Quasi Experimental Design
Quasi Experimental Design
Introduction
There are several types of quasi-experimental designs ranging from the simple to the complex,
each having different strengths, weaknesses and applications. These designs include:
Time-Series Designs
pregnancy prevention program by comparing the pregnancy rates in a high school where the
program is used with pregnancy rates in a high school that does not use the program.
Nonequivalent group design has a built-in threat to internal validity that precludes an
unambiguous cause-and-effect explanation, i.e. assignment bias. The three examples of
nonequivalent group designs are:
It simply compares pre-existing groups and uses a participant characteristic such as gender,
race, or personality to automatically assign participants to groups. There is no random
assignment of participants to groups. The dependent variable is then measured for each
participant to obtain a set of scores within each group. The goal of the study is to determine
whether the scores for one group are consistently different from scores in another group e.g.
mother’s and father’s involvement in the peer relationships of their adolescent children. In
differential research, participant differences in one variable are used to create separate groups,
and measurements of the second variable are made within each group. A correlational study
treats all the participants as a single group and simply measures the two variables for each
individual.
X O (treatment group)
Quasi Experimental Design 3
O = observation, X = treatment
It compares two nonequivalent groups. One group is measured twice – once before a
treatment and once after the treatment. Problem of assignment bias even though it is reduced by
pre and post measurement. Potential problems with differential history effects, differential
instrumentation, differential testing effects, differential maturation and differential regression the
other group is measured at the same two times but does not receive any treatment. This design
can be represented schematically as follows:
O X O (treatment group)
1. History effects: When an event occurs at the same time as treatment and changes
participants’ behavior, this event becomes an alternative explanation for the changes in
participants’ behavior (rather than treatment); thus, participants’ “history” includes events
other than treatment. In pretest-posttest nonequivalent control group design, the threat
from history effects is called differential history effects that differ from one group to
another.
3. Practice effects and fatigue: When the observation of participants involves some form of
testing, it is possible that the testing experience during the first observation will change
the participants and affect their scores during the second observation. Common examples
Quasi Experimental Design 4
4. Maturation: Participants naturally change over time; these maturational changes, not
treatment, may explain any changes in participants during the experiment. In pretest-
posttest nonequivalent control group design, the threat from maturation is called
differential maturation that differs from one group to another.
5. Regression toward the mean: Participants sometimes perform very well or very poorly
on a measure because of chance factors (e.g., luck). These chance factors are not likely to
be present in a second testing, so their scores will not be so extreme — the scores
“regress to the mean.” These regression effects, not the effect of treatment, may account
for changes in participants’ performance over time. In pretest-posttest nonequivalent
control group design, the threat from regression is called differential regression that
differs from one group to another.
Time-Series Designs
Each individual in a group is measured once before and once after a treatment. It does not
control for possible extraneous variables possibly causing change over time. This design can be
represented schematically as follows:
O X O
A series of observations is taken for each participant before a treatment and a series of
observations is taken after the treatment. It eliminates many problems with the pretest-posttest
design, outside events are a confound only if they occur simultaneously with the treatment. This
design can be represented schematically as follows:
O O O X O O O
Time series design – treatment administered by the researcher e.g. anger management
program for students.
Interrupted time-series design – event or treatment is not created by the researcher e.g.
legal change.
OOOXOOONOOOXOOONOOOXOOON
N = no treatment
It reduces likelihood of an external event confound to the treatment. There are differences
between expected temporary and permanent effects.
3. Since quasi-experiments are natural experiments, findings in one may be applied to other
subjects and settings, allowing for some generalizations to be made about population.
Also, this experimentation method is efficient in longitudinal research that involves
longer time periods which can be followed up in different environments.
1. The control allowed through the manipulation of the quasi-independent variable can lead
to unnatural circumstances; although the dangers of artificiality are considerably less
relative to true experiments (quasi-experimental designs are often chosen for field studies
where the random assignment of experimental subjects is impractical, unethical, or
impossible).
2. Also, the lack of random assignment in the quasi-experimental design method may allow
studies to be more feasible, but this also poses many challenges for the investigator in
terms of internal validity.
3. This deficiency in randomization makes it harder to rule out confounding variables and
introduces new threats to internal validity.
4. Because randomization is absent, some knowledge about the data can be approximated,
but conclusions of causal relationships are difficult to determine due to a variety of
extraneous and confounding variables that exist in a social environment.
References
Quasi Experimental Design 7
Psychometrics threats to internal validity. (2009). Retrieved May 22, 2010 from
http://www.psychmet.com/id12.html
http://www.u.arizona.edu/~dusana/psych290Bpresession06/notes/Ch10%20Nonexperime
ntal%20and%20Quasi-Experimental%20Strategies.ppt
http://gsrjr.ncat.edu/psyres/psyresch9.ppt
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Quasi-experimental_design