A Modern Approach To English Grammar An Introduction To Systemic Grammar
A Modern Approach To English Grammar An Introduction To Systemic Grammar
Grammar
An
Introduction
to Systemic
Grammar
James Muir
With the more widespread inclusion of
linguistics in English language courses, there
has been a growing need for a working
grammar between the elementary textbook and
the advanced study. The present book has been
written in response to requests from practising
teachers, undergraduates and college of
education students for an extended presentation
of the author's approach.
This is an essentially practical introduction to
English grammar, based on the model
developed by Professor M A K Halliday,
known as ‘Scale and Category" or (more
recently) 'Systemic' linguistics. The book's
coverage is both comprehensive and detailed:
Part 1 sketches an overall picture of the
grammar; Part 2 provides a comprehensive
description of ‘Surface Grammar' and Part 3
introduces the reader to ‘Deep Grammar’. The
author's writing is as clear as the book's
organisation; all technical terms are fully
explained, and notes have been kept to the
essential minimum.
James Muir writes from many years' experience
of teaching linguistics. He is currently Lecturer
in English at the Jordanhill College of
Education, Glasgow.
£2.20 net
A Modern Approach to English Grammar
A Modern Approach
to English Grammar
An Introduction to Systemic Grammar
James Muir
B. T. Batsford Ltd
London
First published 1972
© James Muir 1972
£ i>ec i a. I
Cell.
PE
I \0(r
MS
To the memory of O.K.S.
Contents
Preface ix
1 General Survey 1
2 Surface Grammar 14
2.1 Morpheme and Word 14
2.1.1 Inflexion 18
2.2 Word and Group 25
2.2.1 Nominal Group 25
2.2.2 Verbal Group 41
2.2.3 Adverbial Group 49
2.3 Group and Clause 51
2.3.1 Complements in Clause Structure 57
2.3.2 Predicators in Clause Structure 59
2.3.3 Adjuncts in Clause Structure 63
2.4 Clause and Sentence 65
3 Deep Grammar 89
3.1 Systems at Clause Rank 93
3.1.1 Mood 93
3.1.2 Theme 97
3.1.3 Information 101
3.1.4 Transitivity 104
3.2 Systems at Group Rank 120
3.2.1 Nominal Group: Number, Case, Gender 120
3.2.2 Systems at d in Nominal Group Structure 128
3.2.3 Systems at Verbal Group: Finiteness, Modality,
Tense, Polarity, Aspect, Voice, Contrast, Focus
(of Contrast) 131
Notes 143
Bibliography 145
Index 147
Preface
X PREFACE
JM
General Survey
We could not readily assign an order to the boy, the patting, the
dog, the boy’s stoop, the dog’s tail. On the other hand, as soon
as you try to report what you have seen, you find that you not
only can, but must, assign an order to it and must break up your
impression into pieces of your own choosing and present them,
not simultaneously, but one by one.
nominal group: the great big dog; the little man; the boy; table
verbal group: bites; will bite; will be biting-, has bitten-, bit
adverbial group: deeply’, quickly-, fast', in the field-, in the arm.
FIGURE 1
Theoretical Categories
->
Unit Class Structure (System)
Simple alpha
t Sentence Compound alpha (beta)
_t
Ind. at alpha (s), P, (c), (a)
Clause Dep. at beta
8 -—
•c Nom. at s/c
o (m), h, (q)
60
<L>
eS Group Verb, at p (a), («), /
U ->->
Adv. at A (0, a, (/)/pc
>
o, _t
•c
o pre-noun at m
co
o subs, at h
Q
u neg. at n __
8 Word aux. verb at a (j>), b, (/)
1cG lex. verb at l
Pi prep, at p
adv. at a
J- bound at p//
Morpheme free at b
Y
GENERAL SURVEY 9
morphology of the nominal group is that it has elements of structure
m, h, q, at which operate members of the unit word. Second, and
consequent on the first point, we never, in fact, discuss only one
unit on the rankscale; any discussion necessarily involves the unit
next above or the unit next below.
Figure 1 is a diagrammatic presentation of what has been dis¬
cussed; and (though perhaps somewhat forbidding at first) has
proved useful as a mnemonic and as an illustration of the grammar
here presented.
The following points may be noted:
(i) The arrowed lines connecting elements of structure and units
indicate the syntactic (‘upward’) and the morphological (‘down¬
ward’) relationships discussed in the preceding paragraph.
(ii) Brackets round elements of structure indicate that these elements
are optional, and are dependent on the presence of the obligatory
element.
(iii) The arrowed lines above elements of structure indicate that the
given sequence of elements is obligatory.
It will perhaps be advantageous at this point to introduce a short¬
hand for marking boundaries of units. The following symbols are
used in this grammar:
Unit Symbol Example
Sentence /// ///The boys reminded Tom that he had
hidden the books///
Clause II ///The boys reminded Tom//that he
had hidden the books///
Group / / //The boys/reminded/Tom//that/he/
had hidden/the books///
Word HI The boys/reminded/Tom//that/he/
had hidden/the books///
Morpheme + / //The boy+s / remind+ed/T om//that/
he/had hid+(d)en/the book+s///
It follows from what has been said that the boundary of any unit
will also be the boundary of all lower units. Thus, a sentence boun¬
dary is also a clause boundary, a group boundary, a word boundary
and a morpheme boundary. The categories so far discussed will
provide:
10 GENERAL SURVEY
(i) a description of the units on the chain of linguistic events; this
means naming patterned stretches on the linear succession of lan¬
guage activity;
(ii) class names for each of these units based on their place of
operation in the chain;
(iii) a functional description based on the relations between classes
of unit: e.g. mh, sp.
Thus we can analyse the sentence the big dog will bite the little man in
the arm (stopping at group rank) as follows:
Ind
s p c A
N V N Ad
m h a / m h P c
m h
The big dog will bite the little man in the arm
Number-
t Countable—
Uncountable
Singular
Plural
(See p. 120#.)
It is evident that grammar extends along two dimensions or axes.
These have been informally designated ‘chain’ axis and ‘choice’
axis: a class denotes the potentiality of operation of a unit on the
chain axis; a systemic term (or feature) denotes the selection made
from a set of mutually exclusive choices at a place on the chain. In
this book the expression ‘Surface Grammar’ denotes all that is
implied by the former, and the expression ‘Deep Grammar’ all that
is implied by the latter.
Chain and choice are presented diagrammatically in Figure 2.
figure 2
(Chain axis: classes of units)
Subject Predicator
Ih
<D
r Singular r Finite
O '[^Plural ’I Non-finite
a
o
" Masculine Present
Feminine Past
X Neuter Future
a
<L>
O
o
X!
r Active
[_ Passive
U *
12 GENERAL SURVEY
(ii) f
1 1
Cl. Cl.
/\ /\
/ \ / \
/ \ / \
S P c s :P A
1 1
l 1
N V N n v Ad.
1 1 1 /\ /\
h l h h a Ip C
Conversely, the two expressions (iii) The boy sings well and (iv) The
material wears well will entail similar surface descriptions. Thus:
Yet native English speakers will agree that (i) and (ii) are gram¬
matically related sentences, but (iii) and (iv) are not. If a grammar
does not show that structurally dissimilar items may be closely
related and that structurally similar items may be quite unrelated,
then the grammar is inadequate as a description of how language
works. The close grammatical relation of clauses (i) and (ii) is
evident only in their deep grammar. The surface differences of these
clauses can be attributed to the selection of different systemic
terms. Thus, clause (i) has selected ‘operative’ and clause (ii) has
GENERAL SURVEY 13
selected ‘receptive’ from the system of transitivity.! It is clear that
the surface grammar alone gives no direct indication of the grammati¬
cal relatedness of clauses. It can be further shown that the original
expression
Tom hit Bill
is related to each of the expressions:
Bill was hit by Tom
Did Tom hit Bill?
It was Tom who hit Bill
Hit Bill, Tom.
by the selection of different systemic terms.
Some systems in English are discussed at length in Chapter 3. In
the view of language presented here, the description of a language
and items in a language requires two components—a surface com¬
ponent and a deep component. It is perhaps also obvious that as
the description of grammar becomes more delicate we are approach¬
ing the problem of meaning; but, properly viewed, this is the direc¬
tion from the beginning—it is not a question in language of ‘ grammar
and meaning’ but a question of ‘grammatical meaning’.
It was stated in the General Survey that word was not the lowest
unit in English grammar, but that a lower unit, the morpheme,
operated at elements of structure of the word. The morpheme is
formally established by comparing words and discovering recurrent
patterns. Native English speakers will presumably agree that
hunting is composed of two elements, hunt + ing, but that stout is not
composed of two elements, st + out. This is so because the native
speaker recognises the occurrence of patterns already evident in the
language. He knows that ing appears after many other elements:
singing, shooting, swimming, turning, hoping etc., and that hunt
appears without other elements (i.e. as a word): tigers hunt their
prey; the hunt was successful; and that ‘hunt’ also occurs before
other endings: hunts, hunted, hunter. On the other hand, he knows
that although out occurs alone and before other endings, st does not
regularly occur with other items. He also knows that when out
occurs alone it has a meaning that it does not have when it occurs
as part of stout. Recognition of the various types of patterning
requires a formulation and extended application of this intuitive pro¬
cess.
If we compare:
eats eating
with walks walking
or
boy boys
with lad lads
MORPHEME AND WORD 15
we can recognise identical stretches vertically and horizontally. We
have, in fact, a ‘square’2 of the form
AC AD
BC BD
man men
boy boys
lad lads
detain contain
deceive conceive.
In such squares our analysis will yield the items de, tain, ceive, con
as elements of structure of words. None of these items, however,
occur alone as do the items boy, lad, man, walk, etc. If we recognise
such squares we must qualify this by saying that the elements of such
words are different from the elements of other words; in fact, we
have a small number of base elements which are bound (do not occur
alone) in contrast to the vast majority of base elements which are
free (which do occur alone). Again we find semantic corroboration
in that we cannot attach meanings to the elements tain, ceive, etc.
as we can with s (plural) or ed (past) or er (agent).
We can, then, identify a word as simple or complex in structure,
as composed of one morpheme (if it has 0 in the square) or more than
one morpheme, if it enters into a square of one of the following
types:
(i) are not recursive; only one inflexional morpheme may occur
in the structure of any one word,
(ii) such an inflexional morpheme always occurs finally in word
structure, and therefore
(iii) if both a derivational and an inflexional morpheme occur in the
structure of a word, then the derivational morpheme must
precede the inflexional morpheme, e.g. novel + ist + s.
(i) may be recursive, e.g. boy + ish + ness (but sequences are fixed,
we cannot have *boy + ness + ish);
(ii) do not form morphological sets; and so cannot be fully accounted
for in grammar; they may be considered as on the border of
grammar and lexis;
(iii) if a derivational and an inflexional morpheme are present in the
structure of a word, then the derivational morpheme must be
non-final as noted above.
2.1.1 Inflexion
It is obvious that the three types of morphological sets above corres¬
pond to the traditional word-classes of verb, noun, adjective. It
must be stressed again that classes of units are determined syntactic¬
ally, not morphologically; thus, boy will be classed as a noun
MORPHEME AND WORD 19
because it patterns in a certain way in syntax (to be described in the
next chapter), not because it enters into the morphological pattern
discussed above. It is because of this that we class sheep also as a
noun which enters into singular and plural contrast though it is
structurally dissimilar to boy. This, of course, is why we included the
man:men/child:children type in the squares. Nevertheless, though
syntactic criteria take precedence over morphological criteria, if
there is some congruence between syntactic and morphological
criteria in a language then this is helpful and can be utilised in descrip¬
tion. In English there is considerable congruence between syntactic¬
ally defined classes of noun, verb, adjective and the morphological
sets discussed above. Thus we can say that:
boy boy's boys boys'
eat eats ate eating eaten
green greener greenest
form a typical noun, verb and adjective paradigm respectively.
Before discussing English inflexions in more detail, it is necessary
to discuss the relations between the morpheme, or more properly
the morpheme class, and its realisation in substance. Anticipating the
material of a future chapter (p. 120), we can say that the word-class
noun is involved in selection from the system of number, which has
the terms singular and plural. If we say that boy is a typical example
of noun singular and that boys is a typical example of noun plural,
it might appear that we are saying that the written <s> or the spoken
fzj is the morpheme plural. But formal units are not the same as the
substance by which they are realised. The <s> and fzj are examples of
how the morpheme plural may be realised, but they are not the
morpheme plural. The morpheme plural, in fact, has no ‘ shape ’; it,
like other units, is an abstraction from the linguistic data. Grammar
is concerned with classes of items, not with particular realisations
in substance. The grammatical analysis of boys is noun: plural and
of men is noun:plural. That the former is formed by adding <s> or
fzj to the singular but the latter is not formed in this way does not
affect the grammatical classification.
The morpheme class plural has various realisations, among which
are vowel alternation man:men, and no overt change from the
singular form sheep‘.sheep (i.e. no change morphologically; we
could say plurality is realised by concord with determiners and
verbs: this sheep is‘.those sheep are', see further p. 120 ff.).
20 SURFACE GRAMMAR
NOUN INFLEXION
English nouns are also inflected for marked case (p. 123#.). This
requires the addition of /s, z, Iz/ to the singular and plural forms;
which particular phoneme occurs depends on the preceding phoneme
(as in plural formation). In spelling, this requires the addition of
<’s> to the singular and <s’> to the plural.
22 SURFACE GRAMMAR
ADJECTIVE INFLEXION
VERB INFLEXION
PERSONAL PRONOUNS
FIGURE 3
Singular Plural
Non Non
s-case Gen. s-case Gen.
s-case s-case
m. he him his
n. it it its
The alternative forms of the genitives indicate that the form without
the <s> (and my) occurs when the item operates at d in nominal
group structure, and the form with the <s> (and mine) occurs when
the item operates at h in nominal group'structure (at c). Thus:
It is her book This book is hers.
It is my book This book is mine.
WORD AND GROUP 25
The previous chapter was concerned with the syntax of the mor¬
pheme and the morphology of the word. This chapter takes one
step up the rankscale, and is concerned with the syntax of the word
and the morphology of the group. Any discussion of group structure
must take account of the different classes of group in English. It is
the case that the units word, clause and sentence have classes
which are similar enough for these units to be assigned basic struc¬
tures irrespective of their various classes; thus in the previous chapter
the structure of the unit word (irrespective of classes such as noun,
verb, etc.) could be generalised as (p), b, (/). The different classes of
the unit group do not have enough in common to admit of such a
generalisation in respect of structure.
In the clause, the big dog will bite the little man in the arm I deeply
there are four elements of structure:
12 3 4
(the big dog) (will bite) (the little man) (in the arm/deeply)
To these elements of structure we can give the well-established names
subject (s), predicator (p), complement (c), adjunct (a). It is evident
that in clause structure the patterns at the elements 1 and 3 above
(i.e. s and c) are alike. Thus, though there are four elements of
structure, we require only three classes of group defined syntactically,
since one class of group operates at two places in clause structure.
Accordingly, the following three classes of group are established:
nominal group (operating at s/c in clause structure)
verbal group (operating at p in clause structure)
adverbial group (operating at a in clause structure).
The elements of structure of these groups, and the word-classes
operating at these elements are discussed in the following sections.
m h q
all the other ten very worn school books in the library
m h q
the man himself.
(i)
h h
These boys are superb examples,
h
The bath I had was very refreshing.
h h
All the books that I read on holiday belonged to my brother,
h
The spot where the river bends is tricky to negotiate.
The words operating at h in the italicised groups are nouns. The
positive characteristics of the word-class noun are that its members
28 SURFACE GRAMMAR
(ii)
h h
Nobody in the room would admit to it.
h h
She danced me off my feet.
h h
Anyone who is an artist will tell you.
h
They all went up the hill.
h
They both play very well.
The words which operate at h in the italicised groups are pronouns.
Pronouns are not usually modified, though they can be qualified.
The qualifiers of pronouns are themselves complete clauses or groups
of somewhat restricted types, as in:
h q
Anyone [who is an artist}
h q
Nobody [in the room].
Some pronouns occur uniquely with other qualifiers:
(a) the so-called ‘ indefinite pronouns’ occur with the item ‘ else ’-
h q
everyone
no one
someone else
somebody
nobody
(b) the ‘plural personal pronouns’ occur with the item ‘both’—
h q
theyl
both
we
Pronouns, then, take virtually no modifiers, and restricted qualifiers;
in this they differ from nouns. In particular, they cannot be modified
by the definite or indefinite article.
WORD AND GROUP 29
(iii)
h
London is the capital.
h
Come to Canada.
h h
Matthews won the cup for Blackpool.
The words operating at h in the italicised groups are proper nouns.
Proper nouns cannot be modified or qualified, and so differ from
other types of headword. The occurrence of groups such as Bonnie
Scotland, Sunny Sussex, does not invalidate this statement. The
adjectives in such groups clearly do not fulfill the usual function of
adjectives in the nominal group. They do not identify the headword
as a particular member of its class: thus, dark night means ‘of the
class night that member which is characterised by darkness', or
sunny day means ‘ a day which is sunny ’; but Sunny Sussex does not
mean ‘of the class Sussex that member which is sunny’, a Sussex
which is srnny'.
(iv)
h
He was old.
h
His attempt was rather feeble,
h
She turned quite pale at this.
The words operating at h in the italicised groups are adjectives.
They have a restricted range of modifiers and qualifiers; they can be
qualified by the items enough, indeed, or by complete clauses or
groups of restricted types:
h q
He is older [than Harry]
h q
He is keener Ithan I am}
m h q
He kept himself as fit [as possible].
Unlike nouns, adjectives cannot be modified by articles or demon¬
stratives ; but they can be modified by the submodifiers very, rather,
quite, etc.
These various characteristics are intended as formal definitions of
30 SURFACE GRAMMAR
Looking more closely at the element d, we find that there are further
sequence restrictions within this:
fall the other
we may have
(both the same
'the all other
the same both
but not *
other the all
same the both.
We have, in fact, three distinct places at d:
WORD AND GROUP 31
d1 (predeictic) all, both, half
d2 (deictic) the, this, that; his, its, John’s, mother’s, my; a, any,
another, no, neither, every, several
d3 (post-deictic) other, same.
their first base element will contain a stressed syllable, final in the
group, but their second element will not, as in:
the Whitehousz
the black bird structure: dh (h compound)
his pocket-book
Nominal groups which have a structure nh will normally have a
stressed syllable on the n element and on the h element. This means,
of course, that h is still identifiable as the final stressed syllable.
Thus:
the coin boxes
the hotel prices structure: dnh
the gold mine
the pa-wenger vehicles
Difficulty of interpretation may arise either when there is more
than one exponent of n, or when there is a structure nh in which one or
the other exponent is compound. The main types are listed below, and
hyphenation is consistently used to indicate compound items though
in some of the (occurring) examples hyphenation was not present.
0)
time-table commitments
bank-rate rise
man-hole cover
The structure of these groups is nh (n compound). Normally such
groups will contain a stressed syllable on the first base element of n
and have an intonation break (//) between n and h. Thus:
time-inb\e/ /commitments
bank-rate//rise
man-hole/[cover
(ii)
school sum-book
London road-map
roadside telephone-boxes
The structure of these groups is nh (h compound). Normally such
groups will contain a stressed syllable on n and a stressed syllable
on the first base element of h, and have an intonation break between
n and h. Thus:
school/1sum-book
London//road-map
roadside/1 telephone-boxes
WORD AND GROUP 35
(iii)
subscriber trunk dialling
Fife County Council
London briar pipes
The structure of these groups is nnh. This structure is perhaps less
common than the preceding two. They are by no means unusual,
however; and they may have several exponents of n: consider—
subscriber trunk dialling
subscriber trunk dialling system
subscriber trunk dialling all-number system
subscriber trunk dialling all-number system fault
subscriber trunk dialling all-number system fault engineer
Structural ambiguity seems highly possible, but in practice the con¬
text of the utterance is usually such that only one interpretation is
possible. (Cf a head buyer (i) in Glasgow, (ii) in Borneo: suggest
different structures!) Nevertheless, the decision between assigning
the structure nnh and nh (with one or the other compound) may on
occasions be difficult and perhaps arbitrary.
m h q
A car [that price] is beyond me
WORD AND GROUP 37
s
h q
Shoes [this size] are usually dearer.
m h q
The snow [on the hill] was deep
m h q
He chose the books [with leather bindings].
s p c A
N v N Ad
h / h P c= M
m h q
I found him in [the house [in the main street.]]
38 SURFACE GRAMMAR
s p c
N v N
h / m h q
He decided on the house [in the country.]
If, however, it means while in the country he made up his mind about
the house then there is an adjunct in the clause structure:
s p c A
N v N Ad
h / m h P c = [N]
m h
He decided on the house in the country.
rhe boys.
*almostj
Since they thus enter the nominal group structure in association
with another element, it seems they ought to be treated as sub¬
modifiers, similar to other sub-modifiers, such as those with adjectives:
m h
d e
s-m adj.
the very old houses
m h m h
d d
di d% *k d2
a lot of the men both of the men
will come'
can come
is coming ■ structure: al
will eat
has eaten
There are only eleven members of the class of auxiliary verb, so
they can easily be listed:
beginning with:
'is, has, was, had Participle: taking
shall!should, canjcould taken/having taken
j may j might, must, ought being about to take
V.
dare, need
44 SURFACE GRAMMAR
(1) Be: this is the only verb in English which has five finite forms—
am, are, is; was were: it also has a base-inf., a t-inf., an -ing and an
-en form.
(2) Have: this verb has three finite forms (have, has, had); it also has
a base-inf., a t-inf. and an -ing form; it has no -en form.
46 SURFACE GRAMMAR
(3) Do: this verb has three finite forms {do, does, did), but no non-
finite forms (though the full verb do has non-finite forms).
(4) Will, shall, can, may: these verbs have two finite forms {will/
would, shalljshould, canjcould, mayjmight), but no non-finite
forms, and no -s forms.
(5) Must, ought, dare, need: these verbs have only one finite form,
and no non-finite forms.
Non-modal—be' have
*-b) do
Auxiliary verb—>
(a), (n), l
48 SURFACE GRAMMAR
where, as before, brackets indicate optional elements of structure,
the arrow indicates obligatory sequence, and the commas indicate
this is a list of elements of structure, not a structure. Thus:
structure: l al anl
comes will come will not come
taken will take won't take
votes did vote didn’t vote.
Longer verbal groups can be described as combinations of the
four basic types. This means that when two or more auxiliary verbs
occur in the verbal group they will combine with each other
and finally with the lexical verb in sequences of the four types.
We can primarily give the structure of such groups, and then de¬
scribe the combinations involved in terms of the four basic types.
Thus:
will be taking structure: aal; (iii), (i).
And so
will have taken structure: aal; (iii), (ii)3,
will be taken structure: aal; (iii), (ii)b-
will have been taken structure: aaal; (iii), (ii)a•, (ii)b-
will have been being taken structure: aaaal; (iii), (ii)a-,(i),(ii)b-
ought to have been taken structure: aaal; (iv), (ii)a,5 (ii)b‘
There is one further point of verbal group structure to be con¬
sidered. If the clause, he decided on the ship means ‘he made up his
mind while he was on the ship’ then the structure is spa:
s p A
he decided on the ship
but if it means ‘he chose the ship’ then the structure is spc and the
p is realised by a phrasal verb:
s p c
he decided on the ship.
There are many instances in English where verb + particle (preposi¬
tion or adverb) operates as a single unit. We can often replace the
verb + particle with a simple verb, as in:
He put down the rebellion: He suppressed the rebellion
She looked after her mother: She tended her mother.
Also, in passive constructions the particle remains with the verb:
The ship was decided on
Her mother was looked after
He was looked up to by his fellows.
It is also the case that which particles go with which verbs (and
WORD AND GROUP 49
with which meaning) is restricted: look up to but not look down to
(but—look down on); take to but not take from.
Such criteria are usually cited to show that verb + particle form a
single unit in such structures, and realise the element p (c) in clause
structure; and are therefore different from verb + adverb realising
the elements p (a) in clause structure.
The concept of the phrasal verb, as this structure is usually
called, is well established in grammatical description, and is a valid
concept. But it must be said that the identification of phrasal verbs
in particular structures often relies heavily on semantic criteria, and
there is often room for disagreement; the line between phrasal verb
+ nominal group (pc) and verb + adverb or preposition (+ nomi¬
nal group) is very difficult to draw.
Nevertheless, there are many examples where the difference is
clear, and many long-established phrasal verbs can be easily recog¬
nised, as in:
s p A s p c
We went to the place: We took to the place.
very nicely'
right there
structure: ta
over here
just before
p
Is John coming ?
Clause included within the structure of another clause (discontinuous
element of sentence structure):
(i) In the first of these clauses the nominal group the boys is the
GROUP AND CLAUSE 55
c of want and the s of to do. This is called a z positive element
(zpos), since it is positively identified as a conflation of s and c.
(See further, pp. 59 ff.)
(ii) In the second clause, if wet, the nominal group cannot be identi¬
fied as s or c because identification of these elements depends on
concord and sequence relationships with P, and this is a minor clause.
In distinction from the previous type, this is called a z negative (zne9).
(iii) The third clause is not a minor clause, nor is the nominal group,
Bill, a conflation of s and c. Though it may immediately precede s,
it is marked off from the rest of the clause by a comma (an intonation
break in the spoken language), and is variable in position—it
follows the p in the second form of the clause. This element is called
a z vocative (zvoc). (See further, pp. 94-95.)
The general picture of English clauses (and clause is perhaps one
of the areas to teach early, and certainly the best area for breaking
into the analysis of texts) is that the structure of the clause is
composed of some configuration of these five elements:
predicator (realised by one or more than one verbal group)
subject
complement • (realised by one or more than one nominal group)
z element
Adjunct (realised by one or more than one adverbial group)
intransitive (no c)
Clause- >
[ transitive-►
single (one c)
double (two c’s)
As in:
structure: sp
he called her
structure: spc
he left a note
58 SURFACE GRAMMAR
There are well over two hundred definitions of the unit sentence.
These fall into various groups or types of definition; each group
usually adds to, or contradicts, the others. Most grammarians seem
to have found it necessary to correct and improve part or all of the
previous definitions of sentence, before each produced the definition
(which it never was, of course). Two things are immediately obvious
from this: i) grammarians are unanimous in recognising the
sentence, but ii) definition of this unit is very difficult.
Many attempts at definition were found to be unsatisfactory or
inadequate because they relied solely on meaning, and attempted to
delimit the sentence as being ‘a complete thought/meaning/idea’, or
even ‘the number of ideas that can be grasped in their relation in
one act of attention’. But the necessary pre-definition of ‘thought/
idea’ and so on (whether or not ‘complete’) makes such definitions
untenable and unworkable.
A second major approach attempts to delimit the sentence by
reference to its constituent elements. In this type of definition we
are told that the ‘secret’ of the sentence is to ‘name some object or
place or person or thing’ and then to ‘say something about that
object or place or person or thing’. Such definitions usually say that
we need a subject and a predicator to make a sentence. This means
that most commands, exclamations and requests would not be con¬
sidered sentences. In such instances, of course, the s is often said to
be ‘understood’. But how much (or little) can be ‘understood’? If
we are allowed to ‘supply’ elements then any expression can be
made to conform to the definition. And it should be noted that
such definitions usually state not only that a s must be present, but
that the s is ‘the thing being talked about’. Even in the simplest of
sentences, say John shot Jim are we talking about John or Jim (after
all, it may be his last chance to be talked about), or shootingl Or,
as seems more reasonable, about John shooting Jim ?
It is not a question of whether such definitions are ‘true’ or ‘false’;
only that they do not provide reliable criteria for grammatical
66 SURFACE GRAMMAR
This definition does not seem inadequate, and for the present model
of grammar may perhaps be paraphrased ‘A sentence is that unit
CLAUSE AND SENTENCE 67
which does not operate in the structure of any higher (grammatical)
unit.’ The whole difficulty of defining the sentence stems from the
fact that it is the highest unit on the rankscale. Other units are identi¬
fied and defined by their operation at elements of structure in the
unit next above; thus, group is that unit which operates in clause
structure, morpheme is that unit which operates in word structure,
and so on. But, in the case of sentence, there is no higher grammatical
unit, so that sentence cannot be defined syntactically.
It should be noted that sentences may, of course, be part of some
larger linguistic context. The completeness or independence of the
sentence is relative to this, because sentences which contain pro-
nominals or anaphoric determiners will necessarily be related to the
antecedents of these pronominals or anaphoric determiners. Thus:
He became headmaster of Rugby
The boys turned the corner, ran down the lane and vanished
That house has stood up well to the gale
are sentences in which the referents of He, the (boys), the (corner),
the (lane)', that (house) must be given by cotext or context if the
sentences are to be understood correctly. But since there is no
grammatical unit above the sentence, this inter-sentence connection
cannot be completely accounted for grammatically. It is, of course, a
linguistic matter, and will be of importance for stylistic studies: indeed,
one linguist has defined stylistics as ‘grammar above the sentence’.
We cannot, therefore, classify sentences according to their opera¬
tion in the structure of the unit next above; the classification must
be effected in a different way from the classification of other units.
The most obvious way is by morphology; we have taken as a prin¬
ciple that morphology is subsidiary to syntax in grammatical classi¬
fication, but in the case of sentence there is no syntax. It seems
important to stress that the morphology of the sentence is not the
morphology of the clause; numerous grammars talk of the subject
(etc.) of the sentence, and exemplify with simple (i.e. one-clause)
sentences. But compound sentences may contain a number of clauses,
and therefore a number of subjects (etc.), no one of which can be
called the subject of the sentence, s, p, c, a are elements of clause
structure: it is complete clauses which operate at elements of sen¬
tence structure.
Classification in terms of morphology will yield a primary divi¬
sion into simple sentences (containing one clause) and compound
sentences (containing more than one clause). Compound sentences
68 SURFACE GRAMMAR
Simple Compound
One independent clause except One independent clause plus
in answers, which may have one or more independent
one dependent clause. clauses and/or one or more
dependent clauses.
Ind + Ind (Ind ...) (Dep ...)
or
Ind + Dep (Ind ...) (Dep ...)
or, more delicately:
,-Simple (one constituent clause)
^—»- .-Compound (more than one Ind clause)
LNon-simple—> -Complex (one Ind and one Dep clause or more)
•-Compound-Complex (more than one Ind and Dep)
It is also usual to classify sentences contextually into statement,
question and command. There are certain correlations between these
contextual classes and punctuation marks:
. statement
? question
! command/exclamation
and with certain intonation contours. They also correlate to a con¬
siderable extent with types of response, in that statement is often
accompanied by ‘continuation signals’ (a-huh, go-on, ye-es), command
is often followed by action response (‘Shut the door’) and question
by oral response or action response; it is also true that in the case
of the simple sentence there are correlations with clause classes to a
large extent:
statement declarative
question interrogative
command imperative.
But the correlations are not one-to-one; there are sentences realised
by a declarative clause which may be contextually classified as
commands, and so on; in particular minor (and therefore moodless)
CLAUSE AND SENTENCE 69
clauses can usually be assigned to a contextual class of sentence
(see pp. 75-76#.).
Since the simple sentence is by definition coterminous with the
one constituent clause there is little more to say about its structure.
Compound sentences consist of more than one clause, and this
raises an immediate question of contextual class: which clause deter¬
mines the contextual class of the sentences. In the sentences:
I came and I saw and I conquered
Do this or do that
Will you stay here or will you go to Canada?
we find, in fact, that there are no problems. The linker and conjoins
like items, and therefore the contextual class of any of the clauses
(considered as a simple sentence) will be the contextual class of the
sentence. Thus:
I came and I saw and I conquered statement
Do this or do that command
Will you stay here or will you go away? question.
In the sentences:
Tell him to come home when the game's over
John cut the rope after Bill had shouted
Keeping to the old route, Bill got home safely
both independent and dependent clauses are involved. It is only the
independent clauses which affect the contextual class of sentence.
If we alter the first sentence to Will you tell him to come home when
the game's over? the contextual class changes from command to
question. If we alter the dependent clause this does not change the
contextual class of sentence:
Tell him to come home when the game's over command
Tell him to come home if he's finished command.
It can be seen at this point that there are two elements of sentence
structure, one of which is obligatory. The obligatory element is
distinguished by two criteria: i) it can stand alone as a simple
sentence; ii) it decides the contextual class of the sentence.
The obligatory element of structure is called alpha (a) and the
optional element of structure is called beta (p). Thus:
When he comes, tell him to eat his lunch structure: Pa
He'll be in tomorrow, if he's free structure: ap
I didn't come because he told me structure: ap
Harry, who had never been before, didn't know structure:
*«P»
70 SURFACE GRAMMAR
The two dependent clauses are also linked, and there is omission of
the binding adverbial group and s in the second. The analysis should
be:
P
Dep & Dep
because I had nothing else to do and no one to see
Cl
Such items can be regarded as realising an adjunct element in clause
structure, and if desired can be superscripted ALink to distinguish
them from other adjuncts.
The second type of linkage, realised by the linkers and, or, but,
is quite different. The position of these linkers is fixed and they
permit ‘branching’, thus:
£
I came in and he went out Cl Cl
Ind Ind
I came in and John went out
And in all of this we would have said nothing general about the
function of and, which is surely the same in all examples.
It seems, in fact, that linkers should not be regarded as constituents
in structure at all, but as realisation of a rank-free system of linkage.
It means in this case that where linkage occurs this is marked (&)
but not accorded constituent status. Thus:
I came in and John went out structure: Ind Cl & Ind Cl
big fish and little fish structure: nom grp & nom grp
fresh and bright structure: epithet & epithet
fish ’n chips base & base
If we now consider a sentence such as:
I stayed in and read a book because I had nothing to do and no
one to see but John went into town to see a friend.
the analysis is
74 SURFACE GRAMMAR
3
a
P
Dep & Dep
because I had nothing to do and no one to see
& a P
Ind Dep
but John went into town to see a friend.
Two points are evident from such an analysis: (i) clauses are not
merely strings of coordinated or subordinated clauses; clauses operate
at elements of sentence structure, the analysis
^-> a(3 & a(3
reflects the structure of the sentence better than
^ —>■ Ind & Ind Dep & Dep & Ind Dep.
(ii) it is evident that within one alpha element ellipsis of s is pos¬
sible:
I stayed and read a book
and within one beta element ellipsis of binding adjunct and subject
is possible:
because I had nothing to do and no one to see.
And it can be seen from such examples that a clause cannot always
be identified as operating at alpha or beta by its own structure. The
clause
I had no one to see
is a dependent clause at beta in a finked series of dependent clauses
at beta in:
because I had nothing to do and I had no one to see,
but is an independent clause operating at alpha in:
(i) I had nothing to do that day, and no one to see (finked series)
(ii) I had no one to see (simple sentence)
We might say that the s of the first clause in a finked series at
alpha or beta is the s of the alpha or beta element, and the binder in
the first beta clause is the binder of the beta element. When we
change s or binder we have a new element of sentence structure.
Thus:
CLAUSE AND SENTENCE 75
3
a
Ind Ind
I came in and John went out.
3
P a
but
P P a
MINOR SENTENCES
A A K
P c P c P c
On [Tuesday] About [nine.] For [a minute.]
s p A
Fog creeping into the cabooses of collier-brigs.
z z
e h d e h
Accidental death. A sure out.
RANKSHIFT
m h
II
P c=[ N]
The houses [by [the river]] . . .
If the nominal group the river had itself a further q element this
would involve multiple rankshift, a feature of some English registers.
SENTENCE ANALYSIS
$
a P
Ind Dep
s p A s p
The place where I was born was rebuilt after it had been bombed
figure 3
3
a
Ind
s p c A
N V N Ad
d h / d h P c
[N]
det noun verb det noun prep d h
det noun
The dog bit the boy in the arm
FIGURE 4
noun
FIGURE 5
a
Ind
s p cE
N V N
h / d h
pron verb det noun
He struck a blow.
a
1
1
Ind
1 ^
1
S p cE
| 1 1
1 1 1
N V N
1
h
1
/ d
/\ h
1 I I 1
1 1 1
pron verb det noun
I 1 1
1
He struck a blow
84 SURFACE GRAMMAR
FIGURE 6
3
a
Ind
s p cE c1
N V N N
dx d2 h / h d e h
det det noun verb pron det adj noun
All the boys called him a good player.
FIGURE 7
(iii) In the evening, Tom and Dick will visit the man who lives there.
a
Ind
A s P cE
Ad N & N V N
P c h h a / d h q
prep pr pr aux verb det noun ttcm
noun noun
s p A
[N] N V Ad
d h h / a
det noun rel verb adv
In the evening Tom and Dick will visit the man who lives there
a
86 SURFACE GRAMMAR
FIGURE 8
0 a
Dep Ind
A s p p cE cE
Ad N V V N N
a h / / h d h
adv pron verb verb pron det noun
When he comes give me a ring.
p-
I
Dep
ASP cE
I
Ad
I
N
I
V V
I
N
I
N
I
a
I
h
I
l /
I
h d
/ '
'h
o;
<u
v
.*0
t*.
as
w
e5
O
O
£
G
*
*«■ • » Lilli
The company, who now employ seventy persons, hope that work will begin at once and that the factory will be ready in time
3
Deep Grammar
(ii) the description will not show what else may have occurred at
any particular place, i.e. from what set of possibilities the particular
occurrence was selected; for example: that a nominal group the
boy represents a choice of singular from the set of possibilities
singular/plural, that a verbal group bites represents the choice
present from the set of possibilities present/past/future.
A further point may be made. Sentences such as It was John who read
the book present some difficulties for analysis. The simplest analysis
would seem to be:
s
It was John who read the book.
But this involves awkwardness in the further analysis of c: John is a
proper noun and as such would not be expected to have a q element,
which would seem to be the only possibility for who read the book.
Usually It is said to be a ‘dummy’ subject and the ‘real’ subject is
90 DEEP GRAMMAR
John. This explanation is well motivated; it certainly seems that a
description which assigns John to subject function is desired. The
explanation of ‘dummy’ and ‘real’ subject goes part of the way to¬
wards explaining such structures, but does not show the relatedness
of this sentence to other sentences, such as:
John read the book.
The book John read (but not the play).
The book was read by John.
It must be shown that the original sentence was a choice from a
number of possibilities, a set called ‘ theme ’ below.
The set of possibilities available at any place specified by the
surface grammar is called a system, and the individual choices are
called terms or features in the system. Thus:
System Terms Environment ('place on the chain ’)
(“Singular
Number- Subject/Complement
-*t Plural
“Present
Tense- -Past Predicator
-Future
(“Indicative
Mood- Major Clause
Hi-Imperative
There are two points to be noted about systems:
figure 10
Any clause which is major and independent must select from the
systems of mood, theme, transitivity and information which are
discussed individually in the following sections.
3.1.1 Mood
There are two points to be noted initially about this system:
(i) it is not to be confused with modality, which is a system whose
environment is the verbal group, and whose features are realised by
modal verbs;
(i) they are often (and may be always) separated from the rest of the
clause by a comma in the written language and an intonation break
in the spoken language. As in
Boys, stop playing when it gets dark
(cf. Boys stop playing when it gets dark).
(ii) Not only may such an element be separated from the rest of the
SYSTEMS AT CLAUSE RANK 95
clause in this way, but also it may appear in clause-final position.
As in
Stop playing when it gets dark, boys
(cf. *Stop playing when it gets dark boys).
(iii) Since imperative clauses cannot occur with certain (‘past¬
time’) adverbial groups, there is no possibility of an imperative
interpretation of:
Mother let us go to the pictures last night
(cf. Mother, let us go to the pictures tonight)
And because of restriction on ‘place’ adjuncts, there is no likelihood
of an imperative interpretation of:
Boys stop playing when it gets dark in China.
It seems best to consider such nominal groups as vocative ele¬
ments ; if anything is consistently present in imperative clauses it is the
feature vocative. The realisation of imperative is absence of subject,
and possible presence of a z vocative (zvoc) element in clause structure.
The initial choice in the mood system is thus between indicative
and imperative. If a clause is indicative, there is a further choice to
be made between declarative and interrogative. A declarative clause
has its subject preceding its predicator, as in:
s p
John is coming
p
The centre scored three goals
s c
He was called a saint.
An interrogative clause has either the subject included in the pre¬
dicator, or an initial wh element (i.e. who, which, where, when,
why, how), or both, as in:
p— s —P
Is John coming?
A P-_ s —p c
When will that centre score a goal?
s p c
Who will be the first to dive?
The Mood system may now be represented:
. rDeclarative
Indicative-
Mnterrogative
Mood-
•-Imperative
96 DEEP GRAMMAR
[r-WH->\
i-Interrogative-H
rSqu
l-Non-Squ
Indicative—m L-Non-WH
•-Declarative
Imperative
A problem in identification of mood features is occasioned by
clauses such as Here come my friends. In this clause the nominal
group my friends would appear to be subject, since a change of
number from plural to singular will necessitate concord with the
verbal group at p: Here comes my friend. Such clauses could be
considered a special type of declarative clause, marked by the presence
of the (unstressed) introductory adjunct. However, such clauses
occur without any introductory adjunct: said John. The main
difference between interrogative clauses and declarative clauses is
that the former have subject included in the predicator, the latter
have subject preceding the predicator; but if there is only one item
realising the verbal group at p then it is not possible to show the dis¬
tinction. There would seem to be possible ambiguity of mood
features. That is, a clause such as has John may, on what has so far
been considered, be either declarative or interrogative. It is ob¬
viously necessary to distinguish between ps declaratives and ps
interrogatives. The mood feature realisation depends on the item
SYSTEMS AT CLAUSE RANK 97
realising the verbal group: if the item is an auxiliary verb then the
clause is interrogative; if the item is a lexical verb then the clause is
declarative. Since ps interrogatives contain only an auxiliary verb
their occurrence will be restricted in the same way as the occurrence
of non-interrogative clauses with only an auxiliary verb in the
verbal group i.e. will be a case of substitution (pp. 42-43). Wemay
therefore incorporate the description of these ps interrogatives in the
description of auxiliaries, and consider other ps clauses as declarative.
We can now say that the feature declarative is realised by either sp
or ps in clause structure, the feature interrogative is realised by psp
in clause structure, and the feature imperative is realised by P with
no s in clause structure. The following pairs of clauses differ in
only one mood feature, whatever their differences in surface
structure:
A B
John comes John, come
indicative imperative
3.1.2 Theme
The preceding section was concerned with presence or absence of s
in clause structure, and with the various arrangements of s and p
as realisations of terms in the mood system. The position of a in
clause structure was discussed previously (pp. 63 ff.). The presence or
absence of c in clause structure is determined by the transitivity
systems, to be discussed in the next section; for the moment we may
say that if a c is present in clause structure it usually occupies the
position immediately after p. In other words, apart from ‘binding’
adjuncts—if, because, etc., and linkers—and, or—which are all
fixed in position and are not relevant to the theme system, we can
say that the usual order for elements of structure in a declarative
clause is s p c a. It is not the case, however, that this order of elements
98 DEEP GRAMMAR
is fixed in English; only that it is being taken as the most normal. We
can regard this as neutral or unmarked, and contrast other permitted
sequences against this norm. What is called the thematic element in
a clause is the first element which results from choice (i.e. excludes
the binders and linkers mentioned above, since their position is
fixed, and not a matter of choice).
We can, therefore, distinguish between clauses unmarked for
theme, and clauses which are marked for theme. An unmarked
theme will be realised by having the mood exponents in initial posi¬
tion; a declarative clause, for example, which has s in initial position
is unmarked for theme. A clause which has an element other than a
mood exponent in initial position will be marked for theme. The
primary distinction in the theme system is thus:
rMarked
Theme-
■-Unmarked.
We may now proceed to consider what different types of marked
theme occur in English.
The c in clause structure normally follows p, and if no A is present
it is clause-final. But the c may occur in clause-initial position. Such
structures are usually discussed in terms of‘inversion’; this name is
appropriate if it means inversion of the most usual order in English;
but if it is taken to mean (as it sometimes seems to be) that there is
some ‘breaking’ or ‘extending’ of grammatical rules involved it is
clearly inappropriate. It is often said that English word order is
fixed, but when examples are given to illustrate the point it is obvious
that it is not ‘ words ’ that are meant, but elements of clause structure
—spc. The elements of clause structure are variable in sequence,
and it is precisely because they are variable that values can be given
to certain sequences in contrast to other sequences which might have
occurred.
Clauses with initial c are perfectly grammatical in English. Thus:
c s p A
Football /’ ll play anytime
c s p A
Him the almighty power hurled headlong
s
These papers you can have (but leave those).
These clauses have marked theme, marked by initial position of c.
This is called c-theme (thematic c).
The possibility of A-theme is similar. The ‘lexical’ adjuncts which
SYSTEMS AT CLAUSE RANK 99
usually occur at position 3 (p. 64) can occur in initial position in
clause structure. The clause is marked for theme by this position of
adjunct. Thus:
On a former occasion we had discussed this
In the evening, we went down to the beach to play
Never has so much been owed f ASP
On the occasion of this anniversary, we drink to .. .
The third of these examples shows that position 2 adjuncts can also
be thematic, and when they are the subject of the clause it is in¬
cluded in the predicator; other examples are:
Seldom have I seen so poor a game
Rarely did I find anything interesting there.
This does not cause ambiguity with the included subject interroga¬
tive clause, because of the thematic adjunct marking the clause.
Often a thematic A correlates with an intonation break in the spoken
language, and this is reflected by the commas in the above examples.
In a declarative clause which is unmarked for theme the subject is
usually ‘ given ’ in that it has already been mentioned in the text or is
known in the situation. It is also usual for the tonic syllable (primary
stress) in a clause to fall on the last lexical item in the clause. This
is what we should expect: if the subject is ‘given’, is the theme of the
clause, then some other element is ‘new’ and so receives the high¬
light afforded by the tonic syllable. So in the clause John read the
book (i.e. not the play) (tonic ‘book’), John is already known and
book is the ‘new’ information and takes the tonic syllable. A subject
can, however, be marked as ‘new’ information by taking the tonic
syllable. John read the book (i.e. not Mary or Bill). This, indeed, is
only one of the possibilities of marked tonicity (pp. 101 Jf.); and could
be included in the information systems. It seems in place here as a
foregrounding of an element of clause structure similar to the fore¬
grounding of c and a discussed above. It is also the case that s-new
often correlates with the fourth, and final, term in the marked theme
types: predicated theme. Some discussion was entered into above
regarding the difficulty of a satisfactory analysis of It is John who
read the book. If we consider this to be the selection of predicated
theme as against non-predicated theme (i.e. John read the book),
then we can say that it is realised in surface structure as
It + ‘be’ -f s (of the non-p theme clause) + wh (i.e. appro¬
priate relative) + p (of the non-P theme clause) + c (of the
non-P theme clause).
100 DEEP GRAMMAR
And if we relate these clauses in this way they will be shown as closely
related, as differing in only one systemic feature:
John read the book indicative: declarative: non-p-theme
It was John who read the book indicative: declarative: p-theme.
We have, therefore, four possible types of marked theme in the
clause:
fs ‘new’
p-theme
rMarked- >
•c-theme
Theme- > •A-theme
-Unmarked.
The theme system applies to interrogative clauses also, and
declarative and interrogative clauses can be considered together in
relation to theme. It may, however, be clearer to add a separate
statement of interrogatives. In wh interrogative clauses, whether
Squ or non-Squ, the wh element normally occurs in initial position.
Thus:
Who is coming?
When is he coming?
Initial wh element is therefore unmarked for theme in interrogative
clauses. An interrogative clause may be marked for theme, if either:
[w . ,
Marked-►
twh non-initial
Lwh tome
Unmarked
3.1.3 Information
Connected speech is a sequence of stretches marked by phono¬
logical breaks. A. A. Hill (Introduction to Linguistic Structures,
New York, 1958) gives the example:
He will act roughly, in the same manner
He will act, roughly in the same manner,
and rightly observes that the commas in the written language reflect
the occurrence of intonation breaks in the spoken language. A
stretch of language between intonation breaks is a tone group.
Within any such tone group there will be one tonic syllable (‘pri¬
mary stress ’). Thus:
He will act roughly, in the same manner
He will act, roughly in the same manner.
Reaching over each tone group, and having its distinctive move¬
ment at the tonic syllable, is one of the possible tones in English.
There are thus three factors to be considered:
‘ stretches ’-►tone groups (tonality)
‘ stress ’-atonic syllable (tonicity)
‘ voice inflection ’-►tone (tone)
102 DEEP GRAMMAR
Such features can be established in phonology, without reference
to grammar. Difficulty in linguistic description is occasioned if such
features of intonation are taken to be direct exponents of gram¬
matical units and classes. The most common statement of this kind
is perhaps that one tone group = one clause. If we consider the ex¬
pression I didn’t go because he told me we know this may occur with
an intonation break after go:
I didn’t go 11 be cause he told me
or without such a break:
I didn’t go because he told me I /.
The difference is a meaningful difference; but it is not a difference
between two clauses and one clause. In either case the sentence
contains two clauses, an independent clause followed by a depen¬
dent clause. So with grammatical classes; it is often said that a
‘rising tone’ signals a question, i.e. an interrogative clause. How¬
ever:
(i) Is John doing it?
(ii) When will John do it?
(iii) John is doing it
may all be said with a ‘falling tone’, but (i) and (ii) are interrogative
and (iii) is declarative.
We can consider phonologic patterns from the ‘phonetic’ point
of view, and discover what phonological patterns are in fact avail¬
able in English; or we can consider phonological patterns from the
grammatical point of view, and discover what grammatical dis¬
tinctions are realised by features of intonation. The latter approach is
adopted here. The distinctions marked by intonation features are
taken to be meaningful and to be grammatical; they are just as much
formal contrasts as are those between (say) singular/plural, past/
present, indicative/imperative. A full description of intonationally
expounded systems would assign such distinctions to the appropriate
places in the grammar; some are concerned at group level, with
different classes of group, some at clause level and so on. The
primary distinctions realised by tonality and tonicity are glanced at
below. No attempt is made at a comprehensive description even at
primary delicacy; the intention is to show how such intonationally
realised choices are accommodated in the grammar (but see also
nominal group structure (pp. 33 Jf.), and the systems of contrast and
focus in the verbal group systems (pp. 139 Jf.)).
Tonality is the distribution of tone groups in an utterance; where
SYSTEMS AT CLAUSE RANK 103
such stretches begin and end. Tonality is obligatory in that speech
will necessarily contain tone groups, but the speaker has the option
as to where they will occur. There is a high correlation between tone
group and clause in English—though this, as was said earlier, should
not be raised to a definition of clause; indeed, it is only after a clause
has been identified that such a statement is possible. This correlation
can be utilised, and one tone group being coterminous with one
clause can be regarded as neutral tonality. Thus:
I didn’t go//because he told me.
There are then two marked possibilities; a tone group may be more
than a clause or less than a clause. The former occurs frequently
when a reporting followed by a reported clause occurs in sentence
structure, as in:
I asked Tom how it was done//
I told him why we did it that way//.
The second possibility, tone group less than a clause, occurs fre¬
quently before adjuncts in clause structure, as in:
He had become a teacher//in defiance of his father
He will act roughly/jin the same manner.
This is also very frequent with thematic adjuncts, as in:
On this great opening day//we must pay tribute to .. .
After a sumptuous meal,//we went back to the . . .
These stretches are called information units; so an information
unit equal to one clause is an unmarked information unit; an
information unit equal to less than or more than a clause is marked.
We can also recognise a neutral or unmarked term in tonicity.
When the tonic syllable is located on the last lexical item in the clause
then the tonicity is unmarked. Thus:
I meet him in the square
I meet him.
Marked tonicity occurs when the tonic falls on some other lexical
item or on a clause-final grammatical item. Thus:
/ meet him in the square
I meet him
It may be alright.
We can summarise this brief consideration of intonation features
by saying that there is a system of information which is concerned
with the distribution of information units (i.e. the location of tone
groups), and with the focusing on a point of information within each
such group (i.e. the location of the tonic syllable). Thus:
104 DEEP GRAMMAR
r Unmarked
Information-
3.1.4 Transitivity9
On pp. 57 ff. the unit clause was discussed in terms of transitivity;
this related to the presence or absence of the element c in clause
structure. The scale-category definition of transitivity is exemplified
in the following quote from Halliday:
The terms ‘intransitive, transitive’ refer to clauses with no c,
at least one c, one c and two c’s respectively.
The main difference between this and traditional accounts is that
here transitivity is being accounted for at clause rank, but traditionally
it is accounted for as a feature of the verb, i.e. at word rank (or, at
best, at (verbal) group rank). The traditional view, as expressed by
Treble and Vallins (An A.B.C. of English Usage, Oxford, 1936) is:
A verb is said to be used transitively when the action or state that
it denotes is regarded as ‘going over’ to, i.e. as directed towards,
an object. When the action or state is regarded as affecting only
the subject, i.e. as not directed towards an object, the verb is
called intransitive; in ‘birds fly’ the verb is intransitive, in ‘boys
fly kites’ it is transitive. Many verbs can thus be used transitively
or intransitively.
The difference between the approaches is not a trivial one. Consider¬
able research work is currently being carried out on the numerous
problems connected with transitivity in English. It is now recognised
that the surface concept of transitivity is the environment for a net¬
work of systems. The relationships between nominal elements of
clause structure (s and c) and verbal elements of clause structure
(p) are various, and are realisations of relationships between ‘par¬
ticipants’ and ‘processes’.
It is evident that the form of the verb alone will not indicate
whether it is transitive or intransitive in Treble and Vallins’ terms:
SYSTEMS AT CLAUSE RANK 105
in their examples birds fly, boys fly kites, the identification of fly as
transitive or intransitive is not made by reference to the form of the
verb fly; it is necessary to observe whether or not there is a c present
(an ‘object’ to which the action ‘passes over’). Since the identifica¬
tion of transitivity types thus requires the recognition of clausal
relations (pc or not pc) then it seems more appropriate to consider
transitivity as a feature of the clause rather than a feature of the
verb.
In any case, the many problems connected with what underlies
such clausal relationships are not revealed by such a definition as
that above. It is not, for example, evident that in
Boys fly kites
He threw the ball
He turned out a great player
quite different features are realised by the elements s and c, though
all the clauses are transitive.
Consideration of such problems perhaps depends on what we want
our grammar to account for; if we want a description of the physically
realised events of language, the various ‘ states ’ in which we find the
language, then a surface description of transitivity is perhaps
sufficient. But if we want to (attempt to) account for the relations
between the various ‘states’ of language, for what underlies the sur¬
face realisations, then something further is required.
Not only the problems posed by the different features realised
(the different ‘ roles ’ fulfilled by s and c) in clauses such as
He sank the boat
He eats fish
He became a teacher,
but the fact that He cuts easily is interpreted differently from He
cut dexterously and He shaves well is interpreted differently from
That barber shaves well is involved in the elucidation of problems of
transitivity.
It has been observed that in the clauses:10
(a) John polished the table
(b) i. The table polished well
ii. The table was polished (by John)
iii. The table was highly polished
(a) is the exception in terms of sequence. It seems reasonable to
assume that the relationship between the table and polished is con¬
stant, though in (a) the table is c and in (b) the table is s; and further,
106 DEEP GRAMMAR
that the relationship between John and polished is constant, though
in (a) John is s, and in (b) ii. John is optionally present in an a. It
follows that if the table and John are realisation of ‘participants’
connected with a process (polishing)—realised as the p of the clause—
then neither of these roles is tied to any one element of clause struc¬
ture.
We may summarise the argument by saying that traditional notions
of ‘actor-action-goal’ in English clauses have good foundation, but
it is important to note that we cannot generalise and say that in
English s = actor, c = goal. There are other types of participants
than the two above; and it is not the case with these that ‘actor’ is
always realised as s and ‘goal’ is always realised as c: it is only the
case that s = ‘actor’ and c = ‘goal’ in a particular type of clause,
and in another type of clause the s may realise the ‘ goal ’. What is
required is an account of different English clause types according
to the relationships realised by the nominal and verbal elements in
clause structure.
Such an account is, in part at least, a description of the systems
for which transitivity is the environment. An attempt to discuss these
is given in the following pages, beginning with the more obvious
distinctions and proceeding thereafter to some more delicate
distinctions. The account is not intended to be comprehensive, but
to illustrate and describe some of the principal options available in
the transitivity systems.
The discussion involves the use of nine examples as clause types,
and these may be listed here for convenience, with the numbering
which they retain throughout the discussion:
(i) The barber shaved ten customers
(ii) Ten customers were shaved
(iii) The soldiers marched
(iv) Mary seemed happy
(v) The sergeant marched the soldiers
(vi) The soldiers were marched
(vii) The barber shaved (sc. ‘himself’)
(viii) The barber shaved (sc. ‘ customers ’)
(ix) The customers shaved (‘ easily ’)
The discussion is broken into five sections for ease of presentation.
Section 1 concerns the primary distinctions, and involves clauses (i)
to (iv); section 2 concerns more delicate distinctions, and involves
also clauses (v) to (vii); section 3 concerns the remaining two
SYSTEMS AT CLAUSE RANK 107
distinctions, and involves clauses (viii) and (ix). Section 4 presents a
summary and discussion of the system network, and section 5 pre¬
sents a brief discussion of verb classification in relation to transitivity
systems.
SECTION 1
rEffective-^r0perat!ve 0)
[-Extensive-► LReceptive (11)
Transitivity—>\ ^-Descriptive (iii)
•-Intensive (iv)
section 2
Indeed, if the clause types discussed in section 1 were the only clause
types in English, then the above description would serve to identify
each type uniquely. But we also find clauses such as:
(v) The sergeant marched the soldiers
(vi) The soldiers were marched.
In surface structure, these resemble clauses (i) and (ii), and are
transforms of each other. They can obviously be labelled operative
and receptive:
Operative Receptive
(i) The barber shaved ten customers (ii) Ten customers were
shaved
(v) The sergeant marched the soldiers (vi) The prisoners were
marched
SYSTEMS AT CLAUSE RANK 111
There are, however, distinctions which show that while (i) and
(ii) are effective clauses as discussed above, (v) and (vi) are descrip¬
tive clauses, and are related to (iii) The soldiers marched.
Effective:operative clauses, such as (i), occur without an overt c
element, but are interpreted in just the same way as when the c
element is present. That is, clauses such as:
That barber shaves well
Mother washes on a Monday
occur regularly and are interpreted as meaning:
That barber shaves (people) well
Mother washes (the clothes) on a Monday
But clauses such as:
The sergeant marched
He sank
are not interpreted as meaning:
The sergeant marched the soldiers
He sank the boat
Semantically, native speakers agree that in the clause:
(i) The barber shaved ten customers
it is the barber who does the shaving', but in the clause:
(v) The sergeant marched the soldiers
it is the soldiers who do the marching (whether the sergeant does so
or not—the clause does not specify this). In other words, the actor is
realised as s in (i) but as c in (v). It is then evident that the sergeant
(i.e. the s) in (v) is fulfilling a different role, is realising a different
feature, from that in any of the other clauses. It is neither actor nor
goal: this feature is called initiator (or causer, reflecting the fact that
the combination of features (extensive) descriptive operative is
one way of expressing ‘causation’ in English). It would appear,
therefore, that there are three possible participant roles which can
be fulfilled by s in descriptive clauses. Thus:
(v) The sergeant marched the soldiers->s as initiator
(descriptive: operative)
(vi) The soldiers were marched->S as actor
(descriptive: receptive)
(iii) The soldiers marched->S as initiator/actor (i.e.
(descriptive: middle, the term combines the roles)
‘middle’ is used to distinguish
from operative and receptive).
Effective clauses, on the other hand, have:
112 DEEP GRAMMAR
(i) The barber shaved ten customers—>>S as actor
(effective: operative)
(ii) Ten customers were shaved->-s as goal
(effective: receptive).
The obvious question is whether there is a type of effective clause
which combines the roles of actor and goal in a middle form, as the
descriptive combines the roles of actor and initiator in a middle form.
In English we have clauses such as:
The barber shaved himself/He shaved himself
where the reflexive complement (cRe/) is treated as an extensive comple¬
ment in clause structure: the reflexive does not take the tonic syllable
in such clauses since it is a grammatical, not a lexical, item; it thus
differs from the ‘ emphatic ’ reflexive as in She asked the question her-
self (cf She asked herself the question). We also find clauses such as:
He shaved
He washed and dressed
which are interpreted as meaning:
He shaved himself
He washed and dressed himself
so that in:
(vii) He shaved
we have a combination of actor and goal in an effective: middle clause.
We can summarise the discussion thus far by saying that effective and
descriptive are different types of extensive clauses; both of these types
have an operative, a middle and a receptive form. Clauses which are:
effective:operative will have s as actor, c as goal
effective: middle will have s as actor and goal combined
effective deceptive will have s as goal and A as actor (option¬
ally)
descriptive: operative will have s as initiator and c as actor
(‘ causative ’)
descriptive: middle will have s as initiator and actor combined
descriptive: receptive will have s as actor and a as initiator
(optionally).
Diagrammatically:
SECTION 3
section 4
Thus, with a total of nine clause types, we can represent the whole
network as in Figure 11.
figure 11
^Effective
-Extensive |-- rGoal realised
f-» LDescripti
LGoal unrealised
Transitivity
-Operative
' Process-oriented
-Middle
LAgent-oriented
L Receptive'
L Intensive.
SYSTEMS AT CLAUSE RANK 115
And the description of the clauses will be:
(i) The barber shaved ten customers spc
extensive: effective/operative: goal realised
(ii) Ten customers were shaved sp
extensive: effective/receptive: agent-oriented
(iii) The soldiers marched SP
extensive: descriptive: middle
(iv) Mary seemed happy spc
intensive
(v) The sergeant marched the soldiers spc
extensive: descriptive: operative
(Vi) The soldiers were marched sp
extensive: descriptive: receptive
(vii) The barber shaved (sc. ‘himself’) sp
extensive: effective: middle
(viii) The barber shaved (‘people’) SP
extensive: effective: operative: goal unrealised
(ix) The customers shaved (‘ easily ’) sp
extensive: effective: receptive: process-oriented.
In the above descriptions, some of the clauses have the same
structural description (three have spc, six have sp) but each has a
unique systemic description. This means that if we consider structure
as the way in which systemic features are realised we can assign
unambiguous descriptions to each clause, but this is not so with
structural description alone. We can, in other words, predict struc¬
ture from system, but not system from structure: given the structural
description spc, it may be any one of three clause types above, and
the structural description sp may represent any one of six clauses
above; but given the feature specification
extensive: descriptive: operative
we can predict the structure spc, and we can further say that it will
be a ‘causative’ clause, with s as initiator and c as actor. From
the hearer’s position, of course, ambiguity is obviously possible
since it is structure he has to interpret. Halliday has pointed out
that:
Children don’t wash easily
may be interpreted as any of the following:
(vii) Children find it difficult to wash themselves
(viii) Children find it difficult to wash things
(ix) It is difficult to wash children.
116 DEEP GRAMMAR
Effective Descriptive
The barber shaved ten customers The soldiers marched
Ten customers were shaved The sergeant marched them
He shaved (sc. ‘himself’) The soldiers were marched
The barber shaved (‘well’)
The customer shaved (‘easily’)
(i) The occurrence of types (viii) and (ix) in the effective paradigm,
as against the non-occurrence of such types in the descriptive
paradigm. We can have in English:
she washes the clothes or she washes
she dressed herself or she dressed
where the alternatives are interpreted alike; but not
he sank the boat or he sank
he grows corn or he grows
without a difference in meaning.
(ii) In the effective paradigm, the operative clause may have either
a cE or a cRef, thus:
He kicked the ball: He kicked himself
He washed the dishes:He washed himself
This is not so with the operative in the descriptive paradigm, thus:
He marched the soldiers: *He marched himself
He sank the boat: *He sank himself
The fact that cRef can occur in descriptive: operative clauses with
certain accompanying adverbial elements of clause structure:
He marched himself down the road
He sank himself into the nearest chair
does not invalidate the important point that in effective: operative
clauses the cRef can occur without such adverbials, but this is not so
in descriptive clauses.
section 5
r Declarative rSqu.
rlndicative- i-WH-
-Interrogative- non-Squ.
r-Mood-
Lnon-WH
LImperative
■ s-new
■p-theme
-Marked —
■ c-theme
r-(Decl. theme) - a-theme
-Unmarked
-Theme
•wh non-initial
-Marked —
•wh initial, tonic
^(Interr. theme)-
-Unmarked
Major
Clause -Effective
■Goal-realised
rExtensive- Goal-unrealised
■Process-oriented
Transi¬
tivity ■Agent-oriented
-Intensive
■Greater
Marked—
Distribution- LLesser
Unmarked
Infor-_
mation Marked
F'ocus ■
-Unmarked
I
120 DEEP GRAMMAR
The unit Group provides the environment for further systems. The
following sections provide a first approach to a description of the
more prominent systems at Nominal and Verbal Group.
3.2.1.1 NUMBER
The system of number is usually regarded as a feature of word-
rank, and as a system having the terms singular and plural. That is:
_ [“Singular
Number-
^Plural
It is true that the terms in the number system are to a large extent
realised morphologically at word-rank; thus we can say:
boy = singular: boys = plural.
SYSTEMS AT GROUP RANK 121
As discussed earlier, however, not all nouns are overtly marked for
number, and those that are can be marked in different ways. There is
not one invariable criterion by which the terms singular and plural
are marked. Rather, there are a number of criteria, one or more of
which may be present in any instance.
Also, it is not the case that only nouns realise terms in the number
system. Words which operate at d and at o in the nominal group also
choose for number. Thus:
Singular Plural
that those
one two (etc.)
There are, therefore, nominal groups which contain a number
realisation at d and/or at o but not at h, and which can be thus
identified as singular or plural groups:
Singular Plural
this sheep those sheep
one sheep two sheep.
Some items which occur at d do not select for number {the, all, my,
etc.), and some items which occur at o do not select for number
(ordinal numerals—-first, second, third, etc.).
Two points emerge from such considerations. First, number
would seem to be more appropriately assigned to group rank, since
it can be realised at any one of three places in nominal group struc¬
ture. Second, since all three places can be filled by words which do not
choose for number, it is possible to have nominal groups which may be
described as uncountable, as opposed to nominal groups which do
choose for number, and which may be described as countable. The
system of number would therefore be more properly represented as:
Singular
Plural
Uncountable
The terms in the system will be realised by certain items at d, at o and
at h. Thus:
at d at o at h
Singular: a, this, that. one any singular noun
each, either, (i.e. any noun mor¬
every phologically marked)
Plural: these, those, two, three any plural noun (i.e.
many, few etc. any noun morpho¬
logically marked)
122 DEEP GRAMMAR *
NOUN
1. Either sg. 2. Always sg. 3. Always pi. 4. Unmarked
or pi.
boy beauty clergy sheep
lad courage people deer
bat despair dregs fish
horse mathematics bellows fowl
etc. etc. etc. etc.
PRONOUN
1. sg. 2. pi. 3. Unmarked
I/me we jus mine/our
hejhim theyj them you/your
she/her his/its
it herjtheir
3.2.1.2 case
The system of case in English nouns has two terms, a marked and
an unmarked term. The unmarked term is usually called the common
case and the marked term the possessive or genitive case. For the
marked case neither of the names is really suitable; the function
of the case is to mark relation certainly, but this relation may be one
of a number of types of relation, and certainly not only one of
possession. It would perhaps be better to use terms such as marked/
unmarked, but the names common and genitive are reasonably
neutral and are well established (though ‘possessive’ is inappro¬
priate). Thus:
124 DEEP GRAMMAR
*
Common Genitive
boy boy's
horse horse's
man man's
■men men's
boys boys'
daughters daughters'
3.2.1.3 GENDER
- Case- p Common
Nominal group- L Genitive
r Masculine
-Feminine
L Gender >
-Neuter
-Unmarked
128 DEEP GRAMMAR
-Non-selective the
3.2.3.1 FINITENESS
3.2.3.2 MODALITY
Since modal verbs are always finite it is evident that only finite verbal
groups will select for modality; and further, since finiteness is realised
at initial element in group structure, then if a modal verb is present
it will be the initial element. There is, in practice, no need to mark
a verbal group finite:modal since the marking modal will presume
marking finite. The choice in the system of modality is a choice
between a marked and an unmarked term, the presence of an element
contrasting with its absence, and can therefore be named modal
and non-modal. Thus:
System Terms Realisation {at initial element)
rModal-*any modal verb
Modality-►
LMon-modal—>-any other group
3.2.3.3 TENSE12
FIGURE 15
Present Past Future
Simple eats ate will eat
Present in is eating was eating will be eating
Past in has eaten had eaten will have eaten
Future in is going to eat was going to will be going to
eat eat
of the restrictions just given this means only four additional struc¬
tures, as shown in Figure 16.
In order to analyse the tense of a compound verbal group it is
figure 16
3.2.3.4 POLARITY
The system of polarity is a system with the terms positive and nega¬
tive, and in this system there is a one-to-one correlation realisation,
because the term negative is always realised by the negator (not, n't).
So this system is simply:
System Terms Realisation
rPositive->-any group without negator
Polarity-
•-Negative- ->not, n't
3.2.3.5 aspect
The system of aspect has been confused with that of tense, and in
certain respects the two do merge. But they are different systems
and should be kept apart in description. Aspect is perhaps best
considered primarily as a distinction between imperfective and
perfective. The difference between the clauses
(i) I have eaten the apple
(ii) I am eating the apple
is (apart from tense) that in (i) attention is drawn to the fact that the
action is completed, but in (ii) attention is directed to the fact that
the action is in progress, is continuing. The contrast is not, however,
between complete/incomplete in reference to a non-linguistic event.
138 DEEP GRAMMAR
3.2.3.6 voice
The system of voice in English has two terms, active and passive,
the former being unmarked and the latter realised by part of the
verb he + -en form of the verb. The system is simply thus:
System Terms Realisation
_ rActive->-any group not marked as passive
^-Passive-Kpart of) be + -en.
This system is obviously closely related to the transitivity systems,
since the distinction between operative (John washed the dishes)
and receptive (The dishes were washed by John) requires, among
other things, an active verbal group at p in the former and a passive
verbal group at p in the latter. But it was pointed out that the cor¬
relation was not one-to-one, because clauses such as
SYSTEMS AT GROUP RANK 139
This poem recites well
This coat buttons easily
It shows to advantage here
are receptive clauses (they have s as goal), though they have active
verbal groups. It would, of course, be possible to talk about receptive/
active and receptive/passive clauses; but this would be combining
features of clause and group rank. It seems more appropriate to
assign the features operative and receptive to clause rank, and the
features active and passive to group rank.
Some verbs do not admit of a passive form (see intensive clauses,
pp. 107 jf.). There are no passive equivalents of
He resembled his brother
He had a great set of cards.
3.2.3.7 CONTRAST
"Present
-Tense > -Past
-Future
Positive
-Polarity-
Verbal Negative
group'
Perfective
Aspect—
Imperfective
["Active
‘Voice —
L Passive
Modality
rContrastive->Focus-> Tense
Contrast-
^Non-contrastive L Polarity
(John) has been asked—finite, non-modal, past in present, positive,
perfective, passive, contrastive/polarity.
Other references:
quirk, r. The Use of English, London: Longmans, Green & Co., 1st
edition 1962, 2nd edition 1968
Bloomfield, l. Language, London: Allen & Unwin Ltd., 1935
anderson, j. ‘Ergative and Nominative in English’, in Journal of
Linguistics, Vol. 4, No. 1, 1968
Greenberg, j. h. Essays in Linguistics, University of Chicago Press,
1957
jespersen, o. A Modern English Grammar on Historical Principles,
Copenhagen, 1940
Index