The Effects of The Internal Control Opinion and Use of Audit Data Analytics On Perceptions of Audit Quality, Assurance, and Auditor Negligence
The Effects of The Internal Control Opinion and Use of Audit Data Analytics On Perceptions of Audit Quality, Assurance, and Auditor Negligence
DERECK BARR-PULLIAM
University of Wisconsin-Madison
[email protected]
HELEN L. BROWN-LIBURD
Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey
[email protected]
KERRI-ANN SANDERSON
Bentley University
[email protected]
August 2017
ABSTRACT:
Using advanced audit data analytics tools, public accounting firms propose that they can analyze
the entire population of accessible client transactions. While firms emphasize the potential benefits
to audit efficiency and effectiveness, they caution that this approach provides no greater than the
current level of “reasonable” assurance than does traditional auditing techniques. In the context of
a subsequent audit failure where an investor has initiated a law suit, we examine whether the audit
methodology (audit data analytics versus traditional auditing techniques) and the type of internal
control opinion (unqualified versus adverse) affect perceptions of audit quality and assurance and
perceptions of auditor negligence. In the wake of auditors issuing more adverse ICFR opinions,
there may be differences in how users perceive assurance levels and attribute blame in
circumstances of an audit failure. We develop our expectations using the theory of blame
attribution. Using a 2x2 full factorial experimental design, we predict and find that when auditors
issue an unqualified ICFR opinion, jurors assess auditors as more negligent when auditors employ
traditional auditing techniques (compared to audit data analytic techniques). In subsequent
analyses, we find that use of audit data analytic tools increases perceptions of audit quality, such
that jurors assess more blame to the plaintiff for their loss, assigning less negligence to the auditor.
We view this finding as indicative of the jurors believing that when the auditor uses audit data
analytics the auditor has exceeded the minimally expected audit testing techniques. Overall, our
study informs regulators, practitioners, and academics about the perceived assurance effects of
using advanced technological tools on the audit as well as the corresponding litigation effects.
KEYWORDS: auditor liability, audit data analytics, audit quality, statistical sampling
This study examines whether an auditor’s testing methodology and the internal controls
over financial reporting (ICFR) opinion affect jurors’ perceptions in a litigation setting.
Specifically, we examine whether indicators of audit quality such as the use of audit data analytics
(ADAs) and issuing an adverse ICFR opinion impact the perceived level of audit quality provided
by, and the amount of blame attributed to auditors when there is a subsequent audit failure. Two
important drivers of audit quality motivate our study. First, audit practitioners and standards setters
believe that leveraging technology to enable ADA techniques, such as testing a full population of
transactions versus a sample, can enhance the quality of financial statement audits. These ADAs
can be used in any phase of the audit, can manifest in various forms, and can revolutionize the
nature, timing, and extent of audit testing (Brown-Liburd, Issa, and Lombardi 2015; Cao,
Chychyla, and Stewart 2015). However, while audit practitioners tout the benefits of ADAs for
audit quality, they caution that these benefits increase neither the actual nor the perceived level of
assurance provided by these audits (EY 2014; KPMG 2014; Deloitte 2016; PwC 2016). While
current auditing standards only require auditors to collect audit evidence using traditional sampling
techniques (PCAOB 2016a), focus on the use of ADAs by auditors is increasing. As a result, it is
important to examine whether financial statement users perceive the use of ADAs as steps taken
to enhance audit quality and whether this enhancement results in fewer auditor negligence verdicts.
A second factor motivating our study is that auditors provide integrated audits attesting to
both the operating effectiveness of ICFR as well as the fairness of presentation of the financial
statements and related disclosures (PCAOB 2007). Audit quality is enhanced when the auditor’s
assessment of the ICFR operating effectiveness informs the level of substantive testing and
perceptions of the likelihood of a material misstatement in the financial statements. The ICFR
1
opinion also provides an immediate and public signal to financial statement users about auditors’
assessment of the quality of financial reporting and signals the quality of the auditor’s work
(Jennings, Pany, and Reckers 2008; Ashbaugh-Skaife, Collins, Kinney and LaFond 2009). For
example, when auditors issue an adverse ICFR opinion they signal financial statement users that
they identified one or more material weaknesses in the company’s system of internal controls and
that there is an increased potential for a material misstatement in the accompanying financial
statements.
Absent an audit failure or additional information such as a critical audit matter paragraph
(PCAOB 2017), the auditor’s report is silent about additional efforts auditors take to improve audit
quality such as the auditor’s testing methodology. In a jury trial, part of an auditor’s defense is a
2016a). We examine whether jurors’ awareness of the use of ADAs moderates the effect that the
We form our predictions based on blame attribution which is a subset of attribution theory
and describes how individuals determine who to hold responsible for an event such as an audit
failure. During a trial, jurors are aware that a misstatement occurred; they hear arguments from
both parties, learn more about the auditor’s approach, and must then determine who is to blame
for the plaintiff’s loss (Lowe and Reckers 1994; Kadous 2000, 2001; Clarkson, Emby, and Watt
2002; Peecher and Piercey 2008; Becker, Lawrence, and Sennetti 2009; Backof 2015). While use
of ADAs and an adverse ICFR opinion likely signal higher audit quality, we predict an interactive
effect of these signals on jurors’ auditor negligence verdicts. Specifically, absent a public and more
salient signal provided by an adverse ICFR opinion, jurors will assess auditor negligence as higher
after an audit failure when the auditor uses traditional sampling techniques relative to when they
2
use ADAs to execute audit testing and when auditors also issue an unqualified ICFR opinion.
auditor’s substantive testing methodology (ADAs versus traditional sampling) and the ICFR
opinion (unqualified versus adverse) and measured jurors’ assessments of auditor negligence. We
also measured jurors’ perceptions of audit quality and attribution of blame for the loss suffered by
investors. ADAs change both the nature of an audit and the extent of audit evidence gathered. Our
study narrows the focus to a type of ADA which utilizes advanced technology to test the population
as controls that examine whether all sales transactions in the sales journal are supported by a
customer order, shipping document, and invoice to validate the transaction information. Results of
this dual-purpose test allow auditors to identify both higher risk transactions and inefficiencies in
established internal control procedures; results also serve as a substantive test in the audit of the
financial statements. Each test could improve two key elements of audit quality: audit efficiency
and effectiveness.
find that the ICFR opinion has a direct effect on jurors’ negligence verdicts, such that an adverse
opinion results in lower perceptions of auditor negligence. We find no direct effect of the audit
expectations, we find an interactive effect which suggests that when an auditor issues an
unqualified ICFR opinion jurors assess negligence as higher when the auditor uses traditional
sampling techniques versus ADAs. In contrast, when auditors issues an adverse ICFR opinion,
3
jurors assess negligence no differently whether the auditor uses traditional sampling techniques or
ADAs.
To further explore these findings, we examine the mediating role of blame attribution on
jurors’ evaluations of auditor negligence. We measure the extent to which participants blame
auditors relative to the extent to which they attribute blame to management and to the investor
(i.e., the plaintiff in our setting). Results demonstrate that when auditors issue an adverse ICFR
opinion, jurors are less likely to hold the auditor responsible and perceive that both management
and the plaintiff are more responsible for the loss suffered by the plaintiff. When the auditor uses
ADAs, the plaintiff is viewed as relatively more responsible for the loss suffered. We view the
mediation results as suggesting that both the adverse ICFR opinion and the use of ADAs represent
strong signals of audit quality that collectively lower the likelihood that jurors perceive auditors as
negligent for a subsequent misstatement in the financial statements. Our supplemental analyses
also provide evidence consistent with the notion that the use of ADAs enhances audit quality and
evidence consistent with practitioners’ caution that such use provides no more than reasonable
Our study informs regulators, practitioners, and academics about the perceived assurance
effects of using one form of ADA in the audit and the impact of the ICFR opinion on litigation
outcomes after a misstatement in the financial statements. For regulators, our study suggests that
financial statement users view the use of ADAs, as measured in our study, as employing a higher
standard of due professional care and view ADAs as steps taken to ensure higher audit quality.
This finding is particularly useful to the PCAOB as it strives to better understand the effects of
ADAs on the audit process and to develop auditing standards that more widely promote such
innovation in audit engagements (e.g., Zhang, Pawlicki, McQuilken, and Titera 2012). In addition,
4
our study is consistent with prior research which suggests an adverse ICFR opinion provides a
salient and useful signal of audit and financial reporting quality (e.g., Wu and Tuttle 2014), but is
one of the first to demonstrate the subsequent effect of this signal on litigation outcomes. For audit
practitioners, our results lend credence to the notion that utilizing ADAs enhances audit quality;
our results also provide some support for the supposition that the increase in audit quality is not
associated with an increase in the perceived level of assurance beyond the standard of reasonable
assurance about whether the financial statements are free of material misstatement (PCAOB 2007).
For researchers, our study highlights how knowledge of auditors’ use of advanced technology to
enable testing that exceeds current auditing standards affects others’ perceptions of audit quality;
In the remainder of the paper, we review prior auditor litigation literature and develop
hypotheses. After this, we provide a summary of the experimental methodology and discuss
results. Finally, we offer concluding remarks and implications for future research.
In auditor negligence cases, jurors evaluate whether auditors exercised due professional
care in conducting the audit (Causey and Causey 1991). Due professional care (also referred to as
“standard of reasonable care”) suggests that the auditors applied a level of reasoning and judgment
like any other careful and competent auditor would in a similar circumstance (Kadous 2000).
Evaluating due professional care requires jurors to evaluate the steps auditors took to detect a
material misstatement and to consider any salient signals auditors provide about the likelihood of
To investigate juror decision making and the assessment of auditor negligence, we model
blame attribution by drawing on attribution theory. Attribution theory describes how individuals
5
assess and attribute responsibility and culpability (i.e., blame attribution) to individuals when there
is a failure (Jennings, Kneer, and Reckers 1993). Kelley and Michela (1980) illustrate both the
about the behavior of the individual and how the circumstances surrounding this behavior are used
by the decision maker to infer cause. Consequences are the expectations that the decision maker
forms related to the information and perceived cause. Specific to our study, jurors’ knowledge and
ICFR opinion and higher quality audit testing by using ADAs can influence jurors’ outcome
expectation and therefore, jurors’ attributions of responsibility for the subsequent audit failure. In
litigation, jurors evaluate each party’s role in the failure before attributing blame or causation to
one, some, or all parties. As such, we argue that under certain conditions, despite a subsequently
discovered material misstatement, jurors could still attribute less blame to the auditor. We develop
The American Institute of Certified Public Accountants (AICPA) defines ADAs as “the
science and art of discovering and analyzing patterns, identifying anomalies, and extracting other
useful information in data underlying or related to the subject matter of an audit through analysis,
modeling, and visualization for planning or performing the audit” (AICPA 2015). ADAs can be
used at any stage of the audit. The use of advanced testing methods such as ADAs could
significantly transform the process of auditing financial statements, resulting in enhanced audit
effectiveness and audit efficiency—both elements and signals of audit quality (Brown-Liburd et
al. 2015; Cao et al. 2015). Public Company Accounting Oversight Board (PCAOB) board member
Steven Harris recently echoed this sentiment suggesting that “these technological tools could allow
6
auditors to make better decisions and assessments throughout the audit…thereby improving audit
quality.” (PCAOB 2016c). However, the Big 4 firms are careful to acknowledge the positive and
negative implications for audit quality in the era of Big Data (e.g., Earley 2015; Liddy 2015; EY
inspection of the entire population of data underlying a company’s financial statements rather than
examining a limited number of transactions using the traditional sampling approach (Brown-
Liburd et al.; Cao et al. 2015; Murphy and Tysiac 2015; PwC 2015; PCAOB 201b). This approach
potentially results in a more effective and efficient audit and mitigates the efficiency-effectiveness
Overall, utilization of ADA tools and techniques provide audit firms the opportunity to
increase the sufficiency of audit evidenced gathered at a similar cost (Byrnes, Criste, Stewart, and
Vasarhelyi 2014); this results in higher audit quality and reduced audit risk. This technology
produces a number of potential exceptions from which auditors may further examine all or a
sample based on available audit resources. Alternatively, using traditional sampling requires
auditors to extrapolate the magnitude of any exceptions to the population. The former approach
allows auditors to focus on higher risk transactions that have the most potential to increase the risk
of material misstatement.
Little empirical evidence exists regarding the impact of ADAs on audit quality. However,
findings from recent studies suggest that some forms of ADAs such as Big Data visualizations
(Rose, Rose Sanderson, and Thibodeau. 2017) and continuous auditing (Barr-Pulliam 2017a,
2017b) affect perceptions of and actual audit quality. Given that ADAs allow auditors to develop
7
greater insights into a client’s business and financial statements, jurors could perceive auditors’
examining the entire population of a client’s transactions as more indicative of a higher quality
audit than when auditors employ traditional sampling techniques. Accordingly, jurors will likely
perceive auditors’ application of ADAs as going beyond the expected standard of performance
from auditors in an analogous situation (“due professional care”) and as taking steps to perform a
higher quality audit (Maksymov and Nelson 2017).1 In an audit failure setting, jurors could be less
likely to assess auditors as negligent when auditors employ ADA techniques and could attribute
this failure to factors outside the auditors’ control such as management collusion. Conversely,
employing ADAs could engender juror perceptions that a competent auditor should be able to
identify an existing misstatement when the auditors examine the entire population of a client’s
transactions.
then lead jurors to assess auditors as more negligent for the subsequent misstatement. Prior auditor
liability research suggest that jurors’ perceptions of auditor competence is both nuanced and
context-dependent when the quality of the audit varies. Awareness of the tactics auditors use to
improve detection of misstatements moderates how jurors assess auditor negligence. For example,
standards of care to which jurors hold auditors varies with the severity of negligence outcomes
(e.g., Kadous 2000; Arel, Jennings, Pany, and Reckers 2012). This prior research also suggests
that the timing of the assessment of auditors’ standard of care (i.e., before or after jurors learn
about steps the auditor took to improve audit quality) also affects jurors’ negligence verdicts
(Maksymov and Nelson 2017). Prior research is mixed, however, on the effect of audit quality on
negligence judgments (Kadous 2000; Reffett 2010; Arel et al. 2012; Maksymov and Nelson 2017).
1
We use the terms “due professional care” and “standard of care” interchangeably.
8
Our study specifically extends auditor negligence studies by Reffett (2010) and Maksymov
and Nelson (2017). Reffett (2010) manipulates audit quality as 1) whether auditors attend to a
fraud cue and 2) whether auditors perform limited or extensive investigation when they identify
the fraud cue. The results of his study suggest no effect of the extent of investigation when auditors
identify the fraud risk, but his results further suggest that jurors assess auditors as more negligent
when auditors identify a fraud risk than when they do not identify the risk and, therefore, performed
no testing. In a series of experiments, Maksymov and Nelson (2017) investigate findings presented
in Kadous (2000) related to jurors’ assessment of due professional care and findings in Reffett
(2010) related to the extent of investigation on juror assessments. The experiment that most closely
relates to our study manipulates audit quality as low or high and operationalizes quality by looking
at both a relatively small and large sample size to investigate an identified fraud risk. The
experiment also examines the timing of jurors’ assessment of due professional care as either before
or after jurors make negligence verdicts. Their findings suggest that, beyond the situational context
that characterizes the aforementioned studies, jurors’ perceptions of the extent to which auditors
actually met the expectation of a reasonable standard of care and jurors’ perceptions of auditors’
specific efforts to improve audit quality facilitate how jurors’ ex post knowledge of a misstatement
We specifically extend their study by examining the effect of audit quality in a context
where auditors use more advanced technology to examine the entire population of revenue
transactions. While Maksymov and Nelson (2017) operationalize high audit quality as a large
sample size versus lower audit quality as a small sample size, they essentially create a traditional
sampling context as compared to our context using ADAs versus traditional sampling.2
2
Our “high” condition is more extensive and more descript.
9
Taken together, these studies suggest that in certain contexts, taking steps to improve audit
quality can be perceived as positive by jurors. We expect that jurors, given their perception of what
is required by current auditing standards, will evaluate auditors’ application of ADAs as going
beyond the expected standard of performance and as taking steps to perform a higher quality audit.
Therefore, in a setting where there is a subsequent audit failure, we expect that jurors will assess
auditors as less negligent when auditors employ ADA techniques versus when auditors use
traditional sampling techniques to perform audit testing. We formally state this expectation in our
first hypothesis:
Internal controls over financial reporting (ICFR) opinion as a signal of audit quality
Accounting and auditing standards suggest that auditors should evaluate their clients’
internal controls over financial reporting (ICFR) to gain an understanding of the financial reporting
environment and to plan the nature, extent, and timing of substantive tests of transactions (e.g.,
SEC 1941; SAS No. 55, AICPA 1990; SAS No. 78, AICPA 1997; SAS No. 94, AICPA 2001).
These standards suggest that the auditor’s ICFR opinion should inform substantive testing and
perceptions of the likelihood of a material misstatement in the financial statements. The auditor’s
assessment of the operating effectiveness of ICFR is important both to financial statement auditors
and to stakeholders in the financial reporting process.3 The same expectation of reasonable
assurance applies to both the ICFR opinion and the opinion on the financial statements. An
unqualified ICFR opinion suggests that established controls may be effective in detecting an
existing material misstatement in the financial statements while an adverse ICFR opinion suggests
3
AS No. 5 (PCAOB 2007) classifies ICFR exceptions as either a control deficiency (least severe), a significant
deficiency, or a material weakness—which is severe enough to result in an adverse ICFR opinion.
10
that such controls may be ineffective. As such, factors that could support an adverse ICFR opinion
should alert the auditor to enhance substantive audit testing to determine if an actual misstatement
exists. The ICFR opinion also serves as a signal to investors about the quality of the financial
statements such that an adverse opinion signals that a misstatement may exist in the financial
statements investors rely on to make investment decisions (Jennings et al. 2008; Ashbaugh-Skaife
Regulators and audit practitioners recognize, and auditing research corroborates, that the
ICFR opinion is an integral signal and component of the quality of a company’s financial reporting
(Jennings et al. 2008; Asare, Fitzgerald, Graham, Joe, Negangard and Wolfe 2013). Prior research
finds that adverse ICFR reports issued by management are associated with reduced share prices
(Hammersley, Myers, and Shakespeare 2008) and higher costs of capital (Ashbaugh-Skaife et al.
2009). Research examining auditor ICFR audit opinions finds an adverse opinion is associated
with increased management turnover (Johnstone, Li, and Rupley 2011). Further, investors assess
higher risks of misstatement in the current financial statements and restatements, higher
information asymmetry and less transparency, higher risk premium and cost of capital, and lower
disclosures about auditor judgments and decisions during the ICFR and financial statement audits,
such as additional procedures used to increase the likelihood of detection of errors or fraud, affect
juror decision making. Focusing on the ex-ante disclosure, Brasel, Doxey, Grenier, and Reffett
(2016) find that jurors assess lower auditor negligence when the auditor signals financial reporting
quality concerns through disclosures made in a critical audit matter paragraph—an ex ante and
11
By issuing an adverse ICFR opinion, auditors inform financial statement users that they
identified one or more material weaknesses in the company’s system of internal controls and that
the material weakness was considered in determining the nature, timing, and extent of audit tests
applied in the audit of the financial statements. As such, we expect that jurors will perceive the
adverse ICFR opinion as a salient signal to investors that there is an increased potential for a
financial statement misstatement. Because of this signal, we expect that jurors will perceive
auditors’ judgments as acceptable and assign less blame to the auditor when a material
for establishing an effective system of internal controls, the ICFR opinion will serve as a salient
environment capable of detecting misstatements. Ceteris paribus, an adverse ICFR opinion signals
that management’s ineffective controls over financial reporting processes makes management at
least partially to blame (contributorily negligent) for material misstatements identified after
issuance of the financial statements. Therefore, we posit that in the instance of an adverse ICFR
opinion, jurors will assign less blame to the auditors. We formally state this expectation in the
following hypothesis:
The interaction of signals provided by audit methodology used and the ICFR opinion
Along with the ICFR opinion, the use of ADAs as an audit testing methodology is likely
to influence jurors’ perceptions of culpability and liability attributed to the auditor. Because ADAs
allow auditors to gain a greater understanding of a client’s operations and the financial reporting
process (Alles 2015; Yoon, Hoogduin, and Zhang 2015), jurors will likely regard auditors’ use of
ADAs as demonstrative of a higher quality audit compared to when auditors utilize traditional
12
auditing techniques. Consideration of both the ex ante (ICFR opinion) and ex post (auditor testing
methodology) disclosures about auditors’ judgments and decisions could have intended and
unintended effects on jurors’ negligence assessments. For example, Backof (2015) finds that
disclosure of the content of audit work paper documentation related to consideration of alternative
germane to our study, Backof additionally finds that when also considering how auditors link their
risk-based approach to specific audit tests, jurors award lower damage awards because they
perceive that the auditor used due professional care compliant with auditing standards prior to the
negligent act.
While the ICFR opinion is an ex ante signal, from a juror’s perspective, as posited in H2,
we also expect that in an adverse ICFR context there are likely several contributory factors that
constrain the effectiveness of audit testing regardless of the methodology auditors employ to detect
misstatements. Therefore, when there is an adverse ICFR opinion, we do not expect a difference
in jurors’ assessment of auditor negligence based on the auditor’s testing methodology (i.e.,
whether auditors perform a higher quality audit as perceived by using an ADA versus a statistical
sampling approach). However, absent a salient signal of ineffective ICFR, jurors may differentially
perceive the adequacy of auditors’ testing techniques. When auditors issue an unqualified ICFR
opinion there is no obvious signal of a potential material misstatement that alerts financial
statement users. In this context, jurors may perceive that auditors are less or more to blame for the
misstatement when auditors do or do not employ ADA techniques that jurors believe extend
beyond a reasonable standard of care compared to a traditional audit testing approach (Kadous
2000; Maksymov and Nelson 2017). We state this expectation in the following hypothesis:
13
HYPOTESIS 3. Jurors will assess auditors as more negligent after an audit failure when
the auditors employ traditional sampling versus ADAs and when auditors also issue
an unqualified ICFR opinion; however, there will be no difference in assessed
auditor negligence regardless of the auditors’ testing approach when auditors issue
an adverse ICFR opinion.
3. Research Design
Participants
Mechanical Turk (MTurk).4 Prior accounting studies also use MTurk participants (called workers)
as a proxy for jurors (Brasel et al. 2016; Grenier, Pomeroy, and Stern 2015; Grenier, Lowe, Reffett,
and Warne 2015; Peecher, Reffett, and Zimbelman 2016; Maksymov and Nelson 2017). A total of
800 workers successfully completed the task and provided an accurate randomly-generated survey
code. These participants received $1 ($800 total). Of 800 workers meeting the inclusion criteria,
222 (27.75%) failed to correctly answer both manipulation checks. The 578 participants who
answered both manipulation check questions correctly received a $0.50 bonus (total of $289).
Total earnings were $1,089 or $1.36. Participants completed the task in an average of 27 minutes,
as a result, our hourly rate of $3.03 [calculated as ($1.36/27 minutes) x 60 minutes] exceeds the
Of our 800 participants, the mean age was 37.1 years old; 58.0% were female, and 14.8%
had a graduate degree. Approximately 11.6% had prior experience serving on a jury, and 23.3%
had prior investment experience. Participants in our sample are comparable with those in recent
studies eliciting juror negligence judgments in auditor litigation cases (e.g., Lowe, Reckers, and
Whitecotton 2002; Backof 2015; Gimbar, Hansen, and Ozlanski 2016). Our participants were
4
Brandon, Long, Loraas, Mueller-Phillips, and Vansant (2014) and Farrell, Grenier, Leiby (2016) discuss online
survey design and provide recruitment tools.
14
appropriate given our research goals (Libby, Bloomfield, and Nelson 2002; Elliott, Hodge,
Experimental materials
In Stage One of the experiment (see Appendix 1), we screened potential participants to
assess jury eligibility. Participants moved to Stage Two if they self-reported that: 1) they were
U.S. citizens, 2) they were at least 18 years of age, and 3) they had taken no more than 2 accounting
or finance courses. After reading and accepting the informed consent agreement and reading
background information provided in Stage Two, participants were presented with information
about the financial statement auditing process. The instrument explains the internal controls over
financial reporting audit, the relationship between internal controls and the substantive testing
procedures in the financial statement audit, as well as key terms such as “reasonable assurance”
and “due professional care.” The instrument also explains the meaning of an unqualified (“clean”)
audit versus an adverse audit opinion for the internal controls over financial reporting as well as
the financial statement audits. We used comprehension check questions for each of these concepts
to ensure participants understood the overall steps auditors perform in the integrated financial
statement audit process and the related auditing terms. Participants were required to answer these
comprehension check questions correctly before they could advance to the next stage of the study.
Participants who answered incorrectly or failed to respond to these questions were not allowed to
continue with the study. This approach ensured that participants understood key auditing concepts
that we described in the case prior to examining the transcript of the legal proceedings.
5
Randomization among the treatment groups was successful. Untabulated analysis of demographic variables
indicate no statistically significant differences between treatment groups for gender, prior jury service, occupation
(whether a lawyer or an investor), age, or level of education.
15
In Stage Three, participants were randomly assigned to one of four experimental conditions
(described below). Each participant assumed the role of a juror and read the transcript of a
hypothetical jury trial related to an audit failure. This hypothetical case is summarized as follows:
The hypothetical company, Rapid Shipping, is a publicly traded shipping company for which the
audit firm issued an unqualified opinion on the financial statements as well as an ICFR opinion
(either clean or adverse). After the issuance of the audit report, a Securities and Exchange
Commission investigation suggested that there was a material misstatement of revenue not
identified by the auditors. A large pension fund investor (the plaintiff) sues the auditor for
negligence alleging that the audit failure was a result of failure to exercise due professional care.
The audit firm contends it used an audit testing methodology that conforms to auditing standards
and exercised sufficient professional judgment. After reading the trial transcript, participants were
given jury instructions and responded to our primary dependent measures and supplemental and
demographic questions.
Independent variables
vs. adverse) and audit methodology (statistical sampling vs. ADA testing). In the ICFR opinion
(OPINION) condition participants were told that the audit firm issued either an unqualified opinion
or an adverse opinion for the audit of internal controls over financial reporting, meaning that the
auditors either did not or did identify weaknesses in internal controls specifically related to internal
controls over the revenue recognition processes. We coded OPINION as “1” when the auditors
issued an adverse ICFR opinion and “0” otherwise. For audit methodology (METHOD) one
population of participants learned that the audit firm used audit statistical sampling techniques to
examine a portion of the shipping transactions. Other participants learned that the audit form used
16
data analytics software to examine the entire population of shipping transactions. We coded
METHOD as “1” when auditors used ADAs and “0” otherwise. In the ADA testing condition, it
was made clear to participants that auditors were using software to execute audit testing.
Participants were told that the audit approach allowed the auditors to draw conclusions about both
the effectiveness of internal controls over shipping transactions and the validity (e.g., occurrence)
Dependent variables
auditor negligence (GUILT). Participants first indicated whether they believed the auditor was
guilty or not guilty (VERDICT; coded as “1” for guilty, and “0” for not guilty). Participants who
selected “guilty” were then asked to indicate the extent of guilt on an anchored scale where 50%
= somewhat completely guilty and 100% = completely guilty. Participants who selected “not
guilty” were asked to respond to a corresponding scale where 50% = somewhat completely not
guilty and 100% = completely not guilty. We recoded and combined the two measures to create
the continuous measure of guilt on a scale ranging from 0% (completely not guilty) to 100%
(completely guilty).7
4. Results
Manipulation check
First, we asked participants to recall the auditor’s testing methodology. We asked, “What
approach did the auditor use to test sales revenue?” Of 800 participants meeting the inclusion
criteria, 666 (83.25%) answered correctly. Second, we asked participants to recall the auditor’s
6
The audit procedure used in the experiment served as a dual-purpose test (i.e., a test of sales transactions designed
to evaluate the effectiveness of controls and detect monetary misstatements).
7
Following Hoffman, Joe, and Moser (2003) and Joe (2003), our two scales exclude 0 - 50% as assessments below
50% guilty effectively signal not guilty verdicts while assessments less than 50% not guilty signal guilty verdicts.
17
ICFR opinion. We asked, “What opinion did Smith CPAs (the auditor) assess regarding the
manipulations for both audit methodology (χ2 = 330.3, p < 0.001) and ICFR opinion (χ2 = 376.3, p
< 0.001). A total of 761 (95.13%) participants passed at least one, and as previously indicated 578
(72.25%) passed both manipulation checks. Our inferences are qualitatively similar excluding
those that passed one or both manipulation checks and we find no significant or systematic
differences along the demographic dimensions or across any of the experimental conditions. Our
Descriptive statistics
attribution and auditor negligence. Overall, 35.1% of jurors found the auditor guilty and 64.9%
found the auditor not guilty of negligence. Jurors were more likely to find the auditor guilty of
negligence when the auditor used traditional sampling techniques and issued an unqualified ICFR
audit opinion (mean = 42.2%). In contrast, jurors found auditors less negligent when the auditor
issued an adverse ICFR audit opinion, regardless of the audit methodology used (mean = 31.8%
Tests of hypotheses
8
The failure rate for each manipulation check is comparable to prior research using electronic survey methods (e.g.,
Andrews, Nonnecke, and Preece 2003; Oppenheimer, Meyvis, and Davidenko 2009) and to paper-and-pencil surveys
(e.g., Kongsved, Basnov, Holm-Christensen, and Hjollund 2007).
18
We tested our hypotheses with an ANOVA model that uses a continuous measure of
auditor negligence (GUILT) as the dependent variable and audit testing methodology (METHOD)
and ICFR opinion (OPINION) as independent variables. Table 3 presents the results of the
ANOVA. Recall that HYPOTHESIS 1 examines whether jurors’ assessments of GUILT will vary
based on the methodology (METHOD) auditors used to test revenue transactions. As noted in panel
A of Table 3, we do not find a significant main effect for METHOD (F1, 796 = 0.08, p = 0.388).
Thus, HYPOTHESIS 1 is not supported. This result suggests that ex post knowledge of the testing
approach employed by auditors, in isolation, does not significantly influence jurors’ evaluation of
auditor negligence. While the audit testing approach may be an important factor that informs
negligence evaluations, jurors likely include additional contextual cues to interpret the associated
implications.
HYPOTHESIS 2 examines whether jurors assess auditors as less negligent when auditors
issue an adverse ICFR opinion or more negligent when auditors issue an unqualified ICFR opinion.
This suggests a main effect for OPINION in our ANOVA model. Descriptive statistics in Table 2
and Figure 1 show a mean for GUILT of 36.04% in the adverse opinion condition and 41.15% in
the unqualified opinion condition. As noted in panel A of Table 3, we find support for
HYPOTHESIS 2 as ANOVA model results show a main effect of OPINION on GUILT (F1, 796 =
4.67, p = 0.016).9 This result indicates significantly lower negligence assessments when auditors
issue an adverse ICFR opinion and suggests that jurors interpret the ICFR opinion as a signal of
9
We also use binary logistic regression to examine the main effects of METHOD and OPINION on the dichotomous
dependent variable VERDICT and find qualitatively similar results.
19
Finally, HYPOTHESIS 3 predicts jurors will assess auditors as more negligent when they
employ traditional sampling versus ADA techniques and issue an unqualified ICFR opinion;
however, there will be no differences in negligence assessments when auditors issue an adverse
analysis to investigate these results. As such, we expected a significant planned contrast comparing
contrast when OPINION would be adverse.10 Consistent with our prediction, panel B of Table 3
shows that jurors assess auditors as significantly more negligent when auditors use traditional
sampling versus ADAs and also issue an unqualified ICFR opinion (mean difference = 6.28; p =
0.030).11 We also find, as predicted, no difference in METHOD when auditors issue an adverse
ICFR opinion (mean difference = -4.94; p = 0.069). Consistent with our theory, we find differences
in jurors’ perceptions of audit testing quality across ICFR opinion contexts. When auditors issue
an unqualified ICFR opinion, jurors perceive ADA testing techniques as higher audit quality
relative to traditional sampling techniques (means = 7.93 vs. 7.38, respectively; p = 0.014, two-
tailed; untabulated). However, when auditors issue an adverse ICFR opinion, jurors perceive no
difference in the quality of the audit based on the audit testing methodology (means = 7.73 [ADA]
vs. 7.58 [Sampling]; p = 0.487, two-tailed; untabulated). These findings suggest that jurors
perceive the ICFR opinion as a signal and that they use the ICFR context and their interpretation
10
Experimental groups tested included: Unqualified-Traditional Sampling (Cell 1); Unqualified-Audit Data Analytics
(Cell 2); Adverse-Traditional Sampling (Cell 3); and Adverse-Audit Data Analytics (Cell 4). For H3, we use the
following contrasts: Cell 1 > Cell 2: 1, -1, 0, 0 and Cell 3 = Cell 4: 0, 0, 1, -1.
11
Following Guggenmos, Piercey, and Agoglia (2016) contrast analysis guidance, we analyze residual between-
cells variance and report p-values adjusted for unequal variances.
20
While prior studies have investigated the effects of audit documentation on jurors’
judgments (e.g., Backof 2015), we examine how the ICFR opinion and audit testing methodology
affect negligence assessments. Our results are consistent with the literature and provide further
evidence that the ICFR opinion is an important signal of potential problems and of the overall
quality of the financial reporting (e.g., Ashbaugh-Skaife et al. 2009). However, we demonstrate
that this perception of quality is more pronounced when the auditor fails to signal a potential
misstatement. Further, we find that the internal controls context differentially affects jurors’ focus
on the quality of the audit testing techniques auditors employ and jurors’ assessments of auditor
negligence. When there is no salient internal controls signal suggesting poor financial reporting
quality, we find that jurors evaluate the use of ADAs as a higher quality testing technique that
demonstrates auditors’ intentions to conduct a quality audit (e.g., Reffett 2010; Maksymov and
Nelson 2017).
Additional analyses
Blame attribution
To further investigate our results, we examine the extent of blame jurors attribute to
auditors, management, and the plaintiff after an audit failure. We use the Preacher and Hayes
(2008) multiple mediator model to determine the effect of OPINION on GUILT through three
measures of BlameAttribution (auditor, management, and plaintiff; see Figure 2). To derive
BlameAttribution, we asked jurors to indicate each party’s responsibility for the plaintiff’s loss on
a 10-point scale anchored on 1 (Not at all responsible) and 10 (Completely responsible).12 Using
all three measures in our mediation analysis allows us to determine the effect of each measure
12
The Blame Auditor scale differs in that it is anchored on 1 (Not at all caused) and 10 (Completely caused).
21
As noted in Figure 2, results of the mediation analysis indicate that the effect of OPINION
Blame Auditor (p = 0.001; 95 CI [-0.928, -0.226]), and Blame Plaintiff (p < 0.001; 95 CI [0.282,
0.987]) is significant. The effects of Blame Auditor (p < 0.001; 95 CI [8.055, 9.419]), and Blame
Plaintiff (p = 0.003, 95 CI [-1.911, -0.576]) on GUILT are also significant. We find that the
BlameAttribution measures fully mediate the relationship between OPINION and GUILT (total
effect of OPINION on GUILT p = 0.031; direct effect p = 0.481).14 These findings provide
evidence that jurors’ ascription of blame to different constituents in the audit process influences
their negligence assessments. Essentially, jurors perceive an adverse ICFR opinion as a warning
from auditors to financial statement users about the quality of the financial reports which investors
should consider when making investment decisions. As such, jurors believe that the plaintiff shares
some responsibility for the loss incurred and abates jurors’ assessment of auditor negligence.15
In untabulated results, we also parse our overall mediation analyses by audit testing
methodology to determine whether, like our ANOVA results for the interaction (HYPOTHESIS
3), the results are driven by auditors’ use of traditional statistical sampling relative to ADAs. For
traditional statistical sampling, we find similar and, in some instances, stronger results than our
overall mediation analysis. We find that the effect of OPINION on GUILT is mediated by each of
13
Statistical inferences are unchanged when we use our binary measure of guilt (VERDICT).
14
Sobel’s tests are significant for judgments of the probability of negligence for Blame Auditor (z = -3.20, p =
0.001) and Blame Plaintiff (z = -2.49, p = 0.013).
15
We also find that jurors blame auditors more in the unqualified versus adverse ICFR opinion conditions (means =
4.86 vs. 4.29, respectively; p < 0.001, one-tailed). Additionally, jurors assess management as being more responsible
when the ICFR opinion is adverse versus unqualified (means = 7.16 and 6.87, respectively; p = 0.033, one-tailed);
which suggests that jurors believe that management is responsible for implementing effective controls over financial
reporting and, therefore, at a minimum contributorily negligent when these controls are deemed ineffective.
22
Blame Auditor (p = 0.002), Blame Management (p = 0.027), and Blame Plaintiff (p < 0.005) is
significant. The effects of Blame Auditor (p < 0.001), Blame Management (p = 0.002), and Blame
Plaintiff (p = 0.007) on GUILT are all also significant. The total (main) effect of OPINION on
GUILT is significant (p = 0.001) as we noted in the ANOVA in Table 3 panel A. Indicating full
= .314). Conversely, when the auditor uses ADAs, the main effect of OPINION on GUILT is
insignificant (p = .871); thus mediation analysis is not appropriate. Taken together, these results
suggest that when the auditor uses ADAs as a testing methodology, the type of ICFR opinion does
not have a differential effect on auditor guilt. In other words, under conditions where the auditor
employs techniques that are perceived to increase the likelihood of detecting misstatements, jurors
To further investigate our results, we analyze jurors’ overall perceptions of audit quality
and assurance that the financial statements are free from material misstatement. We contend that
use of ADAs as a testing methodology signals a higher quality audit.16 In untabulated results, we
find that jurors perceive audit quality as being higher when the auditor uses ADAs versus
traditional statistical sampling audit testing procedures (means = 7. 83 vs. 7.48; p = 0.026, two-
tailed).
Lastly, to examine whether jurors perceive that auditors provide relatively more assurance
when they employ ADAs versus traditional sampling, we examine jurors’ perceptions of assurance
16
Participants responded to the question “Did Smith CPAs intend to conduct a quality audit by using the audit
testing approach they used to perform sales revenue testing?” on a scale ranging from 1 (Not at all intended to
conduct a high-quality audit) to 10 (Completely intended to conduct a high-quality audit).
23
that: 1) internal controls are operating effectively17 and 2) the financial statements are free from
assurance when the auditor uses ADAs versus traditional statistical sampling audit testing
procedures (means = 6.05 vs. 5.79, respectively; p = 0.136, two-tailed; untabulated). Related to
misstatements in the financial statements, we also find that jurors perceive no difference in
assurance when the auditor uses ADAs versus traditional statistical sampling audit testing
procedures (means = 5.84 vs. 5.77, respectively; p = 0.419, two-tailed; untabulated). Taken
together, these audit quality and assurance findings suggest that jurors agree with auditors’
contention that ADAs lead to comparatively higher quality audits but provide no greater assurance
5. Conclusion
In this study, we examine whether and to what extent auditors’ use of audit data analytics
in the substantive testing phase of the audit and the type of internal control over financial reporting
(ICFR) opinion affect both the perceived level of audit quality and jurors’ assessment of auditor
negligence after an audit failure. We develop our predictions based on attribution theory and
particularly blame attribution. To test our expectations, we conducted a 2x2 between-subjects full
factorial experiment where we manipulated the auditor’s testing methodology (audit data analytics
versus traditional sampling) and the ICFR opinion issued (unqualified versus adverse). We also
measured, as a mediating factor, the extent of the attribution of blame for a negligence suit filed
17
Participants responded to the question “What level of assurance that internal controls are operating effectively do
you think Smith CPAs actually provided?” on a scale ranging from 1(No Assurance) to 10 (Absolute Assurance).
18
Participants responded to the question “What level of assurance that the financial statements are free of material
misstatement do you think Smith CPAs actually provided?” on a scale ranging from 1(No Assurance) to 10
(Absolute Assurance), emphasis included in the original instrument provided to participants.
24
participants, we note three main findings. First, we find that the ICFR opinion directly affects how
jurors assess negligence and suggests that adverse opinions provide a signal to financial statement
users of the potential for financial reporting related issues. These adverse opinions result in lower
perceptions of auditor negligence. Second, we find that when auditors issue an unqualified ICFR
quality, jurors make higher negligence assessments when auditors employ traditional statistical
sampling techniques than when they employ ADA techniques. Lastly, mediation analysis indicates
that the effects of the ICFR opinion on jurors’ assessment of auditor negligence is explained by
jurors’ attribution of blame among auditors, management, and the investors who incur a loss by
relying on the financial statements (i.e., the plaintiff). We find that when auditors issue an adverse
ICFR opinion, jurors attribute less blame to auditors (correspondingly, more blame to management
and the investor) for the audit failure resulting in a financial loss thereby assessing auditors as less
guilty of negligence.
Our study is one of the first to directly examine whether the use of more advanced audit
methodologies enhances jurors’ perceptions of audit quality and assurance. Proponents contend
that the use of ADAs will not only enhance audit effectiveness and efficiency but will also result
in reduced audit risk and liability because auditors will be able to achieve a higher level of
assurance. Our results suggest that jurors indeed perceive higher audit quality when auditors use
ADAs, especially in situations where auditors face litigation exposure in an audit failure context.
This study potentially has implications for regulators interested in additional audit quality
indicators and factors to consider if auditing standards require revision to encourage or support
auditors’ leveraging of technology to enhance the efficiency and effectiveness of the audit. Our
study has implications for practitioners interested in or currently using ADAs techniques in the
25
audit process, despite audit practitioners’ assertions that use of these techniques will have no effect
on financial statement users’ perceptions of audit quality and the level of assurance provided.
Our study also contributes to the auditing literature regarding the effects of ICFR opinion
disclosure as well as factors that affect jurors’ negligence judgments. In line with what
Hammersley et al. (2008) find regarding the affect disclosure of ICFR opinions have on stock
prices, we find that the disclosure of ICFR opinions provide a context within which jurors
differentially attribute blame for an audit failure, interpret auditors’ efforts to improve audit
quality, and influence jurors’ negligence decisions. Our study also provides avenues for future
research on the effects of audit testing disclosure and complements prior and contemporaneous
research on the use of technology to enhance the audit process (e.g., Brown-Liburd et al. 2015;
26
References
Alles, M.G., 2015. Drivers of the Use and Facilitators and Obstacles of the Evolution of Big
Data by the Audit Profession. Accounting Horizons, 29(2), pp.439-449.
American Institute of Certified Public Accountants (AICPA). 2001. The Effect of Information
Technology on the Auditor’s Consideration of Internal Control in a Financial Statement
Audit. Statement on Auditing Standards (SAS) No. 94. New York, NY: AICPA.
American Institute of Certified Public Accountants (AICPA). 2007. The Auditor’s Consideration
of Fraud in a Financial Statement Audit. SAS No. 99, edited by American Institute of
Certified Public Accountants (AICPA). New York, NY: AICPA.
American Institute of Certified Public Accountants (AICPA). 2015. Audit Analytics and
Continuous Audit, Looking Toward the Future. New York, NY: AICPA, pp 92-93.
www.aicpa.org/interestareas/frc/assuranceadvisoryservices/downloadabledocuments/
auditanalytics_lookingtowardfuture.pdf
Andrews, D., B. Nonnecke, and J. Preece. 2003. Electronic survey methodology: A case study in
reaching hard-to-involve internet users. International Journal of Human-Computer
Interaction 16 (2): 185-210.
Arel, B., M. M. Jennings, K. Pany, and P.M. Reckers. 2012. Auditor liability: A comparison of
judge and juror verdicts. Journal of Accounting and Public Policy 31 (5): 516–32.
Ashbaugh-Skaife, H., D. W. Collins, W. R. Kinney Jr. and R. LaFond. 2009. The effect of SOX
internal control deficiencies on firm risk and cost of equity. Journal of Accounting
Research 47: 1–43.
Backof, A. G. 2015. The impact of audit evidence documentation on jurors' negligence verdicts
and damage awards. The Accounting Review 90 (6): 2177-2204.
27
Barr-Pulliam, D. 2017a. Internal auditors’ perceptions of the effects of continuous auditing and
their dual role on the likelihood of management opportunism. Working Paper. University
of Wisconsin-Madison.
_____. 2017b. The effects of internal audit quality and earnings management option complexity
on managerial discretion in financial reporting. Working Paper. University of Wisconsin-
Madison.
Becker, C. P., H. J. Lawrence, and J. T. Sennetti. Does the change to principles-based accounting
increase juror assessments of auditor liability? Advances in Accounting Behavioral
Research 4.
Brasel, K.R., Doxey, M.M., Grenier, J.H. and Reffett, A. 2016. Risk disclosure preceding negative
outcomes: The effects of reporting critical audit matters on judgments of auditor
liability. The Accounting Review 91 (5): 1345-62.
Brown-Liburd, H. L., H. Issa, and D. Lombardi. 2015. Behavioral implications of big data's impact
on audit judgment and decision making and future research directions. Accounting
Horizons 29 (2): 451-68.
Byrnes, P., Criste, T., Stewart, T. and Vasarhelyi, M., 2014. Reimagining Auditing in a Wired
World. AICPA White Paper, 11. Available at:
http://www.aicpa.org/InterestAreas/FRC/AssuranceAdvisoryServices/DownloadableDoc
uments/Whitepaper_Blue_Sky_Scenario-Pinkbook.pdf
Cao, M., R. Chychyla, and T. Stewart. 2015. Big Data analytics in financial statement audits.
Accounting Horizons 29 (2): 423-29.
Causey Jr, D. S. and S. Causey. 1991. Duties and Responsibilities of Public Accountants.
Mississippi State, MS: Accountant’s Press.
Clarkson, P. M., C. Emby, and V. W.-S. Watt. 2002. Debiasing the outcome effect: The role of
instructions in an audit litigation setting. AUDITING: A Journal of Practice & Theory 21
(2): 7-20.
Deloitte. 2016. Quality and Innovation: Enhancing the Audit. White Paper. Available at:
http://www2.deloitte.com/global/en/pages/about-deloitte/articles/gr14-audit.html.
Earley, C.E., 2015. Data analytics in auditing: Opportunities and challenges. Business Horizons,
58 (5): 493-500.
28
Elliot, W. B., F. Hodge, J. Kennedy, and M. Pronk. 2007. Are MBA students a good proxy for
nonprofessional investors? The Accounting Review 82 (1): 139-68.
Ernst & Young. 2014. Big data: Changing the way businesses compete and operate. White Paper.
Available at:
http://www.ey.com/Publication/vwLUAssets/EY_-_Big_data:_changing_the_way_
businesses_operate/$FILE/EY-Insights-on-GRC-Big-data.pdf.
Ernst & Young. 2015 How big data and analytics are transforming the audit. Available at:
http://www.ey.com/Publication/vwLUAssets/ey-reporting-issue-9/$FILE/ey-reporting-
issue-9.pdf#page=10
Farrell, A. M., J. H. Grenier, and J. Leiby. 2016. Scoundrels or stars? Theory and evidence on the
quality of workers in online labor markets. The Accounting Review, Forthcoming.
Gimbar, C., B. Hansen, and M. Ozlanski. 2016. The effects of critical audit matter paragraphs and
accounting standard precision on auditor liability. The Accounting Review, Forthcoming.
Grenier, J. H., B. Pomeroy, and M. Stern. 2015. The effects of accounting standard precision,
auditor task expertise, and judgment frameworks on audit firm litigation exposure.
Contemporary Accounting Research 32 (1): 336–57.
Grenier, J. H., D. Lowe, A. Reffett, and R. Warne. 2015. The effects of expert panel
recommendations on juror judgments of auditor negligence. Auditing: A Journal of
Practice & Theory 34 (4): 157–70.
Guggenmos, R., M. D. Piercey, and C. Agoglia. 2016. Making sense of custom contrast analysis:
Seven takeaways and a new approach. Working Paper. Cornell University, University of
Massachusetts Amherst.
Hammersley, J.S., L.A. Myers, and C. Shakespeare. 2008. Market reactions to the disclosure of
internal control weaknesses and to the characteristics of those weaknesses under section
302 of the Sarbanes Oxley Act of 2002. Review of Accounting Studies 13 (1): 141-65.
Hoffman, V., J. Joe, and D. Moser. 2003. The effect of constrained processing on auditors’
judgments. Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (7): 699-714.
Horton, J., and L. Chilton. 2010. The labor economics of paid crowdsourcing. In Proceedings of
the 11th ACM Conference on Electronic Commerce, 209–218. Available at:
http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=1807376
Jennings, M. M., K. Pany, and P. M. J. Reckers. 2008. Internal control audits: Judges' perceptions
of the credibility of the financial reporting process and likely auditor liability. Advances in
Accounting 24 (2): 182-90.
29
__________., Kneer, D.C. and Reckers, P.M., 1993. The significance of audit decision aids and
precase jurists' attitudes on perceptions of audit firm culpability and
liability. Contemporary Accounting Research, 9(2), pp.489-507.
Joe, J. 2003. Why press coverage of a client influences the audit opinion. Journal of Accounting
Research 41 (1): 109-33.
Johnstone, K., C. Li, and K. H. Rupley. 2011. Changes in corporate governance associated with
the revelation of internal control material weaknesses and their subsequent remediation.
Contemporary Accounting Research 28: 331–83.
Kadous, K. 2000. The effects of audit quality and consequence severity on juror evaluations of
auditor responsibility for plaintiff losses. The Accounting Review 75 (3): 327–41.
Kelley, H.H. and Michela, J.L., 1980. Attribution theory and research. Annual Review of
Psychology, 31(1), pp.457-501.
Kongsved, SM., Basnov, M., Holm-Christensen, K., & Hjollund, N.H. (2007). Response rate and
completeness of questionnaires: A randomized study of internet versus paper-and-pencil
versions, Journal of Medical Internet Research 9 (3): e25.
KPMG. 2014. Data & Analytics: Unlocking the Value of Audit. White Paper. Available at:
https://home.kpmg.com/xx/en/home/insights/2015/02/audit-data-analytics-unlocking-
value-of-audit.html.
KPMG. 2012 Leveraging data analytics and continuous auditing processes for improved audit
planning, effectiveness, and efficiency. White Paper. Available at:
https://assets.kpmg.com/content/dam/kpmg/pdf/2016/05/Leveraging-Data-Analytics.pdf
Libby, R., R. Bloomfield, and M. W. Nelson. 2002. Experimental research in financial accounting.
Accounting, Organizations and Society 27 (8): 775–810.
Lopez, T. J., S. Vandervelde, and Y. Wu. 2009. Investor perceptions of an auditor’s adverse
internal control opinion. Journal of Accounting and Public Policy 28 (3): 231-50.
Lowe, D.J., and P.M.J. Reckers. 1994. The expectation gap in the legal system: perception
differences between auditors and judges, Journal of Applied Business Research 10
(Summer): 39-44.
30
__________________________, and S.M. Whitecotton. 2002. The effects of decision-aid use and
reliability on jurors' evaluations of auditor liability. The Accounting Review 77 (1): 185-
202.
Maksymov, E. M. and M. W. Nelson. 2017. Malleable standards of care required by jurors when
assessing auditor negligence. The Accounting Review, Forthcoming.
Murphy, M. L., and K. Tysiac. 2015. Data analytics helps auditors gain deep insight. Available
at: http://www.journalofaccountancy.com/issues/2015/apr/dataanalyticsforauditors.html
Peecher, M. E., and M. D. Piercey. 2008. Judging audit quality in light of adverse outcomes:
Evidence of outcome bias and reverse outcome bias. Contemporary Accounting Research
25 (1): 243-74.
Peecher, M. E., A. Reffett, and A. Zimbelman. 2016. Juror dysfunction: Can auditors reduce the
penalty for narrowly failing to detect fraud? Working paper, University of Illinois, Miami
University, and University of South Carolina.
Preacher, K.J. and Hayes, A.F., 2008. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and
comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research
Methods, 40(3), pp.879-891.
PricewaterhouseCoopers (PwC). 2015. PwC transforms audit practice with data analytics.
Available at: http://www.accountingtoday.com/blogs/debitscredits/news/pwctransforms
auditpracticedataanalytics764351.htm
_____. 2016. Integrated and Financial Statement Audits: Simplifying the Audit Through
Innovation. Available at: http://www.pwc.com/us/en/audit-assurance-services/financial-
statement-audit.html.
Public Company Accounting Oversight Board. 2007. AS 5: An Audit of Internal Control Over
Financial Reporting That Is Integrated with An Audit of Financial Statements.
Washington, DC.
_____. 2016b. AS 1015: Due Professional Care in the Performance of Work. Washington, DC.
_____. 2016. Board Member Steven Harris’ address to the NYSSCPA SEC Conference. New
York, NY. Accessed online at: https://pcaobus.org/News/Speech/Pages/Harris-speech-
NYSSCPA-10-25-16.aspx.
31
_____. 2017. AS 3101: The Auditor's Report on an Audit of Financial Statements When the Auditor
Expresses an Unqualified Opinion. Washington, DC.
Reffett, A. B. 2010. Can identifying and investigating fraud risks increase auditors’ liability? The
Accounting Review 85:21-45.
Rose, A., J. Rose, K. Sanderson, and J. Thibodeau. 2017. When should audit firms introduce
analyses of Big Data into the audit process? Journal of Information Systems, Forthcoming.
Securities and Exchange Commission (SEC). 1941. Amendment of Rules 2-02 and 3-07 of
Regulation S-X. Accounting Series Release No. 21, 11. Fed. Reg. 10921. (February 5)
Washington, DC: GPO.
Wu, Y., and B. Tuttle. 2014. The interactive effects of internal control audits and manager legal
liability on managers’ internal controls decisions, investor confidence, and market prices.
Contemporary Accounting Research 31 (2): 444-68.
Yoon, K., Hoogduin, L. and Zhang, L., 2015. Big data as complementary audit
evidence. Accounting Horizons, 29(2), pp.431-438.
Zhang, L., A. R. Pawlicki, D. McQuilken, and W. R. Titera. 2012. The AICPA assurance
services executive committee emerging assurance technologies task force: The audit data
standards (ADS) initiative. Journal of Information Systems 26 (1):199-205.
32
TABLE 1
Participant demographics
33
TABLE 2
Descriptive statistics
Audit Methodology
Variable Statistical Sampling Audit Data Analytics Total
N = 84 N = 68 N = 152
Guilty (41.6 %) (34.3%) (38.0%)
N = 118 N = 130 N = 248
Unqualified Not Guilty (58.4%) (65.7%) (62.0%)
ICFR Audit Opinion
34
35
TABLE 3
Tests of Hypotheses
45.00%
Extent of Guilt (Continuous Measure)
44.25%
43.00%
41.00%
38.52%
39.00%
37.98%
37.00%
35.00%
33.58%
33.00%
Traditional Sampling Audit Data Analytics
Standardized Coefficients (t-statistics and p-values, two-tailed) shown for each path. *Denotes p-values are significant at least at the 0.05 level.
OPINION = ICFR audit opinion issued (0=Unqualified, 1=Adverse)
GUILT = Jurors assessment of auditor negligence ranging from 0% (Completely not guilty) to 100% (Completely guilty)
BLAME VARIABLES: All measured on a 10-point scale anchored on 1 (Not at all responsible) and 10 (Completely responsible)
• BLAME AUDITOR = “In your opinion, did the auditor’s actions (Smith CPAs) cause the plaintiff’s (Bierhoff Pension Fund) loss?”
• BLAME MANAGEMENT = “In your opinion, to what extent is management of Rapid Shipping (the company) responsible for the subsequent
misstatement in sales revenue?”
• BLAME PLAINTIFF = “In your opinion, to what extent is Bierhoff Pension Fund (the plaintiff) responsible for the losses that they suffered?
38