ELS 102 Theories of Language Module 12-15
ELS 102 Theories of Language Module 12-15
1
• The parietal lobes manage sensation, handwriting, and body position.
• The temporal lobes are involved with memory and hearing.
• The occipital lobes contain the brain's visual processing system.
The brain is surrounded by a layer of tissue called the meninges. The skull (cranium) helps
protect the brain from injury.
Brain Conditions
• Headache: There are many types of headaches; some can be serious but most are
not and are generally treated with analgesics/painkillers.
• Stroke (brain infarction): Blood flow and oxygen are suddenly interrupted to an area
of brain tissue, which then dies. A blood clot, or bleeding in the brain, are the cause
of most strokes.
• Brain aneurysm: An artery in the brain develops a weak area that swells, balloon-like.
A brain aneurysm rupture can causes a stroke.
• Subdural hematoma: Bleeding within or under the dura, the lining inside of the skull.
A subdural hematoma may exert pressure on the brain, causing neurological
problems.
• Epidural hematoma: Bleeding between the tough tissue (dura) lining the inside of the
skull and the skull itself, usually shortly after a head injury. Initial mild symptoms can
progress rapidly to unconsciousness and death, if untreated.
• Intracerebral hemorrhage: Any bleeding inside the brain.
• Concussion: A brain injury that causes a temporary disturbance in brain function.
Traumatic head injuries cause most concussions.
• Cerebral edema: Swelling of the brain tissue in response to injury or electrolyte
imbalances.
• Brain tumor: Any abnormal tissue growth inside the brain. Whether malignant
(cancer) or benign, brain tumors usually cause problems by the pressure they exert
on the normal brain.
• Glioblastoma: An aggressive, malignant brain tumor (cancer). Brain glioblastomas
progress rapidly and are very difficult to cure.
• Hydrocephalus: An abnormally increased amount of cerebrospinal (brain) fluid inside
the skull. Usually this is because the fluid is not circulating properly.
• Normal pressure hydrocephalus: A form of hydrocephalus that often causes problems
walking, along with dementia and urinary incontinence. Pressures inside the brain
remain normal, despite the increased fluid.
• Meningitis: Inflammation of the lining around the brain or spinal cord, usually from
infection. Stiff neck, neck pain, headache, fever, and sleepiness are common
symptoms.
• Encephalitis: Inflammation of the brain tissue, usually from infection with a virus. Fever,
headache, and confusion are common symptoms.
• Traumatic brain injury: Permanent brain damage from a traumatic head injury.
Obvious mental impairment, or more subtle personality and mood changes can
occur.
• Parkinson's disease: Nerves in a central area of the brain degenerate slowly, causing
problems with movement and coordination. A tremor of the hands is a common early
sign.
• Huntington's disease: An inherited nerve disorder that affects the brain. Dementia
and difficulty controlling movements (chorea) are its symptoms.
2
• Epilepsy: The tendency to have seizures. Head injuries and strokes may cause
epilepsy, but usually no cause is identified.
• Dementia: A decline in cognitive function resulting from death or malfunction of
nerve cells in the brain. Conditions in which nerves in the brain degenerate, as well as
alcohol abuse and strokes, can cause dementia.
• Alzheimer’s disease: For unclear reasons, nerves in certain brain areas degenerate,
causing progressive dementia. Alzheimer’s disease is the most common form of
dementia.
• Brain abscess: A pocket of infection in the brain, usually by bacteria. Antibiotics and
surgical drainage of the area are often necessary.
Brain Tests
• Computed tomography (CT scan): A scanner takes multiple X-rays, which a
computer converts into detailed images of the brain and skull.
• Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI scan): Using radio waves in a magnetic field, an
MRI scanner creates highly detailed images of the brain and other parts of the head.
• Angiography (brain angiogram): A special substance doctors call "a contrast agent"
is injected into the veins, and travels into the brain. X-ray videos of the brain are taken,
which can show problems in the brain's arteries.
• Magnetic resonance angiography (MRA): A special MRI scan of the brain's arteries.
An MRA scan may show a blood clot or another cause for stroke.
• Lumbar puncture (spinal tap): A needle is inserted into the space around the spinal
nerves, and fluid is removed for analysis. Lumbar puncture is often done if meningitis
is suspected.
• Electroencephalogram (EEG): Brain activity is monitored through electrodes placed
on the skin on the head. EEG can help diagnose seizures, or other brain problems.
• Neurocognitive testing: Tests of problem-solving ability, short-term memory, and other
complex brain functions. Usually, neurocognitive testing is done through
questionnaires.
• Brain biopsy: In rare situations, a very small piece of the brain is needed to make the
diagnosis of a brain condition. Brain biopsies are generally done only when the
information is needed to provide proper treatment.
Brain Treatments
• Thrombolytics: Clot-busting medicines injected into the veins can improve or cure
some strokes if given within a few hours after symptoms start.
• Antiplatelet agents: Medicines like aspirin and clopidogrel (Plavix) help prevent blood
clots. This can reduce the chance of a stroke.
• Cholinesterase inhibitors: These medicines can improve brain function slightly in mild
or moderate Alzheimer’s disease. They do not slow or prevent Alzheimer’s disease.
• Antibiotics: When a brain infection is caused by bacteria, antibiotics can kill the
organisms and make a cure more likely.
• Levodopa: A medicine that increases brain levels of dopamine, which is helpful in
controlling symptoms of Parkinson’s disease.
• Brain surgery: An operation on the brain can cure some brain tumors. Brain surgery
may be performed any time increased pressure in the brain threatens brain tissue.
• Ventriculostomy: A drain is placed into the natural spaces inside the brain (ventricles).
Ventriculostomy is usually performed to relieve high brain pressures.
3
• Craniotomy: A surgeon drills a hole into the side of the skull to relieve high pressures.
• Lumbar drain: A drain is placed into the fluid around the spinal cord. This can relieve
pressure on the brain and spinal cord.
• Radiation therapy: If cancer affects the brain, radiation can reduce symptoms and
slow the cancer's growth.
Babbling
Within a few weeks of being born the baby begins to recognize it’s mothers’ voice.
There are two sub-stages within this period. The first occurs between birth – 8 months. Most
of this stage involves the baby relating to its surroundings and only during 5/6 – 8 month
period does the baby begin using it’s vocals. As has been previously discussed babies
learn by imitation and the babbling stage is just that. During these months the baby hears
sounds around them and tries to reproduce them, albeit with limited success. The babies
attempts at creating and experimenting with sounds is what we call babbling.
When the baby has been babbling for a few months it begins to relate the words
or sounds it is making to objects or things. This is the second sub-stage. From 8 months to
12 months the baby gains more and more control over not only it’s vocal communication
but physical communication as well, for example body language and gesturing.
Eventually when the baby uses both verbal and non-verbal means to communicate, only
then does it move on to the next stage of language acquisition.
The second stage of language acquisition is the holophrastic or one word stage.
This stage is characterized by one word sentences. In this stage nouns make up around
50% of the infants vocabulary while verbs and modifiers make up around 30% and
questions and negatives make up the rest. This one-word stage contains single word
utterances such as “play” for “I want to play now”. Infants use these sentence primarily
to obtain things they want or need, but sometimes they aren’t that obvious.
For example a baby may cry or say “mama” when it purely wants attention. The
infant is ready to advance to the next stage when it can speak in successive one word
sentences.
4
Two-Word Stage
The two word stage (as you may have guessed) is made of up primarily two word
sentences. These sentences contain 1 word for the predicate and 1 word for the subject.
For example “Doggie walk” for the sentence “The dog is being walked.” During this stage
we see the appearance of single modifiers e.g. “That dog”, two word questions e.g.
“Mummy eat?” and the addition of the suffix –ing onto words to describe something that
is currently happening e.g. “Baby Sleeping.”
Telegraphic Stage
The final stage of language acquisition is the telegraphic stage. This stage is
named as it is because it is similar to what is seen in a telegram; containing just enough
information for the sentence to make sense. This stage contains many three and four
word sentences. Sometime during this stage the child begins to see the links between
words and objects and therefore overgeneralization comes in.
Some examples of sentences in the telegraphic stage are “Mummy eat carrot”,
“What her name?” and “He is playing ball.” During this stage a child’s vocabulary
expands from 50 words to up to 13,000 words. At the end of this stage the child starts to
incorporate plurals, joining words and attempts to get a grip on tenses.
As a child’s grasp on language grows it may seem to us as though they just learn
each part in a random order, but this is not the case. There is a definite order of speech
sounds. Children first start speaking vowels, starting with the rounded mouthed sounds
like “oo” and “aa”. After the vowels come the consonants, p, b, m, t, d, n, k and g. The
consonants are first because they are easier to pronounce then some of the others, for
example ‘s’ and ‘z’ require specific tongue place which children cannot do at that age.
As all human beings do, children will improvise something they cannot yet do. For
example when children come across a sound they cannot produce they replace it with
a sound they can e.g. ‘Thoap” for “Soap” and “Wun” for “Run.” These are just a few
example of resourceful children are, even if in our eyes it is just cute.
Introduction to Syntax
What image do you have in your mind when I say the following words: Venetian
blind? You picture a common window covering, right? Now what happens to that image
when I switch up the order of the words: blind Venetian? It creates a completely different
5
idea, doesn't it? As this example shows, the order in which we use words can be very
important! The term that refers to the order or sequencing of words in a language is
syntax.
Studies show that syntax is learned as young children are exposed to speech with
proper, complex sentence structure. So, how does this process take place? Before babies
say their first word, they have made a lot of progress towards understanding language
and speech. A young child listens and attempts to imitate the sounds it hears. In turn, we
respond to and reinforce these attempts at speech. A young child does not develop this
ability all at one time. Instead, the process consists of a series of developmental stages.
These stages are typically divided into two categories: pre-linguistic and linguistic.
Before a child masters the ability to form words, they will first begin to use specific
sound combinations consistently with specific meaning. This is the early one word period
that begins around 12-19 months of age. An example of this would be a child saying
'baba' every time he wants a bottle of milk. Even though this is not the exact same as the
word bottle, the child is using 'baba' in the same manner as you would use the word
'bottle.'
The later one word period begins around 14-24 months of age. In this stage, the
words used by the child are readily identifiable, and he begins to name and label people
and objects in his environment. A child's typical vocabulary during this period will consist
of words like 'dog,' 'go,' 'daddy' and 'bye-bye.'
6
Next comes the two word period of language development. As the name implies,
this is when he will begin to combine two words together to make simple phrases, such
as 'mommy go' or 'shoe on.' The two word period typically begins from 20-30 months of
age.
The three word period begins around the ages of 28-42 months of age. During this
period, a child adds at least one more word to their phrases and begins to use pronouns.
They may also begin to use articles and simple prepositions. Examples would be 'me go
daddy,' 'you on chair' or 'he kick a ball.'
At around 34-48 months of age, the four word period begins. At this time, the child
will begin to use combinations of four to six words. They will use more prepositions, and
adjectives begin to appear in speech. Examples would be 'Suzy has a little dog' or 'I sleep
on the top bunk.'
Lesson Summary
7
child is not yet able to manipulate these sounds into proper words. Pre-linguistic language
development can be divided into four categories: vegetative sounds, cooing and
laughter, vocal play and babbling.
Behaviourism
The behaviourist psychologists developed their theories while carrying out a series
of experiments on animals. They observed that rats or birds, for example, could be taught
to perform various tasks by encouraging habit-forming. Researchers rewarded desirable
8
behaviour. This was known as positive reinforcement. Undesirable behaviour was
punished or simply not rewarded – negative reinforcement. The behaviourist B. F. Skinner
then proposed this theory as an explanation for language acquisition in humans. In
Verbal Behaviour (1957), he stated:
"The basic processes and relations which give verbal behaviour its special
characteristics are now fairly well understood. Much of the experimental work responsible
for this advance has been carried out on other species, but the results have proved to
be surprisingly free of species restrictions. Recent work has shown that the methods can
be extended to human behaviour without serious modifications." (cited in Lowe and
Graham, 1998, p68)
Skinner suggested that a child imitates the language of its parents or carers.
Successful attempts are rewarded because an adult who recognises a word spoken by
a child will praise the child and/or give it what it is asking for. Successful utterances are
therefore reinforced while unsuccessful ones are forgotten.
Limitations of Behaviourism
While there must be some truth in Skinner's explanation, there are many objections
to it. Language is based on a set of structures or rules, which could not be worked out
simply by imitating individual utterances. The mistakes made by children reveal that they
are not simply imitating but actively working out and applying rules.
For example, a child who says "drinked" instead of "drank" is not copying an adult
but rather over-applying a rule. The child has discovered that past tense verbs are formed
by adding a /d/ or /t/ sound to the base form. The "mistakes" occur because there are
irregular verbs which do not behave in this way. Such forms are often referred to as
intelligent mistakes or virtuous errors.
9
Few children receive much explicit grammatical correction. Parents are more
interested in politeness and truthfulness. According to Brown, Cazden and Bellugi (1969):
"It seems to be truth value rather than well-formed syntax that chiefly governs explicit
verbal reinforcement by parents - which renders mildly paradoxical the fact that the
usual product of such a training schedule is an adult whose speech is highly grammatical
but not notably truthful." (cited in Lowe and Graham, 1998)
There is evidence for a critical period for language acquisition. Children who
have not acquired language by the age of about seven will never entirely catch up. The
most famous example is that of Genie, discovered in 1970 at the age of 13. She had been
severely neglected brought up in isolation and deprived of normal human contact. Of
course, she was disturbed and underdeveloped in many ways. During subsequent
attempts at rehabilitation, her careers tried to teach her to speak. Despite some success,
mainly in learning vocabulary, she never became a fluent speaker, failing to acquire the
grammatical competence of the average five-year-old.
Innateness
Chomsky concluded that children must have an inborn faculty for language
acquisition. According to this theory, the process is biologically determined - the human
species has evolved a brain whose neural circuits contain linguistic information at birth.
The child's natural predisposition to learn language is triggered by hearing speech and
the child's brain is able to interpret what s/he hears according to the underlying principles
or structures it already contains. This natural faculty has become known as the Language
Acquisition Device (LAD). Chomsky did not suggest that an English child is born knowing
anything specific about English, of course. He stated that all human languages share
common principles. (For example, they all have words for things and actions - nouns and
verbs.) It is the child's task to establish how the specific language s/he hears expresses
these underlying principles.
For example, the LAD already contains the concept of verb tense. By listening to
such forms as "worked", "played" and "patted", the child will form the hypothesis that the
past tense of verbs is formed by adding the sound /d/, /t/ or /id/ to the base form. This, in
turn, will lead to the "virtuous errors" mentioned above. It hardly needs saying that the
process is unconscious. Chomsky does not envisage the small child lying in its cot working
out grammatical rules consciously!
10
"It seems to me that the child is born not with a set of linguistic categories but with
some sort of process mechanism - a set of procedures and inference rules, if you will -
that he uses to process linguistic data. These mechanisms are such that, applying them
to the input data, the child ends up with something which is a member of the class of
human languages. The linguistic universals, then, are the result of an innate cognitive
competence rather than the content of such a competence." (cited in Russell, 2001)
1. Slobin has pointed out that human anatomy is peculiarly adapted to the
production of speech. Unlike our nearest relatives, the great apes, we have evolved a
vocal tract which allows the precise articulation of a wide repertoire of vocal sounds.
Neuro-science has also identified specific areas of the brain with distinctly linguistic
functions, notably Broca's area and Wernicke's area. Stroke victims provide valuable
data: depending on the site of brain damage, they may suffer a range of language
dysfunction, from problems with finding words to an inability to interpret syntax.
Experiments aimed at teaching chimpanzees to communicate using plastic symbols or
manual gestures have proved controversial. It seems likely that our ape cousins, while
able to learn individual "words", have little or no grammatical competence. Pinker (1994)
offers a good account of this research.
2. The formation of creole varieties of English appears to be the result of the LAD
at work. The linguist Derek Bickerton has studied the formation of Dutch-based creoles in
Surinam. Escaped slaves, living together but originally from different language groups,
were forced to communicate in their very limited Dutch. The result was the restricted form
of language known as a pidgin. The adult speakers were past the critical age at which
they could learn a new language fluently - they had learned Dutch as a foreign
language and under unfavourable conditions. Remarkably, the children of these slaves
turned the pidgin into a full language, known by linguists as a creole. They were
presumably unaware of the process but the outcome was a language variety which
follows its own consistent rules and has a full expressive range. Creoles based on English
are also found, in the Caribbean and elsewhere.
3. Studies of the sign languages used by the deaf have shown that, far from being
crude gestures replacing spoken words, these are complex, fully grammatical languages
in their own right. A sign language may exist in several dialects. Children learning to sign
as a first language pass through similar stages to hearing children learning spoken
language. Deprived of speech, the urge to communicate is realised through a manual
system which fulfils the same function. There is even a signing creole, again developed
by children, in Nicaragua. For an account of this, see Pinker, 1994 (pp 36-7).(Note: some
of this section is derived from the BBC television documentary The Mind Machine.)
11
Limitations of Chomsky's theory
4 In 1977, Bard and Sachs published a study of a child known as Jim, the hearing
son of deaf parents. Jim's parents wanted their son to learn speech rather than the sign
language they used between themselves. He watched a lot of television and listened to
the radio, therefore receiving frequent language input. However, his progress was limited
until a speech therapist was enlisted to work with him. Simply being exposed to language
was not enough. Without the associated interaction, it meant little to him. Subsequent
theories have placed greater emphasis on the ways in which real children develop
language to fulfil their needs and interact with their environment, including other people.
The Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget placed acquisition of language within the
context of a child's mental or cognitive development. He argued that a child has to
understand a concept before s/he can acquire the particular language form which
expresses that concept. A good example of this is seriation. There will be a point in a
child's intellectual development when s/he can compare objects with respect to size.
This means that if you gave the child a number of sticks, s/he could arrange them in order
of size. Piaget suggested that a child who had not yet reached this stage would not be
able to learn and use comparative adjectives like "bigger" or "smaller".
During the first year to 18 months, connections of the type explained above are
possible to trace but, as a child continues to develop, so it becomes harder to find clear
links between language and intellect. Some studies have focused on children who have
learned to speak fluently despite abnormal mental development. Syntax in particular
does not appear to rely on general intellectual growth.
12
interaction with people who want to communicate with you. Interactionists such as
Jerome Bruner suggest that the language behaviour of adults when talking to children
(known by several names by most easily referred to as child-directed speech or CDS) is
specially adapted to support the acquisition process. This support is often described to
as scaffolding for the child's language learning.
Bruner also coined the term Language Acquisition Support System or LASS in
response to Chomsky's LAD. Colwyn Trevarthen studied the interaction between parents
and babies who were too young to speak. He concluded that the turn-taking structure
of conversation is developed through games and non-verbal communication long
before actual words are uttered.
These theories serve as a useful corrective to Chomsky's early position and it seems
likely that a child will learn more quickly with frequent interaction. However, it has already
been noted that children in all cultures pass through the same stages in acquiring
language. We have also seen that there are cultures in which adults do not adopt special
ways of talking to children, so CDS may be useful but seems not to be essential.
As stated earlier, the various theories should not be seen simply as alternatives.
Rather, each of them offers a partial explanation of the process.
13