Case Formulation
Case Formulation
Kuyken W, Padesky CA, Dudley R. The science and practice of case conceptualization. Behavioural and
Cognitive Psychotherapy 2008. ,36 6 757-768.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1352465808004815
Use Policy:
The full-text may be used and/or reproduced and given to third parties in any format or medium,
without prior permission or charge, for personal research or study, educational, or not for profit
purposes provided that:
The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the
copyright holders.
Robinson Library, University of Newcastle upon Tyne, Newcastle upon Tyne. NE1
7RU. Tel. 0191 222 6000
Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 2008, 36, 757–768
Printed in the United Kingdom First published online 30 September 2008 doi:10.1017/S1352465808004815
PROCESS ISSUES
Willem Kuyken
University of Exeter, UK
Christine A. Padesky
Robert Dudley
Newcastle University, UK
Reprint requests to Willem Kuyken, Mood Disorders Centre, School of Psychology, University of Exeter, Exeter EX4
4QG, UK. E-mail: [email protected]
Introduction
Case conceptualization is described as the “the heart of evidence-based practice” (Bieling
and Kuyken, 2003; p. 53). Most CBT therapists agree that case conceptualization is a core
competency, but there are different views about how to use case conceptualization in practice
(Flitcroft, James, Freeston and Wood-Mitchell, 2007).
Consider the case of Beth (age 20), admitted to a residential unit following an escalating
pattern of self-injury. Her presenting issues included self-injury, post-traumatic stress disorder
(PTSD) and depression. Beth’s assessment data suggested that her mother had longstanding
and severe substance abuse problems. Her father and then a step-father sexually abused Beth
starting when she was 6 years old and continuing until she ran away from home at aged 16.
Throughout childhood, Beth saw school as a refuge and she formed an important relationship
with a female English teacher who supported Beth’s love of poetry. During secondary school
she formed positive relationships with some female peers, most of whom worked on the
school’s in-house student magazine. When Beth left home she moved in with a man in his 20s
who was substance dependent; while intoxicated he could become violent. Beth confided that
she had witnessed frequent domestic violence between her parents and was now entrenched
in a pattern of violence in her own relationship.
Beth’s therapist faces a range of issues commonly faced by CBT therapists.
• Given Beth’s presenting issues, what should be the primary therapy focus?
• In what order should the presenting issues be tackled?
• How do Beth’s presenting issues relate to one another, if at all?
• What CBT protocols are relevant here? What treatment course do I follow if no particular
protocol seems appropriate?
• Should I work with Beth’s history, which seems at face value to be immediately relevant
to her presenting issues? How do I work with her history without exacerbating her post-
traumatic stress disorder?
• What is the goal of the therapy? Beth is apparently a survivor of sustained and chronic abuse
and disadvantage. Is alleviation of her symptomatic distress sufficient or is it important to
include a goal to help rebuild and promote more successful functioning in the future?
In short, Beth’s therapist faces the primary question each CBT therapist asks at the beginning
of therapy: “How do I use my training and experience along with evidence-based therapy
approaches to help this person with these particular issues presented at this time?” For many
clinicians the answer is, “It depends upon the case conceptualization.” And yet research
suggests that different therapists are likely to construct different case conceptualizations to
understand Beth’s presenting issues (Kuyken, Fothergill, Musa and Chadwick, 2005).
This paper describes a new model for case conceptualization that helps answer the questions
above and addresses many of the challenges posed by poor inter-therapist reliability regarding
case conceptualization. First, we offer a definition of case conceptualization1 along with a
summary of the available research. Then we describe a new approach to case conceptualization
that uses the metaphor of a case conceptualization crucible in which a client’s particular
history and presentation is synthesized with theory and research to produce a description and
1 We use the term “case conceptualization” but regard this as synonymous with the term “case formulation”.
Case conceptualization 759
understanding of clients’ presenting issues that can be used to inform therapy. We draw out
implications for training and recommendations for future research.
Is CBT case conceptualization reliable? That is to say, can therapists agree with each other
when asked to conceptualize the same case using the same conceptualization format?
While CBT therapists tend to agree on more descriptive levels of conceptualization (e.g.
the presenting issues), reliability becomes poor at levels requiring greater inference (e.g.
central beliefs, maintenance factors) (e.g. Kuyken et al., 2005; Mumma and Smith, 2001;
Persons, Mooney and Padesky, 1995). There is some emerging evidence that training, therapist
experience, competence and use of more structured conceptualization schematics improve
reliability (Kuyken et al., 2005; Persons and Bertagnolli, 1999). This emergent consensus has
important implications delineated in our approach to case conceptualization below.
Are CBT case conceptualizations valid? That is, do conceptualizations relate meaningfully
to clients’ experience, are they internally consistent/coherent and can they be cross-validated
with other measures of clients’ experiences?
Research in this area has only emerged recently (e.g. Mumma and Mooney, 2007). Several
studies converge on the finding that CBT therapists with greater expertise are more likely to
produce conceptualizations that are higher quality in terms of being more coherent, elaborated,
and concise (Kuyken et al., 2005; Mumma and Mooney, 2007). Similarly, focused training in
case conceptualization improves the coherence and quality of case conceptualization across
therapy modalities (Kendjelic and Eells, 2007). There is no research to date bearing on the
question of whether CBT conceptualizations can be cross-validated with other measures of
clients’ experiences. Additionally, of course, if CBT conceptualizations are not reliable in
content, they cannot achieve standards of validity.
2006; Schneider and Byrne, 1987; Strauman et al., 2006). Resolving this disparity in the
research literature is important to forming a conceptual basis for case conceptualization; we
return to this issue when we describe our approach to case conceptualization.
develop conceptualizations that evolve over time in the context of new information and client
responses to therapy interventions.
Initial conceptualizations are typically quite descriptive; therapists assess clients’ presenting
issues and help the client describe these issues in cognitive and behavioural terms. Beth (the
client described at the beginning of this chapter) and her therapist agreed to focus on three
goals: learn alternatives to self-injury when she felt distressed; reduce her PTSD symptoms;
and return to college. As Beth was able to describe her presenting issues in the context of her
current situation and past history of abuse, these experiences became less frightening to her
and her self-destructive behaviours became more understandable. Beth’s highest distress was
associated with the severity and frequency of her PTSD symptoms and she currently managed
these emotions by cutting herself. Thus, Beth and her therapist chose PTSD and self-harm as
the initial foci of therapy.
Following initial descriptions of presenting issues, case conceptualizations become more
explanatory, identifying triggers and maintenance factors. Here disorder-specific models or
generic approaches like functional analysis (e.g. Kohlenberg and Tsai, 1991) are typically
used. Beth’s therapist used CBT models of PTSD (Ehlers and Clark, 2000), depression (Clark,
Beck and Alford, 1999) and anger (Beck, 2002) to inform Beth’s conceptualization.
For example, Figure 2 shows the first explanatory conceptualization Beth and her therapist
sketched out. It synthesizes a contemporary approach to PTSD (Ehlers and Clark, 2000;
Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell, 2005) with Beth’s experiences to provide an
understanding of how her intrusive images and memories are triggered and how the cutting and
PTSD are maintained (Weierich and Nock, 2008). By sketching this schematic diagram with
Case conceptualization 763
Figure 2. Conceptualization of triggers and maintenance for Beth’s PTSD symptoms and cutting
behaviour to inform treatment plan
Beth in the early phases of therapy she was able to see the links between seemingly disparate
experiences (her memories, PTSD symptoms, self-blaming thoughts and cutting behaviour).
This conceptualization normalized her experiences (“I thought I was going mad”), engaged
her in therapy by providing hope, and provided the rationale for early therapy interventions
focused on exposure and reframing beliefs associated with her trauma memories.
In middle and later stages of CBT, conceptualization uses higher levels of inference to
explain how predisposing and protective factors contribute to clients’ presenting issues.
Predisposing factors help explain why a client is vulnerable to their presenting issues. Protective
factors highlight strengths that can be used to build resilience as described in our third principle
below. Figure 3 shows the type of conceptualization that might be derived throughout therapy
as Beth and her therapist build a shared understanding of Beth’s beliefs and strategies in the
context of her developmental history.
We maintain that one of the reasons research regarding the reliability and acceptability of
CBT conceptualization is not more positive is that typically these studies are based on relatively
unilateral therapist-derived conceptualizations presented to the client in a given therapy session
(e.g. Evans and Parry, 1996). We propose that when conceptualization is collaborative, clients
are more likely to provide checks and balances to therapist reasoning errors, feel ownership
of the emerging conceptualization, and perceive a compelling rationale for treatment.
Empiricism refers to: (i) making use of relevant CBT theory and research in
conceptualizations; and (ii) using an empirical approach in therapy that is based on observation,
evaluation of experience, and learning. At the heart of empiricism is a commitment to using
the best available theory and research within case conceptualizations. Given the substantial
evidence base for many disorder-specific CBT approaches, often there will be a close match
between client experience and theory. For example, a person presenting with panic attacks can
normally benefit greatly from jointly mapping his or her panic experiences onto contemporary
CBT models of panic disorder (Clark, 1986; Craske and Barlow, 2001).
Nonetheless, even when a CBT model is closely matched to a client’s presenting issues it
is important to collaboratively derive the case conceptualization so the client understands the
applicability of the model to his or her issues. Also, throughout therapy, conceptualizations
inform choice points regarding treatment options and resolution of therapy challenges. An
individualized conceptualization will more helpfully inform these choice points. When clients
experience multiple or more complex presenting issues it is often not possible to map directly
to one particular theory and still provide a coherent and comprehensive conceptualization that
is acceptable to the client. Nonetheless, as shown in the case of Beth, the cognitive theory of
PTSD was able to significantly inform work on two of her three goals, reduction of self-harm
Case conceptualization 765
and PTSD symptoms (Figure 2). A more generic longitudinal conceptualization helped her
work towards her third goal, returning to her studies (Figure 3).
Another aspect of empiricism is the use of empirical approaches to clinical decision making.
Therapists and clients develop hypotheses, devise adequate tests for these hypotheses, and then
adapt the hypotheses based on feedback from therapy interventions. This makes CBT an active
and dynamic process, in which the conceptualization guides and is corrected by feedback from
the results of active observations, experimentation and change. An early focus on Beth’s PTSD
symptoms led to rapid therapy gains; these improvements provided support for the validity
and utility of her initial conceptualization (Figure 2).
Case conceptualization requires integration of complex information within the case con-
ceptualization crucible. Moreover, as therapy progresses, therapists and clients typically make
greater inferences as they develop explanatory conceptualizations using data from the client’s
developmental history. We contend that one of the reasons research has failed to demonstrate
that individualization improves therapy outcomes is that the principle of collaborative
empiricism is not practised effectively to manage this complexity. Dietmar Schulte and his
colleagues conducted a series of studies to examine the reasons why deviations from treatment
manuals tend to compromise outcomes (Schulte and Eifert, 2002). They found that therapists
tend to move away from therapy methods (e.g. exposure) to therapy process (e.g. addressing
patients’ motivation) too soon, too often and sometimes for the wrong reasons. Therapists’
decisions about when to deviate from therapy manuals should be made (a) empirically and
(b) collaboratively because these two types of processes allow corrective feedback.
apt metaphor for understanding how to conceptualize an individual’s resilience (Figure 1).
Appropriate theory can be integrated with the particularities of an individual case using the
heat of collaborative empiricism. Because resilience is a broad multi-dimensional concept,
therapists can either adapt existing theories of psychological disorders or draw from a large
array of theoretical ideas in positive psychology (e.g. Snyder and Lopez, 2005).
Beth and her therapist developed a conceptualization of her resilience that recognized her
success in navigating the adversity she experienced: “I did survive the worst of it, sort of”
she acknowledge early in therapy. In middle and later stages of CBT, the conceptualization of
Beth’s resilience informed her decision to set a goal of completing college. Building on her
constructive beliefs (“If I show attitude, I can succeed; others like me”) and strategies (reading
and writing poetry; cultivating “attitude”) she was able to articulate the steps involved in
returning to college (see the left side of Figure 3). Once she succeeded in her admission to
college, Beth was able to use her skills to surmount various obstacles. Subsequent success
experiences reinforced her beliefs and strategies, strengthening her resilience.
It is possible that the research examining the impact of conceptualization on therapy process
and outcome might be more compelling if conceptualizations included client strengths and
resilience. For example, we propose that clients are less likely to find conceptualization
overwhelming and distressing when conceptualization is as much about what is right with
them as about the problematic issues that lead them to seek help. Moreover, as Beth’s case
illustrates, conceptualizations of resilience offer natural pathways toward client goals.
Conclusion
We concur with commentators (Beck, 1995; Butler, 1998) who assert that case concep-
tualization is a foundation of CBT. However, we argue that we need a new approach to case
conceptualization that has the potential to address some of the clinical and empirical challenges
therapists face. Specifically, we advocate therapists navigate clinical and conceptualization
challenges using three guiding principles: collaborative empiricism, evolving levels of
conceptualization and incorporation of client strengths. This approach, briefly outlined here,
is developed fully in Kuyken et al. (2008).
References
Beck, A. T. (2002). Prisoners of hate. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 209–216.
Beck, A. T., Emery, G. and Greenberg, R. L. (1985). Anxiety Disorders and Phobias: a cognitive
perspective. New York: Basic Books.
Beck, A. T., Freeman, A., Davis, D. and Associates (2003). Cognitive Therapy of Personality Disorders.
(2nd ed.) New York: Guilford.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F. and Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression. New
York: Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T., Wright, F. D., Newman, C. F. and Liese, B. S. (1993). Cognitive Therapy of Substance
Abuse. New York: Guilford.
Beck, J. S. (1995). Cognitive Therapy: basics and beyond. New York: Guilford Press.
Beck, J. S. (2005). Cognitive Therapy for Challenging Problems. New York: Guilford.
Bieling, P. J. and Kuyken, W. (2003). Is cognitive case formulation science or science fiction? Clinical
Psychology-Science and Practice, 10, 52–69.
Case conceptualization 767
Butler, G. (1998). Clinical formulation. In A. S. Bellack and M. Hersen (Eds.), Comprehensive Clinical
Psychology (pp. 1–24). New York: Pergamon Press.
Chadwick, P., Williams, C. and Mackenzie, J. (2003). Impact of case formulation in cognitive behaviour
therapy for psychosis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 671–680.
Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T. and Alford, B. A. (1999). Scientific Foundations of Cognitive Theory and
Therapy of Depression. New York: Wiley.
Clark, D. M. (1986). A cognitive approach to panic. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 461–470.
Craske, M. G. and Barlow, D. H. (2001). Panic disorder and agoraphobia. In D. H. Barlow (Ed.),
Clinical Handbook of Psychological Disorders: a step-by-step treatment manual (3rd ed., pp. 1–59).
New York: Wiley.
Eells, T. D. (2007). Handbook of Psychotherapy Case Formulation. (2nd ed.) New York: Guilford Press.
Ehlers, A. and Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 38, 319–345.
Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Hackmann, A., McManus, F. and Fennell, M. (2005). Cognitive therapy
for post-traumatic stress disorder: development and evaluation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43,
413–431.
Evans, J. and Parry, G. (1996). The impact of reformulation in cognitive-analytic therapy with difficult-
to-help clients. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 3, 109–117.
Flitcroft, A., James, I. A., Freeston, M. and Wood-Mitchell, A. (2007). Determining what is important
in a good formulation. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 35, 325–333.
Ghaderi, A. (2006). Does individualization matter? A randomized trial of standardized (focused)
versus individualized (broad) cognitive behavior therapy for bulimia nervosa. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 44, 273–288.
Kendjelic, E. M. and Eells, T. D. (2007). Generic psychotherapy case formulation training improves
formulation quality. Psychotherapy, 44, 66–77.
Kohlenberg, R. J. and Tsai, M. (1991). Functional Analytic Psychotherapy: creating intense and
curative therapeutic relationships. New York: Springer.
Kuyken, W. (2006). Evidence-based case formulation: is the emperor clothed? In N. Tarrier (Ed.), Case
Formulation in Cognitive Behaviour Therapy. Hove: Brunner-Routlege.
Kuyken, W., Fothergill, C. D., Musa, M. and Chadwick, P. (2005). The reliability and quality of
cognitive case formulation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1187–1201.
Kuyken, W., Kurzer, N., DeRubeis, R. J., Beck, A. T. and Brown, G. K. (2001). Response to cognitive
therapy in depression: the role of maladaptive beliefs and personality disorders. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 69, 560–566.
Kuyken, W., Padesky, C. A. and Dudley, R. (2008). Collaborative Case Conceptualization: working
effectively with clients in cognitive-behavioral therapy. New York: Guilford Press.
Mumma, G. H. and Mooney, S. R. (2007). Comparing the validity of alternative cognitive case
formulations: a latent variable, multivariate time series approach. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
31, 451–481.
Mumma, G. H. and Smith, J. L. (2001). Cognitive-behavioral-interpersonal scenarios: interformulator
reliability and convergent validity. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23, 203–
221.
Nelson-Gray, R. O., Herbert, J. D., Herbert, D. L., Sigmon, S. T. and Brannon, S. E. (1989).
Effectiveness of matched, mismatched, and package treatments of depression. Journal of Behavior
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 20, 281–294.
Padesky, C. A. and Greenberger, D. (1995). Clinician’s Guide to Mind over Mood. New York: Guilford
Press.
Pain, C. M., Chadwick, P. and Abba, N. (2008). Clients’ experience of case formulation in cognitive
behaviour therapy for psychosis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 127–138.
Persons, J. B. (1989). Cognitive Therapy in Practice: a case formulation approach. New York: Norton.
768 W. Kuyken et al.