Acab 060
Acab 060
Abstract
The evolution of the concept of a multicomponent working memory is described with particular reference to the contribution from
neuropsychology. Early evidence from patients with the classic amnesic syndrome, together with others showing the opposite
deficit of impaired short-term but preserved long-term memory argued strongly for a separation between long- and short-term
memory systems. Simulation of the short-term deficit in healthy participants using a dual task approach suggested the need to
assume a three component system serving as a multi-purpose working memory comprising an overall attentional control system,
the Central Executive, aided by separable temporary buffer stores for phonological and visuospatial information. An account is
then given of the way in which evidence from patients was combined with the study of healthy participants to test and expand the
model over subsequent years. This led to the need to propose a fourth component, the Episodic Buffer, a system that combines
information from multiple sources and makes it accessible to conscious awareness. I conclude with a brief account of how the
multicomponent approach resembles and differs from that of other current models of working memory.
Keywords: Working memory; Phonological loop; Visuospatial sketchpad; Central executive; Amnesia; Frontal lobes
I am delighted to receive this award, particularly since I cannot claim to be a neuropsychologist. I am however someone
who has benefited greatly during a career of over 50 years from the opportunity to work with a succession of neuropsychologists
all of whom have combined their considerable clinical skills with an interest in applying developments in cognitive psychology
to understanding neuropsychological deficits. I spent 30 years of my research life at the Medical Research Council Applied
Psychology Unit in Cambridge, England which had the remit of combining basic and applied research (Baddeley, 2019). My
neuropsychological research follows this pattern with more applied work principally involving long-term memory (LTM),
much of it concerned with developing tests that apply new developments within cognitive psychology to clinical problems, a
commitment that still continues (Baddeley, Hitch, & Allen, 2019, 2020; Baddeley, Atkinson, Hitch, & Allen, 2021; Laverick
et al., in press), while other work was more theoretically driven, using the rich data from neuropsychological patients to test and
develop cognitive theory. This has been particularly prominent in developing the multicomponent model of working memory.
It is this rather than my more directly applied but more sporadic involvement with LTM that forms the rest of my presentation.
My interest in memory followed a PhD funded by the Royal Mail who were interested in optimizing the design of postal
codes. This was followed by a project on evaluating the quality of telephone lines by requiring people to briefly remember
messages that were presented either in quiet or in noise, the assumption being that noisy speech would be hard to store and
then recall. This was linked to the discovery by my supervisor that short-term memory (STM) appeared to be acoustically based
(Conrad, 1964) and that acoustic similarity between remembered letter sequences (bgvtcp versus kwrsyq) had a major effect on
recall accuracy, even when the letters were presented visually (Conrad & Hull, 1964). If STM was indeed an acoustically based
system the perhaps it would be very sensitive to the auditory quality of the input?
1 Based on the lecture given on receipt of Distinguished Lifetime Contribution to Neuropsychology Award at the 2020 meeting of the National Academy of Neuropsychology on October 16, 2020.
© The Author(s) 2021. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected].
https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acab060 Advance Access publication 23 July 2021
862 A. D. Baddeley / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 36 (2021); 861–873
My own study therefore looked at the effect of input noise on STM but took the further steps switching from letters to words
(e.g., Mat, Can, Man etc. vs. Pit, Day, Cow etc.). This allowed me to evaluate the possibility that any kind of similarity might
have an equivalent effect presenting a major objection to Conrad’s argument for a purely acoustically based store. To test this
also included sequences of adjectives that were either semantically similar (Huge, Big, Large etc.) or dissimilar (e.g., Deep,
Old, Wet etc.). I asked my participants to remember sequences of five words and did indeed find a dramatic effect of acoustic
similarity with around 80% correct for dissimilar words versus 10% for similar sequences, while similarity of meaning showed
a minimal effect (75% vs. 65%, happily supporting my boss’s theory of an acoustic STM system. My second step was to show
that this pattern completely reversed when list length was increase to 10 words requiring several trials to learn. This suggested
the simple conclusion that while STM depended on acoustic coding, LTM was semantically based. This bought me a ticket into
the hot theoretical controversy of the time, whether it was necessary to assume more than one kind of memory. In retrospect,
I realize that this was the true origin of my lifetime preoccupation with working memory, the system that holds information in
mind while thinking.
Throughout the next few years, while maintaining an interest in neuropsychology, my main focus of attention was on the lively
controversy as to the nature and function of STM. This was a very active area with a range of new experimental paradigms being
developed, often with accompanying mathematical or computational models. Much of this work was subsequently captured in
what became known as the “modal model,” presented in an influential paper by Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968). This assumed
that information flowed through a number of sensory memory systems, often termed iconic memory for the visual and echoic
memory for the auditory, before feeding into a short-term store (STS) which then transferred information to LTM. The STS was
assumed to act as working memory, being responsible not only for holding information while it is transferred to LTM, but also for
performing a range of control processes including rehearsal, choice of strategy and decision as to how and when to respond. The
modal model continued to feature in standard texts for many years, but by the early 1970s was already encountering problems,
of which two were substantial.
The first problem involved the assumption that merely holding information in the STS was sufficient to ensure transfer to
LTM; it rapidly became clear that mere time in store was relatively unimportant compared to the operations performed on the
material, with deeper and more elaborate processing leading to better learning as suggested by Craik and Lockhart (1972) in their
Levels of Processing hypothesis. The second problem came from the previously described discovery by Shallice and Warrington
(1970) of patients who had grossly impaired STM, together with apparently normal LTM. How could information get into LTM
if the STM system was impaired? Furthermore, if the short-term system functioned as a general working memory, then such
patients should have extensive cognitive deficits, making everyday life problematic. This was not the case, making it unlikely
that the proposed STS performed all the activities proposed by the modal model. By this time, I had moved from the APU in
A. D. Baddeley / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 36 (2021); 861–873 863
Fig. 1. The original version of the multicomponent model proposed by Baddeley and Hitch (1974).
vocal or sub-vocal rehearsal. Our earlier work on the role of acoustic and semantic similarity implied some form of language-
related short-term storage system while evidence for the importance of rehearsal came from our demonstration of the word
length effect, the observation that verbal memory span is strongly dependent on the spoken length of the words to be recalled
(Baddeley, Thomson, & Buchanan, 1975). Thus when asked to immediately recall sequences of five words, participants recalled
about 90% when these were monosyllables (e.g., stick) but only about 50% when the words comprised five syllables (e.g.,
refrigerator). Furthermore, the relationship between word length and performance breaks down when articulation is disrupted
by the concurrent requirement to repeatedly utter even a single sound such as the word “the,” termed articulatory suppression.
Such suppression of rehearsal also eliminates the phonological similarity effect, but only when presentation is visual. We interpret
this as showing a direct route from audition to the acoustically based storage system, allowing the sound characteristics of the
input to be registered, though not actively rehearsed, implying that with visual presentation, articulation is necessary to convert
the stimulus from a visual to an acoustic code.
To summarize, our simple loop model assumed a memory trace registered in an acoustic or phonological store that fades unless
master any of a set of eight Italian-Russian pairs by the time all of the controls had learnt the whole set (Baddeley, Papagno, &
Vallar, 1988). This provided clear support to the hypothesis of the phonological loop as a language learning device.
In the absence of a second patient, we were unable to replicate directly, moving instead to an approach based on disrupting
the phonological loop in healthy participants. We did this initially by using articulatory suppression, finding that this did indeed
differentially impair acquisition of Finnish vocabulary items in a sample of English speaking participants (Papagno, Valentine,
& Baddeley, 1991) while Papagno and Vallar (1992) showed that, as predicted, foreign language acquisition was differentially
affected by both word length and phonological similarity within the novel word set.
This raised the question of whether a phonological loop limitation might lead to slower development of vocabulary in children.
Susan Gathercole and I were able to test a group of 8-year-old children diagnosed with specific language impairment (SLI) who
had the language development of 6-year-olds, together with normal nonverbal cognition. A preliminary study using existing
tests indicated a particular problem in repeating back unfamiliar spoken non words, a deficit that was not due to problems in
hearing or articulation. We went on to develop a test (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1996) in which the child hears and repeats non
words increasing in length from two syllables (e.g., ballop) to five (e.g., altupatory). We compared our SLI children with two
control groups, one comprised 8-year-olds with normal levels of both verbal and nonverbal development while the second group
comprised 6-year-olds who were matched with the 8 year-old SLI children on language level. The SLI group showed a marked
decline in performance as number of syllables increased, an effect that was substantially greater than that shown in either of the
control groups. (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990).
A further series of studies was concerned with the link between the phonological loop and vocabulary development in healthy
children. Here we found a robust association between our non-word repetition test and vocabulary, tested by speaking a word and
requiring the child to select the correct one of four pictured items. A longitudinal study suggested that this was driven initially
by phonological loop capacity with our non-word recognition test predicting vocabulary a year later rather than the reverse.
The association between non-word repetition and vocabulary is not limited to the initial years but as vocabulary develops the
association becomes reciprocal; having a good vocabulary enhances acquisition of new words (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989a,
1989b), while eventually other factors such as executive processing and exposure to a rich verbal environment become more
important. The non-word repetition test also proved to be closely predictive of problems with reading both at the individual
level (Baddeley & Wilson, 1993), and more broadly (Gathercole, Pickering, Knight, & Stegmann, 2004), leading at a practical
level to its extensive use in assessing reading problems in children. At a theoretical level, it fed into an overall review of the
evidence suggesting the phonological loop might operate more generally as a language learning device (Baddeley, Gathercole,
& Papagno, 1998) and suggesting the need to assume a direct link between temporary storage in the subsystems and long-term
learning (see Fig. 2).
866 A. D. Baddeley / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 36 (2021); 861–873
We proposed a visuo-spatial component to the original model, on the basis of earlier research where we had found a distinction
between visual STM and LTM in amnesic patients resembling its verbal equivalent (Warrington & Baddeley, 1974), and in
the case of visual STM had developed a measure of STM for complex patterns (Phillips & Baddeley, 1971). This was later
developed and published as the visual patterns test, a clinical measure that was shown to distinguish between the visual and
spatial components of working memory (Della Sala, Gray, Baddeley, Allamano, and Wilson (1999).
However, my first attempt to explore the concept of a visuospatial sketchpad systematically came from a study of visual
imagery that was prompted by an experience while on sabbatical leave in California. I became interested in American football
and was listening to the UCLA-Stanford college game while driving along the freeway. I had formed a clear image of the field
and the current play when I noticed that I was weaving from lane to lane and rapidly switched to music! On returning to the
UK I set out to simulate this experience. I did not have access to a driving simulator but instead used a pursuit rotor, an ancient
instrument whereby the participant attempts to keep a stylus in contact with a moving light spot. I was able to combine this
with a series of tasks involving visual or verbal STM, finding that that tracking disrupted the visuo-spatial but not the verbal
However, while this broad model might provide a useful distinction between two potential methods of action control it could
be argued that the SAS itself is simply another homunculus. Further development is thus needed if it is to prove to be theoretically
productive. Shallice chose to tackle the problem of modeling the SAS based an extensive range of neuropsychological studies
of patients with frontal lobe damage, culminating in a detailed computational model (Shallice, 1988, 2002). Lacking the
computational skills to adopt this method, I opted for a different approach. I proposed a list of basic capacities that were likely
to be needed by any model capable of fulfilling the functions of a central executive and went on to try to study them separately.
By systematically attempting to explain how the homunculus performs each of these functions I hoped that eventually with all
the functions explained, the homunculus would be able to retire (Baddeley, 1996, 1998).
We began with a basic question, does the central executive function as a unitary system, or can it be fractionated into separate
components? If so, what might these be? We assume that any adequate executive system would need to focus attention and
to inhibit the processing of irrelevant distractors. It would need to be able to divide attention when simultaneously performing
more than one activity and where necessary to switch attention between tasks. Finally it would require the capacity to access
the two tasks were made very easy. Subsequent studies showed that this was not simply a general deficit in attentional focus since
to the capacity of ad patients to sustain attention in a simple detection task proved equivalent to controls (Baddeley, Baddeley,
Bucks, & Wilcock, 2001; Baddeley, Cocchini, Della Sala, Logie, & Spinnler, 1999). Subsequent research has confirmed the
potential value of dual task performance as a means of early detection (Parra et al., 2009).
While we have by now accumulated extensive evidence for a deficit in dual task performance in ad patients, this does not
necessarily link this deficit either to a frontal lobe location or to lack of executive control of behavior more generally. This issue
was tackled by recruiting a total of 32 patients with well-established frontal lobe lesions, half of whom were independently
categorized as showing evidence of dysfunctional behavioral problems while half did not show such disturbance. All were tested
on dual task performance and on two standard frontal lobe performance measures, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST)
and verbal fluency and were independently rated on a range of carefully specified markers of dysexecutive behavior. When the
two groups were compared, the group with behavioral problems was significantly more impaired on dual task performance,
while no difference emerged between groups in their impaired fluency or WCST performance. Evidence from an association
held without a multidimensional coding system We were also puzzled by data from two highly intelligent but densely amnesic
patients who as expected scored zero on both the Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test and on the delayed prose recall of the
WMS. Atypically however, they performed well on immediate recall of the short story component of the Rivermead test with
scaled scores 12 and 9.5 (Baddeley & Wilson, 2002). How could our model of working memory hold an extended paragraph?
It was time for a rethink.
The result was the proposal of a fourth component to our model, the “Episodic Buffer” (Baddeley, 2000). This was assumed to
be a temporary storage system that was capable of maintaining information based on a range of different codes, bound together
into integrated episodes (see Fig. 3). We assumed that such coded information could arrive either directly through perception,
through the sketchpad or loop, or indirectly from LTM. I made the important further assumption that the contents of the buffer
were accessible to conscious awareness. A final tentative proposal was that access to the buffer was dependent entirely on the
central executive. A significant advantage of our new concept was that it formed a bridge between our own approach and that of
US colleagues notably Cowan’s (1988) embedded-processes model which assumes that working memory reflects the focus of
attention on an activated portion of LTM. The capacity to focus attention in Cowan’s model is broadly equivalent to the central
executive while the activated portion of LTM to the episodic buffer. In short, despite their apparent differences the two models
approach the question of attentional control in broadly similar ways (see relevant chapters in Logie, Camos, & Cowan, 2020 for
further discussion).
Much of our work over the last decade has been concerned with attempting to demonstrate that the concept of an episodic
buffer can indeed be fruitful in generating tractable questions that allow the model to be developed further. For example, we
began as mentioned earlier, with the assumption that the episodic buffer plays an active and essential role in binding features
together into episodes and that information for this is provided via the central executive. By systematically blocking the three
components of the model using dual task methods, we were however able to show that this was incorrect in two ways. First,
the central executive is not essential for providing access and secondly the process of binding typically occurs at points before
accessing the buffer. In the case of visual binding, it appears to occur during the later stages of visual processing while in the case
of language, it typically relies on linguistic processes in LTM. These modifications in turn led us to an area we had previously
neglected, namely a more detailed analysis of the attentional capacities of the central executive. The resulting evidence drove
us to propose a single executive system of limited attentional capacity which could however be divided and biased to focus
principally on either perceptual input or executive processing (Hitch, Allen, & Baddeley, 2020), a proposal that fits neatly with
conclusions from colleagues approaching the issue from the viewpoint of visual attention (Chun, Golomb, & Turk-Browne,
2011).
Our current model is summarized in Figure 4, which emphasizes the various sources of input from perception to working
memory (Baddeley et al., 2019, 2020). Two broad streams of information are proposed, each coming together in the visuospatial
870 A. D. Baddeley / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 36 (2021); 861–873
Fig. 4. The current model illustrating the flow of information from perception to the episodic buffer.
References
Alderman, N. (1996). Central executive deficit and response to operant conditioning methods. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 6, 161–186.
Allport, A. (1984). Auditory verbal short-term memory and conduction aphasia. In Bouma, H., & Bouwhuis, D. G. (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control
and language processes (, pp. 351–364). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
A. D. Baddeley / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 36 (2021); 861–873 871
Allport, A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting attentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In Umilta, C., & Moscovitch, M. (Eds.), Attention
and performance XV (, pp. 421–462). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In Spence, K. W., & Spence, J. T. (Eds.), The psychology
of learning and motivation: advances in research and theory (, Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). New York: Academic Press.
Attneave, F. (1960). In defense of homunculi. In Rosenblith, W. (Ed.), Sensory communication (, pp. 777–782). Cambridge, Mass: Holt, MIT Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1998). The central executive: A concept and some misconceptions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 523–526.
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.
Baddeley, A. D. (2002). Fractionating the central executive. In Stuss, D., & Knight, R. T. (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (, pp. 246–260). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (2019). Working memories: Postmen, divers and the cognitive revolution. Hove: Routledge.
Baddeley, A. D., Baddeley, H., Bucks, R., & Wilcock, G. K. (2001). Attentional control in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 124, 1492–1508.
Baddeley, A. D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R., & Spinnler, H. (1991). The decline of working memory in Alzheimer’s disease: A longitudinal study.
Brain, 114, 2521–2542.
Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. J. (2004). Executive attention, working memory capacity and two-factor theory of cognitive control. In Ross, B. (Ed.), The psychology
of Learning and motivation (, pp. 145–199). New York: Elsevier.
Farah, M. J., Hammond, K. M., Levine, D. N., & Calvanio, R. (1988). Visual and spatial mental imagery: Dissociable systems of representation. Cognitive
Psychology, 20(4), 439–462.
Gärtner, A., & Strobel, A. (2021). Individual differences in inhibitory control: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Cognition, 4, 17. 10.5334/joc.150.
Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1989a). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal
of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989b). The role of phonological memory in normal and disordered language development. In von Euler, C., Lundberg,
I., & Lennerstrand, G. (Eds.), Brain and reading (, pp. 245–255). London: Macmillan Press.
Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language-disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory &
Language, 29, 336–360.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1996). The childrens’ test of nonword repetition. London: Psychological Corporation UK.
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Knight, C., & Stegmann, Z. (2004). Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from National Curriculum
assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 40, 1–16.
Vallar, G., & Papagno, C. (2002). Neuropsychological impairments of verbal short-term memory. In Baddeley, A. D., Kopelman, M. D., & Wilson, B. A. (Eds.),
Handbook of memory disorders (2nd ed., pp. 249–270). Chichester: Wiley.
Warrington, E. K., & Baddeley, A. D. (1974). Amnesia and memory for visual location. Neuropsychologia, 12, 257–263.
Wilson, B. A., Baddeley, A. D., & Young, A. W. (1999). LE, a person who lost her "Mind’s Eye". Neurocase, 5, 119–127.
Wilson, B. A., Cockburn, J., Baddeley, A. D., & Hiorns, R. (1989). The development and validation of a test battery for detecting and monitoring everyday
memory problems. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 855–870.
Wilson, M., & Emmorey, K. (1998). A "word length effect" for sign language: Further evidence for the role of language in structuring working memory. Memory
& Cognition, 26, 584–590.