Conceptualization and Measurement of Interpersonal Communication Motives
Conceptualization and Measurement of Interpersonal Communication Motives
RUBIN zyxwv
Conceptualization and Measurement of
zyxwv
Interpersonal Communication Motives
zyxwv
CAROLE A. BARBATO
Kent State University
The goal of this study was to develop and validate an instrument that could be
used to ascertain motives individuals have for interpersonal communication. A
zyxwvu
28-item scale is presented along with initial information about the scale’s
construct validity. Results indicated there were six prominent motives: pleasure,
affection, inclusion, escape, relaxation, and control. Low communicative appre-
hensives used interpersonal communicationfor pleasure, affection, and control
while high apprehensiues were more aligned with the inclusion motive. The
motives of pleasure, affection; and relaxation were more closely related to
communication satisfaction. In fact, communication satisfaction was best
predicted by low communication apprehension, gender, and the pleasure,
affection, relaxation, and escape communication motives.
I
NTERPERSONAL communication serves a variety of func-
tions in everyday life. It is through communication that people
shape and nurture their self-concepts, make decisions about
zyxwvu
zyxwvuts
The authors wish to thank Tracy Wellmon,Lori Baird, and Cameron Armstrong for their
assistance in conducting the study and Alan Rubin for his thoughtful comments on the
manuscript.
602
zyxwzy
zy
Rubin, P e w , Barbato / COMMUNICATION MOTIVES 603
zyxwv
their lives, share information with others, and express ideas and
innermost feelings. People communicate with others for even more
zyxwvu
specific reasons: to make decisions about where to eat or what
proposal to support, to show others love or to express a protest, to gain
information or advice about an important decision, or just to pass the
time away. As obvious as it is that communication functions as a
method of need-fulfillment, little research has been conducted to
determine why people initiate communication with other people. This
article reports the development of an instrument designed to measure
interpersonal communication motives and provides preliminary evi-
dence of the instrument’sconstruct validity by examining relationships
between these motives and interpersonalpredispositionsand outcomes.
Interpersonal communication research has, for the most part,
focused on how interpersonal interaction takes place-descriptions of
communication behaviors in interpersonal relationships. Studies of
communication relational levels, communication rules, situations,
verbal and nonverbal language, social influence, and relationship
development typify the scope of how interpersonal communication
occurs in everyday life (see, e.g., Knapp & Miller, 1985).Descriptions of
these communicationbehaviors are important first steps in understand-
ing interpersonal communication motives.
Three major lines of research have been developed to describe
interpersonal communication behaviors. The first line focused on
identifying categories of interpersonal behaviors. For example, Boch-
ner, Kaminski, and Fitzpatrick (1977) used Lorr and McNair’s (1965)
Interpersonal Behavior Inventory to determine “self” and “best-liked
other” factors of manifest, observable interpersonal behavior. Ten
behavioral categories were found: control, nurturance, dependency,
detachment-affiliation, deference, mistrust, submissiveness, recogni-
tion, abasement, and sociability. Although respondents reported their
actions, rather than motives for actions, the categories that emerged
zyx
support interpersonal communication motives discussed here.
A second line of research focused on the structure of conversation.
Wish, DAndrade, and Goodnow (1980), for example, formulated a
structural framework for speech acts. They found force to be an
important dimension in speech. Similarly, Triandis (1977) found four
basic dimensions of social behavior: association/dissociation,super-
ordination/subordination,intimacy/formality,and overt/covert. These
dimensions follow from an earlier notion (Triandis, Vassiliou, &
Nassiakou, 1968) that behaviors are manifestations of behavioral
intentions, the “instructions people give to themselves to behave in
604 zyxw
zyxwvuts
zyx
HUMAN COMMUNICATIONRESFARCH / Summer 1988
Functional Approaches to
Interpersonal Communication
An important second step of inquiry into why people communicate
as they do involves functional analysis. Interpersonal communication
literature taking a functional approach reveals that communication
serves a variety of functions. According to Dance and Larson (1976),
communication bonds individuals with the environment by helping
them develop and maintain self-concepts(linking),it enablesindividuals
to move from self-centered to other-centered activities (mentation),
and it allows individuals to regulate their own or others’ behaviors
(regulation). Bochner (1984) viewed persuasion and expression of
affect as two general motives for interpersonal communication that
serve five interpersonal bonding functions: fostering favorable im-
pressions, organizing relationships, constructing/validating conjoint
worlds, expressingfeelingsand thoughts, and protecting vulnerabilities.
Clark and Delia (1979),in describing communication functions as first
steps in forming communication strategies, identified instrumental
zyxw
zyx
zy
Rubin, Perse, Barbato / COMMUNICATIONM O W
zyxwv
identificationof the general functions of media use such as surveillance,
correlation, entertainment, and socialization (Lasswell,1948).Research
then moved to investigating motiues for television viewing (e.g.,
Greenberg, 1974;McQuail, Blumler, & Brown, 1972;Rubin, 1979,1981)
and to developing typologies of motives for viewing specific program
genres (e.g., Rubin, Perse, & Powell, 1985;A. Rubin & R. Rubin, 1982).
Rubin’s (1981)research found that people view television to pass time
or out of habit, for companionship, for arousal or excitement, to view
specific programs, for information or to learn, to relax, to escape or
forget, for entertainment and enjoyment, and for social interaction.
Central to the uses and gratifications perspective is the assumption
that people’s communication choices are purposive, goal-directed
activities (Katz et al., 1974). Basic human needs produce motives that
then lead to behaviors to gratify those needs (Rosengren, 1974). Rubin
and Windahl(l986, p. 191)emphasized that “communication motiues
zy
zyxwv
zy
Rubin, Perse, Barbato / COMMUNICATION MOTIVES
from 18to 60years) were asked to sort 40 statements of why they might
talk with others. Three major interpersonal communication user types
emerged. Other-oriented users talked to others to show concern, to
share and give something to others, but not out of obligation.
Instrumental/self-help users communicated with others either to
confirm self, get advice from others, and to feel less lonely or because
they sought excitement. Expressive-emotionalusers used communica-
tion either to express and share or for entertainment.
Barbato (1986)argued that she found interpersonal motives reflecting
the self-confirmatory, inclusion, and emotion-expressive functions
identified in interpersonal communication research (Benniset al., 1968;
Schutz, 1966), and social utility, personal identity, and entertainment
functions identified in mass communication research (Katz et al., 1973;
McQuail et al., 1972;Rubin, 1981;A. Rubin & R. Rubin, 1982).Thus her
study demonstrated the importance of interfacing the personal and
mediated communicationperspectives and the need to examine further
motives for interpersonal communication.
Convergent Validity
One goal of this research was to create and evaluate an Interpersonal
Communication Motives (ICM) scale. A second goal was to provide
initial evidence for the construct validity of the ICM scale. In particular,
we chose to focus on the convergent validity by examining a set of
demographic variables and two constructs that should be related to
interpersonalcommunicationmotives: the predisposition of communica-
tion apprehension,and the communicationoutcome of communication
satisfaction.
Demographics.Research on the origins of television-viewingpatterns
suggests that demographic variables influence motives and behaviors.
Gender, for example, has been linked to media use patterns: females
were found to be more likely to turn to television for companionship
reasons (Rubin, 1979,1981; A. Rubin & R. Rubin, 1982). Educational
level has been linked negatively to watching television for habit,
entertainment, pass time, and companionship. When older people are
part of larger households, they are less likely to watch television for
companionshipmotives, yet elders with high interpersonal interaction
report using television for conversational topics, entertainment, and
relaxation (A. Rubin & R. Rubin, 1982). Younger people watch
television to pass time, the middle-aged watch to pass time and seek
information, and elders watch to seek information (Jeffres, 1975).
Research has consistentlyconfirmed relationshipsamong age, watching
610 zyxw
zyxwvuts
zyxwvut
HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / Summer 1988
zyx
relation to interpersonal communication use:
(Rl)Will the demographic variables of age, gender, education level, and
household size be related to interpersonal communication motives?
Communication apprehension. Research has demonstrated that
communicationapprehension (CA)affects the amount of interpersonal
interaction and interaction outcomes. High CAs generally avoid
communication situations and find talking to be not at all pleasurable
(Lederman, 1983).When they do communicate, they are perceived as
less competent, appropriate, and attractive (for an overview of the
research findings, see McCroskey, 1977).The strength of the findings
about CA suggested that it would be closely aligned to motives for
interpersonal communication. For example, a related concept, re-
ticence, has been found to affect the interpersonal needs of expressed
affection and expressed inclusion (Rosenfeld& Frandsen 1972;Schutz
1966). Persons who avoid communication have less of a need to
express affection and offer inclusion to others. Therefore, we asked a
second research question:
(R2)Will communication apprehensionbe related to interpersonalcommuni-
cation motives?
Communication satisfaction. Hecht (1978a, 1978c)conceptualized
communicationsatisfaction as the positive reinforcement provided by a
communisation event that fulfills positive expectations. Satisfaction,
Hecht argued, is important because it can validate the outcomes of
various communication studies; positive outcomes of conversations
should be satisfying to the interactants. Varying levels of satisfaction
should be related to need-fulfillment derived from communication.
Because individuals’ communication motives are based on their
expectations about the utility of those motives in fulfilling their needs,
we expected that communication motives would be associated with
global communication satisfaction.
zy
zyxwv
zyx
Rubin, Perse, Barbato / COMMUNICATION MOTIVES
partially related relationships, use too few items, and omit a time frame.
However, Wheeless, Wheeless, and Baus (1984)found that a general
communicationsatisfaction measure was possible in studyingphases of
intimate relationship development. We found it necessary to view
communication satisfaction as a global measure in this study. Re-
spondents were asked to report on multiple motives for interaction
rather than motives for one conversation; cuing respondents to one
particular conversation would not have allowed for equal comparison of
zyx
motives and satisfaction on a more global level. Thus in testing the
convergent validity of the interpersonal communication motive scale,
we asked:
(R3) What is the relationship between interpersonal communicationmotives
and global communicationsatisfaction?
Finally, a multivariate relationship was expected among socio-
demographics, communication apprehension, and communication
satisfaction.According to the uses and gratificationsperspective (Katz
et al., 1974), needs shaped by (a) sociological contexts and (b)
psychological predispositions lead to (c) motives to communicate.
Satisfaction, then, should result from the motives and previous
experiencesin communication.A test of this model involvesestimating
the ability to predict communication satisfaction from motives, and
psychological and sociological factors.
Because this research is based in the psychological perspective
(Fisher, 1978) and draws from uses and gratifications tenets, we
adopted the methodological assumptionsthat people are aware of their
needs when they form motives to communicate (Katzet al., 1974).Thus
self-report methodology was the most appropriate way to investigate
the conceptual filters of interest. Writers have shown that when people
are motivated,they are aware oftheir thoughts and feelings (Greenwald,
1982; Kellogg, 1982).
STUDY 1
Procedure
In Study 1,we solicited statements reflecting a comprehensive range
of interpersonal communication motives. Undergraduate students
enrolled in interpersonal communicationclasses at a large Midwestern
612 zyxw
zyx
zyx
HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / Summer 1988
zy
university were offeredextra credit for completinga 1-daydiary of their
communicationbehaviors,listing all communication events they initiated
(interpersonal and mediated), indicating where and when the event
occurred, to whom they spoke, what was discussed, which medium
was used, and explainingtheir reasons for entering into the communica-
tion. Of the 127 students enrolled, 82 (36 males and 46 females)
completed diaries during the first week of November 1984.
Five graduate students enrolled in a communication research class
in fall 1985 analyzed these diaries, transcribing each interpersonal
communication event and the reason for its initiation. A total of 840
events were identified. These graduate students were then trained to
classify each of the communication events into one of the 18categories
identified earlier. Each transcription was coded by two independent
coders; intercoder reliability, assessed by Scott’s (1955)pi, ranged from
.45 to .67. No diary item failed to fit into one of the 18 categories.
On the basis of the above data, 90 statements (5 statements to
represent each of the 18categories)were drawn either from the diaries
(onlythose receiving agreed-uponclassificationby two coders) or from
the previous research that identified television-viewingmotives (Rubin,
1985).The 90 statements were phrased as 5-point Likert-type items.’
To check the clarity and salience of the 90 items, the Interpersonal
Communication Motives (ICM) scale was administered to 32 under-
graduate students enrolled in two interpersonalcommunicationclasses
in October 1985. Respondents were asked to indicate the extent to
which the 90 items represented reasons why they talk to other people.
After completing the scale, students participated in focus group
discussions (either all male, all female, or mixed gender) to determine
difficultieswith the scale or its items, and to identifyadditional motives.
The students expressed no difficultywith the clarity of the items, but
item analysis and opinions expressed in the focus groups led to slight
alterationsof the category structure. Safety and self-esteemcategories
were eliminated because they were not salient to the respondents and
because there was no variance in their responses. Affection and
altruism categorieswere seen as redundant so they were combined to
represent a more generalized “caring” communication motive that
included both altruism and affection items. In addition, for each
category, the least discriminating scale item was eliminated.
The initial ICM scale, then, consisted of 60 scale items, 4 statements
representing 15 functions of interpersonal communication: caring,
zyxw
zyxw
zy
zyxw
Rubin, Perse, Barbato / COMMUNICATION MOTlvEs 613
STUDY 2
Procedure
Sample
The goal of the next stage of the research was to identifythose items
that would make up the final Interpersonal Communication Motives
scale. Because we hoped to generalize beyond the traditional college
student, a sample representing the general population was sought. To
acquire this varied sample, undergraduate and graduate students
enrolled in research methods courses at the same university during fall
1985 were trained in questionnaire administration and ethics. Each
student was given a packet of questionnairesand instructed to choose a
wide variety of people as respondents. Students were instructed to
attempt an equal split of males and females and to select respondents
with varied educational backgrounds in fivespecific age categories: 13
to 24,25 to 34,35 to 49,50 to 65, and over 65 years. The questionnaires
included the 60-itemICM scale, a request for demographic information
zyxwv
(age, gender, highest level of education completed, and number of
people in their household) and two additional instruments to test the
convergent validity of the scale.
The sample consisted of 504 respondents with ages ranging from 12
to 91 years (M = 40.26, SD = 16.98); 25.2% were between 12 and 24,
17.4% were 25 to 34 years, 24% were 35 to 49,26.2% were 50 to 65, and
7.2% were over 65. Males accounted for 44.8% of the respondents. On
the average, respondents had completed some college (M=3.80 where
3 represented high school graduation and 4 represented some college),
and shared their living quarters with an averageof 3.29 (SD= 2.49) other
people. The sample consisted of residents of Ohio, Pennsylvania, and
West Virginia.
Instrumentation
In an attempt to establish the construct validity of the ICM scale and
to test our model relating motives to communication predispositions
and outcomes, the respondents completed measures of communication
614 zyxw
zyx
zyxwvutsrq
HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / Summer 1988
zyxwv
communication to express caring and appreciation for others.
Factor 3, inclusion (eigenvalue 2.34), accounted for 10.3%of the
total variance. It included three companionship items and one expres-
sive item (“need to talk about my problems sometimes”). This factor
reflected a use of interpersonal communication to be with and share
feelings with others and to overcome loneliness.
Factor 4, escape (eigenvalue 1.89),accounted for 8.8%of the total
variance. This factor was defined by three escape items and one pass
time item (“have nothing better to do”). It reflected an avoidance of
other activities and the use of communication to fill time.
Factor 5, relaxation (eigenvalue = Lll), accounted for 8.5% of the
616 zyxw
zyxwv
zyxwvuts
HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / Summer 1988
TABLE 1
Interpersonal Communication Motives: Primary Factor Loadings
Factor 1 : Pleasure
zyxwvutsr
Because it%fun (3.41,1.08) .76 .14 .16 .07 .22 .01
Because it’s exciting (2.96,1.08) .75 .08 .21 .14 .05 -.OO
To have a good time (3.25,1.06) .73 .12 .11 .16 .07 .05
Because it’s thrilling (2.60,1.13) .72 -.01 .13 .17 .05 .05
Because it’s stimulating (3.35,1.06) .69 .19 .10 .03 .10 .08
Because it’s entertaining (3.55,1 .OO) .67 .04 -.01 .19 .23 .06
Because I enjoy it (3.94,0.97) .62 .25 .12 -.07 .26 -.09
Because it peps me up (3.20,1.07) .62 .13 .30 .18 .29 -.04
Factor 2: Affection
T o help others (3.94,0.93) .ll .83 .06 .04 .ll .08
To let others know I care about
their feelings (3.88,0.92) .16 .74 .18 -.02 .09 -.14
To thank them (3.82,0.97) .01 .73 .03 .12 .08 .19
To show others encouragement
(3.71,0.90) .18 .72 .14 -.01 .ll .06
Because I’m concerned about
them (3.88,0.85) .14 .71 .18 -.05 -.OO -.03
Factor 3: Inclusion
Because I need someone t o talk t o
or be with (3.32,1.20) .24 .ll .76 .13 .09 .03
Because I just need t o talk about
my problemssometimes (3.30,1.18) .ll .18 .73 .18 .16 .07
Because it makes me feel less lonely
(3.03,1.21) .22 .13 .70 .27 .19 .01
Because it’s reassuring t o know someone
is there (3.36,1.08) .23 .34 .68 .07 .22 .04
Factor 4: Escape
To p u t off doing something I should
be doing (2.26,1.16) .23 .02 .lo .81 -.01 .ll
To get away from what I’m doing
(2.61,1.16) .19 .ll .22 .73 .23 .OO
Because I have nothing better t o do
(1.93,1 .OO) .10 -.08 .07 .64 .03 .26
To get away from pressures and
responsibilities (2.56,1.1 3) .12 .04 .34 .61 .28 .04
zyx
zyxw
zyxwvu
zyxw
Rubin, Perse, Barbato / COMMUNICATIONMOTIVES
TABLE 1 Continued
617
zyxwvuts
Interpersonal Motive Items
“I talk t o people
Factor 5: Relaxation
.. . ”
.24
.23
Interpersonal Motive Factors
2
.12
.09
3
.OS
.27
4
.06
.1S
5
.79
.74 -.02
6
.02
Because it’s a pleasant rest (3.04, 1.06) .34 .1S .26 .10 .60 -.11
Because it makes me feel less tense
(2.91, 1.11) .22 .12 .39 .3s 5s -.01
Factor 6: Control
Because I want someone t o do
something for me (2.47,1.07)
T o tell others what t o do (2.76, 1.1 2)
To get something I don’t have
(2.22, 1.08)
.04
-.02 zyxw
.08 -.02
.08 .06
.09 -.12
.16
.08 -.OS
.08
.15
.OO
-.01
NOTE 1: The parenthetical numbers are each item’s mean and standard deviation.
.84
.78
.76
Motives
Pleasure
Affection
Inclusion
Escape
Relaxation
Control
Pleasure
1.oo
.35***
.51***
.41***
.59***
.08*
zyxwvutsr
Affection
zyxwvutsrq
1 .oo
.42***
.15***
.34***
.11**
Inclusion
1.oo
.49***
.59***
.1 o*
Escape
1 .oo
.49***
.25***
Relaxation
1.oo
.02
Control
1.oo
M
3.28
3.85
3.25
2.34
3.09
2.48
SD
0.80
0.71
0.95
0.86
0.85
0.89
alpha
.89
.85
.84
.77
.81
.75
Demographics
Age -.26** * .14** -.14** -.26*** -.05 -.09* 40.26 16.98
Gender .17*** .30*** .29*** .04 .19*** -.21*** 1.55 0.50
Education .01 -.06 -.19*** -.05 -.lo* .la*** 3.80 1.18
House Size -.02 -.04 -.09* -.05 -.05 -.01 3.29 2.49
Communication
zyxwvutsrqpon
Apprehension -.16*** -.12** .11** .04 -.06 -.08* 61.20 13.68 .90
Communication
Satisfaction .36*** .31*** .11** .oo .25*** .01 59.58 7.68 .84
~~.~ ~
NOTE: Because o f missing data, N = 474 for correlations involving interpersonal motive factors; for means and correlations not involving inter-
personal motive factors, N ranges from 468 to 504. For Gender, Male = 1, Female = 2.
* p < .05; **p < .01; * * * p < .001 (two-tailed).
zyxw
zyxwvuzy
zy
zyxwv
Rubin, Pew, Barbato / COMMUNICATION MOTIVES
Convergent Validify
To investigate the demographic correlates of the interpersonal
619
zy
the equation first to see if CA and interpersonal motives would add
substantially to an explanation of satisfactionbeyond the demographics.
The demographic variables entered on the first step explained 3.2%
of the variance (F [4,447] = 3.68, p < .01).Gender was a significant
predictor at this stage (beta = .14, F 8.59, p < .01). Communication
apprehension, entered next, added 24.9% to the total variance (F [5,
4461 34.80,~< .001, beta = -.51). The six motives, entered last, added
significantlyto the equation by increasingthe total variance by 10.6%(F
620 zyxw
zyxwvut
zyxwvutsrq
zyxwv
HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / Summer 1988
zyxw
[ll,4401 = 25.20, p < .001). The final equation accounted for 38.65%of
the variance. The most substantial contributors were pleasure (beta =
.29, F = 18.06, p < .001), affection (beta = .19, F = 16.32, p < .001),
relaxation (beta = .13, F 5.39, p < .001), and escape (beta = -.16, F =
11.09, p < .001).
DISCUSSION
The goal of this investigation was to develop and test an instrument
to measure interpersonal communication motives. The present study
not only yielded reliable 28-and 18-itemscales, but provided information
about how those uses of communication are related to socio
demographic characteristics,predispositions, and communication out-
comes. It also provided confirmatory support for past research
analyzing interpersonal behavior.
Despite the criticisms of FIRO theory (Armstrong& Roback, 1977;
Frandsen & Rosenfeld, 1973; Hinrichsen, Gryll, Bradley, & Katahn,
1975), this study confirms Schutz’s (1966) belief that people use
interpersonal communication to express affection, to seek inclusion,
and to express control. These motives reflect an orientation toward
others and a desire to form and maintain social bonds. In addition,
people use communication for pleasure (entertainment and arousal),
for relaxation, and to escape other activities. Similar typologies have
been developed in uses and gratifications research (e.g., Rubin, 1981,
1983) and have been consistently employed in studies of various
television genres (e.g., Palmgreen, Wenner, & Rayburn, 1980; Perse,
1986; Rubin, 1985; Rubin et al., 1985).
Results indicated that scores on the ICM Control factor were
relatively low in comparison with those of the other motives. Previous
research suggested that the FIRO scale was biased against the control
dimension (Frandsen & Rosenfeld, 1973) and that social desirability
might influence respondents’ control scores in that the wording of some
of the FIRO scale items reveals the scale’s expectations (Hinrichsen et
al., 1975). Also,Wiedemann, Waxenberg, and Mone (1979)found the
control dimension to be unrelated to the areas of inclusion and
affection, results somewhat supported in this investigation.
But, it may be that communicating with others to control is not as
salient as motives to show affection, feel part of a social circle, or to use
communication for pleasure, relaxation, or escape. This suggests that
perspectives that emphasize the functional nature of communication
(e.g., Bochner, 1984)are ignoring more salient motives of communica-
zy
zyxw
zyxwvu
Rubin, Perse, Barbato / COMMUNICATION M O M
zyxw
cation satisfaction. Talking to others for pleasure, relaxation, to
express affection, and to be included were related to higher levels of
communication satisfaction. These findings are similar to media uses
and gratificationsresearch that found that using television for entertain-
ment (pleasure) and relaxation reasons was associated with higher
levels of life satisfaction (A. Rubin & R. Rubin, 1982; Rubin, 1985; R.
Rubin & A. Rubin, 1982). Although Miller and Sunnafrank (1982)
suggest that successful control is a satisfying interpersonal outcome,
results presented here indicate that seeking personal rewards through
interpersonal complianceis not particularly satisfying. Communicating
for control was not related to communication satisfaction.This may be
because of relationship costs associated with gaining compliance
(Sillars, 1980).
Although the relationship between communication apprehension
and satisfactionwas not a focus of our investigation,the findings of the
regression analyses merit comment. Global communicationsatisfaction
was dependent, to some extent, on low communicationapprehension.
Similar results have linked CA to lower levels of job satisfaction
(Falcione, McCroskey, & Daly, 1977). McCroskey (1984) suggested
that communication apprehension is formed and maintained through
cognitive appraisalsof the outcomes (goalachievement)of communica-
tion episodes. When goals are not achieved, negative expectations for
future communication are formed, leading to higher levels of apprehen-
Rubin, Perse, Barbato / zyx
zyxw
COMMUNICATION MOTIVES
zy
(1984)dominance and intimacy categories, Triandis’s (1977)dimension
of association, superordination, and intimacy, Millar and Rogers’s
(1976) control and intimacy dimensions, the Bochner et al. (1977)
dimensions of control, nurturance, and sociability, and Mehrabian and
Ksionzky’s (1972) affiliation dimension are much like the motive
categories of control, inclusion, and affection identified in this study. As
was evident in past research, communication motives are linked to
communicate behaviors.
NOTES
1. All 90 items were phrased in terms to express positive quantities of the motive
being measured. There were two reasons for this. First, this is the common methodology
of uses and gratifications research that has been shown to be reliable and valid. Second,
although attitude scale construction texts recommend that a portion of scale items be
zyxwvu
negatively worded to guard against acquiescence, negatively worded motiveitems usually
are more closely related to each other in factor analyses and reflect avoidances, rather
than motivations (see, e.g., McLeod & Becker, 1981).Because we were interested in the
interrelationships of motive statements, we kept wording consistent so statistical tests
would reflect only differences in content, rather than wording.
2. One of the 60 items on the final form of the questionnaire was inadvertently
repeated (the pass time item of “have nothing better to do”). The second mention of this
item was eliminated from further data analysis to avoid jeopardizing statistical test
assumptions. All results, then, are based on the 59 items that remained.
3. The full correlation matrix is available from the first author.
4. An “other people” factor contained 4 items (“Other people let me know that I
communicate effectively,” “Other people express a lot of interest in what I have to say,”
“Other people genuinely want to get to know me,” “My conversations flow smoothly”),a
“conversation” factor contained 3 items (“In conversations, we each get to say what we
want to,” “In conversations, I feel that we can laugh easily together,” “During
conversations with others, I am able to present myself as I want others to view me”),and a
“negative” factor contained 3 items (“I have better things to do than converse with
others,” “1 do NOT enjoy conversations,” “Nothing is ever accomplished in conversa-
tions”). The remaining items were: “I am very dissatisfied with my conversations,” “We
usually talk about something I am NOT interested in,” “I would like to continue having
zyxw
zyxwvuts
zyxwv
624 HUMAN COMMUMCATION RESEARCH / Summer 1988
conversations like the ones I have now,” “I feel like I can talk about anything with other
people,” “Other people show me they understand what I say,” and “I am very satisfied
with my conversations.” The three factors, the laitem scale, and the 16-item scale
zyxw
essentially produced the same results, so the 16-item scale was used in all data analyses.
REFERENCES
zy
Argyle, M. (1972).Non-verbalcommunicationin human social interaction. In R. A. Hinde
(Ed.), Non-verbal communication (pp. 243-269).Cambridge, MA: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press.
Armstrong, S., & Roback, H. (1977).An empirical test of Schutz’ three dimensional
theory of group process in adolescent dyads. Small Group Behauior, 8,443-456.
Barbato, C. A. (1985,November). Elder’s uses of interpersonal communication. Paper
presented at the meeting of the Speech Communication Association, Denver.
Barbato, C. A. (1986,November). Uses of interpersonal communication. Paper
presented at the meeting of the Speech Communication Association, Chicago.
Bennis, W. G., Schein, E. R., Steele, F. A., & Berlew, D. E. (1968).Towards better
interpersonal relationships.In W. G. Bennis,E. R. Schein, F. A. Steele, & D. E. Berlew
(Eds.), Interpersonal dynamics: Essays and readings on human interaction (pp.
647-674).Homewood, IL Dorsey.
Blumler,J. G. (1985).The social character of media gratifications.In K. E. Rosengren, L.
A. Wenner, & P. Palmgreen (Eds.), Media gratifications research: Current perspec-
tiues (pp. 41-59).Beverly Hills, C A Sage.
Bochner, A. P. (1984).The functionsof human communicationin interpersonal bonding.
In C. C. Arnold & J. W. Bowers (Eds.), Handbook of rhetoricaland communication
theoy (pp. 544-621).Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Bochner, A. P., Kaminski,E. P., & Fitzpatrick, M. A. (1977).The conceptual domain of
interpersonal communicationbehavior: A factor-analyticstudy. Human Communica-
tion Research, 3,291-302.
Burgoon, J. K. (1985).The relationshipof verbal and nonverbalcodes. In B. Dervin& M.
J. Voigt (Eds.),Progress in communication sciences (Vol. 6,pp. 263-298).Norwood,
NJ: Ablex.
Burgoon,J. K., &Hale, J. L. (1984).The fundamentaltopoiof relational communication.
Communication Monographs, 51,193-214.
Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1987).Validation and measurement of the fundamental
zyxwvut
themes of relational communication. Communication Monographs, 54,19-41.
Burke, R. J., & Weir, T. (1976).Some personality differences between members of
one-career and two-career families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38,453-459.
zyxw
Chaffee, S. H. (1986).Mass media and interpersonal channels: Competitive,convergent,
or complementary?In G. Gumpert & R. Cathcart (Eds.),Inter/media: Interpersonal
communication in a media world (3rd ed., pp. 62-80).New York: Oxford University
Press.
Clark, R. A., & Delia, J. G. (1979).Topoiand rhetorical competence. Quarterly Journal
ofspeech, 65,187-206.
Dance, F.E.X., & Larson, C. E. (1976).The function of human communication: A
theoretical approach. New York Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Davis, R. H., Edwards, A. E., Bartel, D. J., & Martin, D. (1976).Assessing television
viewing behavior of older adults. Journal of Broadcasting, 20,69-75.
zyxw
zy
Rubin, Perse, Barbato / COMMUNICATION MOTIVES 625
zyxw
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1%9). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories,
origins, usage and codings. Semiotica, 1,49-98.
Elliott, W. R., & Quattlebaum, C. P. (1979).Similaritiesin patterns of media use: A cluster
analysis of media gratifications. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 43,
61-72.
Falcione, R. L., McCroskey, J. C., & Daly, J. A. (1977).Job satisfaction as a function of
employees’ communication apprehension, self-esteem, and perceptions of their
immediate supervisors. In B. D. Ruben (Ed.), Communication yearbook 1 (pp.
363-375).New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
Fisher, B. A. (1978).Perspectives on human communication. New York: Macmillan.
Frandsen, K. D., & Rosenfeld, L. B. (1973).Fundamental interpersonal relations
orientations in dyads: Empirical analysis of Schutz’s FIRO-B as an index of
compatibility. Speech Monographs, 40,113-122.
Greenberg, B. S. (1974).Gratifications of television viewing and their correlates for
British children. In J. G. Blumler & E. Katz (Eds.), The uses ofmass communications:
Current perspectiues on gratifications research (pp. 71-92).Beverly Hills, C A Sage.
Greenwald, A. G. (1982). Ego task analysis: An integration of research on ego-
involvementand self-awareness.In A. H. Hastorf& A. M. Isen (Eds.), Cognitiuesocial
psychology (pp. 109-147).New York: ElsevierlNorthHolland.
Harrison, R. P. (1973).Nonverbal communication.In I. de S. Pool& W. Schramm (Eds.),
Handbook of communication (pp. 93-115).Chicago: Rand McNally.
Hecht, M. L. (1978a).The conceptualization and measurement of interpersonal
communicationsatisfaction. Human Communication Research, 4,253-264.
zy
Hecht, M. L.(1978b).Measures of communicationsatisfaction.Human Communication
Research, 4,350-368.
Hecht, M. L. (19784.Toward a conceptualization of communication satisfaction.
Quarterly Journal of Speech, 64.47-62.
Hinrichsen, J. J., Gryll, S. L., Bradley, L. A., & Katahn, M. (1975).Effects of impression
management efforts on FIRO-B profiles. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 43,269.
Jeffres, L. W. (1975).Functions of media behaviors. Communication Research, 2,
137-160.
Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M.(1974).Utilization of mass communicationby
the individual. In J. G. Blumler & E. Katz (Eds.), The uses of mass communications:
Current perspectives on gratifications research (pp. 19-32).Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Katz, E., Gurevitch, M., & Haas, H. (1973).On the use of the mass media for important
things. American SociologicalReview, 38,164-181.
Kellogg, R. T. (1982).When can we introspect accurately about mental processes?
Memory and Cognition, 10,141-144.
Knapp, M. L., & Miller, G. R. (1985).Handbookof interpersonalcommunication.Beverly
Hills,C A Sage.
Lasswell, H. D. (1948).The structure and function of communicationsin society. In L.
Bryson (Ed.), The communication of ideas (pp. 37-51).New York: Harper.
Leary, T. (1957).lnterpersonal diagnosis ofpersonolity. New York Ronald.
Lederman, L. C. (1983).High communicationapprehensives talk about communication
apprehension and its effects on their behavior. Communication Quarterly, 31,
233-237.
626 zyxw
zyxwvut
zyxwvut
HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / Summer 1988
zyxwvu
197-209.
Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation andpersonality. New York Harper.
McCroskey, J. C. (1977). Oral communication apprehension: A summary of recent
theory and research. Human CommunicationResearch, 4,78-96.
McCroskey,J. C. (1984).The communicationapprehension perspective. In J. A. Daly &
J. C. McCroskey (Eds.),Auoidingcommunication:Shyness, reticence, and communi-
cation apprehension (pp. 13-38).Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
McCroskey, J. C. (1986). An introduction to rhetorical communication (5th ed.).
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
McLeod, J. M., & Becker, L. B. (1981).The uses and gratifications approach. In D. D.
Nimmo & K. R. Sanders (Eds.), Handbook of political communication (pp. 67-99).
zyxwvu
Beverly Hills, C A Sage.
McQuail, D., Blumler, J. G., & Brown, J. R. (1972). The television audience: A revised
perspective. In D. McQuail (Ed.), Sociologyofmass communications (pp. 135-165).
Middlesex, England: Penguin.
Mehrabian, A., & Ksionzky, S. (1972). Some determiners of social interaction.
Sociometry, 35,588-609.
Melnik, S. R. (1979).The “uses and gratifications”approach in the study of “entertain-
ment” and leisure use. In H. Fischer & s.R. Melnik (Eds.), Entertainment: A cross
cultural examination (pp. 144-152).New York Hastings House.
Mendelsohn, H. (19%). Mass entertainment. New Haven, CT:College and University
Press.
Millar, F. E., & Rogers,E. (1976). A relational approach to interpersonal communication.
In G. R. Miller (Ed.), Explorations in interpersonal communication (pp. 87-104).
Beverly Hills, C A Sage.
Miller, G. R., & Sunnafrank, M. J. (1982). All is for one but one is not for all: A con-
ceptual perspective of interpersonal communication.In F.E.X. Dance (Ed.),Human
communication theory: Comparative essays (pp. 220-242). New York: Harper &
Row.
Palmgreen, P., Wenner, L. A., & Rayburn, J. D. (1980). Relations between gratifications
sought and obtained: A study of television news. Communication Research, 7,
161-192.
zyxwv
Palmgreen, P., Wenner, L. A., & Rayburn, J. D. (1981). Gratifications discrepancies and
news program choice. Communication Research, 8,451-478.
Patterson, M. L. (1983). Nonverbal behavior: A functional perspective. New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Perse, E. M. (1986). Soap opera viewing patterns of college students and cultivation.
Journal ofBroadcasting & Electronic Media, 30, 175-193.
Poole, M. S., & McPhee, R. D. (1985). Methodology in interpersonal communication
research. In M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal
communication (pp. 100-170).Beverly Hills, C A Sage.
Rosenfeld, L. B., & Frandsen, K. D. (1972). The “other” speech student: An empirical
analysis of some interpersonal relations orientations of the reticent student. Speech
Teacher, 21,296-302.
zyxw
zy Rubin, Perse, f3arbato / COMMUNICATION MOTIVES 627
zyxwv
Rosengren, K. E. (1974).Uses and gratifications:A paradigm outlined. In J. G. Blumler &
E. Katz (Eds.),Uses of mass communications: Current perspectives on gratifications
zyxwvut
research (pp. 269-286).Beverly Hills, C A Sage.
Rosengren, K. E., & Windahl, S. (1972). Mass media consumption as a functional
alternative. In D. McQuail (Ed.), Sociology of mass communications (pp. 166-194).
Middlesex, England Penguin.
Rubin,A. M. (1979).Television use by children and adolescents. Human Communication
Research, 5, 109-120.
Rubin, A. M. (1981).An examination of television viewing motivations. Communication
Research, 8,141-165.
Rubin, A. M. (1983).Television uses and gratifications: The interaction of viewing
patterns and motivations.Journal of Broadcasting, 27,3741.
Rubin, A. M. (1984).Ritualized and instrumentaltelevision viewing. Journalof Communica-
tion, 34(3),67-77.
Rubin, A. M. (1985).Uses of daytime television soap operas by college students. Journal
of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 29,241-258.
Rubin, A. M., & Perse, E. M. (1987).Audience activity and soap opera involvement: A
uses and effects investigation.Human Communication Research, 14,246-268.
Rubin, A. M., Perse, E. M., & Powell, R. A. (1985).Loneliness,parasocial interaction, and
local television news viewing. Human Communication Research, 12,155-180.
Rubin, A. M., & Rubin, R. B. (1982).Contextual age and television use. Human
Communication Research, 8,228-244.
Rubin, A. M., & Rubin, R. B. (1985).Interface of personal and mediated communication:
A research agenda. Critical Studies in Mass Communication, 2,3653.
Rubin, A. M., & Windahl, S. (1986). The uses and dependency model of mass
communication. Critical Studies in Mass Communication,3,184-199.
Rubin, R. B., & Rubin, A. M. (1982).Contextual age and television use: Reexamining a
life-positionindicator.In M. Burgoon (Ed.), Communicationyearbook 6 (pp. 583-604).
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Schutz, W. C. (1966).73e interpersonal underworld. Palo Alto, C A Science and
Behavior Books.
zyxwvuts
Scott, W. A. (1955).Reliability of content analysis: The case of nominal scale coding.
Public Opinion Quarterly, 19,321-325.
Sillars, A. L. (1980).The stranger and spouse as target persons for compliance-gaining
strategies: A subjective expected utility model. Human CommunicationResearch, 6,
265-279.
SPSS. (1986).SPSSX user's guide (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Tannenbaum, P. H. (1980).The entertainment functions of television. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Triandis, H. C. (1977).Interpersonal behavior. Monterey, C A Brooks/Cole.
Triandis, H. C. (1980).Values, attitudes, and interpersonal behavior. In M. M. Page
(Ed.), Nebroska symposium on motivation 1979 (pp. 195-259).Lincoln, NE Univer-
sity of Nebraska Press.
Triandis, H. C., Vassiliou, V., & Nassiakou, M. (1968).Three studies of subjective
culture. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph Supplement, 8,
1-42.
Wenner, L. A. (1976).Functional analysis of TV viewing for older adults. Journal of
Broadcasting, 20,7748.
628 zyxwv
zyxw
zyxwvut
zyxwvu
HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / Summer 1988