Scientific American Special Collector's Edition - Winter 2022
Scientific American Special Collector's Edition - Winter 2022
ESTABLISHED 1845
Mind from Matter
In 2016 a panel of physicists, a cosmologist and a philosopher gathered at the American
Secrets of the Mind Museum of Natural History to discuss an idea seemingly befitting science fiction: Are we liv-
is published by the staff ing in a computer simulation? How exactly the flesh and blood of our brain is able to formu-
of Scientific American, late an aware, self-examining mind capable of critical thought remains a mystery. Perhaps
with project management by:
the answer eludes us because, the panel mused, we are the avatars of a higher species’ simu-
lation and simply unable to discover the truth. As intriguing a hypothesis as it is, neurosci-
Editor in Chief: Laura Helmuth
ence has learned enough about our consciousness to counter such a fantastical possibility.
Managing Editor: Curtis Brainard Newly mapped networks within the human brain show regions that fire in concert to cre-
Senior Editor, Collections: ate cognition (page 6). Zapping the brain with magnetic pulses while recording neural activ-
Andrea Gawrylewski ity might soon detect conscious thought, which could be especially useful for patients who
are awake but unable to communicate or respond to external stimuli (page 26). These discov-
Creative Director: Michael Mrak
eries chip away at the isolating experience of humanity and the idea that a person can never
Issue Designer: Lawrence R. Gendron truly know whether anyone but oneself is truly conscious (page 32).
Senior Graphics Editor: Jen Christiansen To some extent, we exist
Associate Graphics Editor: A
manda Montañez in our own bubbles of sub-
jective experience. A grow-
Photography Editor: Monica Bradley
ing body of evidence sug-
Associate Photo Editor: Liz Tormes gests that perception is a
construction of the brain
Copy Director: Maria-Christina Keller
(page 38). Because the brain
Senior Copy Editors:
initiates some actions be
Angelique Rondeau, Aaron Shattuck
fore we become aware that
Managing Production Editor: Richard Hunt we have made a decision,
Prepress and Quality Manager: we might even deduce that
Silvia De Santis each of us is some kind of
Executive Assistant Supervisor: Maya Harty
biochemical puppet, but
experiments confirm that
we do indeed have free will
Acting President: Stephen Pincock
(page 44). And our cogni-
Executive Vice President: Michael Florek tion clearly results from
Vice President, Commercial: Andrew Douglas
highly evolved neural mech-
anisms, common to all of
Publisher and Vice President:
us, for making new memories (page 48), navigating social relationships (page 54) and recog-
Jeremy A. Abbate
nizing faces (page 60). Ultimately a shared sense of reality influences how we perceive our-
Associate Vice President,
Business Development: Diane McGarvey
selves and the formation of “in-groups” and “out-groups,” which can create social and politi-
cal division (page 68).
Marketing Director, Institutional Partnerships For all that, however, a lot happens outside of our awareness. Based on neuronal firing pat-
and Customer Development: Jessica Cole terns, algorithms can infer intended body movements in patients with paralysis of their limbs,
Programmatic Product Manager: Zoya Lysak illuminating the unconscious brain-body dynamic (page 74). During sleep, the brain makes
Director, Integrated Media: Matt Bondlow crucial gains in learning, memory and emotion processing (page 82 and page 88).
And there is much out there that can alter our reality, from drugs to disease. Potentially
Senior Marketing Manager:
Christopher Monello
a third of older COVID patients experience delirium, perhaps increasing their risk for demen-
tia later on (page 116). Psychiatrists now debate whether bouts of mania in which people report
Senior Product Manager: Ian Kelly
enhanced recall, increased empathy and spirituality might constitute a new category of men-
Senior Web Producer: Jessica Ramirez tal disorder (page 110). Ironically, rather than distorting it, substances such as Ecstasy and
Senior Commercial Operations Coordinator: “magic” mushrooms may actually help restore a rational view of the world (page 122).
Christine Kaelin Consciousness may come from an alien programmer, or perhaps it pervades our universe,
not just our brains but all things, as philosopher Philip Goff posits (page 124). Either way, our
Jorm Sangsorn/Getty Images (this page)
columnist John Horgan calls them on page 96—places us at a tantalizing intersection of life
Press Manager: Sarah Hausman
and sentience. What we think matters to us regardless of how the mind arises from matter.
Production Controller: Madelyn Keyes-Milch
Advertising Production Manager: Andrea Gawrylewski
Michael Broomes Senior Editor, Collections, [email protected]
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 1
SPECIAL EDITION
WHAT IS CONSCIOUSNESS?
6 How Matter Becomes Mind
The new discipline of network neuroscience yields
a picture of how mental activity arises from carefully
orchestrated interactions among different brain areas.
By Max Bertolero and Danielle S. Bassett
14 How to Make a Consciousness Meter
Zapping the brain with magnetic pulses while
measuring its electrical activity is proving to be a reliable
way to detect consciousness. B y Christof Koch
20 Can Lab-Grown Brains
Become Conscious?
A handful of experiments are raising questions about
whether clumps of cells and disembodied brains could be
sentient and how scientists would know if they were.
By Sara Reardon
26 The Brain Electric
Electrodes that stimulate brain tissue reveal the
topography of conscious experience. B y Christof Koch
32 How Do I Know I’m Not the Only 4 what is consciousness ?
Conscious Being in the Universe?
The solipsism problem, also called the problem of other
minds, lurks at the heart of science, philosophy, religion,
the arts and the human condition. B y John Horgan
ALTERED REALITY
102 A Disorder of Mind and Brain
unconscious mind 72
A mysterious condition once known as hysteria is
challenging the divide between psychiatry and neurology.
By Diana Kwon
110 The Undiscovered Illness
Hundreds of thousands of people experience mania
without ever getting depressed. Why does psychiatry
insist on calling them bipolar? B
y Simon Makin
116 Could COVID Delirium
Bring On Dementia?
Delirium is very common on COVID wards. Researchers
are testing whether these temporary bouts of confusion
could bring on permanent cognitive decline.
By Carrie Arnold
122 A Psychedelic Renaissance
Psilocybin and MDMA represent a first wave of therapies
for psychiatric disorders that help patients by changing
the way they view reality. B
y Danielle Schlosser and
Thomas R. Insel
DEPARTMENTS
1 FROM THE EDITOR
Mind from Matter
1 24 END NOTE
Does Consciousness Pervade the Universe?
altered reality 100 Philosopher Philip Goff answers questions about
“panpsychism.” B
y Gareth Cook
Articles in this special issue are updated or adapted from previous issues of S cientific American a nd Nature and from ScientificAmerican.com.
Copyright © 2022 Scientific American, a division of Springer Nature America, Inc. All rights reserved. Scientific American Special (ISSN 1936-1513), Volume 31, Number 1, Winter 2022,
published by Scientific American, a division of Springer Nature America, Inc., 1 New York Plaza, Suite 4600, New York, N.Y. 10004-1562. Canadian BN No. 127387652RT;
TVQ1218059275 TQ0001. To purchase additional quantities: U.S., $13.95 each; elsewhere, $17.95 each. Send payment to Scientific American Back Issues, P.O. Box 3187, Harlan Iowa 51537. Inquiries:
fax 212-355-0408 or telephone 212-451-8415. Printed in U.S.A.
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 3
What
Is
Conscıousness?
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 5
how
matter
becomes
mınd The new discipline of network neuroscience
yields a picture of how mental activity arises
from carefully orchestrated interactions
among different brain areas
By Max Bertolero and Danielle S. Bassett
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 9
Decoding 100 Trillion Messages Module 1 Node
The Milky Way h as hundreds of billions of stars—just a fraction of the 100 trillion connections
in our brains that enable us to sense, think and act. To unravel this complexity, network
neuroscientists create a map, or “graph,” consisting of nodes linked by edges that fit into
Local hub
modules, which are tethered to one another with highly connected nodes called hubs.
Connector hub
Brain Modules
Visual
Attention
Frontoparietal control
Somatic motor
Salience
B Connector hubs
Default with the strongest
links to multiple other
Limbic
modules appear in this
side view, colored to
A Seven key modules, indicate the seven
denoted by colors, pivotal brain modules.
spread across sometimes
disconnected areas
of the brain.
C A graph of the human brain’s nodes and edges shows the strongest connector hubs
represented as large circles. Each node’s color represents the module it belongs to.
Nodes can be visualized as repelling magnets with edges between nodes acting
as springs that hold them together. Tightly connected nodes cluster together.
Connector hubs occupy the center because they are well connected to all modules.
Strongest
functions are dedicated to specific tasks, often rep Visual tracking relationship
resented here by psychological tests. The most active Action observation
tasks rise to the top. The visual module, for instance,
Picture naming (silently)
is involved with naming, reading and observing. Many
tasks require multiple modules. For example, a mental Brightness perception
Visual
rotation task recruits both the visual and the attention Picture naming (out loud)
modules. Some modules are entrusted with more Silent reading
abstract tasks. The frontoparietal module engages Visual attention
in switching tasks or recalling lists. The default mode Drawing
module attends to subjective emotional states or passive
Controlling eye movement
listening when a person is at rest.
Mental rotation
Visual control Attention
Pointing
Writing
Imagined movement
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (reasoning)
Counting
Tower of London (complex planning task)
n-back working memory task
Sternberg working memory task Frontoparietal
Task switching control
Word stem completion (out loud)
Free word list recall
Stroop task
Flanker response inhibition task
Detecting vibrations through touch
Finger tapping
Vocal rehearsing Somatic
Small hand movements motor
Whistling
Grasping
Isometric force
Awareness of need to urinate
Stimulation monitoring
Nonpainful electrical stimulation
Breath holding Salience
Word stem completion (silent)
Playing music
Imaging what others think
Categorizing emotional scenes
Passive listening
Lying
Pitch detection
Event recall (episodic memory) Default
Delayed gratification
Word generation (out loud)
Word meaning discrimination
Grammar
Face-emotion identification
Scent detection
Video games
Classical conditioning Limbic
Eating/drinking
Passive viewing
Monetary reward task
Graphics by Max Bertolero (brains and network diagram) and Jen Christiansen (task chart) SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 11
be described by particular patterns tent strengthening of the structur-
among the hub connections. If al connections to hubs throughout
your brain network has strong the course of childhood is associ-
hubs with many connections ated with an increase in the segre-
across modules, it tends to have gation between modules and an
modules that are clearly segregat- augmentation in the efficiency
ed from one another, and you will with which young people perform
perform better on a range of tasks, executive tasks such as complex
from short-term memory to math- reasoning and self-regulation. We
ematics, language or social cogni- have also found that the segrega-
tion. Put simply, your thoughts, tion of modules from one another
feelings, quirks, flaws and mental is more rapid in children who have
strengths are all encoded by the a higher socioeconomic status,
specific organization of the brain highlighting the key impact of
as a unified, integrated network. In their environment.
sum, it is the music your brain Although changes in structural
plays that makes you y ou. connectivity are slow, the reconfig-
The brain’s synchronized mod- uration of functional connections
ules both establish your identity can occur quickly, in a few seconds
and help to retain it over time. The or minutes. These rapid shifts are
musical compositions they play ap- instrumental for moving between
pear to always be similar. The like- tasks and for the massive amount
ness could be witnessed when par- of learning demanded even by a
ticipants in two other studies in the MULTITUDES o f white matter connections in this scan single task. In a set of studies that
Human Connectome Project en- are used to model the brain’s physical pathways— we published from 2011 to 2019, we
gaged in various tasks that involved functional networks use these structural linkages found that networks with modules
short-term memory, recognition of to carry out an array of cognitive tasks. that can change readily turn up in
the emotions of others, gambling, individuals who have greater exec-
finger tapping, language, mathematics, social reasoning and a utive function and learning capacity.
self-induced “resting state” in which they let their mind wander. To better understand what was happening, we used publicly
Fascinatingly, the networks’ functional wiring has more sim- available data from a landmark study known as MyConnectome,
ilarities than expected across all these activities. Returning to in which Stanford University psychology professor Russell
our analogy, it is not as if the brain plays Beethoven when doing Poldrack personally underwent imaging and cognitive apprais-
math and Tupac when resting. The symphony in our head is the als three times a week for more than a year. Whereas modules
same musician playing the same musical genre. This consisten- are mostly autonomous and segregated, at times the brain will
cy derives from the fact that the brain’s physical pathways, or spontaneously reorganize its connections. This property, called
structural connections, place constraints on the routes over the functional network flexibility, lets a node with strong function-
brain’s integrated network that a neural signal can travel. And al connections within a module suddenly establish many con-
those pathways delineate how functional connections—the ones, nections to a different module, changing the flow of information
say, for math or language—can be configured. In the musical through the network. Using data from this study, we found that
metaphor, a bass drum cannot play the melodic line of a piano. the rerouting of a network’s connections changes from day to
Changes in the brain’s music inevitably occur, just as new ar- day in a manner that matches positive mood, arousal and fa-
rangements do for orchestral music. Physical connections un- tigue. In healthy individuals, such network flexibility correlates
dergo alterations over the course of months or years, whereas with better cognitive function.
functional connectivity shifts on the order of seconds, when a
person switches between one mental task and the next. DISSONANT NOTES
Danielle S. Bassett and Matthew Cieslak/University of Pennsylvania
Transformations in both structural and functional connectiv- The configuration o f brain connections also reflects one’s
ity are important during adolescent brain development, when the mental health. Aberrant connectivity patterns accompany depres-
finishing touches of the brain’s wiring diagram are being refined. sion, schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s, Parkinson’s, autism spectrum
This period is of critical importance because the first signs of men- disorder, attention deficit disorder, dementia and epilepsy.
tal disorders often appear in adolescence or early adulthood. Most mental illnesses are not confined to one area of the brain.
One area our research relates to is understanding how brain The circuitry affected in schizophrenia extends quite widely
networks develop through childhood and adolescence and into across the entire organ. The so-called disconnectivity hypothe-
adulthood. These processes are driven by underlying physiolog- sis for schizophrenia holds that there is nothing abnormal about
ical changes, but they are also influenced by learning, exposure the individual modules. Instead the disarray relates to an over-
to new ideas and skills, an individual’s socioeconomic status and abundance of connections between modules.
other experiences. In a healthy brain, modules are mostly autonomous and seg-
Brain-network modules emerge very early in life, even in the regated, and the ability to bring about flexible changes in net-
womb, but their connectivity is refined as we grow up. Consis- work connections is beneficial for cognitive functioning—with-
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 13
HOW TO MAKE A
CONSCIOUSNESS
CONSCIOUSNESS
METER Zapping the brain with magnetic
pulses while measuring its electrical
activity is proving to be a reliable
way to detect consciousness
By Christof Koch
Illustration by Ashley Mackenzie
Subcortical stroke
signal their thoughts and inten-
tions. With proper nursing care to
avoid bedsores and infections, these
Cortical stroke
patients can live for years.
In this first group, clinicians dis-
tinguish several subcategories. Pa-
Severe brain injury
tients in a vegetative state, which is
better described by the less pejora-
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 19
20 | SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN | SPECIAL EDITION | WINTER 2022
Can Lab-Grown Brains
Become Conscious?
A handful of experiments are raising questions about whether
clumps of cells and disembodied brains could be sentient and
how scientists would know if they were • By Sara Reardon
Illustration by Thomas Fuchs
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 21
I
n A ly s s o n M u o t r i ’ s l a b o rat o ry, h u n d r e d s o f m i n i at u r e h u ma n b ra i n s ,
the size of sesame seeds, float in petri dishes, sparking with electrical activity. These
tiny structures, known as brain organoids, are grown from human stem cells and have
become a familiar fixture in many labs that study the properties of the brain. Muotri, a
neuroscientist at the University of California, San Diego, has found some unusual ways
to deploy his. He has connected organoids to walking robots, modified their genomes
with Neandertal genes, launched them into orbit onboard the International Space Sta-
tion and used them as models to develop more humanlike artificial-intelligence sys-
tems. Like many scientists, Muotri temporarily pivoted to studying COVID, using brain
organoids to test how drugs perform against the SARS-CoV-2 coronavirus.
But one experiment has drawn more scrutiny than the oth- to those used in animal research, to guide the humane use of
ers. In August 2019 Muotri’s group published a paper in C ell brain organoids and other experiments that could achieve con-
Stem Cell reporting the creation of human brain organoids that sciousness. In June 2020 the U.S. National Academies of Scienc-
produced coordinated waves of activity resembling those seen es, Engineering and Medicine began a study with the aim of
in premature babies. The waves continued for months before outlining the potential legal and ethical issues associated with
the team shut the experiment down. brain organoids and human–animal chimeras.
This type of brain-wide, coordinated electrical activity is one The concerns over lab-grown brains have also highlighted a
of the properties of a conscious brain. The team’s finding led problem: neuroscientists have no agreed way to define and
ethicists and scientists to raise a host of moral and philosophi- measure consciousness. Without a working definition, ethicists
cal questions about whether organoids should be allowed to worry that it will be impossible to stop an experiment before it
reach this level of advanced development, whether “conscious” crosses a line.
organoids might be entitled to special treatment and rights not The current crop of experiments could force the issue. If sci-
afforded to other clumps of cells and the possibility that con- entists become convinced that an organoid has gained con-
sciousness could be created from scratch. sciousness, they might need to hurry up and agree on a theory of
The idea of bodiless, self-aware brains was already on the how that happened, says Anil K. Seth, a cognitive neuroscientist
minds of many neuroscientists and bioethicists. Just a few at the University of Sussex in England. But, he says, if one per-
months earlier a team at Yale University announced that it had son’s favored theory deems the organoid conscious, whereas
at least partially restored life to the brains of pigs that had been another’s does not, any confidence that consciousness has been
killed hours before. By removing the brains from the pigs’ skulls attained vanishes. “Confidence largely depends on what theory
and infusing them with a chemical cocktail, the researchers we believe in. It’s a circularity.”
revived the neurons’ cellular functions and their ability to
transmit electrical signals. SENTIENT STATES
Other experiments, such as efforts to add human neurons to C r e at i n g a c o n s c i o us s ys t e m m
ight be a whole lot easier
mouse brains, have raised questions, with some scientists and than defining it. Researchers and clinicians define conscious-
ethicists arguing that these experiments should not be allowed. ness in many different ways for various purposes, but it is hard
The studies set the stage for a debate between those who to synthesize them into one neat operational definition that
want to avoid the creation of consciousness and those who see could be used to decide on the status of a lab-grown brain.
complex organoids as a means to study and test treatments for Physicians generally assess the level of consciousness in
human diseases. Muotri and many other neuroscientists think patients in a vegetative state on the basis of whether the person
that human brain organoids could be the key to understanding blinks or flinches in response to pain or other stimuli. Using
uniquely human conditions such as autism and schizophrenia, electroencephalogram (EEG) readings, for instance, research-
which are impossible to study in detail in mouse models. To ers can also measure how the brain responds when it is zapped
achieve this goal, Muotri says, he and others might need to with an electrical pulse. A conscious brain will display much
deliberately create consciousness. more complex, unpredictable electrical activity than one that is
Researchers have been calling for a set of guidelines, similar unconscious, which responds with simple, regular patterns.
But such tests might not adequately probe whether a person to be complex enough to be classed as conscious. And organoids
lacks consciousness. In brain-imaging studies of people who are cannot blink or recoil from a painful stimulus, so they would
in a coma or a vegetative state, scientists have shown that unre- not pass the clinical test for consciousness.
Muotri Lab/University of California, San Diego
sponsive individuals can display some brain activity reminis- In contrast, it is much more likely that an intact brain from a
cent of consciousness—such as activity in motor areas when recently killed pig has the necessary structures for conscious-
asked to think about walking. ness, as well as wiring created by memories and experiences the
In any case, standard medical tests for consciousness are dif- animal had while it was alive. “Thinking about a brain that has
ficult to apply to brain cells grown in dishes or to disembodied been filled with all this, it is hard to imagine that brain would be
animal brains. When Muotri suggested that his organoids’ fir- empty,” says Jeantine Lunshof, a philosopher and neuroethicist
ing patterns were just as complex as those seen in preterm at Harvard University. “What they can do in terms of thinking, I
infants, people were unsure what to make of that. Some re don’t know, but it’s for sure not zero,” Lunshof says. Bringing a
searchers do not consider the brain activity in a preterm infant dead brain back to a semblance of life, as the Yale team did,
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 23
might have the potential to restore a degree of consciousness, necessary to create complex EEG patterns. Still, Lancaster
although the scientists took pains to avoid this by using chemi- admits that for advanced organoids, it depends on the defini-
cal blocking agents that prevented brain-wide activity. tion. “If you thought a fly was conscious, it’s conceivable that an
Researchers agree that they need to take the possibilities organoid could be,” she says.
raised by these studies seriously. In October 2019 U.C.S.D. held Lancaster and most other researchers think that something
a conference of about a dozen neuroscientists and philoso- like a revitalized pig brain would be much more likely to achieve
phers, together with students and members of the public, with consciousness than an organoid. The team that did the work on
the intention of establishing and publishing an ethical frame- the pig brains, led by neuroscientist Nenad Sestan, was trying to
work for future experiments. But the paper was delayed for find new ways to revitalize organs, not to create consciousness.
months, partly because several of the authors could not agree The researchers were able to get individual neurons or groups of
on the basic requirements for consciousness. them to fire and were careful to try to avoid the creation of wide-
spread brain waves. Still, when Sestan’s team members saw what
INCREASINGLY COMPLEX looked like coordinated EEG activity in one of the brains, they
so far nobody h as created consciousness in the lab, say sci- immediately halted the project. Even after a neurology specialist
entists and ethicists who study the issue. But they are asking confirmed that the pattern was not consistent with consciousness,
themselves what to watch out for and which theories of con- the group anesthetized the brains as a precautionary measure.
sciousness might be most relevant. According to an idea called Sestan also contacted the U.S. National Institutes of Health for
integrated information theory, for example, consciousness is guidance on how to proceed. The agency’s neuroethics panel,
a product of how densely neuronal networks are connected including Lunshof and Insoo Hyun, a bioethicist at Case Western
across the brain. The more neurons that interact with one University, assessed the work and agreed that Sestan should con-
another, the higher the degree of consciousness—a quantity tinue to anesthetize the brains. But the panel has not settled on
known as phi. If phi is greater than zero, the organism is con- more general regulations and does not routinely require a bioeth-
sidered conscious. ics assessment for organoid proposals, because its members think
Most animals reach this bar, according to the theory. Christof that consciousness is unlikely to arise. The nih has not arrived at
Koch, who serves on Scientific American’s board of advisers and a definition of consciousness, either. “It’s so flexible, everyone
is chief scientist of the MindScope Program at the Allen Institute claims their own meaning,” Hyun says. “If it’s not clear we’re talk-
for Brain Science, doubts any existing organoid could achieve ing about the same thing, it’s a big problem for discourse.”
this threshold but concedes that a more advanced one might.
Other competing theories of consciousness require sensory FUZZY DEFINITIONS
input or coordinated electrical patterns across multiple brain S o m e t h i n k i t i s f u t i l e t o even try to identify conscious-
regions. An idea known as global workspace theory, for in ness in any sort of lab-maintained brain. “It’s just impossible to
stance, posits that the brain’s prefrontal cortex functions as a say meaningful things about what these bunches of brain cells
computer, processing sensory inputs and interpreting them to could think or perceive, given we don’t understand conscious-
form a sense of being. Because organoids do not have a pre- ness,” says Steven Laureys, a neurologist at the University of
frontal cortex and cannot receive input, they cannot become Liège in Belgium, who pioneered some of the imaging-based
conscious. “Without input and output, the neurons may be talk- measures of consciousness in people in a vegetative state. “We
ing with each other, but that doesn’t necessarily mean anything shouldn’t be too arrogant.” Further research should proceed
like human thought,” says Madeline Lancaster, a developmen- very carefully, he says.
tal biologist at the University of Cambridge. Laureys and others point out that the experience of an organ-
Connecting organoids to organs, however, could be a fairly oid is likely to be very different from that of a preterm infant, an
simple task. In 2019 Lancaster’s team grew human brain organ- adult human or a pig and would not be directly comparable.
oids next to a mouse spinal column and back muscle. When Furthermore, the structures in an organoid might be too small
nerves from the human organoid connected with the spinal col- to have their activity measured accurately, and similarities
umn, the muscles began to spontaneously contract. between the EEG patterns of organoids and of preterm baby
Most organoids are built to reproduce only one portion of brains could be coincidental. Other scientists who work on brain
the brain—the cortex. But if they develop long enough and with organoids also caution against making assumptions about the
the right kinds of growth factor, human stem cells spontane- link between activity patterns in organoids and consciousness.
ously re-create many different parts of the brain, which then “This system is not the human brain,” says Sergiu Pasca, a
begin coordinating their electrical activity. In a study published neuroscientist at Stanford University. “They’re made out of neu-
in 2017, molecular biologist Paola Arlotta of Harvard coaxed rons. Neurons have electrical activity, but we have to think care-
stem cells to develop into brain organoids composed of many fully about how to compare them.”
different cell types, including light-sensitive cells like those Muotri wants his organoid systems to be comparable, in at
found in the retina. When exposed to light, neurons in the least some ways, with human brains so that he can study
organoids began firing. But the fact that these cells were active human disorders and find treatments. His motivation is per-
does not mean the organoids could see and process visual infor- sonal: his teenage son has epilepsy and autism. “He struggles
mation, Arlotta says. It simply means that they could form the hard in life,” Muotri says. Brain organoids are a promising ave-
necessary circuits. nue because they recapitulate the earliest stages of brain wir-
Arlotta and Lancaster think their organoids are too primi- ing, which are impossible to study as a human embryo devel-
tive to be conscious because they lack the anatomical structures ops. But studying human brain disorders without a fully func-
tioning brain, he says, is like studying a pancreas that does not Still, Muotri and others say they would welcome some strict-
produce insulin. “To get there, I need a brain organoid model er guidelines. These could include requiring scientists to justify
that really resembles a human brain. I might need an organoid the number of human brain organoids they use, to use them
that becomes conscious.” only for research that cannot be done in any other way, to
Muotri says he is agnostic about which definition to use to restrict the amount of pain that can be inflicted on them, and to
decide whether an organoid reaches consciousness. At some dispose of them humanely.
point, he says, organoids might even be able to help researchers Having such advice in place ahead of time would help
answer questions about how brains produce conscious states. researchers weigh the costs and benefits of creating conscious
For instance, mathematician Gabriel Silva of U.C.S.D. is studying entities. And many researchers emphasize that such experi-
neural activity in Muotri’s organoids to develop an algorithm ments have the potential to yield important insights. “There are
that describes how the brain generates consciousness. The goal truly conscious people out there with neurological disorders
of his project, which is partially funded by Microsoft, is to create with no treatments,” Lancaster says. “If we did stop all of this
an artificial system that works like human consciousness. research because of the philosophical thought experiment,” she
At the moment, there are no regulations in the U.S. or in adds, “that would be very detrimental to actual human beings
Europe that would stop a researcher from creating conscious- who do need some new treatment.”
ness. The National Academies panel released a report in April Treatments could still, however, be tested in brain organoids
2021 outlining the latest research and what it views as appro- made using mouse stem cells or in regular animal models. Such
priate oversight. Members weighed in on questions such as experiments could also inform discussions about the ethical use
whether to obtain people’s consent to develop their cells into of human organoids. For instance, Hyun would like to see
brain organoids and how to study and dispose of organoids researchers compare the EEG patterns of mouse brain organ-
humanely. The International Society for Stem Cell Research has oids with those of living mice, which might indicate how well
also released organoid guidelines but is not addressing con- human organoids recapitulate the human brain.
sciousness, because it does not think the science is there yet. For his part, Muotri sees little difference between working
Hyun says that the nih neuroethics panel has not yet seen on a human organoid and using a lab mouse. “We work with
any proposals to create complex, conscious organoids that animal models that are conscious, and there are no problems,”
would necessitate new guidelines. And Muotri says he does not he says. “We need to move forward, and if it turns out they
know of anyone else deliberately trying to create conscious become conscious, to be honest, I don’t see it as a big deal.”
Jesse Winter
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 25
The
Brain
Electric
Electrodes that stimulate brain tissue reveal
the topography of conscious experience
By Christof Koch
Illustration by Zara Picken
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 27
“My left foot “I see a star
Patient laughs shifted to the right in the top right
and says they and the sensation center. It was a blue
“felt well went all the way and silver star.”
internally.” up my calf.” “I feel
tingling in
“That my leg.”
felt good; quite “I just had the
erotic. I can’t even, urge to squeeze
um, I felt good. my fingers.
I can’t explain it. They just closed
Um, yeah, you’re by themselves.”
embarrassing me!”
“Pulsating feeling,
Patient mostly in the left
describes a negative chest but also in
emotional feeling, the left arm.”
seemingly localized
in the chest.
Patient
describes
a feeling
of fear.
Patient “Aroused,
says she calm ...
felt dizzy. sexually excited.”
Speech
arrest.
“Feels like
Patient cannot I’m going in a
Source: “Intrinsic Network Architecture Predicts the Effects Elicited by Intracranial Electrical Stimulation
Patient
describes a
feeling of nervous
anticipation.
“It smells
funny.
Negative, like
nail polish.”
“You just
“For being in turned into
this temperature, somebody else. Your
just a little more face metamorphosed.
sensitive. Almost as if Your nose got saggy;
I was in a colder went to the left.
temperature.” Not pretty.”
distorted faces reminiscent riences—were more likely to be active than those toward
the front, which consists of regions of the cortex impor-
of paintings by Salvador Dalí. tant for cognitive activity such as thinking, planning,
moral reasoning, decision-making and intelligence.
Despite their importance for thinking, these regions
reduce the incidence and severity of seizures. Pilot clin- have little to do with consciousness. Indeed, for the past
ical experiments are evaluating the use of such implanted century neurosurgeons have observed that so long as the
electrodes as a visual prosthetic device to enable people eloquent cortex is spared, massive regions of the pre-
with vision impairments to navigate and as a therapy for frontal cortex can be ablated without causing obvious
obsessive-compulsive disorder and depression. deficits in the daily stream of consciousness of these
patients. These regions of noneloquent cortex can mod-
HOT OR NOT ulate consciousness, but they are, by and large, not where
In July 2020 Nature Human Behaviour published an conscious experience appears to originate. That privilege
atlas highlighting locations across the cortex that, when belongs to more posterior regions—the parietal, tempo-
aroused with electrodes, evoked conscious experiences, ral and occipital lobes. Why the physical substrate of our
such as the storm and the disconnected body mentioned mental experiences should be in the back rather than in
earlier. Led by Josef Parvizi, a professor of neurology at the front of the brain remains a mystery.
the Stanford University School of Medicine, the clinical
team collected data from 67 people with epilepsy. The TO SEE OR NOT TO SEE
researchers recorded electrical activity from more than Applying iES to the visual cortex triggers optical sen-
1,500 sites in the cortex, primarily with subdural elec- sations known as phosphenes, brief flashes that resem-
trodes. They mapped the recordings from those sites to ble lightning striking a darkened plain. This observa-
spots on a digital brain model so they could compare data tion is the source of a long-standing dream of a pros-
from different brains (the pattern of ridges and valleys thetic device that restores some vision to people who
that give the organ the look of an oversized walnut dif- are blind. Millions worldwide live with deficits in both
fers from person to person). The team looked for “respon- eyes from retinitis pigmentosa, age-related macular
sive” electrodes that triggered some visual or tactile sen- degeneration, glaucoma, infection, cancer or trauma.
sation, muscle twitching or disrupted speech. If the Doctors, scientists and engineers started pursuing
patient did not feel anything when stimulated, that elec- visual prosthetics in the 1960s but have only recently
trode was marked as nonresponsive. been able to harness the appropriate technology to help
Patients reported a range of electrode-evoked subjec- blind people. One prominent example is a device known
tive experiences: briefly flashing points akin to stars of as Orion, developed by Second Sight Medical Products
light; distorted faces like those in the paintings of Sal- in Los Angeles. A tiny camera, mounted on glasses, con-
vador Dalí; bodily feelings such as tingling, tickling, verts images into pulses and transmits them wirelessly
burning, pulsing and so-called out-of-body experiences; to fire 60 electrodes sitting on the visual cortex. The
fear, unease, sexual arousal, merriment; the desire to handful of people who have had this experimental device
move a limb; the will to persevere in the face of some implanted into their brain perceive clouds of dots that
great but unrecognized challenge. Mere tickling of neu- allow them to navigate. “It’s still a blast every time I turn
ral tissue with a tiny bit of electric current was enough it on,” one study participant reports. “After seeing noth-
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 31
OPINION
How Do I Know
I’m Not the
Only Conscious
Being in the
Universe?
The solipsism problem, also called the
problem of other minds, lurks at the heart
of science, philosophy, religion, the arts
and the human condition
By John Horgan
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 33
I t is a central dilemma of human life—more urgent, arguably,
than the inevitability of suffering and death. I have been brooding
and ranting to my students about it for years. It surely troubles us
more than ever during this plague-ridden era. Philosophers call it
the problem of other minds. I prefer to call it the solipsism problem.
Solipsism, technically, is an extreme form of skepticism, at once
utterly illogical and irrefutable. It holds that you are the only con-
scious being in existence. The cosmos sprang into existence when
you became sentient, and it will vanish when you die. As crazy as this
proposition seems, it rests on a brute fact: each of us is sealed in an
impermeable prison cell of subjective awareness. Even our most inti-
mate exchanges might as well be carried out via Zoom.
You experience your own mind every waking sec-
ond, but you can only infer the existence of other
minds through indirect means. Other people seem to
possess conscious perceptions, emotions, memories,
intentions, just as you do, but you cannot be sure
they do. You can guess how the world looks to me
based on my behavior and utterances, including
these words you are reading, but you have no first-
hand access to my inner life. For all you know, I
might be a mindless bot.
Natural selection instilled in us the capacity for a
so-called theory of mind—a talent for intuiting oth-
ers’ emotions and intentions. But we have a counter-
tendency to deceive one another and to fear we are
that you are conscious, let alone a jellyfish, bot or
doorknob. As long as we lack what neuroscientist
Christof Koch has called a consciousness meter—a
device that can measure consciousness in the same
way that a thermometer measures temperature—the-
ories of consciousness will remain in the realm of
pure speculation.
But the solipsism problem is far more than a tech-
nical philosophical matter. It is a paranoid but under-
standable response to the feelings of solitude that lurk
within us all. Even if you reject solipsism as an intellec-
tual position, you sense it, emotionally, whenever you
feel estranged from others, whenever you confront the
awful truth that you can never know—really know—
being deceived. The ultimate deception would be another person, and no one can really know you.
pretending you are conscious when you are not. Religion is one response to the solipsism problem.
The solipsism problem thwarts efforts to explain Our ancestors dreamed up a supernatural entity who
consciousness. Scientists and philosophers have pro- bears witness to our innermost fears and desires. No
posed countless contradictory hypotheses about matter how lonesome we feel, how alienated from
what consciousness is and how it arises. Panpsychists our fellow humans, God is always there watching
Getty Images ( p receding pages)
contend that all creatures and even inanimate mat- over us. He sees our souls, our most secret selves, and
ter—even a single proton!—possess consciousness. He loves us anyway. Wouldn’t it be nice to think so?
Hard-core materialists insist, conversely (and per- The arts, too, can be seen as attempts to overcome
versely), that not even humans are all that conscious. the solipsism problem. The artist, musician, poet, nov-
The solipsism problem prevents us from verifying elist says, “This is how my life feels” or “This is how life
or falsifying these and other claims. I cannot be certain might feel for another person.” They help us imagine
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 35
How We
Perceive
the World
Our Inner Universes page 38
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 37
OUR
INNER
UNIVERSES Reality is constructed by the brain,
and no two brains are exactly alike
By Anil K. Seth
Illustration by Brook VanDevelder
ties. Primary qualities of an object, such as solidity and occupancy sistent with the patients’ biases, as would be expected if percep-
of space, exist independently of a perceiver. Secondary qualities, in tual predictions were strong in this case. And when the perceived
contrast, exist only in relation to a perceiver—color is a good exam- appearance was inconsistent with preexisting biases, infor
ple. This distinction explains why conceiving of perception as con- mation flow was stronger in the bottom-up direction, suggest-
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 41
ing a “prediction error” signal. This is an exciting new develop- The basic idea is simple. We again prerecorded some panoram-
ment in mapping the brain basis of controlled hallucinations. ic video footage, this time of the interior of our VR lab rather than
In my lab we have taken a different approach to exploring the of an outside campus scene. People coming to the lab are invited to
nature of perception and hallucination. Rather than looking into sit on a stool in the middle of the room and to put on a VR headset
the brain directly, we decided to simulate the influence of over- that has a camera attached to the front. They are encouraged to look
active perceptual priors using a unique virtual-reality setup mas- around the room and to see the room as it actually is, via the cam-
terminded by our resident VR guru, Keisuke Suzuki. We call it, era. But at some point, without telling them, we switch the feed so
with tongue firmly in cheek, the “hallucination machine.” that the headset now displays not the live real-world scene but rath-
Using a 360-degree camera, we first recorded panoramic vid- er the prerecorded panoramic video. Most people in this situation
eo footage of a busy square in the University of Sussex campus on continue to experience what they are seeing as real even though it
a Tuesday at lunchtime. We then processed is now a fake prerecording. (This is actual-
the footage through an algorithm based on ly very tricky to pull off in practice—it re-
Google’s AI program DeepDream to gen-
erate a simulated hallucination. What hap-
OUR PERCEPTIONS quires careful color balancing and align-
ment to avoid people noticing any differ-
pens is that the algorithm takes a so-called COME FROM THE ence that would tip them off to the shift.)
neural network—one of the workhorses of
AI—and runs it backward. The network we INSIDE OUT JUST AS I find this result fascinating because it
shows that it is possible to have people ex-
used had been trained to recognize objects
in images, so if you run it backward, up-
MUCH AS, IF NOT perience an unreal environment as being
fully real. This demonstration alone opens
dating the network’s input instead of its MORE THAN, FROM new frontiers for VR research: we can test
output, the network effectively projects
what it “thinks” is there onto and into the
THE OUTSIDE IN. the limits of what people will experience,
and believe, to be real. It also allows us to
image. Its predictions overwhelm the sen- investigate how experiencing things as be-
sory inputs, tipping the balance of perceptual best guessing to- ing real can affect other aspects of perception. Right now we are
ward these predictions. Our particular network was good at clas- running an experiment to find out whether people are worse at
sifying different breeds of dogs, so the video became unusually detecting unexpected changes in the room when they believe that
suffused by dog presences. what they are experiencing is real. If things do turn out this way
Many people who have viewed the processed footage through (the study is still ongoing, despite being heavily delayed by a glob-
the VR headset have commented that the experience is rather al pandemic), that finding would support the idea that the percep-
reminiscent not of the hallucinations of psychosis but of the ex- tion of things as being real itself acts as a high-level prior that can
uberant phenomenology of psychedelic trips. substantively shape our perceptual best guesses, affecting the con-
More recently, we have been implementing the hallucination tents of what we perceive.
machine in different ways to simulate different kinds of altered
visual experience. By extending our algorithm to include two cou- THE REALITY OF REALITY
pled neural networks—a “discriminator network” much like the The idea that the world of our experience might not be real
one in our original study and a “generator” network that has been is an enduring trope of philosophy and science fiction, as well as
trained to reproduce (“generate”) its input image—we have been of late-night pub discussions. Neo in T he Matrix takes the red
able to model different types of hallucination. For example, we pill, and Morpheus shows him how what he thought was real is
have modeled the complex hallucinatory experiences reported an elaborate simulation, while the real Neo lies prone in a human
by people with Parkinson’s disease and some forms of dementia; body farm, a brain-in-a-vat power source for a dystopian AI. Phi-
the patterned, geometric hallucinations that occur after the loss losopher Nick Bostrom of the University of Oxford has famously
of foveal vision, as happens in Charles Bonnet syndrome; and a argued, based largely on statistics, that we are likely to be living
range of psychedeliclike hallucinations. We hope that by under- inside a computer simulation created in a posthuman age. I dis-
standing hallucinations better, we will be able to understand nor- agree with this argument in part because it assumes that con-
mal experience better, too, because predictive perception is at the sciousness can be simulated—I do not think that this is a safe as-
root of all our perceptual experience. sumption—but it is thought-provoking nonetheless.
Although these chunky metaphysical topics are fun to chew
THE PERCEPTION OF REALITY on, they are probably impossible to resolve. Instead what we have
Although the hallucination machine is undoubtedly trippy, peo- been exploring throughout this article is the relation between ap-
ple who experience it are fully aware that what they are experienc- pearance and reality in our conscious perceptions, where part of
ing is not real. Indeed, despite rapid advances in VR technology and this appearance is the appearance of being real itself.
computer graphics, no current VR setup delivers an experience that The central idea here is that perception is a process of active in-
is sufficiently convincing to be indistinguishable from reality. terpretation geared toward adaptive interaction with the world
This is the challenge we took up when designing a new “sub- through the body rather than a re-creation of the world within the
stitutional reality” setup at Sussex—the one we were working on mind. The contents of our perceptual worlds are controlled hallu-
when Pope Francis convened the retreat with Salva Kiir and Riek cinations, brain-based best guesses about the ultimately unknow-
Machar. Our aim was to create a system in which volunteers able causes of sensory signals. For most of us, most of the time, these
would experience an environment as being real—and believe it controlled hallucinations are experienced as real. As Canadian rap-
to be real—when in fact it was not real. per and science communicator Baba Brinkman suggested to me,
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 43
why we
have
free
wıll
Neurons fire in your head before you become aware
that you have made a decision. But this discovery
does not mean you are a “biochemical puppet”
By Eddy Nahmias
Illustration by Simon Prades
O
ne fall night I lay awake wondering how I should
begin this essay. I imagined a variety of ways I could write
the first sentence and the next and the one after that.
Then I thought about how I could tie those sentences to
the following paragraph and the rest of the article. The
pros and cons of each of those options circled back and
forth in my head, keeping me from drifting off to sleep.
As this was happening, neurons were buzzing away in my brain. Indeed, that
neural activity explains why I imagined these options, and it explains why I am
writing these very words. It also explains why I have free will.
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 45
Increasingly, neuroscientists, psychologists and NOT SO FAST
pundits say that I am wrong. Invoking a number of I call those who contend that science shows that
widely cited neuroscientific studies, they claim that free will is an illusion “willusionists.” There are many
unconscious processes drove me to select the words I reasons to be wary of the willusionists’ arguments. First,
ultimately wrote. Their arguments suggest our con- neuroscience currently lacks the technical sophistica-
scious deliberation and decisions happen only after tion to determine whether neural activity underlying
neural gears below the level of our conscious aware- our imagining and evaluating of future options has any
ness have already determined what we will choose. And impact on which option we then carry out minutes,
they conclude that because “our brains make us do it”— hours or days later. Instead the research discussed by
choosing for us one option over another—free will is willusionists fails to clearly define the border between
nothing more than an illusion. conscious and unconscious actions.
The experiments most often cited to show that our Consider the Libet experiment. It began with study
brains take charge behind the scenes were carried out participants preparing consciously to make a series of
by the late Benjamin Libet in the 1980s at the Univer- repetitive and unplanned actions. When the experiment
sity of California, San Francisco. There he instructed began, they flexed their wrists when a desire arose spon-
study participants outfitted with electrodes on their taneously. The neural activity involved in the conscious
heads to flick their wrists whenever they felt like it. The planning presumably influenced the later unconscious
electrodes detected fluctuations in electrical activity initiation of movements, revealing an interaction
called readiness potentials that occurred about half a between conscious and unconscious brain activity.
second before people made the flicking motion. But Similarly, the 2011 Haynes study, in which people
participants became aware of their intentions to move randomly picked whether to add or subtract over the
only about a quarter of a second before the movement, course of many trials, fails to provide convincing evi-
leading to the conclusion that their brains had decid- dence against free will. Early brain activity that occurred
ed before they became aware of what had happened. four seconds before participants were aware of making
In essence, unconscious brain processes were in the a choice may be an indication of unconscious biases
driver’s seat. toward one choice or the other.
More recent studies using functional MRI have sug- But this early brain activity predicted a choice with
gested the unconscious roots of our decisions begin an accuracy only 10 percent better than random chance.
even earlier. In research published in 2013, neurosci- Brain activity cannot, in general, settle our choices four
entist John-Dylan Haynes of the Bernstein Center for seconds before we act, because we can react to changes
Computational Neuroscience Berlin and his colleagues in our situation in less time than that. If we could not,
had volunteers decide whether to add or subtract two we would all have died in car crashes by now! Uncon-
numbers while in the fMRI scanner. They found pat- scious neural activity, however, can prepare us to take
terns of neural activity that were predictive of wheth- an action by cuing us to consciously monitor our actions
er subjects would choose to add or subtract that to let us adjust our behavior as it occurs.
occurred four seconds before those subjects were aware Willusionists also point to psychological research
of making the choice. showing that we have less conscious control over our
These studies—and others like them—have led to actions than we think. It is true that we are often influ-
sweeping pronouncements that free will is dead. “Our enced unknowingly by subtle features of our environ-
decisions are predetermined unconsciously a long time ment and by emotional or cognitive biases. Until we
before our consciousness kicks in,” Haynes comment- understand them, we are not free to try to counteract
ed to N
ew Scientist, a dding that “it seems that the brain them. This is one reason I think we have less free will
is making the decision before the person.” Others share than many people tend to believe. But there is a big dif-
his opinion. Evolutionary biologist Jerry Coyne has ference between less and none at all.
written: “So it is with all of our ... choices: not one of The Libet and Haynes research deals with choices
them results from a free and conscious decision on our that people make without conscious deliberation at the
part. There is no freedom of choice, no free will.” Neu- time of action. Everyone performs repetitive or habitu-
roscientist Sam Harris has concluded from these find- al behaviors, sometimes quite sophisticated ones that
ings that we are “biochemical puppets”: “If we were to do not require much thought because the behaviors have
detect [people’s] conscious choices on a brain scanner been learned. You put your key in the lock. A shortstop
seconds before they were aware of them ... this would dives for a ground ball. A pianist becomes immersed in
directly challenge their status as conscious agents in playing Beethoven’s M oonlight Sonata.
control of their inner lives.” The reflexive turning of the key, the lunging for the
But does the research really show that all our con- ball, or the depressing of the white and black keys requires
scious deliberation and planning is just a by-product a particular type of mental processing. What I was doing
of unconscious brain activity, having no effect on what on that sleepless night—conscious consideration of alter-
we do later on? No, it does not. There are several rea- native options—is a wholly different activity from engag-
sons to think that those who insist that free will is a ing in practiced routines. A body of psychological research
mirage are misguided. shows that conscious, purposeful processing of our
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 47
The Brain
Learns in
Unexpected
Ways
Neuroscientists Our concepts of how the two and a half pounds
of flabby flesh between our ears accomplish learning date to
have discovered Ivan Pavlov’s classic experiments, where he found that dogs
a set of unfamiliar could learn to salivate at the sound of a bell. In 1949 psycholo-
gist Donald Hebb adapted Pavlov’s “associative learning rule” to
cellular mechanisms explain how brain cells might acquire knowledge. Hebb pro-
posed that when two neurons fire together, sending off impulses
for making simultaneously, the connections between them—the synapses—
grow stronger. When this happens, learning has taken place. In
fresh memories the dogs’ case, it would mean the brain now knows that the
sound of a bell is followed immediately by the presence of food.
By R. Douglas Fields This idea gave rise to an oft-quoted axiom: “Synapses that fire
together wire together.”
Illustration by Eva Vazquez The theory proved sound, and the molecular details of how
synapses change during learning have been described in depth.
But not everything we remember results from reward or punish-
ment, and in fact, most experiences are forgotten. Even when
synapses do fire together, they sometimes do not wire together.
What we retain depends on our emotional response to an expe-
rience, how novel it is, where and when the event occurred and
our level of attention and motivation during the event, and we
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 49
process these thoughts and feelings while asleep. A narrow fo- surprised to find changes in unexpected parts of the brain, in-
cus on the synapse has given us a mere stick-figure conception cluding regions that have no neurons or synapses—areas known
of how learning and the memories it engenders work. as white matter.
It turns out that strengthening a synapse cannot produce a
memory on its own, except for the most elementary reflexes in DEEP LEARNING
simple circuits. Vast changes throughout the expanse of the Consciousness arises from the cerebral cortex, the three-milli-
brain are necessary to create a coherent memory. Whether you meter-thick outer layer of the human brain, so this gray matter
are recalling last night’s conversation with dinner guests or us- layer is where most researchers expected to find learning-
ing an acquired skill such as riding a bike, the activity of mil- induced modifications. But below the surface layer, billions of
lions of neurons in many different regions of your brain must tightly packed bundles of axons (nerve fibers), much like tightly
become linked to produce a coherent memory that interweaves wound fibers under the leather skin of a baseball, connect neu-
emotions, sights, sounds, smells, event sequences and other rons in the gray matter into circuits.
stored experiences. Because learning encompasses so many ele- These fiber bundles are white because the axons are coated
ments of our experiences, it must incorporate different cellular with a fatty substance called myelin, which acts as electrical in-
mechanisms beyond the changes that occur in synapses. This sulation and boosts the speed of transmission by 50 to 100 times.
recognition has led to a search for new ways to understand how White matter injury and disease are important areas of research,
information is transmitted, processed and stored in the brain to but until recently little attention had been given in these inves-
bring about learning. In the past 10 years neuroscientists have tigations to a possible role of myelin in information processing
come to realize that the iconic “gray matter” that makes up the and learning.
brain’s outer surface—familiar from graphic illustrations found In the past 10 or so years studies have begun to find differ-
everywhere from textbooks to children’s cartoons—is not the ences in white matter in brain scans of experts with a variety of
only part of the organ involved in the inscription of a permanent skills, including people with high proficiency in reading and
record of facts and events for later recall and replay. It turns out arithmetic. Expert golfers and trained jugglers also show differ-
that areas below the deeply folded, gray-colored surface also ences in white matter compared with novices, and white matter
play a pivotal role in learning. In just the past few years a series volume has even been associated with IQ. If information pro-
of studies from my laboratory and others has elucidated these cessing and learning arise from the strengthening of synaptic
processes, which could point to new ways of treating psychiat- connections between neurons in gray matter, why does learning
ric and developmental disorders that occur when learning im- affect the brain’s subsurface cabling?
pairments arise. A possible answer began to emerge from cellular studies in
If synaptic changes alone do not suffice, what does happen my lab investigating how synapses—but also other brain areas—
inside your brain when you learn something new? Magnetic res- change during learning. The reason for looking beyond the syn-
onance imaging methods now enable researchers to see through apse was that most of the drugs we have for treating neurologi-
a person’s skull and examine the brain’s structure. In scrutiniz- cal and psychological disorders work by altering synaptic
ing MRI scans, investigators began to notice differences in the transmission, and there is a pressing need for more effective
brain structure of individuals with specific highly developed agents. The present focus on synaptic transmission might cost
skills. Musicians, for example, have thicker regions of auditory us opportunities for better treatments for dementia, depression,
cortex than nonmusicians. At first, researchers presumed that schizophrenia or post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).
these subtle differences must have predisposed clarinetists and In the early 1990s my lab at the National Institutes of Health
pianists to excel at their given skills. But subsequent research and others began to explore the possibility that glia might be
found that learning changes the structure of the brain. able to sense information flowing through neural networks and
The kind of learning that leads to alterations in brain tissue alter it to improve performance. Experimental evidence that has
is not limited to repetitive sensorimotor skills such as playing accumulated since then shows that all types of glial cells re-
a musical instrument. Neuroscientist Bogdan Draganski, cur- spond to neural activity and can modify information transmis-
rently at the University of Lausanne in Switzerland, and his col- sion in the brain. One of the most surprising of these new find-
leagues witnessed increases in the volume of gray matter in ings involves myelin.
medical students’ brains after they studied for an examination. Myelin insulation is formed by layers of cell membrane
Many different cellular changes could expand gray matter vol- wrapped around axons like electrical tape. In the brain and spinal
ume, including the birth of new neurons and of nonneuronal cord, octopus-shaped glial cells (oligodendrocytes) do the wrap-
cells called glia. Vascular changes and the sprouting and prun- ping. In the limbs and trunk, sausage-shaped glial cells (Schwann
ing of axons and dendrites that extend from the main body of a cells) perform the same task. Many oligodendrocytes grip an axon
neuron could also do the same. Remarkably, physical changes in and wrap layers of myelin around it in segments, like the stacked
the brain can happen much faster during learning than might hands of baseball players gripping a bat to determine which team
be expected. Yaniv Assaf of Tel Aviv University and his colleagues bats first. The tiny gap between two myelin segments exposes a
showed that 16 laps around a race track in a computerized video one-micron section of bare axon where ion channels that generate
game were enough to cause changes in new players’ hippo electrical impulses become concentrated. These spaces, known as
campal brain region. Structural alterations in the hippocampus the nodes of Ranvier, act like bioelectric repeaters to relay an elec-
in these gamers make sense because this brain region is critical trical impulse from node to node down the axon. The speed of im-
for spatial learning for navigation. In other studies, Assaf and, pulse transmission increases as more layers of myelin are wrapped
separately, Heidi Johansen-Berg of the University of Oxford were around the axon, protecting it better against voltage loss. Also, as
Astrocyte
Worker Cells
Insulating sheaths made of fatty, white myelin control the rate
at which electrical signals travel along axons. Cells called Axon
oligodendrocytes loop around and wrap myelin on an axon—
and, in some cases, remove it. Small gaps in myelin (nodes of Outer
Ranvier) contain ion channels that generate electrical impulses. tongue
Another cell type, the perinodal astrocyte, stops the secretion
of the myelin-removing thrombin (not shown). Oligodendrocyte
a node of Ranvier becomes squeezed more tightly by the adjoining How, then, does the transmission speed in every link in the
myelin segments, an electrical impulse is initiated more rapidly human brain get timed appropriately so that an impulse arrives
because it takes less time to charge the smaller amount of nodal just when needed? We know that electrical signals shuffle along
Source: “Treadmilling Model for Plasticity of the Myelin Sheath,” by R. Douglas Fields
membrane to the voltage that triggers ion channels to open and at the pace of a slow walk in some axons but blaze away at the
generate an impulse. speed of a race car in others. Signals from two axons that con-
and Dipankar J. Dutta, in Trends in Neurosciences, Vol. 42, No. 7; July 2019
Disorders that damage myelin, such as multiple sclerosis and verge on neurons that act as relay points will not arrive together
Guillain-Barré syndrome, can cause serious disability because unless the travel time from their input source is optimized to
neural impulse transmission fails when the insulation is dam- compensate for differences in the lengths of the two axons and
aged. But until recently, the idea that myelin might be modified the speed at which impulses travel along each link.
routinely by neural impulses was not widely accepted. And even Because myelin is the most effective means of speeding im-
if myelin structure changed, how and why would this improve pulse transmission, axon myelination promotes optimal infor-
performance and learning? mation transmission through a network. If oligodendrocytes
The explanation was hiding in plain sight. It loops back to sense and respond to the information traffic flowing through
the old maxim about neurons firing and wiring together. In any neural circuits, then myelin formation and the way it adjusts im-
complex information or transportation network, the time of ar- pulse-transmission speed could be controlled by feedback from
rival at network relay points is critical—think of missing a con- the axon. But how can myelinating glia detect neural impulses
nection because your flight arrives too late. flowing through axons?
myelin promotes learning by ensuring that various spiking elec- outer layer withdraws into an oligodendrocyte, thinning the
trical signals traveling along axons arrive at the same time in the sheath. Both widening of the nodal gap and thinning of the my-
motor cortex, the brain region that controls movement. Using ge- elin sheath slow the speed of impulse transmission.
netically modified mice with impaired myelination that had been We found that the enzyme’s snipping of these threads that
trained to pull a lever to receive a reward, we found that learning stitch myelin to the axon can be controlled by the perinodal astro-
this task increased myelination in the motor cortex. cyte’s release of an inhibitor of thrombin. We carried out experi-
By using electrodes to record neural impulses, we found that ments on genetically modified mice in which astrocytes released
action potentials were less synchronized in the motor cortices less of this thrombin inhibitor. When we looked at their neurons
of mice with faulty myelination. We then boosted the synchro- with an electron microscope, we could see that the myelin had
nization of spike arrivals in the motor cortex by using opto thinned and that the nodal gap had increased. By using electronic
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 53
THE
BRAIN’S
SOCIAL
ROAD
MAPS
Neural circuits that track our whereabouts
in space and time may also play vital roles in
determining how we relate to other people
By Matthew Schafer and Daniela Schiller
Illustration by Richard Borge
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 55
W e are of ten told that there are no shortcuts in life.
But the brain—even the brain of a rat—is wired in a way that
completely ignores this kind of advice. The organ, in fact, epito-
mizes a shortcut-finding machine.
The first indication that the brain has a knack for finding
alternative routes was described in 1948 by Edward Tolman of the
University of California, Berkeley. Tolman performed a curious
experiment in which a hungry rat ran across an unpainted circular table into a dark, narrow
c orridor. The rat turned left, then right, and then took another
right and scurried to the far end of a well-lit narrow strip,
where, finally, a cup of food awaited. There were no choices to
be made. The rat had to follow the one available winding path,
and so it did, time and time again, for four days.
On the fifth day, as the rat once again ran straight across the
route most likely to bring it to its goal. Quite simply, it must
have built a model of the environment.
Such model building or mapmaking extends to more than
physical space. Mental maps may exist at the core of many of
our most “human” capacities, including memory, imagination,
inferences, abstract reasoning and even the dynamics of social
table into the corridor, it hit a wall—the path was blocked. The interactions. Researchers have begun to explore whether men-
animal went back to the table and started looking for alterna- tal maps document how close or distant one individual is to
tives. Overnight, the circular table had turned into a sunburst another and where that individual resides in a group’s social
arena. Instead of one track, there were now 18 radial paths to hierarchy. How does the brain, in fact, create the maps that
explore, all branching off from the sides of the table. After ven- allow us to make our way about the world?
turing out a few inches on a few different paths, the rat finally
chose to run all the way down path number six, the one leading A SPATIAL MAP
directly to the food. The first hints o f a neural basis for mental maps came in
Taking the path straight to the food cup without prior expe- the 1970s. While studying a brain region called the hippo
rience may seem trivial, but from the perspective of behavioral campus in rodents, John O’Keefe of University College London,
psychologists at the time, the rat’s navigational accomplish- along with his student Jonathan Dostrovsky, discovered a par-
ment was a remarkable feat. The main school of animal learn- ticular class of neurons that becomes active when mice occupy
ing in that era believed that maze behavior in a rat is a matter of specific locations in their environment. Some of these neurons
simple stimulus-response associations. When stimuli in the fired when the animal was in one location, and others switched
environment reliably produce a successful response, neural on when it moved to the next spot on the path along which it
connections that represent this association get strengthened. traveled, as if the cells were specialized to track w here t he ani-
In this view, the brain operates like a telephone switchboard mal was in space. By linking sequences of these “place cells”
that maintains only reliable connections between incoming calls together, researchers were able to reconstruct an animal’s navi-
from our sense organs and outgoing messages to the muscles. But gational trajectory. Work over the intervening decades con-
the behavioral switchboard was unable to explain the ability to firmed the existence of place cells in other animals, including
correctly choose a shortcut right off the bat without having first humans, and clarified many of their properties. Along the way,
experienced that specific path. Shortcuts and many other intrigu- a host of cell types surfaced, each uniquely contributing to the
ing observations along these lines lent support to a rival school of brain’s encoding of spatial representations.
thought promulgated by theorists who believe that in the course In the nearby entorhinal cortex, a region connected to the
of learning, a map gets established in a rat’s brain. Tolman—a hippocampus, a research team led by Edvard Moser and May-
proponent of that school—coined the term: the cognitive map. Britt Moser, former postdoctoral visiting fellows in O’Keefe’s
According to Tolman, the brain does more than just learn laboratory, discovered neurons highly similar to place cells.
the direct associations among stimuli. Indeed, such associa- These cells also fired when an animal was in specific locations.
tions are often brittle, rendered outdated by changes in the But unlike place cells, each of these newly discovered cells
environment. As psychologists have learned in the decades spiked in multiple, regular locations. When mapped onto the
since Tolman’s work, the brain also builds, stores and uses men- animal’s position, the activity patterns of these “grid cells”
tal maps. These models of the world enable us to navigate our resembled highly regular, equilateral triangles. Like a spatial
surroundings, despite complex, changing environments— metric, these cells fired when an animal passed over the vertices
affording the flexibility to use shortcuts or detours as needed. of the triangles. The discovery of these cell types sparked excite-
The hungry rat in Tolman’s experiment must have remembered ment because of the emerging picture of how the brain controls
the location of the food, inferred the angle to it and chosen the navigation. Place cells and grid cells could provide a means to
Increasing Abstraction
Physical space Social information in physical space Social space
More
Self
Power
y y
Less
x x Close Affiliation Distant
locate oneself in space and determine distance and direction. Shaping plans also occurs during sleep. Sequences of place
These navigational tools are crucial for building mental maps. cell activity can be reactivated during sleep to replay the past or
(O’Keefe and the Mosers received the 2014 Nobel Prize in Medi- simulate the future. Without the ability to simulate new behav-
cine or Physiology for their work on place and grid cells.) iors, we would have to explore a multitude of real-world options
A wide variety of information is useful for creating such a before deciding on what action to take. We would be constant
map, and the hippocampus-entorhinal system encodes much of empiricists, able to act only on direct observations. Instead off-
it. Discovering the location of a physical goal is one example: as line simulations give us the ability to envision possibilities
an animal navigates toward an objective, some hippocampal neu- without directly experiencing them.
rons fire depending on the direction and distance to reach it. The
cells increase their firing rate as the animal approaches the goal. MENTAL TIME TRAVEL
Other cells also enter the picture. A dedicated population of T ime and space a re inextricably linked. It is difficult to talk
“reward” cells encodes reward locations across different envi- about time without borrowing a spatial metaphor: time “passes”
ronments, providing a signal to guide an animal’s navigation as we “move” through it. We look “forward” to the future and
(think of an “X” marking the spot of treasure on a pirate’s map). “back” on our past. The same hippocampal-entorhinal system
Other cells track speed and direction and in doing so act like tracks movement through time. Work done largely in the lab of
internal speedometers and compasses that compute an ani- the late Howard Eichenbaum of Boston University revealed neu-
mal’s progress as it travels through the environment. Specific rons in the hippocampal-entorhinal system that encode the
cells that signal the locations of landmarks in the surroundings time course of an animal’s experience. Time cells fire at succes-
serve as references to correct errors in the animal’s trajectory. A sive moments but do not track time in a simple clocklike fash-
ol. 100; October 24, 2018
map must also have edges: cells that fire more as the animal ion. Instead they mark temporal context—stretching or shrink-
approaches the map’s perimeter. ing their firing durations if the length of a task changes, for
For humans, the importance of such an abundance of cell types example. Some time cells encode space as well. In the brain, in
seems obvious: the brain is responsible for knowing the location fact, physical and temporal space may be bound together.
Source: “Navigating Social Space,” by Matthew Schafer and Daniela Schiller, in N euron, V
of home and work, walls and dead ends, a favorite shop or the cor- The discovery of the crucial importance of these brain areas
ner store. It is still a mystery as to how all of this information is in space and time was not totally surprising. Psychologists had
drawn together into a coherent map, but these cells appear to pro- long suspected it to be the case. In 1953 Henry Molaison under-
vide the parts list for the elements of neural mapmaking. went bilateral hippocampal resection surgery to reduce ex
This hippocampal-entorhinal system is more than a map- treme, life-disrupting epileptic seizures. The surgery was success-
maker, though, and the maps are more than a way to locate one- ful at quelling the seizures. But Molaison—known for decades
self in space. These maps also are used for active planning. When only as H.M.—became one of the most renowned cases in the
a rat comes to a junction in a familiar maze, it will pause while history of the brain sciences.
place cell firing sequences that relate to the different options are Molaison could remember most experiences from before
activated, as if the animal is contemplating the choices. his surgery—people he knew and recollections from culture
Humans engage similar processes. Research in participants and politics. But his ability to form such explicit memories
navigating virtual environments while their brains were postsurgery was practically nonexistent. Even so, certain types
scanned with functional magnetic resonance imaging shows of learning and memory remained untouched: he could still
that the hippocampus becomes active in ways consistent with learn some new skills with enough practice. But his recollec-
spatial planning, such as considering and planning routes. tions of new people, facts and events were immediately lost.
Self
Power
Less
x Close Distant
Affiliation
From observing Molaison, neuroscientists discerned that decision-making. Mapping allows relations to be inferred, even
the hippocampus was essential in forming the episodic memo- when they have not been experienced. It also allows for mental
ries that record facts and events. Research on the role of the shortcuts that go beyond the purview of the spatial and tempo-
Sources: “Scientific Background: The Brain’s Navigational Place and Grid Cell System,” by Ole Kiehn and Hans Forssberg, with illustrations by Mattias Karlen.
hippocampus in episodic memory exploded, largely in parallel ral domains. In fact, reasoning using abstract concepts may
to studies on its maplike functions. depend on some of these same neural foundations.
The discoveries about the roles of the hippocampus and ento- In one example of this new line of work, researchers Alexan-
rhinal cortex in spatial navigation and episodic memory were dra Constantinescu, Jill O’Reilly and Timothy Behrens, all then
significant for at least a couple of reasons. The work in spatial at the University of Oxford, asked participants to learn associa-
navigation in rodents marked the first time that a higher-order tions of different symbols with images of “stick” birds with var-
Nobelprize.org; “Navigating Social Space,” by Matthew Schafer and Daniela Schiller, in N euron, V
a central role in the formation and storage of new episodic mem- al association space. Despite neuroimaging being too crude to
ories. These discoveries hinted that mechanisms of spatial and detect actual grid cells in the human brain, imaging conducted
temporal navigation might underlie episodic memory. This syn- during the learned-association testing nonetheless revealed a
thesis is perhaps best explained by the theoretical construct pro- gridlike pattern of activation within the entorhinal cortex.
posed decades earlier by Tolman; both episodic memory and This finding builds on earlier work by Christian Doeller of
spatial navigation might reflect the brain’s formation and use of the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sci-
cognitive maps. ence in Leipzig, Germany, and Neil Burgess of University Col-
Maps are not accurate portraits of the world in all of its com- lege London that first showed an entorhinal gridlike represen-
plexity. Rather they are representations of relations—distances tation in humans navigating a virtual maze. For both physical
and directions between locations and what exists where. Maps and abstract relations, the gridlike organization is highly effi-
reduce a dizzying amount of real-world information into a sim- cient. It makes the linkages of places or concepts more predict-
ple, easily readable format that is useful for effective, flexible able, enhancing how quickly inferences can be made about
navigation. The cell types mentioned earlier (place cells, grid these relations. As in physical space, this organization of infor-
cells and border cells, among others) may piece together such mation allows for inferring shortcuts—relations between ideas
related elements into a mental map, which other brain regions or perhaps analogies, stereotypes and even some aspects of cre-
can then read out to guide “navigation,” amounting to adaptive ativity itself could depend on such inferences.
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 59
FACE
VALUES
Brain regions that process faces reveal deep insights
into the neural mechanisms of vision
By Doris Y. Tsao
Illustration by Brian Stauffer
When I was in high school, I learned one day about the density
of curves in an introductory course on calculus. A simple pair of differ-
ential equations that model the interactions of predators and prey can
give rise to an infinite number of closed curves—picture concentric cir-
cles, one nested within another, like a bull’s-eye. What is more, the den-
sity of these curves varies depending on their location.
This last fact seemed so strange to me. I could easily imagine a finite
set of curves coming close together or pulling apart. But how could an
infinity of curves be denser in one region and less dense in another?
I soon learned that there are different types of infinity with paradoxical
qualities, such as Hilbert’s Hotel (where the rooms are always fully
booked but new guests can always be accommodated) and the Banach-
Tarski apple (which can be split into five pieces and rearranged to make
two apples with the same volume as the original). I spent hours poring
over these mathematical proofs. Ultimately they struck me as symbolic
magic of no real consequence, but the seed of interest had taken root.
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 61
Later, as an undergraduate at the California Institute to identify areas activated by the perception of three-
of Technology, I learned about the experiments of Da- dimensionality in images. I decided to show pictures of
vid Hubel and Torsten Wiesel and their landmark dis- faces and other objects to a monkey. When I compared
covery of how a region in the brain called the primary activation in the monkey’s brain in response to faces
visual cortex extracts edges from the images relayed with activation for other objects, I found several areas
from the eyes. I realized that what had mystified me back that lit up selectively for faces in the temporal lobe (the
in high school was the act of trying to i magine different area underneath the temple)—specifically in a region
densities of infinity. Unlike the mathematical tricks I called the inferotemporal (IT) cortex. Charles Gross, a
had studied in high school, the edges that Hubel and pioneer in the field of object vision, had discovered face-
Wiesel described are processed by neurons, so they ac- selective neurons in the IT cortex of macaques in the
tually exist in the brain. I came to recognize that visual early 1970s. But he had reported that these cells were
neuroscience was a way to understand how this neural randomly scattered throughout the IT cortex. Our fMRI
activity gives rise to the conscious perception of a curve. results provided the first indication that face cells might
The sense of excitement this realization triggered is be concentrated in defined regions.
hard to describe. I believe at each stage in life one has a
duty. And the duty of a college student is to dream, to FACE PATCHES
find the thing that captures one’s heart and seems worth A f ter publ ish ing my work , I was invited to give a
devoting a whole life to. Indeed, this is the single most talk describing the fMRI study as a candidate for a fac-
important step in science—to find the right problem. I ulty position at Caltech, but I was not offered the job.
was captivated by the challenge of understanding vision Many people were skeptical of the value of fMRI, which
and embarked on a quest to learn how patterns of elec- measures local blood flow, the brain’s plumbing. They
trical activity in the brain are able to encode perceptions argued that showing increased blood flow to a brain
area when a subject is looking at faces falls
far short of clarifying what neurons in the
FACE PATCHES DO ACT AS AN ASSEMBLY LINE area are actually encoding because the rela-
tion between blood flow and electrical activ-
TO SOLVE ONE OF THE BIG CHALLENGES OF VISION: ity is unclear. Perhaps by chance these face
HOW TO RECOGNIZE THINGS AROUND US patches simply contained a slightly larger
number of neurons responsive to faces, like
DESPITE CHANGES IN THE WAY THEY LOOK. icebergs randomly clustered at sea.
Because I had done the imaging experi-
ment in a monkey, I could directly address
of visual objects—not just lines and curves but even ob- this concern by inserting an electrode into an fMRI-iden-
jects as hard to define as faces. Accomplishing this ob- tified face area and asking, What images drive single
jective required pinpointing the specific brain regions neurons in this region most strongly? I performed this
dedicated to facial recognition and deciphering their un- experiment together with Winrich Freiwald, then a post-
derlying neural code—the means by which a pattern of doctoral fellow in Margaret Livingstone’s laboratory at
electrical impulses allows us to identify people around us. Harvard, where I was a graduate student. We presented
The journey of discovery began in graduate school at faces and other objects to a monkey while amplifying
Harvard University, where I studied stereopsis, the the electrical activity of individual neurons recorded by
mechanism by which depth perception arises from dif- the electrode. To monitor responses in real time, we con-
ferences between the images in the two eyes. One day I verted the neurons’ electrical signals to an audio signal
came across a paper by neuroscientist Nancy Kanwish- that we could hear with a loudspeaker in the lab.
er, now at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, This experiment revealed an astonishing result: al-
and her colleagues, reporting the discovery of an area most every single cell in the area identified through
in the human brain that responded much more strong- fMRI was dedicated to processing faces. I can recall the
ly to pictures of faces than to images of any other object excitement of our first recording, hearing the “pop” of
when a person was inside a functional magnetic reso- cell after cell responding strongly to faces and very lit-
nance imaging (fMRI) brain scanner. The paper seemed tle to other objects. We sensed we were on to something
bizarre. I was used to the brain being made of parts with important, a piece of cortex that could reveal the brain’s
names like “basal ganglia” and “orbitofrontal cortex” high-level code for visual objects. Marge remarked on
that had some vague purpose one could only begin to the face patches: “You’ve found a golden egg.”
fathom. The concept of an area specifically devoted to I also remember feeling surprised during that first
processing faces seemed all too comprehensible and experiment. I had expected the face area would contain
therefore impossible. Anyone could make a reasonable cells that responded selectively to specific individuals,
conjecture about the function of a face area—it should analogous to orientation-selective cells in the primary
probably represent all the different faces that we know visual cortex that each respond to a specific edge orien-
and something about their expression and gender. tation. In fact, a number of well-publicized studies had
As a graduate student, I had used fMRI on monkeys suggested that single neurons can be remarkably selec-
slanted up or down.
Once again, I was invited to give a job talk at Caltech. termine which c ontrast relations can be used to recog-
Returning, I had more to offer than just fMRI images. nize a face: they should be the ones that are immune to
With the addition of the new results from single-cell re- changes in lighting. For example, “left eye darker than
cordings, it was clear to everyone that these face patch- nose” is a useful feature for detecting a face because it
es were real and likely played an important role in fa- does not matter if a face is photographed with lighting
cial recognition. Furthermore, understanding their un- from above, left, right or below: the left eye is always
derlying neural processes seemed like an effective way darker than the nose (check for yourself ).
to gain traction on the general problem of how the brain From a theoretical standpoint, this idea provides a
represents visual objects. This time I was offered the job. simple, elegant computational mechanism for facial
recognition, and we wondered whether face cells might
CONTRAST IS KEY be using it. When we measured the response of cells to
At Caltech, m y colleagues and I dug deeper into the faces in which different regions varied in brightness, we
question of how these cells detect faces. We took inspi- found that cells often had a significant preference for a
ration from a paper by Pawan Sinha, a vision and com- particular contrast feature in an image.
putational neuroscientist at M.I.T., that suggested fac- To our astonishment, almost all the cells were whol-
es could be discerned on the basis of specific contrast ly consistent in their contrast preferences—just a sin-
relations between different regions of the face—wheth- gle cell was found that preferred the opposite polarity.
er the forehead region is brighter than the mouth re- Moreover, the preferred features were precisely those
gion, for example. Sinha suggested a clever way to de- identified by Sinha as being invulnerable to lighting
From “The Code for Facial Identity in the Primate Brain,” by Le Chang and Doris Y. Tsao, in Cell, Vol. 169, No. 6; June 1, 2017 (f ace images)
three regions: the middle lateral and
middle fundus patches (ML/MF),
Examples of variability the anterior lateral patch (AL) and
the anterior medial patch (AM).
We found striking differences
among these three regions. In ML/
changes. The experiment thus confirmed that face cells MF, cells responded selectively to specific views. For ex-
use contrast relations to detect faces. ample, one cell might prefer faces looking straight
More broadly, the result confirmed that these cells ahead, whereas another might opt for faces looking to
truly were face cells. At talks, skeptics would ask, How the left. In AL, cells were less view-specific. One class of
do you know? You can’t test every possible stimulus. cells responded to faces looking up, down and straight
How can you be sure it’s a face cell and not a pomegran- ahead; another responded to faces looking to the left or
ate cell or a lawn mower cell? This result nailed it for right. In AM, cells responded to specific individuals re-
me. The precise match between the way cells reacted to gardless of whether the view of the face was frontal or
changes in contrast between different parts of the face in profile. Thus, at the end of the network in AM, view-
and Sinha’s computational prediction was uncanny. specific representations were successfully stitched into
Our initial experiments had revealed two nearby cor- a view-invariant one.
tical patches that lit up for faces. But after further scan- Apparently face patches do act as an assembly line
ning (with the help of a contrast agent that increased to solve one of the big challenges of vision: how to rec-
severalfold the robustness of the signal), it became clear ognize things around us despite changes in the way
that there are in fact six face patches in each of the they look. A car can have any make and color, appear
brain’s two hemispheres (making a dozen golden eggs at any viewing angle and distance, and be partially ob-
total). They are distributed along the entire length of the scured by closer objects such as trees or other cars. Rec-
temporal lobe. These six patches, moreover, are not ran- ognizing an object despite these visual transformations
domly scattered throughout the IT cortex. They are lo- is called the invariance problem, and it became clear
cated in similar locations across hemispheres in each to us that a major function of the face-patch network
64 | SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN | SPECIAL EDITION | WINTER 2022 Graphics by Jen Christiansen
is to overcome this impediment.
Given the great sensitivityo
cells in face patches to changes in
f
The Face Code, at Last
facial identity, one might expect Having 50 coordinates that describe shape and appearance allows for a description of neurons’
that altering these cells’ respons- firing in response to a particular face—a description that functions as a code that can be visual-
es should modify an animal’s per- ized geometrically. In this code, each face cell receives inputs for a face in the form of the 50
ception of facial identity. Neuro- coordinates, or dimensions. The neuron then fires with a particular intensity in response to a
scientists Josef Parvizi and Kala- certain face (red outlines), along what is called the preferred axis. The intensity increases steadi-
nit Grill-Spector of Stanford ly (monotonically) along the preferred axis. Furthermore, the response is the same for every
University had electrically stimu- face on an axis at right angles to the preferred axis, even though those faces may look very
lated a face-patch area in human different. This axis model of facial coding differs from a previous exemplar model that suggests
subjects who had electrodes im- that each neuron fires with maximum intensity to a single most preferred face.
planted in their brains for the
purpose of identifying the source Axis Exemplar
Orthogonal axis
of epileptic seizures and found model model
that stimulation distorted the Preferred axis (new) (old)
subjects’ perception of a face.
We wondered whether we Spike in
would find the same effect in nerve activity
monkeys when we stimulated
their face patches. Would doing
so alter the perception only of
faces, or would it affect that of
other objects as well? The bound-
ary between a face and a nonface
object is fluid—one can see a face
in a cloud or an electrical outlet
if prompted. We wanted to use
electrical microstimulation as a
tool to delineate precisely what
constitutes a face for a face patch.
We trained monkeys to report
whether two sequentially pre-
sented faces were the same or dif-
From “The Code for Facial Identity in the Primate Brain,” by Le Chang and Doris Y. Tsao, in Cell, Vol. 169, No. 6; June 1, 2017 (f ace grid)
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 65
insight about why this is important.
A WIN-WIN BET
Corresponding Reconstructed Faces Based on Neuron Activity At a meeting in Ascona, Switzer-
land, I presented our findings on how
we could reconstruct faces using neu-
ral activity. After my talk, Rodrigo
the shape of the face. From these varied shapes, we cal- Quian Quiroga, who discovered the famous Jennifer Ani
culated an average face. We then morphed each face im- ston cell in the human medial temporal lobe in 2005 and
age in the database so its key features exactly matched is now at the University of Leicester in England, asked
those of the average face. The resulting images consti- me how my cells related to his concept that single neu-
tuted the appearance of the faces independent of shape. rons react to the faces of specific people. The Jennifer An
We then performed principal components analysis iston cell, also known as a grandmother cell, is a putative
independently on the shape and appearance descrip- type of neuron that switches on in response to the face of
tors across the entire set of faces. This is a mathemati- a recognizable person—a celebrity or a close relative.
cal technique that finds the dimensions that vary the I told Rodrigo I thought our cells could be the build-
most in a complex data set. ing blocks for his cells, without thinking very deeply
By taking the top 25 principal components for shape about how this would work. That night, sleepless from
and the top 25 for appearance, we created a 50-dimen- jet lag, I recognized a major difference between our face
sional face space. This space is similar to our familiar cells and his. I had described in my talk how our face cells
3-D space, but each point represents a face rather than computed their response to weighted sums of different
a spatial location, and it comprises much more than face features. In the middle of the night, I realized this
just three dimensions. For 3-D space, any point can be computation is the same as a mathematical operation
described by three coordinates (x,y,z). For a 50-D face known as the dot product, whose geometric representa-
space, any point can be described by 50 coordinates. tion is the projection of a vector onto an axis (like the sun
In our experiment, we randomly drew 2,000 faces and projecting the shadow of a flagpole onto the ground).
Doris Y. Tsao (f ace images)
presented them to a monkey while recording cells from Remembering my high school linear algebra, I real-
two face patches. We found that almost every cell showed ized this implied that we should be able to construct
graded responses—resembling a ramp slanting up or a large “null space” of faces for each cell—a series of
down—to a subset of the 50 features, consistent with my faces of varying identity that lie on an axis perpendic-
earlier experiments with cartoon faces. But we had a new ular to the axis of projection. Moreover, all these faces
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 67
RADICAL
CHANGE Uncertainty in the world threatens our sense of self.
To cope, people embrace populism
By Michael A. Hogg
Illustration by Wesley Allsbrook
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 69
ferent from them, they seek homogeneity
and become intoxicated by the freedom to
access only information that confirms
who they are or who they would like to be.
As a result, global populism is on the rise.
has become—a real problem for society. People are sup- S o m e f e at u r e s of groups and social identities are
porting and enabling authoritarian leaders, flocking to especially well suited to reducing self-uncertainty.
ideologies and worldviews that promote and celebrate Most important, groups need to be polarized from
the myth of a glorious past. Fearful of those who are dif- other groups and have unambiguous boundaries that
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 71
Unconscious
Mind
The Intention Machine page 74
Answering Queries
in Real Time while Dreaming page 94
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 75
I g e t g o o s e b u m p s e v e ry t im e I s e e i t. A pa ra ly z e d v o lu n t e e r s i t s i n
a wheelchair while controlling a computer or robotic limb just with his or her
thoughts—a demonstration of a brain-machine interface (BMI) in action.
That happened in my laboratory in 2013, when Erik Sorto, a victim of a gunshot
wound when he was 21 years old, used his thoughts alone to drink a beer without
help for the first time in more than 10 years. The BMI sent a neural message from a
high-level cortical area. An electromechanical appendage was then able to reach out
and grasp the bottle, raising it to Sorto’s lips before he took a sip. His drink came a year after
surgery to implant electrodes in his brain to control signals that govern the thoughts that trig-
ger motor movement. My lab colleagues and I watched in wonderment as he completed this
deceptively simple task that is, in reality, intricately complex.
Witnessing such a feat immediately raises the question of
how mere thoughts can control a mechanical prosthesis. We
move our limbs unthinkingly every day—and completing these
motions with ease is the goal of any sophisticated BMI. Neuro-
scientists, though, have tried for decades to decode neural sig-
age activity over centimeters of brain tissue, capturing the
activity of many millions of neurons rather than that from indi-
vidual neurons in a single circuit. Functional magnetic reso-
nance imaging (fMRI) is an indirect measurement that records
an increase in blood flow to an active region. It can image
nals that initiate movements to reach out and grab objects. Lim- smaller areas than EEG, but its resolution is still rather low.
ited success in reading these signals has spurred a search for new Changes in blood flow are slow, so fMRI cannot distinguish
ways to tap into the cacophony of electrical activity resonating as rapid changes in brain activity.
the brain’s 86 billion neurons communicate. A new generation of To overcome these limitations, ideally one would like to
BMIs now holds the promise of creating a seamless tie between record the activity of individual neurons. Observing changes in
brain and prosthesis by tapping with great precision into the the firing rate of large numbers of single neurons can provide
neural regions that formulate actions—whether the desired goal the most complete picture of what is happening in a specific
is grasping a cup or taking a step. brain region. In recent years arrays of tiny electrodes implanted
in the brain have begun to make this type of recording possible.
FROM BRAIN TO ROBOT The arrays now in use are four-by-four-millimeter flat surfaces
A B M I o p e r at e s by sending and receiving—“writing” and with 100 electrodes. Each electrode, measuring one to 1.5 milli-
“reading”—messages to and from the brain. There are two major meters long, sticks out of the flat surface. The entire array,
classes of the interface technology. A “write-in” BMI generally which resembles a bed of nails, can record activity from 100 to
uses electrical stimulation to transmit a signal to neural tissue. 200 neurons.
Successful clinical applications of this technology are already The signals recorded by these electrodes move to “decoders”
in use. The cochlear prosthesis stimulates the auditory nerve to that use mathematical algorithms to translate varied patterns
enable deaf subjects to hear. Deep-brain stimulation of an area of single-neuron firing into a signal that initiates a particular
that controls motor activity, the basal ganglia, treats motor dis- movement, such as control of a robotic limb or a computer.
orders such as Parkinson’s disease and essential tremor. These read-out BMIs will assist patients who have sustained
Devices that stimulate the retina are currently in clinical trials brain injury because of spinal cord lesions, stroke, multiple
to alleviate certain forms of blindness. sclerosis, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and Duchenne muscu-
“Read-out” BMIs, in contrast, record neural activity and are lar dystrophy.
still at a developmental stage. The unique challenges of reading Our lab has concentrated on people with tetraplegia, who
neural signals need to be addressed before this next-generation are unable to move either their upper or lower limbs because of
technology reaches patients. Coarse read-out techniques al upper spinal cord injuries. We make recordings from the cere-
ready exist. The electroencephalogram (EEG) records the aver- bral cortex, the approximately three-millimeter-thick surface of
object. In contrast, the motor cortex sends a signal for the path professionals from the California Institute of Technology, the
the reaching movement should take. Knowing the goal of an University of Southern California, the University of California,
intended motor action lets the BMI decode it quickly, within a Los Angeles, the Rancho Los Amigos National Rehabilitation
couple of hundred milliseconds, whereas figuring out the trajec- Center, and Casa Colina Hospital and Centers for Healthcare.
tory signal from the motor cortex can take more than a second. The team received a go-ahead from the Food and Drug Admin-
istration and institutional review boards charged with judging
FROM LAB TO PATIENT the safety and ethics of the procedure in the labs, hospitals and
It was not easy t o go from experiments in lab animals to studies rehabilitation clinics involved.
of the PPC in humans. Fifteen years elapsed before we made A volunteer in this type of project is a true pioneer because
the first human implant. First, we inserted the same electrode he or she may or may not benefit. Participants ultimately join
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 77
to help users of the technology who will seek it out once it is
perfected for everyday use. The implant surgery for Sorto, our
first volunteer, took place in April 2013 and was performed by
neurosurgeons Charles Liu and Brian Lee. The procedure
By Thought Alone
went flawlessly, but then came the wait for healing before we For 15 years neuroscientists have built brain-machine inter
could test the device. faces (BMIs) that allow neural signals to move computer
My colleagues at nasa’s Jet Propulsion Laboratory, which cursors or operate prostheses. The technology has moved
built and launched the Mars rovers, talk about the seven min- forward slowly because translating the electrical firing of
utes of terror when a rover enters the planet’s atmosphere neurons into commands to play a video game or move a robot
before it lands. For me it was two weeks of trepidation, wonder- arm involves highly intricate processes.
ing whether the implant would work. We knew in nonhuman A group at the California Institute of Technology has tried
primates how similar areas of the brain functioned, but a to advance the neuroprosthetic field by tapping into high-level
human implant was testing uncharted waters. No one had ever neural processing—the intent to initiate an action—and then
tried to record from a population of human PPC neurons before. conveying the relevant electrical signals to a robotic arm.
During the first day of testing we detected neural activity, Instead of sending out signals from the motor cortex to move an
and by the end of the week there were signals from enough arm, as attempted by other laboratories, the Caltech researchers
neurons to begin to determine if Sorto could control a robot place electrodes in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), which
limb. Some of the neurons varied their activity when Sorto transmits to a prosthesis the brain’s intent to act.
imagined rotating his hand. His first task consisted of turn- Decoding neural signals remains a challenge for neuro
ing the robot hand to different orientations to shake hands scientists. But using BMI signals from the posterior parietal
with a graduate student. He was thrilled, as were we, because cortex, the top of the cognitive command chain, appears to
this accomplishment marked the first time since his injury he result in faster, more versatile control of prosthetic technology.
could interact with the world using the bodily movement of a
robotic arm.
People often ask how long it takes to learn to use a BMI. In
fact, the technology worked right out of the box. It was intui-
tive and easy to use the brain’s intention signals to control the
robotic arm. By imagining different actions, Sorto could
watch recordings of individual neurons from his cortex and
turn them on and off at will.
We ask participants at the beginning of a study what they
would like to achieve by controlling a robot. For Sorto, he
wanted to be able to drink a beer on his own rather than ask-
ing someone else for help. He was able to master this feat
about one year into the study. With the team co-led by re
search scientist Spencer Kellis of Caltech, which included
roboticists from the Applied Physics Laboratory at Johns
Hopkins University, we melded Sorto’s intention signals with
the processing power furnished by machine vision and smart
robotic technology.
The vision algorithm analyzes inputs from video cameras,
and the smart robot combines the intent signal with com-
puter algorithms to initiate the movement of the robot arm.
Sorto achieved this goal after a year’s time with cheers and
shouts of joy from everyone present. In 2015 we published in
Science o ur first results on using intention signals from the
PPC to control neural prostheses.
Sorto is not the only user of our technology. Nancy Smith,
now in her fourth year in the study, became tetraplegic from
an automobile accident about 10 years ago. She had been a
high school teacher of computer graphics and played piano
as a pastime. In our studies with lead team members Tyson
Aflalo of Caltech and Nader Pouratian of U.C.L.A., we found a
detailed representation of the individual digits of both hands
in Smith’s PPC. Using virtual reality, she could imagine and
move 10 fingers individually on left and right “avatar” hands
displayed on a computer screen. Using the imagined move-
ment of five fingers from one hand, Smith could play simple
melodies on a computer-generated piano keyboard.
78 | SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN | SPECIAL EDITION | WINTER 2022 Illustration by AXS Biomedical Animation Studio
The Andersen laboratory at Caltech has pursued development of BMIs that
“read out“ brain signals of an intent to take an action and send them to a robotic
arm that can pick up a glass and allow a tetraplegic patient to drink (1–6). The
BMI provides touch and limb-positioning feedback—“write-in” signals—to the
ARRAY 3 somatosensory cortex that simulates tactile sensations and allows for fine-level
Electrode adjustments to the prosthesis (6–9). The researchers are currently integrating
arrays read out the
intended movements
2 read-out and write-in capabilities to achieve a fully bidirectional BMI.
1
Primary somatosensory
INPUT cortex (hand area)
Signals from sensory and
memory areas of the
cerebral cortex all converge
on the PPC. 9
Primary Episodic memory ARRAY
visual cortex Electrical stimulation in
the somatosensory cortex
produces the sensations
of touch and position from
the robot hand.
4 NEURAL SIGNAL
PROCESSOR
Electronics decode the CONTROL 5 8 STIMULATOR
intention signals quickly COMPUTER The stimulator generates
and formulate commands The commands can be small electric currents
for the robotic arm. coupled with video or to the electrodes of
eye-movement signals
7 CONTROL the stimulation array.
to increase the precision COMPUTER
of the command. Sensors on the robot
fingers and hand
detect position and
touch data, which are
sent to a stimulator.
6
ACTION
The electronically processed brain
signals prod the prosthesis to pick
up a glass, bring it to the lips and hold
it steady, allowing a sip to be taken.
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 79
HOW THE BRAIN REPRESENTS GOALS works, their middle layers developed gain fields, just as was the
W e w e r e t h r i l l e d in working with these patients to find case in the PPC experiments. By mixing signals for visual inputs
neurons tuned to processing signals related to one’s intentions. and eye positions within the same neurons, as few as nine neu-
The amount of information to be gleaned from just a few hun- rons could represent the entire visual field.
dred neurons turned out to be overwhelming. We could decode Recently this idea of mixed representations—populations of
a range of cognitive activity, including mental strategizing neurons responding to multiple variables (as with the gain
(imagined versus attempted motion), finger movements, deci- fields)—has attracted renewed attention. For instance, record-
sions about recalling visual stimuli, hand postures for grasping, ings from the prefrontal cortex show a mixing of two types of
observed actions, action verbs such as “grasp” or “push,” and memory task and different visual objects.
visual and somatosensory perception. To our surprise, inserting This work, moreover, may have a direct bearing in explain-
a few tiny electrode arrays enabled us to decode much of what ing what is happening in the PPC. We discovered this when we
a person intends to do, as well as the sensory inputs that lead to asked Smith, using a set of written instructions, to perform
the formation of intentions. eight different combinations of a task. One of her undertakings
The question of how much information can be recorded from required strategizing to imagine or attempt an action. Another
a small patch of brain tissue reminded me of a similar scientific necessitated using the right and left side of the body; a third
problem that I had encountered early in my career. During my entailed squeezing a hand or shrugging a shoulder. We found
postdoctoral training with the late Vernon Mountcastle at the that PPC neurons mixed all these variables—and the intermin-
Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, we examined how gling exhibited a specific pattern, unlike the random interac-
tions we and others had reported in
lab animal experiments.
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 81
INFECTIOUS
DREAMS
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 83
F
o r m an y o f us, li v i ng in a C OVI D - 1 9 worl d fe e ls as if w e have b e e n
thrown into an alternative reality. We live day and night inside the same walls.
We fear touching groceries that arrive at our doorstep. If we venture into town,
we wear masks, and we get anxious if we pass someone who is not wearing one.
We have trouble discerning faces. It’s like living in a dream.
COVID has altered our dream worlds, too: how much we dream, how many of
our dreams we remember and the nature of our dreams themselves. In early
2020, when stay-at-home directives were put in place widely, society quite unexpectedly expe-
rienced what I am calling a dream surge: a global increase in the reporting of vivid, bizarre
dreams, many of which are concerned with coronavirus and social distancing. Terms such as
coronavirus dreams, lockdown dreams and COVID nightmares emerged on social media. By
April of that year, social and mainstream media outlets had begun broadcasting the message:
the world is dreaming about COVID.
Although widespread changes in dreaming had been reported 12 and older responded, almost all in North America. Results of
in the U.S. following extraordinary events such as the 9/11 attacks these inquiries, published in BMJ Open in December 2020, doc-
in 2001 and the 1989 San Francisco earthquake, a surge of this ument the precipitous surge, the striking variety of dreams and
magnitude had never been documented. This upwelling of dreams many related mental health effects.
is the first to occur globally and the first to happen in the era of Bulkeley’s three-day poll revealed that in March 2020, 29 per-
social media, which makes dreams readily accessible for immedi- cent of Americans recalled more dreams than usual. Solomono-
ate study. As a dream “event,” the pandemic is unprecedented. va and Robillard found that 37 percent of people had pandemic
But what kind of phenomenon is this, exactly? Why was it dreams, many marked by themes of insufficiently completing
happening with such vigor? To find out, Deirdre Barrett, an tasks (such as losing control of a vehicle) and being threatened
assistant professor at Harvard Medical School and editor in by others. Many online posts from the time reflect these findings.
chief of the journal D reaming, i nitiated a COVID dreams survey One person, whose Twitter handle is @monicaluhar, reported,
online in the week of March 22, 2020. Erin and Grace Gravley, “Had a dream about returning as a sub teacher in the fall, unpre-
San Francisco Bay Area artists, launched IDreamofCovid.com, pared. Students were having a difficult time practicing social
a site archiving and illustrating pandemic dreams. The Twitter distancing, and teachers couldn’t stagger classes or have one-on-
account @CovidDreams began operation. Kelly Bulkeley, a psy- one meetings.” And @therealbeecarey said, “My phone had a
chologist of religion and director of the Sleep and Dream Data- virus and was posting so many random pictures from my cam-
base, followed with a YouGov survey of 2,477 American adults. era roll to instagram and my anxiety was at an all time high.”
And my former doctoral student Elizaveta Solomonova, now a More recent studies found qualitative changes in dream
postdoctoral fellow at McGill University, along with Rébecca emotions and concerns about health. Dream reports from Bra-
Robillard of the Royal’s Institute of Mental Health Research in zilian adults in social isolation had high proportions of words
Ottawa and others, launched a survey to which 968 people aged related to anger, sadness, contamination and cleanliness. Text
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 85
This mechanism can break down after severe trauma, how- were circulated widely online, feeding a narrative of pandemic
ever. When this happens, nightmares arise in which the fearful dreams that went viral, influencing people to recall their dreams,
memory is replayed realistically; the creative recombining of notice COVID themes and share them. This narrative may have
memory elements is thwarted. The pandemic’s ultimate impact even induced people to dream more about the pandemic.
on a person’s dreams will vary with whether or how severely Evidence suggests that mainstream media reporting probably
they are traumatized and how resilient they are. did not trigger the surge but may have temporarily amplified its
A second class of theories—also still speculative—may explain scope. The Bulkeley and Solomonova-Robillard polls corroborat-
social distancing themes, which permeated IDreamofCovid.com ed a clear groundswell in dream tweeting during March 2020,
reports. Emotions in these dreams range from surprise to dis- before the first media stories about such dreams appeared; indeed,
comfort to stress to nightmarish horror. Tweets located by the the earliest stories cited various tweet threads as their sources.
@CovidDreams account illustrate how incompatible dream sce- Once stories emerged, more surges in dream reporting
narios are with social distancing—so incompatible that they through early April 2020 were detected by I DreamofCovid.com
often trigger a rare moment of self-awareness and awakening: and @CovidDreams. The format of most early stories almost
“We were celebrating something by having a party. And I woke guaranteed amplification: they typically described some salient
myself up because something wasn’t right because we’re social dream themes observed in a survey and provided a link directing
distancing and not supposed to be having parties.” readers to participate in the same survey. In addition, 56 percent
These theories focus on dreaming’s social simulation function. of articles during the first week of stories featured interviews
The view that dreaming is a neural simulation of reality, analo- with the same Harvard dream scientist, which may have influ-
gous to virtual reality, is now widely accepted, and the notion enced readers to dream about themes repeated by her.
that the simulation of social life is an essential biological function The surge began to decline steadily in late April 2020, as did
is emerging. In 2000 Anne Germain, now CEO of sleep medicine the number of mainstream media articles, suggesting that any
start-up Noctem, and I proposed that images of characters inter- echo-chamber effect had run its course. The final nature of the
acting with the self in dreams could be basic to how dreaming surge remains to be seen. Until C OVID vaccines or treatments
evolved, reflecting attachment relationships essential to the sur- are fully distributed and with waves of future infections or new
vival of prehistoric groups. The strong interpersonal bonds reit- viral variants possible, threats of disease and social distancing
erated during dreaming contribute to stronger group structures are likely to persist. Might the pandemic have produced a lasting
that help to organize defenses against predators and cooperation increase in humanity’s recall of dreams? Could pandemic con-
in problem-solving. Such dreams would still have adaptive value cerns become permanently woven into dream content? And if so,
today because family and group cohesion remain essential to will such alterations help or hinder people’s long-term adjust-
health and survival. It may be the case that an individual’s con- ments to our postpandemic futures?
cerns about other people are fine-tuned while they are in the sim- Therapists may need to step in to help certain people. The
ulated presence of those people. Important social relationships survey information considered in this article does not delve
and conflicts are portrayed realistically during dreaming. into nightmares in detail. But some health-care workers who
Other investigators, such as cognitive neuroscientist Antti saw relentless suffering later themselves suffered with recur-
Revonsuo of the University of Turku in Finland and the Universi- rent nightmares. And some patients who endured the ICU for
ty of Skövde in Sweden, have since proposed additional social days or weeks suffered from horrific nightmares during that
functions for dreaming: facilitating social perception (Who is time, which may in part have been the result of medications
around me?), social mind reading (What are they thinking?) and and sleep deprivation induced by around-the-clock hospital
the practice of social bonding skills. Another theory advanced by procedures and interminable monitor noises and alarms. These
psychology professor Mark Blagrove of Swansea University in survivors will need expert help to regain normal sleep. Thank-
Wales further postulates that by sharing dreams, people enhance fully, specialized techniques are highly effective.
empathy toward others. The range of dream functions is likely to People who are not traumatized but still a little freaked out
keep expanding as we learn more about the brain circuits under- about their C OVID dreams also have options. New technologies
lying social cognition and the roles REM sleep plays in memory such as targeted memory reactivation are providing individuals
for emotional stimuli, human faces and reactions to social exclu- with more control over their dream narratives. For example,
sion. Because social distancing is, in effect, an experiment in learning how to practice lucid dreaming—becoming aware that
social isolation at a level never before seen—and is likely antago- you are dreaming—aided by targeted memory reactivation or
nistic to human evolution—a clash with deep-rooted dream other methods could help transform worrisome pandemic
mechanisms should be evident on a massive scale. And because dreams into more pleasant, maybe even useful, dreams. Simply
social distancing disrupts normal relationships so profoundly— observing and reporting pandemic dreams seems to positively
causing many of us to spend excessive time with some people and impact mental health, as Natália Mota of the Federal University
no time with others—social simulations in dreams may play a of Rio Grande do Norte in Natal, Brazil, found in her studies.
crucial role in helping families, groups, even societies deal with Short of therapy, we can give ourselves permission to ease
sudden, widespread social adaptation. up and to enjoy banking those surplus hours of sleep. Dreams
can be vexing, but they are also impressionable, malleable and
THE ECHO CHAMBER OF SOCIAL MEDIA at times inspirational.
There is one basic question about pandemic dreams that we
would like to nail down: whether the dream surge was amplified Tore Nielsen is a professor of psychiatry at the Université de Montréal and director
by the media. It is quite possible that early posts of a few dreams of the Dream and Nightmare Laboratory there.
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 89
I n Aldous Huxley ’ s Brave New World, a boy memorizes each word of a lecture
in English, a language he does not speak. The learning happens as the boy sleeps within
earshot of a radio broadcast of the lecture. On awakening, he is able to recite the entire
lecture. Based on this discovery, the totalitarian authorities of Huxley’s dystopian world adapt
the method to shape the unconscious minds of all their citizens.
Sleep learning turns up throughout literature, pop
culture and ancient lore. Take Dexter, the lead charac-
ter in the animated television series D exter’s Labora-
tory. In one episode, Dexter squanders his time for
homework, so instead he invents a contraption for
learning to speak French overnight. He wakes up the
next day unable to speak anything but French. The
dling with sleep had begun. In 1927 New Yorker Alois
B. Saliger invented an “Automatic Time-Controlled
Suggestion Machine,” which he marketed as the “Psy-
choPhone,” to allow a recorded message to be replayed
during the night. The setup seemed to evoke Huxley’s
imagined technology except that the user, rather than
the state, could select the message to be played.
idea of sleep learning isn’t just a modern invention. It Saliger’s invention was followed, in the 1930s and
also appears within a centuries-old mind-training 1940s, by studies documenting ostensible examples of
practice of Tibetan Buddhists; a message whispered sleep learning. A 1942 paper by Lawrence LeShan,
during sleep was intended to help a monk recognize then at the College of William & Mary, detailed an
the events in his dreams as illusory. experiment in which the researcher visited a summer
Everyone knows we learn better when we are well camp where many of the boys had the habit of biting
rested. Most people, however, dismiss the notion of their fingernails. In a room where 20 such boys slept,
sleep learning out of hand. Yet a set of new neurosci- LeShan used a portable phonograph to play a voice
entific findings complicates this picture by showing repeating the sentence “My fingernails taste terribly
that a critical part of learning occurs during sleep: bitter.” The string of words recurred 300 times each
recently formed memories resurface during the night, night, beginning 150 minutes after the onset of sleep.
and this playback can help reinforce them, allowing at The experiment continued for 54 consecutive nights.
least a few to be remembered for a lifetime. During the last two weeks of camp, the phonograph
Some studies have even explored whether sleep broke, so the intrepid LeShan delivered the sentence
might be manipulated to enhance learning. They himself. Eight of the 20 boys stopped biting their nails,
reveal that sleep’s program for making daytime mem- whereas none of 20 others who slept without exposure
ories stronger can be boosted using sounds and odors. to the recording did so. These early efforts did not use
Results in rodents have even demonstrated a primi- physiological monitoring to verify that the boys were
tive form of memory implantation: using electrical really asleep, though, so the results remain suspect.
stimulation while animals slept, researchers taught The whole field took a severe hit in 1956, when two
them where they should go in their enclosures on scientists at RAND Corporation used electroencepha-
awakening. Huxley’s imagined version of sleep educa- lography (EEG) to record brain activity while 96 ques-
tion, in which entire texts are absorbed verbatim dur- tions and answers were read to sleeping study partici-
ing the night, is still relegated to the pages of his 1932 pants. (One example: “In what kind of store did Ulys
classic. But experiments now indicate that it is possi- ses S. Grant work before the war?” Answer: “A hardware
ble to tinker with memories while a person is im store.”) The next day correct answers were recalled only
mersed in the depths of slumber, creating the basis for for information presented when sleepers showed signs
a new science of sleep learning. of awakening. These results led to a shift in the field
that persisted for 50 years, as researchers began to lose
THE PSYCHOPHONE faith in sleep learning as a viable phenomenon: partic-
For these techniques t o work, scientists have to ipants in these experiments appeared to learn only if
explore how information can be absorbed when con- they were not really sleeping when information was
sciousness is seemingly on a well-deserved break. presented to them.
Around the time that Huxley was writing B rave New Most scientists during this time tended to avoid the
World, serious explorations into the possibility of med- topic of sleep learning, although a few researchers did
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 91
The Maestros of Slumber
A complex symphony of neural activity governs the connection between sleep and memory
Brain rhythms provide clues t o how sleep helps to store memories ing of connections among neurons to strengthen memory stor-
for later retrieval. One type of neural signal, called a slow wave, age. A dialogue between the hippocampus and the cortex involv-
cycling from 0.5 to four times a second, orchestrates the activity ing all these brain rhythms triggers a set of complex network
of neurons in the cerebral cortex. Each slow oscillation consists interactions. Through this process, known as consolidation, new
of a “down” phase, when neurons are silent, and an “up” phase, information can become integrated with existing memories. The
when they resume activity. This timing pattern helps to reinforce intertwining of memories, moreover, enables the gist of recent
recently formed memories by ensuring that multiple cortical experiences to be extracted to make sense of a complex world.
regions remain in an up state at the same time. Memory difficulties can arise when this neural dialogue be
The up phase can coincide with sleep spindles, brief increases comes impaired. Individuals with major damage centered in the
of a rhythm of 12 to 15 cycles per second. Spindles originate in hippocampus or parts of the thalamus may develop a profound
the thalamus, which serves as a crossroads for information that amnesia. Without the expected interactions with these brain
is transmitted to virtually all parts of the cerebral cortex. Spindles regions during both sleep and waking, the cortex cannot store
have a rhythm of their own, recurring at approximately five-sec- mental records of facts and events known as declarative memo-
ond intervals. They coordinate the activity of sharp-wave ripples ries. In addition, a milder form of memory disorder may result
in the hippocampus. Ripples, for their part, are concurrent with when memory processing during sleep is seriously disrupted.
the replay of memories. Slow waves, all the while, assume the role Deciphering the physiological orchestration of the sleeping
of orchestra conductor: their measured oscillations in the cortex brain is prompting various new strategies for enhancing the brain’s
coordinate the pacing for sleep spindles and sharp-wave ripples. natural rhythms—including stimulation with slow electrical oscil-
The intricate coupling of these oscilla- lations, sounds or gentle motion. These methods echo humans’
tions underlies not only memory natural inclinations to take advantage of a lullaby’s rhythm or the
reactivation but also the alter- rocking of a cradle to lull a baby to sleep. —K .A.P. and D.O.
Spindles
Thalamus
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 93
Answering Queries
in Real Time
while Dreaming
Researchers demonstrate that during REM sleep,
people can hear—and respond to—simple questions such as
“What is eight minus six?”
By Diana Kwon
D
reams are full of possibilities; believed that it was going to be possible— the French group asked its participant—
by drifting into the world beyond but until we actually demonstrated it, we a person with narcolepsy who had expert
our waking realities, we can visit weren’t sure.” lucid-dreaming abilities—to answer yes-or-
magical lands, travel through time and in- For this study, Paller and his colleagues no questions with facial muscle contrac-
teract with long-lost family and friends. recruited volunteers who said they remem- tions rather than eye movements.
The notion of communicating in real time bered at least one dream per week and pro- Across the four studies, there were a to-
with someone outside of our dream vided them with guidance on how to lucid tal of 36 participants and 158 trials during
scapes, however, sounds like science fic- dream. They were also trained to respond which the researchers could verify lucid
tion. A recent study demonstrates that, to to simple math problems by moving their dreaming and attempted to establish con-
some extent, this seeming fantasy can be eyes back and forth—for example, the cor- tact. Answers were considered correct if
made real. rect response to “eight minus six” would be three of four raters were in agreement on
Scientists already knew that one-way to move your eyes to the left and right whether the responses, sometimes very
contact is attainable. Previous studies twice. While the participants slept, elec- subtle movements, were accurate. Correct
have demonstrated that people can pro- trodes attached to their faces picked up responses were given in 18 percent of trials;
cess external cues, such as sounds and their eye movements, and electroencepha- another 18 percent were classified as am-
smells, while asleep. There is also evi- lography (EEG)—a method of monitoring biguous because raters could not come to a
dence that people are able to send mes- brain activity—kept track of what stage of consensus about whether participants gave
sages in the other direction: lucid dream- sleep they were in. a correct response or whether they had re-
ers—those who can become aware they As Paller’s team was conducting these sponded at all. Incorrect responses were
are in a dream—can be trained to signal, experiments, the researchers discovered given in 3 percent of the trials. Overall
using eye movements, that they are in the three groups in Germany, France and the there was no response in 60 percent.
midst of a dream. Netherlands who were trying to accom- One of the co-authors, Karen Konkoly,
Two-way communication, however, is plish the same thing. Instead of competing, a graduate student in Paller’s lab, specu-
more complex. It requires a person who is the groups decided to collaborate. They lates that participants failed to respond in
asleep to actually understand what they carried out similar experiments, though 60 percent of the trials because they sim-
hear from the outside and think about it with slightly different methods of answer- ply did not perceive the incoming com-
logically enough to generate an answer, ing questions and receiving responses. The munication. In those cases, they rarely re-
explains Ken Paller, a cognitive neuro German group, for example, transmitted ported any incorporation of the questions
scientist at Northwestern University. “We its math problems using Morse code, and into their dreams after waking up. But
asleep but did not remember the question technique could be used by people to en- during dreams.”
correctly after waking up. These findings hance problem-solving and creativity, by
were published in February 2021 in Cur- providing a new way to process content in Diana Kwon is a freelance journalist who covers health and
rent Biology. their dreams. the life sciences. She is based in Berlin.
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 95
OPINION
CAN
SCIENCE
ILLUMINATE
OUR
INNER
DARK
MATTER?
Neither introspection nor brain scans
can reveal our deepest thoughts
By John Horgan
KEEP COMING EVEN WHEN WE PAY NO ATTENTION but not conscious of anything. Con-
sciousness without content strikes
TO THEM, AND THEY KEEP CHANGING; NO THOUGHT me as a contradiction, an oxymo-
ron, like a book without words or
IS PRECISELY LIKE ANOTHER. a film without images. And how
would you k now you’re in a state of
pure consciousness? How would
of two hands drawing each other. (Who draws the draw- you remember it? Even Forman admits that states of
er?) Thoughts spring from thoughts and—in ways still pure consciousness, if they exist, are rare.
beyond our ken—from our brains, which contain rough- Meditation is touted as a route to knowledge of your
ly 100 billion neurons linked by one quadrillion synaps- deepest self, your innermost thoughts. I’ve had delight-
es, each of which processes an average of 10 electro- ful experiences meditating, especially on a silent retreat
chemical signals, or action potentials, every second. in 2018. But meditation and other contemplative tech-
If you equate action potentials with the operations niques are designed to control and suppress thoughts
of a computer, as many neuroscientists do, then the rather than to understand them. Meditation is self-
brain carries out 10 quadrillion operations in a typical brainwashing aimed at taming your monkey mind. I
second. That approaches the speed of the world’s fast- don’t want to tame my monkey mind; I want to study
est supercomputers, and the brain may perform expo- it, to comprehend its antics.
nentially more calculations via processes other than ac- Although we may not notice them and may even
tion potentials. The result of all this activity is that brains deny their existence, thoughtless thoughts are always
churn out thoughts as ceaselessly as hearts pump blood. there, underpinned by our brains’ incessant chatter.
As James puts it, thoughts are “continuous,” they Without thoughtless thoughts, we would lack meta-
“flow,” they keep coming even when we pay no atten- thoughts. Thoughtless thoughts are the dark matter of
tion to them, and they keep changing; no thought is pre- our minds, giving shape via hidden mechanisms to what
cisely like another. James thus doubts whether psychol- is observable, visible, luminous in our inner cosmos.
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 99
Altered Reality
A Disorder of Mind and Brain page 102
The Undiscovered Illness page 110
Could COVID Delirium Bring On Dementia? page 116
A Psychedelic Renaissance page 122
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 101
A
DISORDER
MIND
OF
BRAIN
AND
It all
these bizarre conditions, which have historically been
given diverse names, such as hysteria, conversion dis-
order or psychosomatic illness. These labels have,
however, long imposed particular explanations for
what many researchers now regard as a complex ill-
began
ness at the interface of psychiatry and neurology.
Some are still in use today, but the newest name for
these conditions, functional neurological disorder
(FND), is deliberately neutral, simply denoting a prob-
lem in the functioning of the nervous system.
with a
Patients with FND have long struggled to obtain
adequate care. They have been accused of feigning or
imagining symptoms, painfully but often fruitlessly
probed for childhood trauma and dismissed by doc-
tors who did not know how to treat someone who,
cough.
based on all the usual tests, appeared to be healthy.
“For many, many years physicians have underesti-
mated the prevalence of these disorders and the
human toll it takes,” says Kathrin LaFaver, a neurolo-
gist who specializes in movement disorders at Sara-
toga Hospital in New York State. “These people have
Four years ago Tracey McNiven, a Scottish woman really fallen [in the gap] between the fields of neurol-
in her mid-30s, caught a bad chest infection that left ogy and psychiatry.”
her with a persistent cough that refused to subside, Over the past decade or so, however, using tech-
even after medication. A few months later strange niques such as functional magnetic resonance imag-
symptoms started to appear. McNiven noticed numb- ing (fMRI), researchers have begun to understand
ness spreading through her legs and began to feel that what happens in the brains of patients with this enig-
their movement was out of her control. When she matic illness. And by applying new models of how the
walked, she felt like a marionette, with someone else brain works, they are gaining a better understanding
pulling the strings. Over the course of two weeks the of how the condition arises and how it may be treated.
odd loss of sensation progressively worsened. Then,
one evening at home, McNiven’s legs collapsed be ENIGMATIC ILLNESSES
neath her. “I was lying there, and I felt like I couldn’t More than 3 , 0 0 0 years ago Mursili II, king of the
breathe,” she recalls. “I couldn’t feel below my waist.” Hittites, was caught in a terrifying thunderstorm. The
McNiven’s mother rushed her to the hospital where experience left him with a temporary speech impedi-
she remained for more than half a year. ment that went away—only to return several years lat-
During her first few weeks in the hospital, McNiv- er, after the monarch woke from a nightmare about
en endured a barrage of tests as doctors tried to un- the incident. His subjects attributed their king’s curi-
cover the cause of her symptoms. It could be a pro- ous ailment to the wrath of the Storm God, one of the
gressive neurodegenerative condition such as motor most important deities of the ancient civilization.
neuron d isease, they thought. Or maybe it was multi- When modern-day scholars revisited the documents
ple sclerosis, a disease in which the body’s own im detailing the event, they interpreted it as functional
mune cells attack the nervous system. Bafflingly, how- aphonia (the inability to speak).
ever, the brain scans, blood tests, spinal taps and ev- Like the Hittites, people throughout history have
erything else came back normal. turned to the supernatural—gods, witchcraft and de
McNiven’s predicament is not uncommon. Accord- monic possession—to explain illnesses that today
ing to one of the most comprehensive assessments of would likely be diagnosed as FND. According to some
neurology clinics to date, roughly a third of patients historical interpretations, the first scientific attempt
have neurological symptoms that are deemed to be ei- to account for them emerged around 400 b.c.e., when
ther partially or entirely unexplained. These may in- Greek physicians, including Hippocrates, coined the
clude tremor, seizures, blindness, deafness, pain, pa- term “hysteria” to describe a wide collection of ail-
ralysis and coma and can parallel those of almost any ments, among them paralysis, headaches, dizziness
neurological disease. In some patients, such complica- and pain, in the belief that they were caused by the
tions can persist for years or even decades; some peo- uterus (hystera, i n Greek) wandering about the body.
ple require wheelchairs or cannot get out of bed. Al- Hysteria had its heyday in the 19th century, when
though women are more often diagnosed than men, it moved from the womb to the brain. Among several
handful of experiments. In a 2014 study, for exam- the peak in the early 2000s. Even so, hundreds of children have been
ple, Edwards and his team used a task called force diagnosed with the condition in recent years. —D.K.
matching, in which a robotic device presses down
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 107
on a finger and people are asked to match the force set of people with FND, the emotional circuitry might
with their own hand. Healthy people tend to overesti- be hijacking the motor system, Perez explains.
mate the force required by their own hand because Perez’s team has also found that some risk factors
the brain’s expectations “cancel out” some of its force may map onto these circuits. In a study published in
(a similar explanation applies to why you cannot tick- 2020, his group reported that the magnitude of the
le yourself ). People with FND, on the other hand, coupling of the motor regions with the limbic and sa-
were abnormally accurate, indicating that the internal lience areas of the brain positively correlated with the
prediction system was functioning differently. Even degree to which patients experienced physical abuse
so, much more evidence is needed to prove that this during childhood. Perez emphasizes, however, that
mechanism provides a correct and sufficient explana- this will probably be relevant only to the subset of pa-
tion for the condition. tients in whom trauma is present: in his study, a sig-
nificant proportion of patients did
not report any childhood physical
USING NONINVASIVE PROBES, abuse. Still, he notes that these find-
RESEARCHERS ARE FINDING SUBTLE ings point to how a risk factor such
as trauma could alter brain circuits
DIFFERENCES IN THE BRAINS OF in people who develop FND.
Scientists are also investigating
INDIVIDUALS WITH FUNCTIONAL how factors such as stress alter
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 109
110 | SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN | SPECIAL EDITION | WINTER 2022
ILLNESS
UNDISCOVERE
Hundreds of thousands
of people experience mania
without ever getting
depressed. Why does
psychiatry insist on calling
them bipolar?
By Simon Makin
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 111
placed to advance this debate. In 2016 he published a self-study
in the journal Psychosis c ataloguing his symptoms, which in-
clude enhanced recall, increased empathy and spiritual experi-
ences. He has suffered some ill effects, including severe fatigue,
IN OCTOBER
confusion and behavior that caused concern among friends and
colleagues: he once burst into tears while delivering a lecture.
But his professional training has helped him control his im-
pulses and avoid delusional thinking. On balance, he believes
1997,
that his madness, as he calls it, has enriched rather than dam-
aged his life. “I’m aware my case may be atypical,” Ho says. “Pre-
at the age of 58, David Ho had cisely for this reason, it challenges prevailing psychiatric beliefs
an unusual experience while that fail to acknowledge the positive value of mental disorders.”
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 113
She then hopped a freight train, got off in the
middle of rural Tennessee, climbed out of a
Psychic Fuel for
rock-walled valley and wandered into a chapel,
where the pastor was able to glean enough in
the Creative Brain
formation to contact her family. Shortly after re- The mad genius may be more than a cliché
suming the drive home, Lindsey ran away from
Of all the tropes of artists and mental afflictions, the most enduring is the one
her father at a highway rest stop and started
of a genius in the throes of mania. Iconic figures ranging from William Blake
picking daisies in a fenced-off electrical area.
to Ernest Hemingway to Kurt Cobain were known or believed to have bipolar
The police were called again, and although the
disorder. The association has intuitive appeal: the euphoria, abundant energy
officer urged her to leave with her father, she in-
and racing thoughts of mania are credible fuel for creativity.
sisted on being arrested.
Scientific evidence for the association has mostly been inconclusive. Much
In her cell, a guard pepper-sprayed her, and
of the data comes from historical sources, and most accounts are anecdotal.
she ended up in the office of the jail’s counselor.
Modern investigative techniques have revealed surprisingly little about what
Lindsey was barely able to speak at this point,
happens in the brain during mania, partly because brain imaging requires minimal
but she wrote “unipolar” repeatedly on a black-
head movement, so scanning someone in a floridly manic state is a challenge.
board. The counselor then read Lindsey a de-
As a dynamic process involving the interplay of multiple brain networks, creativity
scription of mania. She credits this encounter
is also difficult to research.
as the moment she accepted the need to take
But comparing findings from research into bipolar disorder with certain
medication. The counselor gave her Zyprexa
studies of creativity reveals hints of a link: cognitive “disinhibition” seems to be
(olanzapine), an antipsychotic. She recovered
a feature of both the creative state described as being in the “flow” and altered
and takes it to this day, though not without res-
brain circuits in bipolar disorder.
ervations. “My medication is like a dose of sad-
Brain-imaging studies have found reduced activity in a part of the prefrontal
ness, hunger, fatigue and pain,” she says. Lind-
cortex that helps to regulate emotion, which may be linked to impaired impulse
sey was euphoric throughout her ordeal, even
control and extremes of mood in people with bipolar disorder. (The prefrontal
while being pepper-sprayed. Only the people
cortex is the brain’s “orchestra conductor” responsible for directing various
around her suffer. “I feel like I’ve been blessed
mental processes.) Some of these studies have also found diminished activity
with this illness that makes me so happy,” she
in an area involved in suppressing the kind of spontaneous thought that appears
says, “but I feel selfish because of how it affects
to well up from the unconscious, seemingly out of nowhere.
my family.”
These results are reminiscent of a 2008 study of improvising jazz musicians
Lindsey married Andy, a journalist, in 2015,
and a 2012 study of freestyling rappers, conducted by the team of speech
not long after he witnessed her last hospital-
neuroscientist Allen Braun, then at the National Institutes of Health, which
ization. “It made the relationship stronger in
found reduced activity in the part of the prefrontal cortex that inhibits
the end,” he says. “I got to see her as she clawed
spontaneous cognition. The researchers also found an increase in activity
her way back to sanity. It was impressive.” The
in a section of the prefrontal cortex that is part of the so-called default mode
most im portant factor in her treatment is
network, which revs up when a person is not focusing on a task but rather
whether a physician accepts that she is not bi-
polar. “When that’s ignored, she no longer
trusts that person,” Andy says.
IT ALL GETS REAL convinced the geographical differences are genetic in origin and
A c u r i o us q u i r k i n the tale of this neglected disorder is that that unipolar mania therefore represents a distinct condition.
prevalence estimates vary worldwide and are consistently Getting to the bottom of this question will require large,
higher in non-Western countries. After qualifying in South Af- multicultural international studies. In the meantime, scientists
rica in 1997, psychiatrist Christoffel Grobler worked in an inpa- are trying to compensate for a shortage of data. One reason
tient unit in Ireland, where his bipolar patients were mostly in most early studies failed to find differences between mania and
depressed states. When Grobler returned to South Africa in bipolar disorder may be that they are so slight that they can be
2009, he noticed the opposite pattern: his patients were mostly reliably detected only in large samples. Now in his 90s, Angst
in manic states. To investigate, in 2010 he and his colleagues addressed this problem by consolidating data from nine epide-
interviewed 103 bipolar patients in three hospitals, using a miological studies conducted in the U.S., Germany, Switzerland,
standard diagnostic questionnaire. They found that 32 percent Brazil and Holland. That study, published online in November
of patients qualified as unipolar, defined as having at least five 2018 in Bipolar Disorders, found that people with unipolar ma-
manic episodes over four or more years. “When I present this nia were more likely to be male but less likely to have attempted
at conferences, people come up and say, ‘We see this all the suicide or to suffer from anxiety, drug use and eating disorders.
time,’ ” Grobler says. Angst and his colleagues claim these findings suggest unipolar
Regional variations are tricky to interpret because cultural mania “should be established as a separate diagnosis.”
differences come into play: depression is more likely to be con- Some of these findings align with three reviews of research
sidered part of normal life in Africa, for example. The quality and on unipolar mania published in the past seven years. All three
procedures of health-care systems differ, and other causes, in- found that unipolar mania is less likely to co-occur with anxiety
cluding infection or intoxication, may be a factor. But Grobler is (which often accompanies depression) but more likely to come
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 115
COULD
COVID
DELIRIUM
BRING ON
DEMENTIA? Delirium is very common
on COVID wards.
Researchers are testing
whether these
temporary bouts of confusion
could bring on permanent
cognitive decline
By Carrie Arnold
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 119
an imbalance in neurotransmitters—chemical messen- management of delirium from a pharmaceutical
gers such as dopamine and acetylcholine. standpoint,” Price says.
Inouye’s mounting clinical experience has taught Scientists have developed three hypotheses to ex-
her that regardless of what precipitates delirium, plain how delirium might provoke dementia. One
around 70 percent of those with symptoms eventu- line of thinking holds that an accumulation of toxic
ally recover completely. In the 30 percent who do cellular trash in the brain could cause short-term
not, however, an episode of delirium predicts a delirium and lead to longer-term damage. The body
downward spiral over a period of months that leads usually clears this molecular rubbish by way of the
to profound cognitive impairment or even to symp- bloodstream and the glymphatic system, which is a
toms of dementia. network of channels filled with cerebrospinal fluid.
More formal studies have reinforced the link, to Damage to vessels from an acute episode of delirium
varying degrees. Inouye investigated a group of 560 could persist and trigger dementia, or a brain that
people aged 70 or older who had undergone surgery, experiences delirium could become more prone to
and she saw that cognitive decline over the subse- vascular problems in the future.
quent 36 months was three times faster in those The second suspect is inflammation, which often
who developed delirium than in those who did not troubles people who are hospitalized for infections,
have the condition. A 2020 meta-analysis of 23 stud- respiratory distress or cardiovascular disease. Sur-
ies showed that delirium during a hospital stay was gery and severe infections can cause a buildup of cel-
associated with 2.3 times greater odds of developing lular detritus in the brain, which triggers more in
dementia. And work by a team of Brazilian scien- flamma tion. This short-term, all-hands-on-deck re
tists showed that in a group of 309 people with an action safeguards the brain because it clears the
harmful debris, and the inflam-
mation ultimately dies down.
“WE DON’T UNDERSTAND THE MECHANISMS That is not the case for those who
develop delirium, Inouye says.
OF DELIRIUM AT ALL—WE REALLY DON’T. AND Persistent inflammation can trig-
Hip fractures
institutes around the world have funded a
$7 billion
variety of studies into the long-term cogni-
tive effects of COVID, some of which look
at delirium. One such study in the U.S.
tracked people who were treated in the Delirium and Cognitive Decline
hospital for COVID, many of whom devel- People who experience delirium during hospitalization are at increased risk of
oped delirium during their stay, and mea- cognitive decline after discharge, according to a study of 309 patients in Brazil.
sured their cognitive and psychiatric func-
Dementia after no delirium
tion. An international study is planned
to measure the prevalence of delirium 16%
in patients with COVID in ICUs, as well
Dementia after delirium
as identifying factors that predict long-
term outcomes. 32%
A separate study in Germany and the
U.K. is also tracking neurocognitive out-
comes in people with C OVID to determine
how delirium affects brain function months later. ic interest in the delirium-dementia connection—
Another research project led by a team at Vanderbilt and provide some insight.
is looking for an alternative to commonly used “It’s going to be, I think, a little bit frightening
sedatives such as benzodiazepines, which are known and a little bit enlightening about how illness affects
to increase delirium. The researchers are testing a dementia risk but also about what other lifestyle
sedative called dexmedetomidine to evaluate wheth- and genetic protective factors can influence risk as
er it is a safer option for people who are hospitalized well,” Tronson says. “We’re learning quickly, but
with COVID. there are still a lot of black boxes.”
Inouye and Tronson hope that the funding of
these long-term studies will lead to ongoing scientif- Carrie Arnold is an independent public health reporter based in Virginia.
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 121
OPINION
A Psychedelic Renaissance
Psilocybin and MDMA represent a first wave
of therapies for psychiatric disorders
that help patients by changing the way
they view reality
By Danielle Schlosser and Thomas R. Insel
T
hrough a May 2 02 1 article he where these drugs were lost to science,
w rote in the Independent,
we although they were still used in recre-
learned about Steve Shorney, who ational and religious settings.
lived with depression for most of his life New studies have reignited the hope
despite years of psychotherapy, medica- that psychedelics could be powerful medi-
tion, yoga and many other attempts at cines for mental disorders. In a New Eng-
holistic treatments. With his decision to land Journal of Medicine report, two dos-
enroll in a psilocybin clinical trial at es of the chemical from the psilocybin
Imperial College London, his life “radical- mushroom appeared as effective as six
ly changed.” Psilocybin was different from weeks of escitalopram (Lexapro), a stan-
every other treatment or experience he dard antidepressant, for people with long- PSILOCYBIN MUSHROOMS
had had. As he recalled in the I ndepen- standing moderate-to-severe major de
dent a rticle, “I had seen an alternative pressive disorder (MDD). On many of the
reality, another way of being, and knew secondary measures of depression, such tive, effective and safe treatments. Fewer
beyond anything I’d known before that as remission of symptoms, psilocybin ap than half of people with these disorders
day that life is extraordinary. And in that peared better than the standard treat- respond to medications or psychothera-
moment I felt happier, more alive, and ment, with 57 percent of subjects showing py, and about a third of MDD patients
more Me than I imagined was possible.” remission on psilocybin versus 28 percent have so-called treatment-refractory de
The use of psychedelics, especially psi- in remission with escitalopram, although pression that fails to respond at all. Dis-
locybin and MDMA (also known as Ecsta- the authors caution that more research covering new approaches to treat mental
sy or Molly), is undergoing a renaissance. will be needed to confirm these results. In health conditions is critical. If psychedel-
More than five decades ago psychedelics a study reported in Nature Medicine, ics prove to be effective and safe for these
were an active area of research, with more MDMA was found to be more effective disorders, they could be transformative
than 40,000 patients receiving LSD or psi- than placebo for people with severe post- in two interesting ways.
Roger Cremers/Bloomberg/Getty Images
locybin for alcoholism, anxiety or depres- traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). After First, these medications appear to be
sion. While we do not have rigorous clini- three sessions with MDMA, 67 percent no effective after acute administration. Most
cal trials from that time, the use of these longer met criteria for their diagnosis, psychiatric drugs, such as escitalopram for
drugs garnered both scientific and public and 33 percent showed complete remis- MDD, need to be taken for weeks, months
interest, with Hollywood celebrities such sion, relative to 32 and 5 percent, respec- or years to be effective. Presumably, they
as Cary Grant promoting their use. But tively, after receiving a placebo. control symptoms but do not alter the dis-
the War on Drugs, beginning during the Patients with disorders such as MDD order. When effective, psychedelics appear
Nixon years, led to a long, dark period and PTSD can certainly use more innova- to confer long-term effects, sometimes
SCIENTIFICAMERICAN.COM | 123
END NOTE
Does
Consciousness
Pervade
the Universe?
A philosopher answers questions
about “panpsychism”
By Gareth Cook