Layder Intimacy Power 1 2 3
Layder Intimacy Power 1 2 3
Derek Layder
Intimacy and Power
Also by Derek Layder
Derek Layder
University of Leicester, UK
© Derek Layder 2009
All rights reserved. No reproduction, copy or transmission of this
publication may be made without written permission.
No portion of this publication may be reproduced, copied or transmitted
save with written permission or in accordance with the provisions of the
Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988, or under the terms of any licence
permitting limited copying issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency,
Saffron House, 6-10 Kirby Street, London EC1N 8TS.
Any person who does any unauthorized act in relation to this publication
may be liable to criminal prosecution and civil claims for damages.
The author has asserted his right to be identified
as the author of this work in accordance with the Copyright,
Designs and Patents Act 1988.
First published 2009 by
PALGRAVE MACMILLAN
Palgrave Macmillan in the UK is an imprint of Macmillan Publishers Limited,
registered in England, company number 785998, of Houndmills, Basingstoke,
Hampshire RG21 6XS.
Palgrave Macmillan in the US is a division of St Martin’s Press LLC,
175 Fifth Avenue, New York, NY 10010.
Palgrave Macmillan is the global academic imprint of the above companies
and has companies and representatives throughout the world.
Palgrave® and Macmillan® are registered trademarks in the United States,
the United Kingdom, Europe and other countries.
ISBN-13: 978–0–230–57956–9 hardback
This book is printed on paper suitable for recycling and made from fully
managed and sustained forest sources. Logging, pulping and manufacturing
processes are expected to conform to the environmental regulations of the
country of origin.
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library.
A catalog record for this book is available from the Library of Congress.
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 09
Printed and bound in Great Britain by
CPI Antony Rowe, Chippenham and Eastbourne
Contents
3 Psycho-Emotional Needs 36
Bibliography 180
Index 184
v
Preface and Acknowledgements
This book is about the intimacy and power games that underpin
personal relationships between couples (and, to a lesser extent, friends)
in the modern world. It focuses primarily, but not exclusively, on the
interpersonal dynamics of relationships between couples (and friends)
by asking questions such as ‘How is intimacy “achieved”? How do peo-
ple make close relationships work? What happens when their intimacy
stalls, or breaks down completely?’ These are serious analytic questions
for any science of human behaviour, but are infrequently broached by
sociologists. Yet such questions are too important to be left entirely to
self-help gurus and writers of popular psychology (which is not to deny
the usefulness of some of their contributions).
For these and other reasons, this book concentrates on a fairly
restricted band of interest in intimacy compared with the potential
spread of sociological enquiry. In this respect it is not a critical overview
of existing social research on the many different facets of intimacy. As
I’ve already said, it mainly concerns intimacy in couples and friendship
and, as such, it does not cover issues about parenting or parent–child
interactions. Nor is it explicitly about the role of sexuality in intimacy –
although obviously it is assumed that sexuality frequently plays a major
role in couple intimacy in a way that it doesn’t in friendships. Follow-
ing from this, the book does not explore contrasts between heterosexual
and same sex relationships. It is assumed that although there may be
differences between them, they share much in common in terms of the
human experience of intimacy.
Gender issues do figure quite prominently in the discussion. However,
the main focus is on the debate about the extent to which differences in
intimacy skills and the negotiation of intimate relationships result from
gender influences. The question of gender roles (such as the persistence
of notions of ‘female housewives’ versus ‘male earners’) within marriage
or cohabitation, or the topic of gender inequalities and exploitation,
is not explicitly examined. The main concerns are about how couples
(and/or friends) communicate with each other in the context of differ-
ent types of intimacy and the typical confusions, problems and conflicts
that arise.
vi
Preface and Acknowledgements vii
1
2 Intimacy and Power
Given that couples pursue agendas formed around their emotional and
psychological needs, the question of how they pursue them becomes cru-
cial, and this is the focus of Chapter 7. Each person tends to employ her
or his own favoured methods and means of achieving what they want
or need. Such strategies, ploys and skills are part of a personal repertoire
of control manoeuvres and may be in the service of benign or exploita-
tive motives – and the positive and negative emotions that go along
with them. The emotional architecture that underpins self-identities
plays a large part in shaping personal repertoires of interpersonal con-
trol and influence. In a sense a person’s preferred strategies and ploys
will be directly related to her or his psycho-emotional agenda within
the relationship – what they want, need or desire from intimate part-
ners. But intimate relationships also require joint emotion work in order
to preserve their integrity.
As for specific strategies, ploys or manoeuvres, there is a considerable
range of possibilities depending on whether the relationship is mutu-
ally satisfying or in serious decline. Mutually satisfying relationships can
be expected to include various forms of (psychological) seduction and
persuasion, enrolment, deals and pacts and so on, as well as ‘inverted’
manipulation – like giving away power and emotionally ‘rescuing’ a
partner. At the other end of the scale, strategies include exploitative
manipulation such as emotional blackmail or psychological terrorism.
Both Giddens (1991, 1992) and Beck and Beck-Gernsheim (1995 – orig-
inally published in German in 1990) have developed accounts of the
emergence of ‘the pure relationship’ in the modern era. They claim that
this type of relationship has come to characterise intimate relationships
as societies have moved away from pre-modern or traditional forms in
which intimacy was governed by external criteria and the fixed rules
and values enshrined in tradition. For example, marriage was the result
of parental arrangement in order to cement an economic or political
alliance, while ‘friendship’ hardly existed as a personal tie distinct from
family membership. With the advent of the Western world changes
in social structure have brought about a process of individualisation
10
The Varieties of Couple Intimacy 11
whereby people have become progressively freed from the restraints and
constraints of traditional precepts and certainties, external control and
moral laws, like those of religion and social class. In this respect per-
sonal biographies are now shaped by individual decisions and initiative.
Family, marriage, parenthood, love and sexuality are no longer defined
in a standard fashion; they ‘vary in substance, expectations, norms and
morality from individual to individual and from relationship to relation-
ship’. Love itself becomes ‘a blank that lovers must fill in themselves’
(Beck and Beck-Gernsheim 1995: 5). In the form of the pure relation-
ship love has replaced religion and social class in so far as it ‘gives life
purpose and meaning’ (1995: 182), and has become something akin to
a ‘latter day secular religion’ (1995: 175).
Giddens suggests that the pure relationship ‘exists solely for whatever
rewards that relationship can deliver’ (1991: 6). A pure relationship ‘is
where a social relation is entered into for its own sake’ and ‘is continued
only so far as it is thought by both parties to deliver enough satisfaction
for each individual to stay within it’ (1992: 58). Marriage or cohabita-
tion has become a contract between two equal persons who expect to
be emotionally and sexually satisfied by the relationship or they may
leave and start another relationship with someone else. Friendship in
modernity has likewise become a distinctive form of personal relation-
ship entered into for its own sake. If the friends find they no longer
gain mutual benefit from their bond then it may fall into disuse or be
broken off.
Beck and Beck-Gernsheim emphasise the importance of labour mar-
ket influences on the process of individualisation. Men and women are
released from gender roles but at the same time are forced to build a life
of their own in line with the imperatives of the labour market – such as
training offers, social welfare regulations and benefits and job mobility.
An individual’s biography is planned around the requirements of the
job market.
The fact that increasingly both partners are pursuing their own labour
market biographies (because of women’s greater participation in the
labour force) comes into conflict with the demands of relationships
and this causes the ‘chaos of love’ that characterises modern intimacy.
The freeing up from tradition and external control requires that inti-
mate relationships are conducted and expressed in terms of partner’s
(or friend’s) own rules and decision-making which, as a consequence,
makes them increasingly fragile. At the same time, the fragility of
intimacy is further compounded by the difficulties created by the pur-
suance of two distinct labour market biographies (rather than meshing
12 Intimacy and Power
Protective-dependent love
or ignored as two people blend into one’ (Marshall 2006: 31). After this,
what Marshall calls ‘loving attachment’ usually takes over. The magic
and brilliance of limerence is not enough to sustain a relationship,
something more is needed. An unfortunate myth about romantic love
is contained in the idea that once we have found our partner we can
relax and that love will automatically help us through any problems.
However, in the state of limerence, lovers are bound to each other no
matter how badly they behave, and tend to see each other’s weaknesses
as strengths. This contrasts with ‘loving attachment’, which needs to be
worked at, otherwise it will die. This is made all the more acute because
as couples become long term they become aware of the need to accom-
modate their partner’s differences and weaknesses in order to sustain
intimacy. Also, loving attachment demands that the couple begin to
grapple with the complexities of life and its practical demands (such as
work, having children and so on) and this requires rather more than
their love ‘to keep them warm’.
If the couple don’t develop the necessary skills to sustain intimacy
as relationships develop from limerence to loving attachment, then the
quality of their loving attachment itself may suffer. When this is the
case ‘love’ may be transformed into a third type – ‘affectionate regard’
which is generally what we feel for parents, children, siblings and best
friends. This kind of love makes us want to care for someone, but we do
not feel that our destiny is entwined with theirs in the same way as with
loving attachment. Marshall says that in the ‘I love you but I’m not
in love with you’ syndrome, the ‘I love you’ invariably means ‘I have
affectionate regard for you’.
As noted, those who feel that their relationship has dwindled into
this state often want to end it because it lacks passionate fulfilment and
emotional satisfaction. On the other hand, for a host of reasons, many
people endure such an unsatisfactory state of affairs whilst building up
resentment and anger towards their partner. In such relationships there
is a loss of communication, an inability to reach out to the other either
physically or emotionally, and a tendency to detach and drift apart. Very
often there is ‘polite sex’ rather than ‘intimate love making’ and the cou-
ple may even ‘consider their marriage to be good even if the lovemaking
is routine and unfulfilled’ (2006: 109).
Marshall notes that issues around self-identity and difference often
characterise relationships like this. One aspect of this is that some cou-
ples avoid open confrontation and pretend that their differences do not
exist. But by not acknowledging differences, and confronting the ones
that cause trouble, it is easy to let the relationship drift. Another related
18 Intimacy and Power
problem is that that either one or both will complain of having become
a rather amorphous couple and losing their identity as a consequence.
They fail to strike the balance between finding enough similarities with
their partner to sustain a long-term connection while, at the same time,
having enough differences from them in order to stop the relationship
stagnating. Some ‘friction’ based on difference between partners pro-
vides the spark of passion (Perel 2007). Yet another cause (or indeed
consequence) for relationship stagnation of this kind involves the kind
of emotional infidelity that arises from ‘inappropriate friendships’.
Successful, happy couples are those who have learnt to allow each
other a measure of independence, to compromise over their differences
and take risks (instead of being defensive and putting up barriers). They
also tend to set aside time for one another, to have a laugh together, and
through action rather than words, demonstrate their affection for each
other (Marshall 2006: 183–93). Without these skills intimate relation-
ships are likely to become soured and clogged with resentments and
hurts. As with Reibstein’s analysis, but with more emphasis on its pit-
falls and downsides, Marshall’s analysis opens up the possibility that
intimate relationships are more complex and variable than envisaged in
the pure relationship.
Intimate terrorism
On the basis of his own research evidence, Miller (1995) takes this kind
of approach yet another step further down the road of pessimism about
modern couple intimacy. He suggests that today relationships are best
characterised by the term ‘intimate terrorism’ which conveys the dis-
solution of the myth of romantic love which has been influential for
so long. Drawing on psychology, sociology and literature as well as his
therapeutic practice, Miller argues that modern intimacy has become
contaminated by power and struggles for power in relationships. In this
sense Miller paints a picture that is a far cry from the ideal of the pure
relationship with its preoccupation with egalitarianism and intimacy
as a vehicle for emotional satisfaction. Now I don’t agree that modern
intimacy should be characterised entirely by the notion of emotional
terrorism as Miller seems to suggest, but his account provides an impor-
tant corrective to the overly optimistic and idealised notion of the pure
relationship. Also, Miller’s emphasis on the importance of the relation
between power and love is one that clearly resonates with the wider
framework of my argument.
The Varieties of Couple Intimacy 19
With reference to the latter, Miller often seems to be talking about what
I call ‘manipulative’ or ‘oppressive’ types of intimacy – those in which
exploitative power and control is most evident. By way of contrast, some
of his examples of intimate terrorism come from the ‘milder’ end of the
spectrum and are thus more pertinent to what I call ‘episodic’, ‘semi-
detached’ or ‘pretence intimacy’ in which there still remains some (if
only residual) evidence of benign power and control. Of course, this
underlines the importance of the distinction between different types of
power (which Miller overlooks), for an overall understanding of the full
range of types of intimate relationship.
An ever-present background to Miller’s account of intimate terrorism
is a focus on the ‘disappointments inherent in marriage itself’ or in how
‘romantic love has failed us’ (1995: 79). Partly, disappointment is the
result of the unrealistic burden of expectations placed on love and mar-
riage. More pragmatically, some degree of disappointment inevitably
sets in once the initial passion has run its course, and this poses the
question of how couples deal with it. Often, relationships beset by bitter
irreconcilable quarrels don’t simply end with the partners heading for
the divorce courts. Instead, in many cases, ‘the war gradually attenuates
into a standoff, and the relationship, drained of vitality and meaning,
continues to exist in sullen climate of alienation and chronic discontent’
(1995: 80).
Another theme is the way in which troubled couples attempt to cre-
ate a safety zone in which they can protect themselves from too much
closeness and too much aloneness by ‘carefully editing their intimacy,
perhaps allowing close companionable affection with infrequent or no
sex’, or having ‘lots of sex with little or no demonstrable affection out
of bed’ (1995: 123). This, of course, is an attempt to create a sem-
blance of security in an environment of disappointment, sometimes
even of despair. But it is, ultimately, an empty security based on the
maintenance of a protective distance between them. They go through
fluctuating periods of relative quiet, ‘quarrels, affairs, confessions and
reconciliations, bouts of weeping and hurling ashtrays’ (1995: 133).
Affairs are important both in driving a wedge between the partners and
in creating ‘a buffer of safety from intimacy’ (1995: 136).
The experience of failure and disappointment and the ways in which
couples try to deal with them by creating a buffer zone, which both
keeps them together and keeps them apart at the same time, echo
some of the themes indicated by Marshall. More generally, they speak
to the way in which intimacy may unravel over time. But as I have
already intimated, these themes are relevant not only to the extremes
The Varieties of Couple Intimacy 23
conflicts are a routine feature of even the best intimacy. Here the idea of
‘sustaining a sense of intimacy’ refers to the active and creative strate-
gies that keep a relationship vibrant by feeding the flow of benign power
and influence that energises successful intimacy. It refers to the emo-
tion work required from partners that is essential to maintaining the
vibrancy of intimate relationships.
Jamieson also suggests that the ‘disclosing intimacy’ implicit in the
pure relationship is not the dominant type of intimacy in most couple
relationships. She emphasises a gender dimension here, citing Dun-
combe and Marsden’s (1995) finding that men lack emotional openness
and that they were unwilling to participate in disclosing intimacy. She
also adds that there is much evidence of ‘men’s under-participation in
more practical loving and caring’. She concludes that
Apart from practical care and support, Jamieson also notes that intimacy
can be conveyed through means other than self-disclosure, for exam-
ple through ‘silent intimacy’ where nothing much is said although the
partners ‘express’ love and care through, for example, physical presence
or acts of loyalty. The question of the variable nature of disclosure in
intimacy is important in distinguishing between the different types I
describe later in the chapter. I shall come back to this after reviewing
some of Craib’s (1992, 1994, 1998) criticisms of the pure relationship.
Craib questions the central role that ‘emotional satisfaction’ plays in
the pure relationship. He notes that if there is, indeed, any such thing as
emotional satisfaction, it is of a transient nature. When its effects have
peaked, so to speak, it is always followed by its opposite, the feeling of
dissatisfaction. In this sense satisfaction and dissatisfaction go hand in
hand – ‘simple satisfaction is never possible’ (1994: 123). The idea of
complete satisfaction, especially in intimacy, always remains a fantasy
never a reality, and to expect or demand it from our relationships is to
seek the impossible. Crucially, Craib suggests that imagining that com-
plete satisfaction is the goal of our relationships prevents us learning
from them – psychologically and emotionally – when they fail, or when
love disappears. Instead of enriching our knowledge of intimacy, people
The Varieties of Couple Intimacy 25
and the world around us, the decline of love is experienced as personal
failure and deficiency.
Second, Craib points out that emotional satisfaction is not always
pleasant. There are times when talking to each other (self-disclosure)
actually makes things worse rather than better. Sometimes emotional
satisfaction involves ‘rows attacks, hurting and being hurt; this is part
and parcel of intimacy’ (1994: 124). Craib’s point is not that emotional
satisfaction isn’t part of intimate relationships, but that it is fragile,
unstable and contradictory and can involve negative and destructive
dimensions. Indeed, the very act of falling in love is usually followed
by a degree of disillusion when our partner fails to live up to the role
we have assigned to them. In this respect we should not approach rela-
tionships without the expectation of being unhappy for some of the
time. Craib isn’t saying that mutually destructive unhappiness should
be borne or endured or be better ended. Rather he’s pointing out that
the simple appearance of unhappiness should not be the reason for rais-
ing the possibility of ending the relationship. An essential part of this is
that in any relationship there will be ‘complex inequalities of effort’ and
in which ‘partners must be willing to exploit the other and be exploited
for periods’. In a long-term relationship, ‘hate can play as big a part as
love, and the relationship can contain passionate expressions of both’
(1994: 128).
Finally, Craib claims that ‘the level of self-revelation in a normal
relationship has to be limited to make it work’ (1994: 130). It is not nec-
essarily good for partners to tell each other every time they found other
people attractive, or if they always tell each other what they cannot
stand about each other. Sometimes it is important ‘not to talk’, especially
where this helps to avoid hurtful comments, unreasonable demands or
unwarranted attacks on the other. This continues the theme raised in
Jamieson’s work that self-disclosure in intimacy is much more variable
than that envisaged in the pure relationship. The extent and nature of
disclosure varies according to different types of intimacy.
Types of intimacy
these accounts are entirely consistent with, and provide the substan-
tive basis for, the typology of intimate relationships developed here.
(Although none of the accounts is organised in terms of different types
of intimacy.) The types are defined by the presence of several variable
characteristics of intimate relationships. These include the nature and
extent of disclosure, commitment, trust and sincerity; the emotional
atmosphere; the extent of emotional and sexual infidelity; the focus
or orientation of the partners; the nature of communication between
them; how ‘satisfaction’ is defined within the relationship. The full
range of types commonly found in couples (and to a lesser extent in
friendship) is as follows:
Dynamic
Episodic
Semi-detached
Pretence
Manipulative
Oppressive
Dynamic intimacy
Episodic intimacy
The partners are living virtually separately, but still want to keep the
relationship going as a basic anchor for their lives.
From time to time, partners are caring, supportive and loving. They
are generally courteous and occasionally flatter one another, offering
gifts, sex, companionship and so on. Of course, these may also be used
‘manipulatively’ as ploys to keep the relationship ‘ticking over’, and to
preserve its outward appearance. In this sense intimacy is used selec-
tively to ‘manage’ problems that may threaten the relationship. This
happens, for instance, when couples offer sex as a substitute for conver-
sation or emotional disclosure. If sex is off the agenda, gifts may serve
to offset the lack of emotional connection. Selective intimacy like this
may be relatively ‘silent’ so as to avoid more revealing disclosures that
may occur inadvertently through talk.
Generally in episodic intimacy, disclosure is less authentic. Emotional
commitment has declined, and partners are less open with each other.
This creates a psychological and emotional space that allows them
to ‘engage at a distance’ without appearing too insincere, which is
important because they still need to trust one another to some extent.
Discussion is much less free but not because one partner wants to
dominate proceedings or block information. Rather, it is because they
both selectively withhold their feelings. Such a state of affairs can
lead to estrangement between couples and gives rise to comments like
that from one of Reibstein’s female interviewees when she says of her
husband:
he acts as if I don’t exist. When I walk into a room and all I want
to do is have a chat, tell him about how my day has gone, what the
children are up to, all that kind of stuff, he sits and reads the paper
as if I’m not even there! He doesn’t love me. If he loved me he would
want to listen to me, talk to me about himself. He would want to
spend time with me.
(Reibstein 1997: 13)
bond. The once focal partner is now only one of a number of others with
whom ‘trust commitments’ are made.
In this respect emotional affairs or emotional infidelity – the sharing
of intimate experiences with, say, work friends, rather than partners –
which has been made easier through texts, e-mails and the Internet –
take on an increasing importance. (It may, in effect, be the half-way
stage to full-on sexual infidelity.) Along with such changes, the sincerity
that once cocooned the relationship undergoes a shift of emphasis. Part-
ners deal with one another less ‘authentically’. Their intimacy is more
‘performed’ and manipulative because they no longer see one another
as the only ‘source’ of emotional satisfaction.
Marshall’s (2006) findings indicate the existence of many couples like
this who remain together in spite of a lack of passion in their relation-
ships and their dissatisfaction with the companionate nature of their
everyday life. This suggests a distinction between ‘true’ companionate
relationships in which both partners are happy to settle for this state of
affairs (noted by Giddens 1992: 155), and ‘pseudo’ companionate bonds
that have become so by default because the partners have drifted apart
emotionally. For Marshall these relationships exemplify the theme of
partners ‘loving’ each other, but not being ‘in love’.
Although this kind of intimacy mainly occurs in the ‘second phase’
of a relationship, on occasion, partners may choose it from the start.
Sometimes friends or family members, even couples, want a less intense
relationship because they feel happier in less ‘demanding’ and focused
relationships. Episodic intimacy allows more space and independence
and releases them from the ‘tyranny of responsibilities’ that comes with
the more full-on styles. Of course, a relationship that starts out like this
will only continue to work while both partners feel the same about it.
If one person decides that she or he wants better intimacy they may,
in fact, decide to end the relationship. But there are definite benefits
with the episodic style that shouldn’t be underestimated. Restrained
intimacy of this kind is necessary if partners are uncomfortable with full-
on closeness, but – for whatever reason – want to keep the relationship
going.
Semi-detached intimacy
This type is yet another step away from dynamic intimacy, and again,
usually occurs in the later phases of a relationship. Intimacy may
become semi-detached if one or both partners privately desire greater
closeness, but also realise it isn’t currently possible. Full intimacy may
The Varieties of Couple Intimacy 31
Pretence intimacy
Manipulated intimacy
often alongside other styles. For example, it is common for milder forms
of emotional blackmail and emotional bullying to occur with dynamic,
episodic and semi-detached intimacy.
Oppressive intimacy
The types of intimacy outlined in the previous chapter result from the
combined efforts of the two individuals who make up any particular
couple. The interpersonal dynamics of their day-to-day dealings deter-
mines the quality of their intimacy and stamps a distinctive pattern
on it. Before they became a couple they were independently formed
individuals albeit strongly influenced by their social environments.
Accumulated social experiences before getting together shape each of
them as a particular kind of person – confident and assured, or shy
and diffident, needing constant reassurance or strongly independent.
As such they bring to the relationship certain personal qualities and
capacities that inevitably influence the sort of intimacy they achieve as
a couple. This chapter examines this aspect of individual development
and how it impacts on couple intimacy.
As individuals we look to our intimate relationships to provide us with
emotional satisfaction. But while personal relationships are often grat-
ifying and fulfilling, they can also create frustration, anxiety and even
desperation. Thus intimate relationships are bound up with the ques-
tion of what it is we want, desire and need from our social and personal
lives. How do our most personal relationships enable or prevent us from
being satisfied and happy? In this respect the notion of ‘psychobiogra-
phy’ indicates the way in which a person accumulates a unique profile
of psycho-emotional needs (desires, capacities, dispositions), formed out
of their social experiences over time – from early childhood to later
adult life. Thus individuals have ‘profiles’ suggesting variable levels of
need in relation to security, self-esteem, love, approval, self-worth and
self-respect and so on, and these influence the extent and quality of
the intimacy they achieve in partnerships. Understanding intimacy as
formed out of the combined influence of relatively independent social
36
Psycho-Emotional Needs 37
Some authors have suggested that there are ‘basic’ or ‘core’ emotional
needs that must be satisfied to achieve well-being in mental life (Maslow
1999, Branden 1985). What is meant by ‘satisfied’ or ‘fulfilled’ needs
some clarification because we each have different requirements. What
is ‘good’ or ‘sufficient’ for one person may not be so for another. So
while it is possible to outline basic or core needs, it is not possible to
say with accuracy what any particular individual person wants or needs.
For example, while we all require our own ‘space’, or ‘independence’
and don’t want to be ‘suffocated’ or overwhelmed by others, we each
have different tolerances in this respect. Likewise, humans require some
involvement, companionship and friendship, but we all differ in terms
of how much of them we need. Further, how we balance needs for inde-
pendence and togetherness is a personal issue. While some need greater
independence, others need more involvement and it’s important to bear
this in mind in assessing whether partners’ needs match up with one
another. We must always be aware that there are general basic needs but
that individual requirements vary.
Are some core needs more important than others? The short answer
to this is no. Precisely because they are fundamental, all core needs tend
38 Intimacy and Power
to clamour for attention at the same time. Of course, there may be times
when particular attention has to be paid to a specific need. For example,
if a person is going through a trying time, he or she might feel the need
for more loving gestures – say for kisses and hugs from their partner. But
generally, no one emotional need has more importance than any other.
Each core need feeds into and reinforces the others.
It is possible to talk about the psychological need for security, or the
need for self-confidence, or the need to feel valued, as if they were inde-
pendent of each other. Up to a point it’s useful to identify and isolate
feelings in this way in order to clearly target particular problems or
issues. But in the end, mental health and well-being depends on how
well the system of core needs fits and works together. Feeling under-
valued by a partner or a friend may be a specific problem that needs
to be resolved by getting them to be more appreciative and supportive.
But being undervalued also adversely affects other feelings – of being
loved, of having self-worth, self-respect and so on. As a whole the sys-
tem of core needs is finely balanced with each need interdependent with
the others.
Feeling secure and having good self-esteem are essential for mental
health and happiness. They form a basic anchor for other personal
attributes and feelings. In everyday life we say things like ‘I’m feeling
a bit insecure at the moment’ or that someone is ‘insecure’ about their
looks. These refer to uncomfortable feelings true enough, but not to
life threatening or socially disabling conditions. We often apply such
descriptions to people who otherwise feel fairly secure about themselves.
But insecurity can be more seriously disabling, interfering with the abil-
ity to deal with life and to feel happy. This has been referred to as
‘ontological insecurity’ (Laing 1969, Giddens 1991, Layder 1997), which
involves a cluster of related feelings about personal identity.
The first of these is the feeling of being ‘unreal’ or phoney in which a
person may feel that they are acting a part rather than presenting his or
her ‘real’ self. This may be because they are unhappy with their real self
and ‘hiding’ it by taking on a persona that represents the kind of person
she or he wishes to be. Unfortunately, this involves the mental strain of
trying to conceal bits of the real self that ‘escape’ when the individual’s
guard is down. Along with this sense of falseness an individual might
feel unworthy of any of the good things that have happened (or might
Psycho-Emotional Needs 39
There can surely be no dispute about the importance of love for mental
well-being but it is important to distinguish between different kinds of
love. Perhaps parental love (or that of principal caretaker) is the most
crucial kind because the quality of this bond will affect a child’s abil-
ity to form good loving bonds later in life (Reibstein 1997, Marshall
2006). Where the bond is good a child will, more than likely, be happy
and form good loving relationships. Unhappy childhood experiences
will lead to a more depressed outlook and either a fear of intimacy
itself or an inability to acquire intimacy skills. Although love has sev-
eral forms including that between friends, neighbours, relatives, it is
often assumed that romantic, sexual love provides the most intimate
and intense expression of a loving relationship.
Although loving others and being loved by them are crucial, love
of self is just as important. Without it the capacity to love others is
restricted. If love of others is absent, it is difficult to experience love of
the self although loving acceptance of the self is not the same as narcis-
sism. It’s more about a caring attitude towards oneself, self-acceptance,
integrity and being self-protective when necessary. ‘Self-love’ in this
sense is essential for sustaining self-esteem. Self-hatred or loathing
inevitably leads to negativity about one’s abilities, competence and wor-
thiness and inevitably undermines security and self-esteem. If a loving
self-acceptance is absent, personal happiness and psychological well-
being will be elusive. Self-love is crucial because it is the basis for loving
others. Self-loathing inhibits positive attitudes towards others.
It would seem obvious that intimate relationships depend on the pres-
ence of ‘love’ between the partners, friends or relatives. It is less obvious
that love needs to be ‘worked at’ regularly for it to remain alive and
healthy. All too often people assume that love will take care of itself. But
taking love for granted often occurs when intimacy is on the wane – that
is, drifting from dynamic intimacy into the episodic or semi-detached
types. With the consequent narrowing of self-disclosure that accom-
panies such a drift, the partners never really ask themselves how they
feel towards each other, or discuss the quality of their intimacy. This
is not to say that their happiness is not real, or that the relationship
is a sham, although, of course, any of these possibilities may also be
42 Intimacy and Power
As with love, the need for self-approval and approval by others are
closely linked. If you don’t accept who you are, it will be difficult to
accept others. Also, others may misunderstand this as hostility and hold
back. Constantly searching for approval in adulthood reflects a child-
hood spent seeking, but never finding, parental (or caretaker) approval.
Receiving just the right amount is crucial if a child is to move into
adulthood with a balanced need for approval. If the balance isn’t right,
he or she may become trapped in a constant search for approval, fear-
ing abandonment or the withdrawal of love and leaving them helpless
Psycho-Emotional Needs 43
Feeling intrinsically worthy and that others value you as you value them
are strongly linked to security and self-esteem. A person can’t experience
happiness if she or he feels unworthy and undeserving. The likelihood
is that upbeat or happy thoughts and moods will be undermined by the
nagging doubt that they are deserved. A person may become so used to
this that he or she begins to cling to unhappiness because it feels com-
fortable and ‘secure’, providing them with settled expectations. In fact,
being unhappy may have all sorts of hidden pay-offs that an individual
may be reluctant to give up (Jeffers 1987). Gaining sympathy, attention,
help, support and understanding are major pay-offs. Being unhappy can
also let a person off the hook of self-responsibility in general, so that she
or he is absolved from making decisions about earning a living or caring
for others. It provides a readymade excuse for not taking the initiative in
44 Intimacy and Power
Getting the right balance between being together and doing things
alone can be difficult, particularly in long-standing relationships. An
extreme tendency for partners to favour one side or the other can be
linked to engulfment or abandonment anxieties as noted by Miller
(1995). Less extreme shifts in such tendencies may occur over time. For
instance, one person might increasingly want to do things on his or her
own in response to a partner’s demand for more attention, company
or reassurance, which they find oppressive and stultifying. Regaining a
balance in relationships requires both partners’ willingness to acknowl-
edge that their behaviours have changed and to talk things through.
But because such problems raise sensitive emotional issues, partners are
often unwilling to confront them. Instead, they simply become upset
or resentful about a partner’s behaviour without really knowing why. In
this respect they wage a silent war of attrition.
If someone becomes more independent, say going out alone, or pur-
suing educational studies, or having separate holidays, then a partner
might take this as a form of rejection that subsequently impacts on their
self-esteem and security, making them even more vulnerable. Handling
this without further damage to one another’s pride requires the utmost
delicacy, trust and sensitivity – all of which might already be in short
supply! Certainly, sensitivities may be such that any attempt to confront
such issues (even carefully and lovingly) may cause further emotional
upset.
Often – although by no means inevitably – a long-standing bond
allows greater mutual freedom. For new lovers, being apart, even for
a short period of time, can be painful, almost unbearable, whereas for
those who have been together for many years, temporary separation
may be more easily kept ‘in perspective’. Of course, the opposite might
also be true. Sometimes long-term partners grow so accustomed to con-
stant companionship that the very thought of being separated causes
great stress and anxiety. However, familiarity all too frequently breeds
boredom and irritation so that partners increasingly feel the need for
time apart in order to keep the relationship going at all. In this sce-
nario both partners silently witness the gradual dissolution of common
interests, involvements and activities while at the same time relishing
the greater personal space that this allows. At the same time both may
remain ‘committed’ to the relationship – and this is typical of episodic
and semi-detached intimacy. This may be workable and satisfactory for
both partners although involving a considerable sacrifice of mutual care
and involvement.
46 Intimacy and Power
Unwanted drift
If drifting (as in drifting apart) simply allows partners greater space and
independence then it may be ‘satisfactory’ for both. But many rela-
tionships enter into irreversible decline under the (indifferent) noses of
the partners. Both may regard it as the end-game in a long and silent
(perhaps even bitter) war of attrition. In this sense it represents a kind
of ‘drifting apart’ that developed it own momentum and has come to
an inevitable conclusion (typical of the semi-detached, pretence and
manipulative intimacy). Tragically, there are also cases in which ‘drift-
ing’ is unwanted but the partners seem helpless to halt it. This can end
up with both partners disillusioned and largely estranged from each
other, while remaining puzzled as to why this has happened and why
they didn’t or couldn’t do anything about it. This often occurs because
unspoken feelings are not easy to face up to, even if partners are aware of
them in the first place. Much of the time ‘emotion work’ goes on below
the level of conscious awareness.
A good balance between independence and togetherness is clearly
important to a thriving relationship – although it is often difficult to
achieve. An inability to be alone is as unhealthy as an excessive need
for togetherness. Becoming too dependent suggests insecurity, anxiety
and lack of self-confidence. In ‘oppressive’ intimacy partners drag each
other down because they don’t realise the destructive effects of their co-
dependence. In ‘manipulative’ intimacy where there is one ‘dependent’
partner, responsibility for the relationship largely falls into the hands of
the one who is ‘depended on’. He or she must decide either to live with
the imbalance or to try to ‘make’ the dependent partner aware of the
problem and convince him or her to do something about it.
It is necessary for partners (or friends) to be strong and confident
in order to achieve the right balance of respect for one another’s
boundaries. A healthily independent person is less likely to suffocate
or overwhelm a partner with attention and affection or with fears and
insecurities. Giving a partner the space and freedom to be whom, or
what they want to be, encourages them to do the same in return.
Most people want to ‘fit in’ with others. They don’t want to ‘stick out
like sore thumbs’, as the colloquial phrase has it. This is an important
way of integrating with others on the basis of likeness. In effect, it is
saying, ‘I’m just like you, and you are just like me, and that is what gives
Psycho-Emotional Needs 47
another very well – perhaps too well! And yet this intimate knowledge
may be the very reason why the partners like and trust each other so
much in the first place. In other words, it is their mutual familiarity that
makes them comfortingly attractive.
But there are hidden penalties associated with this. Chiefly, if a rela-
tionship is based on the comforts gained from familiarity and reliability,
then both partners must also accept the lower grade of intimacy that
goes with them. This is because an individual’s unique qualities are
naturally dissolved into a taken-for-granted view of each other. The
warmth and security that comes from being able to depend on each
other becomes more important than individual ‘special’ qualities. So
the hidden penalty is that instead of being loved and regarded as a
unique person – whose essence is never thoroughly ‘known’ – part-
ners are loved precisely because they are reliable and dependable – a
comfortable certainty in an uncertain world.
Both partners may tacitly accept (even desire) this kind of security and
dependability. However, one (or, bizarrely, even both) may also secretly
desire to be loved more deeply and profoundly, and resent the fact that
they aren’t. But the fact that the desire remains private and unspoken
may have a corrosive effect on trust and the sincerity of the partner’s
feelings – as in a move from ‘episodic’ to ‘semi-detached’ intimacy.
Ideally, of course, there should be a balance of dependability and spe-
cialness. Regarding someone as ‘a rock’ or knowing him or her ‘inside
out’ is fine up to a point. But it is of paramount importance that the
‘specialness’ of partners always plays a role in their dealings with, and
feelings for, each other. A partner’s individuality must never be forgotten
if intimacy is to remain fresh and exciting.
51
52 Intimacy and Power
Intimate relationships are about satisfying our own and our partner’s
emotional needs. The best way to make this happen is to influence –
and thus exert some control over – other’s responses. ‘Control’ is not
simply about making someone do something against their wishes – it’s
about ‘influencing’ them in a more positive way. Thus, ‘control’ and
even ‘manipulation’ may be understood in softer, more compassion-
ate ways which include enticing others do what you want them to do
willingly, through influence, persuasion and charm. Good examples are
getting someone to ‘go out on a date’, or trying to impress them in a con-
versation. Convincing an elderly person of the need to lock up securely
at night is ‘controlling’ or ‘manipulative’ in so far as they do what we
think is ‘best’ for them, even though they may not realise it at the time.
Couples (and friends) routinely rely on (intimacy) control games and
relationships habits – that is, habitual ways of dealing with each other.
Influence is achieved by assuring others that we are right rather than by
brow-beating them into agreement or by forcing them into anything.
Nevertheless, we subtly manipulate and ‘benignly’ control them by
being sympathetic or caring rather than exploitative. In this sense a per-
son is free to ‘resist’ your overtures and persuasive powers and, indeed,
they may be finally ‘won over’ only by further persuasion or induce-
ment. This differs greatly from overcoming resistance or reluctance by
threats or punishment.
Mutual benign control continually makes and remakes intimacy, but
when it goes wrong, relationships founder and often break down. Suc-
cessful intimacy allows partners each to get what they want from each
other through their combined, ‘cooperative’ efforts. By contrast, in rela-
tionships where partners are unhappy, this is usually because they are
at odds with each other and dissatisfied with what are giving to, and
receiving from, their relationship. Genuine mutuality is thwarted and
there is no longer a balance of needs and support.
and women are interested in, and want to get different things out of life,
they have a hard time making relationships work. However, an exclusive
concentration on gender differences overlooks the many similar things
that men and women want and need from intimacy. These include a
sense of efficacy based on the ability to benignly control, influence and
attract others, to establish and maintain social bonds and so on. The
personal strategies and skills of men and women are aimed at producing
much the same outcome – influencing, persuading and exerting con-
trol over the feelings and responses of others. It may be that men and
women tend to use different control strategies in different kinds of social
circumstances but these strategies are not exclusive to one gender. We
are all in the business of attracting, influencing and controlling others
as part of a mutual exchange of wants and needs.
Take, for example, basic psychological needs. Unless a person has
enough self-esteem, self-confidence and security, he or she may end up
lonely, depressed, resentful and unhappy, regardless of their gender. Sim-
ilarly, if an individual has enough love and intimacy of the right kind
in their life, then they’ll feel OK about themselves, enjoy life to the full
and assume that nothing will get them down for long. In this respect
basic psychological needs like security and self-reliance are the same the
world over. Obviously, there are cultural, ethnic and gender differences
in people’s styles, behaviour and preferences. But differences in style
often mask the same basic needs for companionship, love, intimacy,
attention, approval, self-esteem, the power to attract, and to be effective
in the world.
These aspects of behaviour and psychology underline the similari-
ties between human beings rather than differences based on gender,
or culture or ethnicity. Unfortunately, many popular psychology books
encourage us to think that men and women want radically different
things from life and relationships. Some have even suggested that men
and women inhabit different worlds (Gray 1992, Moir and Moir 1999,
Pease and Pease 2002). This simply reinforces gender stereotypes by
overlooking our shared humanness and masking the uniqueness of
individuals. The assumption that the influence of gender is overrid-
ing implies that the rich variation in personality and individuality is
unimportant.
No one should be reduced to a gender stereotype. The very core
of individual uniqueness resides in taking responsibility for our own
actions – for ‘who’ we are, what we want and how we live our lives.
Many men cry, are good at expressing their emotions, display sensitiv-
ity and so on, while countless women do not ‘fit’ gender stereotypes.
54 Intimacy and Power
result of not having control over one’s personal life and other people. If,
on the other hand, a person feels that they do have some control – that
she or he can break away from, or overcome, oppressive relationships –
then this will generate self-confidence and self-esteem and, in turn, will
give rise to a fair amount of fun and joy.
The sense of ‘effectiveness’ experienced through the exercise of con-
trol leads to satisfaction, elation and other positive feelings. In this sense
it reminds the individual that she or he ‘is able’ or ‘has the power’ to
have their needs and desires fulfilled through their relationships. Know-
ing this helps individuals develop and retain confidence in personal
qualities, abilities and skills. That is, it bolsters how they think of them-
selves, and thus the image they want to project to others (Goffman,
1971, calls this ‘impression management’). For instance, we may like
to think of ourselves as, say, interesting conversationalists, good neigh-
bours, helpful friends or caring lovers. Accordingly, we will try to convey
this impression to others so that they take it into account and treat us
appropriately.
Being able to rely on such personal qualities and attributes creates
the self-confidence essential for attracting others. Self-belief seems to
stimulate the need in others to keep seeking out our company or to try
to get closer to us. It also reinforces their willingness to provide us with
the things we need, such as love, attention, acceptance and respect. It
almost goes without saying that general self-esteem greatly depends on
the ability to be effective in social relationships.
compete with each other for energy in every encounter to ward off feel-
ings of fear, abandonment, pain and so on, which originally formed in
childhood. Our habitual way of gaining attention and stealing energy
is by enacting ‘control dramas’ through which we make up for a deficit
of attention, self-esteem and self-worth. These control dramas include
intimidating others, being hostile and fault finding, being distant and
aloof, or playing the victim. However, by seeking to manipulate or dom-
inate the other’s attention we become weak and insecure and cut off
from our intuition, creativity and spirituality. In contrast, by surrender-
ing the need to control, we become aligned with mystical and universal
energy (1995: 95, 99).
Miller’s ‘intimate terrorism’ and Redfield and Adrienne’s ‘control dra-
mas’ present an unjustifiably pessimistic and sometimes cynical view of
human nature that fails to recognise the crucial role of benign control
in everyday life. Of course, conflict, power struggles and control dramas
do exist, but it is highly misleading to imply that all of social life is like
this. It is certainly true that some relationships are based on emotional
blackmail, psychological manipulation and so on. There are also many
relationships based on other forms of malign power and control, and
it might be better if they were eradicated. But the point cannot be to
eliminate control in general. Certainly, benign influence is preferable to
control based on manipulation, competition and repression. But benign
control is also essential to personal effectiveness in social relationships,
as well as for good self-esteem, self-worth and self-confidence. With-
out it, it would be impossible for us to draw out appropriate emotional
responses from others.
Benign influence and control is a mutual exchange of psycho-
emotional energy, it is not ‘stealing’ or ‘competing’ for energy at
someone else’s expense. Although the exchange of energy involved may
not always be equal, it is its ‘to and fro’, give-and-take nature that makes
it distinctive. Rather than struggle, conflict and competition, it is best
understood as a process of bonding and rapport that allows everyone
to get something out of an encounter (Collins 2005). At times, self-
assertiveness such as jockeying for conversational turns (having your
say) may, at times, take on a mildly competitive edge, albeit often laced
with humour. But, this always occurs against a backdrop of ‘give and
take’ between those involved and cannot be interpreted as ‘theft’ or a
struggle for dominance. In fact, it is part of a collective invigoration in
which everyone receives emotional energy (Collins 2005).
Benign influence and control thrives on and stimulates creativity and
intuition rather than suppresses them. Consider, for example, trying to
Intimacy and Interpersonal Control 57
benign control never becomes frozen into a fixed inequality – ebbs and
flows in power and control are essential.
There are two dominant interconnected themes that flow through the
foregoing discussion. They are, first, the idea of the individual as ‘a
seeker of control-through-influence’ in relation to his or her psycho-
emotional needs and, second, that the quality of couple intimacy is the
emergent outcome of interpersonal negotiation. To say that the individ-
ual seeks control-through-influence is not to be confused with the idea
of the ‘powerful self’ (identified by Craib [1998] in the work of Giddens
[1991, 1992]). Psychobiographical variations in levels of competence,
self-esteem, self-confidence and security and so on mean that the ability
to influence and control others also varies from individual to individual.
In short, there is no generic ‘tranformative capacity’ as Giddens (1984)
suggests.
In couple intimacy individuals seek to control and influence each
other in order to achieve sexual and psycho-emotional satisfaction.
62 Intimacy and Power
63
64 Intimacy and Power
She goes on to point out that ‘sometimes the gender differences which
matter most are not differences between women and men, but differ-
ences between women and women or men and men’ (Cameron 2007:
51). It is clear that in this sense Cameron’s views directly support and
complement some of the propositions of domain theory, although her
comments are based on evidence and arguments that are quite indepen-
dent of the theory. However, in later parts of the following discussion –
particularly where I discuss the question of power and control – my
approach takes a distinctly different tack from Cameron (and others)
and is, in fact, critical of some of their assumptions.
However, it is important to say that the purpose of the overall discus-
sion is not wholly negative. In a more positive vein, examination of this
work reveals some of the possible differences in intimacy style that may
prove problematic or supportive of good intimacy. It also enables us to
define some of the linguistic and communication skills that are neces-
sary for good intimacy regardless of whether they are possessed and/or
used by men or women. The discussion first examines Tannen’s claims
about fundamental gender differences and then moves on to Baron-
Cohen’s claims, in each case teasing out the purported implications for
couple intimacy.
Deborah Tannen suggests that men and women regard personal rela-
tionships differently and this is expressed in their conversational styles.
Tannen arrives at her overall picture by drawing on her own and others’
research on the ways men and women use language in everyday conver-
sation. By stressing their ‘involvement’ and ‘connection’ with others,
68 Intimacy and Power
as a rejection of her. She would prefer him to consult her not simply
out of courtesy, but because she wants to feel valued and close to him
by taking her wishes into account. However, Cameron contests the idea
that misunderstandings inevitably occur because of fundamental gender
differences. She points out that any exchange between human beings
can go awry ‘simply because language is not telepathy’ (2007: 98) not
because they communicate in fundamentally different ways.
Nevertheless, Tannen insists that the above example also highlights
the fact that men tend to listen for, and concentrate on, the messages
in talk and conversation. Women take much more account of the meta-
messages, which comment on the relationship (its emotional ‘state of
play’). In the previous example, the woman ‘hears’ the meta-message:
‘you are not important enough to me to take your views into account’.
The fact that her partner acted without taking her views and feelings
into account indicates that there is problem between them. According
to Tannen, when the man says ‘oh by the way I said we’d go over to
the Jones’s for dinner tomorrow’, he focuses on the ‘information’ and
doesn’t hear the negative meta-message. He fails to appreciate that the
issue is also one of intimacy, and not simply about facts or information.
Again, however, Cameron points out that research evidence does not
support Tannen’s claims. The idea that men and women have ‘a partic-
ular problem because they differ systematically in their ways of using
language, and that this is a major source of conflict between them, does
not stand up to scrutiny’ (2007: 98–99).
Tannen goes on to suggest that although all humans need both inti-
macy (involvement) and independence, women tend to focus on the
former and men on the latter. However, the very fact that there are
universal human needs for both involvement and independence implies
that they are present to the same extent in men and women. In fact,
Hyde’s (2005) ‘meta analysis’ of studies of gender differences in commu-
nicative behaviour does not support Tannen’s claims here. Again, the
idea of fundamental differences between men and women is eminently
disputable. However, if we consider the question from the point of view
of individual styles and preferences (regardless of gender), it is highly
probable that differences in emphasis on involvement and/or indepen-
dence might cause tension in couples. Individuals (men and women
equally) may find themselves at cross-purposes because they value dif-
ferent things and get what they want in different ways. Individuals
who value independence will use status competition to establish their
‘involvement’ with others. Those who value involvement and solidarity
with others will use connection to establish intimacy.
70 Intimacy and Power
Tannen claims that men more readily use what she refers to as ‘report-
talk’ (giving out information) instead of the ‘rapport-talk’ (getting closer
to each other) favoured by women (Tannen 1992: 77). In conversation
women tend to establish connections with others. They emphasise sim-
ilar experiences and what they have in common with others and are
critical of those who try to stand out or appear superior. Again, there
seems to be scant evidence for these claims. For example, Hyde (2005)
found only tiny (statistically insignificant) differences in assertive and
affiliative speech and self-disclosure between men and women. Never-
theless, Tannen goes on to suggest that women feel most at home with
‘private’ conversation and often approach public situations as if they
were private. For men, by contrast, talk is a way of maintaining sta-
tus and independence in social life. As Tannen says, this is done by
exhibiting knowledge and skill, and by holding centre stage through
verbal performance such as story-telling, joking or imparting informa-
tion. Men are comfortable with ‘public speaking’ in larger groups. They
often approach even the most private situations as if they were public by
offering a ‘report’ rather than rapport. However, it is likely, as Cameron
says, that such examples of differences in behaviour are influenced by
variations in context (such as formal versus informal situations) and
the roles, expectations and obligations that are associated with them
(Cameron 2007: 119).
Tannen asserts that these differences may also be the cause of mis-
understanding and hurt between men and women. For example, while
some men are often lively and entertaining ‘talkers’ at parties or in
the company of mutual friends, at home alone with their partners,
women often complain that the man with whom they are most inti-
mate ‘doesn’t talk to me’ and ‘doesn’t listen to me’. However, according
to Tannen, often men won’t talk about fleeting thoughts and feelings
experienced throughout the day (things that a woman might share with
close friends), because to him they don’t seem important or interesting.
What men regard as ‘important’ (sports, politics, news, current affairs) is
usually different from women (thoughts and feelings, who called, who
they met, what they said). These characterisations are somewhat con-
tradicted by Hyde’s (2005) analysis of a large number of studies which
found minimal differences between talkativeness and speech production
between males and females.
Nevertheless in Tannen’s view, talk, for women, ‘is’ involvement, and
listening shows care and interest. On the other hand, men’s taciturnity is
Gender, Intimacy Styles and Skills 71
perceived as a failure of intimacy: ‘He’s keeping things from her: he’s lost
interest in her; he’s pulling away’ (p. 83). For their part many men don’t
feel the need for talk – the companionability of their partner’s presence
is all they need and cherish. They feel that home is a comforting place
where they don’t need to ‘prove themselves through verbal display’
and are ‘free to remain silent’. For women, on the other hand, ‘home
is a place where they are free to talk, and where they feel the greatest
need for talk, with those they are closest to’ (p. 86). However, Tannen
is remiss for not considering the possibility that such attitudes and pre-
dispositions may derive from a number of factors other than systematic
gender differences – for instance, the influence of class or contextual
or situational factors, or even simply individual (psychobiographical)
preferences.
Differences in listening
Tannen uses Maltz and Borker’s work to show that not only do women
give more listening signals, but also that the signals they give have dif-
ferent meanings for men and women. Women use ‘yeah’ to mean ‘I’m
with you’ (or ‘I’m listening to what you say’), whereas men tend to say
‘yeah’ only when they agree. This may lead to misunderstandings. If
a woman says ‘yeah, yeah’ as a man speaks to her and then says she
doesn’t agree with him, he may think that she is being insincere or that
she was just saying yeah whilst not really listening. On the other hand, if
a man does not say anything while a woman is speaking to him, she may
conclude that he’s not listening at all. According to Tannen, women are
clearly focusing on the meta-messages of talk, while men focus on the
message level. However, to reinforce the point made earlier, Cameron
points out that Maltz and Borker
did not carry out systematic tests to see if men and women inter-
preted the same minimal responses differently. The point of their
article was to raise the possibility that a gender difference might exist,
and suggest that this should be investigated in future research. But
as happens all too frequently with claims about male–female differ-
ences, what was put forward as a speculation soon started to be cited
as a fact.
(Cameron 2007: 83–4)
the way individuals (men and women), control and influence others.
While they may (perhaps at different times) use different strategies of
control and influence, all individuals use their personal power to make
things happen. All individuals exercise personal power and control. In
this sense women aren’t control neutral as Tannen implies. So the real
question is how do people (men and women) get they want? How do
they enlist the help and support of others to make their lives happen? In
this respect indirectness, rapport and involvement are aspects of mutual
benign control. It may be that some individuals are more intimacy-
friendly because they use benign control more readily than others, but
it is still a form of control and influence nevertheless.
Furthermore, Tannen makes a false contrast between getting on with
others and the idea of manipulation. But getting on with others requires
the use of benign control and ‘soft’ manipulation. It involves skilfully
handling people so that they eventually agree to what you want them
to do, while keeping your own self-interests in check. Manipulation is
harder and more exploitative when self-interest overrides others’ inter-
ests, rights and wishes (Layder 2004b). As long as it genuinely embraces
another’s interest then manipulation remains ‘soft’. For example, a per-
son may manipulate (skilfully manage) a partner’s feelings and views to
make him or her want to start a family or decide to emigrate. Of course,
the partner may respond by skilfully persuading them to see or feel oth-
erwise – in short, to change their mind. Longstanding partners often
use such ‘manipulative’ ploys in daily life in relation to both trivial and
more serious issues.
Tannen overlooks the importance of interpersonal control (especially
in women) because she views it negatively as ‘control over’ someone
(stemming from their status), and thus she believes it to be antithet-
ical to intimacy (closeness). In this sense control is exclusively about
making someone do something against her or his will. Thus, Tannen
wrongly concludes that control and intimacy (closeness) are quite sepa-
rate dimensions of social life (2002: 71). But not all control is like this.
As we have seen, mutual benign control is an energising force enabling
people to get closer by calling out positive emotions in one another. It
is both the motive force of intimacy and the glue that bonds people
together for mutual benefit. This kind of power and control is about
using loving and caring gestures in a persuasive manner to bring out
equally loving and caring responses from a partner. In this sense emo-
tions and control are seamlessly interwoven (Layder 2004b). It is clear
that negative emotions (pain, frustration, hurt, sadness, hatred, disap-
pointment) often result from dominative or status control. But it is too
76 Intimacy and Power
likely to suffer from autism and asperger syndrome. For these individ-
uals, Baron-Cohen offers ample evidence of the damaging effects of
extreme systematising on social skills and the general ability to deal
with the most commonplace of social rituals, routines and encounters.
He devotes a whole chapter to describing the sorts of problems experi-
enced by people who are extremely high systematisers and who are also
highly intelligent and successful.
Now while Baron-Cohen provides vivid depictions of both empathis-
ing and systematising skills, we must take care to avoid the stereotype
characterisations of women as ‘natural empathisers’ and men as ‘natu-
ral systematisers’. This is especially important in the light of some of the
evidence counter to Tannen’s (and others) work. It is surely more plausi-
ble to suppose that all human beings need some of both skills depending
on what they are doing and the social circumstances in which they are
doing it. For instance, the technical aspects of flying an aircraft require
an abundance of systematising skills, but when talking to colleagues
such as flight crew or passengers – when they need to employ social
skills – pilots need to display empathy in no small measure (especially
if they are trying to keep passengers calm during an emergency). Like-
wise, systematising skills are involved in the technical aspects of playing
board games, but the interchanges between the game players during the
game are also social in nature and rely a good deal on empathic skills.
The social nature of the activity in conjunction with the social circum-
stances in which it occurs are the key features that both require and
determine displays of empathy or systematising – not some essential
gender or brain sex difference. Sure enough, some individuals may be
more proficient in, or at ease with, certain tasks and skills. But social life
inherently demands both kinds of skills.
In fact, some of Baron-Cohen’s own arguments seem to acknowl-
edge this and undercut the idea of essential differences between men
and women. For example, when he says that empathy is ‘the glue of
social relationships’ and that systematising gets you almost nowhere
in most day-to-day social interaction or the negotiation of personal
life, Baron-Cohen seems to acknowledge that empathy is an intrin-
sic requirement of social life in all its aspects. Also when describing
the mathematician Richard Borcherds, an extreme systemiser, Baron-
Cohen remarks that an ordinary friendly conversation was too much
for him because he couldn’t make sense of the ‘hidden meanings’
behind ‘glances and smiles, of innuendo and double-entendre, of
bluff and deception, embarrassment and camouflaged flirtation’ (2004:
155–8).
Gender, Intimacy Styles and Skills 81
Following on from the idea that individuals’ personal styles and skills
affect the quality of intimacy, this chapter explores the possible impact
of such differences on the day-to-day experiences of couples as they
negotiate their intimacy needs with one another. It examines the
question of whether (and how) different conflict styles impact on
the quality of mutual self-disclosure between couples. As we have
already noted at a number of junctures, self-disclosure is at the heart
of modern intimacy – in the form of the pure relationship (Gid-
dens 1992, Beck and Beck-Gernsheim 1995). In particular the chapter
focuses on differences between direct and indirect styles of communica-
tion in individual partners and how this is reflected in their conflict
styles and arguments. Overall, it is suggested that an over-reliance
on indirectness in one or both partners can lead to the narrowing
down or even disappearance of self-disclosure between partners, which,
in turn, may lead to relationship problems with varying degrees of
seriousness.
As pointed out in previous chapters, individual partners often adopt
rather different strategies of control and influence when it comes to
obtaining their needs and desires. So although they may be agreed
that intimacy is ultimately about creating love, respect, personal sig-
nificance and so on, the partners may be far from harmonised when
it comes to the methods (of power, control and influence) they use to
achieve these ends. Does this mean that they are on a continual collision
course as far as expressing themselves and understanding one another’s
needs are concerned? Does it mean that many couples are doomed to
communication failure and an impoverishment of intimacy, care and
understanding? Are they destined to misunderstand and talk past one
another?
84
Arguments, Indirectness and Non-Disclosure 85
examples like this which are ‘petty’ (as she describes them; ‘fairly incon-
sequential’ as I would characterise them). Far more is at stake (again as
she rightly notes, p. 89) with regard to the other example, of whether
women who are subject to sexual assault are being clear about their own
wishes.
There is, as Cameron acknowledges, a vast difference between these
examples in terms of their practical and emotional implications, but in
giving these extreme cases as her only examples of indirectness she over-
looks many ‘intermediate’ ones that play an important role in everyday
couple intimacy. The latter concerns issues that are not simply about
petty practical matters like emptying the trash, bringing in the gro-
ceries or even, fetching ketchup at family dinners (Cameron 2007: 88).
On the other hand, neither do they involve serious allegations about
criminal assault. First and foremost, the examples I refer to involve
matters of emotional import – coded messages about partners’ feelings
towards each other, the ‘state’ of their relationship, what they love and
cherish about their partner, what they wish they could change about
their partner, or their regrets about what cannot be changed in the
relationship. Such issues are the focus of continuous and routine negoti-
ations between partners, sometimes consciously and deliberately – and
thus directly. More often, however, they are unintentionally and indi-
rectly ‘negotiated’ through actions (gestures) or even, the way things
are talked about. That is, such matters are expressed in a verbally indi-
rect manner through meta-messages – the hidden messages encoded (or
encrypted) in words, actions or gestures – including things left unsaid.
It is these everyday, but non-trivial, kinds of indirectness that can
have a profound impact on couple intimacy because of the confusion,
misunderstandings and tensions that they may cause.
In this respect perhaps the main reason why partners misunderstand
one another has to do with the fact that they have different habitual
ways of expressing both their own feelings and desires, as well as con-
veying what they need from their partners. One person may prefer, or
rely on, ‘indirectness’ in communication, whereas the other might ‘tune
into’, and more readily respond to, directness. For instance, one partner
might say ‘do you want to go out tonight?’ even if it is they themselves
who really want this to happen but also want both of them to enjoy
the evening. In fact, it may be a coded way of saying ‘let’s do some-
thing together’. However, because one person is listening for a direct
message, the other partner may respond ‘no I don’t really feel like going
out’, thinking that it is a question about his or her inclinations or pref-
erences. Such a person may be completely unaware that, in fact, their
Arguments, Indirectness and Non-Disclosure 87
partner was saying something about his or her own desire for them to
do something together. In the event, one person ends up feeling hurt
and irritated by the other’s rejection of the (indirect) request both for
attention and bonding and as a response to this ‘insensitivity’ may even
become difficult and argumentative. The other partner may respond to
such ‘crotchetiness’ by regarding it as ‘irrational’ moodiness. If the first
partner tries to clarify by saying ‘well really, I was saying I’d like us
to go out tonight – and asking you could we do that?’ the other may
well respond by saying ‘well why didn’t you say that, if that’s what you
meant?’ They may go on to think, ‘I can’t win here, he or she asks me
one question, but really means something quite different!’
Crucially, both partners feel misunderstood, hurt and disvalued in the
process. But this is not a ‘surface’ misunderstanding that can be recti-
fied by simply being clearer or by recasting the communication. The
problem lies much deeper than this and has to do with the use of differ-
ent styles and codes of communication. Indirectness trades on a more
social, communal style that draws in the other person in order to cre-
ate rapport or to bond with them. By contrast, directness focuses on
the individuality and distinctiveness of the messages being conveyed.
One focuses more on the importance of the relationship between those
who are communicating, while the other focuses on the informational
content of the message itself.
In this sense direct communication is less about establishing a connec-
tion with the other than with being specific and explicit about what is
being communicated. Directness in this sense is saying what you mean.
We cannot conclude from this that one form of communication (direct-
ness or indirectness) is ‘better’ than the other. Rather, it means that the
foci and priorities of the speakers are different – one being more con-
cerned with the quality and emotional status of a relationship, while
the other focuses on the message itself.
Using meta-messages
Being indirect relies on being able to ‘pick up’ on what is being said
without having to be explicit about it. In the same way, meta-messages
comment on the nature and quality of a relationship. In essence, a
meta-message is an un-verbalised ‘comment’ implicit in the message
that reveals how well or badly those involved are getting on with one
another (Tannen 1992). Strictly, meta-messages represent an added level
or layer of meaning compared with indirectness, pure and simple. In the
previous example in which one partner asks the other if he or she wants
88 Intimacy and Power
to go out, the problem for the ‘receiver’ of the message is figuring out
what is actually meant. Is it something other than it appears – is it being
suggested that they both go out, rather than if she or he wants to go
out? However, if we consider the meta-message level, there is a further
message in which the initiating partner conveys the hope that they can
spend more time together than they have of late. If the other responds
by saying ‘no I don’t feel like going out’, the meta-message sent back
is that he or she doesn’t really care about spending time together. Cru-
cially, at bottom, it means one partner doesn’t care enough about the
relationship – whether it gets better or worse. In this sense the other
doesn’t just simply ignore the first partner’s ‘indirect’ desire for them to
go out, but is also unable to hear the meta-message that suggests that
the relationship is a bit stale and that they both should be making an
effort to make the relationship more enjoyable.
In practice being indirect and using meta-messages often go together,
as in this example. However, they are distinct in a crucial respect.
Indirectness is about saying one thing but really meaning another so
the important issue for the ‘hearer’ is about correctly interpreting the
message. Being indirect involves a verbal message even though it is
ambiguous. With meta-messages, on the other hand, nothing is explic-
itly laid out in words. They must, therefore, be grasped intuitively
through a deep understanding of a person’s feelings or thoughts. Thus,
meta-messages are doubly ambiguous. Whereas indirect messages can
at least be checked against what is said, there is nothing to compare
or measure meta-messages against. Because the meaning is implicit and
un-stated, it requires intuitive, empathic understanding. You either get
it, and share its meaning with the other person, or you don’t. For meta-
messages to have any effect those involved must be able to ‘get’ the
message. If one person can’t do this then it may result in an almost per-
manent state of misunderstanding for both partners, as they continually
‘talk past’ one another.
a bond such as ‘we don’t spend enough time doing things together’, or
‘you think more of your friends than me’. Those who are low on empa-
thy often employ another kind of ‘indirectness’. This manifests itself as
a refusal to disclose feelings – as in prolonged periods of silence, talking
about anything but feelings, emotional withdrawal and so forth. Being
indirect in this way is not linked with bonding and intimacy. Rather it
is about unexpressed anger, fear, pain, hostility, resentment and bitter-
ness. In a similar manner a preference for ‘direct’ facts or information
largely reflects a suppression of feelings.
This suggests that because such individuals don’t generally express
their emotions they may be less ‘in touch’ with feelings of close-
ness, tenderness, caring, vulnerability and so on. It does seem that
a preference for directness suggests a low level of emotional literacy
(Goleman 1996). Second, they will find it difficult to empathise with
the intimacy requests and needs of others, particularly their partners.
If one partner is a typical empathiser who relies on indirectness and
meta-messages, while the other is reliant on direct, information-based
communication and/or non-disclosure – they are on a collision course.
This is because their attempts at exchanging intimacies will be severely
hampered.
Clearly, one conclusion that could be drawn from the above discussion
is that those who rely more on indirect communication are better at
expressing emotion and intimacy. The solution to the problem would
thus appear to be quite simple; that those indirect communicators most
affected need to be educated in the arts of intimacy and empathy
because they need to understand their own and their partner’s emo-
tional and intimacy needs. But would this solve the problem? The crux
of the issue is whether the assumption that indirectness is a better way
of expressing feelings really stands up. It may be true that a facility
for indirectness makes a person more ‘in touch’ with intimate matters,
but are they really ‘good communicators’ of emotion and intimacy? Is
their understanding of these issues superior? Is an upgrading of skills
all that is required to fill the intimacy vacuum? Would it eradicate
misunderstandings and miscommunications?
Indirectness and meta-messages of this type may be intimacy-friendly
but they are also inherently vague, opaque and ambiguous. Even when
such messages are detected in the first place, their meanings are not
straightforward because they can be interpreted in many different ways.
90 Intimacy and Power
But being able to translate inchoate feelings into words is not easy.
And, although an individual may be more in touch with their emo-
tions, they may not necessarily be able to put feelings into words. Nor
might such a person be able to arrange feelings in comparative impor-
tance, and view them from a wider perspective. For example, someone
may feel ‘anger’ or ‘resentment’ towards a partner but be unaware that
this is because of a need for more space and/or to avoid a partner’s
attempts at ‘control’ (as in ‘manipulative’ or ‘oppressive’ intimacy). In
fact, one reason why such individuals use intuitive communication may
be that they are unsure of what their emotions ‘mean’. Their inability
to translate feelings into words prevents them from grasping their real
meaning. If this is so, then they also require an upgrading of intimacy
skills.
To avoid misunderstandings and/or talking ‘at cross purposes’, those
using different styles of communication must adjust to each other. On
the one hand, some individuals are chronically adrift from their own
and other’s inner lives and desperately need better intimacy skills if they
are to sustain relationships with partners who are skilled at intimacy.
They must learn to recognise their emotions and be able to articulate
them, in order to create a mutually satisfying dialogue with partners.
On the other hand, some individuals might have better relationships,
if they were more verbally direct. This is because, first, if their partners
already have few intimacy skills, then being more direct and explicit
would aid in conveying their emotions, wishes and desires. It would
reduce the amount of blind guesswork about feelings and emotions and
thus enhance the quality of their dialogue. Also, these people would
understand themselves better because their instinctive, intuitive emo-
tional experience would be enriched. Better self-knowledge would make
them better communicators of their own wishes and desires. In turn,
this would increase their chances of being understood and having their
own intimacy needs met.
To go back to a previous example, if a partner asks ‘do you feel like
going out for a walk?’ instead of saying ‘I want us both to spend some
time together’ – it is highly likely that the other partner will think they
are being asked solely about their personal preferences. By being more
direct about intuitive feelings and by saying what they really mean, it
is harder for the other to ignore, or simply remain unaware of his or
her feelings. The more discursive and articulate they are, the more they
will have a surer grasp of their own needs. The more explicit a person is,
the greater the likelihood that a partner will satisfy her or his intimacy
needs.
92 Intimacy and Power
Thus although erupting with anger may be a relatively safe and even
‘useful’ way of releasing tension, in the long term it does nothing to
ease or solve relationship problems. First, as Quilliam notes, ‘it puts
your partner on the defensive and by hurting him or her makes retalia-
tion more likely. And if your partner does retaliate, your emotions flare
up again and lead you to strike back in turn. No chance of a peaceful
settlement’ (2001: 71). Perhaps more importantly, there is no chance
of meaningful dialogue about the very issues causing the conflict in
the first place. If a partner using this conflict style is matched against
another who is more empathic and indirect then the scene is set for a
never-ending war arising from a lack of genuine communication.
Another conflict style is ‘retreat’ in which an individual denies their
own and others’ feelings because they create pressure and pain. He or
she withdraws, puts up a wall of silence, avoids interaction and refuses
to admit there is a problem or may even just walk out. It is an attempt
to avoid the discomfort of anger and fear by not expressing them and
is even less helpful than high-level attacks. Avoiding or denying emo-
tions is simply pretending the very problems that are causing pain don’t
exist. But denial and retreat also arise because the individual doesn’t
understand his or her emotional needs, fears and insecurities.
Of course, leaving the room might be a useful way of calming down
if someone is on the point of explosive anger. It provides important
‘time out’ to collect thoughts and recompose oneself. However, in the
long term retreating doesn’t help because the other person is excluded
and none of the problems are resolved – thus the relationship remains
fragile. The only way for the bond to survive (probably as ‘episodic’
intimacy) would be for the person to learn how to handle the feelings
that can no longer be kept under wraps (2001: 71). Clearly, a man or
woman who regularly retreats is shutting off from his or her feelings
and emotional needs and creating a void at the heart of the relationship
(as in ‘semi-detached’ and ‘pretence’ intimacy). The bond will not sur-
vive unless both partners make a determined effort to understand and
readjust to one another.
their own feelings) seems to offer the safest solution while preserving
the relationship intact. An appeaser may think ‘if I give in (he or she)
will love me for it. If I argue and make it difficult they will leave’. As
Quilliam observes, appeasing a partner will keep the peace, but because
this is achieved at the expense of one’s own needs it will have one of
two consequences over the longer term. Suppressed anger may store up
and erupt into worse anger at some future date ensuring further trouble
down the line. On the other hand, the appeaser may remain the victim
of their own fear, which, in the end, paralyses the ability to be assertive
about their own needs and feelings. In time this may transform into
hopelessness and despair, depression or physical illness.
Unlike ‘retreat’, ‘appeasement’ does not indicate that a person doesn’t
know his or her own feelings, or what he or she needs from a partner.
But fearing rejection the individual suppresses her or his own needs in
order to keep a partner ‘sweet’ (typical in ‘episodic’ and ‘semi-detached’
intimacy). The appeaser hopes that unmet needs will (eventually) be
met when his or her partner finally figures out what they are – even
though these needs are not directly expressed. Such hopes are in vain if
he or she is dealing with a partner who only responds to direct, factually
based messages. Such a partner doesn’t pick up unexpressed resentment
about not being properly listened to, or cared about.
Another typically indirect approach to conflict is the ‘low-level
attack’. This includes nagging, whingeing (complaining), being con-
temptuous (Pease and Pease 2002) and emotional blackmail (Forward
and Frazier 1998). These strategies are fraught with the same sort of diffi-
culties as appeasement – as well as some additional ones. In a sense those
who nag, whinge or use emotional blackmail make a definite attempt to
get what they want. But instead of asking upfront, they make ‘demands’
in an unclear and ambiguous way. Also, the fact that these strategies are
indirect and ‘behind the scenes’ makes them inherently mischievous,
and harmful for intimacy. In the final analysis such styles are ‘selfish’ in
that they are exclusively about the perpetrator’s demands and interests.
There is no genuine empathy or rapport since they aren’t agreed upon
or shared.
Nagging involves rather feeble, indirect requests that are heavily guilt-
laden such as ‘I don’t ask you to do much around here’ or ‘You never
take the garbage out’, ‘Why don’t you pick up your clothes?’, ‘I spend
my weekend working my fingers to the bone to make this place look nice
while you just sit there all day watching television’. As Alan and Barbara
Pease (2002) point out, ‘this kind of nagging is pointless, self-defeating
and creates a lose/lose situation. With this approach, nagging becomes a
96 Intimacy and Power
It would seem that arguments are a common feature of all forms of inti-
macy – both satisfying and unsatisfying. But are arguments a natural,
or even ‘necessary’, part of an intimate relationship? Or is it possible
to have an argument-free relationship? This would depend first on the
‘compatibility’ or otherwise of the partners and their style of intimacy.
Thus win-win style arguments – in which partners acknowledge and
listen to one another’s needs and talk flexibly about solutions – are a fea-
ture of ‘dynamic’ intimacy. By contrast, with ‘episodic’, ‘semi-detached’
and ‘pretence’ intimacy, the majority, or even all arguments, may be
by-passed in order to avoid the intensity of feeling they generate. Both
partners may decide that they prefer not to engage in verbal conflict
because of the tensions and anxiety it causes. As a consequence they are
prepared to accept compromises as far as they are possible.
7
Personal Strategies and Repertoires
98
Personal Strategies and Repertoires 99
settling on strategies that suit our own purposes, we also help or hinder
others in pursuing their desires and interests. This makes the distinction
between benign influence and self-interest unclear. The crucial issue is
about how much we help or hinder others, because no matter how well
intentioned we are, our behaviour will always be in part selfish when-
ever we tend to focus on our own satisfaction. In this respect strategies
and repertoires may be distinguished in terms of whether they are based
on benign persuasion and ‘soft’ manipulation, or on self-interest and
exploitation.
Personal magnetism
Being ‘cute’, sexually desirable or beautiful are powerful attractors that
may kick start intimacy by providing an initial spark of interest. But
usually, initial magnetism is not enough to sustain intimacy over longer
periods of time. For this to happen other factors are required. For exam-
ple, sexual attractiveness might not last longer than an immediate
sexual frisson. Staying together may require an extra serving of sensi-
tivity and empathy. Similarly, friends may be initially drawn together
because they both like to look ‘cool’, ‘hard’, ‘demure’ or ‘exciting’. But if
they are to remain friends they must also prove their trustworthiness
and dependability. The magnetism of physical attractiveness is pow-
erfully enhanced when it is combined with other things like charm,
charisma and confidence. ‘Charismatic’ figures (Weber 1964) such as
those who are, or become, leaders (both good and evil, like Christ or
Hitler) or celebrities attract crowds of followers or acolytes. But each one
of us, in our own small way, will usually have some ‘trait’ or skill that
can be used to influence others – such as charm, or being good at chat-
ting. Using our most attractive and persuasive features helps energise
intimate bonds.
In ‘dynamic’ intimacy both partners lead with their best ‘sides’ so that
the relationship remains fresh and positively charged. Magnetic appeal
is used playfully to invite an equally playful response. But attractors can
also be used in a self-serving way to gain the upper hand in a relation-
ship. For instance, charm can be used alongside emotional blackmail or
even intimidation (Horley 2000). Conversely, an apparently submissive
partner might use their vulnerability and helplessness to wrest control
away from a dominant partner. In a similar way the appeal of per-
sonal magnetism may change over time. What was once thought of
as irresistible charm, or beauty, in a loved one may eventually become
Personal Strategies and Repertoires 101
Magnetic seduction
Once personal magnetism is combined with more persuasive skills, it
becomes powerfully seductive. Although ‘seduction’ is most commonly
linked with sexual conquest, more generally it connotes the way in
which individuals entice, beguile or lure others to go along with their
wishes and intentions. In magnetic seduction partners may use personal
attractors like charm, or physical attributes such as hypnotic eyes, sex-
ual allure and so forth, along with persuasive ploys like teasing, flattery,
humour and promises in order to satisfy their own and their partner’s
desires. In this sense magnetic seduction can play a role in both benign
and more manipulative relationships.
Persuasive seduction
This kind of seduction relies on the sheer power of persuasion (Borg
2004) and frequently occurs in romantic partnerships. Persuasive seduc-
tion can occur in many ways – through conversation, ‘heart to hearts’,
hinting, innuendo, ‘helpful’ suggestions, disguised ‘advice’, special
pleading, imploring, begging, promising, arguing, allaying fears and so
on. Examples of this are persuading a partner to do, or agree to, various
things – such as to spend more time together, to go to a particular hol-
iday destination, to be more sexually demonstrative, to visit relatives
less, or even more commonly, to change particular habits, to be more
attentive to the children, to have or not to have children. The same is
true of friendships. Efforts to get friends to share a holiday, to go out
more or less often, to talk about intimate issues, to share leisure activi-
ties, to give up or acquire habits like smoking or drug taking, to decide
what to do about the plight of a mutual friend, to be supportive and
caring, and so on, are the routine stock in trade of friendships. They are
typical issues around which friends negotiate their mutual needs and
desires. As such persuasive seduction follows the mutual give and take
of control and influence, keeping intimacy alive and dynamic.
Of course, persuasive seduction could be used to manipulate or dom-
inate. But sometimes, unequal power is healthy and necessary. For
instance, good parenting depends on controlling and influencing chil-
dren in their ‘best interests’ and with genuine loving concern. Thus
a parent might convince their child that they should visit an elderly
102 Intimacy and Power
eye contact and so on. Being able to articulate feelings and choosing the
‘right’ way of expressing intent and desire are also crucial to the success
of deep knowledge seduction. This is because it requires the interweav-
ing of both partners’ desires and intentions creating psychic support for
one another.
even arrive at ‘working arrangements’ about when and how they will
make love.
These are ‘positive’ deals and pacts in that they only work if the
partners desire the same thing. They don’t work if they suit only one
partner. When a pact has developed over time in a way that supports the
partners’ security and self-esteem, it has an energising effect on the rela-
tionship. Negative pacts, on the other hand, may still be ‘supportive’ but
only in the sense of ‘propping up’ partners’ failings. For example, one
partner may turn a blind eye to the other’s infidelities because they don’t
feel strong enough to challenge the situation. Alternatively, they may
ignore infidelities because they receive material security or psychic sup-
port from being ‘known as a couple’ and having powerful connections.
(The high profile cases of Jeffrey and Mary Archer in the UK and Bill and
Hilary Clinton in the USA are good examples of such relationships.) In a
sense such partnerships are based largely on mutual ‘convenience’ with
both partners getting something out of the deal, but, of course, as forms
of intimacy they are deeply flawed.
Emotion work
In ‘dynamic’ intimacy partners share responsibility for creating and
recreating the emotional heart of their relationship. They take equal
turns in caring and loving, making physical and sexual contact, talk-
ing about feelings, commitment and about how they can change things
for the better. This is the routine ‘emotion work’ necessary for a relation-
ship to keep ticking over properly. Many intimate relationships slowly
(or rapidly) fall apart precisely because one or both partners fail to realise
the need for constant emotion work. Relationships and individuals are
always changing and developing, requiring the renewal of emotional
commitments and responsibilities. Without a balance in emotion work
the relationship itself will not be refreshed or recharged. Even if part-
ners are not particularly good at it, their willingness to learn is essential.
The slightest backing away from emotion work can wreck the fragile
balance on which good intimacy rests. Emotional withdrawal by one
partner may precipitate a similar move by the other who may resent
being left with the main responsibility for emotion work. This more
or less describes what happens when ‘dynamic’ intimacy gives way to
‘episodic’ or even ‘semi-detached’ forms.
A number of possible reasons may account for one partner assuming
total responsibility for emotion work. The other partner may lack skills
or fail to understand the importance of emotion work. Alternatively,
Personal Strategies and Repertoires 105
one person may shirk responsibility especially if the other seems willing
to shoulder the main burden. The one who takes responsibility, in fact,
may not be aware of it until it suddenly occurs to them that their part-
ner never talks about feelings or the future of their relationship. Finally,
someone who carries the emotional load in a relationship may do so
out of insecurity. By immersing themselves in emotion work they gain
a sense of control, which, to an extent, allays their fears that the rela-
tionship might fall apart if they don’t take on the whole burden. But, of
course, by monopolising emotion work they leave little room for their
partner to join in.
Once the responsibility for emotion work is out of kilter a relation-
ship becomes increasingly unsatisfying for one or both partners. If one
person either takes all or refuses to take any responsibility for emo-
tion work, then the other may eventually begin to resent it and enter
into a ‘battle for control’ (Miller 1995). If both partners are happy with
an imbalance in emotion work, it is usually because this conveniently
covers over their mutual failings. This happens in ‘oppressive’ inti-
macy where partners’ co-dependence results from their personal flaws or
weaknesses. In such cases partners manipulate each other and preserve
themselves through a symbiotic relationship. This signals a shift from
‘softer’, to more selfish manipulation – where identities are undermined
and energy dissipates from the bond.
Emotional rescue
A person may give herself or himself emotionally without realising that
their partner isn’t willing to do the same. But instead of giving up on
the relationship, they decide to forge ahead in the hope that they can
‘change’ or repair the flaws in the partner. Any inadequacy, such as an
over controlling or critical nature, chronic insecurity, social anxiety, an
unpredictable temper, can become a ‘project’ for transformation. How-
ever, if the flawed partner doesn’t want to change, the relationship itself
will come under threat. Another problem is that the ‘rescuer’ bases their
liking and attraction for the inadequate partner not on what they ‘are’
but what they are capable of becoming. They fall in love with the idea
of changing him or her into their ‘ideal’, rather than accepting them
for what they are. They may even settle for a partner who doesn’t love
106 Intimacy and Power
them or treats them badly because they believe that they can eventually
change them.
Emotional rescuers sometimes seek to rescue obviously ‘helpless’ part-
ners and, by so doing, convince themselves of their own compassionate
and loving nature. In this sense the ‘rescuer’ may be as inadequate as the
person they are rescuing and, as a consequence, they ‘negatively’ sup-
port each other. Of course, unless the flawed partner is actively willing
to change, the rescuer must resort to various self-deceptions. Thus they
try to convince themselves that they just need a little more time, or that
(unlike his or her previous partners) ‘you’ (the rescuer) will be the one
who ‘loves them enough to change them’ or that you are the only one
who really understands the ‘real’ them (De Angelis 1992). Both the res-
cuer and the rescued deceive themselves by imaging that intimacy can
be sustained in this manner. At best, it may provide negative support; at
worst, it may deteriorate into manipulation or dependence. It has been
suggested that women use this strategy to rescue ‘bad’ men (De Angelis
1992). This indeed is a familiar story linked with women abuse (Horley
2000), but there are many men who also attempt to rescue women who
are inadequate in some way.
Creating dependency
Based as it is on fear of abandonment, the point of this strategy is to
make a partner so dependent that they would never consider leaving
the relationship. While the stronger partner adopts the role of ‘compe-
tent adult’ the other is cast in the role of ‘dependent child’ who ‘ought
to be’ submissive and allow the other to take the main responsibility.
By constantly hearing the message that he or she is essentially a vulner-
able, fragile person, they do indeed become helpless and dependent.
The stronger partner takes them under their wing, so to speak, and
‘makes the world safe’ for them. The more they are defined as depen-
dent, the more convinced that they indeed are, while the competence
and strength of the other is reinforced.
When men do this to women they usually take on the role of father
figure or protector. But this is not simply about caring and protecting
someone who is vulnerable. The point is also to keep his partner in her
place by reminding her that she cannot do without his help in find-
ing her way through the world. He always knows more and therefore
‘knows best’. He may regard her as ‘irrational’ or ‘emotional’, incapable
of dealing with the hard realities of life. If he can convince her that
she is indeed better off by bowing to his ‘superiority’ then she will
Personal Strategies and Repertoires 107
Relinquishing power
On the face of it this would seem an odd strategy. It is an attempt to
acquire rather meagre and seemingly negative ‘advantages’ by giving
away power. If someone fears their partner’s criticisms or disapproval,
they may tolerate lack of respect or mistreatment. The unacceptable
behaviour is more bearable than the rejection. People who are guilt-
ridden or low on self-esteem do this when they allow their partner’s
wants, desires and general life agenda to snuff out the importance of
their own needs and desires. They will often give up their interests,
ambitions, hobbies, friends or family members in order to avoid con-
flict with a partner who doesn’t approve of them. The disapproval is
driven by insecurity and a need to cut a partner off from previous psy-
chic support since it threatens their own control over the relationship
(Horley 2000).
An individual may give away power by trying to become the kind of
person their partner ideally wants. Such a person tries to adapt their per-
sonality and behaviour so that they fit in with a partner’s expectations
and desires. But by trying to become their ideal person important parts
of self-identity are sacrificed. Finally, a person may relinquish power
by giving up on his or her own hopes, desires and dreams in order
to help a partner’s come to fruition. Giving up jobs, or moving to a
different country, relinquishing ambitions are all ways of giving away
power in a relationship (although they may also be part of an agreed
108 Intimacy and Power
deal or pact). De Angelis (1992: 51) suggests that some women cover
up their excellence and competence ‘in order to avoid threatening the
man in their life’ and ‘to make him feel better about himself’. Many
such women hide their talents and accomplishments from the men in
their lives or they talk about themselves in derogatory terms, put them-
selves down, or refuse to ‘take’ compliments in order that the man can
feel more ‘in charge’. By so doing, these women believe that men will
love them more. However, as Angelis points out, this belief is illusory
because most men believe ‘that a woman who exudes self-confidence is
very appealing’ (1992: 56).
Psychological attrition
Psychological attrition occurs when intimacy is at its lowest ebb. It
involves wearing a partner down as if he or she was an opponent
in a war game. The strategy includes criticism, whingeing and nag-
ging, which makes the target feel as though they are constantly being
interrogated. They must justify and account for everything they do
and say. This slowly undermines their defences and so eventually they
cave in to the partner’s demands. A person who uses this strategy is
insecure about his or her tenuous hold on power and their inabil-
ity to use benign control effectively. But it does not rely on actual or
threatened violence – it is a strategy of last resort borne out of desper-
ation. It is the preferred choice of someone who feels powerless and
helpless – who manifestly isn’t good at benign control and influence.
Nagging and whingeing are conflict styles more typically associated
with individuals who rely on indirect forms of communication, often
alongside emotional manipulation or emotional blackmail. A person
who routinely employs psychological attrition is socially ineffectual and
profoundly lacking in the skills normally used to influence and control
others.
Phoney seduction
This is a manipulative version of seduction. The victim is drawn in
by the ‘promise’ that their love, care or attention will be returned in
kind. But the phoney seducer is only concerned with getting her or
his way. Phoney seduction encompasses a wide range of behaviours.
At one extreme there is sexual seduction as ‘conquest’ in which the
seducer moves on to another potential ‘victim’ as soon as sexual grat-
ification has been achieved. Of course, if both partners are fully aware
and accept this in the first place, then there is no deception involved.
Personal Strategies and Repertoires 109
Emotional blackmail
Milder forms of manipulation are commonplace in everyday life. For
example, sexual intimacy can be offered to make a partner suppress their
anger when you admit you’ve crashed the car, or forgotten to pass on an
important message. These are part of the give and take of close relation-
ships and are present in even the most satisfying intimacy. Emotional
blackmail, on the other hand, clearly goes beyond ordinary manipula-
tion ‘when it is used repeatedly to coerce us into complying with the
blackmailer’s demands at the expense of your own wishes and well-
being’ (Forward and Frazier 1998). An emotional blackmailer is someone
close, who threatens – either directly or indirectly – to punish us if we
don’t do what she or he wants. Because many blackmailers are close to us
(like family, friends and colleagues) we prefer to strengthen, rather than
break, our bonds with them. Along with fear, obligation and guilt, the
target is confused – which makes it difficult to refuse the blackmailer’s
demands.
Some blackmailers are aggressive, upfront and confrontational whilst
others are passive, subtle and manipulate ‘behind the scenes’, so to
speak. However, all blackmailers share a fear of change, rejection or a
loss of power and control. As Forward and Frazier observe, blackmailers
do not have an explicit aim to destroy their victims:
Rather, they are people for whom blackmail is the ticket to feeling safe
and in charge. No matter how confident they look on the outside,
blackmailers are operating out of high degrees of anxiety. But then
110 Intimacy and Power
they snap their fingers and we jump. For a moment, our blackmailers
can feel powerful. Emotional blackmail becomes a defence against
feeling hurt and afraid.
(Forward and Frazier 1998: 11)
In this and the next chapter I describe some of the most common
intimacy games played by couples. While Chapter 9 focuses on ‘ener-
gising’ games, this one concentrates on ‘energy-draining’ games – the
ones that cause most trouble in relationships. By describing them first,
it is possible to identify those aspects of a relationship that constantly
create problems and issues and stifle the happiness of the partners. By
first doing this it is perhaps easier to appreciate how they could be trans-
formed or ‘healed’. However, first, it is necessary to understand a little
about the differences between these games.
113
114 Intimacy and Power
Manipulative games
self-esteem, but it cannot bring the real thing. Genuine emotional ful-
filment requires the use of benign influence – it cannot be stolen or
extracted without the willing and empathic participation of another. It
would take a great deal of self-deception for a manipulative person to
be totally unaware of this. So, far from being an energy booster for the
dominant partner, manipulative games actually deplete energy.
The games, therefore, are symbiotic in which the partners are locked
into a mutual dependence, but for different reasons. For manipulation
to work properly, the dominant partner needs someone who will accept
their control and not put up too much resistance. In turn, the submis-
sive partner becomes reliant on the other’s influence because it creates
structure and organisation for their life. At the same time it allows them
to abandon many responsibilities.
she was on him. She even suggested at one point that he should take
a lover (an associate of hers called May Pang) because she needed a
break from his overwhelming dependence and reliance on her. However,
once she decided she was ready to take him back she instructed him to
abandon Pang and summoned him back to their New York apartment.
By all accounts John meekly submitted to these demands seemingly
completely in the thrall of Yoko.
With his almost total emotional and practical submission to Yoko, we
have here a complete reversal of the relationship with Cynthia. Instead
of John being dominant he is now the passive partner with Yoko being
stronger and more pro-active. The cause was threefold. First, there was
a change in John’s personality; and second, Yoko brought out a differ-
ent side of his personality – the vulnerable child to her mother parental
figure. Finally, as the new partner (Yoko) responded to him quite differ-
ently – she wouldn’t tolerate his aggressive bullying side and, in fact,
taught him how to be submissive and respectful towards women. John
moved from a game in which he was the instigator and focal point of
the manipulations that characterised his first marriage, to a subsequent
game in which he was now the object of his partner’s orchestrations.
The more general point illustrated here is that the players are not nec-
essarily permanently stuck in their game roles – although, of course,
while they remain in a particular relationship their roles and behaviour
may be relatively fixed. However, a change of partners may open up the
possibility that dominant and submissive positions in one relationship
may be reversed in another.
Games of attrition
An example
To further illustrate how this works and what I mean by ‘interroga-
tion’, let me draw from my own personal experience – the relationship
between my parents (both of whom passed away some time ago). My
father was an excellent example of the lead player in a game of attri-
tion with my mother as the counter-attacking partner responding to
his never-ending complaints and criticisms. Any aspect of domestic
life could become the focus of his negativity and disapproval and my
mother was always blamed for whatever it was that bothered him. If his
shirt collars were not cleaned perfectly, if he found dust anywhere in
the house, if she failed to iron an item of clothing to his exacting stan-
dards he would begin – in a low key way at first – to let his objections be
known.
Deficit or Energy-Draining Games 121
But these objections didn’t just take the form of isolated comments
bluntly expressed and quickly forgotten. They were woven into a cease-
less carping commentary on the unsatisfactory state of affairs – why
it had arisen and how it could have been avoided. The sequence would
build and build with her fending off the flow of tedious barbs until even-
tually, she would explode in rage. The next phase would be a full-scale
shouting match with her becoming increasingly volatile and aggressive
whilst he continued to stoke things up with his endlessly repeated criti-
cisms and comments. The third phase would be mutual withdrawal into
a vicious spiky silence punctuated by periodic outbursts – the overflow
of after-thoughts on the subject. The subsequent atmosphere would be
unbearably tense, with my parents hardly able to tolerate each other’s
presence. This uneasy cessation of hostilities would last until the next
round was initiated by yet another of my father’s complaints. And so
the pattern would be repeated ad nauseam.
However, my father’s irritations were not limited simply to what
he considered to be his wife’s infractions of her domestic ‘duties and
responsibilities’. He would routinely interrogate her as to what she had
said and to whom, where she went, and what she did when she was out
of his immediate presence. He was particularly concerned about exactly
what she said to other people, the minute details of the conversation or
gossip that had transpired and which friends, neighbours or (extended)
family members were privy to which bits of information. This behaviour
was partly caused by his desperation to keep a secret of the fact that
he was unemployed most of the time (and couldn’t hold down a job
anyway, because of his lack of social skills).
Of course, this made him paranoid about controlling information
about himself and the economic status of the family. Thus his surveil-
lance and interrogation techniques, as they were practised on my
mother (and me to a growing extent), were designed to restrict the flow
of information and preserve his ‘terrible secret’. But he used these tech-
niques in the same manner as his general complaining and criticism.
Instead of a brief, one-off exchange he would keep on and on ask-
ing, probing, demanding, double-checking the facts, searching for any
perceived weaknesses in your story, making sure you were telling him
everything, over and over again, driving my mother (and me) crazy.
Incidentally, as the references to my own involvement may have sug-
gested, children who witness such relationships are not simply external
bystanders. They are as much a part of the relationship as the cou-
ple themselves and as such they live out the emotions, tensions and
conflicts of the partners. In this sense the anxieties and fears that the
122 Intimacy and Power
children ‘inherit’ from this experience begin to have a life of their own.
In my case until I was in my teens I was extremely anxious and fearful
that my mother (who was the central block in my emotional stability)
might leave him (and thus abandon me) and that I would be left to fend
for myself against ‘him’.
him in the way he would have liked. Both deficiencies thwarted his
ambition and culminated in a sense of failure in life – of not being good
enough.
Another question concerns the fact that the apparent objective of the
lead player is to suck the life-energy from their partner until he or she is
broken and defeated. But why would anybody do this? One possibility
is that it is a desperate attempt to resuscitate themselves – their self-
esteem, competence and power – by simply stealing another’s energy. Of
course, the game is tragic because it is so comprehensively self-defeating.
It drains the energies of each of the individuals and the relationship as a
whole to the extent that it creates a huge deficit – a black hole of escaped
energy – making any semblance of intimacy impossible.
After this, however, the game is often abandoned as the partners become
more and more disclosing towards one another.
However, in some established friendships and romantic partnerships,
the game is played on a routine basis and with much greater seriousness
and intensity. In these, the game is no longer exploratory. Rather, it has
become a means of withholding intimacy or keeping it within certain
‘manageable’ limits (typical of ‘episodic’ or ‘semi-detached’ intimacy).
There are three aspects to this. First, the emotional withholding or avoid-
ance is only partial. It is not that the ‘withholder’ is not giving anything
at all; rather, they selectively avoid important relationship issues. For
example, they may be reluctant to say how much they love and care for
their partner, how much they value them, or how much they want to
do things together.
Second, at the same time, the withholding or avoidance is a way
of keeping control. Avoiding vulnerability seems a crucial part of this
game. The player doesn’t say ‘I’m not available at all’. Rather, he or she
communicates to the effect that ‘some part of me is available but there is
also a secret part of me that isn’t’, and this is a seductive ploy that keeps
the other interested. But this secret part can never be divulged because
it would make the person vulnerable and, hence, controllable. Finally,
although the withholder keeps control for himself or herself, they simul-
taneously drain energy from the relationship and, as a consequence,
both partners suffer.
Ego-centred games
The two versions of ego-centred games are very much related, but have
slightly different emphases. In both games one player seeks attention at
the expense of the other. In the first game, ‘poor me’, the lead player
appeals to his or her ‘victim’ status to grab attention and create pity as
126 Intimacy and Power
Types of intimacy
These games are mainly associated with ‘manipulative’ and ‘pretence’
intimacy. In the first, a person uses their ‘victim’ or ‘important’ status
while encouraging their partner to be supportive. If a man ‘leads’, the
relationship may gravitate around him because of his ‘important’ job
(and correspondingly inflated ego), while his ‘stay-at-home’ wife rele-
gates her ‘less important’ responsibilities. Of course, this mirrors the
now somewhat stereotyped division in society between the mainly male
‘public domain’ of work, business and careers, as compared with the pri-
vate, female domain of family life, housework and childrearing (Smith
1988, Cameron 2007). In the public domain general intimacy, caring
and interpersonal relations are subordinated to, and thus neglected, in
favour of the family income, consumption patterns, contact networks
and so on.
An individual might employ the ‘poor me’ variant of the game includ-
ing all manner of emotional blackmails as a means of retaining attention
in the relationship. Thus his or her pleas and appeals may include ‘after
all the sacrifices I’ve made’, or ‘after all I’ve done for you’, or ‘all the
support I given you’, to ‘I could handle this if my health wasn’t so
bad’, or even ‘I always seem to be the one who gets hurt’. In either case
(man or woman as lead) the manipulator tries to guilt trip their partner
into accepting that their desires and interests come second in the rela-
tionship. But it is important to recognise that in ‘poor me’ or ‘look at
me’ the partner is not coerced or intimidated. The partners are ‘willing
slaves’, and may even be actively supportive of their spouses or part-
ners. For all intents and purposes, they are ‘happy’ with the role offered
to them and in this sense the union could be described as ‘harmoniously
imbalanced’.
Deficit or Energy-Draining Games 127
By contrast, when these games are linked with pretence intimacy, nei-
ther partner is willing to accept a subordinate position. Private deals and
pacts serve to equalise the apparent differences in power and control.
For example, a housewife who enthusiastically supports her husband
may do so only if her husband allows her to call the shots in the more
private, domestic sphere. Thus, for example, she may have the deciding
vote in many important matters concerning intimacy (such as lovemak-
ing, disciplining the children, family relations – who is invited over, who
is visited – and so on). In this manner the public image of the couple as
one in which a particular partner is thought to ‘wear the trousers’ may
be an illusion – because both wear them in different contexts.
With pretence intimacy, in the ‘poor me’ and ‘look at me’ games both
partners take the lead – although perhaps at different times. Appeal-
ing to either their victim status or their greater ‘importance’ provides
one, or both, with a means of ‘stealing’ energy from the other. Inter-
estingly, both partners may play the same game, trying to outwit one
another at every turn. Thus they might try to ‘out-victim’ or ‘out-rank’
one another in an attempt to bag more attention and energy from a
rapidly depleting reservoir. In such a case claiming to be a victim is used
in a more aggressive fashion as a means of gaining the upper hand in
the relationship.
These games revolve around the shared fears, insecurities and depen-
dency needs of the partners. Because of their psychological vulnera-
bilities they become over-reliant on one another. But the price they
must pay for the protection afforded by their bond is some loss of their
identities as individuals. Protective unity is achieved at the expense
of independence and self-reliance. Clearly, the partners inhabit a self-
imposed prison but are also painfully aware that they can’t easily escape
from it. Thus, both partners secretly harbour feelings of resentment.
Unpredictable as it may seem, intimacy in these games is a mixture
of the genuine and phoney. The partners possess two overriding, but
strongly opposing needs. Their primary need is to maintain the loyalty,
support and commitment of their partner so that they continue to be a
source of psychological support and confirmation of themselves as indi-
viduals. But this cuts into their ability to give genuine affection, care
and love. Every expression of care and tenderness is compromised by
the need to enslave the other. Thus seduction is always interlaced with
phoney elements. The need to grab and maintain the other’s attention
128 Intimacy and Power
Concluding comment
Partners feel energised if they have some control over one another’s
feelings and responses. The sense of aliveness that comes from a sat-
isfying relationship makes them feel vibrant and enthusiastic about life.
Thus, a sense of control is essential to feelings of competence. Being suc-
cessful in social life depends on mutual benign control, in which there
is an exchange of emotional (psychic) energy. If a relationship is formed
around benign control its non-competitive, creative and intuitive nature
will make it more energising. However, if exploitation or manipulation
is involved, relationships will be energy draining.
In energising games partners cater for one another’s personal needs so
as to create emotional closeness. By supporting one another’s security,
self-esteem, sense of self-worth and so on, they boost their intimacy.
In energy-draining games, intimacy needs are thwarted or suppressed
by a lack of concern about feelings, interests and desires. In everyday
life emotional and identity needs are closely connected with feelings
of security and self-esteem (Layder 2004b). Someone low on security is
often afraid and anxious and has difficulty with intimate relationships
(Laing 1969). A person who lacks self-esteem will feel ‘inappropriate
to life’ (Branden 1985) and its challenges and may shy away from
intimate contact. Other personal needs such as self-confidence, love,
acceptance and approval, self-worth and self-respect, feeling special and
understood, also depend on a foundation of security and self-esteem. In
energy-draining games partners tend to attack or undermine these needs
while in energising games they are supported and enhanced by the use
of positive ‘strokes’ (Berne 1966).
In this chapter I describe five closely linked energising games, which
differ in terms of the psycho-emotional needs they target most. Thus,
while one game emphasises approval and acceptance, another might
130
Energising Games 131
But partners may begin to take one another for granted, creating
resentments and leading to emotional drift.
Of course, real intimacy can only survive if partners don’t take each
other for granted. In this sense, optimal intimacy is always in danger
of ‘going wrong’ and, thus, lasting relationships are those that con-
stantly ‘correct’ themselves before they go awry. Partners spot problems
as, or before, they arise and deal with them constructively. Such rela-
tionships are more resilient because partners are ever alert to danger and
avoid excessive energy loss and emotional drift by continually review-
ing and redefining their relationship. It’s not that energising games are
completely free of disruptive elements. Rather, by approaching prob-
lems constructively and reflectively partners avoid getting bogged down.
In contrast, partners in energy-draining games are continually steal-
ing energy from each other. They are on a downward spiral and the
relationship is constantly losing energy.
Identity-affirming games
must be confident of making things happen and feels that they are not
simply being ‘pushed around’ by forces beyond their control. Partners
who regularly affirm each other’s individuality, therefore, support one
another’s self-confidence. The message sent out is that both partners are
strong and reliable and can be ‘depended upon’.
Each time a person’s unique qualities are noticed and given attention,
the importance of their independence is underscored. This is crucial
because if a person lacks full independence – that is, if they remain
largely dependent on others for approval, for self-esteem and so on, then
this will tend to upset the balance of power and control in the relation-
ship. Of course, in one sense the pull towards independence would seem
the opposite of companionship, friendship or even romance. But inde-
pendence and togetherness are equally important human needs. We all
need some personal space as well as connectedness with others (Layder
1997, 2004a). However, as individuals, we vary in how much we want
of these things and sometimes this makes it difficult to strike the right
balance. Honouring one another’s special qualities, however, may go
some way towards achieving a working balance because it allows – even
encourages – partners to be clearer about how much they value their
independence (or personal space) as against their need for involvement
and connectedness.
In mutually satisfying intimacy there is some merging of the partners’
identities, in so far as they tend to see themselves as a couple rather than
separate individuals (Perel 2007). But it is equally important that a per-
son’s identity is not lost, confused or swamped by the other’s influence.
Truly energising games require a balance between the independence and
the involvement of the partners (Layder 2004a). Being one’s own person
in terms of what an individual thinks and does, and not simply ‘fitting
in’ with other’s wishes must be balanced by a willingness to accommo-
date partner’s needs and interests. If partners attend to other essential
relationship requirements, such as making time for one another, then
mutual respect and trust will be reaffirmed and gratitude and pleasure
will be generated (Reibstein 1997).
In optimally energising intimacy partners respect and value one
another’s unique identities and personal qualities. Thus identity-
affirming games are crucial since everyone needs to be recognised and
valued for who they are as individuals – their personal identity. How-
ever, there do seem to be differences in terms of which aspects of
individuality are most highly valued. Some individuals tend to value
independence more than involvement and hence it might seem that
they would be more at ease with this game while those who value
togetherness more might be less so. However, it may simply be that
Energising Games 137
Empathy games
Such remarks and comments grow out of the shared experiences and
emotional bonding that energise intimate relationships. Combined with
hugging, putting arms round shoulders, kissing, gazing and so on, such
comments convey emotional rapport, support, empathy and attune-
ment. Above all, empathy games depend on how things are said rather
than what is being said (Austin 1962). It’s not only the words themselves
but also the meta-messages of feeling and empathy that are crucial in
conveying that a partner is truly understood. He or she must be con-
vinced that what is said is not simply a string of ‘empty’ words put
together in order to produce an effect – to get him or her ‘on side’, so to
speak. They want to know that what is said is really meant, because their
emotional life is at stake. In order for them to feel valued, appreciated,
loved and cared for, these things must be conveyed authentically and
sincerely.
By engaging in empathy games partners are reaffirming their bond
through experiences, emotions and feelings of pleasure that togeth-
erness can create. At this elemental level deep empathy, frisson and
rapport energise and re-energise intimate relationships. The common
ground of shared feeling is important because it is the fundamental
bedrock of relationships. In fact, it is because of this that differences
in personality, needs and desires can be reconciled or overcome. The
protective emotional wraparound of an intimate bond makes it a secure
refuge that protects us from loneliness. In this sense, empathy games
develop over time as the partners’ shared experiences build up and their
knowledge of one another continues to deepen. But again, it must be
stressed that this is an emotional ‘knowing’ rather than one solely based
on facts and information. The substance of the game, therefore, is deep
knowledge based on mutual self-disclosure.
Altruistic games
This game brings together elements contained in the other four but it
also has features that they do not have. In one sense it’s the complete
opposite of addictive or co-dependent games which are unremittingly
energy draining. In the latter, partners simply ‘co-exist’ and ‘survive’
in stultifying mutual dependence, rather than genuinely ‘live’ together.
They fear that contact with others might jeopardise the intensity, exclu-
sivity and security of their bond, so they jealously monitor one another’s
behaviour. Mutually supportive games reverse these emphases com-
pletely. Partners encourage one another to freely pursue outside contacts
and interests without fear that this will undermine the relationship. The
game encourages a working balance between partners’ independence
and involvement, rather than a fraught and unhealthy symbiosis. The
game is reflected in the following comments:
‘Let’s not fall into a rut, we should try new things together/separately’
‘You should develop your talents in. . . . music/art/selling’
Energising Games 141
‘It’s good that we have separate interests even though we are very
close’
‘I’m really happy that you’ve achieved what you set out to’
‘You never stifle my curiosity and I appreciate that’
‘I like the way you encourage me to be independent – even though
you support me as well’
‘I know you value me as an individual and don’t treat me simply
as . . . your dutiful wife/husband/partner’
‘We don’t try to change one another, and we allow each other freedom
and space’
‘I’m glad you like my friends, they’re very important to me’
‘Spending time apart from each other can help us appreciate what we
mean to one another’
Interweaving games
Relationship rules
satisfying intimacy. But if one (or several) of the games is absent, then
the collective strength and flow of energy will be weakened and the rela-
tionship will hit trouble. When its tributary streams of energy dry up
and peter out, so the overall energy levels fall. The relationship becomes
de-energised, and intimacy deteriorates – as when ‘dynamic’ intimacy
becomes ‘episodic’ or ‘semi-detached’.
10
The Erosion of Intimacy
All intimate relationships have their troubles and testing times. None
are immune from periods in which partners feel that they aren’t getting
what they hoped for from the arrangement. Usually, these are ‘normal’
crises from which the partners recover quickly and resume ‘business as
usual’. Even the very best of intimate relationships encounters such rou-
tine ‘turbulence’. However, other problems may be more traumatic and
severely test the resolve of the partners. How are these problems cre-
ated and what consequences do they have? This chapter traces how the
unravelling or disappearance of energising games can lead to the down-
grading of a relationship – in particular from ‘dynamic’ to ‘episodic’ and
‘semi-detached’ intimacy.
146
The Erosion of Intimacy 147
Episodic intimacy
(1) Intimacy is intermittent and less intense.
(2) Emotional energy and commitment are partly withdrawn.
(3) Relationship is based on compromises and routines.
(4) Intimacy used selectively to manage problems of emotional drift
and estrangement (e.g. sex as substitute for emotional disclosure).
(5) Disclosure is less authentic:
(6) Original partner is no longer the sole anchor of trust or sole source
of emotional satisfaction.
(7) Restrained intimacy allows more personal space and independence.
Semi-detached intimacy
(1) Frustrated desire for greater closeness.
(2) Concealed anger and unspoken resentments.
(3) Emotional withdrawal and drift because of concealed anger and
unspoken resentments.
(4) Relationship of convenience, produced by inertia and/or lack of
alternatives.
The Erosion of Intimacy 151
this is a bad idea even in the best of relationships. Partners must remain
‘in touch’ both superficially, as well as in a deeper sense, to check out
the validity of their assumptions about each other. A relationship will
die unless it is ‘worked at’ through the regular exchange and updating
of feelings and opinions through talk.
If this kind of habituation (getting to know each other ‘too well’) is
coupled with misunderstandings, clashes of styles or mismatching inti-
macy needs, then there is even less incentive for partners to talk. The
same arguments and disputes will tend to be repeated over and over
again, each time with little in the way of resolution or greater under-
standing. This makes individuals all too aware of their powerlessness –
their inability to make a difference to their partner’s views or behaviour.
Over countless instances of such deadlock, they come to regard talk
(and communication in general) as a pointless exercise. Of course, such
a vacuum is likely to produce a festering wound, because important
things are being left unsaid. Crucial feelings are not expressed, and in
their wake leave a trail of misassumptions and ill feelings that remorse-
lessly accumulate over time. But partners caught up in chains of hurtful
misunderstanding are also entangled in the cycle of events described ear-
lier. Their lack of influence on each other simply reinforces their hurt,
frustration and resentments.
Another reason why the ‘keep talking’ rule often disappears from a
relationship agenda is that certain topics – especially emotional or sex-
ual ones – become taboo, giving rise to mutual inhibition. This may
happen if one partner is already sensitive, fearful or inhibited about
expressing feelings, or directly talking about intimacy. If a person rou-
tinely tries to talk about them, but is met with reluctance or steadfast
refusal, an imbalance is created in the relationship. Over time the more
enthusiastic partner may also become ‘sympathetically’ inhibited as he
or she realises that their partner is chronically anxious or shy about
talking about intimate topics. After a number of failed attempts at dis-
cussion, talk in general may be abandoned eventually because of the
embarrassment it causes all round. Again, feelings of powerlessness
create further emotional and communication blockage.
In addition, once off the agenda a sensitive issue becomes even more
highly charged, precisely because it may not be broached. Its very
‘unapproachable’ nature makes it seem more important. As a result spe-
cific problems are intensified because they are left un-addressed and
unresolved. The emotional and communication blockage prevents the
regular injection of positive energy and positive emotions necessary for
closeness. The build up of frustration, irritation and anger, hopelessness,
154 Intimacy and Power
failure and disillusionment can only lead to the more or less permanent
estrangement of the partners.
Displaced arguments
When intimacy is on the slide, certain conflict styles can only make
things worse. For example, in the ‘high-level attack’ (Quilliam 2001, see
Chapter 6) a person explosively vents their anger at the other in a crit-
ical tirade. Because this is an off-loading of venom rather than a true
exchange of feelings, it offers little chance of any meaningful dialogue.
In this sense high-level attacks perpetuate a war of words that has little
to do with real relationship problems. Similarly, in the ‘retreat’ style a
person simply removes himself or herself from a conflict situation and
refuses to engage in discussion or dialogue. In effect, ‘retreaters’ deny
their own feelings as well their partner’s and only worsen any emotional
and communication blockage. Both ‘high-level attacks’ and ‘retreat’ are
particularly destructive of energising games that target individuality,
and emotional understanding (games 2 and 3).
‘Appeasement’ in which a partner backs off from disagreement and
denies her or his own feelings is also rather negative in this respect. But
because it does not involve an attack on partners’ integrity it may work
to shore up, rather than undermine, a relationship. Instead, appease-
ment may allow individuals to enjoy the protective benefits of the
bond, even though they may be emotionally withdrawn. However, if
used too frequently, appeasement may lead to hopelessness, despair and
depression. This is because the suppression of agency and self-efficacy
on which appeasement is based is closely linked to the cycle of emo-
tional blocking. Low-level attacks, such as nagging, complaining, being
contemptuous and emotional blackmail, similarly, don’t involve empa-
thy or connection. They solely reflect the perpetrator’s interests and
The Erosion of Intimacy 155
Perhaps the biggest problems for intimacy stem from habituation – get-
ting to know one another deeply, perhaps too well, over time. This is
inevitable in any longstanding relationship, but if it isn’t carefully han-
dled, it can be the ruin of close relationships. In many cases the effects of
habituation are made worse by differences or changes in partner’s inti-
macy needs. One of the trickiest problems is finding the right balance
between independence and involvement (Miller 1995, Layder 1997,
Perel 2007). This raises very sensitive emotional issues that require subtle
handling at the best of times – if, indeed, partners are willing to confront
them in the first place. For example, a partner might demand more space
and time to himself or herself at various points, while their ‘needier’
partner may resist such ‘pressure’. Tensions around this dilemma can
escalate causing great harm to both partners.
Perhaps most damage is done by the growing over-familiarity of part-
ners who come to know one another so thoroughly that they no longer
hold the same fascination or interest. If partners are aware of the danger
of taking each other for granted, they may make a concerted effort to
counter its effects. For example, they might show greater appreciation
of the trust and dependability that come from familiarity. In fact, these
may provide comfort and stability for the relationship – giving rise to
such sentiments as ‘I love him (or her) because I know him (or her) so
well’, ‘I know where I am with them’, ‘I don’t have to guess what they
want or need, I already know’.
The down side of knowing a partner too well is boredom and pre-
dictability, because he or she never does anything that is surprising or
exciting (Perel 2007). Partners may have traits and habits that are irritat-
ing, and in this sense, familiarity literally breeds contempt. As a result, in
The Erosion of Intimacy 157
The problems posed by habituation are ever present (and become more
important the longer a relationship survives), thus it could be said to
be the ‘master’ problem for intimacy. That is, because it influences and
envelops other more particular problems, it also conditions them. For
example, the issue of frustrated intimacy needs can be made worse by
habituation, as can problems around marriage and children. Emotional
drift may be exacerbated by the stresses and strains that attend childrea-
ring. Different strains and tensions may overlay one another, making
the overall problem all the more difficult.
With the addition of children, marriage (or cohabitation) this can
prove testing for intimacy. Couples sometimes have difficulties with
loyalty issues in moving from their birth families to marital or cohabita-
tion partnerships. Questions about whose needs – parents or partners –
should be given priority become problematic (Reibstein 1997). Is it fea-
sible or fair to spend more time with parents (say, looking after them if
158 Intimacy and Power
they are ill), than caring for your partner when they are most vulnerable
(say, after starting a new job in a new town)?
Reibstein (1997) suggests that the question of divided loyalties is most
pressing at the beginning of marriage or cohabitation, because individ-
uals are, perhaps, still uncertain about how much their partner wants
and needs them. But there seems no good reason why the problems
around divided loyalty should be pertinent only to the early part of a
relationship. Such issues can roll on from year to year and never be sat-
isfactorily resolved. In relationships that oscillate between episodic and
semi-detached intimacy, partners are sensitive about the authenticity
and stability of their ‘loyalties’ to each other. In these cases, recurrent
charges that ‘you think more about your parents than me’ or ‘everyone
else comes before me’ may become part of an established repertoire of
harboured grudges that surface in bouts of bickering and sniping.
For men, a particular problem arises as a result of feeling displaced
by the arrival of a child, who claims the attention and affections of
their mother, and moves the focus for intimacy away from husbands/
partners. Some men feel pushed to the margins of family life. As Reib-
stein (1997: 155) notes, ‘the frequency and satisfaction with sex declines
sharply in the early years of having children . . . with the concomitant
decline in intimacy’. Couples, especially men, often feel pushed apart by
this. Men may develop more distant emotional relationships with their
wives/partners. Of course, this can breed dissatisfactions and strains in
relationships, which, before children, were close.
The middle years of parenthood, when children become more inde-
pendent and begin to leave, is another sensitive period for partners.
They now have more time to spend with each other, but if they do
not use the increased time to refocus on each other – to ‘get to know’
one another again – it sends out a message that they have a low priority
for one another. Even if this is not actually said or implied, it is a tacit
‘void’ in the relationship that has massive emotional resonance. It is a
time when partners may realise that they don’t have (or no longer have)
as much in common as they thought.
Their disappointment and lack of connection in these years may lead
to emotional and communication blockages. More seriously, but typical
of semi-detached intimacy, partners may begin to search for satisfac-
tion outside the bond. So in the post-children phase of a relationship
emotional estrangement may increase as partners find that they want to
spend less and less time together. They may become disillusioned with
the ‘quality’ of the relationship and disappointed that they won’t be
spending their later years looking after, and caring for each other.
The Erosion of Intimacy 159
Whither intimacy?
Interpersonal control
160
The Nature of Modern Intimacy 161
Giddens claims that in the context of the pure relationship mutual self-
disclosure creates strong relations of trust and commitment, which play
a huge psychological role in keeping partners together. Such disclosure
is part of a freely given, psychological and emotional investment in one
another and is reflected in high levels of commitment and trust. How-
ever, it is clear that in some types of intimate relationship trust and
commitment are less freely given and far more ‘conditional’. Even in
relationships offering little in the way of emotional or sexual satisfac-
tion there is, nevertheless, a certain amount of social and psychological
support that derives from being a couple. In these cases commitment
and trust are filtered through a web of pseudo-positive regard for the
benefits (no matter how meagre in some cases) of being together.
Contrary to the assumptions of the pure relationship there isn’t a
clear-cut equation between intimacy and generic forms of commitment,
trust, satisfaction or disclosure. When dynamic intimacy corrodes into
episodic or semi-detached types such phenomena do not disappear alto-
gether; rather, they evolve into hybrid forms. Commitment in particular
becomes gradually emptied out of emotional content and becomes
transmuted into more practical or ‘conditional’ forms involving psy-
chosocial protection. In such cases partners may become preoccupied
with ‘sustaining a sense of intimacy’, not only by repairing or shoring up
troubled relationships (Jamieson 1999), but in the more positive sense
of feeding the flow of benign power that may energise or re-energise
them. Such strategies check any tendency for intimacy to unravel and
degrade into mutual pretence, manipulation or co-dependence.
The same is true for the notion of ‘satisfaction’ in intimacy. In
real intimacy (as opposed to the rather abstract depiction of the pure
The Nature of Modern Intimacy 165
A further reason for the fragility of modern intimacy concerns the bal-
ance between individual’s dependence (togetherness, involvement) on
the one hand and their independence (separateness, individuality) on
the other. An underlying assumption of this study is that we are, in
equal measure, unique, independently acting and thinking individuals
as well as social creatures who at every turn are influenced by our social
environment (Layder 1997). Such an approach opposes a rigid division
between an emphasis on the importance of independence (as in ver-
sions of therapy and feminism) as against an emphasis on dependence –
especially in the context of marital intimacy (Reibstein 1997). A proper
understanding of intimacy must acknowledge the balance between indi-
viduality and togetherness. In this sense, genuine interdependence can
only properly be achieved if individuals have enough self-esteem and
approval to sustain them as truly independent beings. Only then can
the necessary balance of motivation and restraint for mutually satisfying
intimacy be achieved.
The Nature of Modern Intimacy 167
But these are not the only, or necessarily the most important, reasons
for the fragility of modern intimacy. Equally crucial is the routine influ-
ence of interpersonal control and the inherent tensions it produces in
intimate partnerships, particularly those that hinge around the struggle
between dependence and independence.
12
Intimacy, Power and Social
Domains
170
Intimacy, Power and Social Domains 171
Both Giddens, and Beck and Beck-Gernsheim deny, and thus neglect
(more explicitly in the case of Giddens 1987), the relatively autonomous
influence of what I refer to as ‘situated activity (the ‘interaction order’
in Goffmans terms). As a result, their respective analyses overlook
the subtle, but profound effect of the emergent dynamics of inter-
personal negotiations on close relationships. Both Giddens, and Beck
and Beck-Gemsheim adopt theoretical positions that assume a direct
(unmediated) relationship between individuals (as social agents) and
institutional and cultural influences on their behaviour (in Giddens, this
is accomplished via ‘the duality of structure’). Such a theoretical posture
elides the conditioning influence of interactional dynamics that work
to create a plurality of types of intimacy, forms of disclosure, commit-
ment, trust and satisfaction that characterises modern intimacy. Such
variation is lost through theoretical and ontological dissolution. This is
perhaps why the pure relationship seems to brook little in the way of
the alternative modes of intimacy described in the body of this book.
Integral to the pure relationship is the reflexive, autonomous self,
and the individualising processes of modernity. In Giddens’ view self-
reflexivity is intrinsically aligned with egalitarianism, emancipation and
‘the radical democratisation of the personal’ (1992: 182). But this stands
in stark contrast to opposing interpretations that have a much less
optimistic tone. Craib has forcefully criticised this optimistic view by
counter-posing it with an emphasis on the importance of disappoint-
ment in intimate relationships as well as the limitations on autonomy
set by social constraints. Also, as Jamieson reminds us, some authors
(Bellah 1985, Strathern 1992, Hochschild 1994) have interpreted the
constant monitoring of the self by the self as ‘part and parcel of a ram-
pant self-obsessive individualism or consumerism which may threaten
to destroy all intimate relationships’ (Jamieson 1998: 40).
Giddens suggests that ‘autonomy means the successful realisation of
the reflexive project of the self – the condition of relating to others in
an egalitarian way’ (1992: 189). However, an undue emphasis on the
autonomy of the self overlooks the intrinsically situated character of the
self. This, in sum, obscures the manner in which situated activity is rela-
tively independent of subjective aspects of agency as well as institutional
Intimacy, Power and Social Domains 179
factors. Thus again, the decisive mediating and conditioning role of the
interpersonal processes that push towards plurality in modern intimacy
is neglected. Moreover, in many types of intimacy the self is enmeshed
in manipulation and exploitation as much as it is in benignity and
egalitarianism. Much depends on the plethora of personal powers, pre-
dispositions and capacities, as well as routine interpersonal games and
relationship habits, all of which exemplify the complex, nuanced and
multidimensional character of modern intimacy.
Bibliography
180
Bibliography 181
Tannen, D. (2002) I Only Say This because I Love You (London: Virago).
Templar, R. (2006) The Rules of Life (Harlow: Pearson Educational).
The Times (2007) Times, 2 July, 11: 6.
Tolle, E. (2005) A New Earth (London: Penguin).
Vaitkus, S. (1991) How is Society Possible? (Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic).
Weber, M. (1964) The Theory of Social and Economic Organization (New York: Free
Press).
Weinstein, E. and Deutschberger, P. (1963) ‘Some Dimensions of Altercasting’,
Sociometry, 26: 454–66.
Index
Note: Locators in bold type indicate figures, those in italics indicate tables.
abandonment, fear of, 20, 33, 35, 56, benign persuasion strategies
93, 106–7, 115, 117 deals and pacts, 103–4
acceptance, approval and, 42–3 emotion work, 104
addictive/co-dependent games, 127–9 enrolment and people
Adrienne, C., 55–6 management, 103
affectionate regard, 17 personal magnetism, 100–1
altruism, 61, 133, 162 seduction, 101–3
altruistic games, 148–9 supporting roles creation, 99–100
approval, and acceptance, 42–3 Berne, E., 128, 130, 142
arguments, as ‘way of relating’, 93, 97 Bernstein, B., 102, 131
attritional games, 119–20, 122–3 Blumer, H., 174
Austin, J., 138 Borcherds, R., 80–81
autonomy, Giddens on, 178 Borg, J., 101
Borker, R., 71–2
balance of power Branden, N., 37, 39, 54, 60, 92, 130
independence and, 136 Brodsky, A., 34, 128
personal development and, 48, 142, bullying, 9
159
relationship fragility and, 166 Cameron, D., 63, 67, 70, 85–6
shifts in, 58, 61, 76 Cancian, F., 134
Baron-Cohen, S., 63, 66, 76–81, 82 children, effect of energy-draining
Baxter, J., 68 games on, 121–2
Beck, U., 1, 10–11, 165, 178 Collins, R., 175
Beck-Gernsheim, E., 1, 10–11, 165, commitment, couple intimacy vs. the
178 pure relationship, 164
benign control communication
concept analysis, 56 importance of, 152–3
dependence on in dynamic need for empathy, 79
intimacy, 160 competitive behaviour, girls vs.
manipulative-exploitative vs., 51, boys, 68
55, 92 Comte, A., 1
Miller’s failure to recognise, conflict styles
19, 56 direct vs. indirect, 93–7
mutual see mutual benign and eroding intimacy, 154–5
control control
and power balance, 58–9 benign see benign control
process analysis, 57–8 energy-draining games and,
and relationship effectiveness, 56 124–5, 131
seduction games, 133 indirectness in communication
and self-interest, 60–1 and, 92
184
Index 185