Individual Development: Holistic, Integrated Model: David Magnusson
Individual Development: Holistic, Integrated Model: David Magnusson
A s the title of the chapter suggests, the aim of this chapter is to pre-
sent and discuss the main elements of an integrated, holistic model
for individual functioning and development, which can serve as a general
theoretical framework for planning, implementation, and interpretation
of empirical research on specific aspects of individual development. The
motive for such a model is discussed on the background of an analysis of
the present state of affairs, which is characterized more by fragmentation
of developmental subfields than by integration, which is a prerequisite for
further success.
The old holistic view has got new clothes and an enriched content
from three sources during recent decades. The first source is the modern
models for dynamic, complex processes, which have meant a theoretical
and empirical revolution in disciplines that are concerned with such
processes in natural sciences, biology, and medicine. These models em-
phasize the holistic character of the processes and the need for integra-
tion of all operating factors in the theoretical models, which serve as the
theoretical framework for planning, implementation, and interpretation
of empirical research. The second source is the rapid development in sci-
19
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10176-001
Examining Lives in Context: Perspectives on the Ecology of Human Development,
edited by P. Moen, G. H. Elder, Jr., and K. Lüscher
Copyright © 1995 American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
DAVID MAGNUSSON
I N D I V I D UAL DEVELOPMENT: A D E F I N I T I 0 N
Psychological research on individual development is concerned with in-
dividual functioning in terms of thoughts, feelings, actions, and reactions
studied across the lifetime of the individual.
Development of living organisms refers to progressive or regressive
changes in size, shape, and function during the lifetime. Psychological re-
search on individual development covers this process from conception to
death. In this definition, two concepts are essential: change and time. Time
is not the same as development, but development always has a temporal
dimension. Therefore, if a person’s distinctive pattern of characteristics re-
mains unchanged across time, no development has occurred. Consequently,
processes that go on in an unchanged manner, within existing structures,
do not constitute development. Thus, developmental models should be dis-
tinguished from models that analyze and explain why individuals function
as they do in terms of their current psychological and biological disposi-
tions. Because the current functioning of an individual is a result of ear-
lier developmental processes in his or her life course and because this cur-
rent functioning, at the same time, forms the basis for later stages, models
for current functioning and developmental models are complementary.
20
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
S C I E N T I F I C PROGRESS: T H E N E E D F O R
A GENERAL M O D E L O F H O M O
ward in natural sciences have been taken by integration within the inter-
face of what earlier had been conspicuously different disciplines. This
happened first in the interface of physics and chemistry and recently in
the interface of biology, chemistry, and physics. The earlier unambiguous
and clear boundaries between subdisciplines have disappeared.
Also in empirical research on individual functioning, specialization
takes place. In some areas, specialization has been very productive and has
offered important contributions. However, despite the recent indications
of more integration among disciplines, for example, between brain research
and cognitive psychology, research in behavioral sciences in general is still
characterized by what Toulmin (1981) once described as “sectarian rivalry”
(p. 267). During the eighties, researchers, discussing the future prospects
of psychology, described this lack of integration as one of the main obsta-
cles for further, real scientific progress in behavioral sciences (de Groot,
1990; Thomae, 1988). As a matter of fact, this was also an issue of great
concern for Stern (1911) in the beginning of this century.
Thus, fragmentation is still a characteristic of psychological research
on individual functioning at all levels, that is, diversification of research
in specialties with little or no contact across domains. Fragmentation in-
volves content, concepts, research strategies, and methodology. At a
metatheoretical level, it has its roots in the existence of three main ex-
planatory models: mentalistic, biological, and environmental. (The mod-
els are, of course, not mutually exclusive; each one is a matter of empha-
sis.) The distinction between these explanatory models is not only of
theoretical interest. Each of them has had and still has a far-reaching im-
pact on fundamental aspects of societies: social welfare, politics, culture,
21
DAVID MAGNUSSON
nents of a socialization approach (cf. Miller & Dollard, 1941) to social de-
velopment and behavior. An example drawn from theories of moral de-
velopment may illustrate the issue. For Kohlberg (1969), moral develop-
ment is closely related to cognitive development. In contrast, both Marx
and Freud, from very different perspectives, regarded conscience, as the
base for moral choice, as being instilled from the outside through a process
of socialization beyond the individual’s control.
Of course, nothing is wrong with each of the three general explana-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
tory models per se. What is wrong occurs when each of them claims to-
tal supremacy, and that has been the case to an extent that has hampered
real progress both in research and in application.
Now, a basic question to be answered is the following: When research
in natural sciences is characterized by the iterative process of specializa-
tion and integration, why is behavioral research, including developmental
research, on the whole characterized by specialization with only little in-
tegration? The reason is probably complex, but let me point to a possible
explanation.
One condition that facilitates the iterative process of specialization and
integration in natural sciences is the existence of a general theoretical
framework, a general model of nature, for theorizing and conducting em-
pirical inquiry. The fact that we lack a corresponding general theoretical
framework for the formulation of problems, for the development of a com-
mon conceptual space, and for the development and application of ade-
quate methodologies is, in my opinion, an essential obstacle for further
real progress in the behavioral sciences.We need the formulation of a gen-
eral model of homo, which synthesizes and integrates the three metathe-
oretical models briefly described earlier. A modern, integrated holistic
model meets this requirement.
24
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
25
DAVID MAGNUSSON
stage of the process, these emotions affect the individual's behavior and
handling of the environment. They also influence his or her interpreta-
tion of the sequence of changes in the situational conditions and thereby
his or her physiological reactions in the next stage of the process.
Thus, the perceptual-cognitive system and the biological system of an
individual are involved in a continuous loop of reciprocal interaction. The
way this process functions is contingent, among other things, on the en-
28
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
29
DAVID MAGNUSSON
30
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
the individual and the environment. Thus, the mediating system is a func-
tion of the individual's interaction with the environment in the course of
individual development, and it plays a crucial, guiding role in that inter-
action process at each stage of the developmental process.
In some psychodynamic models of individual development and the
functioning of the mental system, the concept of unconscious processes
has played a central role. This debate has been given new fuel during the
past few decades through the growing interest in and understanding of
the parallel processes of controlled (conscious, attended to, and thus sub-
ject to critical analysis) versus automatic (out of attentional focus and
awareness) processing of information (see, e.g., Bowers, 1981; Brewin,
1986; Greenwald, 1992; Norman & Shallice, 1980). This continuously on-
going processing of signals impinging on the senses subliminally renders
new importance to the perceptual-cognitive system; at the same time, it
plays down the central role earlier ascribed to its conscious functioning.
BIOLOGICAL FACTORS IN A
HOLISTIC PERSPECTIVE
Two lines of research on biological factors are of interest in this connec-
tion:
1. As discussed in the introduction, an issue of debate since ancient
times has concerned nature versus nurture: the relative role of hereditary
and environmental factors in individual functioning, currently and in a
developmental perspective.
Since the beginning of the history of differential psychology, the role
31
DAVID MAGNUSSON
of genetic factors has been a main issue (Galton, 1869). After a period,
starting during the 1960s, in which genetic factors were almost abandoned
from the agenda, the development in human genetics has led to a renewed
interest. That various aspects of individual functioning are, to some ex-
tent, determined by inherited properties of the body is supported by much
empirical research (Cairns & Nakelski, 1971; Lagerspetz & Lagerspetz,
1971; Pedersen, 1989; Plomin, Chipuer, & Loehlin, 1990).
The traditional view on the role of genetic factors has been a unidi-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
rectional, cause-effect relation. At a most basic level, the onset and course
of certain developmental sequences may be determined genetically to the
extent that they are common to all individuals. However, even such devel-
opmental sequences as the onset of the menstrual cycle in girls and the reg-
ulation of growth in height are somewhat modifiable by environmental fac-
tors. The individual phenotype develops in the framework offered by the
genotype in a reciprocal interaction process with the environment, a process
that starts at conception and goes on through the life span. On the scene
set by inherited factors, many different plays are possible (Waddington,
1962). Within the limits set by inherited factors, there are large potential-
ities for change, because of the interplay with environmental factors.
Thus, that there is a hereditary predisposition for a certain type of be-
havior does not mean that it cannot be changed by environmental influ-
ences (cf. Angoff, 1988). Cairns (1979), in his evaluation of the role of
heredity and environment in individual differences in aggression, drew the
conclusion that the differences obtained by selective breeding show strong
environmental specificity and can be modified by environmental social
conditions to such an extent that the inherited differences no longer mat-
ter. In well-planned longitudinal studies of newborns, Meyer-Probst,
Rossler, and Teichmann ( 1983) demonstrated that favorable social condi-
tions acted as protective factors for later social development among chil-
dren identified at birth as biologically rich.
In this perspective, current individual functioning is the result of a life
history of a person-environment interaction, in which environmental and
inherited factors participate in a process for which it is not possible to dis-
entangle their relative role at the individual level. The outcome of the process,
32
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
33
DAVID MAGNUSSON
role in the total interaction process and, thus, also in individual develop-
ment. In the interaction process, behavior is influenced by cognitive in-
terpretations of what happens in the outer world (embedded in world-
views, self-perceptions, emotions, values, and needs), by subconscious
automatic processing and by physiological processes. At the same time, it
has a functional role in the total interaction process in two interrelated re-
spects: first, by activities to reach short-term and long-term goals (Pervin,
1983), such as changing the situational conditions to satisfy personal short-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
term and long-term needs and to avoid negative cognitive, mental, or bi-
ological experiences (Magnusson, 198 1) and, second, by adaptation to
other individuals’ behavior to develop and maintain working social rela-
tions (Cairns, 1994). The way the behavioral system of a person functions
in a particular situation at a particular stage of the life cycle, and how suc-
cessfully, is a result of the process of maturation and learning across de-
velopment.
THE ENVIRONMENT IN A
HOLISTIC PERSPECTIVE
In the developmental process of a person, environmental factors play a de-
cisive role (see, e.g., Maccoby & Jacklin, 1983; Radke-Yarrow, 1991). Con-
tact with others is necessary for the development of speech and language
as a tool for thought and for communication (Camaioni, 1989; Tomaselli,
1992); for the development of adequate worldviews and self-perceptions
(Epstein & Erskine, 1983); and for the development of well functioning,
integrated norm and moral systems (Wilson, Williams, & Sugarman,
1967). The importance of contact with others for physical health has been
emphasized in the increasing number of reports from research on social
networks (e.g., Wills, 1984; Wortman & Dunkel-Schetter, 1987).
The individual and his or her environment do not form separate en-
tities. The individual is an active, intentional part of the environment with
which he or she interacts. Individuals meet their environment most di-
rectly in specific situations, which, in turn, are embedded in the larger en-
vironment with physical, social, and cultural properties operating both
34
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
35
DAVID MAGNUSSON
36
I N D I V I D U A L DEVELOPMENT
37
DAVID MAGNUSSON
specific process and what maintains it vary. A psychological factor can start
a biological process that is then maintained by physiological factors, and
psychological factors can start and maintain a process that has been trig-
gered by biological factors. Environmental factors influence a person’s phys-
ical and mental well-being, and at the same time, an individual affects his
or her own environment in many different ways.
The example presented in Figure 1 illustrates how the mental, behav-
ioral, and biological systems of a person are involved in a continuous loop
of reciprocal interaction in a current situation. The example illustrates
how this process is dependent on the character of the specific, proximal
situation that the individual encounters, particularly the situation as it is
perceived and interpreted by the individual. The empirical example about
maturing girls from Stattin and Magnusson (1990) shows how the devel-
opmental processes of the girls and their outcomes in the long range were
dependent on the psychological and biological dispositions of the girls;
the properties of the social, economic, and cultural environments in which
the specific situations that the girls encountered were embedded; and the
interaction among these factors.
38
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
The fact that both persons and environments change across the life span
leads to changes in the character of the interaction between them. The in-
teraction process per se will thus precipitate development. For example, the
character of the interactive process within a family changes across time.
A consequence of the perspective applied here, with methodological
implications, is that changes do not take place in single aspects isolated
from the totality. The extent to which different aspects of individual func-
tioning are influenced by environmental factors in this process varies. For
example, in sexual development, some features, such as gonadal structure
and function, are strongly regulated by biological factors. On the other
hand, other aspects of individual functioning, such as choice of peers and
type of sexual relations, may be strongly open to experiential influences
(Cairns, 1991).
Much debate has been devoted to the issues of stability versus change
and continuity versus discontinuity in individual development. Charac-
teristics of most of these studies are (a) that they deal with data for sin-
gle variables one at a time, for example, aggressiveness, intelligence, and
hyperactivity and (b) that they express temporal consistency of single vari-
ables in terms of relative stability, that is, in terms of stable rank orders of
individuals across time for the variable under consideration (Weinert &
Schneider, 1993).
As emphasized above, a fundamental characteristic of individual func-
tioning as a holistic, dynamic process implies, among other things, that
individuals do not develop in terms of single variables but as total inte-
grated systems. In this perspective, all changes during the life span of a
person are characterized by lawful continuity (Magnusson & Torestad,
39
DAVID MAGNUSSON
as a result of chance events or significant events. This view makes the dis-
pute about whether individual development is characterized by continu-
ity or discontinuity a pseudoissue in developmental research. The inter-
esting aspect of this issue is what determines abrupt changes in the life
course of a person and the kind of mechanisms that underlie such changes.
Individual development is not a process of accumulation of outcomes;
it is rather, at the individual level, a process of restructuring of subsystems
and the whole system within the boundaries set by biological and social
constraints. If one aspect changes, it affects also related parts of the sub-
system and sometimes the whole organism. For example, if one of the nec-
essary operating factors in the coronary system totters, the whole coro-
nary system and the whole organism may be affected. At a more general
level, the restructuring of processes and structures at the individual level
is embedded in and part of the restructuring of the total individual-
environment system.
Much developmental research on stability and change has concen-
trated on stability and change in quantitative terms, and the issue has of-
ten been whether individual development is characterized by more of the
same in a way that is reflected in statistical stability of rank orders of in-
dividuals for the specific variable under consideration. It should be rec-
ognized that the process of developmental change in an individual is char-
acterized by both quantitative and qualitative change. The psychological
significance of a certain state of a certain variable depends on the context
of other, simultaneously operating variables in the system under investi-
gation (i.e., on the pattern of operating factors to which the variable un-
der consideration belongs).
40
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
41
DAVID MAGNUSSON
Speech Preparation
p SBP
DBP
HR
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
Magnitudeof systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), and heart rate
(HR) reactivity in cardiovascular response clusters during preparation of a speech (from
Graber & Huber, 1994).
then twofold: (a) to identify the possible operating factors in the subsys-
tem under consideration and (b) to identify the ways in which these fac-
tors are organized (i.e., the actual working patterns of operating factors).
An empirical illustration to this view is presented by Gramer and Hu-
ber (1994). In a study of cardiovascular responses in what was assumed
to be a stressful situation, they found that the subjects could be classified
in three groups on the basis of their distinct pattern of values for systolic
blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, and heart rate (see Figure 2).
These data demonstrate a basic principle in individual development
42
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES
One of the major goals for scientific work is to arrive at generalizations
about the lawfulness of structures and processes in the space of phenom-
ena that are the objects of interest. In psychology, one of the roads to this
goal has been the systematic study of individual differences (cf. Cronbach,
1957; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985).
43
DAVID MAGNUSSON
terpreted in such terms. This is the case in such studies as those on the re-
lationships among variables as a basis for factor analysis, on the stability
of single variables across time, on the links between environmental fac-
tors and various aspects of individual functioning, and on the develop-
mental background of adult functioning. An example is the research fo-
cusing on the relation between various aspects of individual functioning
and environmental upbringing conditions, on the one hand, and the de-
velopment of adult alcohol abuse and criminal behavior, on the other.
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
44
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
45
DAVID MAGNUSSON
less than 1% with the total variance for registered alcohol abuse at adult
age, when the variance common with all of the other variables was removed.
Thus, the specific contribution of each single variable per se to the predic-
tion of alcohol problems at adulthood was limited.
As illustrated by the entries in Table 1, the specific role of single hypo-
thetical variables in the developmental process is conspicuously overesti-
mated in studies of single variables in isolation from their context of other,
simultaneously operating variables. This is overlooked too often because, fre-
quently, the roles of only one or a few independent variables are studied at
each time. A good prediction is that it would have been possible to publish
at least five studies, independent of each other, demonstrating a significant
correlation between early individual functioning and adult alcohol problems.
For a statistician, the figures presented in Table 1 are not surprising,
considering the intercorrelations among the independent variables. Nor
are the results surprising, if they are interpreted in the perspective of a
holistic, integrated model for individual development.
The analysis just presented shows the limitations of a simplistic vari-
able approach as a basis for understanding and explaining the function-
ing and development of the individual. This does not mean that the vari-
46
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
47
DAVID MAGNUSSON
son, and Torestad (1993), referred to above. The result of the pattern analy-
sis is presented in Table 2.
The patterns in Table 2 are based on data for six variables, which cover
different aspects of problem behaviors: aggressiveness, motor restlessness,
concentration difficulty,low school motivation, underachievement, and peer
rejection. Empirical studies indicate that each of them is a possible operat-
ing factor in the developmental processes underlying adult maladjustment.
Data for each of the variables have been transformed to a scale with values
ranging from 0 to 4,reflecting levels of seriousness of problem behaviors for
boys at the age of 13. The methodology and results of the pattern analysis
were discussed in detail in Magnusson and Bergman (1988). Here, only a few
comments, pertinent to the discussion in this chapter, will be made.
Table 2 demonstrates that the boys could be grouped into eight dis-
tinctly different groups with reference to their pattern of values for the
variables under study. An inspection shows that four out of six problem
behaviors did not appear as single problem clusters for the boys at age 13.
For example, aggressivenessand motor restlessness, which have been stud-
ied extensively in the variable-oriented tradition as separate indicators of
maladjustment in both a cross-sectional and in a longitudinal perspective,
appear only in combination with other indicators. This result is an illus-
tration of the first basic proposition of a holistic model for individual
functioning and individual development: A certain aspect of the total
process cannot be finally studied and understood in isolation from its con-
text of other, simultaneously operating factors. A certain factor, say ag-
gressiveness, does not have a significance of its own per se, independent
of the context of other factors simultaneously working in the individual.
48
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
Cluster meana
1 296 .12
2 23 .30 -
A 3 40 .28 - -2.6
\o
4 61 .39 1.3 1.4 -
5 41 .39 1.5 2.3 1.9 -
6 12 .56 1.7 1.8 2.3 1.9 2.6
7 37 .37 2.3 2.3 1.9 1.3 -
8 22 .48 2.2 2.7 2.6 2.4
It gets its significance from its context. This became even more apparent
in the follow-up study to the age of 24, which was reported by Magnus-
son and Bergman (1988). The long-term significance of a certain factor
was not in the factor itself, it was in the total pattern in which the factor
appeared at the individual level. For example, only when in combination
with other severe problem behaviors at the age of 13 was aggressiveness a
precursor of adult problems.
A number of methods for pattern analysis have been presented and
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
FINAL C O M M E N T S
A holistic, integrated model for individual functioning and development
does not imply that the whole system of an individual must be studied at
the same time. The essential function of the model is that it enables us to
formulate problems at different levels of the functioning of the total or-
ganism, to implement empirical studies, and to interpret the results in a
common, theoretical framework. For a long time, the Newtonian view of
the physical world has served this purpose in the natural sciences. The im-
plication of the acceptance of that model of nature has never been that
the whole universe should be investigated in one and the same study. But
it has enabled researchers concerned with very different levels of the total
50
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
REFERENCES
Angoff, W. H. (1988). The nature-nurture debate, aptitudes, and group differences.
American Psychologist, 43, 7 13-720.
Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (1991). Correlates of the temporal consis-
tency of personality patterns in childhood. Journal of Personality, 59,689-703.
Baldwin, J. M. (1895). Mental development in the child and the race: Method and
processes. New York: Macmillan.
Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1980). Life-span developmental psychol-
ogy. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology
(Vol. 31, pp. 65-110). Palo Alto, C A Annual Reviews.
51
DAVID MAGNUSSON
Bateson, P. (in press). Design for a life. In D. Magnusson (Ed.), The life-span devel-
opment of individuals: Behavioral, neurobiological and psychosocial perspectives.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Bergman, L. R. (1988). Modeling reality. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Studies ofpsychosocial
risks: The power of longitudinal data (pp. 354-366). Cambridge, England Cam-
bridge University Press.
Bergman, L. R. (1993). Some methodological issues in longitudinal research Look-
ing forward. In D. Magnusson & P. Casaer (Eds.), Longitudinal research on in-
dividual development: Present status and future perspectives (pp. 217-241). Cam-
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Bergman, L. R., & Magnusson, D. (1983). The development of patterns of malad-
justment. In Report from the project Individual Development and Environment.
Stockholm: Stockholm University.
Bertalanffy, L., von. (1968). General system theory. New York: Braziller.
Binet, A. (1909). Les idea modernes sur les enfants [Modern ideas about children].
Paris: Ernest Flammarion.
Bishop, Y. M. M., Feinberg, S. E., & Holland, P. W. (1975). Discrete multivariate
analysis: Theory and practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Block, J. (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley, C A Bancroft Books.
Bolles, R. C. (1972). Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning. Psychological Review,
79, 394-409.
Bothe, H. G., Ebeling, W., Kurzhanski, A. B., & Peschel, M. (Eds.). (1987). Dynami-
cal systems and environmental models. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.
Bowers, K. S. (1981). Knowing more than we can say leads to saying more than we
can know: On being implicitly informed. In D. Magnusson (Ed.), Toward a psy-
chology of situations (pp. 179-194). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
52
I N D1 V I D UAL D EV E L 0 PM EN T
53
DAVID MAGNUSSON
54
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
Edelman, G. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory ofneuronal group selection. New
York: Basic Books.
Eigen, M., & Schuster, P. (1979). The hyper cycle-A principle of natural self-organi-
zation. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.
Endler, N. S., & Magnusson, D. (1976). Toward an interactional psychology of per-
sonality. Psychological Bulletin, 83,956-979.
Epstein, S., & Erskine, N. (1983). The development of personal theories of reality
from an interactional perspective. In D. Magnusson & V. L. Allen (Eds.), Hu-
man development: A n interactional perspective (pp. 133-147). New York Aca-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
demic Press.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual differences:A nat-
ural science approach. London: Plenum.
Farmer, J. D., Kaufmann, A., Packard, N. H., & Perelson, A. S. (1987). Adaptive dy-
namic networks as models for the immune system and autocatalytic sets. In
S. H. Koslow, A. J. Mandell, & M. F. Schlesinger (Eds.), Annals ofthe New York
Academy of Sciences: Vol. 504. Perspectives in biological dynamics and theoretical
medicine (pp. 118-131). New York New York Academy of Sciences.
Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. Lon-
don: Macmillan.
Gibson, W. A. (1959). Three multivariate models: Factor analysis, latent structure
analysis and latent profile analysis. Psychometrica, 24,229-252.
Gleick, J. (1987). Chaos: Making a new science. New York Penguin Books.
Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavioral development: Theory.
Developmental Psychology, 27, 4-1 3.
Gramer, M., & Huber, H. P. (1994). Individual variability in task-specific cardiovas-
cular patterns during psychological challenge. German Journal of Psychology, 18,
1-17.
Greenwald, A. G. (1992). Unconscious cognition reclaimed. American Psychologist,
47,766-779.
Gunnar, M. R. (1986). Human developmental psychoendocrinology: A review of re-
search on neuroendocrine responses to challenge and threat in infancy and
childhood. In M. E. Lamb, A. L. Brown, & B. Rogoff (Eds.), Advances in devel-
opmental psychology (Vol. 4). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hess, B., & Mikhailov, A. (1994). Self-organization in living cells. Science, 264,
223-224.
Hunt, J. McV., (1961). Intelligence and experience. New York Ronald Press.
55
DAVID MAGNUSSON
Kagan, J. (1989). Unstable ideas: Temperament, cognition, and self: Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy: Temperament in human nature. New York: Basic
Books.
Kaplan, M. L., & Kaplan, N. R. (1991). The self-organization of human psycholog-
ical functioning. Behavioral Science, 36, 161-179.
Kauffman, S. A. (1993). The origins of order. New York: Oxford University.
Kohlberg, L. ( 1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to
socialization. In D. A. Goolin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and re-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
56
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
Magnusson, D., af Klinteberg, B., & Stattin, H. (1994). Juvenile and persistent of-
fenders: Behavioral and physiological characteristics. In R. D. Ketterlinus & M.
Lamb (Eds.), Adolescent problem behaviors. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Magnusson, D., Anderson, T., & Torestad, B. (1993). Methodological implications
of a peephole perspective on personality. In D. C. Funder, R. D. Parke, C. Tom-
linson-Keasey, & K. Widaman (Eds.), Studying lives through time: Personality
and development (pp. 207-220). Washington, DC: American Psychological As-
sociation.
Magnusson, D., & Bergman, L. R. (1988). Individual and variable-based approaches
to longitudinal research on early risk factors. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Studies ofpsy-
chosocial risk: The power of longitudinal data (pp. 45-61). Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Magnusson, D., & Cairns, R. B. (in press). Developmental science: Principles and il-
lustrations. In R. B. Cairns, G. H. Elder, Jr., E. J. Costello, & A. McGuire (Eds.),
Developmental science. Cambridge, England Cambridge University Press.
Magnusson, D., Dun& A., & Zetterblom, G. (1975). Adjustment: A longitudinal study.
Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell.
Magnusson, D., Stattin, H., & Dun& A. (1983). Aggression and criminality in a lon-
gitudinal perspective. In K. T. van Dusen & S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Prospective
studies of crime and delinquency (pp. 273-301). Boston: Kluwer-Nijhoff.
Magnusson, D., & Torestad, B. (1992). The individual as an interactive agent in the
environment. In W. B. Walsh, K. Craig, & R. Price (Eds.), Person-environment
psychology:Models and perspectives.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Magnusson, D., & Torestad, B. (1993). A holistic view of personality: A model re-
visited. Annual Review of Psychology, 44,427-452.
Meyer-Probst, B., Rossler, H. D., & Teichmann, H. (1983). Biological and psychoso-
cial risk factors and development during childhood. In D. Magnusson & V. L.
57
DAVID MAGNUSSON
58
INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT
59
DAVID MAGNUSSON
Wapner, S., & Kaplan, B. (1983). Toward a holistic developmental psychology. Hills-
dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Watson, J. B. (1930). Behaviorism (2nd ed.). New York Norton.
Weiner, H. (1989). The dynamics of the organism: Implications of recent biological
thought for psychosomatic theory and research. Psychosomatic Medicine, 51,
608-635.
Weinert, F. E., & Schneider, W. (1993). Cognitive, social, and emotional develop-
ment. In D. Magnusson & P. Casaer (Eds.), Longitudinal research on individual
development: Present status and future perspectives (pp. 75-94). Cambridge, En-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
60