Linkage
Linkage
net/publication/272816248
CITATIONS READS
414 1,848
1 author:
Christian Lehmann
Universität Erfurt
125 PUBLICATIONS 2,997 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Christian Lehmann on 14 March 2016.
titulus
Towards a typology of clause linkage
huius textus situs retis mundialis
http://www.christianlehmann.eu/publ/linkage.pdf
annus publicationis
1988
paginae
181-225
Towards a typology
of clause linkage
Christian Lehmann
Abstract
The factors cross-linguistically relevant to clause linkage are surveyed in the form of a
couple of continua. These pertain to: 1. the integration of one clause into the other
(parameters of downgrading of the subordinate clause and of its syntactic level in the main
clause), 2. the expansion vs. reduction of the clauses (parameters of desententialization of the
subordinate clause and of grammaticalization of the main predicate) and 3. the mutual
isolation vs. linkage of the two clauses (parameters of interlacing of the clauses and of
explicitness of linking).
The six parameters hang together in various ways and constitute types of clause linkage.
They are founded in a functional theory of clause linkage which conceives complex sentence
formation as varying between the poles of elaboration and compression. This variation
fulfills the double-sided function of either unfolding a proposition into two or more
component propositions or combining two or more propositions into a composite one.
1. Basic concepts
The aim of this contribution is to give a survey of the most important aspects of complex
sentence formation in the languages of the world. They will emerge as generally applicable
parameters of clause linkage. I will try to ascertain to what degree they correlate and how
clusterings among the possibilities provided by them yield cross-linguistic types of clause
linkage. Most of these parameters have been found relevant to this issue by other authors and thus
make no claim to originality.
The parameters are based on traditional concepts. Some of these are controversial and therefore
have to be defined. Let us start with the most general concepts. A syntagm is relational iff it
contains a grammatical slot which predetermines a grammatical relation to be contracted by it.
For instance, an adverbial phrase is relational because it is by itself capable of modifying
something, especially a verb phrase; but a noun phrase is not relational since it does not, by itself,
contract any grammatical relation (see Lehmann 1985 for details).
A grammatical relation R connecting syntagms X and Y is a relation of dependency iff X
occupies a grammatical slot of Y or vice versa. In a dependency relation, Y depends on X iff X
determines the grammatical category of the complex and thus its external relations.
Non-dependency relations among syntagms are relations of sociation. Among them are
coordination, apposition and others to which we will return. For example, the relation of an
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 2
attribute to its head noun is a dependency relation, but the relation of an apposition to its head
noun is not a dependency relation.
An endocentric construction will be conceived not distributionally, but semantosyntactically, as
a binary syntagm Z with parts X and Y such that Y belongs to the same category as Z and X is an
expansion of Z. The syntactic relation of X to Y may be one of dependency or of sociation.
In the application of the term clause linkage, we will assume a broad concept of the clause which
comprises any syntagm containing one predication. Syntactically, this means that - apart from
nominal clauses - the uppermost controller of dependency in the syntagm is a verbal form. Since
a verbal form may be finite or nonfinite, this includes nominalized clauses. Clause linkage, then,
is a relation of dependency or sociation obtaining between clauses in this sense. In what follows, I
will confine myself to the consideration of binary clause linkage. This should not be understood
as excluding the possibility of more than two clauses being linked at the same level.
Subordination1 may now be conceived as a form of clause linkage.2 If syntagms (clauses) X and
Y are in a relation of clause linkage, then X is subordinate to Y iff X and Y form an endocentric
construction Z with Y as the head. In the course of the paper, subordination will emerge as a
prototypical concept.
The term proposition will be used (instead of `state of affairs') for the semantic correlate of a
(possibly desententialized) clause.
Hypotaxis will be understood as the subordination of a clause in the narrow sense (which
probably includes its finiteness). The definition does not impose any further syntactic or
morphological requirements on the subordinate syntagm and thus corresponds fairly well to
common usage.3
Embedding is the dependency of a subordinate syntagm.
1
The term subordination is applied, in different schools of linguistics, to different kinds of things. In the
broadest use, which may be found in certain trends of European structuralism, the size and nature of the
subordinate element is of no concern. Here subordination practically means the same as dependency. In
the most narrow use, characteristic of classical philology, only finite clauses can be said to be subordinate.
Here subordination practically means the same as hypotaxis; and consequently the two latter terms are
mostly used interchangeably.
There are also differences with respect to the kind of relation presupposed for subordination. Some (e.g.
Touratier 1985) require that the subordinate clause have a syntactic function in the matrix clause. Others
(e.g. Brøndal 1937) admit of subordinate clauses without matrix clause. In addition, there is a mess of
morphological, semantic and logical criteria which have been invoked in order to distinguish
subordination from coordination. As has been shown repeatedly (already in Brøndal 1937), none of these
is crucial, although they may be used to characterize a type of subordination well represented in certain
Indo-European languages.
2
This implies that a syntagm will be said to be subordinate only if it contains a predication, and represents,
thus, a compromise solution as regards the nature of the subordinate element; cf. fn. 1.
3
Matthiessen & Thompson (this vol.) regard the `nucleus-satellite relation' as constitutive for
subordination. On the one hand, they conceive this as a basically textual relation; on the other hand, the
traditional notion of subordination appears to them to be best captured as a grammaticalization of this
relation. It thus comes fairly close to the semantosyntactic notion of endocentricity used above.- It follows
from the above definition that the presence of a subordinate syntagm presupposes the presence of a main
clause - which may be or contain Y - to which it is somehow subordinate (against Brøndal 1937).
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 3
With these definitions, we get hypotaxis and embedding as two particular types of subordination,
the former delimited with respect to the kind of the subordinate syntagm, the latter delimited with
respect to the kind of the relation of subordination.
Current linguistic usage4 does not treat the term coordination as complementary to
subordination, since coordination does not imply that the coordinated elements be of a sentential
or verbal nature. Coordination is a relation of sociation combining two syntagms of the same type
and forming a syntagm which is again of the same type.
Parataxis is the coordination of clauses. No further restrictions are imposed on the kind or
structural means of coordination. In particular, parataxis may be syndetic or asyndetic.
We are now ready to enter into the consideration of the various semantosyntactic parameters
which are relevant for clause linkage across languages. The following will be considered:5
1. the hierarchical downgrading of the subordinate clause,
2. the main clause syntactic level of the subordinate clause,
3. the desententialization of the subordinate clause,
4. the grammaticalization of the main verb,
5. the interlacing of the two clauses,
6. the explicitness of the linking.
The reader will notice that the semantic nature of the relation between the two clauses does not
figure in this list. While this has always played a prominent role in the classification of
subordinate clauses, it does not appear to be constitutive of cross-linguistically valid types of
clause linkage. That is, there is no cross-linguistic notion of, say, the concessive clause which
would possess any constant structural correlates.6 It rather appears that the grammatical types that
will emerge on the basis of the above six parameters cut across the semantically different clause
linkage relations.7
Although these parameteres will be grouped in three pairs, they will be presented separately, as if
they were mutually independent. In '5.1 we shall turn to possible correlations among them.
The requirement in our definition that a subordinate construction must be part of a higher
construction leaves large room for variation. For one thing, the subordinate syntagm may bear a
sociative or a dependent relation to the main clause. For another, it may be subordinate to the
main clause as a whole or rather to some constituent of the main clause. Neither of these
alternatives is clear-cut; the differences are gradual. The two aspects taken together identify what
I will call the integration of the subordinate construction into the main clause.
4
except perhaps for the European schools of structuralism mentioned in fn.1
5
Several of them are reviewed in Haiman & Thompson 1984. For at least some of them, the authors show
that they cannot serve as a basis for the concept of subordination. In what follows, I will use them to
differentiate types of clause linkage.
6
Harris (this vol.) shows that this is even true within one language or a group of closely related languages.
7
The Foley & Van Valin (1984, ch.6.3) semantic bondedness hierarchy embodies a claim to the contrary.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 4
2.1.1. Consider the following examples (brackets set off subordinate syntagms):
E4. quei ager ex privato in publicum commutatus est], de eo agro siremps lex esto.
LAT "Any land that has been converted from private into public, to such land the law shall
apply in the same way." (CIL I2, 585)
8
Paul (1920:145) already speaks of "Herabdrückung eines Satzes zum Satzgliede".
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 5
E3 from Hittite and E4 show a correlative diptych. As Haudry (1973, '3) puts it, this is halfway
between parataxis and hypotaxis. The relative clauses in these examples are subordinate, but not
embedded. As they stand, they could not even be embedded. On the other hand, Latin and, to a
much lesser extent, Hittite have the alternative possibility of embedding similar clauses within the
main clause.
In E5 from Bambara (Mande, West Africa) the situation is but slightly different. The subordinate
clause has to have final position; however, it is not outside the main clause, but rather its last
constituent. The word order rules of Bambara determine sentence-final position for adverbials,
simple or complex. The subordinate clause has the structure of a relative clause which usually,
but not exclusively, appears in a correlative diptych.
E6 from Kobon (Papua New Guinea; see Davies 1981) shows clause chaining. Here the clauses
of a complex sentence come in chains which can grow fairly long. The last verb in the sentence is
the main verb. All the preceding clauses are subordinate to the final one. They and their verbs are
called medial. They lack tense, aspect and mood categories, which are taken to be those of the
final verb, and instead have a special set of person agreement suffixes which signal whether the
subject of the following clause is the same or different from the subject of this clause. We will
return to this point in '4.1.2. Here it suffices to see that although the medial clauses are
subordinate, they cannot be said to be embedded in the final clause. Foley & Van Valin (1984,
ch.6) call this relation `cosubordination'.
Again, there is a small difference between this construction and the Latin conjunct participle of
E7. This is clearly part of the main clause and insofar embedded in it. However, its syntactic
function is not crystal-clear (cf. Pinkster 1984, ch.8). It is a blend, as it were, of an apposition and
an adverbial, and thus not subject to government.
E8 finally shows a typical embedded clause, namely an object clause governed by the main verb.
We may, of course, find examples of more deeply embedded clauses; but the hierarchical
unequality of the two clauses cannot, in principle, become clearer than this.
However, within the constructions in which a verb governs a subordinate clause, differences
which appear to relate to the degree of downgrading of the latter are determined by the nature of
the governing verb. In particular, so-called implicative verbs such as `force' appear to downgrade
the subordinate clause more strongly than non-implicative verbs such as `believe'. This issue is
much investigated presently under the guiding question: What are the factors determining the
form of a subordinate clause, and in particular, how is it determined by the class of the governing
verb? (For some recent research, see Givón 1980, Dixon 1984, Bolkestein 1985 and Carvalho
1985.) The issue is hard to divorce from the control properties of the governing verb, which
codetermine argument sharing among the two clauses. This topic will be taken up in the latter
connection.
2.1.2. An important aspect of the integration of the subordinate syntagm into the matrix clause
which has to be accounted for in terms of its hierarchical downgrading is the sequential position
of the former relative to the latter. Authors have tended to think of positional variability as an
important property of a subordinate clause (cf. Matthiessen & Thompson, this vol., '5.1.5). It
should be emphasized that the position of a subordinate clause as against its main clause is
generally more subject to grammatical constraints than the position of one independent clause as
against its neighbour. While the two independent clauses in E1 may not seem invertible without
damage to the sense, grammar is certainly indifferent as to any order of them. Subordinate
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 6
clauses, however, often find their position subject to grammatical constraints (cf. E13 and E14
below and Lehmann 1982[T], ch.IV.3.3).
The subordinate syntagm may be either positionally included in the main clause, or it may
precede or follow the latter. Accordingly, we speak of central vs. marginal position of the
subordinate syntagm. Many languages possess the constraint that subordinate clauses of a certain
type have to have a marginal position with respect to the main clause. This is not only true of the
adjoined and correlative clauses that we saw in E2 through E4, but also of subordinate clauses
with a more downgraded status. Thus in Hixkaryana (Carib, Brasil) and Abkhaz (North-West
Caucasian), subordinate clauses are lower on the hierarchy, but yet are generally marginal, in
Abkhaz mainly preposed, to the main clause.
Quite generally, the marginal position of a subordinate clause is in accordance with the principles
of functional sentence perspective. Just as elsewhere, sentence-initial position usually identifies
the topic (more precisely, the exposition, in the terms of Lehmann 1984, ch.V.5) of the sentence.
This is well-known from left-dislocated NPs. It is perhaps not so well known that a whole
subordinate clause may also provide a topic for the following main clause.9 Thompson (1984)
shows that English purpose clauses fulfill different discourse functions as they either precede or
follow the main clause. Look at her introductory example, a passage taken from a report on a
voyage in a primitive boat named Brendan:
E9. Brendan was rushing farther and farther out to sea. [To slow her down] we streamed a
heavy rope in a loop from the stern and let it trail in the water behind us to act as a brake,
and, hopefully, to smooth the worst of the wave crests. From the stern also dangled a
metal bucket; only twenty-four hours earlier we had been using it [to cook an excellent
meal of Irish crabs]. Now it clanked mornfully every time a wave broke against it.
(Thompson 1985, '5.1)
The preposed purpose clause formulates a problem which has been expected on the basis of the
text preceding it. This problem requires a solution which is provided by the following main
clause. An initial purpose clause thus acts like other topics in that it "provides a framework within
which the main clause can be interpreted, and that it does this by its role as a link in an
expectation chain." (Thompson 1985, '5.1) A final purpose clause, such as the second one in E9,
does not do this, but merely appends a purpose locally relevant with respect to some semantic
unit in the preceding main clause. Cf. also Chafe 1984.
We have seen one Latin example of this discourse function of left-marginal subordinate clauses in
E4. E10 is another one.
9
Cf. Marchese 1977 for various kinds of subordinate clauses in Godie and Lehmann 1984, ch.V.5.1 for
relative clauses.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 7
E13. Das verstehe ich nicht, zumal die Reise noch so billig war.
GER "That I don't understand, especially as the trip was so cheap."
E14. Fliegen ist viel schöner als Autofahren, nur daß man noch mehr aufpassen muß.
GER "Flying is much finer than car driving, except that one has to pay even more attention."
German subordinate clauses introduced by zumal "so much more so as" and nur daß "except that"
have to follow their main clause, as they do in E13 und E14, and cannot precede it.
Another kind of positional constraint can be seen in the medial clause of Kobon (E6). The
essential point in all the examples is the following: the fact that rules of grammar may determine
the position of the subordinate syntagm in or vis-à-vis the matrix is an aspect of their integration
which follows from their hierarchical downgrading.
2.1.3. We finally return to the gradience illustrated by E1 through to E8. The idea of this
continuum also underlies the "typology of nexus" put forward in Foley & Van Valin 1984, ch.6,
although the details differ. The continuum of hierarchical downgrading is represented in F1.
Hierarchical downgrading is not only an important parameter in the typology of clause linkage,
but is also a central criterion for the traditional notion of subordination; a clause not affected by it
is not called subordinate.
2.2.1. A second aspect of the integration of the subordinate clause into the main clause, closely
related to its hierarchical downgrading, is the variation according to the main clause syntactic
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 8
level which the subordinate clause belongs to. The first to use this parameter for a typology of
complex sentences was T. Milewski (1954). He distinguished three syntactic levels: the sentence
level (above the simple clause), the clause level (within the clause), and the verb level. This
hierarchy is mirrored in Foley & Van Valin's (1984, ch.6) typology of clause linkage according to
their three levels of juncture, viz. periphery, core and nucleus. I will not accept such a threefold
division but rather assume a multiplicity of syntactic levels between the morpheme and the
paragraph, much as in constituent structure grammar.
The guiding idea is that the lower this level, the tighter the subordinate clause is integrated into
the main clause. We may first reconsider the examples already given. In E1 there are only
independent clauses; they are thus on the text level. The subordinate clauses in E2 through E4 do
not form part of the main clause, but are on the same syntactic level as the latter, namely the
sentence level. The subordinate clause in E6 occupies an intermediate position between being
outside and inside the main clause (this adds up to the reasons why it is called medial). The
adverbial clause in E5 is clearly part of the main clause, but on its highest syntactic level
(immediately dominated by S). The conjunct participle of E7 is on some level between clause and
VP. Finally, the subordinate clause in E8 is on the VP level.
Apart from the switch in the order of E5 and E6, this looks like a neat correlation between
hierarchical downgrading and syntactic level. However, a moment of reflection will show that
this need not be so. There can be subordinate predications on very low syntactic levels which are
far from being governed by a main clause constituent. Look at E15.
E15. Tarquinio vero quid impudentius, [qui bellum gereret cum iis [qui eius non tulerant
superbiam]]?
LAT "Again, what is more impudent than Tarquinius, who made war with those who could not
bear his arrogance?" (Cic.Tu.3,27)
Here the first relative clause is appositive, which gives it a place somewhere in the first half of the
continuum of hierarchical downgrading. The second relative clause is restrictive, thus dependent
on the head noun, but still not governed by anything and therefore not at the rightmost pole of F1.
However, the first relative clause is on a fairly low syntactic level, modifying an NP governed by
the predicate. The second relative clause is on an even lower level, if we determine levels by
counting nodes from the root of a constituent structure diagram. If we don't do this, but just count
the distance from the nearest upper S, the second relative clause is still on a lower level than any
subordinate clause in the examples through to E16.
Low syntactic level of a not maximally subordinate syntagm can also be seen in E16.
10
A full treatment would have to investigate the relationship between serial verb constructions and motion
purpose clauses (containing expressions such as `go buy', `come play' etc.), which seem to be more
widespread. Cf. Aissen 1984 for a specimen analysis.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 9
device is operative both in a syntactically regular way, as in E16, and in verb composition, with
various intermediate degrees of lexicalization. What is presently of interest to us is the
hierarchical relation obtaining in verb series and the syntactic level on which they abide. While
detailed investigation is still pending, this much at least appears to be certain: The preceding
verbal is not governed by the following one (or vice versa). It is not even clear that the former
depends on the latter; they might be coordinate (or cosubordinate, as Foley & Van Valin
1984:261f would have it). On the other hand, the main clause syntactic level on which the
preceding verb is adjoined is certainly not above the VP.
From these examples we may conclude that although advanced hierarchical downgrading of the
subordinate clause implies a low syntactic level for it, the converse does not hold. Thus the
relation between the continua of hierarchical downgrading and of syntactic level is one of
unilateral implication.
Serial verb constructions are in a clear grammaticalization relationship with clause chains. The
concomitance of the two clause linkage types, here represented by E6 and E16 for Kobon,
therefore recurs in totally unrelated languages. See, e.g., Todd 1975 for Choctaw.
2.2.2. Up to now the lowest syntactic level illustrated by our examples has been that of the VP.
E17 to E20 are meant to show that the various processes of subordination are also operative at
still lower levels.
Although this parameter constitutes important differences among types of clause linkage, it does
not seem to be crucial for the traditional notion of subordination.
2.3. Before we conclude the chapter on integration, we should at least mention intonation as an
important factor in it (cf. Bolinger 1984). A clause may be downgraded by low pitch and may be
integrated with another clause by the absence of an intonation break between them.
3.1.1. The third of the parameters that structure a typology of clause linkage is the degree to
which the subordinate clause is expanded or reduced (cf. Lehmann 1982[N] for the following). In
the reduction process, it loses the properties of a clause, it is desententialized to varying degrees.
Components of the clause which allow reference to a specific state of affairs are dropped; the
state of affairs is `typified'.11 At the same time, the subordinate clause increasingly acquires
nominal properties, both internally and in its distribution. At the end of this process of
nominalization, the clause becomes a nominal or adverbial constituent of a matrix clause. In
''3.1.2 and 3.1.3, we will deal with the internal and external aspects of desententialization,
respectively.
3.1.2. There is a variety of semantic components and categories with their grammatical correlates
which make up a full-fledged sentence. Let me enumerate here illocutionary force, mood, tense,
aspect, actants and circumstants with their various syntactic functions. We observe, first of all,
that a subordinate clause may not normally have its own illocutionary force. Certain particles
bound up with illocutionary force are not admitted in subordinate clauses. We may mention the
Quechua validator -mi and the Latin assertive particle ne. This is a consequence of the fact that
"illocutionary force is the outermost peripheral operator", as Foley & Van Valin (1984:239) put
it, which means that one sentence, however complex, may have only one illocutionary force.
Look at the following examples:
E22. Non temere est [quod corvos cantat mihi nunc ab laeva manu].
LAT "It is not by chance that the raven now croaks at me from the left side." (Pl.Au.624)
11
Cf. Mackenzie 1984. Dik (1985:11) says: "any form of argument reduction easily leads to
`deactualization' of the meaning of the output predicate-frame, where deactualization means that the output
predicate-frame tends to get a generic, habitual or potential reading rather than a reading which directly
designates some actual state of affairs."
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 11
The structure of the noun clauses in both of these sentences differs minimally from that of a
corresponding independent clause. (The same goes, incidentally, for E2 - E4.) The introductory
subordinating conjunction is even optional in E21. However, even with such a low degree of
desententialization, one thing is missing, viz. the illocutionary force that the corresponding
independent sentences would have. The same can be seen in all the examples so far given except
for E1.
Often the assertive force of the subordinate clause depends on the sentence type and the polarity
of the main clause. Observe E23.
12
Again, this is a scalar phenomenon. As Green 1976 shows, the speaker may choose to couch his main
point in a subordinate clause and then use, in this, functional sentence perspective and word order as
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 12
The next verbal categories that get reduced and finally lost in desententialization are tense and
aspect. We saw that in Kobon medial clauses and non-final serial verbs tense and aspect are
missing and are understood to be those of the final clause or verb, respectively.
characteristic of main clauses. König & Van der Auwera (this vol.) show that main-clause word order in
German subordinate clauses signals non-integration of the latter in the main clause.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 13
E29. labor est [functio quaedam vel animi vel corporis gravioris operis et muneris].
LAT "Labor is the performance of a relatively hard compulsory work by the mind or the
body." (Cic.Tu.2,35)
What was the subject of the finite verb may also appear in the accusative when the verb becomes
nonfinite. This may happen to the causee in a causative construction, as in E20. It also happens in
the accusativus cum infinitivo, as illustrated in E30 - E32.
the modals such as must and may. In Jacaltec, nonfinite subordinate predications cannot be
negated (cf. Craig 1977:242f and Foley & Van Valin 1984:287f).
1979). In E11 it is not the subordinate clause as a whole which shows the ablative (assuming that
this is the appropriate adverbial case), but rather its two main constituents, the NP and the
participle which correspond to the subject and predicate of a finite construction. Similarly in E32,
the nouns corresponding to the subject and the predicate nominal are in the accusative,
vicariously for the subordinate clause as a whole.
In finite subordinate clauses, the solution is to show case on the subordinator. Many of the
subordinative conjunctions, such as quod, cum, qua, ut etc. are old case forms of the relative
pronoun which functioned as a general subordinator. All these are consequences of the fact that a
language of the type Latin belongs to must show syntactic relations on the word.
3.1.4. We are now ready to sum up our observations on the inner and outer processes resulting
from desententialization in a continuum which appears in F3 (cf. Lehmann 1982[N]:76).
F3. Desententialization
sententiality <-------------------------------------------------------> nominality
clause nonfinite construction verbal noun
no illocutionary force
constraints on illocutionary elements
constraints on/loss of modal elements and mood
constraints on/loss of tense and aspect
dispensability of complements
loss of personal conjugation
conversion of subject into oblique slot
no polarity
conversion of verbal into
nominal government
dispensability of subject
constraints
on complements
combinable with adposition / agglutinative case affix / flexive case affix
Strictly speaking, constructions such as the ablativus absolutus or the Romance gerundives in
-ndo are adverbial rather than nominal in nature. I will not here consider the problem of whether
all adverbials can be analyzed as nominals in a certain semantic case and merely note that the
term `nominality' used above should be understood as including adverbiality.
Much of the typological importance of the parameter of desententialization lies in the following
fact: To the degree that the subordinate construction is desententialized, the whole sentence
ceases to be genuinely complex. Looking at things from the point of view of expansion, we may
say that the mere insertion of a verbal noun as a constituent of a sentence does not yet make this
sentence syntactically complex. Cf. Carvalho 1985, Helander 1985 and E36.
than the former. Experience with languages such as Turkish and Japanese, whose subordinate
clauses are more desententialized than standard Indo-European ones, would not appear to invite
the conclusion that sentences containing less desententialized subordinate clauses are ceteris
paribus psychologically less complex.
3.2.1. In the preceding section we saw that a subordinate clause may be reduced to its core, the
predicate. In the following we will see that there is a reduction process in the main clause, too.
However, the kind of desententialization which leads to nominalization does not occur in main
clauses, since it presupposes subordination. What we find, instead, is the grammaticalization of
the word governing the subordinate clause.
Grammaticalization is a diachronic process and a synchronic continuum which lead from lexical
to grammatical items (see Lehmann 1982[T] for details). Among other things, it turns full verbs
into modals and auxiliaries. Since what is grammaticalized are essentially words, the approach of
the present section presupposes that the subordinate clause have a grammatical relation to just one
word in the matrix clause. There is a variety of subordinate constructions we might look at under
this premiss (cf., e.g., Rosén 1985 for cleft sentences). However, the grammaticalization of the
superordinate word leads to special construction types only when that word is a verb. The other
interesting case is when the subordinate clause is governed by an adposition. Adpositions
grammaticalize to case affixes. The combinability of subordinate clauses with adpositions and
case affixes has been dealt with briefly in '3.1.3.
3.2.2. We can start the illustration of the grammaticalization of the superordinate predicate with
causative constructions.
Standard Indo-European languages such as English, German and Latin lack the grammatical
means of forming causative verbs in such regular ways. Instead, the formation of causative verbs
tends to be a matter of the lexicon. Latin has the type illustrated in E38.
E38. Miles picem fervefecit.
LAT "The soldier boiled the pitch."
The underlying simple verb in E38 is fervere "to glow". The formation of causative verbs by
composition with facere "do, make" never was very productive. It was exclusively based on
intransitive verbs, but extended to such bivalent verbs as assuescere "to get used to", whose
ablative, dative or prepositional complement remains totally unaffected by the derivation of
assuefacere "to accustom to". On the other hand, it is certainly no accident that the historically
identical Italian verb fare forms the totally productive analytic causatives seen in E19. Similar
remarks apply to the totally unproductive German causatives of the type trinken "to drink" -
tränken "to make drink".
However, both the grammatical causatives of E19 and E20 and the lexical causatives of E38 etc.
prove the point which is essential here: To the degree that the main clause predicate gets
grammaticalized, the whole sentence ceases to be syntactically complex (cf. Brettschneider 1984,
'5; Drossard 1984, '4). In E20 and E38, there is only one clause, the latter being undebatably a
simple one.
3.2.3. For the sake of variation, let us look at a second series of examples.13 The following are
desiderative constructions.
13
A third one might be provided by the grammaticalization of the governing main verb to a conjunction
introducing the subordinate clause; s. Lord 1976.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 18
an affix. Gradually the main verb gets interpreted as a functive operating on the subordinate
clause, specifically its verb, turning this into a derived verb.
3.2.4. Although the degree of grammaticalization of the superordinate verb constitutes important
differences among types of clause linkage, it appears to play a largely negative role in traditional
accounts of subordination. Namely, constructions with a highly grammaticalized superordinate
verb generally have been neglected altogether in this connection. The continuum is schematized
in F4.
3.3. The conclusion is, of course, that there are two ways of reducing a complex sentence to a
simple one (and conversely, two ways of expanding a clause to a complex sentence). First, we
may desententialize the subordinate clause, turning it into a simple constituent of the main clause.
Second, we may grammaticalize the governing verb, turning it into an affix which modifies the
meaning of the semantically subordinate verb. In both cases, the subordinate verb becomes a
constituent of the main clause: in the first case, a dependent one, in the second case, its main verb.
4.1. Interlacing
4.1.1. Another parameter structuring subordination is the degree to which the linked propositions
are interlaced.14 The semantic aspect of interlacing is that the two propositions share some
elements of their meanings. Its syntactic correlate are the non-specification of the common
elements in one of the propositions and/or the syntagmatic interweaving of the two propositions.
Cf. Davison 1979, '4.1 and Foley & Van Valin 1984, ch.6.3.
In principle, the linked propositions may share any bit of meaning one may please to imagine. I
will not systematically cover the possibilities here, but mention just three: the sharing of
predicates, of tense and aspect, and of actants.
The two propositions may share their predicate. When this is expressed only once, we get a kind
of brachylogy known as gapping. Here are two examples:
E42. [ut ager ... sine cultura fructuosus esse non potest], sic sine doctrina animus.
LAT "As a field cannot be fertile without cultivation, so the mind (cannot be fruitful) without
instruction." (Cic.Tu.2,13)
14
Szantyr (1972:526) seems to be using the term synsemy for this phenomenon. The older term
`synsemanticity' has had too many applications to be recommendable.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 19
E43. magis ea percipimus ..., quae nobis ipsis ... eveniunt, quam illa, [quae ceteris].
LAT "We perceive more strongly those things which happen to ourselves than those which
(happen) to others." (Cic.off. 1,30)
In E42 he whole predicate of the main clause except for the privative adjunct is gapped on
identity with the preceding subordinate clause. Conversely in E43, the predicate of the
subordinate clause, except for the benefactive adjunct, is gapped on identity with the preceding
main clause. Gapping works essentially the same way in parataxis and in hypotaxis. However,
there are no subordinate constructions specialized for the case that a certain predicate, or a
predicate of a certain kind, is shared among the two clauses. This is why shared predicates are not
central to this section.
The second kind of interlacing that I will mention only briefly concerns tense and aspect. Their
interlacing means that the tense and aspect of the subordinate clause are partly or wholly
determined by those of the main clause (cf. Van Valin 1984 on `grammatical category
dependence').15 Partial dependence of the tense of the subordinate clause on that of the main
clause occurs already at a level of weak desententialization in the form of consecutio temporum.
The subordinate constructions of E7, E30 and E31, on the one hand, and of E11 and E12, on the
other, signal only simultaneousness with and temporal priority to the main clause, respectively,
the tense itself being determined by the main clause. Similarly, the subordinate verbs of E26 and
E34.a only show that their time is not posterior and not prior, respectively, to main clause time.
Complete dependence of subordinate tense and aspect can be seen in the Kobon examples E6 and
E16, as well as in the infinitives of E17 - E19.
4.1.2. We now turn to the central issue of the present section, which is the interlacing of actants
of the main and subordinate clauses. I will skip here the whole issue of relative clauses and just
observe that the correlative diptych, as in E3 and E4, is essentially held together by anaphora, i.e.
by the fact that the two correlative clauses share an actant (or another nominal or adverbial
concept). The fact that backwards anaphora is allowed in subordinate clauses but heavily
constrained16 in main clauses is another instance of the interlacing of actants that I will just
mention.17
Many languages make a distinction in the inflection of a non-final (medial) verb depending on
whether the verb of the following clause has the same or a distinct subject. Kobon does this in
finite medial verbs, which have two oppositive paradigms of personal endings, one signalling
`same subject', the other `different subject'; cf. E6. Quechua signals sameness vs. difference of
subject at the end of most of its subordinate clauses, e.g. the temporal clauses in E44.18
15
The interlacing of tense can also be regarded as a special case of relativized deixis known from indirect
(as opposed to direct) speech.
16
The three examples of backwards pronominalization in main clauses offered in Matthiessen &
Thompson, this vol., '5.4.2 show merely that after pronominalizing a referent, we can again make full
lexical reference to him - for emphasis' sake, presumably.
17
Cf. also Tao 1985 for the role of zero anaphora in clause linkage.
18
Cf. the similar situation described for Malayalam in Jayaseelan 1984.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 20
its own subject (E45.a), whereas it must reduce to an infinitive if the subordinate action is one of
the higher subject (b).
On the other hand, an object clause occurs after verbs of cognition, as in E46, and even
occasionally as a headless relative clause after a plain bivalent verb, as in E47.
19
Prolepsis is to be clearly distinguished from second position of the conjunction within the subordinate
clause. This is, as Marouzeau 1946 shows quite conclusively, an instance of Wackernagel's enclitic
position and does not lend any special relief to the word preceding the conjunction.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 22
4.2.1. We are now coming to a traditional issue, the problem of syndesis and asyndesis.21 I had
already remarked in the introduction that there is much terminological confusion in this area.
Therefore I should like to stress again that the presence or absence of a connective device
between two clauses has nothing to do with parataxis vs. hypotaxis, but is exclusively a question
of syndesis. In particular, it is not the case that either the concept of hypotaxis or the concept of
subordination require the use of a conjunction, as has been claimed variously. Let us therefore
look first at paratactic, then at hypotactic constructions with regard to syndesis (cf. Brettschneider
1984:14f as to indicative vs. predicative representation of the interclausal relation).
E53. [Several chapters on a repelled assault of the enemy. First sentence of new section:]
His rebus gestis cum omnibus de causis Caesar pacatam Galliam existimaret atque ita
hieme in Illyricum profectus esset, quod eas quoque nationes adire et regiones cognoscere
volebat, subitum bellum in Gallia coortum est.
LAT "These things being done, Caesar had every reason to assume that Gaule was now
pacified. Thus in the winter he set off for Illyria, because he wanted to visit those peoples,
20
Cf. Davison 1979:122. Foley & Van Valin 1984, ch.6.3.2 claim the first factor to be relevant, too, but do
not present convincing evidence for it.
21
In traditional grammar, these terms have normative-stylistical connotations. In particluar, asyndesis is
often understood as the absence of a linking device where one would be expected.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 23
too, and to learn about the area. There a sudden war broke out in Gaule."
(Caes.B.G.3,7,1)
E54. Todas essas ações do departamento evidentemente não tinham outro motivo do que
não dificultar ou ainda impossibilizar o meu trabalho. Assim sendo, eu prefiro pôr o meu
posto á disposição.
POR "All those actions of the department obviously had no other motive than to render my
work difficult or even impossible. This being so, I prefer to vacate my post."
E55. Il lavoro in questo istituto mi soddisfà completamente, ed anche i colleghi sono molto
gentili. Ciononostante devo dichiarare che esiste un problema insuperabile.
ITA "Work in this institute satisfies me completely, and also the collegues are very friendly.
This not withstanding, I have to admit that there is an unsurmountable problem."
E56. Ihr Angebot befriedigt meine Ansprüche vollkommen; deswegen nehme ich es dankend
an.
GER "Your offer serves my wants completely; therefore I accept it gratefully."
22
For early insights in this matter, see Paul 1920:148f.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 24
E61. [Nonostante l'òpera fosse molto rumorosa], mi addormentai nel secondo atto.
ITA "Although the opera was very noisy, I fell asleep in the second act."
E66. Atque ibi [vehementissime perturbatus] Lentulus tamen et signum et manum suam
cognovit.
LAT "And there Lentulus, being extremely embarrassed, yet recognized his signature and
hand." (Cic.Cat.3, 12)
The subordinate construction in E66 is strongly desententialized, but its semantic relation to the
matrix clause is made explicit in the main clause conjunction tamen "nevertheless".23
4.2.3. Thus, there does not appear to be a strong relationship between desententialization and
explicitness of linking. I delay the discussion of the correlations which do hold to the next section
and conclude this section with the continuum of explicitness of linking.
The following six parameters have been found relevant to the understanding of clause linkage and
subordination:
1. the hierarchical downgrading of the subordinate clause (F1),
2. the main clause syntactic level of the subordinate clause (F2),
3. the desententialization of the subordinate clause (F3),
4. the grammaticalization of the main verb (F4),
5. the interlacing of the two clauses,
6. the explicitness of the linking (F5).
Each of these parameters is construed as a continuum extending between two opposite poles. The
question now arises as to the independence of the parameters. Note that our six continua are
directed in a parallel way. Always looking from left to right, we find the first two continua
extending from parataxis to embedding and from sentence to word level, respectively. The
continuum of desententialization is between the poles of maximal sententiality and nominality.
The continuum of the grammaticalization of the main verb starts from an independent predicate
and ends with a grammatical operator. Interlacing of the two clauses varies between their total
disjunctness and their maximal identity. Finally, the continuum of explicitness of the linking has
explicit syndesis at its left and asyndesis at its right pole. Rather than trying to make the intuitive
23
E66 thus shows that Matthiessen & Thompson's (this vol., '5.1) claim that markers of subordination
mark satellites is not valid for Latin.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 26
parallelism among the continua explicit, I will pass to commenting on individual correlations
between pairs of them.
As regards the relationship between the first two continua, we had seen in '2.2 that advanced
hierarchical downgrading of the subordinate clause implies a low syntactic level for it. We will
thus be justified if in the following we take advanced downgrading as a sufficient condition for
high integration.
High integration of the subordinate into the main clause correlates positively with its
desententialization.24 More precisely, nominalized subordinate constructions can easily be
downgraded, since they acquire the distributional properties of a nominal expression (cf. Meillet
1921). We may also say that nominalization necessitates at least some downgrading, since a
reduced construction cannot remain totally independent. However, maximal nominalization does
not presuppose maximal downgrading, since a nominalized verbal may be relatively independent
(say, as an adjunct, or even left-dislocated) just to the degree that any NP may be. The Latin
participial constructions, e.g. in E7, illustrate this.
Strong grammaticalization of the governing predicate presupposes either advanced
desententialization or strong interlacing. This is because the grammaticalization turns the
governing predicate into a grammatical operator on the subordinate construction, but at the same
time cuts down its syntactic scope (see Lehmann 1982[T], ch.IV.3.1). Thus the grammaticalized
predicate must be an operator on a construction of relatively low complexity (cf. Foley & Van
Valin 1984, ch.6.4.5 on the correlative reduction of both the choice of the governing verb and the
syntactic level of the linkage). This is possible either through desententialization of the
subordinate construction or by having the operator apply, through interlacing, only to one
constituent of the subordinate construction, normally the predicate. Desententialization of the
complement is evidenced by the grammatical causatives and desideratives in E19 and E40,
respectively, while interlacing can be seen in the derivational counterparts, E38 and E41.b. Both
desententialization and interlacing occur in E51 and E52. The latter point would also be
illustrated by E48, except that here the main verb is not strongly grammaticalized.
Interlacing of clauses as brought about by raising operations presupposes downgrading, thus,
integration of the subordinate clause. This is because raising is controlled by the main predicate,
which means that the subordinate clause is governed by it. There are, indeed, no instances of
prolepsis except out of subject and object clauses. As far as switch-reference is concerned,
Haiman (1983:120) shows "that DS clauses are less dependent than are SS clauses".
Interlacing of clauses as brought about by dependent subject control leads to desententialization
of the subordinate clause. This follows necessarily, insofar as dependent subject control means
non-finiteness of the dependent verb and this means strong desententialization. Cf. Givón 1980,
'3 and Cooreman 1984 on the correlation between the "binding scale" and desententialization. As
for switch-reference, SS clauses exhibit stronger grammatical category dependence than DS
clauses (Haiman 1983:121).
Explicitness of linking correlates with syntactic level, because the semantic relation linking
clause A to clause B is rather constrained if the linkage pertains to a low syntactic level of B,
24
Cf. Foley & Van Valin 1984, ch.6.4.1 on the correlation between their "syntactic bondedness" and
nominalization, and also Givón 1980, Dixon 1984, Bolkestein 1985 and Carvalho 1985 on the correlation
between the type of the governing verb and the desententialization of the governed clause.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 27
whereas more diverse semantic relations may obtain on higher levels of B; cf. Foley & Van Valin
1984:196.
Explicitness of linking has some relationship to desententialization. We saw that explicit
conjunctions, either coordinative or subordinative, may have the function of an adverbial in the
linked clause, whereas pure subordinators do not have such a dependent status within the
subordinate clause, but are just operators applied to it. From this it follows that those explicit
conjunctions find their place within the linked clause without necessarily changing its status very
much, whereas the pure subordinators do downgrade the clause they are applied to (cf. Carvalho
1985). However, this consideration only applies to explicit linkers which do function as
adverbials within the linked clause; they do not apply, e.g., to E60.
Finally, asyndesis (lack of explicitness of linking) correlates with dependent subject control and
thus with interlacing. This is because the syntactic relation of the subordinate construction to the
matrix clause may be determined either by some property of the former or by some property of
the latter. If the subordinate clause is linked to the main clause by some explicit connective, this
determines its syntactic relation to the main clause and makes the subordinate clause seek its own
place in the complex sentence. However, this is unnecessary and even impossible when the
subordinate clause is controlled by the main verb.
It should be clear that the correlations and implicative relationships formulated above represent
tendencies, not laws. If any of them were perfect and exceptionless, it would be no use to
establish distinct parameters. Nevertheless, to the degree that they do obtain, it makes sense to
combine our six continua to the synopsis in F6.
F6 makes us see the common functional denominator underlying the various continua: all of them
extend from a pole of maximal elaboration to a pole of maximal compression (or condensation)
of lexical and grammatical information.25 This means that two opposing forces are at work in
clause linkage. The first acts towards the elaboration of a phrase into a more fully developped
construction which contains its own predication with all the accessories. Methodologically, this
implies starting from the simple independent clause and gradually elaborating it into a complex
sentence by expanding its constituents into clauses. To this corresponds the definition of the
subordinate clause as "a member of a sentence having a form similar to that of a sentence"
(Jespersen 1937:166). This point of view has been predominant in the traditional analysis of the
subordinate clause. It has also been useful in the analysis of serial verb constructions as a
technique of valence increasing (cf. Foley & Van Valin 1984, ch.5.2) and of cleft sentences as a
technique in functional sentence perspective (cf. Lambrecht, this vol.).
The opposing force acts towards the compression of a full-fledged clause to a nominal or
adverbial constituent of a matrix clause. Methodologically, this implies a derivation of complex
sentences which starts from a set of complete clauses, reduces one of them through
desententialization and combines them into one complex sentence by embedding them into each
other. This point of view has been prevalent both in traditional diachronic approaches to complex
sentence formation and in the transformational analysis of the subordinate clause.
Once we realize that the opposing forces are complementary, we will combine the two
methodological viewpoints.26 In a functional framework, clause linkage may be viewed as either
representing two states of affairs as so tightly interconnected that they form one complex state of
affairs (compression), or on the contrary analyzing one state of affairs as composed of two
(elaboration; cf. Slotty 1936). In either case the cognitive relatedness of the two states of affairs is
mirrored in the way they are linked in language.
The complex continuum of F6 also provides a framework for a typology of clause linkage. Types
may be found on this continuum to the degree that the properties and processes assembled on the
six parameters tend to cluster. F6 tells us that there are two extreme types of clause linkage, at
either of the two poles of the continuum. On the one hand, we have a combination in which both
the two clauses and the linkage are maximally elaborated. They are both independent and
syntactically equal, each of them is a full clause. They are linked by an explicit connective
device; and this contains all that is semantically common to the two clauses. This type may be
represented by E53.
On the other hand, we have one clause containing, apart from the main predicate, a reduced
predication. This may be the subordinate clause. It is then embedded into the main clause on a
25
It should be noted that this is the common denominator worked out by the Cologne research group
UNITYP for their universal functional dimensions. The opposite principles are there called predication
and indication, respectively. Cf., in general, Seiler 1983, and on clause linkage in particular,
Brettschneider 1980 and 1984.
26
Looking back, it should be noted that the unilinear order in which each of the continua has been
presented was due to expository simplicity.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 29
low syntactic level, strongly nominalized and interlaced with the main predication and lacks any
specification of the relation linking it to the latter. This type is represented by E45.b. Or again the
matrix predication may be the reduced one. Then this is grammaticalized, but the other
characteristics of the reduced type hold as well. E20 embodies this type.
Between these extreme types, there is a great variety of intermediate and concomitant types. The
intermediate types differ from the extreme types merely in that all the parameters are developed
to a non-extreme, but parallel degree. The concomitant types result from the fact that correlation
among the parameters is not perfect. For example, we quite freely get completely asyndetic
clauses which otherwise show no signs of reduction. (Note that there was no claim that absence
of downgrading correlated with explicitness of linking.) Some of these intermediate and
concomitant types are well represented in the languages of the world. Among them are, from left
to right in F6:
- the correlative diptych illustrated by E3 and E4;
- the weakly desententialized noun clause introduced by a universal subordinator, as in E21 and
E64;
- the more strongly nominalized noun clause with a subordinating verb suffix illustrated in E26;
- the strongly nominalized adverbial clause represented by such absolute constructions as in
E11;
- the infinitival depending on a modal verb, as in E40 and E65;
- verb serialization as exemplified in E16.
Comparative linguists have always felt that the prevalence of such a construction type may
characterize - together with other features - a given language. For instance, the prevalence of the
correlative diptych is characteristic of Hittite; the strongly nominalized adverbial clause is typical
for Quechua and Tamil. Future research will ascertain which features tend to cluster, what are the
principles intrinsic to the model of F6 that account for such clustering, and how the different
construction types go together with other properties of each language, thus constituting more
complex and richer linguistic types.27
Abbreviations
Language names
BAMbara KOBon
FREnch LATin
GERman PORtuguese
HITtite QUEchua
ITAlian SANskrit
JACaltec TURkish
JAPanese WALbiri
27
Comrie 1984 on the typological connections of control properties, and Mithun 1984 on subordination in
polysynthetic languages, are contributions to this goal.
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 30
References
Aissen, Judith L. 1984, "Control and command in Tzotzil purpose clauses". BLS 10:559-571.
Biraud, Michèle 1985, "Propositions pour une description du système des subordonnants en
latin". Calboli (org.) 1985.
Bird, Charles S. 1968, "Relative clauses in Bambara". JWAL 5:35-47.
Bolinger, Dwight 1984, "Intonational signals of subordination". BLS 10:4501-413.
Bolkestein, Machtelt A. 1985, "Parameters in the expression of embedded predications: Latin
subordination and nominalization". Calboli (org.) 1985.
Bolkestein, Machelt A. et al. (eds.) 1985, Predicates and terms in functional grammar. Dordrecht
: FORIS.
Brettschneider, Gunter 1980, "Zur Typologie komplexer Sätze". Brettschneider, Gunter &
Lehmann, Christian (eds.) 1980, Wege zur Universalienforschung. Sprachwissenschaftliche
Beiträge zum 60. Geburtstag von Hansjakob Seiler. Tübingen : G. Narr; 192-198.
Brettschneider, Gunter 1984, "PARTIZIPATION verknüpft mit NEKTION". akup 59:2-22.
Brøndal, Viggo 1937, "Le problème de l'hypotaxe: réflexions sur la théorie des propositions".
Mélanges linguistiques et philologiques offerts ü M. Aleksander Belic 1937:241-249.
Calboli, Gualtiero (org.) 1985, Third Colloquium on Latin Linguistics. Bologna, April 1-4, 1985.
Carvalho, Paulo de 1985, "Subordination, parties de langue et signification. A propos de la
`construction' des `verbes de sentiment' (verba affectuum) et des `verbes de perception,
jugement et affirmation' (sentiendi, iudicandi, dicendi)". Calboli (org.) 1985.
Chafe, Wallace 1984, "How people use adverbial clauses". BLS 10:437-449.
Christol, Alain 1985, "Prolèpse et syntaxe indo-européenne". Calboli (org.) 1985.
Cole, Peter 1982, Imbabura Quechua. Amsterdam : North-Holland (LDS, 5).
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 31
Coleman, Robert G. 1985, "Absolute constructions: A Latin case history". Calboli (org.) 1985.
Comrie, Bernard 1984, "Subject and object control: syntax, semantics, pragmatics". BLS
10:450-464.
Cooreman, Ann 1984, "A semantic basis for the choice of complement clause types in
Chamorro". BLS 10:572-582.
Craig, Colette Grinevald 1977, The structure of Jacaltec. Austin & London : University of Texas
Press.
Davies, John 1981, Kobon. Amsterdam : North-Holland (LDS, 3).
Davison, Alice 1979, "Some mysteries of subordination". Studies in the Linguistic Sciences
9:105-128.
Delbrück, Berthold 1871, Der Gebrauch des Konjunktivs und Optativs im Sanskrit und
Griechischen. Halle : Waisenhaus (Syntaktische Forschungen, 1).
Dik, Simon 1985, "Formal and semantic adjustment of derived constructions". Bolkestein et al.
(eds.) 1985:1-28.
Dixon, R.M.W. 1984, "The semantic basis of syntactic properties". BLS 10: 583-595.
Drossard, Werner 1984, "KAUSATIVIERUNG und TRANSITIVIERUNG im Tagalog". akup
60:1-25.
Foley, William A. & Van Valin, Robert D., Jr. 1984, Functional syntax and universal grammar.
Cambridge : Cambridge University Press (Cambridge Studies in Linguistics, 38).
Givón, Talmy 1980, "The binding hierarchy and the typology of complements". Studies in
Language 4:333-378.
Green, Georgia M. 1976, "Main clause phenomena in subordinate clauses". Language
52:382-397.
Haiman, John 1978, "Conditionals are topics". Language 54:564-589.
Haiman, John 1983, "On some origins of switch reference marking". Haiman & Munro (eds.)
1983:105-128.
Haiman, John & Munro, Pamela (eds.) 1983, Switch-reference and universal grammar.
Proceedings of a Symposium on Switch-Reference and Universal Grammar. Amsterdam &
Philadelphia : Benjamins (TSL, 2).
Haiman, John & Thompson, Sandra A. 1984, "`Subordination' in universal grammar". BLS
10:510-523.
Hale, Kenneth L. 1976, "The adjoined relative clause in Australia". Dixon, Robert M.W. (ed.)
1976, Grammatical categories in Australian languages. Canberra : Australian Institute of
Aboriginal Studies (Linguistics Series, 22); 78-105.
Haudry, Jean 1973, "Parataxe, hypotaxe et corrélation dans la phrase latine". BSL 68(1):147-186.
Helander, Hans 1985, "Causal subordinate clauses and other expression of cause". Calboli (org.)
1985.
Hoff, François 1985, "Les ablatifs absolus irréguliers: un nouvel examen du problème". Calboli
(org.) 1985.
Jayaseelan, K.A. 1984, "Control in some sentential adjuncts of Malayalam". BLS 10:623-633.
Jespersen, Otto 1937, Analytic syntax. New York etc. : Holt, Rinehart & Winston (Transatlantic
Series in Linguistics). Reimpression 1969.
Lakoff, George 1984, "Performative subordinate clauses". BLS 10:472-480.
Lehmann, Christian 1979, "Zur Typologie des Lateinischen". Glotta57:237-253.
Lehmann, Christian 1982, Thoughts on grammaticalization. A programmatic sketch. Köln :
Institut für Sprachwissenschaft der Universität (akup, 48).
Christian Lehmann, Towards a typology of clause linkage 32
Van Valin, Robert D. 1984, "A typology of syntactic relations in clause linkage". BLS
10:542-558.
Wendt, Heinz Friedrich 1972, Langenscheidts Praktisches Lehrbuch Türkisch. Berlin etc. :
Langenscheidt.