it is safe to eat. They will eat small quantities at first, which,
poisoning within the first 24 hours, unless a lethal dose is likely to have
calciferol, act relatively quickly and if the rat recovers from the illness,
any future encounters with the same formulation, or even the same
bait base, or the poison in a different base, are likely to be avoided. This
type of bait container used as well as the location of the bait point and
may last for the lifetime of the affected animals. The use of plain bait for
a period before laying the poison bait (i.e. prebaiting) often increases
between rats over access to bait may explain why treatments with
9
potent anticoagulants are not noticeably shorter than those with the less
toxic compounds, given that less bait has to be eaten in order to receive
a lethal dose. Some individuals may only get to the bait once others
other, they tend to make several brief visits to a food source in order to
mouthful and then often take it somewhere else to eat. Small particles,
transported, but larger items may be partially eaten only and the
poison bait. Placing bait directly into a rat’s burrow may make feeding
‘safer’ for the occupant, but unfortunately it does not stop visitors
‘stealing’ bait particles. (See below: S.4 –Selecting the best bait to
use)
had a good track record with regard to safety and efficacy compared
with many earlier rodenticides (e.g. zinc phosphide, red squill (the
10
To overcome resistance to the first anticoagulants, such as warfarin,
How quickly this can occur is not fully understood. Partly, it depends on
infestation are highly resistant to one or both of them, then very little
resistance, the outcome is less certain and may depend on the rate at
remove the practical problems associated with control failure but is likely
11
degrees of resistance. This may be avoided if steps are taken to
anticoagulant rodenticides.
normal blood clotting function. They may obtain this from several
(particularly pig and poultry food), as rats may be able to get the
former.
made between those that can cause mortality after only one feed and
12
those that require several feeds over perhaps 2 or more days. Broadly,
rats will eat about 10% of their body weight each day and to obtain a
diet containing all the essential nutrients for a healthy life, they will
probably have more than one source of food. Thus, where rats only
take part of their daily requirement from the bait point, it is an advantage
meaning that rats can receive a fatal dose in one feed, not that, in
may consume more bait than is needed to kill them, increasing the
pets, wildlife and humans must be considered at risk and bait should be
13
Type of rodenticide Active ingredient Formulations
likely that when ‘resistant’ populations are encountered, some rats will
short term, but repeated use of the same active ingredient will increase
14
attempts will be prolonged or may fail. Hence, it is difficult to recommend
first. Always choosing the least potent anticoagulant first may waste
and wildlife. The aim is to strike the right balance between efficacy and
England, in parts of East Wales and the West Midlands and in the
should be fully effective against most, if not all, rat populations across
the whole country, but the restrictions on their use mean that they
15
particular care is required in their use. They are toxic to other animals
quicker than anticoagulants, those rats not killed after their first or
second feed are likely to develop bait aversions. To minimise that effect,
taken that prebaiting does not also encourage non-target animals to eat
the poison bait. It is usually not worth keeping the poison bait down for
longer than 5-7 days with calciferol, because any rats not poisoned by
then are unlikely to be controlled – the product label should give advice
on this matter.
mould inhibitors. Whether rats will eat a bait or not depends both on
what they are used to eating and on their individual preferences. Within
a wild rat population, baits highly palatable to some individuals may not
16
If it is possible to purchase a rodenticide concentrate, this should
be mixed with food that the rats are currently eating (provided that food
processed cereals, such as oatmeal and cut wheat, for making up baits.
less acceptable to rats than coarser-grained ones. Adding corn oil and
sugar, which may also mask the taste of a rodenticide, can enhance bait
rapidly dries out in hot weather and may either sprout or go mouldy,
for mixing with dry concentrates than liquid (oil) ones. (Note: It is a
palatability.)
The cereals used for baits must be free from insects and mites so
that these pests are not spread to the sites that are being treated. A
17
Rat removing a pellet from a box: rats will remove any kind of food
from any location where they don’t want to stay for more than a
few seconds. Preventing such transfer does not always persuade
them to eat food on the spot; they may ignore it instead.
rats start to eat the bait as soon as it is laid. Although rats appear to eat
bait particles are ideal. Larger food items, such as blocks, may be less
uptake. Moreover, as such items take some time to eat, rats may prefer
to eat them somewhere else, rather than risk attack from other rats or
commercially available bait boxes may have a rod inside them for
holding blocks to encourage a rat to eat them in situ, but this is not
18
break it into smaller pieces and carry each piece away separately or
unusually clean, with no crumbs of bait remaining and few smear marks
on them, then it should be suspected that the bait has been transferred
food and may carry away rodenticide baits: they may be present in and
distributed inside and outside buildings and over adjacent ground. If field
19
the frequency of fresh signs may vary considerably. In these
Moreover, rats can move long distances in one night (500 m is common,
but 3 km or more is possible), and it may not be apparent that rats from
distant colonies are visiting the main population. Also, such visits may
treatment begins the location of bait points can be plotted on the map as
the cover over the point. It might also be useful to mark on the map the
distribution of rat signs and any obvious food sources such as stored
Rats are more likely to eat the bait sooner if it is laid beside runs,
in holes, along walls and in sheltered places, but it is not safe just to lay
20
rodenticide bait on the ground without some kind of cover to stop other
animals eating it. The HSE leaflet Safe use of rodenticides on farms
and holdings gives good advice on suitable bait containers and the use
Two types of bait box: the one on the left has a lockable lid and high
internal baffles to prevent children, pets and wild animals bigger than rats from
reaching the bait – such containers are often described as ‘tamper-resistant’.
The box on the right has a low baffle and is less resistant to non-target
interference, but is probably more ‘rat-friendly’.
Rats typically visit bait containers for a few seconds only, time
They may stay longer if other (friendly) rats are with them, so that bait
boxes that allow ‘communal’ feeding may be better than ones with high
internal baffles that rarely allow more than one rat to feed at a time,
21
Regardless of container type, if the rats are not already familiar
with it, they may avoid it initially, which will delay bait uptake and
bait containers several days in advance of laying the bait so that the
laid at least 3-5 days and boxes at least 5-7 days before the treatment
begins. If despite such actions, the majority of rats take little or no bait
over the first 2 weeks, then such treatments will probably make slow
(see Section 7.1). Otherwise, the long bait exposure periods will put
has been eradicated and before a new one appears. Immigrant rats are
unlikely to distinguish containers from any other object and thus should
not specifically avoid them. Baiting can then begin as soon as rat signs
invading rats are likely to colonise and to check them regularly to make
sure that access has not been blocked by growing vegetation. The
22
tracking patches made of fine sand or smoothed mud to record
Bait points should be sited close to fresh rat signs, such as by the
side of, or on, well-used runs, near active burrows or gaps in vegetation.
If rats are running along the inside or outside walls of a building, the
their way to their normal feeding location. Suitable places for baits
should be where rats might nest such as junk piles, dung heaps, under
pallet stacks, beneath discarded machinery and rubbish tips. Bait points
can also be laid around the bottom of straw stacks, along field
23
Bait containers will normally be spaced several metres
apart, but they can be closer together if rat activity locally is
intense.
distributed, it is likely that the density of rats, and hence bait points, will
number of bait points above this average density does not appear to
outcome. The location of the individual points and the willingness of rats
that find them to take the bait are probably of more importance.
24
If too few points are laid, this will only become apparent after the
treatment has started. If more than one quarter of the points with a bait
take are ‘complete’ (i.e. all the bait has been eaten by rats) at one
would be a mistake to remove too early baits that have not been
touched, as rats need time to find them and overcome any hesitancy
errors of bait placement can be rectified with some loss of efficiency, but
not overall effectiveness. This is not necessarily true for treatments with
bait point in order that they eat sufficient of the poison bait.
each point initially, particularly if the site is one where rats are likely to
25
When inspecting the baits, the quantity of bait should be doubled
where complete takes occur. Other bait points with partial takes
can be smaller at first, (e.g. 100 g), particularly if all the rats are known
or are likely to be susceptible. Initial bait size may be even smaller (50
used against indoor infestations. The important point is that rats should
be able to find sufficient bait whenever they want to, so that they ingest
a lethal dose as soon as possible. This may mean that baits are
line with the rats’ needs. Moreover, by matching bait quantity to the
numbers of rats willing to eat it, there will be less surplus bait available
and simultaneously the amount of bait per point can be reduced. If rats
appear reluctant to eat any bait, as they may do around a grain store,
alternatively the bait replaced with plain food until such time as the rats
26
Treatments with calciferol produce better results if effective
can be used without a prebait, but the outcome is less certain. At all bait
points, it is best to lay plain food of the same type that will be mixed
with the poison (or of similar type to the ready-to-use bait) until as many
rats as possible are eating it. The delay in finding these new food
sources and then the suspicious nature of the animals towards them
means that takes build up slowly and only as the rats’ confidence grows
does the total bait take begin to stabilise. The initial size of prebaits can
should be maintained by ‘doubling up’ all ‘complete’ takes. For large rat
although smaller ones may need only 3-5 days. The plain bait should be
checked daily, especially if the rats are readily attracted to it. If the rats
appear reluctant, then less frequent checks can be made until the takes
start building up. The rats may be judged to be feeding freely when
about the same total amount of bait has been eaten for 2-3 consecutive
days. Then all the prebait can be replaced with poison bait at all points
27
prebaiting period short as this may increase the proportion of rats that
are sublethally poisoned and hence likely to become bait shy. If non-
target animals visit the same bait points as rats, those points should be
may ignore the bait completely or eat it without hesitation. During the
first week of baiting, at least two visits may be required to see how the
the baits are still fresh, search for dead rats and ensure that other
poisoning may appear after about 4 days and then others in the
population may succumb over the following days and continue doing so
for several days after the last take of bait. While rats often die
28
underground, many do not and thus a search for bodies must be carried
predators and scavengers eating them. Hence, during the first 2 weeks,
supply of bait, but also to recover dead or dying rats. If all is well and
bait take and rat signs are clearly decreasing, 1-2 visits per week may
suffice thereafter, unless there are concerns about the safety of the
clearly declining, bait points that have not had a take for 2-3 days may
be removed.
Calciferol baits can be left down for about 5-7 days, but should
be checked daily. Rats may die 4-10 days after consuming a lethal
dose, thus searching for bodies may need to continue beyond the end of
29
2. to leave some containers in place and bait them with plain food
protected and further visits made to the site to monitor for signs
of reinfestation);
order to stop a new infestation arising, but carries some risks if care is
not taken. When no rats are present, permanent baits should not be
30
animals, particularly small mammals and small birds, may enter bait
stations if there are no rats to deter them. Both they and the predators
hazard for predators and scavengers if dead bodies are not found and
other wild animals are likely to be short of alternative food. For example,
birds such as finches and hedge sparrows are prone to enter bait boxes
checking more often than those placed indoors, provided the risk of non-
should be:
31
1. a map of the site showing all the bait points;
2. the name of the product being used or the active ingredient and
non-target interference).
4. indicate when dead rats are likely to appear and therefore the
32
resistance. Before that loss becomes serious, alternative methods of
evidence of success during the first week. By the end of the second
week, however, there should be clear signs that the treatment is being
effective. With rat populations that are relatively easy to attract to bait
points, the peak consumption of bait and the maximum number of bait
points visited often occur within the first week or soon after. Thereafter,
there should be a steady decline in takes and points visited. A small rise
in takes during, say, the third, fourth or fifth weeks should not be
3. during the first two weeks each ‘active’ point has a take >50 g
33
4. most fresh rat droppings are coloured by the warning dye that
5. dead rats appear, but if not, fresh rat signs are diminishing.
When rats eat poison baits, their droppings may be coloured by the warning
dye that manufacturers add to their products. A BLUE dye is common, but
other dyes (e.g. RED) may also be used. The difference between coloured
1. most of the bait has been eaten at all points with a good
34
7. IS THE TREATMENT WORKING? INDICATORS OF POOR
CONTROL
Symptoms
a) Few, if any, ‘active’ points (i.e. baits with fresh rat signs near them),
b) Less than 50% of the ‘active’ bait points have a take, especially
c) The amount eaten at each point is insignificant (<50 g over 2-3 days)
Possible causes
• Too many points have been located in places that rats visit
• Bait points have been placed just off well-used runs so that
35
avoided. If an infestation is light, fresh signs may be difficult to
find and the best place for a point is not immediately obvious.
• Bait containers have been put in place on the first day of the
• Rats have found the bait points, but then actively avoided them
3. Unattractive formulation
rats from their usual food. Tracks indicate that rats have visited
• Baits have been placed too close to the rats’ normal food
source, which the animals visit first to feed on and then are not
36
• Rats used to feeding on loose grain have not recognised
Remedies
controlled:
37
Placing bait inside active burrows is the most effective means of
encouraging rats to consume lethal amounts, but avoid overfilling or
blocking the entrance with heavy objects as the burrow may be
abandoned before any bait is eaten.
where rat burrows are located away from buildings and should
it with plain bait. This reduces the risk of other animals being
38
• Destabilising the habitat by moving objects around can
from day to day. For example, stacks of bales that rats live in
bait. Doing so will kill only a few rats and may induce bait
7.2 Good takes initially, partial control, then poor bait takes
Symptoms
that may rise during the first 1-2 weeks before falling away quickly to
39
particular points, or several adjacent points, have had very little bait
period), bait takes drop rapidly after 1-2 days reaching near zero by
c) Dead rats are found and some areas become clear of infestation.
Possible causes
• The reasons given under 1 above also apply, but the effects
2. Bait aversion
• Some rats have become bait shy, because they felt ill within a
40
• If an anticoagulant formulation is being used, a more probable
rats.
Remedies
and bait base, but also the container and the location of the bait
left, but may be the only option if the rats are anticoagulant
resistant.
41
use bromadiolone on whole wheat, or vice versa. If that does
burrow baits are rejected, the rats may have unusual taste
Symptoms
c) Bait takes cease at specific points, but then recur after 7 days.
d) After 2 weeks rat signs not diminishing, very few dead rats seen.
e) Most fresh rat droppings are coloured by the dye in the rodenticide
bait.
Possible causes
1. Under-baiting (a, b)
those that come later. Should they die, other rats may find
sufficient bait and start to feed. The treatment may still succeed
but only slowly. Alternatively, most rats may make many short
42
visits to the bait points consuming a small quantity each time, so
that by the time the bait runs out, the majority have not acquired
block. Generally, the larger the bait particle, the more likely it will
and then carried away piece by piece. Transferred bait may not
they visit and they also eat certain foods in ways distinct from
eaten, the size of the incisor tooth marks distinguishes rats from
mice.
43
• Small birds (e.g. chaffinches, sparrows) tend to scatter loose-
grain bait over the bottom of the container and may also leave
that do not have baffles to stop them. If the birds cannot reach
all the bait, the bait remaining may show a characteristic ‘V’
4. Reinvasion (c)
few days) if rats are close (i.e. <200 m) to the site. Baiting
44
detect. The only indication will be that fresh signs suddenly
5. Resistance (a, b, d, e)
is most unlikely.
kill very few rats. Similarly, very few will die if most are highly
45
successful treatments leaves an increasing proportion of
Remedies
if the rats that do it are not eating the bait they carry away, but
46
blocks/place packs with whole wheat (i.e. split open place
bait. Mice and voles may eat any type of bait at any time,
visiting them.
extending the treated area in any direction where rats are found.
and ditches should be included. Bait to the limit of the visible rat
47
signs along the field margins. If some rats are coming from more
distant places and are visiting a site for the first time or visit
As farm rats may travel 500 m or more in one night, the source
when fields are ploughed up and any grain left by the combines
is buried.
48
• Resistant rats should be eliminated without applying further
but only inside buildings with limited access for rats to the
outside.
49
The end of a treatment is the appropriate time to carry out or
food and shelter. Removing these vital resources is one of the most
them.
• Remove food at night put out for pets and birds during the day.
50
Non-poisonous bait blocks can be used
to indicate the presence of rats
for the control of rat infestations and for that purpose they are generally
51
that happening, it is essential to minimise the selection pressure for
The ease with which rodenticides can be used should not deter
occupiers from determining why infestations arose in the first place and
52
GLOSSARY
Aluminium phosphide: see Fumigation
action.
drain pipes) can be just as effective as bait dispensers, but may need
accidental poisoning.
53
Bait shyness: Typically results in little or no bait being eaten.
the individuals that carry bait away then abandon particles in the open.
but a lethal dose can be ingested after one feed. Nevertheless, rats
54
to the compound have been discovered in Berkshire and Yorkshire to
possible.
to kill resistant rats and mice. The mode of action is to stimulate the
arteries and kidneys. Calciferol is also toxic to other mammals and birds
animals.
has been eaten by rats between two inspection visits. Can be denoted
by ‘C’ on record sheets. Too many complete takes during the first week
55
Concentrate: A rodenticide dissolved in an edible oil or mixed
dry with a finely ground cereal that requires further dilution in order to
formulations, allowing a bait base that they are already eating or one
more attractive to be used instead. Damp baits are often very attractive
to rats and whole wheat soaked in cold water for a few hours has proved
56
second-generation compounds is not uniform within or between
ingesting doses that are only slightly larger than that normally lethal to
susceptible animals, while others can feed for several days on nothing
following an inspection that finds no bait remaining and signs that it was
all eaten by rats. For example, if 200g has been eaten, replenish the
57
group (indane-diones) to all other anticoagulants (hydroxycoumarins),
with the same restrictions on its use as brodifacoum (indoors only). The
attractive to rats. The base may be a cereal such as whole or cut wheat,
mixed with the same food that the rats are eating.
58
burrows phosphine gas fills tunnel systems when a pellet or tablet of
evolve.
materials/empty storage cases etc. can achieve the same effect. As rats
often nest outdoors, but may find their food indoors, the aim is to make
foraging trips more risky. The consequence should be that predators will
take more rats or that rats will move away to find better cover. Either
Typically around farm buildings this can be dung heaps, junk piles, pallet
where the soil is easy to excavate. Rats may also exploit hollows walls
and false ceilings and often dig under buildings if the foundations are
59
Rats can find shelter in ‘natural’ features such as along hedgerows,
have central fixing holes so that if desired they can be secured in place
to prevent rats carrying them away. In such cases, rat activity is likely to
texture and taste) that are ‘natural’ to rats should always be acceptable,
harmless. It’s been argued that intensive control of rats with baits and
60
traps over many decades has led to the selection of individuals in some
distinguish it from ‘NT’ (no take) or ‘C’ (complete take) or ‘D’ (disturbed).
It may be useful to indicate how much bait has been eaten for each
detected by rats.
61
Permanent baiting: Maintaining quantities of rodenticide bait
site at all times whether or not rats are present. Permanent baiting may
keep the door shut! Barriers such a rodent-proof doors and fences have
effective.
bag (sachet). Like wax blocks, easy to use, but unless secured in place
may be carried away by rats. Also, wild rats do not seem to readily open
sealed sachets, which means the bait inside them may not be eaten for
62
Prebaiting: A technique introduced to encourage rats to eat
individual rats to eat larger and larger quantities at each visit, as well as
attracting increasing numbers of rats away from their usual food source.
amount of bait (in total) being eaten by rats each day for 3 days.
63
test. In the UK, resistance to the non-anticoagulant, calciferol, has never
been found.
scavengers, such as red kites, eat rats or other small animals that have
eaten poison bait and then they become casualties as well. While
only in the UK, but also in other European countries and the USA. They
retain the same mode of action as the previous compounds and are
considerably more toxic not only to rats, but also to other species.
64
animals to breed amongst themselves. To take advantage of the
anticoagulants should not be used again until the resistant strain has
died out. Recent research suggests that for some resistant rats, this
toxic to the target species that a lethal dose can be ingested after one
feed. The term has tended to replace ‘acute’ that was used to describe
commonly replaced with ‘multi-feed’. Even warfarin will kill some rats
after one feed, but the majority of a population will only succumb after 2
or more feeds. Equally, resistant rats may need more than one feed to
that only eat very small amounts when visiting bait points.
symptoms of poisoning appear a long time after a rat has fed on bait, so
that it is unable to associate its illness with a particular food type or its
aversions developing.
65
Vitamin K: The antidote to anticoagulant poisoning. Natural
anticoagulants.
of warfarin and other similar compounds that a single lethal dose is often
several times greater than the sum of 4-5 daily doses that also cause
death.
66
Wax block: A type of formulation in which whole, finely- or
coarsely- ground cereals are mixed with a quantity of paraffin wax and
multiple gnawing edges and have a central hole for securing them in
place, such as inside a box. An early use of block baits was to anchor
them in manholes to control sewer rats, since coarse cereal baits could
easily be washed away. However, the results were variable, not least
because it was suspected that rats did not always recognise block baits
67