0% found this document useful (0 votes)
71 views

Science and Law Statistics Primer

This document provides an introduction to using statistics in legal proceedings. It discusses how statistical tools can help answer questions around descriptive data, inference from samples to populations, evaluating scientific evidence, and predicting outcomes. Statistical science provides techniques for quantifying some uncertainties, like margins of error, but not all uncertainties can be precisely measured. The primer aims to help legal professionals understand how to evaluate evidence presented in court that has a statistical basis. It covers common types of scientific evidence like trace evidence and impression evidence in appendices.

Uploaded by

Sankaty Light
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
71 views

Science and Law Statistics Primer

This document provides an introduction to using statistics in legal proceedings. It discusses how statistical tools can help answer questions around descriptive data, inference from samples to populations, evaluating scientific evidence, and predicting outcomes. Statistical science provides techniques for quantifying some uncertainties, like margins of error, but not all uncertainties can be precisely measured. The primer aims to help legal professionals understand how to evaluate evidence presented in court that has a statistical basis. It covers common types of scientific evidence like trace evidence and impression evidence in appendices.

Uploaded by

Sankaty Light
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 72

THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

The use
of statistics
in legal
proceedings
A PRIMER FOR COURTS
2 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

The use of statistics in legal proceedings:


a primer for courts
Issued: November 2020 DES6439
ISBN: 978-1-78252-486-1

The text of this work is licensed under the terms


of the Creative Commons Attribution License,
which permits unrestricted use provided the
original author and source are credited.

The licence is available at:


creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0

Images are not covered by this licence.

To request additional copies of this document


please contact:

The Royal Society


6 – 9 Carlton House Terrace
London SW1Y 5AG
T +44 20 7451 2571
E [email protected]
W royalsociety.org/science-and-law

This primer can be viewed online at


royalsociety.org/science-and-law
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 3

Contents
Summary, introduction and scope 5

1. What is statistical science? 6


1.1 Use of statistical science and types of evidence 8
1.2 Communication of the probative value when statistical science is used 10

2. Probability and the principles of evaluating scientific evidence 13


2.1 What probability is not 13
2.2 Personal probabilities 14
2.3 Datasets containing relevant past observations 16
2.4 Probative value expressed as a likelihood ratio 17
2.5 Bayes’ theorem and the likelihood ratio 20

3. Issues with the potential for misunderstanding 22


3.1 Prosecutor’s fallacy 22
3.2 Defence attorney’s fallacy 22
3.3 Combining evidence 22
3.4 Coincidences and rare events 24
3.5 Interpretation of ‘beyond reasonable doubt’ and ‘balance of probabilities’ 24

4. The role of expert witnesses and what should be expected from them 25

5. Conclusions and the future 27

Appendices 29
Appendix 1. The use of probability 29
Appendix 2. Evaluation of trace evidence 38
Appendix 3. Evaluation of impression evidence 46
Appendix 4. Statistical significance 57
Appendix 5. Causation and relative risk 59

References 67
4 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Science and the law primers


Foreword
The judicial primers project is a unique collaboration between members of the judiciary,
the Royal Society and the Royal Society of Edinburgh. The primers have been created
under the direction of a Steering Group initially chaired by Lord Hughes of Ombersley
who was succeeded by Lady Justice Rafferty DBE, and are designed to assist the
judiciary when handling scientific evidence in the courtroom. They have been written
by leading scientists and members of the judiciary, peer reviewed by practitioners and
approved by the Councils of the Royal Society and the Royal Society of Edinburgh.

Each primer presents an easily understood, accurate position on the scientific topic in
question, and considers the limitations of the science and the challenges associated
with its application. The way scientific evidence is used can vary between jurisdictions,
but the underpinning science and methodologies remain consistent. For this reason
we trust these primers will prove helpful in many jurisdictions throughout the world
and assist the judiciary in their understanding of scientific topics. The primers are not
intended to replace expert scientific evidence; they are intended to help understand
it and assess it, by providing a basic, and so far as possible uncontroversial, statement
of the underlying science.

The production of this primer on the use of statistics in legal proceedings has been
led by Professor Niamh Nic Daéid FRSE. We are most grateful to her, to the Executive
Director of the Royal Society, Dr Julie Maxton CBE, the Chief Executive of the Royal
Society of Edinburgh, Dr Rebekah Widdowfield, and the members of the Primers
Steering Group, the Editorial Board and the Writing Group. Please see the back page
for a full list of acknowledgements.

Sir Venki Ramakrishnan Dame Anne Glover


President of the Royal Society President of the Royal Society of Edinburgh
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 5

Summary, introduction and scope

The aim of this primer is to provide assistance to the judiciary and legal professionals
in understanding the principles of evaluating evidence (that has a statistical basis)
presented in the courts. The primer is presented in two parts. The first part provides
a general introduction to the use of statistical and probabilistic tools within legal
processes with some examples presented, including some relating to evidence types
commonly presented to the courts.

The second part consists of five appendices.

Appendices 1 – 3 provide specific information about how statistical and probabilistic


tools may be used in assisting the delivery of evaluative opinions by forensic
practitioners relating to common types of scientific evidence encountered primarily
in criminal cases, for example trace evidence (eg fibres, glass, DNA) and impression
evidence (eg footwear marks, toolmarks, fingerprints).

Appendices 4 and 5 relate to specific statistical methods and to their use in assessing
statistical significance and relative risk. These areas generally have more relevance in
civil proceedings.

This short guide cannot equip the judiciary and legal professionals with all the
necessary skills required, but it should be useful for signposting where problems
may arise and where external expertise may be needed.
6 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

1. What is statistical science?


Reasoning about data is increasingly recognised as an essential skill for modern life.
Fact-finding and the assessment of expert evidence in court cases often requires an
understanding of probability, statistics and numbers. Various different statistical and
probabilistic tools can be used to address different questions relating to the context
of individual cases presented to the courts and the choice of which tools to use will
depend on the questions that need to be addressed. Standard types of questions
which can be answered by statistical science may be categorised as follows:

• Descriptive statistics: eg What is the number of rapes reported in the country?


How many drugs of a particular type are found in drug seizures?

• Inference from observed data to a larger population: eg Given the responses


to the British Crime Survey, what is the estimated number of illegal drug users
in the UK?

• Inference from observed data to a scientific conclusion: eg Did the exposure to


the emissions from an incinerator raise the risk of birth defects1?

• Prediction: eg Given a set of characteristics, what is the chance that the accused
will reoffend2?

• Evaluation: The evaluation of scientific findings in court uses probability as a measure


of uncertainty. This is based upon the findings, associated data, expert knowledge,
case-specific propositions and conditioning information3.

All these situations are characterised by uncertainty, and probability theory provides
the tools and language for handling and communicating uncertainty.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 7

Statistical science has developed a wide range of powerful techniques for quantifying
the impact of some sources of uncertainty, eg calculating margins of error from a survey,
measuring the support for a proposition (also called a hypothesis) from observed data
or assessing the probability of a future event. Other sources of uncertainty are not
so easily quantified but can still be informally assessed and communicated, eg those
arising from the reliability of survey respondents, the quality of scientific studies and
the relevance of available and good quality datasets to the facts of a legal case.

Unavoidable uncertainty about the future is often termed chance, also known as
aleatory uncertainty, and the assignment of probabilities to future events is familiar.
Legal cases generally deal with uncertainty in the sense of lack of knowledge, also
known as epistemic uncertainty. Fortunately, the theory of probability can still be applied
in this context. Uncertainty of measurement can also arise and this, in general, can be
characterised for objective measurements (eg how much of a controlled substance may
be in an analysed sample) but is more challenging for more subjective measurements
(eg in the examination of toolmarks).
8 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

1.1 Use of statistical science and types of evidence


Statistical science can be called upon to support expert knowledge when dealing
with a variety of types of evidence and proceedings. These include:
• evaluation of DNA evidence4;
• evaluation of trace evidence, eg fibres, glass, paint or firearms discharge residues
(Appendix 2);
• evaluation of pattern-matching evidence, eg toolmarks, ballistics and fingerprint
evidence (Appendix 3); and
• causation of illness or injury in a civil case, where it may be helpful to apply
epidemiological research (the study of occurrence, aetiology, prognosis and
treatment of illness in populations) to individual cases (Appendix 5).

The way in which statistical science may be used in a legal context is illustrated in
Figure 1.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 9

FIGURE 1

The process by which statistical science may be used in legal proceedings and in
which relevant past data are used to draw conclusions about the facts of a current case.

1. Identification of an observation related to a specific item of evidence relevant


to some aspect of the current case.

2. Providing a list of appropriate potential propositions concerning the


evidence, which may include prosecution allegations and any defence
alternative assertions.

3. Identifying resources containing relevant past data or creating new


relevant datasets.

4. Methods for using the accumulated information in a database (if available) to


derive a numerical or verbal expression of the probative value of the observation
related to the item of evidence with regard to the competing propositions.

5. Communication of probative value of evidence.


10 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

1.2 Communication of the probative value when statistical science is used


When conclusions based on statistical science are drawn from data, it is crucial that
the data and the reasoning supporting those conclusions are transparent. Under the
term ‘intelligent transparency’ Baroness Onora O’Neill5 has argued that the data
and reasoning must be:
• accessible: ie easily available and not, for example, hidden behind
a proprietary algorithm;
• understandable: to everyone involved in the case, including a jury;
• useable: they address current specific concerns; and
• assessable: where the ‘working’ is open to scrutiny by legal and other professionals.

There is always uncertainty involved in statistics, particularly in assessing the


relevance of available historical data to a current case. There may also be some
extent of disagreement between different professionals, but this may not be of real
substance. Statistical conclusions are only as reliable as the model (and data) from
which they are derived.

Estimates are neither wholly right nor wholly wrong, conclusions are not mechanistic
and sometimes the only database available is the experience of the professional. In
such situations transparency is particularly necessary and the experience needs to be
documented with emphasis on the relevance to the case in question. The professional
judgement of the appropriate experts (expert knowledge) is inevitably involved in each
stage of the process outlined in Figure 1. Probability is a conceptual device that helps
us think and reason logically when faced with uncertainty about the occurrence of a
questioned event in the past, present or the future.

Probability helps us think clearly and coherently about uncertain events.


THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 11

FIGURE 2

Expected frequency tree when repeating a double-flip of a coin 100 times. We would
expect the first flip to be heads in 50 of these experiments, and both flips to be heads
in 25.

Second
25
First
Heads
50
Tails
Heads 25

100

Tails 25
Heads
50
Tails
25

By way of an example to explain probability, we can use the idea of ‘expected


frequency’ (such a frequentist approach is not appropriate in forensic inference
where probability is conditional and personal but is used here to explain the notion
of probability). When faced with a question concerning likely outcomes if a coin is
flipped twice, you ask yourself: What would I expect to happen if I tried the experiment
many times? Take the example that you repeated this double-flip experiment 100
times. As shown in Figure 2, in 25 out of these 100 repeats you would expect to get
two heads. Therefore, the reasoning goes, the probability that on a particular attempt
you would get two heads is 1 in 4, or ¼. Which, fortunately, is the correct answer.
This probability can be equivalently expressed as a fraction (¼), a decimal (0.25),
a percentage (25%), a proportion (1 out of 4) or betting odds (3 to 1 against).
12 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Probability follows basic rules in which:


• probabilities of all possible events add to 1 (eg in Figure 2 there are four possible
events, so their probabilities add to 1); the probability of not getting two heads is
¾ or 75%;
• probabilities are multiplied for sequences of events which are independent
(eg the probability of two heads in a row is ½ × ½ = ¼); and
• probabilities are added when considering probabilities of separate
(mutually exclusive) sets of events (eg the probability of getting two
heads or two tails is ¼ + ¼ = ½).

It is a common misapprehension that probabilities can only be used for future events
with some randomness. While it is true that an event has either happened or not,
many statisticians will feel that it is reasonable to assign probabilities to our personal
uncertainty about unknown facts, as the following example shows.

Suppose I have a coin and I ask you for your probability that it will come up heads.
You answer “50:50”, or similar (50% or ½). Then I flip it, cover up the result before either
of us sees it and again ask for your probability that it is heads. You may, after a pause,
say “50:50”. Then I take a quick look at the coin, without showing you, and repeat the
question. Again, if you are like most people, you eventually say “50:50”. This simple
exercise reveals a major distinction between two types of uncertainty: what is known
as aleatory uncertainty before I flip the coin – the ‘chance’ of an unpredictable event –
and epistemic uncertainty after I flip the coin – an expression of our personal ignorance
about an event that is fixed but unknown. In forensic science we are almost always
concerned with epistemic uncertainty about the facts of a past situation.

Probabilities are commonly used to express epistemic uncertainty in legal settings.


Section 2 provides more detail on the meaning of probability in such contexts, including
Bayes’ theorem and the role of the likelihood ratio (LR).
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 13

2. P
 robability and the principles of
evaluating scientific evidence
Specifically, in a legal context, probability can help fact-finders assess the impact of
evidence on the truth or otherwise of a particular proposition. It has a well-documented
history in academic legal literature6. Each item of evidence can be used to support one
or more proposition(s). Evidence may, on occasion, point directly to incriminating or
exculpating a suspect of a particular crime, but will more likely have probative value in
discriminating between competing propositions for either the source of some material
found in relation to a scene of the crime or an alleged activity connected to a crime.

For example, competing source-level propositions concerning a fragment of glass found


on a suspect’s clothing might include that the glass fragments came, or did not come,
from a particular broken window. Competing activity-level propositions might be that the
suspect broke or did not break the particular glass in question at a particular time.

2.1 What probability is not


Probability is not an inherent property of material or objects. For example, a pack of
playing cards does not possess a specific probability for an ace being drawn from it.
But you may say, given your current state of knowledge about the properties of the
pack, “that the probability of drawing an ace from a pack of playing cards is 4 in 52, or
1 in 13”. Such a probability is based on certain assumptions. It is assumed that, among
other considerations, (i) it is a full pack of 52 cards; (ii) it is well shuffled; (iii) the cards are
exactly the same shape, size and condition so as not to influence the physical act of a
draw; and (iv) the person drawing the card will do so without any bias. The probability
figure of 1/13 (or roughly 8%) is your own best assessment, based on your knowledge of
the composition of a full pack of cards and based on the implicit assumptions outlined.
It is a probability with which most of us would agree and it is one that would work
well in gambling games, but it is not an inherent property of the aces in the pack or
of any of the other cards in the pack. The implication here is that even in cases with
well-structured chance devices, such as a deck of cards, if you have already been
dealt a couple of aces, your probability for the next card being an ace has dropped
considerably. Probability is personal: it is personal in the sense that it depends on the
knowledge available to the person making the judgement and on the assumptions
they make.
14 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

But that is not to say that personal probability is conjured up on a whim or a preference,
or to suggest it is not based on acquired data. Where relevant data are available, it is
expected that they will be taken into account in assigning a probability. For example,
suppose that reliable information is available on the proportion of individuals in a target
population that possess a particular observable feature, such as skin, hair or eye colour.
Then, our assessment that a person drawn randomly from that population will show a
particular feature of interest ought to be informed by the available knowledge about the
composition of the population. When new testing systems are used, the frequencies of
given traits in a population will not be widely known.

2.2 Personal probabilities


We make personal assignments of probability every day:

• What is the probability that I will miss the bus this morning if I have one more
cup of coffee?

• What is the probability that I will be caught if I break into this property?

In such circumstances, the probabilities that we assign, albeit not mathematically


evaluated or even verbalised in this way, will depend on our knowledge and
understanding of the factors and risks involved. Such probabilities are also known
as personal ‘degrees of belief’.

Some people may attempt to answer the above questions by thinking of past
experiences in similar situations. For example, they may consider how many mornings
in the past they have missed the bus when having one more cup of coffee, though this
may give rise to many other questions, such as the extent to which today’s morning is
comparable to previous experiences.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 15

Statisticians and scientists refer to data on the proportion of times an event has occurred
as a relative frequency. However, and most importantly, there are some situations
for which relative frequencies cannot be meaningfully conceived. In legal cases, for
example, the fact-finder must deal with singular, non-repeatable, one-off events for
which the notion of relative frequency may not be helpful. This does not preclude
the possibility that useful frequency data may be available (eg scientific data on the
occurrence of genetic features or the prevalence of a disease) to help decide aspects
of the case (examples of this type of use are in Appendices 4 and 5). Where such data
are available and are relevant, they ought to be used in probability assignment as one
source of information among others.

Experts assign personal probabilities based on their experience, knowledge and


understanding of their type of expert evidence. However, a challenge with such
probabilities is the potential influence of cognitive effects. The reliability of expert-
assigned probabilities is determined by various factors, including:

• the extent and relevance of the expert’s experience;

• the ability of the expert to compile and store systematically those experiences
in their memory;

• the expert’s ability to recall accurately the relevant data;

• the expert’s ability to avoid and mitigate against bias while inputting expert
knowledge; and

• calibration, ie measuring the extent to which those events assigned a probability


of (say) 40% actually do have a relative frequency of occurrence close to 40%.

In general, the more that experts base their assignments of probability on relevant,
shared and robust data, the greater is the trustworthiness of those assignments. The
more they base their assignments on their recalled experience and knowledge and
on their intuition, the more those assignments will be open to justified challenge.
16 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

2.3 Datasets containing relevant past observations


Evaluation of evidence using likelihood ratios (LRs) often involves the use of datasets
and statistical assumptions. It is important that these datasets, assumptions and
calculations are clearly stated and appropriate for the problem. Validation tests should
be carried out to gauge the statistical assumptions and to ensure that the LR values
presented in court are reliable. Datasets should be from relevant populations and
should not bias the results of the analysis.

The Court of Appeal decided in Regina v Abadom7 that an expert is entitled to draw
upon material produced by others in the field in which his or her expertise lies, and
indeed where any reliable data are available that bear upon the question the expert
is addressing. It is part of the duty of the expert to take this into account. So, a crucial
judgement concerns the reliability and relevance of available data. The dataset
should have high intrinsic quality, reliability and high relevance to the question being
addressed by the use of the data. A national dataset of informally collected examples
of glass or footwear from people’s homes may be of limited relevance to local criminal
investigations but of value in informing background abundance of the items in question,
while local datasets collected to address specific aspects of criminal incidents or of
suspect populations may have high relevance. Ideally, there will be an appropriately
large enough random sample from a population that matches agreed features of the
case, but this is a high bar that is rarely achieved and means that expert judgement and
full transparency are required to deal with such limitations.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 17

2.4 Probative value expressed as a likelihood ratio


Technically, the LR is the probability of the evidence assuming that proposition A is true
divided by the probability of the evidence assuming tha proposition B is true:

probability of the evidence, if A is true


LR =
probability of the evidence, if B is true

LRs are typically attached to DNA evidence in which a ‘match’ of some degree is found
between the suspect’s DNA profile and the DNA profile derived from a trace found at
the scene of a crime. The two competing hypotheses are that the DNA profile in the
recovered trace material originates from the suspect or it originates from someone else,
so that we can express the LR as:

probability of the DNA profile ‘match’, if the suspect left the trace
LR =
probability of the DNA profile ‘match’, if the trace was left by
someone else

The ‘DNA evidence’ is the suspect’s DNA together with the DNA trace from the crime
scene. For the specific situation when the trace contains plenty of DNA and it is deemed
to have come from one person, the LR above can be written, after some mathematical
operations and given some assumptions, as:

1
LR =
random match probability

The random match probability is the probability of finding an evidence match if selecting
at random from within a particular population. For example, in the context of a DNA
sample8, it is the probability of observing a DNA profile of an unknown person that is the
same as the DNA profile from a crime scene stain (and assuming a particular population
genetic model). Typical LRs for DNA evidence are in the millions or billions, although
the exact values may be contested, such as when there are complications due to the
traces containing a mix of DNA from multiple people. Further information is provided in
Forensic DNA analysis: a primer for the courts9.
18 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Table 1 shows an example of a verbal scale used for communicating LRs (a similar
example can be found in Willis10). In most cases (including DNA at an activity level where
the activity that caused the DNA to be deposited is the issue) the LR will be based on
a semi-quantitative (ie an order of magnitude) assessment and a verbal equivalent may
be presented to the court. The reason that verbal expressions are defined numerically
is to provide a consistency in their use rather than to translate available numbers into a
common language. In those cases where a quantitative assignment is possible there is
a strong argument for presenting the LR value to the court without a verbal qualifier but
also an argument for avoiding the risk that lay persons (eg juries) may misunderstand
conclusions stated in numbers as absolute measurements. An LR equal to 1 supports
neither proposition preferentially.

The LR is not a specific measurement, but rather is the weight of evidence of the
scientific findings in two competing scenarios (prosecution and defence). There will
almost always be some natural variation in LRs depending on different assumptions
and the quality and relevance of the datasets and what is known about the transfer,
persistence, recovery and background abundance of the particular type of evidence
under scrutiny. The value of the LR on the scale shown in Table 1 is preferably assigned
based on robust data extracted from a relevant dataset.

With good quality and relevant data it may be possible to generate a numerical
assessment using the LR relating to evidential support. However, often the available
data are either poor or non-existent (particularly true for knowledge relating to the
transfer of material and its persistence once transferred). In such cases, the expert
forms a personal opinion based on domain knowledge of processes and on personal
experience that can be disclosed and audited. In these situations, verbal expressions
or orders of magnitude of the LR may be helpful and the basis of any statement of
expert opinion formed this way must always be made clear.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 19

TABLE 1

Example of verbal interpretations of likelihood ratios (LRs) – in this case for source-
level propositions.

Value of LR Verbal equivalent Supported proposition

<0.000001 Extremely strong support

0.0001-0.000001 Very strong support

0.001 – 0.0001 Strong support


Different source
0.01 – 0.001 Moderately strong support

0.1 – 0.01 Moderate support

<1 – 0.1 Limited (or weak) support

About 1 Neutral Neither proposition is supported


in preference to the other

>1 – 10 Limited (weak) support

10 – 100 Moderate support

100 – 1,000 Moderately strong support


Same source
1,000 – 10,000 Strong support

10,000 – 1,000,000 Very strong support

>1,000,000 Extremely strong support


20 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

2.5 Bayes’ theorem and the likelihood ratio


Bayes’ theorem provides a general rule for updating probabilities about a proposition in
the light of new evidence. It says that:

the LR x the prior


the posterior (final) odds for a proposition =
(initial) odds for the proposition

For example, suppose a hypothetical screening test for doping in sports is claimed to
be ‘95% accurate’, meaning that if an athlete is doping there is a 95% chance (probability
0.95; sensitivity) of obtaining a positive test result, and if the athlete is a non-doper
there is a 95% chance (probability 0.95; specificity) of obtaining a negative test result.
Such general performance characteristics have been determined through tests under
controlled conditions, ie by applying the test in so-called ground truth cases, where it is
known whether a tested person is doping or not.

Assuming that the odds of an athlete taking drugs prior to being subject to a screening
test are 1 in 50 (1:50), then if an athlete tests positive what is the probability that they are
truly doping?

The LR (explained in Section 2.4) is the probability of a positive test given the
proposition that the athlete is doping (95%) divided by the probability of a positive test
given the proposition that the athlete is not doping (5%, ie 1 - specificity). This ratio is 19
(LR = 0.95/0.05 = 19).

Bayes’ theorem tells us that the posterior odds of the athlete having taken drugs can be
computed by multiplying the prior odds of that proposition by the LR provided by the
positive test. In this form, we have to work with odds not probability. Odds are related
mathematically to probability and a very simple conversion can be used to give the
value for probability where the odds of m:n correspond to the probability m/(m + n).
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 21

So, for the doping example,

• the prior odds for the proposition ‘athlete is doping’ versus ‘athlete is not doping’
are 1:50, which correspond to a probability of 1/(1 + 50) or a prior probability of
approximately 0.02 (the actual value is 0.0196, which is equivalent to 1.96%);

• the LR is 0.95/0.05 = 19; and

• therefore, by Bayes’ theorem, the posterior odds that the athlete is doping are (1:50)
x 19 = 19:50, giving a posterior probability of doping of 19/(19 + 50) ≈ 0.28 or 28%.

So, even though drug testing could be claimed to be ‘95% accurate’ (based on the
sensitivity and specificity metric) this does not mean that, in the event of a positive result,
there is a 95% chance that the athlete is doping. In this example, the probability that the
athlete is doping, given a positive test result, is approximately 28%. The posterior odds
that an athlete is doping crucially depend on the prior odds for the proposition ‘athlete is
doping’ versus ‘athlete is not doping’ (in the example this was 1:50) prior to considering
the result of the screening test (the LR result). This means that if conclusions are drawn
from test results in isolation there could be misinterpretations of what is meant by the
accuracy of the test. This could cause conclusions such as athletes being incorrectly
accused of doping because they failed a drug test.

In practice, the Court of Appeal has ruled that Bayes’ theorem should not be used by
a jury to combine and weigh evidence11, but LRs assessed by experts are permitted if
they have a sound basis. Many real-world cases involve multiple and related items of
evidence, making probabilistic inference much more complex and intricate than the
illustrative doping example given above.
22 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

3. Issues with the potential for


misunderstanding
3.1 Prosecutor’s fallacy
The prosecutor's fallacy occurs when the probability of the evidence (matching DNA
profile, glass fragment of the same refractive index as the fragment recovered from
the target window, etc) given innocence (the random match probability) is incorrectly
interpreted as the probability of innocence given the evidence. This is formally known
as ‘transposing the conditional’, and is a clear breach of logic that becomes obvious
when the correct statement ‘If it’s a dog, the chances are very high that it has four legs’
is transposed to ‘If an animal has four legs, the chances are very high that it’s a dog’.

3.2 Defence attorney’s fallacy


The defence attorney’s fallacy occurs when it is reported how many people with
the matching characteristics are likely to be found in a defined population (eg the
population of the UK). This assumes that the perpetrator is part of some arbitrarily
large population and that there is no other information available, so that everyone is
equally likely to be the perpetrator. Under these assumptions, it is deduced that there
is a small probability that the suspect is the perpetrator. It is a fallacy to infer from this
that the evidence is weak.

3.3 Combining evidence


A standard statistical approach is to include all evidence in a single calculation, each
item weighted by its relative strength expressed by LRs. This technique is used, for
example, in email spam filters and in Case study 1.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 23

CASE STUDY 1

An archaeological case
On Saturday 25 August 2012, archaeologists began an excavation for Richard III’s
remains by digging in a car park in Leicester. Within a few hours they found their first
skeleton and the question was whether this was Richard III. Table 2 shows the specific
items of evidence and their likelihood ratios (LRs) regarding the propositions that the
skeleton was that of Richard III. These LRs were, as far as possible, based on sound
statistical evidence, but there was inevitably some uncertainty so that conservative
values were assigned, and verbally interpreted here (because of their uncertainty
and therefore qualitative nature) using the terms in Table 1.

Probability theory permits, given certain assumptions about the evidence, the
multiplication of these LRs to give a final number that represents ‘extremely strong’
evidence to support the proposition that the skeleton was, rather than was not, that of
Richard III. Of course, the final assignation of the skeleton would not be based on the
LR alone but would involve other evidence as well.

TABLE 2

Likelihood ratios (LRs) assessed for items of evidence found on a skeleton


recovered in Leicester.

Evidence LR Verbal equivalent


(conservative
assignment)

Radiocarbon dating CE 1456 – 1530 1.8 Weak support

Age and sex of skeleton 5.3 Weak support

Scoliosis 212 Moderately strong support

Post-mortem wounds 42 Moderate support

Mitochondrial DNA match 478 Moderately strong support

Y chromosome not matching 0.16 Weak evidence against

Combined evidence 6.5 million Extremely strong support


24 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Such an approach is used when combining multiple aspects of a forensic scientist’s


findings, eg when combining individual peaks within a DNA profile. Some contrast
this with the way in which the law approaches fact-finding in civil cases: ‛If a legal rule
requires a fact to be proved (a “fact in issue”), a judge or jury must decide whether or
not it happened. There is no room for a finding that it might have happened. The law
operates a binary system in which the only values are 0 and 1. The fact either happened
or it did not12.’

In statistical science, however, the uncertainty regarding facts is carried through any
chain of reasoning and influences trust in the final conclusions. The expert can properly
ask what the level of probability or uncertainty is. However, courts are perfectly used to
bringing into the calculation of a primary conclusion uncertain disputed facts along the
way and without necessarily resolving each uncertainty. Sometimes evidence going to
disputed contributory facts, when combined with other evidence going to a different
disputed contributory fact, may enable a conclusion to be reached safely on the
principal fact in issue. Likewise, juries are commonly directed that they do not need to
resolve every dispute in the evidence, so long as they are satisfied beyond reasonable
doubt of the guilt of the accused. Some disputed facts can safely be left unresolved and
scientific findings will generally have a degree of uncertainty rather than a definite value.

3.4 Coincidences and rare events


Intuition is notoriously poor at assessing how ‘surprising’ an event is. Just because
an event is exceedingly rare for a particular person (eg winning the lottery at odds
of 45 million to 1 against) does not necessarily mean it is a surprising event to occur
to someone (because of the large number of tickets sold). When three major plane
crashes occurred within an eight-day period in 2014, it appeared to many to be beyond
coincidence. But there is around a 60% probability that such a ‘cluster’ will happen at
some point over a ten-year period.

3.5 Interpretation of ‘beyond reasonable doubt’ and ‘balance of probabilities’


These might appear to be expressions of probability for either criminal guilt or on
behalf of one of the sides in a civil case, respectively. In fact, they relate to the strength
of the evidence required by the legal system, the prior odds provided for, or against,
a particular explanation of events and the relative merits and losses associated with
accurate and erroneous consequences of a decision.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 25

4. The role of expert witnesses and what


should be expected from them
The guidelines for expert witnesses in England and Wales13 provide a useful reminder
that expert witnesses have a duty to give independent, impartial and unbiased
evidence, and not to stray outside their area of expertise. Crucially, any reference
to data that have been collected by others has to be justified, and their limitations
communicated clearly. Similar considerations apply in civil cases14.

The Forensic Science Regulator has the responsibility for reporting standards used
by forensic experts and reports must be structured to provide an understanding of
the probative value of the evidence. Crucially, legal professionals should be able
to recognise complex and non-standard situations in which an expert in probability,
forensic inference or statistical reasoning may need to be consulted.

Communication of probability is fraught with the potential for misunderstanding. One


only has to look at the classic problem of the prosecutor’s fallacy to see what may
happen when such misunderstanding occurs. Judgments from the Court of Appeal (in
England and Wales) relating to expert evidence also reveal a variation in the way in
which the Court understands and accepts or rejects probabilistic evidence. In some
ways, this misunderstanding reflects a basic difference in the manner by which people
reason and discuss issues. Some prefer a narrative style of argument while others prefer
a numerical approach. This difference does not necessarily correlate with whether you
are a scientist or a lawyer.

It is important also to recognise with whom the expert is attempting to communicate.


In a criminal court it will be with the magistrates or the jury; in civil litigation it will be
with the judge. The onus is on the expert to use simple and accurate language in the
communication of their evidence so that they are understood. It is equally important that
the lawyers ask the experts questions such that the full nature of the evidence together
with its strengths and limitations are exposed to the recipients (magistrates, judge, jury).
26 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

The Forensic Science Regulator is developing a quality standard for forensic


practitioners on the development of evaluative opinions. This will set out a standardised,
transparent approach to the expression of opinions for forensic science disciplines,
based on case-specific propositions. Where it is not possible for numerical values to
be assigned to probabilities, a revised verbal scale, with fewer categories than
presented in Table 1, is being considered. The standard is also expected to require
each expert to set out the specific data and/or experience and expertise on which their
evaluation is based.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 27

5. Conclusions and the future


As forensic tests become more sensitive, and the amount and complexity of scientific
evidence increases, there will be a need for more sophisticated models and statistics
to obtain meaningful inferences and interpretation of the evidence given the specific
circumstances of a case. This includes the generation of ground truth datasets (where
the provenance of the data is known) relating to trace evidence and pattern evidence
as well as an understanding of the transfer, persistence, recovery and background
abundance of materials in general and in case-specific circumstances. The development
of concepts of ‘calibrated knowledge’ and their use in decision-making in respect to
the generation of propositions and the evaluation of these propositions within the
context of cases are highly relevant. There is an increasing use of LR calculations for
pattern evidence where the LR is obtained by dividing two probability assignments. Both
require judgement on the part of the expert based on a corpus of knowledge that can
be divided into two broad categories:

[S] Knowledge derived from robust systematic studies, ideally published, where the
relevant features have been measured and studied statistically.

[E] Knowledge derived from personal experience, ie the expert’s training and
professional experience in their forensic specialism.

Published scientific data are used wherever possible as a basis for these assessments.
If relevant published data are not available, then data from unpublished sources or
ad hoc experiments may be used as long as they have been peer reviewed and
documented on file. Knowledge such as personal experience in similar cases and
peer consultations may be used provided that the practitioner can justify their use
and demonstrate their basis15. In addition to the nature of the knowledge invoked, it
is critically important that the expert discloses transparently the nature, provenance,
extent and relevancy of the knowledge used to inform their LR. Transparency is
paramount to ensure scrutiny and ultimately to allow courts to assess credibility, ie
how LRs were derived and their robustness. Because LRs may be based on different
experts’ knowledge, there may be a legitimate and understandable difference in opinion
between two experts.
28 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

These developments will require improvements to the content of expert reports, to


the competence of counsel to understand such reports and interrogate experts and to
the ability of the judiciary to handle such evidence appropriately. These improvements
will require actions by many stakeholders, including the Forensic Science Regulator
to set standards for reporting, the academic and professional bodies to agree that the
education of legal professionals should include dealing with scientific evidence, and
training related to scientific evidence and its evaluation within the judicial colleges.

The lack of a common language among experts, lawyers, judges and lay people
about what is meant by probability and statistics and how these concepts are used
by experts to provide answers to specific case-related questions remains challenging.
This can lead to misunderstandings and confusion.

There are also gaps in data and knowledge related to many types of evidence, including
how materials transfer between people and between people and surfaces. Similarly,
data on the persistence of materials once transferred and on background abundance
of materials are sparse. This requires a greater reliance on the expert’s knowledge and
understanding of evidence and applying this to specific case circumstances.

In some circumstances where data are well known and well defined (eg repetitive
measurements made by a scientific instrument) significance testing (Appendix 4) can
be undertaken to provide a fundamental tool in uncovering relevant information about
the data and what inferences can be made. An example may be the measurement
of uncertainty or error relating to the determination of alcohol or drugs in a sample.
Concepts such as causation and relative risk (Appendix 5) can be explored with the help
of statistical methods; however, the significance of such associations remains primarily a
matter of expert judgement.

This primer forms only a basic introduction to the evaluation of evidence based on
statistical and probabilistic reasoning.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 29

Appendix 1: The use of probability


Probability in a legal context
With the exception of evidence that is eliminative, scientific findings rarely provide
evidence that is conclusive for a particular questioned event. The probative value of
scientific findings needs to be assessed and taken into account by the fact-finders when
considering whether a contested event in the past, for example whether the defendant
committed the offence, is true. The notion of probability applies not only to the ultimate
question but also to intermediate questions, such as: What is the probability that the
defendant grabbed the victim’s (complainer in Scotland) clothing at the relevant time?

In criminal cases, the fact-finder must believe beyond reasonable doubt and be sure
that the event occurred. There have been attempts to define what ‘beyond reasonable
doubt’ and to ‘be sure’ mean in numerical, probabilistic terms but there is no agreement
on, or even a strong drive to adopt, a particular number.

In contrast, the fact-finder in civil cases relies on the concept of balance of probabilities.
One common understanding is that the fact-finder must form a judgement, based on the
strength of the evidence, as to whether their belief for the plaintiff’s contention is greater
than or less than 50%. The notion of probability may also be invoked in court when
expert evidence is being adduced.

As an example, suppose the court is being presented with evidence of a particular


bloodstain pattern in a case of assault. The expert testifies that it is ‛highly probable’
the bloodstain pattern would be observed on the defendant’s shoes if the defendant
had, as alleged, kicked the victim. The notion here is of ‛observations’ and expert
expectations of observing material/analytical results, etc if a proposition were true rather
than to discuss the ‘cause’ of the bloodstain pattern. Discussion of ‘cause’ requires the
generation of explanations after the observations have been made.

To avoid possible confusion, the expert should explain to the court that ‛highly probable’
is a description of their expectation that this particular pattern of bloodstaining on the
defendant’s shoes would have been observed if the defendant had, as alleged, kicked
the victim. But, for balance, it is necessary also to ask: What is the probability that this
pattern of bloodstaining would have been observed if the defendant had not kicked the
victim? The answer to this second question may be, for example, ‛very low’.
30 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

These assignments are the expert's probability judgements made after taking into
account the results of any experimentation aligned to the case circumstances, the body
of documented knowledge and data in the specialism, as well as their own experience
in the field. There are no such things as the ‛right’ or ‛correct’ probabilities in this case.

Expert explanations, or expressions of possibility


There is a form of expert opinion that does not involve assessment of probability and is
classified16 as an explanation (possibility). Phrases that an expert will use to express an
explanation include ‛consistent with’, ‛could have’ and ‛cannot exclude’. For example,
the expert may explain that “the finding of a fibre on the clothing of a suspect which
is indistinguishable from fibres of the complainer’s clothing could be the result of
secondary rather than primary transfer”. Such explanations, however, are generally
unhelpful for fact-finders when deciding on the truth of a contested event because
merely presenting explanations bears the potential of suggesting that the expert is
opining directly on the probability of the competing versions of the event rather than on
the value of the findings within the framework of circumstances of the alleged activities.

Explanations may offer assistance during the investigative proceedings but are
limited insofar as there is no assessment of the probative value of the findings. The
explanations may not be an exhaustive list of possibilities and there is no assessment
of how probable each explanation may be, rendering them generally not useful for
decision-making. However, while exploring alternative explanations (causes) for the
evidence is a perfectly valid procedure before a trial, questions about alternative
explanations may be posed later in court by defence counsel to dilute the force of
the principal conclusions.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 31

The basis for assigning probabilities


All probability assignments can be viewed as being conditioned on some form of
pre-existing information. That information could include relevant details of the
circumstances of the case in question, actuarial data, technical data, expert knowledge,
results of competency testing, etc. It is important for all those people who use and rely
on probability assignments to understand the notion of conditioning.

It is important for the people who make probability assignments to declare the
conditioning factors that have influenced those assignments – and it is important for the
recipients of expert information to probe the foundations of those declarations. It is also
vital that the fact-finder is made fully aware of those influences. The type and quantity
of conditioning information taken into account will vary depending on, first, the role of
the person assigning the probability within the fact-finding process and, second, the
question being asked.

For some roles and some types of question, the basis for assigning a probability may be
straightforward, but for others assigning a probability may be problematic because of a
lack of knowledge or a lack of relevant data or because it is not within the competence
of the person being asked the question. Case studies 2 and 3 provide two examples in
a criminal context to illustrate the sources of expert probability assignments.
32 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

CASE STUDY 2

A DNA case
The fact-finder is presented with expert evidence of matching DNA profiles extracted
from a sample from a defendant and from a bloodstain left at the scene of a crime.
A question for the fact-finder would be: Is the DNA extracted and analysed from
the bloodstain that of the defendant? And the ultimate question would be: Is the
defendant guilty of an offence? It is the fact-finder’s role to answer the two questions.
Whether they do so probabilistically is entirely their choice. It is not the expert’s role
to answer the two questions, however tempting it may be to answer the first question.
What the expert can do is to provide the fact-finder with their expert, justified view on
the probability of observing the DNA evidence, ie DNA profile from the bloodstain and
the defendant’s DNA profile, under two competing propositions. If we look at the first
question, the pair of competing propositions would be:

• Hp (the prosecution proposition): The DNA originated from the defendant.

• Hd (the defence proposition): The DNA originated from someone unrelated to


the defendant.

The expert can help the fact-finder by providing their probability of obtaining the DNA
evidence if the DNA had originated from the defendant and, alternatively, if it had
originated from someone unrelated to the defendant.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 33

The first probability assignment, ie the probability of obtaining the DNA


evidence if the DNA from the scene had come from the defendant, would be
based on the expert’s knowledge of the reliability of the process of DNA profiling
in producing the true profile from a stain. The expert should base their assessment
on whatever relevant data there may be. Turning to the alternative proposition,
additional considerations for assigning a probability of the DNA evidence come
into play. Under the proposition that the DNA had originated from someone
unrelated to the defendant, the probability of obtaining a match with the defendant
depends additionally on the proportion of individuals who have this DNA type in
the relevant population.

There are databases of DNA profiles from samples of people from various
ethnic groupings. These databases can be consulted to assess the rarity of the
(matching) DNA profile. That statistic can then be used as a basis, among other
considerations (such as genetic relationships among individuals), for assigning a
probability of obtaining a match IF the bloodstaining had originated from someone
unrelated to the defendant. What the expert brings to that assignment is knowledge
and understanding of the impact of relatedness among people, in the form of a
population-genetic model, and of choosing the most relevant database(s) for the case
in question. It is not just a simple question of using a frequency of occurrence as a
probability: a more subtle treatment is required.
34 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

CASE STUDY 3

A firearm discharge residue case


A person has been accused of discharging a firearm during the commission of a
robbery. The firearm was not recovered but spent cartridge cases were found at the
scene. Swabs were taken from the hands of the defendant about five hours after
the incident. Subsequent chemical analysis of the swabs revealed the presence of a
small amount of firearm discharge residue (FDR, also called a gunshot residue, GSR)
that had the same qualitative composition as the reference FDR recovered from the
cartridge cases.

The fact-finder’s questions would include: Did the FDR on the defendant’s hand
swabs come from the gun that fired the cartridges? And: What is the probability that
the defendant fired the gun, given matching FDR had been found? The expert’s
role is to offer probabilities for observing the particular FDR, given the truth of the
prosecution proposition and defence alternative that flow from the questions facing
the fact-finder. Looking at the issue of whether the defendant fired the gun, the two
competing propositions would be:

• Hp: The defendant fired the gun (at the relevant time).

• Hd: The defendant did not fire the gun (it was some other person).

Note that, for a sensible definition of the alternative proposition, it is relevant to


enquire, if possible, about what the defendant says. There may be situations in
which the defendant asserts that he was a bystander, or that he provided first aid to
the victim. These details are relevant to help specify the alternative proposition and
task-relevant conditioning information. Defining the alternative proposition as the
simple negation of the prosecution’s proposition is usually not sufficient. The expert
should be able to offer probabilities for obtaining the FDR evidence given the truth
of these propositions.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 35

The task to be addressed is that the propositions are contested – we do not know
which of the competing propositions is true. Did the defendant or someone else fire
the gun? The principles of inductive logic (where the conclusion may be probable
based upon the evidence presented) dictate that, to assess the probability that an
uncertain proposition is true, the probabilities of the evidence under the competing
propositions need to be considered (see Section 2.5 on doping). So, how does the
expert assign such probabilities?

Under the first proposition


The defendant fired the gun (at the relevant time): the expert would rely on whatever
is known generally about transfer and persistence of FDR and apply that knowledge
to assign a probability for obtaining the evidence under this proposition. The expert
may consider undertaking experiments to replicate as far as possible the conditions
of the incident, to provide more data and knowledge. The probability that they assign
will be their best assessment of the probability of obtaining the evidence. Other
experts may disagree with the assignment but, with transparent communication and
explanation, the expert should be able to demonstrate and justify how they arrived
at a particular probability.

Under the alternative proposition


The defendant did not fire the gun (at a relevant time, it was some other person):
the expert would use whatever survey or other data there may be to help them
consider the probability of obtaining the evidence under this proposition, and given
the task-relevant information. Again, this will be the expert’s considered view on that
probability. Generally, the body of knowledge upon which the expert relies should be
available for auditing and disclosure.
36 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

How do we know if the expert’s probability assignments are reliable?


The answer to that lies, first, in the expert being able to explain in a transparent
way the basis of their assignment and, second, in the expert revealing the results
of any relevant calibration of their past opinions through, for example, competency
testing. The expert should be able to defend their opinion upon challenge. Through
transparency and effective explanation on the part of the expert, the fact-finder should
be able to understand the basis of the declared probabilities and take a view on how
to incorporate the expert’s opinion in the fact-finder’s own reasoning process.

How may experts use probability to assist fact-finders in their decision-making?


It is important for all participants in the justice process to understand and be clear
about several key issues and questions in the use of probability. These are:

• The essential distinction between probabilities for evidence (usually the expert’s
observations and analytical results) and probabilities for propositions (ie the facts
in issue).

• Whether probabilities for propositions are being assigned before or after expert
evidence is presented. If before the consideration of expert evidence, these
probabilities are called prior probabilities; if after the presentation of expert evidence,
they are called posterior probabilities (see Section 2.5 on the doping example).

• Who is best placed, in terms of their roles in the legal process, to provide these
different types of probability?

• What is the information that has conditioned (or influenced) the assignment of
probabilities to the propositions, and is it relevant to the task?

Generally, the probabilities for obtaining expert observations, conditioned on the truth
or otherwise of the proposition in question, are in the domain of the experts. The expert
should have sufficient data and the knowledge and understanding of the evidence to
assign defensible, informed probabilities. The expert should be able to convey and
explain these probabilities to the fact-finder to help them deliberate on the truth or
falsehood of the proposition in question. It is the fact-finder who has received other,
non-expert evidence in a case and who is therefore in the best position to take a view
on the truth of the proposition in question. However, this is not a hard-and-fast rule and
there will be some situations in which the expert can provide informed probabilities for
the truth of the proposition.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 37

What are the limitations of using probability?


Perhaps the main limitation is the lack of a common understanding among experts,
lawyers, judges and lay people of the notion of probability and the extent
to which it can be applied. Until there is a shared understanding and a common
language about probability, and an agreement on how best to express probability,
then misunderstandings and confusion will occur, resulting in expert evidence being
valued inappropriately or perhaps ruled as inadmissible when it might be helpful and
valid. Even among experts, there is misunderstanding about, and a variation in, the
adoption of probability. In the field of DNA profiling, practitioners use probabilistic
software for the logical interpretation of complex DNA mixtures. Without such software,
the interpretation of such mixtures would be very difficult. Discussion of the potential
and limitations of such software is provided in Forensic DNA analysis: a primer for
the courts17. In other fields, however, practitioners are only just beginning to explore
probabilistic thinking. The Royal Statistical Society and the Inns of Court College
of Advocates have together produced guidance on the use of probability18 that
provides a good starting point.

Another limitation is the lack of relevant data to inform probabilities in some areas of
expertise. In some areas, such as in textile fibres and ‛touch’ DNA, there is an extensive
body of research and survey data on which to draw. In other areas, such as toolmarks
or ballistics, there is only limited published research on important considerations
relevant for assessing probative value. In the absence of reliable, informative and
structured data, the expert must rely on their knowledge and understanding of the
evidence type, provided that the basis of such opinion is documented, can be audited
and is disclosed. It is in such areas particularly that evidence of the reliability of the
expert’s opinions would be highly desirable.
38 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Appendix 2: Evaluation of trace evidence


Trace evidence refers to any material that is transferred between persons or objects
during contact and is commonly recovered in connection with an alleged crime. The
term is often used to refer to the collection of materials frequently encountered by
forensic scientists such as glass, paint, fibres, firearm discharge residue and DNA.

In forensic casework, the objective is often to compare known material and questioned
material. Either can originate from the suspect or from the scene. Examples include
glass from a window compared with glass fragments recovered from a suspect or fibres
from a suspect’s jumper compared with recovered fibres from a victim. The material
might be compared at the source level (eg are the fragments from the same window or
not?) or at the activity level (eg did the suspect break the window or not?). Source level
is rarely sufficient to assist the questions relevant to the case. Glass recovered from
a suspect that is indistinguishable from a broken window is of little value without an
assessment of how probable such a finding is if the suspect broke the window versus
that the suspect had nothing to do with the breaking.

In order to tackle the question of source, forensic laboratory analysis is typically


undertaken to measure different features or characteristics of the known and
questioned material. To interpret the weight of the evidence (or likelihood ratio (LR))
when comparing material at the source-level usually requires evaluating the similarity
of the features of the two sets of material (how closely they match each other based on
the analysis undertaken) and the rarity of the observed features. The rarer the features
observed, the stronger the evidence may be. See Appendix 3 for more details on
source-level comparisons.

Analysing trace evidence at the source level alone, without reference to the activities
associated with the evidence, can be misleading. It is often the case that the most
relevant questions are related to the activities which may have led to the trace
materials being transferred. In order to obtain results that are helpful to address these
questions and that are not misleading, other factors need to be considered in addition
to the source-level questions. These other factors include the probabilities of transfer,
persistence and recovery of the material in the context of the alleged activities.
Statistical approaches which only assess the similarity and rarity of the materials can
miss factors which affect relevance within the context of the circumstances of a given
case – this can have a major impact on the evidential weight of the findings.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 39

Trace evidence must be viewed in the context of the case. As described in Section
2, at least two competing propositions should be addressed. In addressing these for
a specific case, the expert considers how probable the findings are in each of these
competing propositions. The result is presented in the form of an LR. This highlights
that the results do not have a stand-alone value; rather, their value is dependent on the
proposition being addressed or the questions to be answered.

The issues to be considered to address activity-level propositions are similar for all
trace materials but the factors affecting the issues vary from one material to another.
To assess how probable it is to find matching materials (glass, fibres, etc) if a particular
action took place, data on transfer, persistence and recovery are needed as well as data
on how common the materials under consideration are in the environment. Levels of
background abundance are also needed when considering the findings if the alternative
is true, ie that the activity did not take place or was not carried out by the suspect.
An example is knowing the background abundance of groups of glass fragments which
would be found on clothing in a given population. Sometimes, where insufficient data
are available, expert judgement or personal opinion are used to assign the required
probabilities. This should be made explicit in the report. Ideally ad hoc experiments
should be carried out in the absence of data.

Assumptions and data


When evaluating LRs for trace evidence, statistical assumptions are needed both at
the source level and at the activity level. For example, it may be assumed that the
distribution of measurements follows a particular statistical model or that two events
that are alleged to have occurred are statistically independent of one another. These
assumptions will depend on the type of evidence and the competing propositions being
considered. For some evidence types and propositions, the statistical assumptions are
well understood and validated. For others the methods are less well developed. When
analysing evidence at the activity level, propositions can be very case dependent. As
a result, the statistical approaches and datasets used to evaluate the evidence may be
based on the personal judgement of the forensic expert.
40 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Where any statistical assumptions or datasets have been used to evaluate evidence,
these should be clearly explained and justified in the case report. It is important that
checks have been carried out to test whether the statistical assumptions used are
appropriate for the evidence type and the propositions being assessed. One way of
doing this is to test the statistical approach on an existing dataset where the ground
truth is known and to assess the proportion of times that the LR gives a misleading
or incorrect result.

For example, for a source-level comparison this would mean evaluating the proportion
of times that the LR is greater than 1 when the two sets of material are from different
sources and the proportion of times that the LR is less than 1 when the two sets of
material are from the same source. Both proportions should be small in a model that
fits the evidence type for which it is being used.

Fibres
Fibres are shed from surfaces of various materials such as clothing, carpets
and car seats. They vary greatly in composition and colour. Studies have found that
fibres which are common, such as blue wool, have not been detected on surfaces
in high numbers except in areas where a known source has been in contact. Hence,
fibres can be very useful in reconstructing the activities that occurred during the contact
between textiles. The tendency to shed fibres is governed by many factors, including
the looseness of the weave, the size of the fibres and the age of the garment. This is
easy to visualise when we consider the difference between the shedding of a new
carpet and that of one that has been in place for some time.

Whether fibres transfer or not depends on the shedability and on the type of contact.
Information on both factors is needed to assess the range of fibres likely to be
transferred. A smooth shell suit will not be expected to yield transferred fibres even
if the contact is prolonged while a woollen jumper will give rise to transferred fibres
with limited contact. Little peer reviewed published literature exists in relation to the
shedability of fibres. These considerations highlight why case context is important
in assigning the probabilities of the findings in competing scenarios. Fibres are best
considered in the totality of the case and it is rarely useful to consider only their
sources or the presence of a single fibre.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 41

Transfers of fibres can be by direct contact (eg from the suspect’s clothes to the
victim’s clothes) or by indirect contact (eg fibres transferred from the victim’s clothes
to the suspect’s clothes via an intermediary object); the latter is called secondary
transfer. It is not possible for the expert to opine on whether the transfer of fibres was
primary or secondary transfer, or whether or not the transfer occurred during the alleged
activity. However, fibre experts are well placed to assess the probability of particular
findings in either scenario.

Example 1: competing sources of fibres


A sexual assault is reported as having taken place in the bedroom of a residence.
This is denied by the suspect, who alleges that consensual sex occurred in the living
room and that he never entered the bedroom. Fibres are an ideal evidence type to
help distinguish between these two scenarios. For example, a large number of fibres
indistinguishable from the bedcover would be expected to be recovered from the
suspect’s socks and fibres indistinguishable from the socks and the bedcover would be
expected to be recovered from the victim if the assault took place in the bedroom.

A large number of fibres indistinguishable from the bedcover would not be expected
to be recovered from the suspect’s socks given the alternative scenario. In this case, the
LR (ie the weight to be assigned to the findings) would involve the relative consideration
of these probabilities and the pair of competing propositions would be:

• Hp: The assault took place in the bedroom.

• Hd: Consensual sex took place in the living room.

Given the above assignments, if a large number of matching fibres were obtained,
we would expect a value of the LR above 1, supporting the proposition that the
assault occurred in the bedroom. The actual values for the probabilities will depend on
factors such as the tendency of the socks and the bedcover to shed fibres, the time
between the incident and the seizure and examination of the items, the frequency of
occurrence of fibre types in given situations and the statistical assumptions used to
link these factors together. The number of recovered indistinguishable fibres from the
bedcover are relevant because it is possible that a small number of bedcover fibres
may be present in the living room. Much of this becomes a matter of professional
judgement and experience.
42 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Example 2: missing fibre types


In an alleged assault, the victim (complainer in Scotland) is wearing a green T-shirt
and the suspect a red football supporter’s jersey. The jersey is found to consist of a
range of coloured polyester and cotton fibres. Shedding tests show that the fibres
shed in roughly equal proportions. The red polyester fibres recovered from the victim’s
T-shirt are found to be indistinguishable from the suspect’s jersey but no cotton fibres
are recovered. No green fibres matching the victim’s T-shirt are recovered from the
suspect’s jersey. Here we have an example of the findings not fitting the expectations
in the context of the case. The pair of competing propositions would be:

• Hp: The suspect grappled with the victim at a given time.

• Hd: The suspect never had any contact with the victim.

With the information above, the expert will inform that the findings are unlikely given Hp
but more expected given Hd. The LR will be less than 1, ie supporting Hd.

This example is designed to highlight the necessity of considering the findings in


competing scenarios and considering the context of the case. The matching red fibres
viewed in isolation are misleading no matter how robust the analytical tests applied or
how rare are the fibre types.

Glass
When a window is broken a large number of fragments fall back in the direction of the
blow. Thus, a person delivering the blow is expected to have small fragments in their
hair or on the surface of their clothing depending on how close they were to the window
as it was breaking, the height of the window, the type of glass and the activities that
followed19. One of the main tests used to examine glass fragments at the source level
consists of measuring the refractive index, which varies both within a pane of glass
and between sources of glass. The refractive indices of the glass fragments from the
window are measured and glass fragments from the suspect are also analysed and put
into matching and non-matching groups of glass if glass of more than one refractive
index is present20. However, assessing the closeness of the ‘match’ between glass
recovered from clothing and the window glass does not provide sufficient information
to evaluate propositions concerning whether or not the suspect broke the window.
Even when additional analytical tests are applied, this activity-related question is not
answered. Other information is required relating to, for example, how glass fragments
are transferred and retained following the breaking of glass objects, or how prevalent
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 43

glass fragments are on surfaces not directly connected to a recent break.


Glass fragments can be exchanged following a large range of daily activities.

To evaluate the glass fragment results, information is needed on how probable the
findings are if the suspect broke the window(s) against how probable the findings
are if the suspect had nothing to do with breaking the window(s). To address the first
probability, two possible ways that the glass fragments can arise must be considered
– either fragments were transferred when the window was broken and non-matching
glass, if present, was already on the clothing or no glass was transferred from the
window and all the glass on the clothing, both matching and non-matching, was
already there. To assess the probability of the findings (ie glass matching the window
found on the clothing), if the suspect had nothing to do with breaking the glass, we
need information on the probability of finding glass on innocent members of the
population. This information is critical, and it is useful to consider a population as close
to the suspect as possible. One well-known dataset considers glass on the clothing of
persons who come to the attention of the police rather than the general population.

Firearm discharge residue (FDR)


FDR is a combination of small particles produced when a gun is fired following the
explosion in the barrel. This evidence type is complicated, consisting of a non-uniform
population of particles which includes so-called ‘unique’ particles containing lead,
barium and antimony. However, to be considered FDR, other characteristic particles
also need to be present. Accepting this means that a single particle is not FDR.

Different ammunitions give rise to different residues, but a very high number have
similar compositions providing little discrimination. Even when the ammunition type is
known, it is considered good practice to compare residue from the discharged cartridge
case, barrel of the gun or the bullet hole – the known material(s) – and the residue
recovered from the hands or clothing of the suspect – the questioned material(s). This is
because variation in the proportions of particles can occur. It is common to see reports
from forensic scientists in which the number of particles recovered is factually reported,
or a statement that FDR was detected. Such statements can often be accompanied by
a disclaimer that the findings are ‘consistent with’ the suspect being close to a person
firing the gun or touching a surface with firearm residue on it. Similarly, a negative
finding is explained away by loss or time delay. The evaluation of the meaning of such
findings in the context of the case is generally left out of the report.
44 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

However, as with other trace evidence types, the more relevant question is whether
or not the suspect undertook an activity which could result in the scientific findings. In
the case of FDR, it is more meaningful to address whether the findings are more or less
likely if the person fired or did not fire the gun in the circumstances of the case. These
assessments will go beyond statements of consistency but should be qualified
in terms of probability.

Example
In a given set of circumstances, the expert may indicate that the probability of finding
FDR on a person who fired a gun is high if the person is sampled soon after the gun had
been fired. That expectation will be balanced against the probability of the finding if the
suspect did not fire the gun. The latter probability is low as surveys on members of the
general population show few instances of FDR. Depending on the circumstances, finding
FDR on a person’s hands is expected to provide support for firing of a gun rather than not
firing a gun, ie the LR will be greater than 1.

To inform these probabilities and assign a meaningful LR, information is needed on the
type of weapon and length of time between the alleged firing and sampling. Ideally, tests
should be carried out under the conditions of the known circumstances of the case.
Data regarding the presence of FDR as a background in a given population (of individuals
or objects) is also required to assess how prevalent the material may be considering
activities other than discharging a firearm. At the current time the understanding of the
transfer, persistence, recovery and background abundance of FDR is limited.

Drugs on banknotes
Banknotes seized from people who have been found guilty of drug crime on average
have higher levels of drug contamination than banknotes found in general circulation.
Different analytical techniques can be used to obtain measurements of drug traces
on banknotes and can result in different measurements. Hence it is important that
comparisons are made using datasets obtained using the same analytical technique. For
some drugs (eg cocaine, which is found on most banknotes) the measurements of drug
found on the set of seized banknotes are related to the quantities of drug on the notes.
For other drugs (such as heroin) the measurements might simply be the presence or
absence of the drug on each banknote.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 45

The strength of this evidence in relation to the following propositions can be evaluated
using an LR and the pair of competing propositions would be:

• Hp: The banknotes are associated with a person involved in drug crime.

• Hd: The banknotes are from general circulation.

Statistical models can be used to evaluate the LR; for some examples, see Wilson
et al.21. The assumptions behind the statistical models must be checked and the
models validated. Selecting suitable databases for these statistical models can be a
challenge22 as there may be both regional variations and variations over time associated
with particular drug use behaviours in different areas. Having relevant localised
ground truth data (analysed samples from known locations over different known time
periods) is essential to assess whether this is the case. It is important that the datasets
are consistent with the propositions. For example, if the propositions are specific
to a particular drug, then the dataset should also be specific to that drug over the
appropriate time period. It is also difficult to obtain a dataset of banknotes ‛associated
with a person involved in drug crime’, which can make it difficult to estimate the
probability of the findings under Hp. It is therefore key that the statistical assumptions
supporting data selection are described and justified.

Many of the general guidelines described in the four scenarios presented


above are applicable to other types of evidence. The same can be said of the
challenges posed by the limited availability of ground truth databases and lack of
experimental studies for estimating transfer, persistence and recovery probabilities
for most types of trace evidence. Going forward, another challenge will be to keep
up with emerging materials such as glass for smartphones and organic ammunition
for guns. Successful quantification of the evidential value of such trace evidence
and understanding of the limitations of the methods developed will depend on the
availability of reliable databases and experiments simulating case scenarios for the
different types of trace evidence. It will also rely on appropriate probabilistic and
statistical models, which will need to be tested and validated for each type of trace
evidence and for different sets of competing propositions.
46 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Appendix 3: Evaluation of
impression evidence
The purpose of this appendix is to explain the type and significance of the conclusions
reached by forensic experts dealing with impression evidence. It will also explain the
basis (statistical or otherwise) upon which these conclusions are formed.

What is ‛impression evidence’?


The term ‘impression evidence’ is used here to refer to the field of forensic examination
in which an expert is asked to compare, primarily using visual methods, items of
disputed source with items of known source. These items are called ‘impressions’
because they are marks or signs left by a person or an object following physical contact
with a surface. The field covers a large number of forensic specialisms, each having its
own terminology when referring to these impressions (Table 3). In this appendix,
the generic term of ‘impression’ will be used when the argument holds for all types
of impressions. When making examples in a given forensic specialism, its specific
terminology will be used.

Questioned impressions are of disputed sources (a person or an object). They generally


have been left unintentionally and recovered in association with the investigation of
some criminal activity (eg a fingermark in blood recovered from a crime scene). Known
impressions are of undisputed sources (person or object) and have been obtained
under known and controlled conditions (eg reference fingerprints from a known
individual taken in police custody).

What is the purpose of the forensic examination?


For all these specialisms, the purpose of the forensic examination is to help assess
whether or not questioned impression(s) originated from the source(s) that produced the
reference impressions. The issue can typically be phrased as follows:

• Were the (finger-/palm-) marks recovered at the scene left by this individual or by
some other unknown person?

• Were the (footwear-/tool-) marks recovered at the scene made by this object
(shoe or tool) or another unknown shoe or tool?

• Was the bullet recovered from a body fired by the seized firearm or by some
other unknown firearm?
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 47

TABLE 3

Some forensic specialisms and relevant terminology.

Forensic specialism Questioned Known


(as colloquially referred to) impression impressions

Fingerprint examination Fingermark Finger prints


Palmmark Palm prints

Barefoot examination Barefoot mark Barefoot prints


(with no visible, friction-
ridge, skin impressions)

Footwear mark examination Footwear mark Known shoes and their associated
reference prints (reference prints
taken in two dimensions or
impressions in three dimensions)

Toolmark examination Toolmark Known tools and their associated


reference impressions made on a
soft surface

Firearm examination Marks on a questioned Known bullets fired by a weapon


fired bullet
Marks on a questioned Known cartridges fired by a weapon
fired cartridge case
48 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

It is important to stress that the issue of origin (or source) in all of these specialisms
is generally associated with an implied activity made by a person or carried out using
an object. It is these implied activities that led to the production of the questioned
impressions (Table 4).

These activities are often not referred to specifically in the expert’s report but remain
implied. Indeed, experts will not systematically envisage all conceivable possibilities for
a questioned impression to be produced but will consider the most reasonable activity
arising from the case circumstances. For example, experts, unless instructed otherwise,
will not account for fanciful scenarios such as:

• a fingermark not being left on the surface by a living hand but using a forged
dummy finger obtained from an individual; and

• a footwear mark in the snow being the result of the landing of a shoe after being
discarded from a car.

TABLE 4

Implied activities resulting in the impression.

Forensic specialism Implied activity

Fingerprint examination The individual handled an object or touched a surface


with bare hands.

Barefoot examination The individual walked barefoot on a surface.

Footwear mark examination A person wearing these shoes walked on the floor.

Toolmark examination A person using this tool forced the safe, the door or
the window.

Firearm examination A person with that firearm fired a cartridge that led to a
bullet and a cartridge case.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 49

How is the comparison work carried out?


For all of the above forensic specialisms, the forensic examination of a given questioned
impression starts with its analysis, where the examiner assesses the relevant visible
features, gathers as much information as possible from the questioned impression
alone and assesses if it can be used for comparison purposes. Questioned impressions
deemed of no value will not be compared any further. The term ‘no value’ will
typically be assigned to impressions showing insufficient detail to allow a meaningful
comparison. Only impressions that are declared ‘of value’ will be compared (side by
side or by superimposition) with potential sources in the form of known impressions.
The potential sources have been generated either through police investigations (eg an
arrest or a seizure) or by searching a database holding known impressions, attributed
to individuals, that have been previously put on record. The comparison stage consists
of deciding if the features of the questioned impression observed in analysis are in
sufficiently close agreement or disagreement with the submitted known impressions.

Relevant features are specific to the forensic specialism and are detailed in
Table 5. They can be shared by many (such as the manufacturing size of a tool shared
by all tools produced of that size) or by a few (such as the acquired damage in the
form of cuts on the outsole of a shoe). In other words, the discriminative power
of relevant features varies depending on the specific type of feature considered.
Furthermore, depending on their size and on how the impression is produced (types
of surfaces, residue on the surface, movement, materials, etc), features may not be
reproduced in the impressions and even when features are made by the same source
(eg a shoe) impressions are never identical. Hence, features in disagreement may be
found between impressions, despite sharing the same source. This is because the
respective impressions have been subject to distortion, movement, superimposition or
background noise or have changed appearance over time. On the other hand, while
findings in agreement should be observed when the impressions were produced by
a common source, they may also be found when they were produced by different
sources. Matches between different sources are known as adventitious matches
because another source has produced, by chance, the same level of agreement.
50 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

TABLE 5

Relevant features of different evidence types.

Forensic specialism Relevant features (not exhaustive)

Fingerprint examination The general flow of the friction-ridge skin (papillary lines), often
classified in general patterns such as arches, loops and whorls.
The ridge endings and bifurcations (referred to as minutiae
or Galton’s details) made by the papillary lines and the
combination thereof.
The marks left by scars or other damage.
The specific shapes of edges of the papillary lines and the
pores present in them.

Barefoot examination The size (length and width) of the foot from heel to toes and the
relative size and position of the toe impressions.

Footwear mark The overall manufacturing design of the outsole (the geometric
examination elements of the design and how they are arranged relative to
each other).
The size of the outsole as specified by the manufacturer.
The general level of wear of the outsole at the time of the
impressions.
The acquired features shown in the impressions in the form
of cuts, removal of material or damage to the outsole.

Toolmark examination The width and size of the tool and its shape as given by the
manufacturer.
The acquired defects on the surface, removal of material and small
imperfections of the surface due to its usage.

Firearm examination The calibre, number, widths and twist of the lands and grooves of
the barrel through which a bullet was fired.
The relative positions of the firing pin, extractor and ejector coming
in contact with a cartridge case.
The striated marks, due to usage, left on the impressions of lands
and grooves on a fired bullet.
The breech face impression left on the back of the cartridge
when fired.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 51

The results of all these observations on both the known-source and the questioned
impression (in agreement or disagreement) are what will be called the comparison
findings. These findings will then be evaluated with regards to the proposition
of common source against the proposition of different sources. This evaluation
stage encapsulates the interpretation of the findings and generation of associated
conclusions. In the UK the evaluation is carried out holistically by the expert using their
knowledge and experience. It is more infrequent that such an evaluation is undertaken
using a likelihood ratio (LR) approach. However, such approaches are encouraged
by forensic practitioners and are used elsewhere, particularly in continental Europe.
Finally, it is customary for each examination followed by a conclusion to be reviewed
independently by a second examiner. This is called the verification stage. The four
above stages (analysis, comparison, evaluation and verification) are generally referred to
by the acronym ACE-V. In the UK, it is common to use this approach for the comparison
of fingerprint evidence but ACE-V is not necessarily used for other types of evidence
involving the comparison of visual patterns.

What conclusions do experts reach following a comparison?


In all specialisms, it will be customary for the expert, when the comparison findings
allow, to reach a conclusion of either an ‘identification’ or an ‘exclusion’. An identification
is declared when the examiner is subjectively convinced that the mark could not have
been made by any other source, even if all other possible sources have not been
examined. This means that the expert takes a decision on the issue of the source of
the impression.

The term ‘identification’ refers to the decision of the expert that the questioned and
known impressions originated from the same source. It is a categorical opinion, and
should not be misconstrued as being a factual certainty. No forensic examination
covered in this appendix can claim to factually demonstrate the source of a questioned
impression. A decision of ‘identification’ is not a fact; it is the opinion of an expert based
on their measurements, observations and experience and it is a statement of an expert’s
probability that the impression was made by different sources other than the questioned
source is so small that it is negligible.
52 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

The opinion of an ‘identification’ should not be taken as stating, for example:

• that the impression was associated with a specific individual or object to the
exclusion of all others in the world; or

• that it is absolutely certain (or with a 100% certainty) that a specific individual or an
object is the source of the questioned impressions; or

• that it is the result of the comparison of all impressions in the world’s population.

The term ‘exclusion’ refers to the decision of the expert that the questioned and known
impressions did not originate from the same source; in other words, they have been
produced by different sources. When a decision of ‘identification’ or ‘exclusion’ cannot
be reached, the expert may either, depending on the forensic specialism:

• indicate that the comparison is ‘inconclusive’; such a conclusion could be more


than the expression of neutral findings but does not provide ‘certainty’ in the eyes
of the examiner; or

• indicate the strength of support the findings will bring to the question of the source;
that strength will be qualified either verbally or numerically.

Table 6 shows these options as practised in a few of the forensic specialisms


considered. Resorting to ‘inconclusive’ as opposed to a more nuanced range
of possibilities expressing graded degrees of support depends on the habit of the
expert or on institutional/organisational policy. For example, while many fingerprint or
firearm experts will only ever report ‘inconclusive’ in instances where they are unable
to either ‘identify’ or ‘exclude’, other experts in those fields will qualify their
‘inconclusive’ conclusions.

How do experts decide on an identification or an exclusion?


A decision on identification is taken by the expert when, in their opinion, there are
insufficient features in disagreement to conclude that the impressions came from
different sources and that the observed features (between questioned and known
impressions) are in sufficient correspondence such that the expert would not expect
to see them repeated if that impression came from a different source. As indicated
before, these conclusions are not facts and the expert must concede that there is
some probability, however small, that their decision might be erroneous.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 53

A conclusion of ‘inconclusive’ means that no categorical decisions (‘identification’


or ‘exclusion’) have been made and that no meaningful assessment of its value is
given to the court. This may be due to various reasons, among them the low quality of
the impressions, the failure of the known impressions to fully represent the source, or
comparison findings that are not judged sufficiently discriminating to make a categorical
decision. However, the findings in such an ‘inconclusive’ case may still provide support
one way or the other towards the issue of source. In such cases, depending on the
forensic specialism, the expert may provide guidance as to the weight to be assigned
to these comparison findings in favour of the proposition of common source or of
different sources.

This can be done by expressing the degree of support that the findings provide in
favour or against these propositions using an LR based on a qualitative assessment for
impression evidence and expressed using a scale such as that provided in Table 1.

TABLE 6

Some of the range of conclusions reached.

Conclusions Meaning of conclusions

Exclusion Exclusion

Inconclusive The findings provide (qualified) support for the questioned


impression originating from another source rather than the
source under examination.
The findings do not provide support either for the source
under examination or for another source.
The findings provide (qualified) support for the questioned
impression originating from the source under examination
rather than another source.

Identification Identification
54 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

For example, in a case involving a footwear mark where the expert noted a
correspondence between a mark and a sole in terms of overall design, size, general
level of wear and the presence of three cuts located in the heel area, and there is no
significant discrepancy, they may state:

• the comparison findings, in my opinion, provide extremely strong support


for the view that the mark has been left by that sole as opposed to some other
sole; or

• the comparison findings are more than 1,000 times more likely to be observed if
the mark has been left by that sole rather than if the mark has been left by some
other sole.

If the findings had been different, for example if the general level of wear on both the
mark and the sole were different and not easily reconcilable (with potential additional
wear over the time period between the recovery of the mark and its seizure), the expert
may express (reversing the propositions):

• that the comparison findings, in my opinion, provide moderate support for the view
that the mark has been left by some other sole; or

• that the comparison findings are between 10 and 100 times more likely to be
observed if the mark has been left by some other sole.

The LR is always assigned numerically first and then, if so chosen, translated into a verbal
expression. In most cases involving impression evidence, the LR will be expressed
by an order of magnitude (10, 100, 1,000, 10,000, 1,000,000, etc). If this methodology is
used then an LR should be the prerequisite required to reach any conclusion, including
an identification or exclusion decision. An LR is obtained by dividing two probability
assignments. Both require judgement on the part of the expert based on a corpus of
knowledge that can be divided into two broad categories:

[S] Knowledge derived from robust systematic studies, ideally published, where the
relevant features have been measured and studied statistically.

[E] Knowledge derived from personal experience, ie the expert’s training and
professional experience in the forensic specialism.

Because LRs may be based on different experts’ knowledge, there may be a legitimate
and understandable difference in opinion between two experts. Table 7 gives, for a few
specialisms, examples of the type of knowledge used by experts.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 55

TABLE 7

Types of knowledge used in some forensic specialisms.

Forensic specialism Nature of the knowledge used by the examiner to assign a likelihood
ratio (LR)

Fingerprint [S] Relative frequencies of fingerprint general patterns or of types of


examination minutiae observed on fingerprints. LR computations obtained from
a statistical model.
[E] Knowledge on the distortion marks may show in comparison with
their corresponding prints owing, for example, to the elasticity of
the skin or the movements of the hand when grasping an object.
Personal experience derived from the systematic observations of
fingerprints from different individuals.

Footwear mark [S] Studies on the relative frequencies of the general designs of the
examination outsoles and their sizes in the selected population. Data showing
how wear develops on used outsoles.

[E] Knowledge of the variations in size between a mark and its


corresponding shoe owing to its deposition on the floor. Knowledge
on the occurrence and persistence of inclusions such as stones
caught between polyurethane structures of the outsole.

Firearm examination [S] Studies associated with the systematic search of matching features
occurring on bullets fired by different firearms. Data associated with
the evolution of striated features on bullets due to their successive
firings in a given barrel.
[E] Knowledge of how features produced by the manufacturing
process can be distinguished from features acquired through the
use of a firearm.

Barefoot [S] Studies on the variability observed between features measured on


examination barefoot impressions from different individuals.
[E] Knowledge of the variations between barefoot impressions from
the same donor. The variations can be due to the donor, the walked
surface or the residue left on the surface (eg blood).
56 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Before an expert decides on an identification or an exclusion, they assess the


comparison findings in the form of an LR. The expert decides an identification when
the LR is sufficiently high in their opinion to safely take that decision bearing in mind
all other relevant evidence. Conversely, the expert decides an exclusion when the
LR is sufficiently low for them to decide accordingly. What represents an LR as being
sufficiently high or sufficiently low cannot be precisely defined and has an element
of subjective judgement.

Use of a likelihood ratio approach in practice in the UK


The explicit use of the LR approach for the interpretation of impression evidence is
not yet the norm in the UK in all specialisms involving impression evidence but may
be used for some evidence types (such as footwear comparison). The LR should be
used as a measure of support for hypotheses in regards to identification or exclusion
of a potential source in the context of impression evidence. This re-emphasises that
the decision reached by the expert is an expression of personal belief and not a
statement of fact. For example, if an expert when concluding a fingerprint identification
ultimately decided for themselves that, given the LR, the identification can be made and
that the risk of an erroneous identification is, for them and in the case circumstances,
acceptable, then this may be incorrect. Information not known to the expert (eg that
the defendant was in prison at the time of the alleged burglary where marks have
been left) may provide a completely different perspective on the case, requiring the
expert to revise their opinion. An opinion of ‘identification’ in these circumstances goes
beyond the assessment of the comparison of findings and requires that the expert
takes into account additional elements not of a scientific nature (eg the broad number
of individuals or objects that could be the source of the impression). In these forensic
specialisms, where the court allows the expert to consider non-scientific evidence, the
court defers to the expert on the issue of the source of an impression. That delegation
of the source decision has been the general practice in the UK. However, if courts
decide not to allow experts to go that far, then the LR would be an appropriate way to
express the strength of the expert’s findings.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 57

Appendix 4: Statistical significance


Significance testing is a fundamental tool for scientific discovery that is widely used
in medical and other scientific literature as a basis for making claims. The logic of the
procedure is somewhat analogous to a criminal case. The steps for a significance test
are as follows:

• A null hypothesis is presented. This is generally the proposition that the discovery is
false, eg a pharmaceutical has no beneficial effect or the Higgs boson does not exist.
This null hypothesis is set up as a default assumption and is only rejected if there is
sufficiently convincing evidence.

• A test statistic is chosen, for which large values would tend to cast doubt on the null
hypothesis. For example, the average observed benefit in patients within a control
group compared with the average observed benefit in patients given a particular drug.

• Data are collected and the observed value for the test statistic is calculated.

• The probability of getting the observed value (or a more extreme value) of the test
statistic given that the null hypothesis is assumed to be true is calculated. This is
known as the P-value.

• If the P-value is very small, then the null hypothesis is rejected. The definition of
‘small’ depends on the stringency required: a standard threshold for declaring
statistical significance (that a difference between the tested data and the expected
value is a real difference and not just due to chance alone) is to find a P-value of
less than 0.05 (1 in 20).

For example, new pharmaceuticals generally require at least two independent


clinical trials with P-values less than 0.05 to be able to claim they are effective.

To claim the existence of the Higgs boson, physicists required a P-value of less
than 1 in 3.5 million.
58 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

The P-value is essentially a measure of the incompatibility between the observed data
and a pre-specified hypothesis: if the P-value is very small, either the null hypothesis
is true and a very surprising event has occurred or the null hypothesis is false. Many
problems can arise in the use and interpretation of statistical significance testing.

• The P-value is the probability of extreme evidence, given that the null hypothesis is
true, but it is often interpreted as the probability that the null hypothesis is true, given
the evidence. This is an example of the prosecutor’s fallacy.

• If the null hypothesis is not rejected, it does not mean it is true. This is similar to
someone who is not found guilty in an English court: they are found ‘not guilty’ rather
than ‘innocent’.

• With a large dataset, a statistically significant result may not necessarily be of any
practical significance, eg the difference in cure rates between a new and a standard
drug might be only 1%.

• If an exploratory analysis of data without a pre-existing hypothesis suggests a


hypothesis, an independent set of data should be used to test that hypothesis.
This is known as a confirmatory analysis.

• It is poor scientific practice to conduct multiple tests and only report the most
significant – this is very likely to be a false discovery and will give biased estimates.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 59

Appendix 5: Causation and relative risk


Statistical methods cannot establish proof of a causal relationship in an association.
The causal significance of an association is a matter of judgement that goes beyond
any statement of statistical probability. To judge or evaluate the causal significance of
the association between the attribute or agent and the disease, or effect upon health,
a number of criteria must be utilised, none of which is an all-sufficient basis for judgment.

Relative risk (RR) estimates are used with increasing frequency in toxic tort/delict
litigation as evidence for a causal link between the putative toxic exposure and the
personal injury sustained by the claimant. The simplistic phrase ‘doubling the risk’ is
unhelpful, because there is rarely a single risk with no variation by age and sex, and
rarely only one estimate, let alone a very precise estimate of a particular RR.

An RR, or risk ratio, measures the size of the effect of a given risk factor on disease
rates in specific populations. It describes the proportional increase in the probability of
an event occurring in a group exposed to some condition, as measured from a baseline
probability of an event occurring in a comparison group that has not been exposed
to the condition. For example, men who smoke 15 – 24 cigarettes per day have an RR
of lung cancer compared with never-smokers of about 26. The RR of lung cancer for
regular drinkers of more than four glasses of wine per day compared with those who
drink a glass of wine per week is about 3.2, averaged across smoking habits. After
adjusting for smoking habits and other factors, the RR is 1.423.

The RR of lung cancer with heavy drinking is more than halved by adjusting for smoking;
this illustrates the importance of having a careful definition of a causal hypothesis
and not drawing conclusions from a single estimate. Estimates from a range of
epidemiological studies are needed. A medical statistician or epidemiologist will usually
consider how study results relate to the viewpoints identified by Sir Austin Bradford Hill
for the assessment of causality24.
60 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

The Bradford Hill criteria apply to general scientific conclusions for populations. But we
may also be interested in individual cases, say in civil litigation where courts need to
decide whether a particular exposure (say the asbestos encountered in a job) caused a
negative outcome in a specific person (say John Smith’s lung cancer).  It can never be
established with absolute certainty that the asbestos was the cause of the cancer, since
it cannot be proved that the cancer would not have occurred without the exposure.
But some courts have accepted that, on the ‘balance of probabilities’, a direct causal
link has been established if the RR associated with the exposure is greater than two25.
But why two?
 
Presumably the reasoning behind this conclusion is as follows26:

• Suppose that, in the normal run of things, out of 1,000 men like John Smith, 10 would
get lung cancer. If asbestos more than doubles the risk, then if these 1,000 men had
been exposed to asbestos, perhaps 25 would develop lung cancer.

• Of those exposed to asbestos who go on to develop lung cancer, fewer than half
would have got lung cancer if they had not been exposed to the asbestos.

• So more than half of the lung cancers in this group will have been caused by
the asbestos. 

• Since John Smith is one of a group who was diagnosed with lung cancer, then on
the balance of probabilities his lung cancer was caused by the asbestos.

These viewpoints are a common-sense approach to systematically evaluating a range


of evidence. This section discusses the main concepts required to understand research
findings. Discussion of the link between legal and scientific questions can be found in
Dawid et al.27 and Hutton28.

Rates, ratios and risks


• Rate or risk rate is the number of new cases identified out of the population
under study. If 6 out of 100 babies are born with Down’s syndrome, the risk is 6%.
The time scale is not specified.

• An incidence rate is the number of new cases identified in a given time period
divided by the total life time lived in that period by the population under study. If 6
new cases of lung cancer are identified in a year, and 1,000 people were monitored
during that year, the incidence rate is 6 per 1,000 person-years.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 61

• RRs are used to compare incidence rates in two groups (hence rate ratio), usually an
exposed group compared with a non-exposed group. Given a lung cancer incidence
rate of 6 per 1,000 person-years in chimney sweeps, and 2 per 1,000 person-years in
the general population, the RR for chimney sweeps is 6/2 = 3.

• Excess risk is the difference between incidence rates in two groups. The excess risk
of lung cancer for chimney sweeps is 6 - 2 = 4 per 1,000 person-years.

Epidemiology, basic principles and study types


Epidemiological methods are used to investigate and estimate the frequency of medical
conditions, the risk factors for those conditions, the effects of interventions and causes
of ill-health and recovery.

Case reports or series


Medical symptoms and conditions which appear to be unusual or novel are typically
reported by describing a single case of a person with the conditions, or two or three
cases. The role of these reports is both to alert colleagues to look out for similar cases
and to encourage explanations and further investigation.

Retrospective case-control studies


Case-control studies are efficient for investigating rare or newly emerging conditions
or diseases, or those with a long gap between exposure and outcome, and allow
multiple risk factors to be considered29. A study begins with finding a list of cases,
such as people with throat cancer. Controls are selected to be people who are similar
to the cases, for example in age, sex and socio-economic status, but without the
disease. Exposure of both groups to factors such as radiation, as well as aspects of
health such as weight and smoking, is assessed using medical or occupational records.
Participants might be asked to recall exposure. Clear diagnostic criteria for the disease,
and an equally thorough search for possible exposures among cases and controls, are
important. By design, case-control studies investigate a single disease or condition. The
main disadvantage of case-control studies is that they are open to several biases30. Bias
can arise both through the quality of routine records and because the people with the
disease are likely to take more effort to remember risk factors.
62 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Retrospective and prospective cohort studies


Cohort studies are observational studies which follow a group, or cohort, of subjects
over time. The more explicit name ‘longitudinal cohort study’ stresses the time aspect.
Cohort studies in health research aim to assess the possible factors in the development
of disease or disability. The cohort must be explicitly defined, with inclusion criteria (eg
employment in the civil service in 1967) and exclusion criteria (eg pre-existing cardiac
disease). Basic characteristics of the cohort, such as age, sex, social status and current
exposure to a range of factors are recorded.

A prospective cohort study actively enrols the defined cohort and collects baseline
information. Subsequent health status is observed through follow-up. A retrospective
cohort study, or historical cohort study, uses data from past records. In studies of health
issues arising from occupational exposure, data from routine employment medical
examinations and health and safety records can be used. A retrospective cohort study
might provide information relatively quickly.

If the retrospective cohort continues to be followed after the first phase, the study
includes both retrospective and prospective data. Cohort studies can provide
information on a range of factors affecting a range of health states. It is easier to
ensure consistency of measurement or recording of exposure factors and diagnosis in
prospective cohorts than in retrospective cohorts or case-control studies. It is important
that possible exposure or risk factors are clearly defined and that consistent effort is
made to obtain data from the whole cohort. However, cohort studies are typically more
expensive because they are larger. The results of a prospective cohort study can only
be observed after some time, potentially a long time for slowly developing conditions.

Registry data and databases


A disease or case register is a database which is intended to include all cases of a
procedure, condition or disease in an identified population. Registers are used for
clinical and service purposes as well as for epidemiological research. Successful
registers need clear aims, appropriate data collection and validation systems, as well as
regular analysis and feedback to interested parties. These requirements indicate that a
multidisciplinary team, stable funding and relevant leadership are important. As disease
registers contain medical information, both social attitudes to confidentiality and the
jurisdiction will influence data collection and completeness31; Nordic registers have high
coverage rates as consent is not required32.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 63

Clinical databases, such as a list of children referred to a specialist hospital, are not
regarded as a disease register, because a relevant population cannot be identified.
Parents might travel across regions or countries to seek help33. If cases are collected
from a service which focuses on provision for those with cognitive deficits, accurate
clinical diagnoses and reliable information on people with normal or good cognitive
ability will not be available34, 35.

Randomised controlled trials


Patients who have agreed to enter the trial are allocated at random to receive
one of the trial treatments. The role of randomisation is to minimise bias arising from
both patients or doctors who have strong beliefs about what the response to treatment
ought to be. Randomisation also balances factors such as age, underlying health and
compliance with treatment across the groups in combination with allocation concealment
(blinding). With effective randomisation, the probability of differences between the
trial groups in responses and adverse effects arising by chance can be calculated.
A potential disadvantage of randomised controlled trials is that only a selected
population is included.

Systematic review
The Cochrane Collaboration36 defines a systematic review as a review of a clearly
formulated question that uses systematic and explicit methods to identify, select
and critically appraise all relevant research, and to collect and analyse data from the
studies that are included in the review. Statistical methods (meta-analysis) may or may
not be used to analyse and summarise the results of the included studies, depending
on the quality and quantity of information. Some systematic reviews can only provide
descriptions of the main features of the included studies. A summary of the evidence
based on the data for each patient in each study is generally regarded as the optimal
approach, but this is acknowledged often to be impractical owing to constraints of
confidentially and time. Systematic reviews are contrasted with narrative or expert
reviews, which are based only on research that is known by, easily available to or
acceptable to the reviewers.
64 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Bias
In epidemiology and statistics, bias typically refers to estimates which systematically
misrepresent the quantity of interest. As a simple example, if a lecturer asks the 10
students, out of a class of 100 students, who have come to all his lectures whether his
lectures are worth attending, the answers given cannot be assumed to represent the
view of the whole class.

Reporting standard
In order to assess the quality of the research described in a published article, sufficient
information is required. When medical statisticians began to assess the quality of
medical research publications, one difficulty was the lack of information provided
regarding the design and analysis of studies37, 38, 39. Since 2010, a series of statements
and guidelines, with accompanying checklists, have been published to facilitate
understanding of the study and assessment of the validity of results and conclusions.
These can be found on the website of the Equator (Enhancing the Quality and
Transparency of Health Research) Network40.

Causality and the Bradford Hill criteria


Suppose there is a qualifying association: a large and statistically significant association
between a possible causal agent and a particular pathology. If this observed association
cannot be easily explained by confounding and bias, further consideration of cause and
effect is worthwhile. Confounding describes the situation when a risk factor other than
the exposure in question is associated with both the exposure and the outcome.
If the risk factor can be precisely measured, the effect of confounding can be estimated
and the association between the exposure and outcome adjusted for the confounder.
Higher alcohol consumption is associated with higher rates of lung cancer, but also with
higher rates of smoking. In order to separate the effects of alcohol consumption and
tobacco smoking on lung cancer, accurate records of alcohol and tobacco consumption
are required.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 65

Often a confounder cannot be precisely estimated and there may be various factors to
consider as outlined in the Bradford Hill criteria illustrated in Regina v Abadom, 198341.

Strength
‘The strength of the association is expressed as a comparison between a standard or
unexposed population and a population exposed to the putative causal agent. The RR
of lung cancer for smokers is higher compared to never-smokers’42. ‘Strength’ is not well
defined, although an RR of 5 would generally lead to further investigation. However,
the baseline rate of the condition should also be taken into account. Headlines in
2000 which reported a doubling of risk of deep vein thrombosis associated with oral
contraceptives, without reporting the absolute risk of 2 per 10,000 users per year,
resulted in many women abruptly stopping taking their pills. Doubling the risk increases
the absolute rate to 4 per 10,000 users.

Consistency
‘If an association is repeatedly observed by different people, in different places,
circumstances and times, it is more reasonable to conclude that the association is not
due to error, or imprecise definition, or a false positive statistical result’43. Further, the
association should be observed in studies with a high methodological standard.

Specificity
‘Consideration should also be given to whether particular diseases only occur among
workers within particular occupations. This is a supporting feature in some cases, but
in other cases one agent might give rise to a range of reasons for death’44. The best
example of a simple specific causal agent is thalidomide: the congenital deformity
known as phocomelia is seen almost exclusively in the population of individuals
exposed to thalidomide during gestation.

Temporality
‘This requires causal factors to be present before the disease’45.

Dose-response curve, or biological gradient


‘If the frequency of a disease increases as consumption or exposure to a factor
increases, this supports a causal association, for example, increasing levels of smoking
associated with increased frequency of lung disease supports the hypothesis that
smoking causes lung disease’46.
66 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

Plausibility
Biological plausibility that an effect is causal for an outcome to occur must be based
on scientific reasoning or data, not just prior beliefs. Laboratory experiments might
be possible, especially if the outcome effect can be modified by an appropriate
experimental regime. However, extrapolation from animal experiments to humans is
not straightforward. In the development of drugs, randomised experiments are required.
For side effects with long-term treatments, or industrial exposures, other study designs
are used.

Coherence
‘A cause and effect interpretation should not seriously conflict with generally known
facts of the development and biology of the disease’47.

Experiment
‘Sometimes evidence from laboratory or field experiments might be available’48.

Analogy
Bradford Hill commented: “In some circumstances it would be fair to judge by analogy.
With the effects of thalidomide and rubella before us we would surely be ready to
accept slighter but similar evidence with another drug or another viral disease in
pregnancy.” This criterion has a limited role. If the criteria are not met, one cannot
conclude that there is not a causal association. The conclusion is that there might be
direct causal explanation, or an indirect explanation, or even that the association arose
from some aspects of data collection or analysis. Competing explanations should be
considered that might include unmeasured confounding factors or alternative factors
which have an association of similar strength to the putative causal factor49.
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 67

References
1. Straight Statistics. 2009 Question marks over Corby judgement. See https://straightstatistics.fullfact.org/
article/question-marks-over-corby-judgement (accessed 26 May 2020).

2. The Law Society Gazette. 2018 Police chief explains ‘justice by algorithm’ tool.
See https://www.lawgazette.co.uk/news/police-chief-explains-justice-by-algorithm-tool-/5067033.article
(accessed 26 May 2020).

3. Willis W. 2015 ENFSI guideline for evaluative reporting in forensic science. European Network
of Forensic Science Institutes. See http://enfsi.eu/wp-content/uploads/2016/09/m1_guideline.pdf
(accessed 26 May 2020).

4. Royal Society and Royal Society of Edinburgh. 2017 Forensic DNA analysis: a primer for the courts.
See https://royalsociety.org/-/media/about-us/programmes/science-and-law/royal-society-forensic-dna-
analysis-primer-for-courts.pdf (accessed 24 August 2020).

5. BBC Reith Lectures. 2002 A question of trust. See http://www.bbc.co.uk/radio4/reith2002/lectures.shtml


(accessed 26 May 2020).

6. Anderson T, Schum D, Twining W. 2005 Analysis of evidence (2nd edn, Law in Context). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511610585.

7. Regina v Abadom. 1983 1 Weekly Law Reports 126.

8. The Council of the Inns of Court and the Royal Statistical Society. 2017 Statistics and probability
for advocates: understanding the use of statistical evidence in courts and tribunals.
See https://www.statsref.com/ICCA-RSS-guide.pdf (accessed 26 May 2020).

9. Op. cit. note 4.

10. Op. cit. note 3.

11. Regina v Adams. 1996 England and Wales Court of Appeal (Criminal Division) 222.

12. Lord Hoffmann. 2009 Re B (Children) 1 AC 11.

13. Part 19, the Criminal Procedure Rules and Criminal Practice Direction 2015 (as amended 2018, 2019).
See https://www.justice.gov.uk/courts/procedure-rules/criminal/docs/2015/crim-proc-rules-2015-part-19.pdf
(accessed 26 May 2020).

14. Part 35, Civil Procedure Rules 1998 (as amended 2019). See https://www.justice.gov.uk/courts/procedure-
rules/civil/rules/part35 (accessed 26 May 2020).

15. Op. cit. note 3.

16. Jackson G, Aitken C, Roberts P. 2015 Case assessment and interpretation of expert evidence – guidance
for judges, lawyers, forensic scientists and expert witnesses. London: Royal Statistical Society.
68 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

17. Op. cit. note 4.

18. Op. cit. note 8.

19. Curran J, Hicks T, Buckleton J. 2000 Forensic interpretation of glass evidence. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press LLC.

20. Ibid.

21. Wilson A, Aitken C, Sleeman R, et al. 2014 The evaluation of evidence relating to traces of cocaine
on banknotes. Forensic Science International, 236, 67 – 76.

22. Aitken C, Wilson A, Sleeman E, et al. 2017 Distribution of cocaine on banknotes in general circulation
in England and Wales. Forensic Science International, 270, 261 – 266.

23. Bagnardi V, Randi G, Lubin J, et al. 2009 Alcohol consumption and lung cancer risk in the Environment
and Genetics in Lung Cancer Etiology (EAGLE) study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 171, 36 – 44.

24. Hill A. 1965 The environment and disease: association or causation? Proceedings of the Royal Society
of Medicine, 58(5), 295 – 300.

25. Spiegelhalter D. 2019 The art of statistics: learning from data. London: Pelican Books.

26. Ibid.

27. Dawid A, Faigman D, Fienberg S. 2014 Fitting science into legal contexts: assessing effects of causes
or causes of effects? Sociological Methods and Research, 43, 359 – 421.

28. Hutton J. 2018 Expert evidence: civil law, epidemiology and data quality. Law, Probability and Risk.
17(2), 101 – 110.

29. Altman D. 1991 Practical statistics for medical research. London, Chapman and Hall, 93 – 96.

30. Sackett D. 1979 Bias in analytical research. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 32, 51 – 63.

31. Strauss D, Shavelle R. 1998 Life expectancy of adults with cerebral palsy. Developmental Medicine
& Child Neurology, 40, 369 – 375.

32. Ludvigsson J, Häberg S, Knudsen G, et al. 2015 Ethical aspects of registry-based research in the
Nordic countries. Clinical Epidemiology, 7, 491 – 508.

33. Hutton J. 2015 Weighing privacy against effective epidemiology. Developmental Medicine & Child
Neurology, 57, 595 – 596.

34. Hutton J. 2006 Cerebral palsy life expectancy. Clinics in Perinatology, 33, 545 – 555.

35. Hutton J, Eccles M, Grimshaw J. 2008 Ethical issues in implementation research: a discussion
of the problems in achieving informed consent. Implementation Science, 3, 52.

36. Cochrane UK. See http://uk.cochrane.org/ (accessed 26 May 2020).


THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 69

37. Op. cit. note 35.

38. Op. cit. note 36.

39. Gore S, Altman D. 1982 Statistics in practice. London: British Medical Association.

40. Equator Network. See https://www.equator-network.org (accessed 26 May 2020).

41. Op. cit. note 7.

42. Op. cit. note 8.

43. Op. cit. note 8.

44. Op. cit. note 8.

45. Op. cit. note 8.

46. Op. cit. note 8.

47. Op. cit. note 8.

48. Op. cit. note 8.

49. Op. cit. note 8.


70 THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS

The members of the groups involved in producing this primer are listed below.
The members acted in an individual and not organisational capacity and declared
any conflicts of interest. They contributed on the basis of their own expertise and
good judgement. The Royal Society and the Royal Society of Edinburgh gratefully
acknowledge their contribution.

Primer lead Writing group


Professor Niamh Nic Daéid FRSE Associate Professor Alex Biedermann
Professor Christophe Champod
Editorial board
Professor Jane Hutton
Professor Colin Aitken
Professor Graham Jackson
Professor Sheila Bird OBE FRSE
Lord Kitchin
Sheriff Lorna Drummond QC
Dr Tereza Neocleous
Lord Kitchin
Professor Sir David Spiegelhalter FRS
Professor Niamh Nic Daéid FRSE
Dr Sheila Willis
Professor Sir Bernard Silverman FRS
Dr Amy Wilson
Professor Sir David Spiegelhalter FRS
THE USE OF STATISTICS IN LEGAL PROCEEDINGS: A PRIMER FOR COURTS 71

Primer steering group Acknowledgements


Dame Anne Rafferty DBE This project would also not have been
possible without contributions and support
Lord Hughes of Ombersley
from a range of individuals. In particular
Professor Dame Sue Black DBE FRSE we wish to thank:
Sir Charles Godfray CBE FRS The Rt Hon the Lord Burnett of
Lord Justice Peter Jackson Maldon, Lord Chief Justice of England
and Wales
Dame Ottoline Leyser DBE FRS
The Rt Hon Lord Carloway, Lord President
Dr Julie Maxton CBE
of the Court of Session and Lord Justice
Dame Angela McLean DBE FRS General

Professor Niamh Nic Daéid FRSE Sir Venkatraman Ramakrishnan, President


of the Royal Society
Sir Muir Russell KCB FRSE
Dame Ann Glover, President of the Royal
Professor Sarah Skerratt
Society of Edinburgh
Lord Turnbull
Mr Justice Wall We also wish to thank the Royal Statistical
Society for contributing their time and
expertise to this project.
The Royal Society is a self-governing Fellowship The Royal Society of Edinburgh (RSE), Scotland’s
of many of the world’s most distinguished scientists National Academy, is a leading educational charity
drawn from all areas of science, technology, which operates on an independent and non-party-
engineering, mathematics and medicine. The political basis to provide public benefit throughout
Society’s fundamental purpose, as it has been Scotland. Established by Royal Charter in 1783 by
since its foundation in 1660, is to recognise, key proponents of the Scottish Enlightenment, the
promote and support excellence in science and RSE now has around 1600 Fellows from a wide
to encourage the development and use of science range of disciplines. The work of the RSE includes
for the benefit of humanity. awarding research funding, leading on major
inquiries, informing public policy and delivering
The Society’s strategic priorities are: events across Scotland to inspire knowledge
• Promoting excellence in science and learning.

• Supporting international collaboration


• Demonstrating the importance of science
to everyone
For further information
For further information The Royal Society of Edinburgh
The Royal Society 22 – 26 George Street
6 – 9 Carlton House Terrace Edinburgh EH2 2PQ
London SW1Y 5AG
T +44 131 240 5000
T +44 20 7451 2571 E [email protected]
E [email protected] W rse.org.uk
W royalsociety.org/science-and-law
Scottish Charity No SC000470
Registered Charity No 207043

9 781782 524861

ISBN: 978-1-78252-486-1
Issued: November 2020 DES6439

You might also like