Capítulo 4 Pavlovian Conditioning
Capítulo 4 Pavlovian Conditioning
stimulus?
which stimulus can serve as a conditioned stimulus in classical
conditioning depends on the unconditioned stimulus that is used?
associative learning is possible in the random control procedure?
Pavlovian conditioning is involved in a wide range of behaviors, including
preferences and aversions, fears and phobias, drug tolerance and
addiction, and maternal and sexual behavior?
conditioning were conducted with visual CSs (the sight of the food that was to be placed in the
dog’s mouth) rather than auditory cues. Second, the story suggests that classical conditioning
primarily involves the modification of visceral and glandular responses. B. F. Skinner elevated
this implication to an axiom, postulating that classical conditioning can only modify glandular
and visceral responses (e.g., Skinner, 1953). However, subsequent research has shown this
assumption to be unwarranted (Domjan, 2016). Pavlovian conditioning can produce many
types of CRs, including approaching a signal for food or approaching the food cup, both of
which are skeletal rather than glandular or visceral responses.
APPETITIVE CONDITIONING
Appetitive conditioning is frequently investigated with pigeons and laboratory rats. Pigeons
that serve in appetitive conditioning experiments are mildly hungry and are tested in a small
experimental chamber called a Skinner box (see Figure 4.1). The CS is a circular spot of light
projected onto a small plastic disk or touch screen above a food cup. Pecks at the light are
automatically detected. The conditioning procedure consists of turning on the key light for a
few seconds and then presenting a small amount of food.
FIG URE 4. 1
Copyright © 2017. American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
Typical trial for conditioning sign tracking or autoshaping in pigeons. The conditioned stimulus (CS) is
illumination of a small circular disk or pecking key for 6 seconds. The unconditioned stimulus (US) is
access to food for 4 seconds. CS–US trials are repeated, with an intertrial interval of about 1 minute.
After a number of pairings of the key light with food, the pigeons come to approach and
Domjan, Michael. The Essentials of Conditioning and Learning, American Psychological Association, 2017. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kuleuvenul/detail.action?docID=5116675.
Created from kuleuvenul on 2018-10-01 03:09:22.
peck the key when it is lit (Hearst & Jenkins, 1974; Tomie, Brooks, & Zito, 1989). The
conditioned approach and pecking behavior develop even if the key light is located some
distance from the food cup (Boakes, 1979). The light becomes a signal for food, and the
pigeons go where the light is located. Hence, this type of conditioned responding is called sign
tracking. Because the procedure results in the pigeons pecking the response key without
elaborate training or shaping by the experimenter, the procedure is also called autoshaping.
Laboratory rats are also used in studies of Pavlovian conditioning with food as the US. If
an auditory cue is used as the CS that is presented before each food delivery, the rats approach
and search the food cup as the CR to the auditory cue (Meyer, Cogan, & Robinson, 2014). This
type of behavior is referred to as goal tracking because the CR tracks the location of the goal
object or food. Sign tracking and goal tracking have been also found in sexual conditioning
with domesticated quail, where the CS comes to signal access to a potential sexual partner
(Burns & Domjan, 2001).
Whether sign tracking or goal tracking develops as the CR depends on the CS that is
employed and other details of the conditioning procedure. In addition, there are significant
individual differences that determine which type of conditioned behavior occurs. These
individual differences have been discovered in studies with rats using a retractable lever
apparatus (Flagel, Akil, & Robinson, 2009). In these experiments, the extension of a response
lever into the experimental chamber was used as the CS. Each conditioning trial started with
extension of the response lever, followed by the delivery of food into a food cup (see Figure
4.2 for an example of a rat in lever-press apparatus). With this procedure, about one third of
the rats developed sign tracking and approached and made contact with the lever. Another third
of the rats showed goal tracking and approached and searched the food cup. The remaining
subjects showed a combination of these responses.
Copyright © 2017. American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
Domjan, Michael. The Essentials of Conditioning and Learning, American Psychological Association, 2017. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kuleuvenul/detail.action?docID=5116675.
Created from kuleuvenul on 2018-10-01 03:09:22.
FIG URE 4. 2
Rat in a conditioned suppression experiment. Pressing the response lever occasionally produces a pellet
of food. Periodically a tone is presented, ending in a brief shock through the grid floor. The rat comes to
suppress lever pressing during the tone.
Individual differences in sign tracking versus goal tracking have attracted a great deal of
interest because they are reflections of individual differences in the propensity to acquire
incentive motivation. Incentive motivation plays a major role in addictions where exposure to
Copyright © 2017. American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
signals for the drug of choice makes the procurement of an appetitive reinforcer irresistible.
Individuals addicted to alcohol, for example, find the urge to drink irresistible when they see
or smell an alcoholic drink. For them, the sight and smell of alcohol has become a strong
incentive stimulus. Responses to the sight and smell of alcohol are sign tracking CRs (Zito &
Tomie, 2014). Fascinating research has shown that individual differences in sign tracking and
goal tracking are genetically based and related to neurobiological differences associated with
impulsivity and drug abuse (Flagel et al., 2011).
visceral response similar in form to the UR (salivation to food powder). These features of the
CR were considered to be universal characteristics of classical conditioning during much of
the 20th century. Pavlovian conditioning was considered to be primarily a mechanism for
adjusting physiological and glandular responses to the environment through experience
(Skinner, 1938), and the CR was assumed to always be similar to the UR (e.g., Mackintosh,
1974). However, neither of these assumptions is valid for the laboratory situations that are
commonly used in contemporary research on Pavlovian conditioning.
Sign tracking, goal tracking, and conditioned eye-blink responses are skeletal rather than
glandular responses. In eye-blink conditioning, the CR is similar to the UR. But this is not the
case in fear conditioning. Here the foot shock that serves as the US elicits a vigorous startle
and jump response, but the CS comes to elicit a contrasting freezing response. In many
Pavlovian situations, the CR is not similar to the responses that are elicited by the US.
If we cannot assume that the CR will always be similar to the UR, how can we predict
Domjan, Michael. The Essentials of Conditioning and Learning, American Psychological Association, 2017. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kuleuvenul/detail.action?docID=5116675.
Created from kuleuvenul on 2018-10-01 03:09:22.
what kind of behavior will develop with Pavlovian conditioning? This remains a challenging
question. A promising approach to answering the question is based on the identification of
preexisting behavior systems that may be activated by a Pavlovian conditioning procedure.
I introduced the concept of behavior systems in Chapter 2. The concept is relevant to the
present discussion because the US in a Pavlovian conditioning procedure activates the
behavior system relevant to that US. Presentations of food to a hungry animal activate the
feeding system, presentations of shock activate the defensive behavior system, and
presentations of a sexual US activate the reproductive behavior system. The CR that develops
depends on how the CS becomes incorporated into the behavior system activated by the US
(Domjan & Krause, in press).
The feeding system, for example, involves a sequence of response modes starting with
general search and then moving on to focal search and ingestive or consummatory behavior
(see Figure 4.3). If a CS is presented before the animal receives each portion of food, the CS
will become incorporated into one of the response modes of the feeding behavior system,
which will in turn determine what type of CR the organism will perform (Timberlake, 2001). If
the CS becomes incorporated into the focal search mode, the CR will consist of focal search
responses such as sign tracking or goal tracking (Wasserman, Franklin, & Hearst, 1974). In
contrast, if the CS becomes incorporated into the ingestive, consummatory response mode, the
CR will involve handling and chewing the CS (Boakes, Poli, Lockwood, & Goodall, 1978).
FIG URE 4. 3
Copyright © 2017. American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
Behavior systems and Pavlovian conditioning. Conditioning procedures with food as the unconditioned
stimulus (US) involve the feeding system. As a result of pairings of the conditioned stimulus (CS) with food,
the CS becomes incorporated into the feeding system and comes to elicit food-related responses.
Pavlovian conditioning procedure. This phenomenon is called the latent inhibition effect
(Lubow & Weiner, 2010).
Studies of the latent inhibition effect are usually conducted in two phases, the preexposure
phase and the conditioning phase. In the preexposure phase, participants are given repeated
presentations of the stimulus that will be used later as the CS. For example, a tone that
subsequently will be paired with food may be presented a number of times during the
preexposure phase. During this phase, the tone is presented by itself, without the US. After the
preexposure phase, the tone is paired with the food US, using conventional classical
conditioning procedures. The typical outcome is that CS preexposure retards the subsequent
development of conditioned responding to the tone.
The CS preexposure effect has been interpreted as reflecting attentional processes.
Repeated presentations of a tone (for example) during the preexposure phase are assumed to
reduce the participant’s attention to the tone, and this in turn is assumed to disrupt subsequent
Domjan, Michael. The Essentials of Conditioning and Learning, American Psychological Association, 2017. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kuleuvenul/detail.action?docID=5116675.
Created from kuleuvenul on 2018-10-01 03:09:22.
Pavlovian conditioning of the tone (e.g., Schmajuk, 2010). Because of the involvement of
attentional processes, the latent inhibition effect has become popular as a technique for
studying brain mechanisms and disorders such as schizophrenia that involve deficits in
attention (Lubow, 2011).
Domjan, Michael. The Essentials of Conditioning and Learning, American Psychological Association, 2017. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kuleuvenul/detail.action?docID=5116675.
Created from kuleuvenul on 2018-10-01 03:09:22.
FIG URE 4. 4
Procedure and results of the experiment by Garcia and Koelling (1966) demonstrating selective
associations in aversion learning.
Like laboratory rats, people also learn aversions to stimuli selectively. People who
experience some form of gastrointestinal illness are more likely to learn an aversion to a novel
food they ate just before becoming sick than they are to learn an aversion to other types of
stimuli they may have encountered. Consistent with the selective association effect, people do
not report acquiring a food aversion if they hurt themselves in a physical accident or if they
develop an irritating skin rash (Logue, Ophir, & Strauss, 1981; Pelchat & Rozin, 1982). Only
illness experiences are effective in inducing a food aversion.
Since the initial demonstration of the selective association effect in aversion learning, such
Copyright © 2017. American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
effects have been found in other forms of learning as well. For example, Shapiro, Jacobs, and
LoLordo (1980) found that pigeons are more likely to associate a visual stimulus than an
auditory stimulus with food. However, when the birds are conditioned with shock, the auditory
cue is more likely to become conditioned than the visual cue. Selective associations also occur
in primate fear conditioning (Mineka & Öhman, 2002). Monkeys and people learn to be fearful
of the sight of snakes more easily than the sight of flowers. This seems to be the result of an
evolutionary predisposition. Enhanced sensitivity to the sight of snakes has been observed in
human infants as young as 8 to 14 months of age (LoBue & DeLoache, 2010).
FIG URE 4. 5
Diagram of the discriminative control procedure for Pavlovian conditioning. Two types of trials occur in
Copyright © 2017. American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
random alternation. On some trials, one conditioned stimulus, the CS+, is paired with the unconditioned
stimulus (US). On the remaining trials, another conditioned stimulus, the CS−, is presented alone.
Stronger conditioned responding to CS+ than to CS− is evidence of associative learning rather than some
form of sensitization.
What would happen if presentations of the US only sensitized responding to the light and
tone CSs? Sensitization is not based on an association and thus does not depend on the pairing
of a stimulus with the US. Therefore, sensitization is expected to elevate responding to both the
CS+ and the CS−. If only sensitization occurred in the discriminative control procedure, the
participants would respond to the CS+ and CS− in a similar fashion.
How about associative learning? In contrast to sensitization, associative learning should
be specific to the stimulus that is paired with the US. Therefore, associative learning should
elevate responding to the CS+ more than the CS−. Greater responding to the CS+ than to the CS−
Domjan, Michael. The Essentials of Conditioning and Learning, American Psychological Association, 2017. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kuleuvenul/detail.action?docID=5116675.
Created from kuleuvenul on 2018-10-01 03:09:22.
in the discriminative control provides strong evidence of associative learning.
The discriminative control procedure permits the evaluation of associative effects within a
single group of subjects (based on how those subjects respond differently to the CS+ and the
CS−). Another approach that is frequently used is the unpaired control procedure. In this
procedure, the CS and US are presented repeatedly, but the stimulus presentations are
deliberately scheduled so that the CS and US never occur together or on the same trial. This
procedure is administered to a control group, which is compared with an experimental group
that receives the CS paired with the US. Greater responding in the paired group compared with
the unpaired group is considered evidence of associative Pavlovian conditioning.
times relative to the flower. After observing numerous presentations of both objects, the
research participants are asked to indicate their judgment as to the strength of the causal
relation between them.
Studies of human causal judgment are analogous to studies of Pavlovian conditioning in
that both involve repeated experiences with two events and responses based on the extent to
which those two events are related to one another. In view of these similarities, one might
expect that there is considerable commonality between the results of causal judgment and
Pavlovian conditioning experiments. That expectation has been borne out in numerous studies
(Allan, 2005), suggesting that Pavlovian associative mechanisms may play a role in the
numerous informal judgments of causality we all make in the course of our daily lives.
I described earlier in this chapter how Pavlovian conditioning can result in the acquisition
of fear. The mechanisms of fear conditioning are of considerable interest because of the role of
fear conditioning in anxiety disorders, phobias, and panic disorder (Craske, Hermans, &
Vansteenwegen, 2006; Oehlberg & Mineka, 2011). As I already discussed, Pavlovian
Domjan, Michael. The Essentials of Conditioning and Learning, American Psychological Association, 2017. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kuleuvenul/detail.action?docID=5116675.
Created from kuleuvenul on 2018-10-01 03:09:22.
conditioning is also involved in drug tolerance and addiction (Siegel, 2008). Cues that reliably
accompany drug administration can come to elicit drug-related responses through conditioning.
In discussing this type of learning on the part of crack addicts, Dr. Scott Lukas of McLean
Hospital in Massachusetts described the effects of drug-conditioned stimuli by saying that
“these cues turn on crack-related memories, and addicts respond like Pavlov’s dogs”
(Newsweek Staff, 2001, p. 40).
Pavlovian conditioning is also involved in infant and maternal responses during nursing.
Suckling involves mutual stimulation for the infant and mother. To successfully nurse, the
mother has to hold the baby in a special position, which provides special tactile stimuli for
both the infant and the mother. The tactile stimuli experienced by the infant may become
conditioned to elicit orientation and suckling responses on the part of the baby (Blass,
Ganchrow, & Steiner, 1984). Olfactory cues experienced by the infant also become
conditioned during suckling episodes. Infants come to prefer suckling-associated cues, with the
preference evident as long as a year after the conditioning episode (Delaunay-El Allam et al.,
2010).
Pavlovian conditioning is also important in learning about sexual situations. Clinical
observations indicate that human sexual behavior can be shaped by learning experiences, but
the most extensive experimental evidence for sexual conditioning has been obtained in studies
with laboratory animals (Domjan & Akins, 2011). In these studies, males typically serve as
participants, and the US is provided either by the sight of a sexually receptive female or by
physical access to a female. Subjects come to approach stimuli that signal the availability of a
sexual partner. The presentation of a sexually CS also facilitates various aspects of
reproductive behavior. After exposure to a sexual CS, males are quicker to perform copulatory
responses, compete more successfully with other males for access to a female, show more
courtship behavior, release greater quantities of sperm, show increased levels of testosterone
and luteinizing hormone, and produce more offspring.
Copyright © 2017. American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
Summary
Although studies of Pavlovian conditioning began with the conditioning of salivation and other
glandular responses in dogs, contemporary investigations focus on conditioning skeletal
responses in sign tracking, fear conditioning, and eye-blink conditioning. These investigations
have shown that different types of CRs can develop, depending on the nature of the CS and the
behavior system activated by the US.
Because Pavlovian conditioning involves the learning of an association between a CS and
a US, behavioral changes due to mere repetition of the CS and US have to be excluded. The
random control procedure is not effective in this regard because it can result in associative
learning. Although an entirely satisfactory control procedure is not available, the
discriminative control and unpaired control procedures are reasonably effective. In the
discriminative control procedure, one CS is paired with the US and another CS is presented
Domjan, Michael. The Essentials of Conditioning and Learning, American Psychological Association, 2017. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kuleuvenul/detail.action?docID=5116675.
Created from kuleuvenul on 2018-10-01 03:09:22.
without the US. Differential responding to the two CSs provides evidence of associative
learning. In the unpaired control procedure, the CS is presented at times when the US is certain
to not occur.
Pavlovian conditioning may occur wherever one event reliably precedes another.
Examples include causality judgments, drug tolerance and addiction, suckling and nursing, and
learning to predict potential sexual encounters.
Suggested Readings
Bouton, M. E., Mineka, S., & Barlow, D. H. (2001). A modern learning theory perspective on
the etiology of panic disorder. Psychological Review, 108, 4–32.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.4
Domjan, M. (2005). Pavlovian conditioning: A functional perspective. Annual Review of
Psychology, 56, 179–206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141409
Flagel, S. B., Akil, H., & Robinson, T. E. (2009). Individual differences in the attribution of
incentive salience to reward-related cues: Implications for addiction.
Neuropharmacology, 56(Suppl. 1), 139–148.
Papini, M. R., & Bitterman, M. E. (1990). The role of contingency in classical conditioning.
Psychological Review, 97, 396–403.
Siegel, S. (2008). Learning and the wisdom of the body. Learning & Behavior, 36, 242–252.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/LB.36.3.242
Technical Terms
Appetitive conditioning
Associative learning
Copyright © 2017. American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
Autoshaping
Aversive conditioning
CS–US relevance
Conditioned response (CR)
Conditioned stimulus (CS)
Conditioned suppression
Discriminative control
Latent inhibition
Random control
Selective association
Sign tracking
Skinner box
Taste-aversion learning
Unconditioned response (UR)
Domjan, Michael. The Essentials of Conditioning and Learning, American Psychological Association, 2017. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kuleuvenul/detail.action?docID=5116675.
Created from kuleuvenul on 2018-10-01 03:09:22.
Unconditioned stimulus (US)
Unpaired control procedure
Copyright © 2017. American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
Domjan, Michael. The Essentials of Conditioning and Learning, American Psychological Association, 2017. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kuleuvenul/detail.action?docID=5116675.
Created from kuleuvenul on 2018-10-01 03:09:22.