Webpdf
Webpdf
SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
What are the ultimate motives that instigate individuals’ behavior? What are
the aims of social perception? How can an individual’s behavior be described
both from the perspective of the actor and from the perspective of an observer?
These are the basic questions that this book addresses using its proposed agency-
communion framework. Agency (competence, assertiveness) refers to existence of
an organism as an individual, to “getting ahead” and to individual goal-pursuit;
communion (warmth, morality) refers to participation of an individual in a larger
organism, to “getting along” and to forming bonds.
Each chapter is written by experts in the field and uses the agency-communion
framework to explore a wide variety of topics, such as stereotypes, self-esteem,
personality, power, and politics.
The reader will profit from the deep insights given by leading researchers.
The variety of theoretical approaches and empirical contributions shows that the
parsimonious and simple structure of two types of content in behavior, motives,
personality, self-concept, stereotypes, and more helps to build an overarching
frame to different phenomena studied in psychology.
Current Issues in Social Psychology is a series of edited books that reflect the state-
of-the-art of current and emerging topics of interest in basic and applied social
psychology.
Each volume is tightly focused on a particular topic and consists of seven to ten
chapters contributed by international experts. The editors of individual volumes
are leading figures in their areas and provide an introductory overview.
Example topics include: self-esteem, evolutionary social psychology, minority
groups, social neuroscience, cyberbullying and social stigma.
Typeset in Bembo
by Apex CoVantage, LLC
CONTENTS
Index 181
CONTRIBUTORS
The present Chapter 2 (Chan, Wang, and Ybarra) will be specifically con-
cerned with the very basic evolutionary meaning of agency and communion.
Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002; Gebauer, Wagner, Sedikides, & Neberich, 2013; Hel-
geson & Fritz, 1999; Judd, James-Hawkins, Yzerbyt, & Kashima, 2005; Paulhus &
John, 1998; Paulhus & Trapnell, 2008; Wiggins, 1979, 1991; Wojciszke, 2005;
Ybarra et al., 2008). Despite the different names, the constructs all share a common
core, which refers to agency and communion (see Abele & Wojciszke, 2014, for an
overview).
A look into the literature in motivation, personality, social, and cross-cultural
psychology shows that the agency-communion framework is indeed overarching.
The two themes of agency and communion have, for instance, been shown to
emerge in autobiographical narratives of both adults (Diehl, Owen, & Youngblade,
2004; Uchronski, 2008) and children (Ely, Melzi, Hadge, & McCabe, 1998). They
emerged in research on fundamental motives and values. For instance, McAdams
(1988) distinguished between the intimacy motive (affiliation, communion) and
the power motive (influence, uniqueness, agency); in a similar way, Hogan (1982)
framed his socio-analytic theory around agency and communion, labeling the
two primary human motives “getting ahead” (agency) and “getting along” (com-
munion). Horowitz and colleagues (2006) even used the terms “agentic motive”
(individual influence, control, or mastery) and “communal motive” (connection,
participation in a larger unit with other people). Values can also be distinguished
into these broad classes of content (Rokeach, 1973; Schwartz, 1992; Trapnell &
Paulhus, 2012): people have been shown to differ in the degree to which they
value motives of getting ahead (exemplified by power, expertise, success, etc.)
versus getting along (exemplified by relational obligations, “purpose” in life, and
sacrificing for others; Trapnell & Paulhus, 2012).
The agency-communion conceptualization has been shown to map onto
personality. In particular, work on the circumplex model (Wiggins, 1979, 1991)
showed that personality can be represented with the dimensions of dominance–
submissiveness (agency) and nurturance–cold-heartedness (communion). Paulhus
and colleagues (Paulhus & John, 1998; Paulhus & Trapnell, 2008) extended this
reasoning, using the agency-communion framework to distinguish between dif-
ferent self-presentational styles: an agentic self-presentational style tends to form a
“super-hero” impression (a person presenting him/herself as being able to master
every challenge), whereas a communal self-presentational style tends to form a
“saint” impression (a person presenting him/herself as always acting in moral ways).
In social psychology, the agency-communion conceptualization is prominent
in person perception (Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011; Abele & Wojciszke, 2007; Woj-
ciszke, 2005; Ybarra, Chan, & Park, 2001), group perception (Brambilla, Rusconi,
Sacchi, & Cherubini, 2011; Brambilla, Sacchi, Rusconi, Cherubini, & Yzerbyt,
2012; Leach, Ellemers, & Barreto, 2007), self-perception (Gebauer et al., 2013;
Wojciszke, Baryla, Parzuchowski, Szymkow, & Abele, 2011), gender (Eagly, 1987)
and actor–observer differences in impression formation (Abele, Bruckmüller, & Woj-
ciszke, 2014; Abele & Wojciszke, 2007). It is important in stereotype research ( Fiske
et al., 2002) and even in applied settings like power ( Cislak, 2013), organizations,
marketing, and brand perception (Kervyn, Chan, Malone, Korpusik, & Ybarra,
4 Andrea E. Abele and Bogdan Wojciszke
2014; Andrei, Zait, Vatamnescu, & Pinzaru, 2017). While the majority of this
work has been conducted with North American and Western European samples,
there is some evidence suggesting cultural universality of the Big Two (Abele
et al., 2016; Abele, Uchronski, Suitner, & Wojciszke, 2008; Saucier et al., 2014;
Wojciszke & Bialobrzeska, 2014; Ybarra et al., 2008).
The present Chapter 3 on social cognition (Wojciszke and Abele), Chapter 4 on
stereotypes (Fiske), Chapter 5 on self-concept and self-esteem (Abele and Hauke),
Chapter 6 on motives (Locke), Chapter 7 on social desirability (Paulhus), Chap-
ter 8 on grandiose narcissism (Gebauer and Sedikides), and Chapter 9 on gender
(Sczesny, Nater, and Eagly) elaborate these issues.
Brack, 2013). Whereas persons with more power are driven in their actions and
perceptions by agentic rather than communal considerations, persons with less
power show the opposite pattern. The broader political and cultural context may
also matter in this respect. Researchers begin to study these questions, though
respective research is still scarce. The present chapters 12 (Cislak and Cichocka)
and 13 (Bruckmüller and Methner) deal with these issues (see also Chapter 2).
Book contents
The present book describes the ubiquity of the agency-communion framework
in different fields of psychology. It integrates for the first time several approaches
building on the agency-communion conceptualization, which have been devel-
oped in different fields of psychology, more or less apart from each other. By
bringing together these different approaches we want to facilitate an overview of
the agency-communion framework in different settings; we want to demonstrate
the theoretical and empirical fruitfulness of the agency-communion framework;
we want to show connections between so far unrelated areas of research; and we
also want to suggest new directions of research, as outlined above.
In the second chapter, entitled “Connect and strive to survive and thrive: the
evolutionary meaning of communion and agency,” Chan, Wang, and Ybarra take
an evolutionary point of view and discuss that in order to survive, humans must
affiliate with cooperative others. To thrive, humans must pursue skills and goals.
The authors explore how evaluating other people on the dimensions of communion
6 Andrea E. Abele and Bogdan Wojciszke
and agency is adaptive in service of these two fundamental needs of surviving and
thriving. In particular, communion serves primarily to detect who poses a threat
to group life and should be avoided; agency serves primarily to detect who poses
an opportunity for goal pursuit and should be approached. The authors show how
knowing whether others are communal and agentic can help people acquire status
and assess whether others are suitable partners for long-term mating and parenting.
Chapter 3, entitled “Agency and communion in social cognition,” by Wojciszke
and Abele, gives an overview of these content dimensions, presents the dual per-
spective model of agency and communion and discusses its empirical support. This
model states that the Big Two are closely tied to the basic perspectives in social
interaction, the agent perspective vs. the recipient perspective. Because agents are
focused on goal completion and monitor efficiency of their actions, agentic catego-
ries dominate their perceptions. Recipients are focused on identification of action
goals/consequences and monitor the social value of the observed action (whether
the actor’s intentions are beneficial or detrimental for others), hence communal
categories dominate their perceptions. The agentic perspective is applied to own
actions and actions of close others as well as those on whom the perceiver is inter-
dependent. The recipient perspective is applied to actions of other people, especially
remote ones. The chapter also discusses further factors that affect inference and
processing of agency and communion and shows that the Big Two dimensions are
crucial in perception of organizations, commercial products, and brands.
In the fourth chapter, entitled “Warmth and competence are parallels to com-
munion and agency: Stereotype Content Model” Fiske summarizes 20 years of
research on her Stereotype Content Model (SCM). The SCM proposes that what
people want to know first about other individuals and groups is their intent for
good or ill – their warmth. People’s second information-seeking priority is whether
the others can enact their intent – that is, their competence. SCM data come from
varied methods, and the dimensions generalize across domains. Many researchers
followed this approach, suggesting its utility, but future challenges remain, as the
closing section illustrates.
In Chapter 5, “Agency and communion in self-concept and self-esteem,” Abele
and Hauke build on symbolic interactionism and show that in a similar vein as actor–
observer differences, the self-concept also contains an “actor” component (“how do
I see myself ”; called self-as-identity) and an “observer” component (“how do others
see me”; called self-as-reputation). By distinguishing these two components they can
reconcile a seeming contradiction in research on self-esteem: whereas several theories
on self-esteem suggest that it is dominated by communion, research rather shows
that it is dominated by agency. Abele and Hauke argue and show that the self-as-
identity component is dominated by agency, but the self-as-reputation component
is also saturated with communion. They further demonstrate the fruitfulness of dis-
tinguishing competence and assertiveness as facets of agency as well as warmth and
morality as facets of communion.
In Chapter 6, entitled “Agentic and communal motives,” Locke discusses that
motives to approach communion (e.g., form partnerships), avoid communion
The Big Two as an Overarching Framework 7
(e.g., limit obligations), approach agency (e.g., enhance status), and avoid agency
(e.g., sidestep conflicts) are universals because each has reliably – under certain
circumstances – proven adaptive throughout human evolutionary history. Guided
by upward, connective, downward, and contrastive social comparisons, people
selectively invest in social goals that promise to be fulfilling (e.g., befriending like-
minded others) and divest from goals that threaten to be frustrating (e.g., attack-
ing stronger rivals). Individuals who can harness agency toward communal ends
tend to experience better psychological, physical, and social outcomes. However,
individuals differ in their inclinations towards agentic and communal motives due
to factors such as life history (e.g., unpredictable rearing environments), life stage
(e.g., parenthood), gender, and general sensitivities to costs/rewards. Variations in
social motives across persons and situations are partly mediated by variations in
oxytocin (which tends to amplify communal motives to protect and nurture close
others and social bonds) and testosterone (which tends to amplify agentic motives
to vigorously defend and enhance social rank).
In Chapter 7, “The Big Two dimensions of desirability,” Paulhus argues that
the concept of social desirability has been typically viewed as a single continuum
ranging from undesirable to desirable. However, that conception has led to confu-
sion because of restricting desirability to its social aspect. The author argues for
distinguishing two desirability factors corresponding to the evaluative aspects of
agency and communion. Key evidence comes from factor analyses of desirability
ratings: rather than one, up to 10 dimensions emerge. The predominance of agen-
tic and communal forms of desirability becomes evident when (a) factor extrac-
tion is limited to two factors or (b) responses are collected under stress or speeded
conditions. The bidimensional approach has at least two important implications:
(1) measures of desirable responding must be partitioned into agentic and commu-
nal positivity, and (2) desirability ratings of traits (and other self-descriptors) must
be scored separately for agentic and communal positivity. The two major factors of
social desirability scales – egoistic and moralistic enhancement – are underpinned
by agentic values and communal values, respectively.
In Chapter 8, “Agency and communion in grandiose narcissism,” Gebauer and
Sedikides argue that global self-evaluations (e.g., self-esteem, self-enhancement,
humility) have been typically characterized as saturated with both agentic and com-
munal content. An exception seemed to be grandiose narcissism, a self-evaluation
aspect long thought as inherently agentic to a degree that “communal narcissism”
was considered an oxymoron. Still, the authors summarize theoretical and empiri-
cal evidence that grandiose narcissism has also a communal aspect in addition to
the well-recognized agentic one. The chapter summarizes the agency-communion
model of grandiose narcissism both theoretically and for the evidence for the model’s
six validity criteria.
In Chapter 9, entitled “Agency and communion: their implications for gender
stereotypes and gender identities,” Sczesny, Nater, and Eagly show that agency and
communion are also essential to understanding the psychology of gender. Gen-
der stereotypes, in their descriptive and prescriptive forms, follow from a societal
8 Andrea E. Abele and Bogdan Wojciszke
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the authors of this volume for their inspiring contribu-
tions, their commitment to a really “joint” project and also for their straightfor-
ward cooperation and “communal” email contacts.
10 Andrea E. Abele and Bogdan Wojciszke
We are also thankful for the financial support we received during a long phase
of cooperation. Support came from the Alexander von Humboldt foundation to
both authors (Humboldt 3.4 Fokoop), by grants from the German Research Coun-
cil to Andrea E. Abele (DFG Ab 45/10–1, 10–2) and by 2012/04/A/HS6/00581
grant from Narodowe Centrum Nauki to Bogdan Wojciszke.
References
Abele, A. E., & Brack, S. (2013). Preference for other persons’ traits is dependent on the
kind of social relationship. Social Psychology, 44, 84–94.
Abele, A. E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”? Preferential pro-
cessing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 935–948.
Abele, A. E., Bruckmüller, S., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). You are so kind – and I am so kind
and smart: Actor-observer differences in the interpretation of on-going behavior. Polish
Psychological Bulletin, 45, 394–401.
Abele, A. E., Cuddy, A. J. C., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2008). Fundamental dimen-
sions of social judgment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1063–1065.
Abele, A. E., Hauke, N., Peters, K., Louvet, E., Szymkow, A., & Duan, Y. P. (2016). Facets
of the fundamental content dimensions: Agency with competence and assertiveness:
Communion with warmth and morality. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1810.
Abele, A. E., Uchronski, M., Suitner, C., & Wojciszke, B. (2008). Towards an operation-
alization of the fundamental dimensions of agency and communion: Trait content rat-
ings in five countries considering valence and frequency of word occurrence. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1202–1217.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self
versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 751–763.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition:
A dual perspective model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195–255.
Andrei, A., Zait, A., Vatamnescu, E.-M., & Pinzaru, F. (2017). Word-of-mouth generation
and brand communication strategy: Findings from and experimental study explored
with PLS-SEM. Industrial Management and Data Systems, 117, 478–495.
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: An essay on psychology and religion. Chicago,
IL: Rand McNally.
Brambilla, M., Rusconi, P., Sacchi, S., & Cherubini, P. (2011). Looking for honesty: The
primary role of morality (vs. sociability and competence) in information gathering.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 135–143.
Brambilla, M., Sacchi, S., Rusconi, P., Cherubini, P., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2012). You want to
give a good impression? Be honest! Moral traits dominate group impression formation.
British Journal of Social Psychology, 51, 149–166.
Cislak, A. (2013). Effects of power on social perception: All your boss can see is agency.
Social Psychology, 44, 138–146.
Diehl, M., Owen, S. K., & Youngblade, L. M. (2004). Agency and communion attributes
in adults’ spontaneous self-representations. International Journal of Behavioral Develop-
ment, 28, 1–15.
Eagly, A. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social role interpretation. Hillsdale: Law-
rence Erlbaum.
Ely, R., Melzi, G., Hadge, L., & McCabe, A. (1998). Being brave, being nice: Themes of
agency and communion in children’s narratives. Journal of Personality, 66, 257–284.
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Co.
The Big Two as an Overarching Framework 11
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereo-
type content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and
competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.
Gebauer, J., Wagner, J., Sedikides, C., & Neberich, W. (2013). Agency-communion and
self-esteem relations are moderated by culture, religiosity, age, and sex: Evidence for the
“self-centrality breads self-enhancement” principle. Journal of Personality, 81, 261–275.
Helgeson, V. & Fritz, H. (1999). Unmitigated agency and unmitigated communion: Dis-
tinctions from agency and communion. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 131–158.
Hogan, R. (1982). A socioanalytic theory of personality. In M. Page (Ed.), Nebraska Sympo-
sium on Motivation (pp. 336–355). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Horowitz, L. M., Wilson, K. R., Turan, B., Zolotsev, P., Constantino, M. J., & Henderson,
L. (2006). How interpersonal motives clarify the meaning of interpersonal behavior: A
revised circumplex model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 67–86.
Judd, C. M., James-Hawkins, L., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Kashima, Y. (2005). Fundamental
dimensions of social judgement: Understanding the relations between judgments of
competence and warmth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 899–913.
Kervyn, N., Chan, E., Malone, C., Korpusik, A., & Ybarra, O. (2014). Not all disasters are
equal in the public eye: The negativity effect on warmth in brand perception. Social
Cognition, 32, 256–275.
Kervyn, N., Yzerbyt, V. Y., Judd, C. M., & Nunes, A. (2009). A question of compensation:
The social life of the fundamental dimensions of social perception. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 96, 828–842.
Leach, C. W., Ellemers, N., & Barreto, M. (2007). Group virtue: The importance of moral-
ity (vs. competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 234–249.
Markey, P. M. (2002). The duality of personality: Agency and communion in person-
ality traits, motivation, and behavior. (63), ProQuest Information & Learning, US.
Retrieved from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=
2002-95022-259&site=ehost-live
McAdams, D. P. (1988). Power, intimacy, and the life story: Personological inquiries into identity.
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Paulhus, D. L., & John, O. P. (1998). Egoistic and moralistic biases in self-perception: The
interplay of self-deceptive styles with basic traits and motives. Journal of Personality, 66,
1025–1060.
Paulhus, D. L., & Trapnell, P. D. (2008). Self-presentation of personality: An agency-
communion framework. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook
of personality psychology: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 492–517). New York, NY: Guil-
ford Press.
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York, NY: Free Press.
Saucier, G., Thalmayer, A. G., Payne, D. L., Carlson, R., Sanogo, L., Ole-Kotikash, L., ...
Zhou, X. (2014). A basic bivariate structure of personality attributes evident across nine
languages. Journal of Personality, 82, 1–14.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content of values: Theoretical advances and
empirical test in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 1–65.
Trapnell, P. D., & Paulhus, D. L. (2012). Agentic and communal values: Their scope and
measurement. Journal of Personality Assessment, 94, 39–52.
Uchronski, M. (2008). Agency and communion in spontaneous self-descriptions: Occur-
rence and situational malleability. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1093–1102.
Wiggins, J. S. (1979). A psychological taxonomy of trait-descriptive terms: The interper-
sonal domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 395–412.
12 Andrea E. Abele and Bogdan Wojciszke
Wiggins, J. S. (1991). Agency and communion as conceptual coordinates for the under-
standing and measurement of interpersonal behavior. In W. Grove & D. Ciccetti (Eds.),
Thinking clearly about psychology: Essays in honor of Paul Everett Meehl (pp. 89–113). Min-
neapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Wojciszke, B. (2005). Morality and competence in person and self-perception. European
Review of Social Psychology, 16, 155–188.
Wojciszke, B., Baryla, W., Parzuchowski, M., Szymkow, A., & Abele, A. E. (2011). Self
esteem is dominated by agentic over communal information. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 41, 617–627.
Wojciszke, B., & Bialobrzeska, O. (2014). Agency versus communion as predictors of self-
esteem: Searching for the role of culture and self-construal. Polish Psychological Bulletin,
45, 469–479.
Ybarra, O., Chan, E., & Park, D. (2001). Young and old adults’ concerns about morality
and competence. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 85–100.
Ybarra, O., Chan, E., Park, H., Burnstein, E., Monin, B., & Stanik, C. (2008). Life’s recur-
ring challenges and the fundamental dimensions: An integration and its implications for
cultural differences and similarities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1083–1092.
2
CONNECT AND STRIVE TO
SURVIVE AND THRIVE
The evolutionary meaning of communion
and agency
Humans have a universal drive to understand other people’s intentions and behav-
ior. From an evolutionary perspective, being perceptive about others in the social
world – especially strangers – comes with a host of benefits that promote survival
(Cosmides & Tooby, 1992). Through evaluating others, people decide whether a per-
son poses a threat or an opportunity. People need these questions answered quickly
and accurately enough. Fortunately, humans have a well-developed, parsimonious
method for perceiving others, where individuals and situations that we encounter are
quickly evaluated under two dimensions, known as communion and agency.
In this chapter, we start by defining what these two fundamental dimensions
refer to in the context of person evaluation, and we provide evidence that people,
across culture and contexts, readily evaluate people with this communion-agency
lens. Then, we explain how these two dimensions provide functional benefits in
how effectively people can (a) connect with others and (b) reach their goals – two
core human motivations related to survival. Finally, we illustrate how knowing
how communal and agentic other people are can confer specific benefits for solv-
ing three recurring evolutionary challenges: acquiring status, long-term mating,
and reproducing.
(a) what this person’s intentions are, and (b) how these intentions facilitate or hinder
our survival, belonging, and goal-pursuit. To answer these questions, people ben-
efit from knowing whether others are worthy of connecting (i.e., communal) and
whether they are competent (i.e., agentic).
Termed the “Big Two,”1 the dimensions of communion and agency account
in large measure for how people think about others (Bakan, 1966). Communion
refers to individuals’ pro-sociality, affiliative tendencies, and kindness. For exam-
ple, a highly communal individual may be described as someone who is fair,
good-natured, honest, loyal, selfless, sincere, and truthful (Wojciszke, Baryla, Par-
zuchowski, Szymkow, & Abele, 2011). Agency refers to individuals’ abilities, skills,
and ambitions. A highly agentic individual may likewise be described as someone
who is clever, competent, efficient, energetic, intelligent, knowledgeable, and logi-
cal (Wojciszke et al., 2011). Thus, communion refers to how well individuals connect
with other people, whereas agency refers to how well individuals can achieve goals.
These two dimensions are orthogonal but can interact to produce different
impressions (Kervyn, Yzerbyt, & Judd, 2010). This interaction is key from an
evolutionary perspective. For example, someone who is categorized as low on
communion may be perceived as untrustworthy, yet whether they are seen as a
threat to an individual or group depends on whether they are also perceived to
be sufficiently agentic (i.e., competent) to pursue any malicious intentions ( Fiske,
Cuddy, & Glick, 2007). As we will discuss, how someone is perceived and treated
often depends on the perception generated by the interaction of these dimensions
(i.e., opportunity or threat) and the goals of the perceiver.
Much research shows that when people generate descriptions and judgments of
themselves, other individuals, or groups, they rely on these dimensions of com-
munion and agency. For example, when people are asked to describe themselves
or other people in their own words, between 75% and 85% of their open responses
contain descriptions that fall under the dimensions of communion or agency
(Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011, 2013; Uchronski, 2008). Across cultures, whether
individuals or groups are concerned, impressions of their communion and agency
largely account for the variance in global perceptions of them ( Cuddy et al., 2009;
Kervyn, Yzerbyt, Demoulin, & Judd, 2008; Phalet & Poppe, 1997; Wojciszke,
Bazinska, & Jaworski, 1998). This innate preparedness to structure and see the
world through the lens of communion and agency suggests doing so helps meet
fundamental needs and was functional in solving recurring evolutionary chal-
lenges. We now turn to discussing how these dimensions of communion and
agency can respectively help people connect and strive.
protection, reducing threats, and the sharing of skills and resources (West, Grif-
fin, & Gardner, 2007). Thus, people are acutely attuned to whether other people
show traits that signal that they are affiliative.
Selection likely favored affiliating with cooperative, agreeable people who con-
tribute to social groups, and avoiding freeloading, cheating people who deplete
from group life (Neuberg & Cottrell, 2008). Thus, individuals need to be assured
that others are cooperative enough to provide these benefits (vs. confer harm or
threats to them), and that when they help others, others can be trusted to return the
favor (i.e., reciprocal altruism) (Trivers, 1971). Indeed, research shows that in social
dilemmas, socially agreeable individuals are more likely to change their individual
behaviors to favor what would be best for the group, suggesting there is a strong
basis to assume that highly communal people confer direct benefits for group life
(Koole, Jager, van den Berg, Vlek, & Hofstee, 2001).
How do communal evaluations inform these decisions about affiliation, trust,
and cooperation? First, communal traits reflect how affiliative others are. Second,
they signal whether a person is likely to violate the rules of group life. Third,
they provide cues of resource value, or the likelihood that a person will share a
resource, cooperate, and reciprocate ( Scholer & Higgins, 2008). Knowing some-
one is immoral, hostile, or disagreeable – negative communal qualities – are salient
threat cues that they will be unaffiliative, violate the rules of social life, and be of
low resource value. Conversely, positive communal evaluations (e.g., honest and
moral) reduce perceptions of threat, resulting in a more positive evaluation overall
(Brambilla, Sacchi, Rusconi, Cherubini, & Yzerbyt, 2011). Thus, communal traits
broadly inform whether an interaction will be a cooperative or threatening one.
example, although people are adept at detecting who to trust, people may be
even more sensitive at detecting those who violate the rules. After being exposed
to faces of individuals identified as cheaters or as trustworthy individuals, people
were more likely to remember the cheaters’ faces a week later (Mealey, Daood, &
Krage, 1996).
This sensitivity to negative communion evaluations may stem from evolution-
ary mechanisms designed to err on the side of caution when assessing and reducing
threats (i.e., error management theory) (Nesse, 2005). That is, the consequences
of misjudging someone as uncooperative (when they are cooperative) is likely to
be less costly, harmful, or lethal than the consequences of misjudging someone as
cooperative (when they are not). Thus, when cues like cooperation are difficult
to detect accurately, it is often better to err on the evaluation of less consequence
when choosing to enter social interactions. Consistent with this notion, when ini-
tial communal evaluations are negative, they are resistant to revision even in light
of future information or behavior (Reeder & Brewer, 1979; Ybarra, Chan, & Park,
2001). For example, after being told someone is hostile, all future behavior from
that individual is likely to be filtered through this “hostile” label, coloring even
subsequently positive behavioral information. However, when initial communal
evaluations are positive, only one instance of negative information is necessary
to irreversibly update one’s global evaluation in a negative direction (Reeder &
Brewer, 1979; Skowronski & Carlston, 1989; Ybarra et al., 2001).
In sum, relative to other information obtained from evaluations, communal
information is primary, spontaneous, salient, and well-remembered. These features
serve the function of detecting who to affiliate with and who to avoid or treat with
caution. Knowing this information helps individuals avoid threats to survival, and
to select groups and others who can be relied on in order to meet their own goals.
our attention to the agency dimension, which helps answer this question (Peeters &
Czapinski, 1990). Of course, given that these evaluations still involve others, and con-
sistent with the preeminence and dominance of communion in people’s evaluations,
our discussion of agency is intertwined with the fact that agentic evaluations invari-
ably interact with communal evaluations to provide an informative impression.
Evaluating others on agency is concerned with assessing how others provide
value (or lack thereof) for one’s own goals. Specifically, being able to correctly
identify those to imitate, those who will share skills or knowledge, and those who
will provide favors or direct assistance, confers benefits when striving to get ahead.
Threats can not only arise from affiliating with those who are uncommunal, but
also those who are unagentic. For example, investing effort in imitating those who
are incompetent may lead to wasted time or stifled skill development.
Further, people need to identify those who will interfere with their goal pursuit
and identify those who are competitors when resources are limited or obtained
through competition. For example, if someone is evaluated relative to oneself as
highly agentic, they are likely to have increased influence over other people and
resources. Should this person also be highly communal, they may be seen as an
admired teacher that one should seek out for access, assistance, or modeling in order
to reach one’s own goals. In contrast, should this person not be very communal, they
may be seen as an unhelpful competitor one should avoid wasting time on when
seeking assistance but instead be vigilant of in competition (c.f. Cuddy, Glick, &
Beninger, 2011). In sum, knowing who is agentic can aid individuals’ goal pursuit.
Consistent with the notion that agency serves functions related to individual
goal pursuit, how interested people are in processing agentic information depends
on how relevant the situation is to their goal-pursuit (Abele & Wojciszke, 2014).
For example, whether meeting a stranger or a doctor, people should want to deal
with someone who is kind, polite, and caring. Thus, people are concerned with
communal evaluations as a default. However, relative to a stranger, people should
place more weight on evaluating their doctor on an agentic dimension, given
that a doctor is more important for one’s personal goals (i.e., maintaining health).
When it comes to evaluating others on agency, context plays more of a role in how
people approach and process information from this dimension.
Similarly, the goals and skills people concern themselves with will vary depend-
ing on environment and context. For example, individuals living in the tundra
require different skills than those living in the desert. Likewise, what is considered
to be a coveted goal, or what types of behaviors or traits confer respect with one’s
peers, are likely to vary among different cultures. Accordingly, whereas people
tend to highly agree on what traits and behaviors are communal, what constitutes
agentic traits, behaviors, and practices are more likely to vary depending on the
specific environment and culture individuals find themselves in and the opportu-
nities afforded to them (Ybarra et al., 2008).
Given the relevance of agency to goal pursuit, in contrast to communion, a
focus on processing and weighting positive over negative information is likely to
18 Todd Chan, Iris Wang, and Oscar Ybarra
confer advantages for individuals seeking assistance in meeting their goals. That
is, the costs of identifying someone as unagentic when they are agentic is likely to
be more costly than identifying someone as agentic when they are not. Misiden-
tifying someone as unagentic (when they are) may lead to missed opportunities
to learn or missing out on resources due to underestimating one’s competitors;
conversely, the costs of misidentifying someone as agentic (when they are not)
may be wasting time working with someone who turns out to be of little assis-
tance or needlessly exerting energy competing with someone who turns out not
to be a competitor. Further, because it is relatively difficult to “fake” competence,
instances of competence are more likely to be true indicators of positive agency. In
contrast, instances of failure can be attributed to various idiosyncratic reasons, and
without additional evidence, may lead to misidentifying someone as unagentic
when they in fact are ( Skowronski & Carlston, 1989). Thus, it is likely more costly
to prematurely dismiss someone as ineffective when they are not than to work
with someone who turns out to be so.
Indeed, research shows that agentic evaluations show an asymmetrical sensitiv-
ity, but in this case to positive information (Reeder & Brewer, 1979; Skowron-
ski & Carlston, 1989). When people are in competitive contexts, for example, they
report wanting to know more about the positive strengths and qualities of their
opponent over their weaknesses, and remember this positive agentic information
better (Chan & Ybarra, 2002). Thus, in their overall evaluations of agency, people
are more likely to weight information that suggests another person is able and
competent over information that shows that they are failing or incompetent.
Gaining status
Increasing one’s status is one challenge humans continuously strive to solve. Essen-
tially, obtaining status is a relative goal, a goal to increase one’s standing, impor-
tance, and recognition in the social hierarchy ( Fiske, 2010). Increasing one’s status
brings a host of benefits, such as increased access to resources, favors, esteem from
others, and mates (de Waal & de Waal, 2007; van Vugt & Tybur, 2015). Status is
initially acquired by demonstrating physical dominance or that one has relatively
unique skills, resources, or information. Again, what skills and information are
valued in a social context depend on the ecology in which people live, which
dictate what skills are advantageous for problem solving. Nonetheless, given that
status is inherently imbued with agentic characteristics (being competent, unique,
Evolutionary Meaning of Communion and Agency 19
influential, and powerful), people who want to attain status should demonstrate
agentic behaviors (cf. Cheng, Tracy, & Henrich, 2010).
For people seeking status, evaluations of agency and communion are critical.
First, given that increasing status inherently depends on others’ relative standing,
people first need to acquire accurate information about the social hierarchy, which
they appear very motivated to do (Moors & De Houwer, 2005). Given the tight
link between agency and status, having evaluations of others’ agency is necessary
to situate one’s own status among those who are higher and lower than oneself on
the hierarchy (de Waal, 1986). Second, after knowing the status hierarchy, people
can leverage this information to “get ahead” by identifying those on the hierarchy
whom they can affiliate with, model, or seek assistance from in developing skills
that can increase their own status. Individuals with high status receive benefits and
resources from transferring their knowledge to others (van Vugt & Tybur, 2015).
Of course, whether someone is an eager teacher will also depend on how commu-
nal that person is, or how willing that person is to share knowledge and resources,
consistent with the proposal that agentic traits usually interact with communal
ones ( Cuddy et al., 2011).
At the same time, people can use this information to identify who their com-
petitors are, and whether they should compete with these individuals or avoid
them ( Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2008). If the higher-status individual is one who is
competing for the same resource or pursuing the same goal, the person may pose a
threat that may be best dealt with by avoiding this individual; if this higher-status
person is pursuing a different goal, the person may be a potential mentor. Alterna-
tively, if it is known that a lower-status individual is the person who is competing
for the same resource, people may leverage this information to compete more
fiercely in order to protect their own status. In sum, deftly recognizing agency and
communion, with their links to striving and connecting, directly informs how
individuals may most adaptively navigate the challenge of increasing one’s status.
How can evaluations of communion and agency inform whether someone meets
these criteria? Communal attributes – being kind, gregarious, and sociable – are
associated with increased cooperation, loyalty, and fidelity in relationships, and as
such, individuals searching for long-term relationships tend to desire a partner who
is, first and foremost, highly communal (Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Li et al., 2002;
Nettle & Clegg, 2008). These traits also help inform whether individuals will invest
in their dependent offspring. Communal traits signal that an individual possesses
the nurturing qualities that are critical for effective parenting. Women tend to be
viewed as more communal than agentic, perhaps reflective of the fact that they are
the sex that provides greater investment in the nurturing of children (L. B. Lueptow,
Garovich, & Lueptow, 1995; Williams & Best, 1982).
However, people also report a strong desire for an agentic partner. In fact, of the
Big Five, after agreeableness (i.e., communion), openness to experience, linked with
creativity and intelligence (Moutafi, Furnham, & Crump, 2006), is the most valued
factor in a mate (Botwin, Buss, & Shackelford, 1997). Both males and females report
desiring a partner that is open, curious, interesting, and intellectual, all agency-
related attributes, suggesting that these qualities signal fitness in a partner (Buss,
1989). For example, these traits show that an individual possesses the capability to
protect their offspring, acquire resources, and pass on skills and knowledge.
Although agentic information is valued by both sexes, it appears that females
are more likely to privilege evaluations on this dimension (e.g., of intelligence,
resources, status) in the context of long-term mating (Buss, 1989). Females put
forth greater investment in the bearing and rearing of a child (Trivers, 1972). As
such, they are oriented towards choosing a mate that can acquire and invest sus-
tained resources in themselves and their offspring (Buss, 1989).
At the same time, like status, males also need to know other males’ level of agency
to effectively pursue their goal of attracting and retaining a mate. Given that agentic,
high-status individuals (especially males) attract more mates, to effectively engage
and succeed (get ahead) at intra-sexual competition requires being aware of the rela-
tive success, status, and resources of one’s competitors (Cummins, 2005). In sum,
using agency to supplement evaluations of communion when evaluating individuals
as mating partners can provide a wealth of information that can facilitate the process
of finding and attracting a suitable long-term mate as well as partner in parenting.
Conclusion
Over human evolutionary history, humans have had two fundamental moti-
vations: connect with others and develop personal skills and goals. Neither of
these motivations can be accomplished alone, however, and those who succeed at
meeting these motivations know who to leverage for assistance and who to avoid
as a threat. Evaluating others on how communal and agentic they are serves to
parsimoniously inform this question. In particular, as illustrated in this chapter,
they can confer direct benefits for how people can efficiently acquire status and
Evolutionary Meaning of Communion and Agency 21
evaluate people for their long-term potential as mates and as parents. Ultimately,
these two dimensions serve to guide how humans process information in the way
that is most adaptive to their surviving and thriving.
Note
1 Different lines of research have termed these dimensions differently (e.g., warmth and
competence; dominance and warmth) (see Abele & Wojciszke, 2014).
References
Abele, A. E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”? Preferential pro-
cessing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(5), 935–948.
Abele, A. E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2013). The Big Two of agency and communion in lan-
guage and communication. In J. Forgas, O. Vincze, & J. Laszlo (Eds.). Social Cognition
and Communication (S. 173–184). Psychology Press, New York.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition:
A dual perspective model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195–255.
Ackerman, J. M., Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2012). Evolutionary perspectives on social
cognition. In S. Fiske & N. McCrae, The Sage handbook of social cognition (pp. 451–473).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: An essay on psychology and religion. Chicago,
IL: Rand McNally.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497–529.
Botwin, M. D., Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). Personality and mate prefer-
ences: Five factors in mate selection and marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality, 65(1),
107–136.
Brambilla, M., Sacchi, S., Rusconi, P., Cherubini, P., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2011). You want to
give a good impression? Be honest! Moral traits dominate group impression formation.
British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(1), 149–166.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses
tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12 (1), 1–14.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective
on human mating. Psychological Review, 100 (2), 204–232.
Chan, E., & Ybarra, O. (2002). Interaction goals and social information processing: Underesti-
mating one’s partners but overestimating one’s opponents. Social Cognition, 20 (5), 409–439.
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Henrich, J. (2010). Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foun-
dations of human social status. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 334–347.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In J. Barkow,
L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the genera-
tion of culture (pp. 163–228). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cottrell, C. A., Neuberg, S. L., & Li, N. P. (2007). What do people desire in others? A
sociofunctional perspective on the importance of different valued characteristics. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92 (2), 208.
Cuddy, A. J., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimen-
sions of social perception: The Stereotype Content Model and the BIAS map. Advances
in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 61–149.
22 Todd Chan, Iris Wang, and Oscar Ybarra
Cuddy, A. J., Fiske, S. T., Kwan, V. S., Glick, P., Demoulin, S., Leyens, J. P. et al. (2009).
Stereotype Content Model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some dif-
ferences. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48 (1), 1–33.
Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Beninger, A. (2011). The dynamics of warmth and competence
judgments, and their outcomes in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 31,
73–98.
Cummins, D. (2005). Dominance, status, and social hierarchies. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The
handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 676–697). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
De Bruin, E. N. M., & van Lange, P. A. M. (2000). What people look for in others: Influ-
ences of the perceiver and the perceived on information selection. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 26 (2), 206–219.
de Waal, F. B. (1986). The integration of dominance and social bonding in primates. The
Quarterly Review of Biology, 61(4), 459–479.
de Waal, F. B., & de Waal, F. B. (2007). Chimpanzee politics: Sex and power among apes. Bal-
timore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Fiske, S. T. (2010). Interpersonal stratification: Status, power, and subordination. In S. T.
Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition, Vol. 2 (pp. 941–
982). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition:
Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83.
Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., Lüdtke, O., & Neberich, W. (2014). Agency-communion
and interest in prosocial behavior: Social motives for assimilation and contrast explain
sociocultural inconsistencies. Journal of Personality, 82 (5), 452–466.
Kervyn, N., Yzerbyt, V. Y., Demoulin, S., & Judd, C. M. (2008). Competence and warmth
in context: The compensatory nature of stereotypic views of national groups. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 38 (7), 1175–1183.
Kervyn, N., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Judd, C. M. (2010). Compensation between warmth and
competence: Antecedents and consequences of a negative relation between the two
fundamental dimensions of social perception. European Review of Social Psychology, 21(1),
155–187.
Koole, S. L., Jager, W., van den Berg, A. E., Vlek, C. A. J., & Hofstee, W. K. B. (2001).
On the social nature of personality: Effects of extraversion, agreeableness, and feedback
about collective resource use on cooperation in a resource dilemma. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(3), 289–301.
Landrum, A. R., Mills, C. M., & Johnston, A. M. (2013). When do children trust the
expert? Benevolence information influences children’s trust more than expertise.
Developmental Science, 16 (4), 622–638.
Leary, M. R. (2007). The curse of the self: Self-awareness, egotism, and the quality of human life.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Li, N. P., Balley, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., & Linsenmeier, J. A. (2002). The necessities and
luxuries of mate preferences: Testing the tradeoffs. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 82 (6), 947–955.
Lueptow, L. B., Garovich, L., & Lueptow, M. B. (1995). The persistence of gender stereo-
types in the face of changing sex roles: Evidence contrary to the sociocultural model.
Ethology and Sociobiology, 16 (6), 509–530.
Mealey, L., Daood, C., & Krage, M. (1996). Enhanced memory for faces of cheaters. Ethol-
ogy and Sociobiology, 17(2), 119–128.
Miller, G. F., & Todd, P. M. (1998). Mate choice turns cognitive. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
2 (5), 190–198.
Evolutionary Meaning of Communion and Agency 23
Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2005). Automatic processing of dominance and submissive-
ness. Experimental Psychology, 52 (4), 296–302.
Moutafi, J., Furnham, A., & Crump, J. (2006). What facets of openness and conscientious-
ness predict fluid intelligence score? Learning and Individual Differences, 16 (1), 31–42.
Nesse, R. M. (2005). Natural selection and the regulation of defenses: A signal detection
analysis of the smoke detector principle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26 (1), 88–105.
Nettle, D., & Clegg, H. (2008). Personality, mating strategies, and mating intelligence. In
G. Geher & G. Miller (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the minds reproduc-
tive system (pp. 121–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Neuberg, S. L., & Cottrell, C. A. (2008). Managing the threats and opportunities afforded
by human sociality. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 12 (1), 63–72.
Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The dis-
tinction between affective and informational negativity effects. European Review of
Social Psychology, 1(1), 33–60.
Phalet, K., & Poppe, E. (1997). Competence and morality dimensions of national and eth-
nic stereotypes: A study in six eastern-European countries. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 27(6), 703–723.
Reeder, G. D., & Brewer, M. B. (1979). A schematic model of dispositional attribution in
interpersonal perception. Psychological Review, 86 (1), 61.
Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (1996). Strategic self-promotion and competitor derogation:
Sex and context effects on the perceived effectiveness of mate attraction tactics. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 70 (6), 1185–1204.
Scholer, A. A., & Higgins, E. T. (2008). Distinguishing levels of approach and avoidance:
An analysis using regulatory focus theory. In Handbook of approach and avoidance motiva-
tion (pp. 489–503). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1989). Negativity and extremity biases in impression
formation: A review of explanations. Psychological Bulletin, 105(1), 131.
Stirrat, M., & Perrett, D. I. (2010). Valid facial cues to cooperation and trust: Male facial
width and trustworthiness. Psychological Science, 21(3), 349–354.
Todorov, A., Baron, S. G., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Evaluating face trustworthiness: A
model based approach. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(2), 119–127.
Tomasello, M. (1998). Emulation learning and cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sci-
ences, 21(5), 703–704.
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology,
46 (1), 35–57.
Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. G. Campbell (Ed.), Sex-
ual selection and the descent of man (pp. 136–179). Chicago, IL: Aldine Publishing Company.
Uchronski, M. (2008). Agency and communion in spontaneous self-descriptions: Occur-
rence and situational malleability. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38 (7), 1093–1102.
van Vugt, M., & Tybur, J. M. (2015). The evolutionary foundations of status hierarchy. In
D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 788–809). Hoboken, NJ:
Wiley.
West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007). Evolutionary explanations for coopera-
tion. Current Biology, 17(16), R661–R672.
Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1982). Measuring sex stereotypes: A thirty-nation study. New-
bury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Wojciszke, B., Baryla, W., Parzuchowski, M., Szymkow, A., & Abele, A. E. (2011). Self-
esteem is dominated by agentic over communal information. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 41(5), 617–627.
24 Todd Chan, Iris Wang, and Oscar Ybarra
Wojciszke, B., Bazinska, R., & Jaworski, M. (1998). On the dominance of moral categories
in impression formation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24 (12), 1251–1263.
Ybarra, O., Chan, E., & Park, D. (2001). Young and old adults’ concerns about morality
and competence. Motivation and Emotion, 25(2), 85–100.
Ybarra, O., Chan, E., Park, H., Burnstein, E., Monin, B., & Stanik, C. (2008). Life’s recur-
ring challenges and the fundamental dimensions: An integration and its implications
for cultural differences and similarities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38 (7),
1083–1092.
Zebrowitz, L. A. (2017). First impressions from faces. Current Directions in Psychological Sci-
ence, 26 (3), 237–242.
3
AGENCY AND COMMUNION IN
SOCIAL COGNITION
Bogdan Wojciszke and Andrea E. Abele
Social cognition has been a thriving area of research for the last 40 years, becoming
a dominant approach to study human social behavior and its underlying processes.
The focus on cognitive and affective processes has been typically accompanied by
the implicit assumption that processes are content free, that all sorts of content are
processed in the same way. However, a large body of data accrued in the last 20 years
revealed two separate dimensions of content – agency and communion, the “funda-
mental dimensions” or “Big Two” (Abele & Wojciszke, 2007; Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick,
2007; Judd, James-Hawkins, Yzerbyt, & Kashima, 2005; Peeters, 2008; Paulhus &
Trapnell, 2008) – universally present in the perception of the social environment
(persons, groups) and the self. This provokes the question why there are these two
types of content and whether they are processed in the same manner.
The present chapter gives an overview of these content dimensions; it presents
a model offering a theoretical explanation for the duality of content as well as dis-
cusses its empirical support. This model (the dual perspective model of agency and
communion; Abele & Wojciszke, 2014) states that the Big Two are closely tied to
the basic perspectives in social interaction, the agent perspective vs. the recipient
perspective. Agentic content is more salient in the agent perspective and commu-
nal content is more salient in the recipient perspective. We will also discuss further
factors that affect inference and processing of agency and communion. Finally,
we go beyond person perception and discuss how the two types of content shape
perceptions of organizations, commercial products, and brands.
TABLE 3.1 Factor loadings of 10 content dimensions for the 300 trait names (based on data
of Abele & Wojciszke, 2007)
hand, social perception does not only reflect social reality but is also functional and
serves the perceiver’s goals. Regarding the self, this means identifying facilitating
and inhibiting conditions for own plans, evaluations, and goal pursuit (see also
Chapter 5, Abele & Hauke, in this book). Regarding others, this means adequate
identification of their intentions – whether they are beneficial or harmful (which
requires communal inference) for the self and probability of completion of these
intentions (which requires agentic inferences) (Fiske et al., 2007; Wojciszke, 1994).
As the above analysis shows, this A and C framework has seen broad usage. None-
theless, there are differences in the specific ways in which this framework is concep-
tualized in the various research traditions (see Abele et al., 2016; Abele & Wojciszke,
2014). It has therefore been recently suggested that A and C both are two-faceted.
In the case of A, these are the facets of assertiveness (agency-assertiveness; AA)
and competence (agency-competence; AC). Assertiveness reflects the motivational
and volitional component of A, and competence reflects its ability component.
In the case of C, the facets are warmth (communion-warmth; CW) and morality
(communion-morality; CM). While warmth pertains to being benevolent to people
in ways that facilitate affectionate, cooperative relations with them, morality refers
to being benevolent to people in ways that facilitate correct and principled relations
with them by the adherence to ethics and important social values. Both warmth-
and morality-related traits are intentional, and hence motivational, components.
Table 3.2 shows how these facets of A and C can be operationalized.
Whereas most of the theories and studies into the A/C framework have so far
been concerned with A/C, and not with their facets, research into the facets is
emerging and we will briefly cover it in the following sections (see also chapters 4,
5, and 9 in this book).
TABLE 3.2 Items measuring communion facets of morality and warmth, as well as agency
facets of assertiveness and competence (based on data of Abele et al., 2016)
Agency Communion
Assertiveness Competence Warmth Morality
of content reflect the two perspectives taken when observing the social world
(Abele & Wojciszke, 2007, 2014; Wojciszke, Baryla, Parzuchowski, Szymkow, &
Abele, 2011): these are the perspectives of an actor or agent (person who performs
an action) and that of an observer or recipient (person who is on the receiving
end of the action). Agents are focused on goal completion and monitor efficiency
of their actions, hence agentic categories dominate their perceptions. Recipients
are focused on identification of action goals/consequences and monitor the social
value of the observed action (whether the actor’s intentions are beneficial or det-
rimental for others), hence communal categories dominate their perceptions.
The agentic perspective is applied to own actions and actions of close others as
well those on whom the perceiver is interdependent. The recipient perspective is
applied to actions of other people, especially remote ones. We review three main
hypotheses of the DPM and discuss their empirical support.
Primacy of communion
The first hypothesis states that communal categories are primary in social cognition.
This ensues from the fact that social cognition is mainly about inferring goals, inten-
tions (cf. Fiske, the present volume), and traits, with the latter being also heavily based
on intentions. Support for the general primacy of communal content comes from
studies showing its preferential processing. Communal traits are faster recognized in a
lexical decision task than agentic traits (Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011, Study 1; Ybarra,
Chan, & Park, 2001), they are faster categorized as positive of negative, and commu-
nal traits are faster inferred from behavior than agentic traits (Abele & Bruckmüller,
2011, Studies 2 and 3). Also the valence of trait descriptors is much more dependent
on their saturation with communal than with agentic content, and this holds for
several European and Asian languages (Abele et al., 2016; Abele & Wojciszke, 2014).
Whereas perceived agentic qualities decide on significant matters such as hir-
ing, firing, and (sometimes) promotions, communal qualities decide on most vital
matters, such as identity and life-death rulings. A study with family members of
patients afflicted by neurodegenerative diseases showed that the former perceived
large changes in a patient’s identity when the patient’s moral faculty was injured
Agency and Communion in Social Cognition 29
legitimize the existing social order (see Fiske, chapter 4, this volume). Whereas
competence is attributed to high-status groups, thereby justifying their privileged
position, lack of communion is attributed to antagonistic groups, thereby justi-
fying the conflict. These stereotypes also influence the perception of individuals
belonging to the respective groups. Individuals belonging to high-status groups
are attributed more competence than low-status group members, and individu-
als from highly competing groups are perceived as less communal than individuals
from cooperating groups (Russell & Fiske, 2008). The reliance on stereotypes is
moderated by affective states of the perceiver, as, for instance, compared with
negative mood, positive mood leads to a stronger focus on agency in perceiving
of an unknown man, and a stronger focus on communion in perceiving of an
unknown woman ( Szymkow, 2014).
Finally, inferences of the Big Two are amenable to different aspects of social
context. For instance, ascribing agency or communion to others may also influ-
ence how these others perceive the self. In a recent study by Dufner, Leising, and
Gebauer (2016), it was shown that agency follows a “zero-sum principle.” Agency
is related to dominance and status, which can be attained only at the expense of
others. Hence, people who see others as high in agency are perceived as low in
agency themselves. In contrast, communion follows a “non-zero-sum principle”:
communion results in cooperative social relations and is typically reciprocated.
Therefore, people who see others as high in communion are perceived as high in
communion themselves.
A classical difference in processing information on the Big Two is an asym-
metrical diagnostic value of positive and negative information in the two domains.
Whereas in the agency (ability) domain positives weigh more than negatives, in the
communal (morality) domain negatives influence impressions much stronger than
positives. In effect, in the ability domain it is much easier to change impressions
from negative to positive than vice versa, while in the morality domain it is much
easier to change impressions from positive to negative than vice versa (Reeder,
1993; Reeder & Coovert, 1986). These asymmetries have been explained in vari-
ous ways, the most popular being the difference in implicit trait-behavior relations
on which lay perceivers rely (Reeder, Pryor, & Wojciszke, 1992). In the ability
domain, high results are strongly related to – and indicative of – high ability, while
low results can happen to anybody due to lack of motivation, strong impediments,
or exhaustion. In the morality domain, immoral behaviors are strongly related
to – and indicative of – immoral traits, while moral behavior may be shown by
anybody, because it is socially expected and enforced. However, recent research
suggests a more parsimonious explanation of these asymmetries as a result of dif-
ferential frequencies of positive and negative behaviors in the two domains. In
the morality domain, positive behaviors are much more frequent than negative
ones (because the latter are discouraged and punished) and therefore have a lower
diagnostic value. In contrast, in the ability domain, negative behaviors are more
frequent than positive ones, because the latter require rare abilities. These asym-
metries were empirically shown in a study that also revealed a direct link between
34 Bogdan Wojciszke and Andrea E. Abele
Conclusions
We have shown that the “Big Two” are in fact “Big Two” content dimensions that
dominate inferences of persons, the self, of groups, and even of organizations and
Agency and Communion in Social Cognition 35
References
Aaker, J. L., Garbinsky, E. N., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Cultivating admiration in brands:
Warmth, competence, and landing in the “golden quadrant.” Journal of Consumer Psy-
chology, 22, 191–194.
Aaker, J. L., Vohs, K. D., & Mogilner, C. (2010). Non-profits are seen as warm and for-
profits as competent: Firm stereotypes matter. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 224–237.
Abele, A. E., & Brack, S. (2013). Preference for other persons’ traits is dependent on the
kind of social relationship. Social Psychology, 44, 84–94.
Abele, A. E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”? Preferential pro-
cessing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 935–948.
Abele, A. E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2013). The Big Two of agency and communion in lan-
guage and communication. In J. Forgas, O. Vincze, & J. Laszlo (Eds.), Social cognition
and communication (S. 173–184). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Abele, A. E., Bruckmüller, S., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). You are so kind – and I am kind
and smart: Actor-observer differences in the interpretation of on-going behavior. Polish
Psychological Bulletin, 45(4), 394–401. DOI: 10.2478/ppb-2014-0048
Abele, A. E., Hauke, N., Peters, K., Louvet, E., Szymkow, A., & Duan, Y. P. (2016). Facets
of the fundamental content dimensions: Agency with competence and assertiveness:
Communion with warmth and morality. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1810.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective
of self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 751–763. DOI:
10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.751
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition:
A dual perspective model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195–255.
Andrei, A., Zait, A., Vatamnescu, E.-M., & Pinzaru, F. (2017). Word-of-mouth generation
and brand communication strategy: Findings from and experimental study explored
with PLS-SEM. Industrial Management and Data Systems, 117, 478–495.
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: An essay on psychology and religion. Chicago,
IL: Rand McNally.
Baryla, W., & Wojciszke, B. (2018). Success leads to agentic cognition: Two field studies.
Social Psychological and Personality Science.
Baryla, W., & Wojciszke, B. (2017). The morality neglect effect: Ignoring information on own
morality. Manuscript in preparation.
Brambilla, M., & Leach, C. L. (2014). On the importance of being moral: The distinctive
role of morality in social judgment. Social Cognition, 32, 397–408.
36 Bogdan Wojciszke and Andrea E. Abele
Carney, D. R., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Yap, A. J. (2015). Review and summary research on the
embodied effects of expansive (vs. contractive) nonverbal displays. Psychological Science,
26, 657–663.
Chandler, J., & Schwarz, N. (2009). How extending your middle finger affects your per-
ception of others: Learned movements influence concept accessibility. Journal of Experi-
mental Social Psychology, 45, 123–128.
Christy, A. G., Kim, J., Vess, M., Schlegel, R. J., & Hicks, J. A. (2017). The reciprocal rela-
tion between perceptions of moral goodness and knowledge of others’ true selves. Social
Psychological and Personality Science, 8, xx.
Cislak, A. (2013). Effects of power on social perception: All your boss can see is agency.
Social Psychology, 44, 139–147.
Cislak, A. (2014). Impact of conflict resolution strategies on perception of agency, com-
munion and power roles evaluation. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 45, 426–433.
Cislak, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2008). Agency and communion are inferred from actions serving
interests of self or others. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1103–1110.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Beninger, A. (2011). The dynamics of warmth and compe-
tence judgments and their outcomes in organizations. Research in Organizational Behav-
ior, 31, 73–98.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Wilmuth, C. A., & Carney, D. R. (2012). The benefits of power posing
before a high stakes social evaluation. Harvard Business School Working Paper No.
13-027.
De Bruin, E. N. M., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2000). What people look for in others: Influ-
ences of the perceiver and the perceived on information selection. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 26, 206–219.
Dufner, M., Leising, D., & Gebauer, J. E. (2016). Which basic rules underlie social judg-
ments? Agency follows a zero-sum principle and communion follows a non-zero-sum
principle. Personality and Soocial Psychology Bulletin, 42, 677–687.
Ellemers, N. (2017). Morality and the regulation of social behavior. London: Routledge.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social perception:
Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 77–83.
Goodwin, G. P., Piazza, J., & Rozin, P. (2014). Moral character predominates in person
perception and evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 1–21.
Guinote, A., Cotzia, I., Sandhu, S., & Siwa, P. (2015). Social status modulates prosocial
behavior and egalitarianism in preschool children and adults. Proceedings of National
Academy of Science, 112, 731–736.
IJzerman, H., Szymkow, A., & Parzuchowski, M. (2016). Warmer hearts, and warmer but
noisier rooms: Communality does elicit warmth, but only for those in colder ambient
temperatures: Commentary on Ebersol et al. (2016). Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
chology, 67, 88–90.
Judd, C. M., James-Hawkins, L., Yzerbyt, V., & Kashima, Y. (2005). Fundamental dimen-
sions of social judgment: Understanding the relations between judgments of compe-
tence and warmth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 899–913.
Kervyn, N., Chan, E., Malone, C., Korpusik, A., & Ybarra, O. (2014). Not all disasters are
equal in the public eye: The negativity effect on warmth in brand perception. Social
Cognition, 32, 256–275.
Kervyn, N., Fiske, S. T., & Malone, C. (2012). Brands as intentional agents framework:
How perceived intentions and ability can map brand perception. Journal of Consumer
Psychology, 22, 166–176.
Kouchaki, M., & Gino, F. (2016). Memories of unethical actions become obfuscated over
time. Proceedings of National Academy of Science, 113, 6166–6171.
Agency and Communion in Social Cognition 37
Landy, J. F., Piazza, J., & Goodwin, G. P. (2016). When it’s bad to be friendly and smart:
The desirability of sociability and competence depends on morality. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 1272–1290.
Lynott, D., Corker, K. S., Wortman, J., Connell, L., Donnellan, M. B., Luca, R. E., &
O’Brien, K. (2014). Replication of “Experiencing physical warmth promotes interper-
sonal warmth” by Williams and Bargh (2008). Social Psychology, 45, 216–222.
Markey, P. M. (2002). The duality of personality: Agency and communion in personality traits,
motivation, and behavior. (63), ProQuest Information & Learning, US. Retrieved from:
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2002-95022-
259&site=ehost-live Available from EBSCOhost psyh database.
Mende-Siedlecki, P., Baron, S. G., & Todorov, A. (2013). Diagnostic value underlies asym-
metric updating impressions in the morality and ability domains. The Journal of Neuro-
science, 33, 19406–19415.
Parzuchowski, M., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Hand over heart primes moral judgment and
behavior. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 38, 145–165.
Paulhus, D. L., & Trapnell, P. D. (2008). Self-presentation of personality: An agency-
communion framework. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook
of personality psychology: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 492–517). New York, NY: Guil-
ford Press.
Peeters, G. (2008). The evaluative face of a descriptive model: Communion and agency in
Peabody’s tetradic model of trait organization. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38,
1066–1072.
Reeder, G. D. (1993). Trait-behavior relations in dispositional inference. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 586–593.
Reeder, G. D., & Coovert, M. D. (1986). Revising an impression of morality. Social Cogni-
tion, 4, 1–17.
Reeder, G. D., Pryor, J. B., & Wojciszke, B. (1992). Trait-behavior relations in social infor-
mation processing. In G. R. Semin & K. Fiedler (Eds.), Language, interaction and social
cognition (pp. 37–57). London: Sage Publications.
Russell, A. M. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2008). It’s all relative: Competition and status drive inter-
personal perception. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1193–1201.
Strohminger, N., & Nichols, S. (2015). Neurodegeneration and identity. Psychological Sci-
ence, 26, 1469–1479.
Szymkow, A. (2014). Whether you are smart or kind depends on how I feel: The influence
of positive and negative mood on agency and communion perception. Polish Psychologi-
cal Bulletin, 45, 434–443.
Szymkow, A., Chandler, J., IJzerman, H., Parzuchowski, M., & Wojciszke, B. (2013).
Warmer hearts warmer rooms: How positive communal traits increase estimates of
ambient temperature. Social Psychology, 44, 167–176.
Todorov, A., Olivola, C. Y., Dotsch, R., & Mende-Siedlecki, P. (2015). Social attributions
from faces: Determinants, consequences, accuracy, and functional significance. Annual
Review of Psychology, 66, 519–545.
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interper-
sonal warmth. Science, 322, 306–307.
Wilson, J. P., & Rule, N. O. (2015). Facial trustworthiness predicts extreme criminal-
sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, 26, 1325–1331.
Wojciszke, B. (1994). Multiple meanings of behavior: Construing actions in terms of com-
petence or morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 222–232.
Wojciszke, B., & Abele, A. E. (2008). The primacy of communion over agency and its
reversals in evaluations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1139–1147.
38 Bogdan Wojciszke and Andrea E. Abele
Wojciszke, B., Baryla, W., Parzuchowski, M., Szymkow, A., & Abele, A. E. (2011). Self-
esteem is dominated by agentic over communal information. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 41, 617–627.
Wojciszke, B., Bazinska, R., & Jaworski, M. (1998). On the dominance of moral categories
in impression formation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1245–1257.
Ybarra, O., Chan, E., & Park, D. (2001). Young and old adults’ concerns about morality
and competence. Motivation and Emotions, 25, 85–100.
Ybarra, O., Chan, E., Park, H., Burnstein, E., Monin, B., & Stanik, C. (2008). Life’s recur-
ring challenges and the fundamental dimensions: An integration and its implications for
cultural differences and similarities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1083–1092.
4
WARMTH AND COMPETENCE
ARE PARALLELS TO
COMMUNION AND AGENCY
Stereotype Content Model
Susan T. Fiske
Background on intent
As lay psychologists, we infer what other people are trying to and can do, Heider
(1958) proposed. The attribution of trying is essentially intent, which perceivers
40 Susan T. Fiske
base on cues to goal motivation, persistence, and satisfaction. In daily life, people
infer intent motivation based on the other person’s choices and attention (Fiske,
1989), as well as consistency, distinctiveness, and consensus (Kelley, 1972). The core
of mindreading is attributing a predisposition that is essentially the other person’s
intent (Jones & Davis, 1965). Perceivers often display a dispositional bias, and one
explanation is gaining a sense of control. Knowing the other’s intent helps people
adjust their own behavior, so a dispositional inference is pragmatic because it shapes
a perceiver’s own interaction goals: social thinking is for social doing ( Fiske, 1992).
If perceived intent matters, perceived negative intent matters even more. Nega-
tive outcomes get attributed to people, more than to random causes (Burger, 1981;
Malle, 2006; Shaver, 1985). This is especially true for worse events: the more
extremely bad, the more people seek human intent as a cause. Conversely, human
intent makes harm seem worse, even when it is not objectively worse (Ames &
Fiske, 2013a, 2015). Intent, called warmth in our model, is central to interpersonal
perception.
Moving up a level, dispositional (intent) attribution has some intergroup par-
allels to individual person perception, especially in perceivers’ strong reaction to
negative intent. People show dispositional biases to explain the negative behavior
of outgroups and the positive behavior of ingroups (Pettigrew, 1979). That is, out-
groups’ behavior supposedly results from consistently bad intentions, whereas the
ingroup has consistently good intentions.
perceivers’ focus on outgroup intent seems functionally plausible, for the same
reasons as it does for individual intent, noted earlier.
children women
middle
5
gays Christians
elderly men
liberals blue
collar whites
Hispanics
4 Catholics Jews
young
WARMTH
blacks
teenagers white Asians
poor conservatives collar
3 rvativ
atheists
es
Muslims rich
1
1 2 3 4 5 6
COMPETENCE
FIGURE 4.1 Typical Stereotype Content Map, United States. Circles represent cluster
analysis results.
(Data from Kervyn, Fiske, & Yzerbyt, 2015)
42 Susan T. Fiske
substantial effect sizes (Kervyn et al., 2015). The warmth of intent, measured as
both friendliness and trustworthiness, seems established as a primary dimension.
Surveys
The SCM paradigms started with surveys (Fiske, 2018). Because the questions ask
participants to respond from society’s perspective, minimizing social desirability
44 Susan T. Fiske
and tapping into consensus, use of a representative versus a convenience sample has
not mattered much: everyone knows the societal stereotypes of common groups,
including their own, so ingroup favoritism is minimal and rarely contaminating.
The first step asks one sample to name groups in society, then identify their
own groups and the most negatively viewed groups (people often neglect these lat-
ter two types). Groups mentioned by at least 15% of the sample meet SCM criteria
to be in common discourse. The second step then asks a new sample to rate the
selected groups on warmth and competence traits (see Table 4.1), and sometimes
Cultural comparisons
Surveys conducted in countries beyond the US (about 50 at this time) provide
cultural comparisons. Many groups appear in similar locations around the globe:
countries’ own citizens typically appear both warm and competent; undocu-
mented migrants stereotypically seem low on both. Rich people come across as
competent but cold, whereas older people are deemed warm but incompetent.
Cultural variants are informative in their idiosyncrasies, but also come in
regional and other systematic patterns ( Fiske & Durante, 2016). East Asian samples
do not self-promote their societal ingroups as much as Western samples do; East
Asian ingroups locate themselves in the modest middle of the space, but all their
outgroups land in the common quadrants anyway.
Another comparison depends on national economic indicators. Unequal countries
especially use the ambivalent (mixed) quadrants, lowering the warmth-competence
correlation (Durante et al., 2013). Equal countries (as indicated by the UN Gini
index of income inequality) show a simpler warmth x competence map, more of
a vector from low on both dimensions (migrants) to high on both (citizens and all
groups eligible for government support). In equal countries, competition is seen
as neither competent nor warm. Unequal countries, in contrast, tolerate competi-
tion (only natural under the circumstances). They separate deserving poor (elderly,
disabled, children) and undeserving poor (undocumented migrants, drug addicts,
homeless). Likewise, they distinguish the deserving rich (professionals, middle class)
and the undeserving rich (lawyers, politicians, CEOs). Unequal countries have more
explaining to do (Fiske, 2011; Durante & Fiske, 2017).
Another cultural dimension distinguishes nations’ use of the SCM space,
namely peace-conflict. The most peaceful countries behave as the most equal
ones do (and indeed they overlap, as in Scandinavia): peaceful countries identify
most societal groups as “us,” with a few outcasts (“them”), as noted. At the other
extreme, high-conflict countries also identify us versus them, where the adversar-
ies may be internal (civil war) or external (international war). Extremes of peace
and conflict correspond to simpler stereotype maps. Intermediate peace-conflict,
as in the US and the Americas generally, promotes the more complex maps with
heavy use of the ambivalent quadrants (Durante et al., 2017).
Archival data
SCM evidence generalizes over time, going back several decades in two more
open-ended datasets. A reanalysis of Katz and Braly’s (1933) 84-adjective checklist
data, as well as three subsequent replications roughly 20 years apart (Bergsieker,
Leslie, Constantine, & Fiske, 2012), readily codes into warmth and competence
46 Susan T. Fiske
dimensions, applied to 10 social groups. Each study maps the groups onto warmth
x competence dimensions. Another open-ended coding, this time of fascist maga-
zines’ depictions of social groups, also reproduces SCM dimensions (Durante, Vol-
pato, & Fiske, 2010).
Experiments
Besides surveys, experiments show that the hypothesized predictors (cooperative
or competitive interdependence and status) can respectively predict warmth and
competence at the intergroup level ( Caprariello et al., 2009) and the interpersonal
level (Russell & Fiske, 2008), as noted earlier. Interpersonal interactions trade off
warmth and competence as a function of impression formation goals, including
status divides (see Fiske, 2015, 2018). The point here is that people use the dimen-
sions when they are up close and personal, not just as abstract descriptions of
society on surveys.
Bio-behavioral data
People’s bio-behavioral responses also distinguish groups by warmth and compe-
tence, likewise suggesting that societal stereotypes have individual significance. In
one study, photographs of individuals ostensibly from the low-low, disgust quad-
rant (homeless people, drug addicts) decreased perspective-taking, attributions of
mind, and activation of brain regions associated with social cognition; this quadrant
also increased insula activation, consistent with disgust ratings (e.g., Harris & Fiske,
2006).
The envy quadrant contains high-competence, low-warmth groups such as
rich people and business people. When these groups encounter everyday adver-
sity (getting splashed by a cab), perceivers are unsympathetic, even pleased, as in
Schadenfreude (e.g., Cikara & Fiske, 2011). Facial electrodes record a tendency to
smile at their bad events, uniquely for this quadrant.
The pity quadrant – high-warmth, low-competence groups such as disabled
or older people – remains an ongoing project for SCM research. Clearly, blame
matters, with selective empathy only for misfortunes neither caused nor sustained
by the individual (Wu & Fiske, in press). Negative outcomes caused by fate do
elicit pity, but negative outcomes caused by intentional behavior not only seem
worse (Ames & Fiske, 2013a), they also elicit resentment ( Weiner, Perry, & Mag-
nusson, 1988).
The pride quadrant, though not studied as such from a bio-behavioral per-
spective, probably enhances mentalizing – reading the other’s mind and inten-
tions. Interdependence activates brain regions associated with in-depth social
cognition, specifically for unexpected information about an ally (that is, under
cooperative interdependence, as opposed to competitive independence; Ames &
Fiske, 2013b).
Stereotype Content Model 47
Extensions
The SCM extends to detailing stereotypes of specific groups, such as analyses of
the default older person as “doddering but dear” or the default female as nice but
not smart (see Fiske, 2015, for references). SCM also describes subtypes of ethnic
groups, sexual minorities, genders, disabilities, and mental illnesses ( Fiske, 2015).
The SCM applies to other entities that have intent, such as animals ( Sevillano &
Fiske, 2016) and corporate brands (Aaker, Garbinsky, & Vohs, 2012; Fournier &
Alvarez, 2012; Kervyn, Fiske, & Malone, 2012). Robots sometimes seem to have
intent, especially when they are more human-like or animal-like than machine-
like (Lee, Lau, & Hong, 2011).
Future directions
If SCM dimensions and the parallels within the agency-communion frameworks
are so fundamental, then they should appear implicitly as well as explicitly. Implicit
associations do indicate warmth and competence, measured separately, for two
contrasting outgroups (nursery school teachers and corporate lawyers) ( Carlsson &
Björklund, 2010). Sometimes, implicit and explicit SCM stereotypes may diverge,
perhaps due to social desirability concerns that encourage explicit reporting of
ambivalent stereotypes (something good about every group) but implicit associa-
tions being more uniformly negative (Rohmer & Louvet, 2012).
Besides implicit associations, research also is expanding stimulus modali-
ties beyond verbal indicators and photographs to perceiving outgroup individu-
als’ faces, a process that also appears to be encoded as warmth and competence
(Imhoff, Woelki, Hanke, & Dotsch, 2013). These dimensions fit other work show-
ing trustworthiness and competence in more general face perception (Willis &
Todorov, 2006).
Besides modality (implicit/explicit, visual/verbal) as a moderator, another
moderator appears to be global or local perspective, as reflected in Chapter 14
in this volume, which finds different dimensions when participants take a more
abstract viewpoint. Our adversarial collaboration finds SCM dimensions when
raters take a neighborhood or psychological approach, but the ABC dimensions
when raters take a more distant stance (Nicolas et al., unpublished).
As a result of such empirical encounters and other conceptual encounters, such
as this volume, theory and measurement will also develop as conceptual defini-
tions become more nuanced. That is, SCM work now returns to including both
sociality and morality as reflecting warm intent. Competence too needs to include
both ability and agency/assertiveness, all reflecting status. As unproblematic as
those may seem, the meanings of both warmth and competence may differ across
settings. For example, a doctor might be warm and competent in ways that differ
48 Susan T. Fiske
from a farmer’s warmth and competence, though both appear high in both dimen-
sions (Fiske & Dupree, 2014).
Conclusion
The SCM developed out of basic person perception research applied to intergroup
perceptions ( Fiske et al., 2002). In a range of theory and method developments,
SCM has benefited from broad international and local perspectives, and doubtless
this will continue in interdependent efforts like this volume.
References
Aaker, J. L., Garbinsky, E. N., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Cultivating admiration in brands:
Warmth, competence, and landing in the “golden quadrant.” Journal of Consumer Psy-
chology, 22 (2), 191–194.
Abele, A. E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”? Preferential
processing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(5),
935–948.
Abele, A. E., Hauke, N., Peters, K., Louvet, E., Szymkow, A., & Duan, Y. (2016). Facets
of the fundamental content dimensions: Agency with competence and assertiveness:
Communion with warmth and morality. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1810.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self
versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 751. a.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content: A dual perspective
model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195–255.
Ames, D. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2013a). Intentional harms are worse, even when they’re not.
Psychological Science, 24 (9), 1755–1762.
Ames, D. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2013b). Outcome dependency alters the neural substrates of
impression formation. NeuroImage, 83, 599–608.
Ames, D. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2015). Perceived intent motivates people to magnify observed
harm. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112 (12), 3599–3605.
Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy-
chology, 41, 1230–1240.
Bergsieker, H. B., Leslie, L. M., Constantine, V. S., & Fiske, S. T. (2012). Stereotyping by
omission: Eliminate the negative, accentuate the positive. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 102 (6), 1214–1238.
Burger, J. M. (1981). Motivational biases in the attribution of responsibility for an acci-
dent: A meta-analysis of the defensive-attribution hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 90,
496–512.
Caprariello, P. A., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Fiske, S. T. (2009). Social structure shapes cultural
stereotypes and emotions: A causal test of the Stereotype Content Model. Group Pro-
cesses and Intergroup Behavior, 12, 147–155.
Carlsson, R., & Björklund, F. (2010). Implicit stereotype content: Mixed stereotypes can be
measured with the implicit association test. Social Psychology, 41, 213–222.
Cikara, M., Eberhardt, J. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2011). From agents to objects: Sexist attitudes
and neural responses to sexualized targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 540–551.
Cikara, M., & Fiske, S. T. (2011). Bounded empathy: Neural responses to outgroup targets’
(mis)fortunes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3791–3803.
Stereotype Content Model 49
Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2007). The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup
affect and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 631–648.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Competence and warmth as universal
trait dimensions of interpersonal and intergroup perception: The Stereotype Content
Model and the BIAS Map. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
(Vol. 40, pp. 61–149). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., Kwan, V. S. Y., Glick, P., Demoulin, S., Leyens, J.-Ph., ...
Ziegler, R. (2009). Stereotype Content Model across cultures: Towards universal simi-
larities and some differences. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 1–33.
Dépret, E., & Fiske, S. T. (1999). Perceiving the powerful: Intriguing individuals versus
threatening groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(5), 461–480.
Durante, F., & Fiske, S. T. (2017). How social-class stereotypes maintain inequality. Current
Opinion in Psychology, 18, 43–48.
Durante, F., Fiske, S. T., Gelfand, M., Crippa, F., Suttora, C., Stillwell, A., ... Teymoori, A.
(2017). Ambivalent stereotypes link to peace, conflict, and inequality across 38 nations.
PNAS: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 114 (4), 669–674.
Durante, F., Fiske, S. T., Kervyn, N., Cuddy, A. J. C., Akande, A., Adetoun, B. E., ...
Storari, C. C. (2013). Nations’ income inequality predicts ambivalence in stereo-
type content: How societies mind the gap. British Journal of Social Psychology, 52,
726–746.
Durante, F., Volpato, C., & Fiske, S. T. (2010). Using the Stereotype Content Model to
examine group depictions in Fascism: An archival approach. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 40, 465–483.
Ellemers, N., Pagliaro, S., Barreto, M., & Leach, C. W. (2008). Is it better to be moral than
smart? The effects of morality and competence norms on the decision to work at group
status improvement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6), 1397–1410.
Ellemers, N., & Van der Toorn, J. (2015). Groups as moral anchors. Current Opinion in
Behavioral Sciences, 6, 189–194.
Fiske, S. T. (1989). Examining the role of intent: Toward understanding its role in stereo-
typing and prejudice. In J. Uleman & J. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought: The limits of
awareness, intention, and control (pp. 253–283). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Fiske, S. T. (1992). Thinking is for doing: Portraits of social cognition from daguerreotype
to laserphoto. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 877–889.
Fiske, S. T. (2011). Envy up, scorn down: How status divides us. New York, NY: Russell Sage
Foundation.
Fiske, S. T. (2015). Intergroup biases: A focus on stereotype content. Current Opinion in
Behavioral Sciences, 3, 45–50.
Fiske, S. T. (2018). Stereotype content: Warmth and competence endure. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 27(2), 67–73.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social perception:
Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 77–83.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype
content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and com-
petition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.
Fiske, S. T., & Dupree, C. (2014). Gaining trust as well as respect in communicating to
motivated audiences about science topics. PNAS: Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the United States of America, 111(suppl. 4), 13593–13597.
Fiske, S. T., & Durante, F. (2016). Stereotype content across cultures: Variations on a few
themes. In M. J. Gelfand, C.-Y. Chiu, & Y.-Y. Hong (Eds.), Handbook of advances in
culture and psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 209–258). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
50 Susan T. Fiske
Fournier, S., & Alvarez, C. (2012). Brands as relationship partners: Warmth, competence,
and in-between. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22 (2), 177–185.
Gray, K., Knobe, J., Sheskin, M., Bloom, P., & Barrett, L. F. (2011). More than a body:
Mind perception and the nature of objectification. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 101(6), 1207–1220.
Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuro-imaging
responses to extreme outgroups. Psychological Science, 17, 847–853.
Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy Review, 10, 252–264.
Heflick, N. A., Goldenberg, J. L., Cooper, D. P., & Puvia, E. (2011). From women to
objects: Appearance focus, target gender, and perceptions of warmth, morality and
competence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(3), 572–581.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York, NY: Wiley.
Imhoff, R., Woelki, J., Hanke, S., & Dotsch, R. (2013). Warmth and competence in your
face! Visual encoding of stereotype content. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 386.
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in
person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2,
pp. 220–266). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Katz, D., & Braly, K. (1933). Racial stereotypes of one hundred college students. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 28, 280–290.
Kelley, H. H. (1972). Attribution in social interaction. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H.
Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behav-
ior (pp. 1–26). Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
Kervyn, N., Fiske, S. T., & Malone, C. (2012). Brands as intentional agents framework:
Warmth and competence map brand perception, Target Article. Journal of Consumer
Psychology, 22, 166–176.
Kervyn, N., Fiske, S. T., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2013). Integrating the Stereotype Content Model
(warmth and competence) and the Osgood semantic differential (evaluation, potency,
and activity). European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(7), 673–681.
Kervyn, N., Fiske, S. T., & Yzerbyt, Y. (2015). Foretelling the primary dimension of social
cognition: Symbolic and realistic threats together predict warmth in the stereotype
content model. Social Psychology, 46, 36–45.
Kervyn, N., Yzerbyt, V., & Judd, C. M. (2010). Compensation between warmth and com-
petence: Antecedents and consequences of a negative relation between the two fun-
damental dimensions of social perception. European Review of Social Psychology, 21(1),
155–187.
Lee, S., Lau, I. Y., & Hong, Y. (2011). Effects of appearance and functions on likability and
perceived occupational suitability of robots. Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision
Making, 5(2), 232–250.
Leyens, J.-Ph., Cortes, B. P., Demoulin, S., Dovidio, J., Fiske, S. T., Gaunt, R., ... Vaes, V.
(2003). Emotional prejudice, essentialism, and nationalism. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 33, 703–718.
Loughnan, S., & Haslam, N. (2007). Animals and androids: Implicit associations between
social categories and nonhumans. Psychological Science, 18, 116–121.
Malle, B. F. (2006). The actor-observer asymmetry in attribution: A (surprising) meta-
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 895–919.
Mattan, B., Kubota, J., & Cloutier, J. (2017). How social status shapes person perception
and evaluation: A social neuroscience perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
12, 468–507.
Stereotype Content Model 51
Nicolas, G., Bai, X., & Fiske, S. T. (in preparation). Natural language analysis of information
seeking and stereotype content. Princeton University.
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning.
Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Peeters, G. (1992). Evaluative meanings of adjectives in vitro and in context: Some theo-
retical implications and practical consequences of positive-negative asymmetry and
behavioral-adaptive concepts of evaluation. Psychologica Belgica, 32 (2), 211–231.
Pettigrew, T. F. (1979). The ultimate attribution error: Extending Allport’s cognitive anal-
ysis of prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 5, 461–476.
Richetin, J., Durante, F., Mari, S., Perugini, M., & Volpato, C. (2012). Primacy of warmth
versus competence: A motivated bias? The Journal of Social Psychology, 152 (4), 417–435.
Rohmer, O., & Louvet, E. (2012). Implicit measures of the stereotype content associated
with disability. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(4), 732–740.
Russell, A. M., & Fiske, S. T. (2008). It’s all relative: Social position and interpersonal per-
ception. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1193–1201.
Sevillano, V., & Fiske, S. T. (2016). Warmth and competence in animals. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 46 (5), 276–293.
Shaver, K. G. (1985). The attribution of blame: Causality, responsibility, and blameworthiness.
New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Weiner, B., Perry, R. P., & Magnusson, J. (1988). An attributional analysis of reactions to
stigmas. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 738–748.
Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms
exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17, 592–598.
Wojciszke, B. (2005). Morality and competence in person-and self-perception. European
Review of Social Psychology, 16 (1), 155–188.
Wojciszke, B. et al. (1998). On the dominance of moral categories in impression formation.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1245–1257.
Wu, J., & Fiske, S. T. (in press). Disability’s incompetent-but-warm stereotype guides
selective empathy: Distinctive cognitive, emotional, and neural signatures. In D. Dunn
(Ed.), Disability: Social psychological perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Ybarra, O. et al. (2001). Young and old adults’ concerns about morality and competence.
Motivation and Emotion, 25, 85–100.
5
AGENCY AND COMMUNION
IN SELF-CONCEPT AND IN
SELF-ESTEEM
Andrea E. Abele and Nicole Hauke
The content dimensions of agency (A) and communion (C) are basic to many psy-
chological phenomena, among others to the self-concept and to its evaluative com-
ponent, self-esteem. A focuses on the individual person and the pursuit of personal
goals. Themes of A are self-realization, striving for power and status, and acting in
one’s own interest. Hogan (1982) summarized these themes under the label “getting
ahead.” C focuses on community and social integration. Themes of C are the for-
mation and maintenance of social relationships, striving for harmony, and acting in
the interest of others. Hogan (1982) summarized these themes under the label “get-
ting along.” These fundamental dimensions reflect the two recurring challenges of
human life: pursuing individual goals and belonging to social groups (Ybarra et al.,
2008; chapter 2 this volume).
The self-concept is what we think about the self. A person may think of the self
as being friendly and empathetic, smart, but a little indecisive; another person may
think of the self as being determined, competent, fair, but reserved. These descrip-
tions are communal (friendly, empathetic, fair, and – somewhat negative – reserved)
and agentic (smart, determined, competent, and – somewhat negative – indecisive).
A person may also think of own behavior in terms of A and C. As behaviors are open
to different interpretations, a person has some flexibility in using agentic versus com-
munal interpretations. Helping someone in need may be interpreted as “friendly” (“I
am a friendly person and this is why I am helping”), a communal characteristic; or it
may also be interpreted as “competent” (“I know what has to be done in this situa-
tion”), an agentic characteristic.
Whereas the self-concept relates to a person’s thinking about the self, self-esteem
reflects a person’s overall subjective evaluation of his or her own worth (Rosenberg,
1965). The current chapter discusses how a person’s self-concept regarding A and C
is related to his or her self-esteem. Put simply, is a more agentic or a more commu-
nal self-concept related to higher self-esteem? Is the self-esteem of individuals who
Agency and Communion in Self-Concept 53
interpret own behavior more in terms of A higher (or lower) than the self-concept
of individuals who interpret own behavior more in terms of C?
The chapter is organized into three sections. The first section discusses dif-
ferent approaches to self-esteem and derives hypotheses how A and C are related
to a person’s self-esteem. The second section gives an overview of the empirical
evidence. The third section presents a novel framework for A, C, and self-esteem.
It will be shown that (a) looking at facets of the fundamental dimensions, asser-
tiveness and competence in case of A, warmth and morality in case of C, helps
to clarify how the fundamental dimensions and self-esteem relate to each other;
and (b) that the perception of the self can be distinguished into two perspectives,
the “actor” perspective, e.g., the person thinks about how he/she sees the own
person (we call it “self-as-identity”), and the “observer” perspective, e.g., the per-
son thinks about how others might see him/her (we call it “self-as-reputation”).
This distinction helps to understand why specific facets of A and C are related to
self-esteem. A, particularly agency-assertiveness, is of particular relevance in the
self-as-identity perspective, and communion-morality is of particular relevance in
the self-as-reputation perspective.
claims that the purpose of self-esteem is to buffer the potentially paralyzing terror
that humans experience on contemplating death. Self-esteem is maintained by
fostering culturally shared world-views and values. As culturally shared values
are especially communal values (helping those in need, being fair and just, etc.;
Paulhus & Trapnell, 2008; Schwartz & Bardi, 2001), self-esteem should be related
to the expression of communal values.
A theory that somehow integrates mastery approaches and social monitoring
approaches to self-esteem is self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1995). It
states that high self-esteem should result from the fulfillment of the three basic
psychological needs, which are relatedness, competency, and autonomy. Relat-
edness, of course, has to do with how others perceive the self; competency and
autonomy have to do with mastery and with pursuing own goals.
The Dual Perspective Model of A and C (Abele & Wojciszke, 2007, 2014; see
Chapter 3, this volume), finally, was developed to analyze the different mean-
ings A and C characteristics have in the perception of actors and observers in an
interaction. As a side aspect of this model, it was predicted that self-esteem should
be more related to A self-perception than to C self-perception because A charac-
teristics are more self-profitable than C characteristics (Peeters, 2008) and help to
“get ahead.”
What does this mean regarding the current questions? Except for the dual per-
spective model (Abele & Wojciszke, 2007, 2014), the above theories are not explicit
about the association of a more or less agentic or communal self-perception with
self-esteem. The constructs discussed (dominance, status, relatedness, autonomy,
competency, cultural world-views) can be related to A and C, but they were
not conceptualized according to the fundamental dimensions. Also the aims of
the theories are in part different from the current questions. Nevertheless, they
more or less explicitly put different emphasis on the association between A vs. C
self-perception and self-esteem. Dominance theory ( Barkow, 1980), self-efficacy
theory (Bandura, 1977), hierometer theory (Mahadevan et al., 2016), and the dual
perspective model (Abele & Wojciszke, 2014) implicate that self-esteem is more
related to self-perception on A than on C. Sociometer theory (Leary, 2012) and
terror-management theory ( Solomon et al., 1991) implicate that self-esteem is
more related to self-perception on C than on A. Self-determination theory impli-
cates that both self-perception on A and on C are related to self-esteem. The next
section will give an overview of respective findings.
Empirical evidence
Research investigating the association of agentic and communal self-perceptions
with self-esteem was conducted with samples from different countries, different
educational backgrounds, and different ages. It also applied different methodologi-
cal approaches (overview see Table 5.1).
One approach was asking people to remember events that had influenced their
self-esteem in a positive or negative direction and later to categorize these events
TABLE 5.1 Studies investigating the association of agency, communion, and self-esteem
Abele and Wojciszke 61 Polish university students remember events that have participants reported more agentic than
(2007, Study 2) influenced SE communal events
Wojciszke and Abele 120 Polish college students remember events that have participants reported more agentic than
(2008, Study 1) influenced SE communal events
Wojciszke, Baryla, all samples: self-ratings of A/C
Parzuchowski, Szymkow,
and Abele (2011)
Study 1 62 pupils + Rosenberg SE (1965) A strong predictor of SE, C not significant
Study 2 (a) 170 students + self-liking/self-competence in all samples: A strong predictor of SE, C not
(Tafarodi & Milne, 2002) significant
(b) 88 students + name-letter-preference
implicit SE (Koole, Govorun,
Cheng, & Gallucci, 2009)
(c) 90 employees + state SE (Heatherton & Polivy,
1991)
(d) 162 Polish employees of an + narcissistic personality inventory
international private company (Raskin & Hall, 1979)
(e) 53 state clerks + Rosenberg SE (1965)
Study 3 182 university students + Rosenberg SE (1965) even if importance of C higher than A: A
+ importance of A/C strong predictor of SE, C not significant
Wojciszke and 128 university students priming of positive/negative A vs. positive A priming increase in SE,
Sobiczewska (2013) C behavior negative A priming decrease in SE, C
priming no influence on SE
Li, Tseng, Wu, and Chen 190 Taiwanese university students self-rating of A/C + Rosenberg SE A significantly correlates with SE, C not
(2007, Study 2) (1965)
(Continued)
TABLE 5.1 (Continued)
according to agentic vs. communal content (Abele & Wojciszke, 2007; Wojciszke &
Abele, 2008). Usually, events with agentic content (successes, failures), but not with
communal content were recalled. In other studies, participants’ self-perception
regarding A and C as well as their self-esteem were measured (Wojciszke et al.,
2011). Results showed that self-perception on agency significantly predicted self-
esteem, whereas self-perception of communion was no predictor. This pattern of
results was replicated in different samples ranging from young pupils through uni-
versity students and young competitive employees to older state clerks. In addition,
different measures of self-esteem were used and the agency-over-communion effect
was found throughout these samples and measures. Interestingly, even if partici-
pants reported that C would be more important for them personally than A, only
A was a significant predictor for self-esteem. A third methodological approach was
experimental priming of positive (successes) or negative (failures) information about
agentic behavior or positive (norm-maintenance) or negative (norm-breaking)
information about communal behavior and then to measures the participants’ self-
esteem. In line with the previous findings priming of successes led to an increase
in self-esteem and priming of failures led to a decrease in self-esteem. In contrast,
priming of norm-maintenance vs. norm-breaking had no influence on self-esteem
(Wojciszke & Sobiczewska, 2013).
The studies reviewed so far were all conducted with Polish participants. But the
stronger influence of agency than communion on self-esteem was not only found
in Poland but also in other European countries as well as in the USA, Australia,
China, and Taiwan (Abele et al., 2016; Bi et al., 2013, Study 1; Gebauer et al., 2013;
Li et al., 2007).
However, there are also findings showing that under specific conditions com-
munion may also be associated with self-esteem (Bi et al., 2013; Gebauer et al., 2013).
Bi et al. (2013, Study 2), for instance, showed that the well-established agency over
communion effect could be found in Northern China. In contrast, in Southern
China, where communal values are of particular importance, both agency and com-
munion were associated with self-esteem. Besides cultural norms, self-centrality of
traits also plays an important role as demonstrated in a large-scale study by Gebauer
and colleagues (2013). In a very large sample of participants from 11 European coun-
tries, agency generally was a stronger predictor of self-esteem than communion.
However, there were four moderators of the relative strength of these two predictors
of self-esteem. The influence of agency on self-esteem was particularly strong in
relatively agentic countries (high culture-level agency score), among non-religious
individuals, men, and younger adults. Conversely, there were also specific sub-
samples where the inverted effect could be found. Among older religious women in
Germany (a country with a relatively high culture-level communion; cf. Gebauer
et al., 2013), communion was a stronger predictor of self-esteem than agency.
In sum, there is empirical evidence that agency generally dominates self-esteem.
This finding is in accord with mastery approaches to self-esteem (dominance the-
ory, Barkow, 1980; self-efficacy theory, Bandura, 1977; hierometer theory, Mahade-
van et al., 2016), but less with social-monitoring approaches (sociometer theory,
58 Andrea E. Abele and Nicole Hauke
Leary, 2012; terror-management theory, Solomon et al., 1991). It is in line with the
dual perspective model (Abele & Wojciszke, 2014) and it does not contradict self-
determination theory.
However, as this overview of the empirical findings shows, communion can
also impact self-esteem, but to a lesser degree and under specific conditions only.
Hence, the question arises how these findings can be integrated into a cohesive
theoretical framework.
dominance view of self-esteem (Barkow, 1980; Mahadevan et al., 2016) as well as the
dual perspective model (Abele & Wojciszke, 2007, 2014), agency-assertiveness is most
closely related to self-esteem. Supporting a success/failure view of self-esteem (Ban-
dura, 1977; James, 1890), agency-competence is also related to self-esteem. How-
ever, supporting a sociometer view of self-esteem (Leary, 2012; Leary & Downs,
1995) as well as self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1995), communion is also
related to self-esteem, but it is only one facet: communion-warmth does not relate
to self-esteem, but only communion-morality. This is not exactly what sociometer
theory and self-determination theory might predict: “relatedness” (one of the basic
needs defined by Deci & Ryan, 1995) and “relational value” (Leary, 2012) should be
manifested in a positive association between self-perception on communion-warmth
and self-esteem. However, the higher weight of morality than warmth in self-esteem
nicely fits to the literature on group perception that also shows that morality, not
warmth (or sociability) is relevant in the evaluation of groups (Ellemers, 2017). In
sum, except of very specific circumstances (see Bi et al., 2013; Gebauer et al., 2013),
self-esteem seems to be more related to A, e.g., “getting ahead” than to C, e.g., “get-
ting along” (see also Gebauer et al., 2015). However, having a self-concept of a moral
person (the CM facet of C) also adds to a positive self-esteem, but seeing the self as
warm and friendly (the CW facet of C) has no effect at all.1
What do these findings mean and how could they be integrated into an agency-
communion model of self-perception and self-esteem? The dual perspective model
states that social behavior always implicates the perspective of either the actor
or the observer; actors are more focused on agentic qualities and observers are
more focused on communal qualities. We suggest to extend this model to the self
0.6
0.53
0.5
0.4
β coefficient
0.3
0.2
0.14 0.13
0.1
0.00
0
Agency Agency Communion Communion
Assertiveness Competence Morality Warmth
FIGURE 5.1 Self-esteem regressed on the facets of agency and communion across six
different countries (Australia, China, France, Germany, Poland, USA; N = 1.803; R2 = .43,
p < .001).
60 Andrea E. Abele and Nicole Hauke
that also implicates two perspectives. As has already been discussed by symbolic
interactionism, there are two kinds of self-concept, the “I” (personal self) and
the “me” (social self, “looking glass self,” Cooley, 1902; Mead, 1934), that only
together constitute a person’s self. The huge literature on impression formation
also suggests that the self is constituted by an “internal” and an “external” (self-
presentation) part. More specifically, the “moral self” has been differentiated into
an “internal” vs. an “external” moral self, too (Aquino & Reed, 2002).
The extended Dual Perspective Model of the Self (Hauke & Abele, submitted)
also distinguishes between two perspectives of the self and considers the relative
importance of A and C from these two perspectives. In the “actor” perspective a
person concentrates on the self and his/her goals, plans, and self-view. This actor
perspective of the self is called self-as-identity. In the “observer” perspective a per-
son looks at the self from the outside (“looking glass”), monitors the social envi-
ronment for cues indicating low or declining relational evaluation and considers
how others may see and evaluate him/her. For instance, a person may think of the
self as “determined” (actor-self), but may at the same time be concerned if others
might interpret this behavior as “rude” (observer-self). This observer perspective
of the self is called self-as-reputation. The two perspectives on the self are inter-
twined, and the “actor” self develops in interaction with the “observer” self; how-
ever, for analytical reasons it is useful to distinguish them as separate constructs.
Analogous to the dual perspective model of social interaction, the extended Dual
Perspective Model of the Self predicts that A is more relevant in the actor perspec-
tive of the self, whereas C is more relevant in the observer perspective of the self. In
the above example (thinking of oneself as “determined,” but worrying if others may
see the behavior as “rude”), the characteristic “determined” (e.g., an agentic-assertive
trait) is profitable from the perspective of the “actor-self,” but may at the same time
be detrimental from the perspective of the observer-self. Hence, the actor-self should
monitor more his/her A traits and the observer-self should monitor more his/her C
traits. Moreover, considering the facet approach to A and C, the assertiveness facet
should be even more relevant for the actor-self than the competence facet of A, and
the morality facet of C should be even more relevant for the observer-self than the
warmth facet (cf. Pagliaro, Ellemers, Barreto, & Di Cesare, 2016). This is exactly
what we showed in a series of studies (Hauke & Abele, submitted). Participants were
provided with gossip stories and should imagine being the target of this gossip. The
gossip was either about lack of agency-assertiveness, lack of agency-competence, lack
of communion-warmth, or lack of communion-morality. Participants should report
the degree of identity threat and the degree of reputation threat they would experi-
ence in such a situation. In accord with our hypotheses, participants reported more
identity threat in the agency-assertiveness gossip condition than in the other three
conditions, and they reported more reputation threat in the communion-morality
condition than in the three other conditions.
In a further study, participants should recall events that had either led to a
high degree of identity threat or to a high degree of reputation threat. If asked
to recall an event in which they had experienced identity threat, participants
Agency and Communion in Self-Concept 61
reported more situations with agentic content (both failures in competence and
failures in assertiveness) than situations with communal content. Conversely, if
asked to recall an event in which they had experienced reputation threat, they
reported more situations featuring communion-morality than situations featuring
communion-warmth or agency. These findings are again supporting our pre-
dictions that the actor-self considers more agency-related content (particularly
agency-assertiveness), whereas the observer-self considers more communion-
related (especially communion-morality-related) content.
Conclusions
The reviewed evidence suggests that agency and communion and their facets
are useful constructs in understanding the dynamics of self-esteem. Self-esteem
is strongly related to a person’s self-concept concerning agentic qualities. These
qualities help him/her to efficiently pursue goals, to establish and maintain status
in social hierarchies, and ultimately to have an evolutional advantage. However,
acting only in the sense of getting ahead with one’s own goals may not lead to
the desired ends because others would not like such a person and might oppose
him/her. Communal qualities are therefore also important to establish and main-
tain one’s status in a community and in social relationships more generally. The
C facet of warmth is important for bonding and relational issues, but respective
characteristics seem to be more or less taken as granted and self-perception regard-
ing CW is not much related to self-esteem. The C facet of morality is the one that
counts. Trustworthiness, reliability, and fairness are desired in others and they
are desired for the self, as well. They are relevant for reputational issues, and – as
self-evaluation is related to both the “actor” and the “observer” self – they are also
relevant for self-esteem.2 Only experiencing the self as both agentic and moral
establishes a high self-esteem.
Notes
1 It might be argued that the lacking effect of CW on self-esteem is due to a ceiling effect
and to low variability. People usually rate themselves high on CW and the standard devia-
tion of respective ratings is low. However, people also rate themselves high on CM and the
standard deviation of these ratings is also low. Hence, this is no reason why CW does not
impact self-esteem.
2 Interestingly, a recent theory of status attainment (Bai, 2017) discusses three routes to sta-
tus: dominance (here AA), competence (here AC), and virtue (here CM).
References
Abele, A. E., Hauke, N., Peters, K., Louvet, E., Szymkow, A., & Duan, Y. P. (2016). Facets
of the fundamental content dimensions: Agency with competence and assertiveness:
Communion with warmth and morality. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1810.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self
versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 751–763.
62 Andrea E. Abele and Nicole Hauke
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition:
A dual perspective model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195–255.
Aquino, K., & Reed, A. (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 83, 1423–1440.
Bai, F. (2017). Beyond dominance and competence: A moral virtue theory of status attain-
ment. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21, 203–227.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psycho-
logical Review, 84, 191–215.
Barkow, J. (1980). Prestige and self-esteem: A biosocial interpretation. In D. Omark, F.
Strayer, & D. Freedman (Eds.), Dominance relations: An ethological view of human conflict
and social interaction (pp. 319–332). New York, NY: Garland STPM Press.
Bi, C., Ybarra, O., & Zhao, Y. (2013). Accentuating your masculine side: Agentic traits
generally dominate self-evaluation, even in China. Social Psychology, 44, 104–109.
Brambilla, M., & Leach, C. W. (2014). On the importance of being moral: The distinctive
role of morality in social judgment. Social Cognition, 32 (4), 397–408.
Brambilla, M., Rusconi, P., Sacchi, S., & Cherubini, P. (2011). Looking for honesty: The
primary role of morality (vs. sociability and competence) in information gathering.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 135–143.
Brambilla, M., Sacchi, S., Pagliaro, S., & Ellemers, N. (2013). Morality and intergroup rela-
tions: Threats to safety and group image predict the desire to interact with outgroup
and ingroup members. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49 (5), 811–821.
Brambilla, M., Sacchi, S., Rusconi, P., Cherubini, P., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2012). You want to
give a good impression? Be honest! Moral traits dominate group impression formation.
British Journal of Social Psychology, 51, 149–166.
Carrier, A., Louvet, E., Chauvin, B., & Rohmer, O. (2014). The primacy of agency over
competence in status perception. Social Psychology, 45, 347–356.
Cooley, C. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York, NY: Scribener’s.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy: The basis for true self-esteem. In M.
Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem (pp. 31–49). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Ellemers, N. (2017). Morality and the regulation of social behavior. London: Routledge.
Gebauer, J. E., Leary, M. R., & Neberich, W. (2012). Unfortunate first names: Effects
of name-based relational devaluation and interpersonal neglect. Social Psychological and
Personality Science, 3(5), 590–596.
Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., Wagner, J., Bleidorn, W., Rentfrow, P., Potter, J., & Gos-
ling, S. (2015). Cultural norm fulfillment, interpersonal belonging, or getting ahead?
A large-scale cross-cultural test of three perspectives on the function of self-esteem.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 526–548.
Gebauer, J. E., Wagner, J., Sedikides, C., & Neberich, W. (2013). Agency-communion and
self-esteem relations are moderated by culture, religiosity, age, and sex: Evidence for the
“self-centrality breads self-enhancement” principle. Journal of Personality, 81, 261–275.
Hauke, N., & Abele, A. E. (submitted). Two faces of the self: Actor self and observer self are dif-
ferently related to agency and communion.
Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a scale for measuring
state self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 (6), 895–910.
Hogan, R. (1982). A socioanalytic theory of personality. In M. Page (Ed.), Nebraska sympo-
sium on motivation (pp. 336–355). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Huang, X. T., & Yu, H. (2002). Confirmatory factor analysis of the construct validity of
the self-worth scale for adolescents. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 34 (5), 511–516.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company.
Agency and Communion in Self-Concept 63
Kervyn, N., Fiske, S. T., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2015). Forecasting the primary dimension of
social perception: Symbolic and realistic threats together predict warmth in the Stereo-
type Content Model. Social Psychology, 46 (1), 36–45.
Koole, S. L., Govorun, O., Cheng, C. M., & Gallucci, M. (2009). Pulling yourself together:
Meditation promotes congruence between implicit and explicit self-esteem. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 45(6), 1220–1226.
Leach, C. W., Ellemers, N., & Barreto, M. (2007). Group virtue: The importance of moral-
ity (vs. competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 234–249.
Leary, M. R. (2012). Sociometer theory. In P. M. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T.
Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 151–159). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Leary, M. R., & Downs, D. (1995). Interpersonal functions of the self-esteem motive: The
self-esteem system as a sociometer. In Kernis, M. (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem
(pp. 123–144). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Li, S., Tseng, L., Wu, C., & Chen, C. (2007). Development of the agency and communion
scale. Social Behavior and Personality, 35(10), 1373–1378.
MacDonald, G., Saltzman, J. L., & Leary, M. R. (2003). Social approval and trait self-
esteem. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 23–40.
Mahadevan, N., Gregg, A., Sedikides, C., & de Waal-Andrews, W. (2016). Winners, losers,
insiders, and outsiders: Comparing hierometer and sociometer theories of self-regard.
Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–19.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago, IL: University Press.
Pagliaro, S., Ellemers, N., Barreto, M., & Di Cesare, C. (2016). Once dishonest, always
dishonest? The impact of perceived pervasiveness of moral evaluations of the self on
motivation to restore a moral reputation. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 586.
Paulhus, D. L., & Trapnell, P. D. (2008). Self-presentation of personality: An agency-
communion framework. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook
of personality psychology: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 492–517). New York, NY: Guil-
ford Press.
Peeters, G. (2008). The evaluative face of a descriptive model: Communion and agency in
Peabody’s tetradic model of trait organization. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38,
1066–1072.
Raskin, R. N., & Hall, C. S. (1979). A narcissistic personality inventory. Psychological
Reports, 45(2), 590.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Uni-
versity Press.
Schwartz, S. H., & Bardi, A. (2001). Value hierarchies across cultures: Taking a similarities
perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32 (3), 268–290.
Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1991). A terror management theory of social
behavior: The psychological functions of self-esteem and cultural worldviews. Advances
in Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 93–159.
Tafarodi, R. W., & Milne, A. B. (2002). Decomposing global self-esteem. Journal of Person-
ality, 70 (4), 443–483.
Wojciszke, B., & Abele, A. E. (2008). The primacy of communion over agency and its
reversals in evaluations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1139–1147.
Wojciszke, B., Baryla, W., Parzuchowski, M., Szymkow, A., & Abele, A. E. (2011). Self
esteem is dominated by agentic over communal information. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 41(5), 617–627.
64 Andrea E. Abele and Nicole Hauke
Wojciszke, B., & Sobiczewska, P. (2013). Memory and self-esteem: The role of agentic and
communal content. Social Psychology, 44, 95–102.
Ybarra, O., Chan, E., Park, H., Burnstein, E., Monin, B., & Stanik, C. (2008). Life’s recur-
ring challenges and the fundamental dimensions: An integration and its implications for
cultural differences and similarities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1083–1092.
6
AGENTIC AND COMMUNAL
SOCIAL MOTIVES
Kenneth D. Locke
Social motives – motives that energize and guide social life – can be organized
into two broad categories: agentic and communal (Horowitz et al., 2006; Wig-
gins, 1991; see also Abele & Wojciszke, this volume, and the other chapters in
this volume). Agentic social motives induce people to stand out and get ahead –
for example, by demonstrating or asserting superior skill, influence, achievement,
worth, or power (Hogan & Roberts, 2000). Communal social motives induce
people to fit in and get along – for example, by emphasizing their commonalities
or showing how they are kind, cooperative, trustworthy, and generous partners.
Agentic and communal motives are fundamental and universal elements of human
nature (Anderson, Hildreth, & Howland, 2015; Baumeister & Leary, 1995), shap-
ing and being shaped by the opportunities and challenges of social life throughout
our evolutionary history (Chan, Wang, & Ybarra, this volume).
The current chapter shows that agency and communion function as cardinal
axes along which we chart the course of our social lives. The chapter’s first sec-
tion explores how any direction we take – approaching agency, approaching
communion, avoiding agency, and avoiding communion – can lead to good and
bad outcomes. Therefore, as explained in the chapter’s second section, we rely
on upward, connective, downward, and contrastive social comparisons to steer
us away from agentic and communal goals that are likely to be frustrating (e.g.,
competing with others whose assets decisively exceed our own) and towards
those that are likely to be fulfilling (e.g., partnering with others with whom
we share core attitudes and aims). The chapter’s third section describes how, in
addition, individuals differ in dispositions to incline in particular directions (e.g.,
towards agency, away from communion) due to factors such as life history, life
stage, gender, and general sensitivities to costs or rewards. Finally, the chapter’s
fourth section examines the regulation of agentic and communal motives by
testosterone and oxytocin.
66 Kenneth D. Locke
that suggest you would never fall as low) excite agentic motives (Buunk & Ybema,
1997; Lockwood, Shaughnessy, Fortune, & Tong, 2012; Wheeler, Martin, & Suls,
1997). Conversely, contrastive comparisons with upward targets that suggest that
you can never rise as high (plus connective comparisons with downward targets that
suggest you might fall as low) dampen agentic motives. Upward contrastive com-
parisons can also undermine communal motives and even provoke hostile impulses
towards the superior target (Lam, Van der Vegt, Walter, & Huang, 2011; Tesser,
1988). Narcissistic individuals – who characteristically show stronger agentic than
communal motives (Locke, 2000; Trapnell & Paulhus, 2012) – appear especially
willing to denigrate or distance themselves from those who outperform them, thus
sacrificing relationships to protect their illusions of superiority (Morf & Rhodewalt,
1993; Nicholls & Stukas, 2011).
A shared consensus about who is superior can obviate potentially costly competi-
tions. Indeed, individuals may deliberately avoid competitions by portraying them-
selves as inferior and ineffectual (e.g., “I am too timid to take charge”). However,
assuming submissive, unagentic stances – if done chronically or excessively – can
contribute to depression (Taylor, Gooding, Wood, & Tarrier, 2011). Studies of psy-
chiatric patients found that depressed individuals gave disproportionate importance
to unagentic goals (e.g., to avoid being confronted, humiliated, or scorned), and,
during treatment, successfully calming these motives predicted reductions in dis-
tress (Locke et al., 2017; Thomas, Kirchmann, Suess, Bräutigam, & Strauss, 2012).
Approach/avoidance
Individual differences in general propensities to approach rewards or avoid costs
may help explain individual differences in propensities to approach/avoid agency
and communion. Supporting this hypothesis, agentic and communal motives are
positively associated with extraversion (a trait linked to reward sensitivity and
approach motives) and negatively associated with neuroticism (a trait linked to
punishment sensitivity and avoidance motives) (Corr, DeYoung, & McNaughton,
2013; Gable, Reis, & Elliot, 2003; Locke & Heller, 2017). A specific avoidance goal
that may specifically moderate communal motives is disease avoidance. People
who are chronically prone or situationally primed to feel repulsed by communi-
cable pathogens tend to report lower levels of communion (e.g., friendliness, trust)
toward strangers and foreigners, and instead may emphasize ingroup and family
communion ( Fincher & Thornhill, 2012; Murray & Schaller, 2016).
and kin, strangers, and even other species (Goetz, Keltner, & Simon-Thomas, 2010 ;
Preston, 2013; Tomasello, 2014). Harnessing agency towards genuinely communal
ends is the essence of the adult developmental task of generativity (Erikson, 1950)
and normative conceptions of heroism (Frimer, Walker, Lee, Riches, & Dunlop,
2012; Kinsella, Ritchie, & Igou, 2015).
Alas, most people are not heroes. Communal motives, though expansive in prin-
ciple (Singer, 1981), are often disconcertingly narrow in reality. For example, parents
tend nurture their own children more than others’ children and their biological chil-
dren more than their stepchildren, and fathers may better nurture their children who
resemble them more (Del Giudice & Belsky, 2010). Moreover, many people experi-
ence tensions rather than synergies between their agentic and communal motives.
Such tensions can arise between agentic motives to acquire new sexual partners or
produce more children and communal motives to invest in and nurture the children
and partner one already has (Durante, Eastwick, Finkel, Gangestad, & Simpson, 2016;
Fletcher, Simpson, Campbell, & Overall, 2015). Perhaps because of such tensions,
men with stronger agentic power motives or weaker communal affiliation motives
feel more constrained by fatherhood (Ruppen, Waldvogel, & Ehlert, 2016).
Life history theory suggests that a key moderator of communal motives to invest
in relationships, children, and society is social/environmental unpredictability, espe-
cially during childhood (Del Giudice, Gangestad, & Kaplan, 2015; Ellis, Figueredo,
Brumbach, & Schlomer, 2009). From the perspective of natural selection, if chil-
dren’s life spans are unpredictable, there may be little benefit in investing in nurtur-
ing a particular child; if others’ fidelity is unpredictable, there may be little benefit
in investing in a long-term relationship; and if the wider world is unpredictable,
there may be little benefit in investing in improving your society. Indeed, exposure
to unpredictable environments predicts more aggression, relationship instability, and
narcissistic, Machiavellian, and antisocial personality traits (e.g., Ellis et al.; Jonason,
Icho, & Ireland, 2016) – i.e., traits reflecting diminished communal (but undimin-
ished agentic) motives (Locke, 2000; Locke & Christensen, 2007).
Sex differences
Because of constraints imposed by gestation, lactation, and menopause, males can
potentially have a greater number of children, while females are required to make
a greater minimum physiological investment in each child. Consequently, females
tend to be choosier regarding with whom they will mate, obligating mate-seeking
males to engage in intra- and inter-sexual competition (Buss, 1995). Generations of
differential selection pressures favoring males pursuing rank and females providing
care could lead to sex-linked differences in social motives; and indeed, compared to
men, women typically place more importance on communion and less importance
on agency (Locke & Heller, 2017; Schwartz & Rubel, 2005; see also chapter 9).
Sex differences in social motives may help explain sex differences in preferences for
power versus status (Hays, 2013). Whereas power/dominance entails demonstrating
you can and will use force or resources to punish and reward others, status/prestige
Agentic and Communal Social Motives 71
entails demonstrating you can and will use your skills or assets to benefit others
(Cheng, Tracy, Foulsham, Kingstone, & Henrich, 2013; Magee & Galinsky, 2008).
Locke and Heller (2017) found that in the workplace people with stronger agentic
motives were more likely to want power, have power, and have their job satisfaction
depend on their having power; in contrast, people with stronger communal motives
were more likely to have status and to prefer status to power. Moreover, women’s
tendency to have stronger communal motives and weaker agentic motives than men
partly explained women’s stronger preference to have status rather than power.
Social chemistry
Hormones and neuropeptides – most notably testosterone and oxytocin – help
regulate agentic and communal motives, thus potentially contributing to the indi-
vidual differences described above. Testosterone appears to amplify agentic motives
to enhance and defend one’s social rank. Oxytocin appears to amplify communal
motives to nurture and protect one’s social bonds and significant others.
Oxytocin
Oxytocin levels – whether measured or manipulated – are positively associated
with engaged, nurturing, protective parental behavior ( Feldman & Bakermans-
Kranenburg, 2017; Bakermans-Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn, 2017). Oxytocin
levels increase after birth for both mothers and fathers, and involved fathers who
interact with their infants show oxytocin levels comparable to that of mothers.
During our evolutionary history the role of oxytocin has progressively expanded
from facilitating parenting to facilitating other attachments, including romantic
relationships (Fletcher et al., 2015; Griskevicius et al., 2015). For example, men in
committed relationships who received oxytocin experienced their partner as more
attractive ( Scheele et al., 2013). However, among individuals prone to feeling inse-
cure or vulnerable in relationships, elevating oxytocin may amplify those feelings
and thus activate self-protective rather than communal behavior (Bartz, 2016).
More broadly, oxytocin heightens social concerns and facilitates bonding and
benevolence among ingroup members, especially very close others (MacDonald &
MacDonald, 2010). Simultaneously, oxytocin may sharpen ingroup-outgroup
boundaries, and intensify wary, competitive, or hostile behavior toward poten-
tially threatening outgroup members ( Shalvi & De Dreu, 2014). Tellingly, priming
the parental care motive produces similar effects, heightening aversion to poten-
tially threatening others, such as strangers and distrusted outgroups (Eibach &
Mock, 2011; Gilead & Lieberman, 2014).
Testosterone
Testosterone levels are positively correlated with self-report, observational, and
implicit measures of agentic motivation (Knight & Mehta, 2014; Turan, Guo,
72 Kenneth D. Locke
Boggiano, & Bedgood, 2014). Individuals with higher testosterone levels are more
prone to desire an elevated social position and pursue assertive, competitive, or
aggressive actions in order to attain and retain social rank (Mehta & Josephs,
2011). Testosterone also activates sexual and mating motives (Muller, 2017), but
may inhibit bonding and nurturing (van Anders, Goldey, & Kuo, 2011; Roney &
Gettler, 2015). For example, higher testosterone levels predict being less committed
to one’s current partner and more interested in alternative partners (Wardecker,
Smith, Edelstein, & Loving, 2015), being more averse to intimate conversa-
tions following sexual activity (Denes, Afifi, & Granger, 2017), and among men
responding less sympathetically to infant cries (Fleming, Corter, Stallings, &
Steiner, 2002).
More generally, testosterone may stimulate agentic motives while suppressing
communal motives. For example, men with higher testosterone levels tend to be
more egocentric and antisocial (Johnson, Leedom, & Muhtadie, 2012; Wright
et al., 2012) and express weaker communal motives (Turan et al., 2014). Interest-
ingly, men’s testosterone levels decline when they transition from mate-seeking to
committing to a romantic partner or becoming a resident father – i.e., life circum-
stances in which rebalancing social motives away from agency (competing for new
mating opportunities) and toward communion (caring for one’s existing relation-
ship and offspring) would generally have been adaptive (Roney & Gettler, 2015).
play a greater role in operating our homes and businesses as well as our financial,
power, and communication systems. While AIs are motivated to achieve specific
aims, they must also accept the limits of their agency (e.g., not try to exceed speed
limits or pass faster vehicles). Furthermore, they should want to avoid connections
with untrustworthy human or non-human agents (e.g., potential security threats),
while also wanting to form and maintain mutually beneficial connections with
agents whose goals align with theirs (e.g., with whom they can share pertinent
information), which requires demonstrating their own trustworthiness. In other
words, the more powerful and autonomous the AI, the more it should be regulated
by a mixture of agentic, unagentic, communal, and uncommunal motives that
can be flexibly applied to complex and novel situations. Thus, our understanding
of the two fundamental social motives may help us not only to enhance human
relating, but also to successfully weave AIs into the fabric of society.
References
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (this volume). Introduction: The Big Two of agency and
communion as an overarching framework in psychology. In A. E. Abele & B. Woj-
ciszke (Eds.), Agency and communion in social psychology. Abingdon, UK: Routledge.
Anderson, C., Hildreth, J., & Howland, L. (2015). Is the desire for status a fundamental
human motive? Psychological Bulletin, 141, 574–601.
Bahns, A. J., Crandall, C. S., Gillath, O., & Preacher, K. J. (2017). Similarity in relation-
ships as niche construction: Choice, stability, and influence within dyads in a free
choice environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112, 329–355.
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2017). Protective parenting:
Neurobiological and behavioral dimensions. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 45–49.
Barclay, P. (2016). Biological markets and the effects of partner choice on cooperation and
friendship. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 33–38.
Bartz, J. A. (2016). Oxytocin and the pharmacological dissection of affiliation. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 104–110.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differences: Origins through sexual selection. Ameri-
can Psychologist, 50, 164–171.
Buunk, B. P., & Ybema, J. F. (1997). Social comparisons and occupational stress: The
identification-contrast model. In B. P. Buunk & F. X. Gibbons (Eds.), Health, coping, and
well-being: Perspectives from social comparison theory (pp. 359–388). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chan, T., Wang, I., & Ybarra, O. (this volume). Connect and strive to survive and thrive:
The evolutionary meaning of communion and agency. In A. E. Abele & B. Wojciszke
(Eds.), Agency and communion in social psychology. Abingdon, UK: Routledge.
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A., & Henrich, J. (2013). Two ways to
the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social
rank and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 103–125.
Church, A. T., Katigbak, M. S., Locke, K. D., Zhang, H., Shen, J., ... Ching, C. M. (2013).
Need satisfaction and well-being testing self-determination theory in eight cultures.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44, 507–534.
74 Kenneth D. Locke
Corr, P. J., DeYoung, C. G., & McNaughton, N. (2013). Motivation and personality: A
neuropsychological perspective. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 158–175.
Crocker, J., Canevello, A., & Brown, A. A. (2017). Social motivation: Costs and benefits of
selfishness and otherishness. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 299–325.
Cundiff, J. M., & Smith, T. W. (2017). Social status, everyday interpersonal processes, and
coronary heart disease: A social psychophysiological view. Social and Personality Psychol-
ogy Compass, 11. Advance online publication.
Del Giudice, M., & Belsky, J. (2010). Sex differences in attachment emerge in middle
childhood: An evolutionary hypothesis. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 97–105.
Del Giudice, M., Gangestad, S. W., & Kaplan, H. S. (2015). Life history theory and evo-
lutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology (2nd ed.,
pp. 88–114). New York, NY: Wiley.
Denes, A., Afifi, T. D., & Granger, D. A. (2017). Physiology and pillow talk: Relations
between testosterone and communication post sex. Journal of Social and Personal Rela-
tionships, 34, 281–308.
Durante, K. M., Eastwick, P. W., Finkel, E. J., Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2016).
Pair-bonded relationships and romantic alternatives: Toward an integration of evolu-
tionary and relationship science perspectives. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
53, 1–74.
Eibach, R. P., & Mock, S. E. (2011). The vigilant parent: Parental role salience affects par-
ents’ risk perceptions, risk aversion, and trust in strangers. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 47, 694–697.
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). The impact of
harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life his-
tory strategies. Human Nature, 20, 204–268.
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Co.
Feldman, R., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2017). Oxytocin: A parenting hormone.
Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 13–18.
Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2012). Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative soci-
ality: The cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 35, 61–79.
Fleming, A. S., Corter, C., Stallings, J., & Steiner, M. (2002). Testosterone and prolactin
are associated with emotional responses to infant cries in new fathers. Hormones and
Behavior, 42, 399–413.
Fletcher, G. J., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., & Overall, N. C. (2015). Pair-bonding,
romantic love, and evolution: The curious case of Homo sapiens. Perspectives on Psycho-
logical Science, 10, 20–36.
Frimer, J. A., Walker, L. J., Lee, B. H., Riches, A., & Dunlop, W. L. (2012). Hierarchical
integration of agency and communion: A study of influential moral figures. Journal of
Personality, 80, 1117–1145.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Elliot, A. J. (2003). Evidence for bivariate systems: An empiri-
cal test of appetition and aversion across domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37,
349–372.
Gebauer, J. E., Wagner, J., Sedikides, C., & Neberich, W. (2013). Agency communion
and self esteem relations are moderated by culture, religiosity, age, and sex. Journal of
Personality, 81, 261–275.
Gilead, M., & Lieberman, N. (2014). We take care of our own: Caregiving salience increases
out group bias in response to outgroup threat. Psychological Science, 25, 1380–1387.
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: An evolutionary analy-
sis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 351–374.
Agentic and Communal Social Motives 75
Griskevicius, V., Haselton, M. G., & Ackerman, J. (2015). Evolution and close relation-
ships. In M. Mikulincer, J. Simpson, & J. Dovidio (Eds.), APA handbook of personality
and social psychology, Vol. 3: Interpersonal relations (pp. 3–32). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Hays, N. A. (2013). Fear and loving in social hierarchy: Sex differences in preferences for
power versus status. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 1130–1136.
Helgeson, V. S., & Fritz, H. L. (2000). The implications of unmitigated agency and
unmitigated communion for domains of problem behavior. Journal of Personality, 68,
1031–1057.
Hogan, R., & Roberts, B. W. (2000). A socioanalytic perspective on person-environment
interaction. In W. B. Walsh, K. H. Craik, & R. H. Price (Eds.), New directions in person-
environment psychology (pp. 1–24). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence-Erlbaum.
Horowitz, L. M., Wilson, K. R., Turan, B., Zolotsev, P., Constantino, M. J., & Henderson,
L. (2006). How interpersonal motives clarify the meaning of interpersonal behavior: A
revised circumplex model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 67–86.
Jonason, P. K., Icho, A., & Ireland, K. (2016). Resources, harshness, and unpredictability:
The socioeconomic conditions associated with the dark triad traits. Evolutionary Psy-
chology, 14, 1–11.
Johnson, S. L., Leedom, L. J., & Muhtadie, L. (2012). The dominance behavioral system
and psychopathology: Evidence from self-report, observational, and biological studies.
Psychological Bulletin, 138, 692–743.
Kammrath, L. K., & Scholer, A. A. (2011). The Pollyanna Myth: How highly agreeable
people judge positive and negative relational acts. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
letin, 37, 1172–1184.
Kinsella, E. L., Ritchie, T. D., & Igou, E. R. (2015). Zeroing in on heroes: A prototype
analysis of hero features. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 114–127.
Knight, E. L., & Mehta, P. H. (2014). Hormones and hierarchies. In J. T. Cheng, J. L.
Tracy, & C. Anderson (Eds.), The psychology of social status (pp. 269–301). New York,
NY: Springer.
Križan, Z., & Smith, R. H. (2014). When comparisons divide. In Z. Krizan & F. X. Gibbons
(Eds.), Communal functions of social comparison (pp. 60–94). New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Lam, C. K., Van der Vegt, G. S., Walter, F., & Huang, X. (2011, June). Harming high
performers: A social comparison perspective on interpersonal harming in work teams.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 588–601.
Leary, M. R., & Cottrell, C. A. (2013). Evolutionary perspectives on interpersonal accep-
tance and rejection. In C. N. DeWall (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of social exclusion
(pp. 9–19). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Locke, K. D. (2000). Circumplex scales of interpersonal values: Reliability, validity, and
applicability to interpersonal problems and personality disorders. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 75, 249–267.
Locke, K. D. (2003). Status and solidarity in social comparison: Agentic and communal
values and vertical and horizontal directions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
84, 619–631.
Locke, K. D. (2011). Circumplex measures of interpersonal constructs. In L. M. Horowitz &
S. Strack (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal psychology: Theory, research, assessment, and thera-
peutic interventions (pp. 313–324). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Locke, K. D. (2014). Circumplex scales of intergroup goals: An interpersonal circle model
of goals for interactions between groups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40,
433–449.
76 Kenneth D. Locke
Nicholls, E., & Stukas, A. A. (2011). Narcissism and the self-evaluation maintenance
model: Effects of social comparison threats on relationship closeness. Journal of Social
Psychology, 151, 201–212.
Ojanen, T., Grönroos, M., & Salmivalli, C. (2005). An interpersonal circumplex model
of children’s social goals: Links with peer-reported behavior and sociometric status.
Developmental Psychology, 41, 699–710.
Preston, S. D. (2013). The origins of altruism in offspring care. Psychological Bulletin, 139,
1305–1341.
Rek, I., & Dinger, U. (2016). Who sits behind the telephone? Interpersonal characteristics
of volunteer counselors in telephone emergency services. Journal of Counseling Psychol-
ogy, 63, 429–442.
Robinson, O. C. (2013). Values and adult age: Findings from two cohorts of the European
Social Survey. European Journal of Ageing, 10, 11–23.
Roney, J. R., & Gettler, L. T. (2015). The role of testosterone in human romantic relation-
ships. Current Opinion in Psychology, 1, 81–86.
Roney, J. R., & Von Hippel, W. (2010). The presence of an attractive woman elevates
testosterone and physical risk taking in young men. Social Psychological and Personality
Science, 1, 57–64.
Ruppen, J., Waldvogel, P., & Ehlert, U. (2016). Implicit motives and men’s perceived con-
straint in fatherhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1856.
Schaller, M., Kenrick, D. T., Neel, R., & Neuberg, S. L. (2017). Evolution and human moti-
vation: A fundamental motives framework. Social and Personality Psychology Compass,
11(6), e12319.
Scheele, D., Wille, K. M., Kendrick, K. M., Becker, B., Gunturkun, O., Maier, M., &
Hulemann, R. (2013). Oxytocin alters the human reward system to maintain romantic
love. Pharmacopsychiatry, 46-A93.
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (Eds.). (2010). Implicit motives. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Schultheiss, O. C., & Hale, J. A. (2007). Implicit motives modulate attentional orienting to
facial expressions of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 13–24.
Schwartz, S. H., & Rubel, T. (2005). Sex differences in value priorities: Cross-cultural and
multimethod studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 1010–1028.
Shalvi, S., & De Dreu, C. K. (2014). Oxytocin promotes group-serving dishonesty. Proceed-
ings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 5503–5507.
Singer, P. (1981). The expanding circle. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Smith, T. W., & Jordan, K. D. (2015). Interpersonal motives and social evaluative threat:
Effects of acceptance and status stressors on cardiovascular reactivity and salivary cor-
tisol response. Psychophysiology, 52, 269–276.
Taylor, P. J., Gooding, P., Wood, A. M., & Tarrier, N. (2011). The role of defeat and entrap-
ment in depression, anxiety, and suicide. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 391–420.
Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social behavior. Advances
in Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 181–227.
Thomas, A., Kirchmann, H., Suess, H., Bräutigam, S., & Strauss, B. M. (2012). Motiva-
tional determinants of interpersonal distress: How interpersonal goals are related to
interpersonal problems. Psychotherapy Research, 22, 489–501.
Tomasello, M. (2014). The ultra-social animal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 187–194.
Trapnell, P. D., & Paulhus, D. L. (2012). Agentic and communal values: Their scope and
measurement. Journal of Personality Assessment, 94, 39–52.
Trucco, E. M., Wright, A. G. C., & Colder, C. R. (2013). A revised interpersonal circum-
plex inventory of children’s social goals. Assessment, 30, 98–113.
78 Kenneth D. Locke
Trucco, E. M., Wright, A. G. C., & Colder, C. R. (2014). Stability and change of social
goals in adolescence. Journal of Personality, 82, 379–389.
Turan, B., Guo, J., Boggiano, M. M., & Bedgood, D. (2014). Dominant, cold, avoidant,
and lonely: Basal testosterone as a biological marker for an interpersonal style. Journal of
Research in Personality, 50, 84–89.
van Anders, S. M., Goldey, K. L., & Kuo, P. X. (2011). The steroid/peptide theory of social
bonds: Integrating testosterone and peptide responses for classifying social behavioral
contexts. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 1265–1275.
van de Ven, N., Hoogland, C. E., Smith, R. H., van Dijk, W. W., Breugelmans, S. M., &
Zeelenberg, M. (2014). When envy leads to schadenfreude. Cognition and Emotion, 29,
1007–1025.
Wardecker, B. M., Smith, L. K., Edelstein, R. S., & Loving, T. J. (2015). Intimate rela-
tionships then and now: How old hormonal processes are influenced by our modern
psychology. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 1, 150–176.
Weinberger, J., Cotler, T., & Fishman, D. (2010). The duality of affiliative motivation. In
O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 71–88). New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Wheeler, L., Martin, R., & Suls, J. (1997). The proxy model of social comparison for self-
assessment of ability. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 54–61.
Wiggins, J. S. (1991). Agency and communion as conceptual coordinates for the under-
standing and measurement of interpersonal behavior. In D. Cicchetti & W. M. Grove
(Eds.), Thinking clearly about psychology: Essays in honour of Paul Meehl, Vol. 2: Personality
and psychopathology (pp. 89–113). Minneapolis, MN: University Minnesota Press.
Wright, N. D., Bahrami, B., Johnson, E., Di Malta, G., Rees, G., Frith, C. D., & Dolan, R. J.
(2012). Testosterone disrupts human collaboration by increasing egocentric choices. Pro-
ceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1736), 2275–2280.
7
THE BIG TWO DIMENSIONS
OF DESIRABILITY
Delroy L. Paulhus
Introduction
The varied papers in this volume are testament to the breadth of application of the
two meta-dimensions, agency and communion. I will use the term desirability as
synonymous with positive evaluation and argue that the broad influence of the agency-
communion distinction can be traced to two distinct ways in which people evaluate
themselves, other people, and questionnaire items. Whereas it has long been assumed
that evaluation is unidimensional – that is, every stimulus can be rated on one dimen-
sion from bad to good – I will argue that evaluation is bidimensional. This dual evalu-
ation emanates from the fact that both agency and communion are desirable qualities.
by Sam Messick (1960) and Nancy Wiggins (1966). Messick came up with nine
factors; the first two corresponded to agentic and communal desirability, respec-
tively. Similarly, Wiggins came up with six, including separate factors for agentic
and communal desirability.
When limited to two factors, factor analyses of desirability ratings yield a clear
result: agentic and communal evaluation – with comparable sizes (Carey & Paulhus,
2008). That result supports the claim that agency and communion predominate in
evaluations as well as in self-ratings (Caruana, Lefeuvre, & Mollaret, 2014; Bruce &
Paulhus, 1990).
Rater differences
Many traits are judged positively by some people and negatively by other people.
If so, desirability scale values may show moderate means but bimodal distributions
(Abbott, 1975). For example, the trait “conservative” is evaluated positively by half
the people and negatively by the other half. Also, psychopaths often show evalu-
ations that are the reverse of most raters. For example, psychopaths view “nasty”
and “aggressive” as highly desirable (Buckels & Paulhus, 2017). Finally, there are
individual differences in people’s motivation to evaluate at all (Jarvis & Petty, 1996).
Such rater differences add further evidence against the notion that desirability is
unidimensional.
Context differences
Other research has established that evaluation of the same behavior can differ dra-
matically depending on the context in which it is rated (Ferris et al., 2010). One
paradigm that illustrates this inconsistency is the simulation of job applications.
Subjects are asked how they would promote themselves if applying for diverse jobs
(Holden et al., 2003; Bruce & Paulhus, 1990). Traits that were rated as desirable for
the social worker position (e.g., empathy, nurturance) were not rated as desirable
for a position as a military drill instructor.
Note that all subjects in this research were asked to answer as if they were “fak-
ing good.” So apparently the word “good” changes meaning quite fluidly across
contexts. By limiting the context to job applications, this paradigm permits con-
trol over extraneous factors that would apply when desirability is compared across
work vs. home or relationship contexts (e.g., Block, 1961).
Another classic example is the way in which desirability changes meaning with
the gender of the target being rated. Sandra Bem (1974) collected desirability rat-
ings of traits (a) when applied to men and (b) when applied to women. The differ-
ences in these desirability ratings – sometimes drastic – were used to develop Bem’s
theory of psychological androgyny. Individuals who rated both kinds of traits as
desirable were gifted with the label androgynous. Interestingly, these two clusters
of traits were later demonstrated to tap agency and communion (Wiggins & Hol-
zmuller, 1981).
82 Delroy L. Paulhus
Fundamental values
The predominance of agency and communion in analyses of evaluation springs
from the two underlying values (Locke, 2000; Trapnell & Paulhus, 2012). The
Locke measures are scored according to dyadic interactions, whereas the Trapnell
and Paulhus measures are more global in nature.3 In the analysis by Paulhus and
John (1998), the triggering of values is the first step in a cascade that causes a bias
in favor of agentic or communal evaluations and eventual behavior.
Priming by context
Agentic and communal values can also be primed by context. The ease with
which judges can alter the weighting of dimension is exemplified by Sherman,
Mackie, and Driscoll (1990 ). Before evaluating political candidates, subjects
were primed with an agentic dimension (forging foreign policy) or a commu-
nal one (taking care of home citizens). Depending on which dimension was
primed, the evaluation of candidates was reversed. In short, evaluation is a very
pliable factor.
Hence, it is not surprising that the relative impact of agentic and communal
values also varies across applications. In terms of categorization speed, communion
takes precedence (Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011). In terms of impact on self-esteem,
agency takes precedence (Wojciszke, Baryla, Parzuchowski, Szymkow, & Abele,
2011; but see also Abele & Hauke, this volume). Although human judgment can
operate at varying degrees of complexity, it may be that the two-factor level of
agency-communion is the optimal level for everyday cognition (Rosch & Mervis,
1975).
The Big Two Dimensions of Desirability 83
Self-enhancement
Whether the trigger is fundamental values or context, individual differences in
self-enhancement emerge in both agentic and communal contexts. However, the
nature of enhancement differs qualitatively (Paulhus & John, 1998). In agentic
contexts, the enhancement has an egoistic flavor with an exaggerated sense of intel-
ligence, creativity, and overall competence. In communal contexts, the enhance-
ment has a moralistic flavor with an exaggerated sense of moral superiority (see
Gebauer & Sedikides, this volume).
environmental in origin and tended to increase correlations among the Big Five
factors. Our reasoning followed Hogan (1983) in recognizing that children are
socialized to seek two broad goals in life: “getting along” (communion) and “get-
ting ahead” (agency). Whenever activated, these motives can simplify the usual
five-dimensional personality structure to appear more bidimensional.
As a result, two second-order factors often emerge when factoring the cor-
relations among the Big Five traits (e.g., DeYoung, Peterson, & Higgins, 2002;
Digman, 1997). Although the latter researchers use different labels, the correspon-
dence with agency and communion is hard to ignore. As noted earlier, the bidi-
mensionality also becomes apparent when self-reports are collected under speeded
or stressful conditions (Paulhus et al., 1989).
Conclusions
Desirability doesn’t lie in the trait. It’s not an inherent property. Instead, it’s the
result of a process of considering the implications of personal qualities within an
86 Delroy L. Paulhus
Notes
1 Some rating instructions use labels that are explicitly communal. For example, Anderson
(1968) asked respondents to rate the “likableness” of 555 personality trait words (also
Schönbach, 1972).
2 See Brown (1986) for other examples.
3 Note that the popular Schwartz value model forces agency and communion to be in
opposition by ipsatizing the value ratings (Schwartz, 1992).
4 Nonetheless, to avoid such critiques, alternative approaches that are more objective in nature
have been developed and validated (see Paulhus & Holden, 2010).
5 Cross-dimensional influence of agency and communion is discussed in detail by Yzerbyt
(this volume) as well as by Judd and colleagues (2005).
References
Abbott, R. D. (1975). Improving the validity of affective self-report measures through
constructing personality scales unconfounded with social desirability: A study of the
Personality Research Form. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 35, 371–377.
Abele, A. E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”? Preferential pro-
cessing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 935–948.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition:
A dual perspective model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195–255.
Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and control-
lability of trait adjectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1621.
Anderson, N. H. (1968). Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 9, 272.
Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.
Block, J. (1961). Ego identity, role variability, and adjustment. Journal of Consulting Psychol-
ogy, 25, 392–397.
Bochner, S., & Van Zyl, T. (1985). Desirability ratings of 110 personality-trait words. Jour-
nal of Social Psychology, 125, 459–465.
Brown, R. (1986). Social psychology. New York, NY: Guilford.
Bruce, N. M., & Paulhus, D. L. (1990). Dimensions of faking in job applications. Presented at
the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Boston.
The Big Two Dimensions of Desirability 87
Buckels, E. E., & Paulhus, D. L. (2017). Why do psychopaths score lower on impression manage-
ment scales? The role of antisocial identity. University of British Columbia, under review.
Carey, J. M., & Paulhus, D. L. (2008, August). Factors of self-presentation: Agency and com-
munion? Presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Boston.
Caruana, S., Lefeuvre, R., & Mollaret, P. (2014). Looking for performance in personality
inventories: The primacy of evaluative information over descriptive traits. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 622–635.
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behav-
ioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 25, 215–224.
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of
psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349–354.
de Vries, R. E., Zettler, I., & Hilbig, B. E. (2014). Rethinking trait conceptions of social
desirability scales: Impression management as an expression of honesty-humility.
Assessment, 21, 286–299.
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2002). Higher-order factors of the Big
Five predict conformity: Are there neuroses of health? Personality and Individual Differ-
ences, 33, 533–552.
Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73, 1246–1256.
Dubois, N. (2000). Self-presentation strategies and social judgments: Desirability and social
utility of causal explanations. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 59, 170–182.
Edwards, A. L. (1957). The social desirability variable in personality assessment and research. New
York, NY: Dryden.
Emler, N. (1990). A social psychology of reputation. European Review of Social Psychology,
1, 171–193.
Ferris, G. R., Munyon, T. P., Basik, K., & Buckley, M. R. (2010). The performance evalu-
ation context: Social, emotional, cognitive, political, and relationship components.
Human Resource Management Review, 18, 146–163.
Hampson, S. E., Goldberg, L. R., & John, O. P. (1987). Category-breadth and social-
desirability values for 573 personality terms. European Journal of Personality, 1, 241–258.
Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1951). The MMPI manual. New York, NY: Psychologi-
cal Corporation.
Hogan, R. (1983). A socioanalytic theory of personality. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation,
29, 55–89. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Holden, R. R., Book, A. S., Edwards, M. J., Wasylkiw, L., & Starzyk, K. B. (2003). Experi-
mental faking in self-reported psychopathology: Unidimensional or multidimensional?
Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1107–1117.
Holden, R. R., & Fekken, G. C. (1989). Three common social desirability scales: Friends,
acquaintances, or strangers? Journal of Research in Personality, 23, 180–191.
Holden, R. R., & Passey, J. (2009). Social desirability. In M. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.),
Individual differences in social behavior (pp. 441–454). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Irwing, P., Booth, T., Nyborg, H., & Rushton, J. P. (2012). Are g and the General Factor of
Personality (GFP) correlated? Intelligence, 40, 296–305.
Jackson, D. N., Wroblewski, V. R., & Ashton, M. C. (2000). The impact of faking on
employment tests: Does forced choice offer a solution? Human Performance, 13, 371–388.
Jarvis, W. B. G., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The need to evaluate. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 70, 172–194.
John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1993). Determinants of interjudge agreement on personality
traits: The Big Five domains, observability, evaluativeness, and the unique perspective
of the self. Journal of Personality, 61, 521–551.
88 Delroy L. Paulhus
Judd, C. M., James-Hawkins, L., Yzerbyt, V., & Kashima, Y. (2005). Fundamental dimen-
sions of social judgment: Understanding the relations between judgments of compe-
tence and warmth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 899–913.
Leising, D., Borkenau, P., Zimmermann, J., Roski, C., Leonhardt, A., & Schütz, A. (2013).
Positive self-regard and claim to leadership: Two fundamental forms of self-evaluation.
European Journal of Personality, 27, 565–579.
Locke, K. D. (2000). Circumplex scales of interpersonal values: Reliability, validity, and
applicability to interpersonal problems and personality disorders. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 75, 249–267.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1983). Social desirability scales: More substance than style.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 882.
Messick, S. (1960). Dimensions of social desirability. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 279–287.
Nederhof, A. J. (1985). Methods of coping with social desirability bias: A review. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 15, 263–280.
Neuberg, S. L., & Newsom, J. T. (1993). Personal need for structure: Individual differences
in the desire for simpler structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 113.
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning.
Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two-component models of socially desirable responding. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 598–609.
Paulhus, D. L. (1991). Measurement and control of response bias. In J. P Robinson, P. R
Shaver & L. S Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes
(pp. 17–59). San Diego: Academic Press.
Paulhus, D. L. (2002). Socially desirable responding: The evolution of a construct. In
H. Braun, D. N. Jackson, & D. E. Wiley (Eds.), The role of constructs in psychological and
educational measurement (pp. 67–88). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Paulhus, D. L. (2017). Socially desirable responding. In V. Zeigler-Hill & D. Marcus (Eds.),
Wiley encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. New York, NY: Springer.
Paulhus, D. L., Graf, P., & Van Selst, M. (1989). Attentional load increases the positivity of
self-presentation. Social Cognition, 7, 389–400.
Paulhus, D. L., & Holden, R. R. (2010). Measuring self-enhancement: From self-report
to concrete behavior. In C. R. Agnew, D. E. Carlston, W. G. Graziano, & J. R. Kelly
(Eds.), Then a miracle occurs: Focusing on behavior in social psychological theory and research
(pp. 227–246). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Paulhus, D. L., & John, O. P. (1998). Egoistic and moralistic biases in self-perception: The
interplay of self-descriptive styles with basic traits and motives. Journal of Personality, 66,
1025–1060.
Peabody, D., & Goldberg, L. R. (1989). Some determinants of factor structures from
personality-trait descriptors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 552–567.
Pinker, S. (2005). So how does the mind work? Mind & Language, 20, 1–24.
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1996). Pluralistic ignorance and the perpetuation of social
norms by unwitting actors. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 161–209.
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Person-
ality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 54, 890–895.
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of
categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.
Rosenberg, S., & Sedlak, A. (1972). Structural representations of perceived personality trait
relationships. Multidimensional Scaling, 2, 134–162.
The Big Two Dimensions of Desirability 89
The year is 50 BC. Gaul is entirely occupied by the Romans. Well, not entirely ...
One small village of indomitable Gauls still holds out against the invaders.
– Asterix the Gaul ( Goscinny & Uderzo, 1969)
The Romans had occupied Gaul, just as the Big Two (agency and communion)
currently “occupy” self-evaluation (Abele & Hauke, this volume; Paulhus, this
volume). And just as Gaul, self-evaluation is not entirely occupied by the Big Two.
To date, hardly any self-evaluative concept receives more scientific attention than
grandiose narcissism – a global self-evaluation characterized by excessively exalted
self-importance, entitlement, and social power – and that concept still holds out
against the Big Two “invaders.” The classic view of grandiose narcissism considers
grandiose narcissism inherently agentic and, thus, rejects a communal form of this
construct. Here, we summarize theoretical reasons and empirical evidence for the
applicability of the Big Two to grandiose narcissism.
The chapter is divided into four parts. The first part reviews the classic view
of grandiose narcissism. The next part reviews evidence that the Big Two pervade
global self-evaluations, resulting in a simple – but consequential – argument: if the
Big Two are generally applicable to global self-evaluations, the Big Two should also
apply to grandiose narcissism. The third part describes a theoretical model that is
based on that argument, the agency-communion model of grandiose narcissism
(ACM; Gebauer, Sedikides, Verplanken, & Maio, 2012) in its revised, minimalist
version (minimalist-ACM; Gebauer et al., 2018). The final part reviews empirical
evidence for the minimalist-ACM.
relation within 20+ samples largely from the US, the UK, and Germany, also find-
ing a positive relation of moderate size. In addition, these authors probed for cross-
cultural differences in a large study from 50+ countries. They obtained a positive,
non-perfect relation between agentic and communal narcissism in all countries,
ranging in size between moderate and large.
Concluding remarks
The Big Two have figured prominently in the self-evaluation literature. Yet, the
classic view in the narcissism literature maintains that they are inapplicable to
grandiose narcissism. In contrast, we summarized theoretical reasons and empiri-
cal evidence for an agency-communion model of grandiose narcissism (Gebauer
et al., 2012) in its minimalist version (Gebauer et al., 2018). According to this
model, the Big Two apply to grandiose narcissism, just as they apply to other
global self-evaluations: grandiose narcissism comprises an agentic factor (agen-
tic narcissism) and a communal factor (communal narcissism). The evidence we
reviewed satisfies six key validity criteria of the minimalist-ACM. We conclude,
rather boldly, by returning to our opening quote: To consider grandiose narcissism
inapplicable to the Big Two is as inaccurate as to consider Asterix’s village part of
Gaul’s actual history.
Notes
1 Shavelson et al. (1976) also proposed two additional self-esteem facets. One facet (physi-
cal self-esteem), however, is not psychological. The other facet (emotional self-esteem) is
psychological, but does not tap into psychological content.
2 Fleming and Watts (1980) found a third factor, which they interpreted as global, content-
free self-esteem.
3 Heatherton and Polivy (1991) reported a third dimension (appearance), but that dimen-
sion is physical, not psychological.
4 We use the following terms to describe effect sizes (Cohen, 1988). Small: .10 r < .20,
small-to-moderate: .20 r < .30, moderate: .30 r < .40, moderate-to-large: .40 r < .50,
large: r > .50.
References
Abele, A. E., Cuddy, A. J., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2008). Fundamental dimensions
of social judgment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1063–1065.
Abele, A. E., & Hauke, N. (this volume). Agency and communion in self-perception. In
A. E. Abele & B. Wojciszke (Eds.), Agency and communion in social psychology. Abingdon,
UK: Routledge.
Allison, S. T., Messick, D. M., & Goethals, G. R. (1989). On being better but not smarter
than others: The Muhammad Ali effect. Social Cognition, 7, 275–295.
Back, M. D., Küfner, A. C. P., Dufner, M., Gerlach, T. M., Rauthmann, J. F., & Denissen,
J. J. A. (2013). Narcissistic admiration and rivalry: Disentangling the bright and dark
sides of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 1013–1037.
Agency and Communion in Grandiose Narcissism 99
Barry, C. T., & Malkin, M. L. (2010). The relation between adolescent narcissism and
internalizing problems depends on the conceptualization of narcissism. Journal of
Research in Personality, 44, 684–690.
Bosson, J. K., Lakey, C. E., Campbell, W. K., Zeigler-Hill, V., Jordan, C. H., & Kernis,
M. H. (2008). Untangling the links between narcissism and self-esteem: A theoretical
and empirical review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1415–1439.
Brown, A. A., Freis, S. D., Carroll, P. J., & Arkin, R. M. (2016). Perceived agency medi-
ates the link between the narcissistic subtypes and self-esteem. Personality and Individual
Differences, 90, 124–129.
Brown, R. P., Budzek, K., & Tamborski, M. (2009). On the meaning of narcissism. Person-
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 951–964.
Cain, N. M., Pincus, A. L., & Ansell, E. B. (2008). Narcissism at the crossroads: Phenotypic
description of pathological narcissism across clinical theory, social/personality psychol-
ogy, and psychiatric diagnosis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 638–656.
Campbell, W. K. (2001). Is narcissism really so bad? Psychological Inquiry, 12, 214–216.
Campbell, W. K., Bonacci, A. M., Shelton, J., Exline, J. J., & Bushman, B. J. (2004). Psy-
chological entitlement: Interpersonal consequences and validation of a new self-report
measure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 83, 29–45.
Campbell, W. K., & Foster, J. D. (2007). The narcissistic self: Background, an extended
agency model, and ongoing controversies. In C. Sedikides & S. J. Spencer (Eds.), The
self (pp. 115–138). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Campbell, W. K., Rudich, E., & Sedikides, C. (2002). Narcissism, self-esteem, and the
positivity of self-views: Two portraits of self-love. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
letin, 28, 358–368.
Carlson, E. N., Naumann, L. P., & Vazire, S. (2011). Getting to know a narcissist inside and
out. In W. K. Campbell & J. D. Miller (Eds.), The handbook of narcissism and narcissistic
personality disorder: Theoretical approaches, empirical findings, and treatments (pp. 283–299).
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Carlson, E. N., Vazire, S., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2011). You probably think this paper’s about
you: Narcissists’ perceptions of their personality and reputation. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 101, 185–201.
Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. (1979). Interpersonal attraction in exchange and communal rela-
tionships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 12–24.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Davis, D. E., Hook, J. N., Worthington, E. L., Jr., Van Tongeren, D. R., Gartner, A. L., Jen-
nings, D. J., & Emmons, R. A. (2011). Relational humility: Conceptualizing and mea-
suring humility as a personality judgment. Journal of Personality Assessment, 93, 225–234.
Dickinson, K. A., & Pincus, A. L. (2003). Interpersonal analysis of grandiose and vulner-
able narcissism. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 188–207.
Emmons, R. A. (1987). Narcissism: Theory and measurement. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 52, 11–17.
Fatfouta, R., Zeigler-Hill, V., & Schröder-Abé, M. (2017). I’m merciful, am I not? Facets
of narcissism and forgiveness revisited. Journal of Research in Personality, 70, 166–173.
Finkel, E. J., Campbell, W. K., Buffardi, L. E., Kumashiro, M., & Rusbult, C. E. (2009).
The metamorphosis of Narcissus: Communal activation promotes relationship commit-
ment among narcissists. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1271–1284.
Fleming, J. S., & Watts, W. A. (1980). The dimensionality of self-esteem: Some results for
a college sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 921–929.
Gabriel, M. T., Critelli, J. W., & Ee, J. S. (1994). Narcissistic illusions in self-evaluations of
intelligence and attractiveness. Journal of Personality, 62, 143–155.
100 Jochen E. Gebauer and Constantine Sedikides
Gebauer, J. E., & Sedikides, C. (in press). Communal narcissism: On the theoretical and
empirical support for a new form of grandiose narcissism. In A. D. Hermann, A.
Brunell, & J. Foster (Eds.), The handbook of trait narcissism: Key advances, research methods,
and controversies. New York, NY: Springer.
Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., Lüdtke, O., & Neberich, W. (2014). Agency-communion
and interest in prosocial behavior: Social motives for assimilation and contrast explain
sociocultural inconsistencies. Journal of Personality, 82, 452–466.
Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., & Schrade, A. (2017). Christian self-enhancement. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 786–809.
Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., Verplanken, B., & Maio, G. R. (2012). Communal narcissism.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 854–878.
Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., Wagner, J., Bleidorn, W., Rentfrow, P. J., Potter, J., & Gos-
ling, S. D. (2015). Cultural norm fulfillment, interpersonal belonging, or getting ahead?
A large-scale cross-cultural test of three perspectives on the function of self-esteem.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 526–548.
Gebauer, J. E., Żemojtel-Piotrowska, M. A., Sedikides, C. et al. (2018). Grandiose narcissism
comprises agentic and communal narcissism: Theoretical and empirical review and implications for
narcissism theory. Unpublished manuscript, University of Mannheim, Germany.
Giacomin, M., & Jordan, C. H. (2014). Down-regulating narcissistic tendencies: Commu-
nal focus reduces state narcissism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 488–500.
Giacomin, M., & Jordan, C. H. (2015). Validating power makes communal narcissists less
communal. Self and Identity, 14, 583–601.
Goscinny, R., & Uderzo, A. (1969). Asterix the Gaul. Leicester, UK: Brockhampton Press.
Grijalva, E., & Zhang, L. (2016). Narcissism and self-insight: A review and meta-analysis of
narcissists’ self-enhancement tendencies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 3–24.
Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a scale for measuring
state self-esteem. Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 895–910.
Hendin, H. M., & Cheek, J. M. (1997). Assessing hypersensitive narcissism: A re-examination
of Murray’s Narcissism Scale. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 588–599.
Hill, P. C., & Laney, E. K. (2017). Beyond self-interest: Humility and the quieted self.
In K. W. Brown & M. R. Leary (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypo-egoic phenomena
(pp. 243–255). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Janis, I. L., & Field, P. B. (1959). Sex differences and factors related to persuasibility. In C. I.
Hovland & I. L. Janis (Eds.), Personality and persuasibility (pp. 55–68). New Haven, CT:
Yale University Press.
John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1994). Accuracy and bias in self-perception: Individual dif-
ferences in self-enhancement and the role of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 66, 206–219.
Köpetz, C., Faber, T., Fishbach, A., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2011). The multifinality con-
straints effect: How goal multiplicity narrows the means set to a focal end. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 810–826.
Krizan, Z., & Herlache, A. D. (2018). The narcissism spectrum model: A synthetic view of
narcissistic personality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22, 3–31.
Leary, T. (1957). Interpersonal diagnosis of personality. New York, NY: Ronald.
Luo, Y. L., Cai, H., Sedikides, C., & Song, H. (2014). Distinguishing communal narcis-
sism from agentic narcissism: A behavior genetics analysis on the agency: Communion
model of narcissism. Journal of Research in Personality, 49, 52–58.
Maxwell, K., Donnellan, M. B., Hopwood, C. J., & Ackerman, R. A. (2011). The two
faces of Narcissus? An empirical comparison of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory
Agency and Communion in Grandiose Narcissism 101
and the Pathological Narcissism Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50,
577–582.
Miller, J. D., & Campbell, W. K. (2008). Comparing clinical and social-personality con-
ceptualizations of narcissism. Journal of Personality, 76, 449–476.
Miller, J. D., Hoffman, B. J., Gaughan, E. T., Gentile, B., Maples, J., & Campbell, W. K.
(2011). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: A nomological network analysis. Journal
of Personality, 79, 1013–1042.
Miller, J. D., Price, J., Gentile, B., Lynam, D. R., & Campbell, W. K. (2012). Grandiose and
vulnerable narcissism from the perspective of the interpersonal circumplex. Personality
and Individual Differences, 53, 507–512.
Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: A dynamic
self-regulatory processing model. Psychological Inquiry, 12, 177–196.
Nehrlich, A. D., Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., & Schoel, C. (in press). Agentic narcissism,
communal narcissism, and prosociality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. doi:
10.1037/pspp0000190
Paulhus, D. L. (2001). Normal narcissism: Two minimalist views. Psychological Inquiry, 12,
228–230.
Paulhus, D. L., & John, O. P. (1994, August). How many dimensions of evaluation are there?
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Los
Angeles, CA.
Paulhus, D. L., & John, O. P. (1998). Egoistic and moralistic bias in self-perceptions: The
interplay of self-deceptive styles with basic traits and motives. Journal of Personality, 66,
1025–1060.
Pincus, A. L., Ansell, E. B., Pimentel, C. A., Cain, N. M., Wright, A. G. C., & Levy, K. N.
(2009). Initial construction and validation of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory.
Psychological Assessment, 21, 365–379.
Raskin, R., Novacek, J., & Hogan, R. (1991a). Narcissism, self-esteem, and defensive self-
enhancement. Journal of Personality, 59, 19–38.
Raskin, R., Novacek, J., & Hogan, R. (1991b). Narcissistic self-esteem management. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 911–918.
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Person-
ality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 54, 890–902.
Roberts, C. R., & Wood, W. J. (2003). Humility and epistemic goods. In M. De Paul &
L. Zagzebski (Eds.), Intellectual virtue: Perspectives from ethics and epistemology. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Uni-
versity Press.
Sedikides, C., & Campbell, W. K. (2017). Narcissistic force meets systemic resistance: The
energy clash model. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 400–421.
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self-enhancement. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 60–79.
Sedikides, C., Rudich, E. A., Gregg, A. P., Kumashiro, M., & Rusbult, C. (2004). Are nor-
mal narcissists psychologically healthy? Self-esteem matters. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 87, 400–416.
Shavelson, R. J., Hubner, J. J., & Stanton, G. C. (1976). Self-concept: Validation of con-
struct interpretations. Review of Educational Research, 46, 407–441.
Tafarodi, R. W., & Milne, A. B. (2002). Decomposing global self-esteem. Journal of Per-
sonality, 70, 443–483.
102 Jochen E. Gebauer and Constantine Sedikides
Vazire, S., & Funder, D. C. (2006). Impulsivity and the self-defeating behavior of narcis-
sists. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 154–165.
Vernon, P. A., Villani, V. C., Vickers, L. C., & Harris, J. A. (2008). A behavioral genetic
investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5. Personality and Individual Differences, 44,
445–452.
Wiggins, J. S. (1991). Agency and communion as conceptual coordinates for the under-
standing and measurement of interpersonal behavior. In W. M. Grove & D. Ciccetti
(Eds.), Thinking clearly about psychology, Vol. 2: Personality and psychopathology (pp. 89–113).
Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Wink, P. (1991). Two faces of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61,
590–597.
Żemojtel-Piotrowska, M., Clinton, A., & Piotrowski, J. (2014). Agentic and communal
narcissism and subjective well-being: Are narcissistic individuals unhappy? A research
report. Current Issues in Personality Psychology, 2, 10–16.
Żemojtel-Piotrowska, M., Czarna, A. Z., Piotrowski, J., Baran, T., & Maltby, J. (2016).
Structural validity of the Communal Narcissism Inventory (CNI): The bifactor model.
Personality and Individual Differences, 90, 315–320.
Żemojtel-Piotrowska, M., Piotrowski, J., & Maltby, J. (2015). Agentic and communal nar-
cissism and satisfaction with life: The mediating role of psychological entitlement and
self-esteem. International Journal of Psychology, 52, 420–424.
9
AGENCY AND COMMUNION
Their implications for gender stereotypes
and gender identities
Agency and communion represent the two fundamental modalities of human nature.
These dimensions, the so-called Big Two, represent self- versus other-orientation.
As stated by Abele and Wojciszke (2014, p. 196), “Agentic content refers to goal-
achievement and task functioning (competence, assertiveness, decisiveness), whereas
communal content refers to the maintenance of relationships and social function-
ing (benevolence, trustworthiness, morality).” These dimensions constitute meta-
concepts of human values, motives, traits, and behaviors. As we explain in this
chapter, agency and communion are essential to the analysis of gender stereotypes
and identities and their consequences.
Lippa, Preston, & Penner, 2014), men still dominate most blue-collar jobs, many
of which have strength-intensive components. Yet, men’s greater size and strength
are much less influential overall because most occupations now favor brains over
brawn, and technology lessens the strength demands of most kinds of physical
work.
Despite these changes, occupations have remained profoundly sex-segregated.
Women are overrepresented in occupations that especially reward social skills (e.g.,
nursing, teaching children) and underrepresented in things-oriented occupations
(most STEM fields and mechanical and construction trades; Lippa et al., 2014).
The proportion of women is also low in occupations that especially reward agency
(e.g., top leadership roles; European Commission, 2017). Sociologists thus refer to
horizontal gender segregation, by which women and men have occupations favoring
different traits and abilities, and vertical segregation, by which men are concentrated
in occupations that yield greater status and power (Levanon & Grusky, 2016).
Social role theory proposes that everyday observations of the differing roles of
women and men provide information from which people derive gender stereotypes
(Eagly, 1987; Wood & Eagly, 2012). The resulting beliefs that women and men dif-
fer in agency and communion reflect essentialism, or the tendency to infer that dif-
ferent human essences underlie differences in behavior (Prentice & Miller, 2006).
People may assume that such essences follow from social or biological causes ( Ran-
gel & Keller, 2011). These stereotypic beliefs have considerable accuracy at group
level, that is, pertaining to women and men in general, due to their grounding in
observations of group members’ behaviors in their typical social roles (Koenig &
Eagly, 2014). In this sense, stereotypes reflect social reality (Jussim, 2012). How-
ever, they are of course not accurate for individuals who are atypical of their sex.
The perception of both sexes is influenced by their memberships in social groups
in addition to gender (intersectionality; Shields, 2008). Hence, studying gender along
with groupings by sexual orientation, ethnicity, age, and social class reflects the
complexity of people’s lives. Intersectional stereotypes can contain distinct elements
beyond gender stereotypes. For instance, gender stereotypes are closest to those of
Whites, whereas stereotypes about Black women are somewhat different from ste-
reotypes about women in general and Blacks in general (Ghavami & Peplau, 2013).
Gender stereotypes continue to receive support from contemporary occupa-
tional and domestic role segregation (Levanon & Grusky, 2016). Thus, a com-
parison of gender-stereotypical beliefs in the United States at earlier and recent
time points has revealed approximately the same agentic and communal beliefs
(e.g., Haines, Deaux, & Lofaro, 2016). However, studies have failed to identify
women’s gain in stereotypical competence, which presumably has occurred because
of their shift to paid employment and their greatly increased higher education. For
example, US survey research by the Pew Research Center (2015) found that respon-
dents believed that women were higher than men on competence traits such as
organized, innovative, and intelligent, yet lower on agentic traits such as ambitious
and decisive. Also, research on so-called dynamic stereotypes has shown a narrative
of change whereby people believe that women have become and are continuing
106 Sabine Sczesny et al.
to become more agentic, whereas men are more constant in their attributes (Diek-
man & Eagly, 2000). In reality, women appear to have gained stereotypical com-
petence but much less agency given their slow rise into roles demanding qualities
such as dominance and competitiveness.
Gender identity
Gender identity is individuals’ self-definition as female or male, which is based
on their biological sex as interpreted within their culture (Wood & Eagly, 2015).
When people describe who they are, most indicate that being a man or woman or
boy or girl is important to their identity and ascribe at least some gender-stereo-
typical traits to themselves.
The most basic aspect of gender identity is an existential sense of oneself as
female or male, which ordinarily corresponds to one’s biological sex. Psycholo-
gists have invented various direct and indirect methods for assessing this basic or
existential categorization of oneself as male or female. The most popular measure
adapts Luhtanen and Crocker’s (1992) collective self-esteem scale with items such
as “Being a woman [man] is an important reflection of who I am.”
At an early age, children typically learn that there are two sexes and that they
belong to one of these groupings (Martin & Ruble, 2010). Awareness of self and
others as male or female, which emerges by around 18 months of age, further
develops as children learn what this classification means in their culture. Observa-
tions of other boys and girls motivate children to act similarly by, for example,
playing with gender-typical toys.
As children mature, their personal experiences and observations of others
shape their ideas about the sexes into gender stereotypes, which form one basis
for their identities as they incorporate the cultural meanings of gender into their
own psyches. To the extent that people value their female or male group member-
ship, they tend to self-stereotype by ascribing culturally feminine or masculine
attributes to themselves. The link between self-categorization in a social group
and the application of the group stereotype to oneself is a key principle of social
identity theory (Abrams, Thomas, & Hogg, 1990). For example, among those
who value belonging to their male or female social category, women may regard
themselves as caring and compassionate and men regarded themselves as strong
and competitive.
To assess this self-stereotyping aspect of gender identity, psychologists typi-
cally obtain self-reports from women and men of their agentic and communal
personality traits by having them respond on rating scales (Bem, 1974; Spence &
Helmreich, 1980). These dimensions of personality thus match gender stereotypes,
and, in fact, were derived from earlier research demonstrating these stereotypes
(Rosenkrantz, Vogel, Bee, Broverman, & Broverman, 1968). Implicit measures
have also been adapted to assess this trait aspect of gender identity (e.g., Green-
wald & Farnham, 2000).
People act on their gender identities through self-regulatory processes, by
which they control their behavior to conform to their identity (Wood, Chris-
tensen, Hebl, & Rothgerber, 1997). Persons who value their identity as a woman
or a man experience positive affect when acting consistently with their personal
gender standards and negative affect when acting in ways that depart from these
standards. These emotions then guide their future actions.
Implications for Gender 109
Conclusion
The Big Two – agency and communion – are overriding themes in psychological
gender research. Social scientists, especially psychologists, have widely adopted
these concepts to describe gender stereotypes and gender identity and their con-
sequences for behavior. Reflecting psychologists’ idea that masculine stereotypes
have served largely as negative forces slowing women’s attainment of gender
equality, it seemed that change in women’s roles would boost their agency. Indeed,
women more often complete higher education, have taken up paid work and
entered many higher status occupational roles. What many scholars of gender have
missed is the preservation of the agency-communion divide in social roles, despite
these changes. Women remain concentrated primarily in communally demanding
occupations. In addition, women’s entry into agentically demanding occupations
such as management and law has triggered internal resegregation. Women are
underrepresented in the subareas of these professions regarded as more agenti-
cally demanding (e.g., top leadership roles in corporations and government) and
overrepresented in the subareas regarded as more communally demanding (e.g.,
human resources management; Levanon & Grusky, 2016). In addition, men have
shown little movement into female-dominated roles, either in the workplace or
in families ( Croft et al., 2015). This neotraditional division of labor perpetuates
gender-stereotypical beliefs about agency and communion as well as matching sex
differences in gender identity.
Understanding of these phenomena could be furthered by information from a
broader range of cultures. Although some researchers have incorporated data from
many nations (e.g., Williams & Best, 1982), the majority of studies come mainly from
Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic cultures (Henrich, Heine, &
Norenzayan, 2010), with the United States being decidedly overrepresented.
As another limitation, theoretical and empirical work on agency and com-
munion has often been restricted to a binary view that neglects the intersections
of gender with other group memberships. Also neglected are increasing trends
toward gender and sexual fluidity. Future research should expand these themes to
enlarge the understanding of gender in its varied, contemporary manifestations.
Another emerging theme is that the Big Two sometimes decompose into three
or four components. For example, agency sometimes has two components, asser-
tiveness and competence, and communion also can have two components, warmth/
sociability and morality (see Abele et al., 2016). The assertiveness/competence dif-
ferentiation is important to gender stereotypes because, as we have suggested,
contemporary stereotypes portray women as less assertive than men but not neces-
sarily less competent.
In conclusion, psychologists have made remarkable progress in understanding
the phenomena of gender. Nevertheless, in addition to remedying deficits we
noted in cultural breadth, intersectionality, and subcomponents of agency and
communion, psychologists of gender should reach beyond disciplinary bound-
aries to take into account the important research conducted by sociologists and
112 Sabine Sczesny et al.
References
Abele, A. E. (2003). The dynamics of masculine-agentic and feminine-communal traits:
Findings from a prospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 768–
776. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.768
Abele, A. E., Hauke, N., Peters, K., Louvet, E., Szymkow, A., & Duan, Y. (2016). Facets of
the fundamental content dimensions: Agency with competence and assertiveness: Com-
munion with warmth and morality. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fpsyg.2016.01810
Abele, A. E., & Spurk, D. (2011). The dual impact of gender and the influence of timing of par-
enthood on men’s and women’s career development: Longitudinal findings. International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 35, 225–232. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025411398181
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition:
A dual perspective model. In M. P. Zanna & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Advances in experimental
social psychology (Vol. 50, pp. 195–255). Burlington: Academic Press.
Abrams, D., Thomas, J., & Hogg, M. A. (1990). Numerical distinctiveness, social iden-
tity and gender salience. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 87–92. https://doi.org/
10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00889.x
Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0036215
Biernat, M., & Kobrynowicz, D. (1997). Gender-and race-based standards of competence:
Lower minimum standards but higher ability standards for devalued groups. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 544–557.
Bosak, J., & Sczesny, S. (2008). Am I the right candidate? Self-ascribed fit of women and men to
a leadership position. Sex Roles, 58, 682–688. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9380-4
Bosson, J. K., Vandello, J. A., Burnaford, R. M., Weaver, J. R., & Arzu Wasti, S. (2009).
Precarious manhood and displays of physical aggression. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 35, 623–634. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208331161
Cohn, S. (2017). The determinants of the division of labor between men and women in
paid employment in the global North and South: How occupational sex-typing informs
the study of gender and development. Sociology of Development, 3, 1–23. https://doi.
org/10.1525/sod.2017.3.1.1
Conway, M., Pizzamiglio, M. T., & Mount, L. (1996). Status, communality, and agency:
Implications for stereotypes of gender and other groups. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 71, 25.
Croft, A., Schmader, T., & Block, K. (2015). An underexamined inequality: Cultural and
psychological barriers to men’s engagement with communal roles. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 19, 343–370. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868314564789
Cuddy, A. J. C., Wolf, E. B., Glick, P., Crotty, S., Chong, J., & Norton, M. I. (2015). Men as
cultural ideals: Cultural values moderate gender stereotype content. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 109, 622–635. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000027
Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., & Steele, C. M. (2005). Clearing the air: Identity safety moderates
the effects of stereotype threat on women’s leadership aspirations. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 88, 276–287. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.276
Implications for Gender 113
Deaux, K., & Lewis, L. L. (1984). Structure of gender stereotypes: Interrelationships among
components and gender label. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 991.
Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: Women and
men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1171–
1188. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167200262001
Donnelly, K., & Twenge, J. M. (2017). Masculine and feminine traits on the Bem Sex-Role
Inventory, 1993–2012: A cross-temporal meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 76, 556–565. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0625-y
Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female lead-
ers. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573
Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of lead-
ers: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 3–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0090375
Eagly, A. H., & Mladinic, A. (1994). Are people prejudiced against women? Some answers
from research on attitudes, gender stereotypes, and judgments of competence. European
Review of Social Psychology, 5, 1–35. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792779543000002
European Commission. (2017). Smarter, greener, more inclusive? Indicators to support the Europe 2020
strategy (Statistical Books No. doi: 10.2785/760192). Luxembourg. Retrieved from Euro-
stat: http://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/en/web/products-statistical-books/-/KS-EZ-17-001
Evers, A., & Sieverding, M. (2014). Why do highly qualified women (still) earn less? Gen-
der differences in long-term predictors of career success. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
38, 93–106. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684313498071
Ghavami, N., & Peplau, L. A. (2013). An intersectional analysis of gender and ethnic ste-
reotypes: Testing three hypotheses. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37, 113–127. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0361684312464203
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117,
21–38.
Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the implicit association test to measure
self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1022–1038.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022
Haines, E. L., Deaux, K., & Lofaro, N. (2016). The times they are a-changing ... or are they
not? A comparison of gender stereotypes, 1983–2014. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40,
353–363. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684316634081
Haynes, M. C., & Heilman, M. E. (2013). It had to be you (not me)!: Women’s attributional
rationalization of their contribution to successful joint work outcomes. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 956–969. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213486358
Heilman, M. E. (1983). Sex bias in work settings: The lack of fit model. Research in Orga-
nizational Behavior, 5, 269–298.
Heilman, M. E. (2012). Gender stereotypes and workplace bias. Research in Organizational
Behavior, 32, 113–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.riob.2012.11.003
Heilman, M. E., Block, C. J., & Martell, R. F. (1995). Sex stereotypes: Do they influence
perceptions of managers? Journal of Social Behavior, 10, 237–252.
Heilman, M. E., & Wallen, A. S. (2010). Wimpy and undeserving of respect: Penalties for
men’s gender-inconsistent success. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 664–667.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.01.008
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? The
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–135. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
Hoyt, C. L., & Murphy, S. E. (2016). Managing to clear the air: Stereotype threat, women, and
leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 27, 387–399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.11.002
114 Sabine Sczesny et al.
Janssens, A. (1997). The rise and decline of the male breadwinner family? An overview
of the debate. International Review of Social History, 42, 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1017/
S0020859000114774
Jussim, L. (2012). Social perception and social reality: Why accuracy dominates bias and self-
fulfilling prophecy. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Koch, A. J., D’Mello, S. D., & Sackett, P. R. (2015). A meta-analysis of gender stereotypes
and bias in experimental simulations of employment decision making. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 100, 128–161. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036734
Koenig, A. M., & Eagly, A. H. (2005). Stereotype threat in men on a test of social sensitiv-
ity. Sex Roles, 52, 489–496. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-3714-x
Koenig, A. M., & Eagly, A. H. (2014). Evidence for the social role theory of stereotype
content: Observations of groups’ roles shape stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 107, 371–392. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037215
Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., & Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereo-
types masculine? A meta-analysis of three research paradigms. Psychological Bulletin,
137, 616–642. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023557
Kosakowska-Berezecka, N., Besta, T., Adamska, K., Jaśkiewicz, M., Jurek, P., & Vandello,
J. A. (2016). If my masculinity is threatened I won’t support gender equality? The role of
agentic self-stereotyping in restoration of manhood and perception of gender relations.
Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 17, 274–284. https://doi.org/10.1037/men0000016
Leslie, L. M., Manchester, C. F., & Dahm, P. C. (2017). Why and when does the gender
gap reverse? Diversity goals and the pay premium for high potential women. Academy
of Management Journal, 60, 402–432. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2015.0195
Levanon, A., & Grusky, D. B. (2016). The persistence of extreme gender segregation in the
twenty-first century. American Journal of Sociology, 122, 573–619.
Lippa, R. A., Preston, K., & Penner, J. (2014). Women’s representation in 60 occupations
from 1972 to 2010: More women in high-status jobs, few women in things-oriented
jobs. PLOS One, 9, e95960. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095960
Livingston, R. W., Rosette, A. S., & Washington, E. F. (2012). Can an agentic Black woman
get ahead? The impact of race and interpersonal dominance on perceptions of female
leaders. Psychological Science, 23, 354–358. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611428079
Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1992). A collective self-esteem scale: Self-evaluation of one’s
social identity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 302–318.
Maass, A., Cadinu, M., Guarnieri, G., & Grasselli, A. (2003). Sexual harassment under
social identity threat: The computer harassment paradigm. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 85, 853–870. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.853
Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2010). Patterns of gender development. Annual Review of
Psychology, 61, 353–381. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100511
Moss-Racusin, C. A., & Johnson, E. R. (2016). Backlash against male elementary educa-
tors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 379–393. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12366
National Research Council. (2010). Gender differences at critical transitions in the careers of sci-
ence, engineering, and mathematics faculty. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
Nentwich, J. C., Poppen, W., Schälin, S., & Vogt, F. (2013). The same and the other: Male
childcare workers managing identity dissonance. International Review of Sociology, 23,
326–345. https://doi.org/10.1080/03906701.2013.804295
Pew Research Center. (2015). Women and leadership: Public says women are equally quali-
fied, but barriers persist. Washington, DC. Retrieved from Pew Research Center: www.
pewsocialtrends.org/2015/01/14/women-and-leadership
Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are
allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes.
Implications for Gender 115
Williams, M. J., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2016). The subtle suspension of backlash: A meta-
analysis of penalties for women’s implicit and explicit dominance behavior. Psychological
Bulletin, 142, 165–197. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000039
Williams, W. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2015). National hiring experiments reveal 2: 1 faculty pref-
erence for women on STEM tenure track. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
112, 5360–5365. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1418878112
Wood, W., Christensen, P. N., Hebl, M. R., & Rothgerber, H. (1997). Conformity to sex-
typed norms, affect, and the self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,
523–535. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.523
Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2012). Biosocial construction of sex differences and similarities
in behavior. In J. M. Olson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychol-
ogy (Vol. 46, pp. 55–123). Burlington: Academic Press.
Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2015). Two traditions of research on gender identity. Sex Roles,
73, 461–473. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-015-0480-2
10
DIMENSIONAL COMPARISON
THEORY AND THE AGENCY-
COMMUNION FRAMEWORK
Friederike Helm and Jens Möller
The present chapter is concerned with the formation of agentic and communal self-
concepts. Agentic and communal terms are central to self-perception (e.g., Abele &
Wojciszke, 2007; Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007; Judd, James-Hawkins, Yzerbyt, &
Kashima, 2005; Wojciszke, 2005). For instance, Diehl, Owen, and Youngblade
(2004) showed that 99% of adults’ self-descriptions could be categorized as agentic
or communal terms. Uchronski (2008) similarly found that 74% of the partici-
pants’ self-descriptions could be categorized as communal or agentic (the remain-
ing referred to affective states). The most frequently stated term in the communion
area was “helpful,” and in the agency area was “ambitious.” Herzog and Markus
(1999) presented their participants with a list of 44 attributes gathered from free
self-descriptions, and found that certain attributes were considered important by
a majority of their participants: these were agentic (e.g., “to be independent”) and
communal characteristics (e.g., “to be a good friend”).
Whereas agency and communion are positively associated in the perception of
others (Wojciszke & Abele, 2008; Wojciszke, Abele, & Baryla, 2009), they are more
or less uncorrelated in the perception of the self (Abele, 2003; Abele & Spurk, 2011;
Uchronski, 2008).
What is the reason for these different relations of agency and communion in self-
perception versus the perception of others? Dimensional comparison theory (DCT;
Möller & Marsh, 2013) might provide an answer. After introducing DCT and respec-
tive findings we will apply this theory to the agency/communion area. Finally, we
will outline some ideas for further research on the application of assumptions of the
DCT to the formation of agentic and communal self-perceptions.
reference model (I/E model; e.g., Marsh, 1986; Möller & Köller, 2001), examining
the formation of academic ability self-perceptions. The I/E model describes the
joint operation of social and dimensional comparison processes in the formation
of verbal and mathematical self-concepts. Social comparisons are conducted in an
external frame of reference when students compare their achievement with the
achievements of their classmates. For example, if a student’s math achievement is
better than that of his/her classmates, this student’s math self-concept is also likely
to be higher. Dimensional comparisons are conducted in an internal frame of ref-
erence when students compare their achievement in a given subject with that in
another subject. For example, a student might compare his/her verbal versus math
achievement. If the verbal achievement is better than the math, verbal self-concept
will increase and math self-concept will decrease, as a result of dimensional com-
parison. In the process of making dimensional comparisons between the verbal
and the math domains, students seem to emphasize their achievement differences.
It follows that the self-concept in both domains is less closely associated than is the
achievement in both domains.
Möller, Pohlmann, Köller, and Marsh (2009), in a meta-analysis of 69 studies,
have confirmed the assumptions of the I/E model: positive effects of achievement
on the self-concept in the corresponding domain as a result of social comparisons,
and negative effects of achievement on non-corresponding self-concept as a result of
dimensional comparisons (see Figure 10.1). The average correlation between math
and verbal achievements was positive and strong (r = .67), and much higher than
the average correlation between math and verbal self-concepts (r = .10). The two
horizontal paths relating math achievement to math self-concept (.61) and verbal
achievement to verbal self-concept (.49) were substantial and positive – a result of
social comparison processes. The two cross paths leading from verbal achievement
to math self-concept (− .27) and mathematics achievement to verbal self-concept
(− .21) were negative – a result of dimensional comparison processes. Moderator
.61
MAch MSelf
7
-.2
.67 .10
-.2
1
VAch VSelf
.49
FIGURE 10.1 The I/E model: results of a meta-analytic path analysis of the relations
between math and verbal achievement and math and verbal self-concept (from Möller
et al., 2009). MAch = math achievement; MSelf = math self-concept; VAch = verbal
achievement; VSelf = verbal self-concept.
Dimensional Comparison Theory 119
p < .01). As well, these correlations for other-ratings of agency and communion
were significantly higher than correlations for self-ratings (r = 0.10, p < .05; peer/
self: z = 2.03, p < .05; teacher/self: z = 2.26, p = .01).
Using structural equation modeling, testing the assumptions of the DCT model
for agency and communion, positive paths emerged from peer- and teacher-ratings
to corresponding self-ratings (see Figure 10.2), indicating social comparison effects:
a student perceiving that his/her peers ascribe a higher level of agency/communion
to him or her will develop a more positive self-concept in the agentic/communal
area. Most importantly for the application of DCT to agency and communion,
negative paths from peer- and teacher-ratings of agency/communion to self-ratings
of communion/agency were shown, indicating dimensional comparison effects.
More positive other-perceptions of a student’s agency/communion resulted in less
positive student self-perception of communion/agency. Thus, students on the one
hand seem to make inferences from peer- and teacher-perceptions of their agentic
and communal characteristics to their self-perception in the agentic and communal
area, e.g., social comparisons. On the other hand, there are also hints on the pres-
ence of contrastive dimensional comparisons, as there were negative associations
between other-ratings and self-ratings in the non-corresponding domain. Accord-
ingly, contrastive dimensional comparisons might be one reason for the lower
agency/communion correlation in self-perception than in other-perception.
In a second field study, Helm and Möller (in prep.) assessed agentic and com-
munal self-assessments and teacher-ratings at two times during a school year
(beginning of the school year [t1]) and mid-term [t2]) and regressed self- and
other-ratings at t2 on self- and other-ratings at t1. In line with the assumptions,
there were positive effects of self-perception at t1 on self-perception at t2 and of
other-ratings at t1 on other-ratings at t2 (see Figure 10.3), indicating a certain sta-
bility of self- and other-perception. Secondly, there was a positive effect of agen-
tic other-perception at t1 on agentic self-perception at t2. The positive effect of
.42**/.45**
Peer/Teacher: Self:
Agency Agency
–.14*/–.13* –.21**/–.07
Peer/Teacher: Self:
Communion Communion
.34/.24**
FIGURE 10.2 Path model for the relationships between peer-ratings (before the slash)
and teacher-ratings (after the slash) of students’ agentic and communal traits and
students’ self-ratings; *p < .05; **p < .01.
Dimensional Comparison Theory 121
.22**
Teacher: Agency t1 Self: Agency t2
.49**
–.10*
.40** .45**
.76**
.34** Self: Agency t1 Teacher: Agency t2 .17**
.12** .12*
–.19**
FIGURE 10.3 Path model for the relationships between teacher-ratings of students’
agentic and communal traits and students’ self-ratings at t1 and t2; non-significant
paths are shown in gray, dashed lines. +p < .06; *p < .05; **p < .01.
and complete more tests. Low self-esteem participants showed the opposite effect.
Hence, Brown and Smart’s (1991) studies suggest contrastive dimensional com-
parisons between agency and communion, but only for high self-esteem partici-
pants. The authors interpret this finding with the assumption that people with a
high self-esteem protect their general positive self-image after failure feedback by
emphasizing other positive qualities. In another experimental study, Möller and
Savyon (2003) showed dimensional comparison effects of feedback about intel-
ligence on honesty self-perception. Participants received feedback on anagram
tasks, indicating that they had solved a low or a high percentage of these tasks.
As expected, participants in the failure condition rated themselves as being more
honest than did students who received positive feedback on the anagram tasks.
Helm et al. (2017) conducted additional experimental studies demonstrating
the effects of agency feedback on communal self-perception. In one study, these
authors showed that communal self-perception was lower in the group with high
agency feedback than in the group with low agency feedback. In the other study,
feedback on communal tasks was given and the effect on the agentic self-concept
was measured. Agentic self-perception was lower in the group with high com-
munion feedback than in the group with low communion feedback. All these
experiments support the conclusion drawn from the field studies, that contrastive
dimensional comparisons between feedback in these two areas may be at work
when self-perception in agency and communion is assessed.1
Conclusion
The present chapter showed that DCT is a promising theoretical approach not only
in the field of educational psychology and the formation of academic self-concepts,
but also in the field of social psychology and the formation of self-concepts on
the Big Two of agency and communion. Dimensional comparisons might be the
mechanism that explains why agency and communion are uncorrelated in indi-
viduals’ self-ratings, whereas they are correlated in the ratings of other persons.
Besides emphasizing the general importance of dimensional comparisons in
forming one’s agency- and communion-related self-concept, the present findings
also suggest that individuals (shown here for students) develop their agentic and
communal self-perceptions partly as a result of the feedback they get from peers
and teachers. Positive feedback in one domain might have negative consequences
for self-perception in the other domain instigated by dimensional comparison.
Moreover, self-perceptions in the two areas might have consequences for sub-
sequent self-perceptions in the non-corresponding area, leading to the cement-
ing of already existing over-accentuated differences in agentic and communal
self-perceptions.
The question of why and under what circumstances contrastive dimensional
comparisons between agency and communion take place in the formation of self-
concepts in the two areas is worthwhile to pursue further. Such research questions
could be answered by introspective studies like the diary studies by Möller and
Dimensional Comparison Theory 123
Husemann (2006). These authors asked their participants to record the dimensional
comparisons that they carried out in everyday life and the situations in which these
comparisons were carried out. Furthermore, motivations for dimensional compari-
sons between agency and communion might be examined in experimental studies
manipulating participants’ motivation and then giving them the opportunity to
conduct different kinds of comparisons between different areas.
Longitudinal studies could analyze the development of the relation between
agentic and communal self-perceptions over the life span. DCT suggests that one
main motivation for dimensional comparisons is the self-differentiation moti-
vation, that is, the motivation to establish a differentiated picture of one’s own
strengths and weaknesses. This motivation can be assumed to be especially strong
in adolescence, as a central task in this life stage is the preparation of important life
decisions, for example, which career path to follow. A differentiated self-concept
about one’s own relative strengths and weaknesses can be assumed to be helpful
for such decisions. Agency and communion, as outlined above, might be of crucial
psychological significance in this stage as well, as they capture two central recur-
ring challenges of human life: pursuing individual goals and belonging to social
groups (Ybarra et al., 2008). Hence, a differentiated self-concept in these two
personality areas is likely to be of high importance for the management of central
life tasks, and contrastive dimensional comparisons between the two areas might
be especially pronounced in adolescence. Yet, as Abele and Wojciszke (2014) state:
“The two life tasks or challenges are, of course, not independent. It is important
for agency to be mitigated by communion and vice versa” (p. 9). Accordingly,
later in life, a central task might be the integration of both self-concept domains.
Research on the association between agentic and communal self-perception in dif-
ferent age groups could answer the question if an over-accentuation of differences
in one’s own agentic and communal qualities is more pronounced in certain life
stages than in others.
Another question regarding dimensional comparisons between agency and
communion is whether this comparison type only occurs in the formation of
self-perceptions, or whether the formation of other-perceptions is influenced by
dimensional comparisons as well. The DCT, as outlined, on the one hand assumes
that dimensional comparisons are motivated by the need to form a picture of one’s
own strengths and difficulties, in order to facilitate decision making. This moti-
vation seems to be relevant to the formation of self-concepts rather than to the
formation of impressions of others. Yet, DCT moreover assumes that dimensional
comparisons under certain conditions are carried out when people evaluate the
warmth and competence of groups or other people, an assumption supported by
various studies (see Yzerbyt, this volume). Yet, for evaluations of other people, dif-
ferent motivations guiding the comparison processes leading to these evaluations
can be assumed than for self-evaluations. Comparison processes in the formation
of self- and other-evaluations in the agentic and the communal area and possibly
diverging conditions under which these comparisons are carried out should be
examined further.
124 Friederike Helm and Jens Möller
Note
1 There is only one study that does not fit this pattern: Abele, Rupprecht, and Wojciszke
(2008) gave positive or negative feedback on anagram tasks and measured self-perception
of agency and communion both before and after task completion. These authors found
that agency became lower after failure feedback, but communion remained unchanged.
References
Abele, A. E. (2003). The dynamics of masculine-agentic and feminine-communal traits:
Findings from a prospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(4),
768–776.
Abele, A. E., Rupprecht, T., & Wojciszke, B. (2008). The influence of success and failure
experiences on agency. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 436–448.
Abele, A. E., & Spurk, D. (2011). The dual impact of gender and the influence of timing
of parenthood on men’s and women’s career development: Longitudinal findings. Inter-
national Journal of Behavioral Development, 35, 225–232.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self
versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 751–763.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content: A dual perspective
model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195–255.
Brown, J. D., & Smart, S. (1991). The self and social conduct: Linking self-representations
to prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 (3), 368–375.
Diehl, M., Owen, S., & Youngblade, L. (2004). Agency and communion attributes in
adults’ spontaneous self-representations. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
28, 1–15.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition:
Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 77–83.
Helm, F., Abele, A. E., Müller-Kalthoff, H., & Möller, J. (2017). Applying dimensional
comparison theory to the fundamental dimensions of social judgment: Agency and
communion. Learning and Individual Differences, 54, 116–125.
Herzog, A. R., & Markus, H. R. (1999). The self-concept in life span and aging research.
In Handbook of theories of aging (pp. 227–252). New York, NY: Springer Publishing Co.
Judd, C. M., James-Hawkins, L., Yzerbyt, V., & Kashima, Y. (2005). Fundamental dimen-
sions of social judgment: Understanding the relations between judgments of compe-
tence and warmth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 899–913.
Marsh, H. W. (1986). Verbal and math self-concepts: An internal/external frame of refer-
ence model. American Educational Research Journal, 23, 129–149.
Marsh, H. W., & Köller, O. (2004). Unification of theoretical models of academic self-
concept/achievement relations: Reunification of east and west German school systems
after the fall of the Berlin Wall. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 264–282.
Marsh, H. W., Kong, C. K., & Hau, K. T. (2001). Extension of the internal/external frame
of reference model of self-concept formation: Importance of native and nonnative lan-
guages for Chinese students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 543–553.
Möller, J., & Husemann, N. (2006). Internal comparisons in everyday life. Journal of Edu-
cational Psychology, 98 (2), 342.
Möller, J., & Köller, O. (2001). Dimensional comparisons: An experimental approach to
the internal/external frame of reference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(4),
826–835.
Möller, J., & Marsh, H. W. (2013). Dimensional comparison theory. Psychological Review,
120 (3), 544–560.
Dimensional Comparison Theory 125
Möller, J., Müller-Kalthoff, H., Helm, F., Nagy, N., & Marsh, H. W. (2016). The general-
ized internal/external frame of reference model: An extension to dimensional compari-
son theory. Frontline Learning Research, 4 (2), 1–11.
Möller, J., Pohlmann, B., Köller, O., & Marsh, H. W. (2009). A meta-analytic path analysis
of the internal/external frame of reference model of academic achievement and aca-
demic self-concept. Review of Educational Research, 79 (3), 1129–1167.
Möller, J., Retelsdorf, J., Köller, O., & Marsh, H. W. (2011). The reciprocal internal/
external frame of reference model: An integration of models of relations between
academic achievement and self-concept. American Educational Research Journal, 48(6),
1315–1346.
Möller, J., & Savyon, K. (2003). Not very smart, thus moral: Dimensional comparisons
between academic self-concept and honesty. Social Psychology of Education, 6 (2), 95–106.
Strickhouser, J. E., & Zell, E. (2015). Self-evaluative effects of dimensional and social com-
parison. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 59, 60–66.
Uchronski, M. (2008). Agency and communion in spontaneous self-descriptions: Occur-
rence and situational malleability. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1093–1102.
Wojciszke, B. (2005). Morality and competence in person-and self-perception. European
Review of Social Psychology, 16 (1), 155–188.
Wojciszke, B., & Abele, A. E. (2008). The primacy of communion over agency and its
reversals in evaluations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1139–1147.
Wojciszke, B., Abele, A. E., & Baryla, W. (2009). Two dimensions of attitudes: Liking
depends on communion, respect depends on agency. European Journal of Social Psychol-
ogy, 39, 973–990.
Ybarra, O., Chan, E., Park, H., Burnstein, E., Monin, B., & Stanik, C. (2008). Life’s recur-
ring challenges and the fundamental dimensions: An integration and its implications for
cultural differences and similarities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1083–1092.
11
THE DIMENSIONAL
COMPENSATION MODEL
Reality and strategic constraints on warmth
and competence in intergroup perceptions
Vincent Yzerbyt
as more recent contributions (Phalet & Poppe, 1997) on the issue of attitudes and
stereotypes. This model offers a rich account of the antecedents and consequences
of the specific views that social perceivers form about the people and groups that
comprise their social world. Indeed, a central tenet is that two structural dimen-
sions characterize social relations. First, people and groups differ in the extent to
which they possess status, power, and resources. Second, people and groups also
cooperate or compete with each other. These two unmistakable features of social
interactions constrain the way perceivers form their impressions of groups and
group members. In turn, the latter shape people’s affective reactions and orient
their behaviors ( Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2008).
Clearly, the two structural aspects of competition/cooperation and resources
orchestrate people’s views at the psychological level. On the one hand, the assumed
intentions of the target (do the members of this group mean well, does this social
target harbor positive goals?) translate into judgments of warmth/communion.
On the other, the power and resources believed to characterize the target (is this
group in a position to make its intentions come true, given the goals of this social
target, are the necessary means available?) convert into judgments of competence/
agency. Importantly, whereas earlier work on social perception stressed the impor-
tance of evaluative consistency, with judgments falling by and large on a single
dimension ranging from bad to good, SCM researchers expected and repeatedly
found that these two dimensions are orthogonal and form a bidimensional space
crossing low to high competence and low to high warmth.
In spite of the wide acclaim of the SCM and the impressive amount of support-
ive evidence (Fiske, 2015; Fiske, this volume), even SCM researchers note that a
substantial number of the groups tend to fall in the ambivalent quadrants, i.e., the
high-competence-low-warmth quadrant and the low-competence-high-warmth
one (Durante et al., 2013, 2017). Moreover, experimental work by SCM scholars
on such specific groups as career women (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2004) and old
people ( Cuddy, Norton, & Fiske, 2005) suggest that ambivalence is often the pair
of glasses through which perceivers appraise particular group members. Finally,
the related work on ambivalent sexism conducted by Glick and Fiske (1996) reveals
that hostile and benevolent sexism largely portray women in terms that correspond
to these two quadrants. Hostile sexism has it that women are skilled, yet sly and
ill-intentioned creatures, globally tempting and using men to take advantage of
them. In contrast, benevolent sexism conceives of women as adorable yet fragile
people, worthy of love but not quite able to navigate the social world. In sum, the
orthogonality of the fundamental dimensions of warmth and competence may
well apply as a rule, but a negative relation tends to emerge between the two fun-
damental dimensions whenever one focuses on perceptions of particular pairs of
groups in the context of actual social interactions.
Turning to the intergroup literature, a seemingly different account emerges
with respect to the origins and functions of stereotypes. According to Social Iden-
tity Theory (SIT; Abrams & Hogg, 1988; Tajfel & Turner, 1979), stereotypes serve
to explain, rationalize, and justify the social world as well as intergroup behavior.
128 Vincent Yzerbyt
That is, stereotypes not only account for the nature of people and their relations,
but they also serve a series of motives (Yzerbyt & Corneille, 2005). This is because
people derive their sense of worth from their membership into social groups. In
other words, social perceivers’ needs in terms of self-regard are satisfied to the
extent that they belong to groups that come across as valuable, preferably better
than other groups. This search for positive distinctiveness is believed to account for
many of the biases that materialize in stereotypes, prejudice, and discriminatory
behaviors in the real world (for a review, see Yzerbyt & Demoulin, 2010).
To be sure, people’s love for their group does not necessarily mean that they
will consider their ingroup superior to other groups on all counts. Doing so would
not only be delusional, but probably counterproductive as well. In fact, reflecting
on the specific measurement tools mobilized by the social identity researchers,
Mummendey and Schreiber (1983) conjectured that ingroup bias may seem to
emerge inevitably because group members face only one judgment dimension on
which they can differentiate between the ingroup and the outgroup. After all, this
methodological option mimics the realistic conflict theory setting imagined by
Sherif (1966) and it is thus hardly surprising that groups are bound to compete, if
only symbolically, in order to secure a dominant position in such a situation. Inter-
estingly, these authors show that, when a more varied set of dimensions is available
for evaluation purposes, group members mobilize only a subset of characteristics
to affirm their superiority. Establishing ingroup bias on some selected dimension
would seem to give the possibility to bear with outgroup bias on other, less crucial,
dimensions. Upon scrutiny, the criteria chosen to materialize the dominant posi-
tion of one’s group are far from being indifferent. To be sure, reality constraints
are entering the picture and group members on both sides of the fence are likely to
consider them, but the real question is how these checks translate into judgments.
Are there lawful connections between “objective” aspects of the intergroup situa-
tion and the more subjective understanding of the groups and people in presence?
And what are the factors that modulate the resulting picture? SIT remains mostly
silent about these questions. As the next section shows, this is where the work on
the Stereotype Content Model (SCM: Fiske et al., 2002) comes in handy.
6.00
5.00
4.00
French judging French judging Belgians judging Belgians judging
French Belgians French Belgians
FIGURE 11.1 Competence and warmth of French and Belgians targets for French and
Belgian judges.
6.00
Competence
5.00
Warmth
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
–1.00
High judging high High judging low Low judging high Low judging low
–2.00
FIGURE 11.2 Competence and warmth rating of high and low targets for high and
low judges.
The Dimensional Compensation Model 131
e.g., competence. Whereas the majority of these (6) were positive for one of the
two groups, they were negative for the other. As for the remaining behaviors, they
were either neutral on both dimensions (4) or pertained to the other dimension,
half of them (2) being positive and half (2) negative. Next, participants sorted
the cards according to groups, read all sorted behaviors again and, to encourage
impression formation, wrote a short text about each group. Finally, they rated
both groups on four scales that measured competence (capable, skilled, lazy, dis-
organized) and four that tapped warmth (sociable, caring, insensitive, unfriendly).
As expected, participants noticed the built-in difference between the two
groups, whether competence or warmth was manipulated. More importantly, they
compensated on the other dimension. Thus, even when perceivers contemplate
unknown groups, their judgments reveal a negative relation between competence
and warmth. Compensation even showed up in negative correlations between
the group differences on the two dimensions, that is, the larger the perceived dif-
ference between the groups on the manipulated dimension, the larger the perceived
difference between them on the other dimension in the opposite direction. Along
with several others (Judd et al., 2005; Kervyn, Yzerbyt, Judd, & Nunes, 2009;
Yzerbyt, Kervyn, & Judd, 2008), this experiment clearly demonstrates perceivers’
propensity to imbue a group that is more competent (warm) than another with
comparatively less warmth (competence).
Contrary to Yzerbyt et al.’s (2005) initial study, the above participants did not
belong to either the Blue or the Green group. A follow-up experiment (Judd et al.,
2005, Expt. 5) examined this issue using a minimal group paradigm. Participants
learned that their profile on an initial perceptual task made them members of
the high- or the low-competence group before receiving the group information.
Compensation emerged but remarkably, and replicating Yzerbyt et al.’s earlier
findings, participants of each group enhanced the positive regard in which they
held their own group relative to their regard for the other group (see Figure 11.2).
That is, compared to the members of the low-competence group, the members
of the high-competence group saw more of a difference in competence between
the two groups and less of a difference in warmth. Still, even they were unable to
deny the compensation as they acknowledged that their own group might be less
warm than the other group. These judgments emerge even though the objective
information provided to participants indicates that both groups are comparable on
the second dimension.
Consequences of compensation
Compensation emerges in surprisingly diverse settings, sometimes with non-trivial
and even counterintuitive consequences (for reviews, see Kervyn, Yzerbyt, &
Judd, 2010; Yzerbyt, 2016). One illustration of the startling impact of compensa-
tory judgments is how they guide information-gathering strategies (Kervyn et al.,
2009). Indeed, a most striking phenomenon in social perception concerns percep-
tual and behavioral confirmation ( Snyder, 1984): perceivers are particularly adept at
132 Vincent Yzerbyt
verifying their prior views of others. In particular, they shape other people’s behav-
ior and have them support their favored conclusions ( Snyder, 1984), a phenomenon
known as self-fulfilling prophecy. Interestingly, research has always stressed that
perceivers notice information and generate hypotheses of a similar valence, leading
to a halo confirmation effect.
We wanted to see whether compensation instead could materialize in the judg-
ments and behaviors of the target people. In a first experiment (Kervyn et al.,
2009, Expt. 1), participants underwent the Judd et al. (2005) manipulation with
one additional twist. Specifically, participants received a list of questions and had
to select those they found most useful in gaining further information. For the
questions pertaining to the manipulated dimension, participants selected the ones
implying the high (low) end of the dimension to be asked to the high (low) group.
More interestingly, they also selected questions manifesting a compensatory pat-
tern on the unmanipulated dimension. A second experiment (Kervyn et al., 2009,
Expt. 2) looked at the bias in the answers made available as a result. Pretest par-
ticipants answered the questions selected in Experiment 1 and experimental par-
ticipants received their answers. Some read the answers to 10 questions most often
selected for the high-competence group and the answers to the 10 questions most
often selected for the low-competence group. Others read the answers to the ques-
tions posed to the high and the low warmth groups. In both cases, the group
impressions formed by experimental participants revealed compensation on the
other dimension.
In a final experiment (Kervyn et al., 2009, Expt. 3), we tested the viability
of this behavioral confirmation process in actual interactions. We invited three
participants at a time to the lab for an interview scenario. Two of the participants,
the interviewees, were made to believe that they were each member of one of two
groups while the third, the interviewer, asked a series of 20 questions. The 10 ques-
tions selected most often for the high-competence (warmth) group were posed to
the corresponding group member and the 10 questions selected most often for the
low-competence (warmth) group were asked to the other interviewee. Participants
then rated the two interviewees and their groups. For interviewers, compensation
emerged whether the judgments concerned the interviewees or their groups. For
the interviewees, compensation materialized in the ratings of the groups and of
the other interviewee (only when the manipulation concerned competence). Only
self-ratings failed to show compensation. In sum, compensation not only shapes
social perception but it also constrains behaviors, even shaping the views of the
group members about the groups in presence. Clearly, thus, compensation affects
people’s judgments in a wide variety of ways (for a review, see Kervyn et al., 2010).
in isolation or when more than two targets are considered. We tested this idea
by having participants evaluate only one group, either the high-competence or
the low-competence group, and rate the group on both competence and warmth
(Judd et al., 2005, Expt. 4). Interestingly, the high-competence group ended up
being judged slightly warmer than the low-competence group. There was thus no
evidence of compensation and even a tendency for a halo effect. These data not-
withstanding, it is often difficult to avoid comparison in the social domain. The
above pattern emerged in a study where respondents learned about a new group in
a rather decontextualized setting. As the abundant literature on social comparison
stresses, social perceivers are prone to compare any social target with others, in
particular when they are themselves members of one of the groups. In short, halo
is hard to obtain when comparison concerns intrude the situation.
Another remarkable feature of the situations examined in early compensation
work has to do with the gap between the groups on one of the two dimensions as
well as the absence of conflict. We decided to investigate the impact of these two
factors more systematically in a series of studies (Cambon, Yzerbyt, & Yakimova,
2015; Cambon & Yzerbyt, 2016). Using a minimal group paradigm ( Cambon
et al., 2015, Expt. 1), we had groups of four to six participants fill in a bogus
personality test measuring either their competence or their warmth. In the asym-
metrical conditions, one-half of the group members received high scores and the
remaining ones low scores on the manipulated dimension and were then assigned
to two different subgroups on the basis of this score. In the symmetrical condi-
tions, all participants received either a high or a low score on the manipulated
dimension and joined the two groups on a random basis. Subgroups then went to
separate rooms and filled in some questionnaire. The topic of this questionnaire
allowed manipulating the level of symbolic conflict by telling the subgroup mem-
bers that the other subgroup in the session had either the same or a different view
on the topic. Participants then rated their subgroup and the other subgroup on
both dimensions. Depending on conditions, the manipulated dimension was thus
the ingroup’s preferred dimension, i.e., the ingroup allegedly scoring high on this
dimension, or the outgroup’s preferred dimension, i.e., the ingroup allegedly scor-
ing low on this dimension. As predicted, compensation emerged only when there
was a clear difference on the manipulated dimension between the two groups and
conflict was absent (see Figure 11.3, left panel). At the other extreme, the lack of
initial difference and the presence of conflict led participants to express strong
ingroup bias on both dimensions (see Figure 11.3, right panel).
A second experiment ( Cambon et al., 2015, Expt. 1) turned to existing groups
and had psychology students evaluate their ingroup as well as a very superior
(medical students), a superior (economy students), an equal (sociology students),
an inferior (special education students), or a very inferior (auxiliary nurses) out-
group. We also manipulated the level of conflict. Rather than a symbolic conflict,
we relied on realistic threat. The outgroup department was or was not likely to
move and occupy the psychology building, one of the nicest buildings on campus.
In line with predictions (see Figure 11.4), compensation emerged more strongly
134 Vincent Yzerbyt
7
Ingroup
6
Outgroup
5
1
Ingroup preferred Outgroup preferred Ingroup preferred Outgroup preferred
No Conflict and Difference Conflict and No Difference
FIGURE 11.3 Ratings of ingroup and outgroup on ingroup and outgroup preferred
dimension as a function of conflict and intergroup difference.
5
Low conflict
4
High conflict
3
2
1
0
–1
–2
–3
–4
–5
Very superior Superior Equal Inferior Very inferior
feedback regarding their status) makes them more sensitive to the norms toward
(non-)discrimination. Such data go a long way to confirm that sensitivity to ambi-
ent norms may come as a viable reason for high-status group members to compen-
sate, a pattern known as “noblesse oblige” (Vanbeselaere, Boen, Van Avermaet, &
Buelens, 2006).
What about the low-status group members? In all likelihood, the key here is
again the search for positive distinctiveness. However, their outgroup bias on com-
petence can hardly rests on magnanimity. In contrast, it is their ingroup bias on
warmth that ought to be seen as a direct response to their predicament. Given the
social hierarchy in terms of prestige, status, and resources, promoting one’s group
on warmth is the safest way to ensure a comparative edge to the members of the
low-status group. Building upon several lines of research (Abrams & Hogg, 1988;
Lemyre & Smith, 1985), we would argue that self-esteem is at the heart of the pro-
cess for low-status groups. Interestingly, if the dividends of compensation reside
in the ability to secure positive self-esteem, then it should be possible to reassure
people via alternative means. This rationale is consistent with several strands of
work (Becker, 2012; Derks, van Laar, & Ellemers, 2007; Fein & Spencer, 1997)
and holds that compensation serves an affirmation function. In our experiment,
we did or did not give participants the possibility to self-affirm before describ-
ing both groups (Cambon & Yzerbyt, 2018). We predicted that only participants
given the opportunity to self-affirm would not need to compensate, because the
affirmation manipulation would have boosted their self-esteem. We predicted that
these effects would take place only among low-status group members for two
reasons. First, the status feedback should not threaten high-status group members’
self-esteem, so the latter should not experience any need to restore it. Second,
high-status group members’ self-esteem is likely to be high, and there is evidence
that self-affirmation fails to affect high self-esteem participants. Our data fully
confirmed these predictions.
To sum up, the search for positive distinctiveness is a key aspect underlying
compensation in group members’ judgments. However, this search materializes in
very different ways for members of the high-status and of the low-status groups
essentially because of higher reality constraints for competence than for warmth.
Although high-status groups are tempted to favor the ingroup on all counts, they
are at least sensitive to social norms of non-discrimination. The story for low-
status group members is entirely different as they depend on favorable warmth
ratings to restore their threatened self-esteem.
Conclusions
When people evaluate their ingroup and some outgroup, they often avoid favoring
their ingroup on all counts. Instead of derogating the outgroup across the board,
group members are keen to select only certain aspects to secure their positive dis-
tinctiveness while conceding some value to the outgroup on other aspects. In the
present chapter, we relied on these two dimensions of warmth and competence
138 Vincent Yzerbyt
evidence by the SCM ( Fiske et al., 2002) to examine the way group members
appraise intergroup comparisons (Yzerbyt, 2016). Our research shows that the two
fundamental dimensions offer the perfect ground for this compromising posture
as members of real but also of minimal groups manifest a so-called compensation
pattern along competence and warmth. In other words, when a group is judged to
be better on the competence dimension than the other group, it also tends to be
rated as less warm. This robust pattern shapes group members’ judgments as well
as their behaviors (for a review, see Kervyn et al., 2010).
A very important message of our research is that the standing of the groups
cannot be interpreted as pointing to some kind of inherent characteristics. Rather,
group members modulate their stereotypes as a function of the specific com-
parison context, i.e., the status/power and the cooperation/competition relations
characterizing the groups in presence. Even more striking, our data show that
the presence of intergroup conflict and the illegitimacy of the relative status dif-
ference both disrupt compensation ( Cambon & Yzerbyt, 2016). In line with the
justification role of stereotypes stressed by Tajfel (1981), this finding doves nicely
with the idea that compensation is a form of intergroup perception that signals and
contributes to the status quo of the social hierarchy. Several recent efforts suggests
that ambivalence on the two dimensions emerges in the context of unequal social
systems (for a review, see Durante & Fiske, 2017) and is observed more in countries
characterized by moderate levels of conflict than in countries with very high or
very low levels of conflict (Durante et al., 2017). By directly manipulating conflict
levels and status differentials, our own work thus contributes to this literature by
offering unique insights with respect to causality.
Importantly, while group members’ goal in ingroup and outgroup seems to be
one of securing positive distinctiveness, the specific compensation pattern typi-
cally results from additional and indeed different concerns depending on whether
people belong to the high-status or the low-status group in the given context.
At the very least, normative pressures condemning discrimination are one set of
considerations leading the dominant groups to give up superiority on warmth. In
contrast, given the reality constraints of the status difference and the resulting dif-
ferentiation on competence, dominated groups find themselves tempted to claim
greater warmth in an attempt to enhance their self-esteem.
As it turns out, the dynamics of intergroup comparison whereby group members
may feel more or less constrained by their relative position on competence or warmth
is reminiscent of social comparison work in the interpersonal domain. In line with
the logic underlying our dimensional compensation model in intergroup contexts, a
compensation pattern would also be expected to emerge when individuals compare
themselves with each other. The confrontation with another person who is decid-
edly more competent or warmer is likely to have non-trivial consequences on how
people want to see themselves on the other dimension (see Chapter 10).
Although the dynamics of intergroup stereotyping are a complex matter, this
chapter shows that substantial progress is possible by bringing together such various
research traditions as social perception on the one hand and intergroup relations
The Dimensional Compensation Model 139
on the other. At the same time, several questions remain unanswered. For instance,
and in light of contemporary efforts stressing a series of nuances within each of
the fundamental dimensions (Abele, Cuddy, Judd, & Yzerbyt, 2008; Abele et al.,
2016), future research may want to consider whether compensation equally applies
to the sub-dimensions of competence/agency, i.e., skills and assertiveness, and
the sub-dimensions of warmth/communion, i.e., sociability and morality. Also,
the specific role of compensation in ongoing interactions between groups and
group members and its underlying mechanisms remain important topics for future
endeavors. Indeed, giving up warmth on the part of high-status groups does not
seem to derive solely from compliance with anti-discrimination norms but may
likely serve other purposes, in relation to the justification function of stereotypes.
In our view, a thorough understanding of the motives and contexts that prevail
in the formation of intergroup stereotypes constitutes a captivating item on the
research agenda.
References
Abele, A. E., Cuddy, A. J., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2008). Fundamental dimensions
of social judgment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1063–1065.
Abele, A. E., Hauke, N., Peters, K., Louvet, E., Szymkow, A., & Duan, Y. (2016). Facets
of the fundamental content dimensions: Agency with competence and assertiveness:
Communion with warmth and morality. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–17.
Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. (1988). Comments on the motivational status of self-esteem in
social identity and intergroup discrimination. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18,
317–334. DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2420180403
Becker, J. C. (2012). The system-stabilizing role of identity management strategies: Social
creativity can undermine collective action for social change. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 103, 647–662. DOI: 10.1037/a0029240
Cambon, L., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2016). Compensation is for real: Evidence from existing
groups in the context of actual relations. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations. DOI:
10.1177/1368430215625782
Cambon, L., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2018). Two routes toward compensation: An investigation
into the mechanisms of compensation for high-and low-status groups. Journal of Experi-
mental Social Psychology (in press).
Cambon, L., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Yakimova, S. (2015). Compensation in intergroup relations:
An investigation of its structural and strategic foundations. British Journal of Social Psy-
chology, 54, 140–158. DOI: 10.1111/bjso.12067
Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2004). When professionals become mothers,
warm doesn’t cut the ice. Journal of Social Issues, 60, 701–718.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal
dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS Map.
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 61–149.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Norton, M. I., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). This old stereotype: The stubbornness
and pervasiveness of the elderly stereotype. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 265–283.
Derks, B., Van Laar, C., & Ellemers, N. (2007). Social creativity strikes back: Improving
low status group members’ motivation and performance by valuing ingroup dimen-
sions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 470–493. DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.375
140 Vincent Yzerbyt
Durante, F., & Fiske, S. T. (2017). How social-class stereotypes maintain inequality. Current
Opinion in Psychology, 18, 43–48.
Durante, F., Fiske, S. T., Gelfand, M. J., Crippa, F., Suttora, C., Stillwell, A., ... Björklund, F.
(2017). Ambivalent stereotypes link to peace, conflict, and inequality across 38 nations.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 201611874.
Durante, F., Fiske, S. T., Kervyn, N., Cuddy, A. J., Akande, A. D., Adetoun, B. E., ...
Barlow, F. K. (2013). Nations’ income inequality predicts ambivalence in stereo-
type content: How societies mind the gap. British Journal of Social Psychology, 52 (4),
726–746.
Fein, S., & Spencer, S. J. (1997). Prejudice as self-image maintenance: Affirming the self
through derogating others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 31–44. DOI:
10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.31
Fiske, S. T. (2015). Intergroup biases: A focus on stereotype content. Current Opinion in
Behavioral Sciences, 3, 45–50. DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.010
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereo-
type content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and
competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory: Differentiating hostile
and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 491–512.
Jacoby, J., & Sassenberg, K. (2011). Interactions do not only tell us when, but can also tell
us how: Testing process hypotheses by interaction. European Journal of Social Psychology,
41, 180–190.
Judd, C. M., James-Hawkins, L. Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Kashima, Y. (2005). Fundamental
dimensions of social judgment: Understanding the relations between judgments of
competence and warmth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 899–913. DOI:
10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.899
Katz, D. & Braly, K. W. (1935). Racial prejudice and racial stereotypes. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 30, 175–193.
Kervyn, N., Yzerbyt, V. Y., Demoulin, S., & Judd, C. M. (2008). Competence and warmth
in context: The compensatory nature of stereotypic views of national groups. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1175–1183.
Kervyn, N., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Judd, C. M. (2010). Compensation between warmth and
competence: Antecedents and consequences of a negative relation between the two
fundamental dimensions of social perception. European Review of Social Psychology, 21,
155–187. DOI: 10.1080/13546805.2010.517997
Kervyn, N., Yzerbyt, V. Y., Judd, C. M., & Nunes, A. (2009). A question of compensation:
The social life of the fundamental dimensions of social perception. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 96, 828–842.
Lemaine, G. (1974). Social differentiation and social originality. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 4, 17–52. DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2420040103
Lemyre, L., & Smith, P. M. (1985). Intergroup discrimination and self-esteem in the mini-
mal group paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 660–670. DOI:
10.1037/0022-3514.49.3.660
Leyens, J.-P., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Schadron, G. (1994). Stereotypes and social cognition. London:
Sage Publications.
Mummendey, A., & Schreiber, H.-J. (1983). Better or just different? Positive social identity
by discrimination against, or by differentiation from outgroups. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 13, 389–397.
Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & Turner, J. C. (1994). Stereotyping and social reality. Oxford,
UK: Basil Blackwell.
The Dimensional Compensation Model 141
Owuamalam, C. K., Wong, K. X., & Rubin, M. (2016). Chubby but cheerful? Investigating
the compensatory judgments of high, medium, and low status weight groups in Malay-
sia. Cogent Psychology, 3, 1188441. DOI: 10.1080/23311908.2016.1188441
Phalet, K., & Poppe, E. (1997). Competence and morality dimensions of national and eth-
nic stereotypes: A study in six eastern-European countries. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 27(6), 703–723.
Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (2001). Responses to other-imposed pro-Black pressure:
Acceptance or backlash? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 486–501. DOI:
10.1006/jesp.2001.1478
Sherif, M. (1966). In common predicament: Social psychology of intergroup conflict and cooperation.
Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
Snyder, M. (1984). When belief creates reality. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experi-
mental social psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 248–306). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup relations. In W. G. Aus-
tin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The psychology of intergroup relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole.
Tausch, N., Kenworthy, J. B., & Hewstone, M. (2007). The confirmability and discon-
firmability of trait concepts revisited: Does content matter? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 92, 542–556. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.542
Vanbeselaere, N., Boen, F., van Avermaet, E., & Buelens, H. (2006). The Janus face of
power in intergroup contexts: A further exploration of the noblesse oblige effect. The
Journal of Social Psychology, 146, 685–699. DOI: 10.3200/SOCP.146.6.685-699
Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2016). Intergroup stereotyping. Current Opinion in Psychology, 11, 90–95.
DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.06.009
Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Cambon, L. (2017). The dynamics of compensation: When ingroup
favoritism paves the way for outgroup praise. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
43, 587–600. DOI: 10.1177/0146167216689066
Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Corneille, O. (2005). Cognitive process: Reality constraints and integrity
concerns in social perception. In J. F. Dovidio, P. Glick, & L. Rudman (Eds.), On the
nature of prejudice: 50 years after Allport. London, UK: Blackwell.
Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Demoulin, S. (2010). Intergroup relations. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, &
G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 1024–1083). Hobo-
ken, NJ: Wiley.
Yzerbyt, V. Y., Kervyn, N., & Judd, C. (2008). Compensation versus halo: The unique
relations between the fundamental dimensions of social judgment. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1110–1123. DOI: 10.1177/0146167208318602
Yzerbyt, V. Y., Provost, V., & Corneille, O. (2005). Not so competent but warm ... really?
Compensatory stereotypes in the French-speaking world. Group Processes and Intergroup
Relations, 8, 291–308. DOI: 10.1177/1368430205053944
12
POWER, SELF-FOCUS, AND
THE BIG TWO
Aleksandra Cislak and Aleksandra Cichocka
Overview
Agency and communion are the “Big Two” in social cognition (Abele & Wojciszke,
2014). Agentic versus communal behavior and agentic versus communal social
cognition are closely tied to perspective: agency corresponds to the perspective of
the actor and reflects self-interest, while communion corresponds to the perspec-
tive of the observer and reflects other-interest (Abele & Wojciszke, 2014; Cislak &
Wojciszke, 2008). Social power is also closely tied to perspective (Galinsky, Magee,
Inesi, & Gruenfeld, 2006; Gruenfeld, Inesi, Magee, & Galinsky, 2008): the power-
ful have a different focus than the powerless. The present chapter integrates the
literature on social power and the Big Two, reviewing empirical work on how the
power perspective affects agentic and communal contents of perception, evaluation,
and behavior. Specifically, we make the theoretical argument that due to self-focus,
power promotes an agentic mind-set. People behave in a most agentic fashion when
they have power, and in a least agentic fashion when they lack power (Moskowitz,
Suh, & Desaulniers, 1994). Even more importantly, people in a position of power
tend to interpret others’ behavior in terms of agency rather than communion (Cislak,
2013), and base their overall evaluation of powerless individuals on the assessment
of their agency (Cislak, 2014). Thus, power reverses the usually observed primacy
of communion (Wojciszke & Abele, 2008; Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011) towards the
primacy of agency.
In this chapter, we will first shortly discuss the Big Two and the concept of
power, including its positive and negative consequences, such as goal-directed agen-
tic behavior and objectification. We will then show that power leads to self-focus.
We will argue that the focus on the self is the psychological mechanism that stands
behind the positive and negative aspects of agency stemming from holding high-
power positions.
Power, Self-Focus, and the Big Two 143
Social power
The vast literature on social dimensions coincides with recent findings from the
field of the effects of social power. Social power is defined as asymmetric control
over valued resources in a social relationship (Magee & Galinsky, 2008). Impor-
tantly, power is by nature a relational concept: it concerns relations between indi-
viduals, rather than individuals themselves (Emerson, 1962). As we will describe
in more detail in the next sections of this chapter, this type of social asymmetry
has been consistently shown to undermine social relations and result in corrup-
tion. But power and some of its less desired effects should neither be equated
with merely holding a high position within a social or organizational hierarchy,
nor with being a leader. In fact, the psychological effects of power often stand in
contrast to the leadership demands (Maner & Mead, 2010), that is, pursuing col-
lective good and focusing on others. Although power may stem from holding a
high position, its effects are qualified by the effects of other sphere of control also
increasing with position, namely personal control. While exploitation and aggres-
sion are positively related to power, they are negatively related to personal control
(Cislak, Cichocka, Wojcik, & Frankowska, 2018).
In this chapter we focus on the psychological effects of power as a type of asym-
metric social relation involving control over others. We attempt to develop an
agentic model of power. To do so, we begin by analyzing how power influences
144 Aleksandra Cislak and Aleksandra Cichocka
At the same time, power seems to make people more authentic. It promotes
self-driven, situationally unconstrained behavior (Keltner et al., 2003). Power-
induced asymmetries affect conformity and creativity, resulting in powerful peo-
ple being more self-reliant and more independent from situational pressures and
others’ opinions (Galinsky, Magee, Gruenfeld, Whitson, & Liljenquist, 2008). In
a similar vein, power increases self-reliance both on the inner states and traits. For
example, power increases reliance on bodily information and as a result powerful
(but not powerless) individuals eat more when hungry and eat more appetizing
than non-appetizing food ( Guinote, 2010). Powerful people tend to also rely more
on their own preferences, for example on preferred styles of information process-
ing (Kossowska, Guinote, & Strojny, 2016). But power also promotes authenticity
by intensifying the effects of individuals’ initial predispositions on their percep-
tion and behavior. Power reinforces the effects of both exchange and prosocial
orientation, thus making prosocially-oriented individuals even more prosocial
( Chen, Lee-Chai, & Bargh, 2001) and more accurate in assessing emotional states
of others ( Côté et al., 2011). Thus, power may sometimes increase prosociality and
stimulate communal mind-set but only by means of reinforcing the initial proso-
cial orientation, that is, through enhancing focus on self.
Taken together, empirical evidence from various research fields such as social
neuroscience, cognitive, organizational, and social psychology converges in show-
ing that power promotes self-focus, which may stand behind the far-reaching
effects of power. As powerful people are focused on the self, they should also pay
more attention to the agency dimension, because it brings them closer to goal-
focus and is also more self-profitable. Here, we claim that by promoting self-focus,
power leads to an agentic mind-set manifested in a wide range of effects across
different domains.
subordinates and thus can ignore the communal aspect of perception. Those who
occupy higher organizational positions tend to perceive others in an instrumental
way, especially their own employees (Gruenfeld et al., 2008).
Interpersonal behavior
Social role theory (Eagly, 1987) posits that some social roles require and are expected
to be associated with more agentic behaviors, while other social roles require and are
expected to be associated with more communal behaviors. These role requirements
stand behind perceived and expected differences in behavior. In their refinement of
the social role theory Moskowitz et al. (1994) hypothesized that the factor control-
ling the relationship between roles/occupations and agency is power. Individuals in
roles associated with holding a higher power position reveal more agency-related
interpersonal behaviors than individuals holding lower power positions. In an analy-
sis set in natural settings and actual power differences, Moskowitz et al. (1994) found
that the same people revealed more agentic behaviors in the interactions in which
they played the role of the supervisors than in interactions in which they had been
supervised (regardless of their gender). Thus, power was shown to be associated with
agency also on the behavioral level. In contrast, no power differences were found for
communal behaviors.
Such differences were, however, observed in subsequent work. Individuals holding
relatively low positions within the group hierarchy showed more communal and pro-
social behavior (Guinote, Cotzia, Sandhu, & Siwa, 2015). The agentic-communal dis-
tinction is also well-illustrated by research in the field of consumer behavior (Rucker
et al., 2011; Rucker, Galinsky, & Dubois, 2012). Rucker et al. (2011) found that low-
power participants bought three times more than high-power participants when
purchasing for others, but at the same time they acquired only half of high-power
individuals’ purchases when buying for themselves. This is in line with work showing
that social class and experienced power promotes selfishness, rather than unethical-
ity per se (Dubois et al., 2015). Thus, the behavior of the powerful is intended to be
agentic (see Cislak & Wojciszke, 2008) rather than immoral. These results speak to
the agentic mind-set framework developed in this chapter.
This has important social implications. The results reviewed here shed light
on the long-standing discussion regarding the corruptive effects of power. Power,
rather than directly corrupting people, simply leads to self-focus. This can explain
the increased preference for self-focus values such as self-enhancement and open-
ness to change and the decreased preference for other-focus values, i.e., self-
transcendence and conservation. In turn, the self-focus is manifested in a wide
range of social effects, from egocentric perspective and selfishness, through agency-
focus, to increased authenticity in the expression of one’s values and orientations.
The agentic focus promotes action and attainment of goals, which can be, though
are not necessarily, also the goals of the persons over whom the powerful have
control. Thus, power enables people to get what they aim for. But power also pro-
motes instrumental perceptions of others that may disrupt workplace social rela-
tions. Therefore, it creates a tension with the demands of leadership, which involves
focus on group goals and others (Maner & Mead, 2010). Therefore, the agentic
mind-set of the powerful may increase organizational performance, but it may also
be harmful for those organizations, in which productivity depends on the infor-
mational and social benefits of concentrating on others (such as companies with a
primary strategic focus on innovation; see Dezsö & Ross, 2012). In the long run,
a diminished communal mind-set may undermine social responsibility concerns
resulting both in unethical and suboptimal decisions.
Importantly however, the otherwise lower agentic focus of the powerless may
be enhanced by demonstrating the illegitimacy of the power relationship. When a
power relationship is not perceived as justified and legitimate, those at the bottom
gain action orientation and agency-focus typical for those at the top (Lammers,
Galinsky, Gordijn, & Otten, 2008). Thus, power and agency-focus remain in the
dynamic interplay shaping social hierarchies.
References
Abele, A. E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”? Preferential pro-
cessing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 935–948.
Abele, A. E., Hauke, N., Peters, K., Louvet, E., Szymkow, A., & Duan, Y. (2016). Facets
of the fundamental content dimensions: Agency with competence and assertiveness:
Communion with warmth and morality. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1810.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self
versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 751–763.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition:
A dual perspective model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195–255.
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: An essay on psychology and religion. Chicago,
IL: Rand McNally.
Chen, S., Lee-Chai, A. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). Relationship orientation as a moderator
of the effects of social power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 173–187.
Cislak, A. (2013). Effects of power on social perception: All your boss can see is agency.
Social Psychology, 44, 139–147.
Cislak, A. (2014). Impact of conflict resolution strategies on perception of agency, com-
munion and power roles evaluation. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 45, 426–433.
Power, Self-Focus, and the Big Two 151
Cislak, A., Cichocka, A., Wojcik, A., & Frankowska, N. (2018). Power corrupts, but con-
trol does not: What stands behind the effects of holding high positions. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218757456
Cislak, A., & Wojcik, A. (2017). Power and values of managers: A transnational study. Manu-
script in preparation.
Cislak, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2006). The role of self-interest and competence in attitudes
toward politicians. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 37, 203–212.
Cislak, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2008). Agency and communion are inferred from actions
serving interests of self or others. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1103–1110.
Côté, S., Kraus, M. W., Cheng, B. H., Oveis, C., van der Löwe, I., Lian, H., & Keltner, D.
(2011). Social power facilitates the effect of prosocial orientation on empathic accuracy.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 217–232.
Dezsö, C. L., & Ross, D. G. (2012). Does female representation in top management
improve firm performance? A panel data investigation. Strategic Management Journal,
33, 1072–1089.
Dubois, D., Rucker, D. D., & Galinsky, A. D. (2015). Social class, power, and selfishness:
When and why upper and lower class individuals behave unethically. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 108, 436–449.
Dubois, D., Rucker, D. D., & Galinsky, A. D. (2016). Dynamics of communicator and
audience power: The persuasiveness of competence versus warmth. Journal of Consumer
Research, 43, 68–85.
Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Emerson, R. M. (1962). Power-dependence relations. American Sociological Review, 27, 31–41.
Fiske, S. T. (1993). Controlling other people: The impact of power on stereotyping. Ameri-
can Psychologist, 48, 621–628.
Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to action. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453–466.
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Gruenfeld, D. H., Whitson, J. A., & Liljenquist, K. A.
(2008). Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for creativity, conformity,
and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1450–1466.
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (2006). Power and perspec-
tives not taken. Psychological Science, 17, 1068–1074.
Gruenfeld, D. H., Inesi, M. E., Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Power and the
objectification of social targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 111–127.
Guinote, A. (2007a). Power and goal pursuit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33,
1076–1087.
Guinote, A. (2007b). Power affects basic cognition: Increased attentional inhibition and
flexibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 685–697.
Guinote, A. (2010). In touch with your feelings: Power increases reliance on bodily infor-
mation. Social Cognition, 28, 110–121.
Guinote, A., Cotzia, I., Sandhu, S., & Siwa, P. (2015). Social status modulates prosocial
behavior and egalitarianism in preschool children and adults. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 112, 731–736.
Hogeveen, J., Inzlicht, M., & Obhi, S. S. (2014). Power changes how the brain responds to
others. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 755–762.
Keltner, D. J., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition.
Psychological Review, 110, 265–284.
Kohn, M., & Słomczyński, K. M. (1990). Social structure and self-direction: A comparative
analysis of the United States and Poland. Cambridge, MA: B. Blackwell.
152 Aleksandra Cislak and Aleksandra Cichocka
Kossowska, M., Guinote, A., & Strojny, P. (2016). Power boosts reliance on preferred pro-
cessing styles. Motivation and Emotion, 40, 556–565.
Kraus, M. W., Côté, S., & Keltner, D. (2010). Social class, contextualism, and empathic
accuracy. Psychological Science, 21, 1716–1723.
Lammers, J., Galinsky, A., Gordijn, E. H., & Otten, S. (2008). Illegitimacy moderates the
effects of power on approach. Psychological Science, 19, 558–564.
Lammers, J., Stapel, D., & Galinsky, A. (2010). Power increases hypocrisy. Psychological
Science, 21, 737–744.
Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Social hierarchy: The self-reinforcing nature of
power and status. The Academy of Management Annals, 2, 351–398.
Maner, J. K., & Mead, N. L. (2010). The essential tension between leadership and power:
When leaders sacrifice group goals for the sake of self-interest. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 99, 482–497.
Marx, K. (1992/1844). Early writings (R. Livingstone and G. Benton, Trans.). London: Pen-
guin. (Original work published 1844.)
McAdams, D. P., Hoffman, B. J., Day, R., & Mansfield, E. D. (1996). Themes of agency and
communion in significant autobiographical scenes. Journal of Personality, 64, 339–377.
Moskowitz, D. S., Suh, E. J., & Desaulniers, J. (1994). Situational influences on gender
differences in agency and communion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66,
753–761.
Obhi, S. S., Swiderski, K. M., & Brubacher, S. P. (2012). Induced power changes the sense
of agency. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1547–1550.
Peeters, G. (1992). Evaluative meanings of adjectives in vitro and in context: Some the-
oretical implications and practical consequences of positive negative asymmetry and
behavioral-adaptive concepts of evaluation. Psychologia Belgica, 32, 211–231.
Piotrowski, J., & Wojciszke, B. (2015). Agentic thinking about others makes them closer.
Polish Psychological Bulletin, 46, 523–534.
Rucker, D. D., Dubois, D., & Galinsky, A. D. (2011). Generous paupers and stingy princes:
Power drives consumer spending on self versus others. Journal of Consumer Research, 37,
1015–1029.
Rucker, D. D., Galinsky, A. D., & Dubois, D. (2012). Power and consumer behavior:
How power shapes who and what consumers value. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22,
352–368.
Rudman, L. A., Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Nauts, S. (2012). Status incongru-
ity and backlash effects: Defending the gender hierarchy motivates prejudice against
female leaders. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 165–179.
Schwartz, S. H. (2012). An overview of the Schwartz theory of basic values. Online Read-
ings in Psychology and Culture, 2, 11.
See, K., Wolfe Morrison, E., Rothman, N. B., & Soll, J. B. (2011). The detrimental effects
of power on confidence, advice taking, and accuracy. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 116, 272–285.
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, R. (1999). Social dominance theory: A new synthesis. In J. Sida-
nius & R. Pratto (Eds.), Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppres-
sion (pp. 31–57). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Smith, P. K., Jostmann, N. B., Galinsky, A. D., & van Dijk, W. W. (2008). Lacking power
impairs executive functions. Psychological Science, 19, 441–447.
Tost, L. P., Gino, F., & Larrick, R. P. (2012). Power, competitiveness, and advice taking: Why
the powerful don’t listen. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117, 53–65.
van Kleef, G. A., Oveis, C., Homan, A. C., van der Löwe, I., & Keltner, D. (2015). Power
gets you high. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 472–480.
Power, Self-Focus, and the Big Two 153
van Kleef, G. A., Oveis, C., van der Löwe, I., LuoKogan, A., Goetz, J., & Keltner, D.
(2008). Power, distress, and compassion. Psychological Science, 19, 1315–1322.
Wicklund, R. A. (1975). Objective self-awareness. Advances in Experimental Social Psychol-
ogy, 8, 233–275.
Woike, B. A. (1994). The use of differentiation and integration processes: Empirical studies
of “separate” and “connected” ways of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 67, 142–150.
Wojciszke, B. (1994). Multiple meanings of behavior: Construing actions in terms of com-
petence or morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 222–232.
Wojciszke, B., & Abele, A. E. (2008). The primacy of communion over agency and its
reversals in evaluations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1139–1147.
13
THE “BIG TWO” IN CITIZENS’
PERCEPTIONS OF POLITICIANS
Susanne Bruckmüller and Nicole Methner
questions and new insights into voters’ perceptions of politicians, and (b) to illus-
trate how this may also enhance social psychologists’ understanding of the Big Two.
2014; see also Abele & Hauke, this volume; Fiske, this volume; and Sczesny, Nater,
& Eagly, this volume). Whether it is important and meaningful to distinguish
between these sub-facets or not depends on the research question at hand.
Big Two framework Bertolotti Caprara et al. Chen et al. Funk (1999) Kinder Pancer et al. Wojciszke and
(Abele et al., 2016) et al. (2013) (1997, 2002) (2014) (1986) (1999) Klusek (1996)
Communion
Morality Morality Trustworthiness Integrity Integrity Integrity Morality
Warmth Friendliness Likability Empathy Empathy Charisma Likability
Agency
Competence Leadership Competence Leadership Competence Competence Competence
Assertiveness Energy Dominance Effectiveness Leadership
Note: The grouping of concepts into the Big Two framework is based on their respective operationalizations.
The “Big Two” in Perceptions of Politicians 159
at the same time, they need to be responsive to the needs and desires of the popu-
lace (a communal task; McGraw, 2002). Understanding agency and communion
as two separate dimensions may help researchers and political strategists to realize
that to be responsive to public opinion and therefore to be communal does not
necessarily reduce politicians’ agency, a criticism sometimes leveled at politicians
who appear too responsive to public opinion (see Jacobs & Shapiro, 1994). Relat-
edly, a Big Two framework might also shed some light on the question why it is
precisely these two dimensions that matter for the perception of politicians.
The underlying theory to explain why agency and communion are the primary
dimensions of social perception and judgment is that person perception fulfills the
goal to facilitate (safe and beneficial) interactions with others (Gibson, 1979). For
this end, perceivers first and foremost need to know whether another person is
a friend or a foe, that is, whether they have benevolent or malevolent intentions
towards the perceiver ( Fiske et al., 2007; Wojciszke & Abele, this volume; Ybarra,
this volume). Communion is a key indicator of such benevolence/malevolence.
Perceivers also need to know whether the other person is able and likely to act
on their benevolent or malevolent intentions (i.e., their agency, Fiske et al., 2007).
This explains why in most contexts, communion is the primary dimension for
person perception (Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011; Abele & Wojciszke, 2007). It also
explains why high assertiveness and competence do not always lead to a more
favorable evaluation, as the meaning of agency for the perceiver depends on the
perceived person’s communion (see Paulhus, this volume). A competent benevo-
lent person is great; a competent villain is all the more dangerous (see Fiske et al.,
2007). Thus, a Big Two framework not only provides insights into why morality
(the primary sub-facet of communion, Brambilla & Leach, 2014) and competence
(a sub-facet of agency) are central for the evaluation of politicians and for voters’
election choices ( Chen et al., 2014; Kinder, 1986; Lodge et al., 1989; Miller et al.,
1986; Mondak, 1995). It also points to situations when crafting a campaign around
a candidate’s competence is promising and when it may backfire.
Another realization of Big Two research is that this general primacy of commu-
nion is moderated by the type of relationship between the perceiver and the perceived
person. In situations in which the perceiver’s outcomes depend on the actions of
another person, agency gains importance, as this other person’s agency will be key for
obtaining own desired outcomes. For example, one would prefer one’s lawyer to be
friendly and moral, but one would care at least as much whether they are competent
and assertive (Abele & Brack, 2013; Abele & Wojciszke, 2007; Cislak & Cichocka,
this volume; Wojciszke & Abele, this volume). Arguably, this is the case with politi-
cians as well, at least with those that hold public office or run for one. One may even
predict that the relative importance of agency and communion in politicians varies
depending on whether a politician holds office or not; for the importance of a candi-
date’s perceived agency, it may even matter whether or not voters perceive a realistic
chance for this candidate to actually take office after the election.
However, a Big Two framework may not only be useful on such a broad
conceptual level. A more specific example that we will illustrate in more detail
160 Susanne Bruckmüller and Nicole Methner
represent us, but not so much in other contexts of person perception. This is, of
course, an empirical question.
Similarly, research on the perception of politicians might inspire hypotheses
on moderating factors and boundary conditions for the primacy of communion
over agency. Above we already hypothesized that the relative importance of a
politician’s perceived communion and agency may depend on whether this politi-
cian currently holds office or not. Another important moderator may be party
affiliation or, more generally, group membership. Despite communion being the
primary dimension of person perception, and even though the general stereotype
of politicians is that they are immoral (e.g., Birch & Allen, 2010; Hatier, 2012),
party supporters evaluating their respective party leader rely first and foremost on
a leader’s competence ( Capelos, van den Akker, & van der Eijk, 2007). One may
speculate that the association between a party leader and the self, as well as related
social identity processes (Tajfel & Turner, 1986) cause party supporters to simply
take party leaders’ communion for granted and to thus focus on leaders’ agency
instead. A similar process (taking own communion for granted; see Bi, Ybarra, &
Zhao, 2013) has been suggested as explanation for the dominance of agency over
communion in self-perception (Wojciszke, Baryla, Parzuchowski, Szymkow, &
Abele, 2011; but see also Abele & Hauke, this volume).2
As a further example, research on voters’ perceptions of politicians suggests
a possible boundary condition for the asymmetry observed in the diagnostic-
ity of positive versus negative agentic versus communal information in impres-
sion formation ( Skowronski & Carlston, 1989). In impression formation, one
immoral act influences overall morality judgments much more than one moral act
(that is, negative morality-related behavior is more diagnostic). At the same time,
one competent act is more influential than one incompetent act (that is, positive
competence-related behavior is more diagnostic; Skowronski & Carlston, 1989).
However, a particularly strong cue in impression formation is expectancy viola-
tion (Jussim, Coleman, & Lerch, 1987), and citizens in Europe and North America
generally perceive politicians as immoral and not trustworthy (e.g., Birch & Allen,
2010; Hatier, 2012). Hence, politicians might be a case where positive morality-
related information is more diagnostic than negative information, because it is
unexpected. In our experiments summarized above (Methner et al., 2017), we
found some initial support for this hypothesis, as expectancy violation played an
important role for the influence of accepting criticism on trust in politicians.
Finally, within social cognition the realization that the Big Two can be mean-
ingfully conceptualized as containing two sub-facets each (namely, morality and
warmth for communion and competence and assertiveness for agency) is relatively
recent (e.g., Abele et al., 2016; Brambilla & Leach, 2014; Carrier et al., 2014). Yet,
while political scientists have used a variety of different content dimensions to
study the perception of politicians, one early and particularly prominent concep-
tualization has four dimensions that exactly match these sub-facets (namely integ-
rity, empathy, competence, and leadership; Kinder, 1986). An earlier look into the
respective literature could have saved psychologists quite a bit of time. We hope
The “Big Two” in Perceptions of Politicians 163
that the parallels we have pointed out in this chapter will help researchers across
disciplines to save time on some future key insights.
Acknowledgments
We thank Julia Dupont, University of Koblenz-Landau, for valuable comments on
an earlier version of this chapter.
Notes
1 Caprara and Zimbardo (2004) do mention in passing that their dimensions of energy and
friendliness bear resemblance to the dimensions outlined by Bakan (1966).
2 For the evaluation of one’s ingroup as a whole, morality appears to be the most important
dimension (Leach, Ellemers, & Barreto, 2007). However, this does not preclude a motiva-
tion to take for granted the morality of (the leader of) a group one highly identifies with.
References
Abele, A. E., & Brack, S. (2013). Preference for other persons’ traits is dependent on the kind
of social relationship. Social Psychology, 44, 84–94. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/
a000138
Abele, A. E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”? Preferential pro-
cessing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 935–948.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.03.028
Abele, A. E., Hauke, N., Peters, K., Louvet, E., Szymkow, A., & Duan, Y. (2016). Facets
of the fundamental content dimensions: Agency with competence and assertiveness:
Communion with warmth and morality. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1810. https://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01810
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of
self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 751–763. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.751
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition:
A dual perspective model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50 (50), 195–255.
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-800284-1.00004-7
Adam, S., & Maier, M. (2010). Personalization of politics: A critical review and agenda for
research. Annals of the International Communication Association, 34, 213–257, https://doi.
org/10.1080/23808985.2010.11679101
Armstrong-Taylor, P. (2012). When do politicians lie? The B.E. Journal of Economic Analysis
and Policy, 12 (3), Article 6. https://doi.org/10.1515/1935-1682.3103
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: An essay on psychology and religion. Chicago,
IL: Rand McNally.
Bertolotti, M., Catellani, P., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2013). The “Big Two” in
political communication: The effects of attacking and defending politicians’ leadership
or morality. Social Psychology, 44 (2), 117–128. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000141
Bi, C., Ybarra, O., & Zhao, Y. (2013). Accentuating your masculine side: Agentic traits
generally dominate self-evaluation, even in China. Social Psychology, 44 (2), 103–108.
https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000144
Birch, S., & Allen, N. (2010). How honest do politicians need to be? The Political Quarterly,
81(1), 49–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-923X.2010.02066.x
164 Susanne Bruckmüller and Nicole Methner
Brambilla, M., & Leach, C. W. (2014). On the importance of being moral: The distinc-
tive role of morality in social judgment. Social Cognition, 32, 397–408. https://doi.
org/10.1521/soco.2014.32.4.397
Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American Voter.
New York, NY: Wiley.
Capelos, T., van den Akker, R., & van der Eijk, S. (2007). Like leader, like party: Leader-
ship assessments and party images in NL. Conference Papers: Midwestern Political Science
Association, 1–26.
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Consiglio, C., Picconi, L., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2003). Per-
sonalities of politicians and voters: Unique and synergistic relationships. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 84, 849–856. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.849
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1997, February 6). Politicians’ uniquely
simple personalities. Nature, 385, 493. https://doi.org/10.1038/385493a0
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2002). When parsimony subdues dis-
tinctiveness: Simplified public perceptions of politicians’ personality. Political Psychology,
23, 77–96. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00271
Caprara, G. V., Schwartz, S., Capanna, C., Vecchione, M., & Barbaranelli, C. (2006). Per-
sonality and politics: Values, traits, and political choice. Political Psychology, 27, 1–28.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2006.00447.x
Caprara, G. V., & Zimbardo, P. (2004). Personalizing politics: A congruency
model of political preference. American Psychologist, 59, 581–594. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0003-066X.59.7.581
Carraro, L., Castelli, L., Breazu, I., Campomizzi, G., Cerruto, A., Mariani, M., & Toto, I.
(2012). Just ignore or counterattack? On the effects of different strategies for dealing
with political attacks. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42 (6), 789–797. https://doi.
org/10.1002/ejsp.1884
Carrier, A., Louvet, E., Chauvin, B., & Rohmer, O. (2014). The primacy of agency over
competence in status perception. Social Psychology, 45, 347–356. https://doi.org/10.1027/
1864-9335/a000176
Catellani, P., & Alberici, A. I. (2012). Does the candidate matter? Comparing the voting
choice of early and late deciders. Political Psychology, 33, 619–634. https://doi.org/10.1111/
j.1467-9221.2012.00891.x
Chen, F. F., Jing, Y., & Lee, J. M. (2014). The looks of a leader: Competent and trustwor-
thy, but not dominant. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 51, 27–33. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.10.008
Cislak, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2008). Agency and communion are inferred from actions
serving interests of self or others. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1103–1110.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.554
Craig, S. C., Rippere, P. S., & Grayson, M. S. (2014). Attack and response in political cam-
paigns: An experimental study in two parts. Political Communication, 31(4), 647–674.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2013.879362
Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73(6), 1246–1256. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1246
Ehrke, F., Bruckmüller, S., & Steffens, M. (2016). Weniger kompetent, aber dafür wärmer?
Zum Einfluss sozialer Vielfalt von Parteien auf politisches Vertrauen. [Less competent
but warmer? On the influence of social diversity of political parties on political trust].
Politische Psychologie, 1, 28–45.
Ehrke, F., Bruckmüller, S., & Steffens, M. (2017). How does the social diversity of political
parties affect citizens’ trust in them? The role of perceived warmth, competence, and similarity.
Manuscript in preparation.
The “Big Two” in Perceptions of Politicians 165
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cogni-
tion: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 77–83. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005
Funk, C. L. (1999). Bringing the candidate into models of candidate evaluation. The Journal
of Politics, 61, 700–720. https://doi.org/10.2307/2647824
Gebauer, J. E., Wagner, J., Sedikides, C., & Neberich, W. (2013). Agency-communion and
self-esteem relations are moderated by culture, religiosity, age, and sex: Evidence for the
“self-centrality breeds self-enhancement” principle. Journal of Personality, 81, 261–275.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00807.x
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Hatier, C. (2012). “Them” and “us”: Demonising politicians by moral double standards.
Contemporary Politics, 18 (4), 467–480. https://doi.org/10.1080/13569775.2012.728033
Hogan, R. (1983). A socioanalytic theory of personality. In M. Page (Ed.), Nebraska sympo-
sium on motivation (pp. 336–355). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Holtz-Bacha, C. (2006). Personalisiert und emotional: Strategien des modernen Wahlkamp-
fes [Personalized and emotional: Strategies of modern election campaigns]. Aus Politik
und Zeitgeschichte, 7, 11–19.
Holtz-Bacha, C., Lessinger, E.-M., & Hettesheimer, M. (1998). Personalisierung als Strat-
egie der Wahlwerbung [Personalization as strategy of political advertisement]. In K.
Imhof & P. Schulz (Eds.), Die Veröffentlichung des Privaten–Die Privatisierung des Öffentli-
chen (pp. 240–250). Wiesbaden, Germany: Westdeutscher Verlag.
Jacobs, L. R., & Shapiro, R. Y. (1994). Issues, candidate image, and priming: The use of
private polls in Kennedy’s 1960 presidential campaign. American Political Science Review,
88, 527–540. https://doi.org/10.2307/2944793
Judd, C. M., James-Hawkins, L., Yzerbyt, V., & Kashima, Y. (2005). Fundamental dimen-
sions of social judgment: Understanding the relations between judgments of compe-
tence and warmth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 899–913. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.899
Jussim, L., Coleman, L. M., & Lerch, L. (1987). The nature of stereotypes: A compari-
son and integration of three theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 (3),
536–546. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.536
Kinder, D. R. (1986). Presidential character revisited. In R. R. Lau & D. O. Sears (Eds.),
Political cognition: The nineteenth Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition (pp. 233–256).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.
Lau, R. R. (1982). Negativity in political perception. Political Behavior, 4, 353–377. https://
doi.org/10.1007/BF00986969
Leach, C. W., Ellemers, N., & Barreto, M. (2007). Group virtue: The importance of moral-
ity (vs. competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 93(2), 234–249.
Lodge, M., McGraw, K. M., & Stroh, P. (1989). An impression-driven model of candidate
evaluation. American Political Science Review, 83, 399–419. https://doi.org/10.2307/1962397
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality
across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81–90.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.81
McGraw, K. M. (2002). Manipulating public opinion. In B. Norrander & C. Wilcox (Eds.),
Understanding public opinion (pp. 265–280). Washington, DC: CQ Press.
McGraw, K. M. (2003). Political impressions: Formation and management. In D. O. Sears,
L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology (pp. 394–432). New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
166 Susanne Bruckmüller and Nicole Methner
Methner, N., Bruckmüller, S., & Steffens, M. C. (2017). Can accepting criticism be an effective
impression management strategy for politicians? A comparison with denials and a counterattack.
Manuscript in preparation.
Miller, A. H., Wattenberg, M. P., & Malanchuk, O. (1986). Schematic assessments of
presidential candidates. American Political Science Review, 80, 521–540. https://doi.
org/10.2307/1958272
Mondak, J. J. (1995). Competence, integrity, and the electoral success of congressional
incumbents. Journal of Politics, 57, 1043–1069. https://doi.org/10.2307/2960401
Pancer, S. M., Brown, S. D., & Barr, C. W. (1999). Forming impressions of political lead-
ers: A cross-national comparison. Political Psychology, 20 (2), 345–368. https://doi.
org/10.1111/0162-895X.00148
Paulhus, D. L., & Trapnell, P. D. (2008). Self-presentation of personality: An agency-
communion framework. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook
of personality psychology: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 492–517). New York, NY: Guil-
ford Press.
Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1989). Negativity and extremity biases in impression
formation: A review of explanations. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 131–142. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0146167291174009
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of inter-group behavior. In S.
Worchel & L. W. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–48). Chicago,
IL: Nelson-Hall.
Wiggins, J. S. (1979). A psychological taxonomy of trait-descriptive terms: The interper-
sonal domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(3), 395–412. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.3.395
Wojciszke, B., Baryla, W., Parzuchowski, M., Szymkow, A., & Abele, A. E. (2011). Self-
esteem is dominated by agentic over communal information. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 41, 617–627. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.791
Wojciszke, B., & Klusek, B. (1996). Moral and competence-related traits in political per-
ception. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 27, 319–324.
14
RETHINKING THE NATURE AND
RELATION OF FUNDAMENTAL
DIMENSIONS OF MEANING
Alex Koch and Roland Imhoff
Abele, 2011). People rated politicians, women, men, the self, traits, grandiose self-
thoughts, and experienced events on agency and communion (Abele & Wojciszke,
2007; Cislak & Wojciszke, 2008; Gebauer, Sedikides, Verplanken, & Maio, 2012;
Sczesny, 2003), categorized cultural practices as agentic and/or communal (Ybarra
et al., 2008), rated groups on status, competence, warmth, and competition with
other groups (Fiske et al., 2002), and inferred agency and communion from behav-
iors (Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011, Study 3). There are, however, some studies that
test the priority of the Big Two by allowing people to employ other meaning
dimensions.
For example, Abele and Bruckmüller (2011, Study 4) asked people to provide
up to six descriptions of an acquaintance and found that 79% of all open responses
could be reliably categorized as relating to agency or communion. Uchronski (2008)
showed that 74% of up to 12 open self-descriptions (up to six and six descriptions
of the self at home and at work, respectively) related to agency or communion.
In a study by Wojciszke, Bazinska, and Jaworski (1998), all 10 personality traits
listed by >20% of participants as most important in others related to competence
(2) or morality (8). While it is possible that many of the both categorized (i.e., as
relating to agency or communion) and uncategorized descriptions (also) relate to
one or a few meaning dimensions other than the Big Two, these and other mostly
data-driven studies (see Abele & Wojciszke, 2014) clearly showed that agency/
communion is a satisfying (but not necessarily the optimal) model of how people
make sense of the generic individual, specific others, and the self.
Following the data-driven research cited in the previous paragraph, we also
sought to provide an impartial look at the nature of prioritized dimensions by
examining the dimension(s) that people spontaneously focus on to make sense
of their social environment (Koch, Imhoff, Dotsch, Unkelbach, & Alves, 2016).
In contrast to previous approaches construing social environment in the sense of
a single person, we were interested in how participants spontaneously compare
multiple groups. Importantly, the population of relevant social groups was unclear.
According to Brunswik’s (1955) notion of representative design, free choice of
dimension(s) is necessary but not sufficient to study prioritized meaning in an
ambiguous population. In addition, the population must be clarified and perceiv-
ers must respond to samples that represent the now unambiguous population –
otherwise generalizing results beyond the lab is out of reach. For example, if
perceivers respond to targets theoretically constrained to differ in agency but
not communion, they will not prioritize communion because it is uninforma-
tive (Tversky, 1977). Importantly, in many studies on fundamental meaning (e.g.,
Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011, Study 3), perceivers responded to targets that by
design differed greatly on the Big Two but no other dimension. If targets in the
population of social groups also differ greatly on other dimensions, however, such
studies may overlook fundamental meaning regardless of perceivers’ free choice of
dimension(s). To avoid this, besides designing free choice of dimension(s), in our
data-driven studies perceivers responded to target distributions that represented
the population of social groups well.
Rethinking Fundamental Dimensions of Meaning 169
We reasoned people know best which targets represent the population of social
groups well. Thus, we gave perceivers free choice of targets, too (Koch, Imhoff, et al.,
2016). That is, initial samples of perceivers listed social group-representative targets,
and we gave subsequent samples of perceivers free choice to relate the most fre-
quently named targets to any desired dimension(s). Following previous data-driven
research, we instructed perceivers to judge targets’ similarity. Similarity needed to
be interpreted before it could be judged. For example, the similarity of doctors and
bankers needed to be interpreted with respect to agency, communion, or something
else before it could be judged. If agency, the two targets would be judged as similar.
If communion, they would be judged as different. Crucially, perceivers were free to
prioritize any desired dimension(s) to interpret and judge targets’ similarity.
White collar
Christians
Athletes Religious
Jocks
Democrats Whites
Jews
Liberals Men
Parents Elderly Conservative
beliefs (B)
Women
Progressive Middle class Asians
beliefs (B) Nerds Students Muslims
Lesbians Hipsters Teenagers
Homosexuals Goths
Hippies Hispanic Blue collar
Transgender Working class
Blacks
Immigrants
Lower class
Poor
Homeless
Drug addicts
Low
agency (A)
FIGURE 14.1 People’s mean similarity map of social groups according to the ABC model.
Figure 14.1 plots A and B where they best explained the groups’ configuration
in the similarity map. C could not be modeled as a dimension spanning the map.
Thus, inconsistent with the Big Two ( Fiske et al., 2002), results showed that people
had freely chosen agency (A) and beliefs (B) but not communion (C) to interpret
and judge the similarity of the groups, suggesting that the dimensions that people
spontaneously use to make sense of their social world are A and B but not C. In six
studies, this AB model of fundamental meaning replicated across ~4,000 people,
two national contexts, four group sets compiled in different ways, three similar-
ity measures, and three similarity judgment instructions (global, character, and
personal encounter similarity; Koch, Imhoff, et al., 2016).
As outlined above, people can use the Big Two to differentiate all sorts of social
entities varying from countries to brands. Thus, evidence that people spontane-
ously use AB to differentiate entities other than social groups would further vali-
date the AB model. There were only a few job groups among the social groups we
had examined (Koch, Imhoff, et al., 2016, see Tables 1, 5, and 6). We thus took job
groups as a separate entity domain. We used the same design and analysis as out-
lined above (two studies, N ~1,800, two national contexts), except that people did
not list job groups at first. Instead, we examined all 150 basic, distinct job groups
listed by the US Department of Labor (i.e., the entire entity domain). The first
two dimensions that people spontaneously used to spatially arrange the job groups
were agency/competence (i.e., A) and progressiveness (i.e., B; designers, artists,
etc. were high scorers, whereas morticians, firefighters, etc. were low scorers).
Next, we replicated spontaneous differentiation on A and B with all 84 basic, dis-
tinct job groups listed by the International Standard Classification of Occupations
( Imhoff, Koch, & Flade, 2017). Next, we generalized the AB model from social
and job groups to the 48 US mainland states (two studies, N ~1,800, two similar-
ity measures; Koch, Kervyn, Kervyn, & Imhoff, in press). Taken together, these
lines of data-driven research provided support for a third fundamental dimension
of meaning overlooked in theory-driven research on the Big Two: B as in beliefs
ranging from conservative to progressive.
In fact, B became known decades before our work, namely as tight-loose in
Peabody’s person perception and group stereotypes models (1985; see Peeters,
2008), as backward-modern in Jones and Ashmore’s (1973) group stereotypes
model, as conservation-openness to change in Schwartz and Bilsky’s (1987) human
values model, as conventional-alternative in Pattyn and colleagues’ (2013) person
perception model, etc. These likewise data-driven research lines further support B
as a fundamental dimension of meaning.
The Big Two model is supported by effects of AC information on social cog-
nition. Thus, for B to be seriously considered as a fundamental dimension, B
information should influence social cognition, too. First, we showed effects of
B information on social inference. People inferred that members of social groups
more similar in stereotypic B are more likely to be in the same place at the same
time, and are more likely to like one another (Koch, Imhoff, et al., 2016, see
Study 7). People also inferred that citizens of US states more similar in stereotypic
172 Alex Koch and Roland Imhoff
B are more likely to like one another (Koch et al., in press, see Study 3). Next,
we showed effects of B information on source memory. People read statements
by members of stereotypically conservative and progressive job groups and later
guessed who had said what. They confused members of different conservative
(progressive) groups more often than conservatives with progressives and vice
versa ( Imhoff et al., 2017, see Study 3). We also showed effects of B information on
attitude generalization. People recalled positive or negative police officer behav-
iors and then evaluated all 150 basic, distinct job groups listed by the US Depart-
ment of Labor. Job groups more similar to police officers in stereotypic B were
evaluated more positively after recall of positive compared to negative police offi-
cer behavior ( Imhoff et al., 2017, see Study 4). In sum, B information influenced
social inference, source memory, and attitude generalization (N ~1,000), further
raising our confidence that B is a fundamental dimension of meaning.
average, and high in A should disagree on targets’ C, and the relation between
targets’ A and C should actually not be curvilinear for everybody but negative
and positive for perceivers who see the self as low and high in A, respectively. So,
inconsistent with the original ABC model of fundamental meaning, the relation
between targets’ A and C might depend on perceivers’ A, but Koch, Imhoff, and
colleagues (2016) overlooked this controversy because they had analyzed mean
rather than individual A and C ratings. The same might have been the case for the
relation between targets’ B and C. We tested this next.
In four studies (N ~1,000) conducted in the US, Germany, India, and South
Africa, people rated psychologically relevant groups on A, B, or C twice, allowing
to decompose the total A, B, and C variance into three distinct shares: differences
between groups that people agreed on (consensus), group-unspecific differences
between people (controversy #1), and group-specific differences between people
(controversy #2). As predicted, people disagreed on groups’ C but not so much on
A and B. In four additional studies (N ~1,000), US Americans rated psychologically
prevalent US groups on A, B, and C. We explained groups’ C by groups’ A and
B, people’s self-rated A and B, and the interactions target A*perceiver A and target
B*perceiver B. The two interaction terms were significant, sizable, and supported
our updated theorizing that targets’ C increases with perceiver-target similarity in
A and B. As predicted, for perceivers low (high) in A the relation between targets’
A and C was negative (positive). Likewise, for perceivers conservative (progressive)
in B the relation between targets’ progressiveness and C was negative (positive).
In sum, reexamining the nature and relation of prioritized dimensions, we
took a data-driven approach, focused on social groups as targets, and advanced
our knowledge in three steps. First, we rediscovered B and found that A and B
are orthogonal dimensions. Second, we found that across perceivers of all kinds,
targets’ C increased with their averageness in A and B, respectively (Koch, Imhoff,
et al., 2016). And third, impressions of C turned out to increase with perceiver-
target similarity in A and B. Thus, C peaking for targets average in A and B only
applied for perceivers who themselves were average in A and B. For perceivers
extreme in A and/or B, C instead peaked for targets extreme in A and/or B in the
same way as the respective perceiver (e.g., perceivers conservative in B trusted the
most in targets also conservative in B; see Figure 14.2). We revised the ABC model
accordingly, concluding that there is controversy on C (Koch, Nicolas, et al., 2017).
Peak C
(high A) Rich
Politicians
Celebrities Upper class
Conservatives
Republicans
White collar
Christians
Athletes Religious
Jocks
Democrats Whites
Jews
Liberals Women Men
Parents Elderly Peak C
Peak C (cons B)
(average
Middle class Asians
AB)
Peak C
Muslims
(prog B) Nerds Students
Lesbians Hipsters Teenagers
Homosexuals Goths Hispanic Blue collar
Transgender Hippies Working class
Blacks
Immigrants
Lower class
Poor
Homeless
Drug addicts
Peak C
(low A)
FIGURE 14.2 Communion depends on people’s own positon in the groups’ mean
similarity map.
Koch, Imhoff, and colleagues (2016), for example, predicted social groups’
mean C ratings from the groups’ coordinates in people’s mean similarity map
(i.e., people’s mean spatial arrangement solution). However, people disagree on
groups’ C (Koch, Nicolas, et al., 2017). Thus, aggregating C ratings across people
separately for each group leveled out a lot of within-person variance in groups’
C (e.g., person 1 trusts Democrats but not Republicans, whereas person 2 trusts
Republicans but not Democrats; aggregating C ratings across persons 1 and 2, the
two groups will appear equally trusted), possibly obscuring people’s spontaneous
usage of C (i.e., little variance in groups’ mean C ratings, little covariance with
the groups’ coordinates in people’s mean similarity map). Aggregating spatially
arranged similarity across people separately for each unique pair of groups possibly
Rethinking Fundamental Dimensions of Meaning 177
also leveled out a lot of within-person variance in groups’ C. For example, three
persons spontaneously use C to judge three groups’ similarity. Person 1 trusts ath-
letes and gamers (distrusts politicians) and thus drags them to one end (the other
end) of the screen, person 2 trusts athletes and politicians (distrusts gamers) and
thus drags them to one end (the other end) of the screen, and person 3 trusts gam-
ers and politicians (distrusts athletes) and thus drags them to one end (the other
end) of the screen. Aggregated across persons 1–3, the three unique pairs of groups
that can be formed with the groups will appear equally similar, again obscuring
people’s spontaneous usage of C (i.e., little variance in groups’ mean pairwise simi-
larity, little covariance with the groups’ mean C ratings).
To test whether aggregating C ratings and spatial similarity arrangement across
people obscured their spontaneous usage of C, in two new studies (N ~400) we
circumnavigated aggregation by predicting groups’ (A, B, and) C as rated by single
individuals by the groups’ coordinates in the same single individuals’ spatial simi-
larity arrangement map. Results confirmed that people spontaneously used A, B,
as well as C to spatially arrange the groups, a major step towards reconciling the
ABC model (Koch, Imhoff, et al., 2016) with the Big Two model (e.g., Fiske et al.,
2002; Fiske, this volume) claiming that people prioritize A and C to make sense of
the social world. As we found that different people used different combinations of
dimensions, one important avenue for future research is to clarify when and why
people use A, B, and/or C to differentiate social groups, job groups, etc.
Conclusion
The data-driven research presented in this chapter explored the meaning dimension(s)
that people prioritize to mentally organize and compare multiple targets in
ambiguous populations (e.g., social groups). To this end, in addition to free
choice of dimension(s) our study designs included free choice of targets, too. For
the population of social (and job) groups, we conclude four points: (1) In addi-
tion to agency and communion, a third prioritized dimension is beliefs ranging
from conservative to progressive. (2) Perceiving others as conservative-progressive
serves the purpose to balance exploitation (routine, safe choices) and explora-
tion (alternative, risky choices). (3) Communion increases with perceiver-target
similarity in agency and beliefs. Thus, the relation between targets’ agency and
communion is negative, curvilinear, and positive for perceivers low, average, and
high in agency, respectively. Likewise, the relation between targets’ progressiveness
and communion is negative, curvilinear, and positive for perceivers conservative,
neutral, and progressive in beliefs, respectively. As a result, there is controversy on
who is communal. (4) People spontaneously use A, B, and C to mentally organize
and compare targets.
Points 3 and 4 resulted from a collaboration with Susan Fiske’s lab at Princeton
University and Vincent Yzerbyt’s lab at the Catholic University Louvain-la-Neuve.
We are thankful for their input and look forward to further reexaminations of the
nature and relation of prioritized meaning.
178 Alex Koch and Roland Imhoff
References
Abele, A. E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”? Preferential pro-
cessing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 935–948.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self
versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 751–763.
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition:
A dual perspective model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 195–255.
Alves, H., Koch, A. S., & Unkelbach, C. (2016). My friends are all alike: The relation
between liking and perceived similarity in person perception. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 62, 103–117.
Alves, H., Koch, A. S., & Unkelbach, C. (2017a). The “common good” phenomenon: Why
similarities are positive and differences are negative. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 146, 512–528.
Alves, H., Koch, A. S., & Unkelbach, C. (2017b). Why good is more alike than bad: Pro-
cessing implications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 72–82.
Alves, H., Unkelbach, C., Burghardt, J., Koch, A. S., Krüger, T., & Becker, V. D. (2015). A
density explanation of valence asymmetries in recognition memory. Memory & Cogni-
tion, 43, 896–909.
Wojciszke, B., Bazinska, R., & Jaworski, M. (1998). On the dominance of moral categories
in impression formation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1251–1263.
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to
future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.
Brunswik, E. (1955). Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psy-
chology. Psychological Review, 62, 193–217.
Cislak, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2008). Agency and communion are inferred from actions
serving interests of self or others. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1103–1110.
Cuddy, A. J., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2007). The BIAS map: behaviors from intergroup
affect and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 631–648.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype
content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and com-
petition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.
Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., Verplanken, B., & Maio, G. R. (2012). Communal narcissism.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(5), 854–878.
Grant, A. M., & Schwartz, B. (2011). Too much of a good thing: The challenge and oppor-
tunity of the inverted U. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 61–76.
Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., & Alford, J. R. (2014). Differences in negativity bias underlie
variations in political ideology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 297–307.
Hout, M. C., Papesh, M. H., & Goldinger, S. D. (2013). Multidimensional scaling. Wiley
Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4, 93–103.
Imhoff, R., & Koch, A. (2017). How orthogonal are the Big Two of social perception? On
the curvilinear relation between agency and communion. Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 12, 122–137.
Imhoff, R., Koch, A., & Flade, F. (In press). (Pre)occupations: A data-driven map of job
stereotypes and their consequences for categorization and evaluation. Journal of Experi-
mental Social Psychology.
Jones, R. A., & Ashmore, R. D. (1973). The structure of intergroup perception: Categories
and dimensions in views of ethnic groups and adjectives used in stereotype research.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25, 428–438.
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political ideology: Its structure, func-
tions, and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 307–337.
Rethinking Fundamental Dimensions of Meaning 179
Koch, A., Alves, H., Krüger, T., & Unkelbach, C. (2016). A general valence asymmetry
in similarity: Good is more alike than bad. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 42, 1171–1192.
Koch, A., Imhoff, R., Dotsch, R., Unkelbach, C., & Alves, H. (2016). The ABC of stereo-
types about groups: Agency/socioeconomic success, conservative-progressive beliefs,
and communion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 675–709.
Koch, A., Imhoff, R., Dotsch, R., Unkelbach, C., & Alves, H. (2017). Minding conservative-
progressive beliefs is for balancing exploitation and exploration. Manuscript in preparation.
Koch, A., Kervyn, N., Kervyn, M., & Imhoff, R. (in press). Studying the cognitive map of
the U.S. states: Ideology and prosperity stereotypes predict interstate prejudice. Social
Psychological and Personality Science.
Koch, A., Nicolas, G., Imhoff, R., Unkelbach, C., Terache, J., Yzerbyt, V., & Fiske, S.
(2017). Stereotypes about groups’ communion/warmth are not consensual: Implications for rec-
onciling the ABC of stereotypes with the SCM. Manuscript in preparation.
Lammers, J., Koch, A., Conway, P., & Brandt, M. J. (in press). The political domain
appears simpler to the politically extreme than to political moderates. Social Psychological
and Personality Science.
Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., ...
Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon
Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 75–84.
Pattyn, S., Rosseel, Y., & Van Hiel, A. (2013). Finding our way in the social world: Explor-
ing the dimensions underlying social classification. Social Psychology, 44, 329–348.
Peabody, D. (1985). National characteristics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Peeters, G. (2008). The evaluative face of a descriptive model: Communion and agency in
Peabody’s tetradic model of trait organization. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38,
1066–1072.
Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a universal psychological structure of human
values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 550–562.
Sczesny, S. (2003). A closer look beneath the surface: Various facets of the think-manager–
think-male stereotype. Sex Roles, 49 (7–8), 353–363.
Tversky, A. (1977). Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84, 327–352.
Uchronski, M. (2008). Agency and communion in spontaneous self-descriptions: Occur-
rence and situational malleability. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1093–1102.
Wojciszke, B., Baryla, W., Parzuchowski, M., Szymkow, A., & Abele, A. E. (2011). Self-
esteem is dominated by agentic over communal information. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 41, 617–627.
Ybarra, O., Chan, E., Park, H., Burnstein, E., Monin, B., & Stanik, C. (2008). Life’s recur-
ring challenges and the fundamental dimensions: An integration and its implications for
cultural differences and similarities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1083–1092.
INDEX
Note: Page numbers in italics indicate figures and in bold indicate tables on the corresponding
pages.