Sde 201
Sde 201
STUDY MATERIAL
VI SEMESTER (CUCBCSS)
CORE COURSE
For
BSC
COUNSELLING PSYCHOLOGY
UNIVERSITY OF CALICUT
SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION
Calicut University P.O. Malappuram, Kerala, India 673 635
School of Distance Education
UNIVERSITY OF CALICUT
SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION
STUDY MATERIAL
VI Semester (CUCBCSS)
CORE COURSE:
COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY
Prepared by :
Sri. ELDHOSE N J
Research Scholar,
Department of Psychology,
University of Calicut.
COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY
MODULE 1
Cognition refers to the processes through which information coming from the
senses is transformed, reduced, elaborated, recovered and used. The term information
refers to sensory input from the environment that informs us about what is happening
there. Cognitive processes, thus are the processes involved in knowing about the world.
They are importantly, attention, perception, learning, memory, and higher order processes
like thinking, language, decision making and problem solving. The branch of memory
that deals with cognitive process is called cognitive psychology.
PERSPECTIVES OF COGNITION
Information Processing Theory
The information processing theory is a cognitive approach to understanding how
the human mind transforms sensory information. The model assumes that information
that comes from the environment is subject to mental processes beyond a simple
stimulus-response pattern. "Input" from the environment goes through the cognitive
systems which is then measured by the "output". Information that is received can take
several paths depending on attention, encoding, recognition, and storage. The central
executive feature controls how much information is being processed, though more
primitive sensory areas of the brain first accept environmental input. The theory looks at
real time responses to presented stimuli and how the mind transforms that information.
The model is used in several areas of research such as; cognitive development,
neuroscience, social learning, and artificial intelligence.
Over long spans of time individuals process information with greater efficiency.
The model assumes that through the process of maturation one develops greater abilities
to attend to stimulus, recognize patterns, encode, and retrieve information. Over the life
span individuals experience more information, associations, and ways to categorize the
input. The process may seem passive, but the model assumes that input from the
environment is actively being transformed and rehearsed thus becoming a part of long-
term memory. For environmental information to become a part of long-term memory one
must attend to, rehearse, and make sense of the stimuli. The interaction between nature
and nurture coincide for changes in development. The model does not attempt nor can it
distinguish between the two.
Information processing theory combines elements of both quantitative and
qualitative development. Qualitative development occurs through the emergence of new
strategies for information storage and retrieval, developing representational abilities
HISTORY
Philosophical Antecedents of Psychology
Historians of psychology usually trace the earliest roots of psychology to two
approaches to understanding the human mind:
• Philosophy seeks to understand the general nature of many aspects of the world, in part
through introspection, the examination of inner ideas and experiences
• Physiology seeks a scientific study of life-sustaining functions in living matter,
primarily through empirical (observation-based) methods.
Two Greek philosophers, Plato (ca. 428–348 B.C.) and his student Aristotle
(384–322 B.C.), have profoundly affected modern thinking in psychology and many
other fields. Plato and Aristotle disagreed regarding how to investigate ideas. Plato was a
rationalist. A rationalist believes that the route to knowledge is through thinking and
logical analysis. That is, a rationalist does not need any experiments to develop new
knowledge. A rationalist who is interested in cognitive processes would appeal to reason
as a source of knowledge or justification.
In contrast, Aristotle (a naturalist and biologist as well as a philosopher) was an
empiricist. An empiricist believes that we acquire knowledge via empirical evidence—
that is, we obtain evidence through experience and observation. In order to explore how
the human mind works, empiricists would design experiments and conduct studies in
which they could observe the behavior and processes of interest to them. Empiricism
therefore leads directly to empirical investigations of psychology.
Most psychologists today seek a synthesis of the two approaches. They base
empirical observations on theory in order to explain what they have observed in their
experiments. In turn, they use these observations to revise their theories when they find
that the theories cannot account for their real-world observations.
The contrasting ideas of rationalism and empiricism became prominent with the
French rationalist René Descartes (1596–1650) and the British empiricist John Locke
(1632–1704). Descartes viewed the introspective, reflective method as being superior to
empirical methods for finding truth. He maintained that the only proof of his existence is
that he was thinking and doubting. Descartes felt that one could not rely on one’s senses
because those very senses have often proven to be deceptive (think of optical illusions,
for example). Locke, in contrast, had more enthusiasm for empirical observation. Locke
believed that humans are born without knowledge and therefore must seek knowledge
through empirical observation. Locke’s term for this view was tabula rasa (meaning
“blank slate” in Latin). The idea is that life and experience “write” knowledge on us. For
Locke, then, the study of learning was the key to understanding the human mind. He
believed that there are no innate ideas.
behaviorism, it adopts precise quantitative analysis to study how people learn and think;
like Gestaltism, it emphasizes internal mental processes.
Psychobiology
One of Watson’s former students, Karl Spencer Lashley (1890–1958), brashly
challenged the behaviorist view that the human brain is a passive organ merely
responding to environmental contingencies outside the individual. Instead, Lashley
considered the brain to be an active, dynamic organizer of behavior. Lashley sought to
understand how the macro-organization of the human brain made possible such complex,
planned activities as musical performance, game playing, and using language. None of
these activities were, in his view, readily explicable in terms of simple conditioning.
Donald Hebb (1949) proposed the concept of cell assemblies as the basis for
learning in the brain. Cell assemblies are coordinated neural structures that develop
through frequent stimulation.
Linguist Noam Chomsky stressed both the biological basis and the creative
potential of language. He pointed out the infinite numbers of sentences we can produce
with ease. He thereby defied behaviorist notions that we learn language by reinforcement.
Even young children continually are producing novel sentences for which they could not
have been reinforced in the past.
Technological Underpinnings
By the end of the 1950s, some psychologists were intrigued by the tantalizing
notion that machines could be programmed to demonstrate the intelligent processing of
information. By 1956 a new phrase had entered our vocabulary. Artificial intelligence
(AI) is the attempt by humans to construct systems that show intelligence and,
particularly, the intelligent processing of information. Chess-playing programs, which
now can beat most humans, are examples of artificial intelligence
Ulric Neisser’s book Cognitive Psychology (Neisser, 1967) was especially critical
in bringing cognitivism to prominence by informing undergraduates, graduate students,
and academics about the newly developing field. Neisser defined cognitive psychology as
the study of how people learn, structure, store, and use knowledge.
Allen Newell and Herbert Simon (1972) proposed detailed models of human
thinking and problem solving from the most basic levels to the most complex. By the
1970s cognitive psychology was recognized widely as a major field of psychological
study with a distinctive set of research methods.
MODULE 2
COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
Neuropsychology is the study of the effects of lesions or dysfunctions of the Central
Nervous System (CNS) on cognition and behaviour. Neuropsychology is useful for both
research and clinical purposes:
1. It explores the functional architecture of the mind and its neural correlates;
2. It provides patients with a diagnosis and possibly with rehabilitative recommendations.
A revolution occurred when patient-based neuropsychology and cognitive psychology
eventually came together. The main tenet of cognitive psychology is that mental activity (i.e.
cognition) is information processing. By varying stimuli and instructions to the subjects, and
by measuring their responses, cognitive psychologists make inferences about the information
processing that intervenes between stimulus and response.
The term cognitive neuropsychology is applied to the analysis of those handicaps in
human cognitive function that result from brain injury. Cognitive neuropsychology is
essentially interdisciplinary, drawing both on neurology and on cognitive psychology for
insights into the cerebral organization of cognitive skills and abilities. Cognitive function is
the ability to use and integrate basic capacities such as perception, language, actions,
memory, and thought. The focus of clinical cognitive neuropsychology is on the many
different types of highly selective impairments of cognitive function that are observed in
individual patients following brain damage. The functional analysis of patients with selective
deficits provides a very clear window through which one can observe the organization and
procedures of normal cognition. Clinical cognitive neuropsychology has been successful in
demonstrating a large number of dissociations between the subcomponents of cognitive
skills. This enables to conclude that such components are dependent on distinct neural
systems. cognitive neuropsychologists seek to explain the patterns of impaired and intact
cognitive performance seen in brain-injured patients in terms of damage to one or more of
the components of a theory of normal cognitive functioning and, on the other hand, to draw
conclusions about normal, intact cognitive processes from observed disorders.
COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE
Cognitive neuroscience is the study of how the brain enables the mind. Brain science
explores how individual neurons operate and communicate to form complex neuronal
architectures that comprise the human brain. Cognitive science uses the experimental
methods of cognitive psychology and artificial intelligence to create and test models of
higher-level cognition such as thought and language. Cognitive neuroscience bridges these
two domains. It maps higher-level cognitive functions to known brain architectures and
known modes of neuronal processing. Cognitive neuroscientists tend to have a background in
experimental psychology, neurobiology, neurology, physics, and mathematics.
MODULE 3
Memory is our ability to encode, store, retain and subsequently recall information and
past experiences in the human brain. It can be thought of in general terms as the use of past
experience to affect or influence current behaviour. Memory is the sum total of what we
remember, and gives us the capability to learn and adapt from previous experiences as well
as to build relationships. It is the ability to remember past experiences, and the power or
process of recalling to mind previously learned facts, experiences, impressions, skills and
habits.
MODELS OF MEMORY
THE TRADITIONAL MODEL OF MEMORY OR STAGES OF MEMORY MODEL
Richard Atkinson and Richard Shiffrin (1968) proposed an alternative model that
conceptualized memory in terms of three memory stores:
a sensory store, capable of storing relatively limited amounts of information for very
brief periods;
a short-term store, capable of storing information for somewhat longer periods but of
relatively limited capacity as well; and
a long-term store, of very large capacity, capable of storing information for very long
periods, perhaps even indefinitely
The model differentiates among structures for holding information, termed stores, and
the information stored in the structures, termed memory. Today, cognitive psychologists
commonly describe the three stores as sensory memory, short-term memory, and long-term
memory. Also, Atkinson and Shiffrin were not suggesting that the three stores are distinct
physiological structures. Rather, the stores are hypothetical constructs—concepts that are not
themselves directly measurable or observable but that serve as mental models for
understanding how a psychological phenomenon works.
Sensory Store
The sensory store is the initial repository of much information that eventually enters
the short- and long-term stores. Strong evidence argues in favor of the existence of an iconic
store. The iconic store is a discrete visual sensory register that holds information for very
short periods. Its name derives from the fact that information is stored in the form of icons.
These in turn are visual images that represent something. Icons usually resemble whatever is
being represented. Similarly, auditory information enter the echoic store.
Short-Term Store
The short-term store does more than hold onto a few items. It also has some control
processes available that regulate the flow of information to and from the long-term store,
where we may hold information for longer periods. Typically, material remains in the short-
term store for about 30 seconds, unless it is rehearsed to retain it. Information is stored
acoustically (by the way it sounds) rather than visually (by the way it looks). In general, our
immediate (short-term) memory capacity for a wide range of items appears to be about seven
items, plus or minus two. An item can be something simple, such as a digit, or something
more complex, such as a word. Information can be retained in short term store through
maintenance rehearsal.
Long-Term Store
Theoretically, the capacity of Long Term Store could be unlimited, the main
constraint on recall being accessibility rather than availability. Duration might be a few
minutes or a lifetime. Suggested encoding modes are semantic (meaning) and
visual(pictorial) inthe main but can be acoustic also. Long term store is broadly divided into
two: declarative memory, consisting of semantic memory (which stores factual knowledge)
and episodic memory (which stores life events); and procedural memory. For information to
enterlong term store, elaborative rehearsal is needed. Information are meaningfully stored in
long term store using associations with existing information, conceptual hierarchies,
organisation and imagery.
THE LEVELS-OF-PROCESSING MODEL
A radical departure from the three-stores model of memory is the levels-of-processing
framework, developed by Craik & Lockhart, which postulates that memory does not
comprise three or even any specific number of separate stores, but rather varies along a
continuous dimension in terms of depth of encoding. In other words, there are theoretically
an infinite number of levels of processing (LOP) at which items can be encoded through
elaboration—or successively deeper understanding of material to be learned. There are no
distinct boundaries between one level and the next. The emphasis in this model is on
processing as the key to storage. The level at which information is stored will depend, in
large part, on how it is encoded. Moreover, the deeper the level of processing, the higher, in
general, is the probability that an item may be retrieved. The levels-of-processing framework
can also be applied to nonverbal stimuli.
The model describes three levels of processing:
The first level is simply perception, which gives us our immediate awareness of the
world around. At a deeper level, the structural features of the input (what it sounds or looks
like) are analysed. At the deepest level, meaning of the input is analysed, giving the best
memory.
As in the stages of memory model, rehearsal plays a major role in the deeper processing of
information. But according to this model, simply repeating the information- maintenance
rehearsal- is not enough. For deeper levels to be reached, the rehearsal must be elaborative.
Elaboration refers to the degree to which the incoming information is processed so that it can
be tied to, or integrated with, existing memories.
Cognitive Psychology Page 14
School of Distance Education
VISUAL PERCEPTION
There are different views on how we perceive the world. These views can be
summarized as bottom-up theories and top-down theories. Bottom-up theories describe
approaches where perception starts with the stimuli whose appearance is taken through
the eye. Therefore, they are data driven (i.e., stimulus-driven) theories. Many theorists
prefer top-down theories, according to which perception is driven by high-level cognitive
processes, existing knowledge, and the prior expectations that influence perception.
These theories then work their way down to considering the sensory data, such as the
perceptual stimulus.
BOTTOM-UP THEORIES
The four main bottom-up theories of form and pattern perception are direct
perception, template theories, feature theories, and recognition-by-components theory.
Theory Of Direct Perception
According to Gibson’s theory of direct perception, the information in our sensory
receptors, including the sensory context, is all we need to perceive anything. As the
environment supplies us with all the information we need for perception, this view is
sometimes also called ecological perception. In other words, we do not need higher
cognitive processes or anything else to mediate between our sensory experiences and our
perceptions. Existing beliefs or higher-level inferential thought processes are not
necessary for perception.
Template Theories
Template theories suggest that we have stored in our minds myriad sets of
templates. Templates are highly detailed models for patterns we potentially might
recognize. We recognize a pattern by comparing it with our set of templates. We then
choose the exact template that perfectly matches what we observe. We see examples of
template matching in our everyday lives. Fingerprints are matched in this way.
Feature-Matching Theories
Yet another alternative explanation of pattern and form perception may be found
in feature-matching theories. According to these theories, we attempt to match features of
a pattern to features stored in memory, rather than to match a whole pattern to a template
or a prototype. In Oliver Selfridge’s Pandemonium Model, there are four kinds of
demons: image demons, feature demons, cognitive demons, and decision demons. The
“image demons” receive a retinal image and pass it on to “feature demons.” Each feature
demon calls out when there are matches between the stimulus and the given feature.
These matches are yelled out at demons at the next level of the hierarchy, the “cognitive
(thinking) demons.” The cognitive demons in turn shout out possible patterns stored in
memory that conform to one or more of the features noticed by the feature demons.
MODULE 4
TRADITIONAL THEORIES OF INTELLIGENCE
MODULE 5
CONTEMPORARY THEORIES OF INTELLIGENCE
GARDNER’S THEORY OF MULTIPLE INTELLIGENCE:
Howard Gardner in his book ‘‘Frames of Mind, The Theory of Multiple
Intelligence’’ (1983), puts forth a new and different view of human intellctual
competencies. He argues boldly and cogently that we are all born with potential to
develop a multiplicity of Intelligence, most of which have been overlooked in our testing
society, and all of which can be drawn upon to make us competent individuals. The
potential for musical accomplishments, bodily mastery and spatial reasoning, and the
capacities to understand ourselves as well as others are, Gardner argues, ‘‘the multiple
forms of intelligence that we must add to the conventional—and typical tested—logical
and lingustic skills long called I.Q.’’.
The multiple intelligence theory is that people possess eight types of intelligence :
linguistic, logical, spatial, musical, motor ability, interpersonal, intrapersonal and
naturalistic intelligence.
Visual-Spatial - think in terms of physical space, as do architects and sailors. Very aware
of their environments. They like to draw, do jigsaw puzzles, read maps, and daydream.
They can be taught through drawings, verbal and physical imagery. Tools in educating
them include models, graphics, charts, photographs, drawings, 3-D modelling, video,
videoconferencing, television, multimedia, texts with pictures/charts/graphs.
Bodily-kinesthetic - use the body effectively, like a dancer or a surgeon. Keen sense of
body awareness. They like movement, making things, touching. They communicate well
through body language and be taught through physical activity, hands-on learning, acting
out, role playing. Tools include equipment and real objects.
Musical - show sensitivity to rhythm and sound. They love music, but they are also
sensitive to sounds in their environments. They may study better with music in the
background. They can be taught by turning lessons into lyrics, speaking rhythmically,
tapping out time. Tools include musical instruments, music, radio, stereo, CD-ROM,
multimedia.
Interpersonal - understanding, interacting with others. These students learn through
interaction. They have many friends, empathy for others, street smarts. They can be
taught through group activities, seminars, dialogues. Tools include the telephone, audio
conferencing, time and attention from the instructor, video conferencing, writing,
computer conferencing, E-mail.
Intrapersonal - understanding one's own interests, goals. These learners tend to shy
away from others. They're in tune with their inner feelings; they have wisdom, intuition
and motivation, as well as a strong will, confidence and opinions. They can be taught
through independent study and introspection. Tools include books, creative materials,
diaries, privacy and time. They are the most independent of the learners.
Linguistic - using words effectively. These learners have highly developed auditory
skills and often think in words. They like reading, playing word games, making up poetry
or stories. They can be taught by encouraging them to say and see words, read books
together. Tools include computers, games, multimedia, books, tape recorders, and lecture.
Logical -Mathematical - reasoning, calculating. Think conceptually, abstractly and are
able to see and explore patterns and relationships. They like to experiment, solve puzzles,
ask cosmic questions. They can be taught through logic games, investigations, mysteries.
They need to learn and form concepts before they can deal with details
Naturalistic: An ability to identify and distinguish among different types of plants,
animals, and weather formations that are found in the natural world.
Gardner accentuates two primary claims: l) All individuals possess the full range of
intelligences—the intelligences are what that define human beings, cognitively speaking;
(2) No two individuals, not even identical twins, exhibit precisely the same profile of
intellectual strengths and weaknesses. These constitute the principal scientific claims of
the theory.
STERNBERG’S TRIARCHIC THEORY
Psychologist Robert Sternberg (1985) has constructed a three—pronged, or
triarchic theory of intelligence. Sternberg states that Intelligence is: "Purposive adaptation to,
shaping of, and selection of real-world environments relevant to one's life". Purposive means
that intelligence is directed towards goals, however vague or subconscious they may be.
This means that intelligence is indicated by one's attempts to adapt to one's environment.
The Three types of intelligence are :
Analytical Intelligence (componential) —is what we generally think of as academic
ability. It enables us to solve problems and to acquire new knowledge. Problem—solving
skill include encoding information, combining and comparing pieces of information and
generating a solution. Sternberg believes that Analytical Intelligence is based on the joint
operations of metacomponents, performance components and knowledge acquisition
components of intelligence
Metacomponents: control, monitor and evaluate cognitive processing. These are
the executive functions to order and organise performance and knowledge
acquisition components. They are the higher-order processes that order and
organise the performance components. Used to analyze problems and pick a
strategy for solving them. They decide what to do and the performance
components actually do it.
Internal motivation. A passion to work for internal reasons that go beyond money and
status -which are external rewards, - such as an inner vision of what is important in life, a
joy in doing something, curiosity in learning, a flow that comes with being immersed in
an activity. A propensity to pursue goals with energy and persistence. Hallmarks include
a strong drive to achieve, optimism even in the face of failure, and organizational
commitment.
Empathy. The ability to understand the emotional makeup of other people. A skill in
treating people according to their emotional reactions. Hallmarks include expertise in
building and retaining talent, cross-cultural sensitivity, and service to clients and
customers.
Social skills. Proficiency in managing relationships and building networks, and an ability
to find common ground and build rapport. Hallmarks of social skills include effectiveness
in leading change, persuasiveness, and expertise building and leading teams.
PASS THEORY OF INTELLIGENCE
Neuropsychological research has consistently demonstrated that the brain is made up
of interdependent, but separate, functional systems. Neuroimaging studies and clinical
studies of individuals with brain lesions make it clear that the brain is modularized. Based
on A.R. Luria’s (1966) seminal work on the modularization of brain function, and
supported by decades of neuroimaging research, J.P. Das, first in 1975, proposed the
PASS theory of intelligence. This theory divides intelligence into four interrelated
cognitive processes:
1. Planning: This is the ability to make decisions about how to solve problems and
perform actions. It involves setting goals, anticipating consequences and using
feedback. Planning also involves the attention-arousal, simultaneous and
successive processing functions described below, and is associated with the frontal
lobes of the brain.
2. Attention-Arousal: This involves the ability to selectively attend to stimuli while
ignoring other distractions. Individuals with Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD)
have impairments in this area. The arousal functions are generally associated with
the brain stem and thalamus, whereas the higher attentional processes are thought
to be related to the planning functions of the frontal lobe.
3. Simultaneous Processing: This involves the ability to integrate separate stimuli
into a cohesive, interrelated whole. Simultaneous processing is necessary for
language comprehension, as in: “Who is the person in the following statement:
My mother’s father was his only son?” The occipital and parietal lobes are
thought to be important for these functions.
4. Successive Processing: This involves the ability to integrate stimuli into a
sequential order. An example of this process is the sequencing of letters and words
Cognitive Psychology Page 23
School of Distance Education
*******