Belletti - Truncation Vs Reduction in Development.10.06.2021
Belletti - Truncation Vs Reduction in Development.10.06.2021
In early stages of their linguistic development, young children are known to undergo a period
in which they may make use of somewhat incomplete clausal forms to express contents which,
in the adult grammar, would be expressed through a full clause, with all relevant inflectional
features and functional positions present and properly realized, according to the general
A much-studied stage of this type is the Root Infinitive/RI stage (up to around 3 years of age),
discussed in Rizzi 1993/94 and much related work (e.g. Wexler’s 1994 Optional Infinitives).
Following Rizzi’s original proposal and its later developments, the RI stage may be
characterized as resulting from a truncation operation of the syntactic tree of the clause,
available to the developing child. Such truncation option should be favoured to some extent in
early stages as it allows for an overall less complex syntactic computation of the clause, less
costly for the child’s developing grammar (Rizzi 2006): a smaller portion of the complete
adult grammar of different languages (as in e.g. German Topic-drop constructions). Thus,
truncation constitutes a possible parametric option that young children (may) exploit during
their development.
It can also be the case that in early stages, the syntactic tree be only partly accessible to the
developing child. Thus, only those syntactic constructions and computations that involve the
portion of the syntactic tree structure available to the child at that given point would be possible,
1
In a cartographic articulated CP, truncation may take place at different levels. The point is addressed below.
1
whereas those that involve bigger portions of the tree, would be impossible, hence absent. This
is the ‘growing tree’ logic, recently presented in Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi (2021). As
discussed there, ‘growing tree’ and ‘truncation’ define different stages, as the latter process
optionally applies on an already fully-grown syntactic tree, possibly to allow for a more
parsimonious computation for the developing system, in the terms described. Truncation is
thus interpreted as a reduction operation, taking place on the completely grown syntactic tree.
Further reduced constructions of various types are also present in different languages (beside
e.g. the aforementioned Topic-drop construction). Adult grammars handle such constructions
in different ways, in compliance with general properties of the specific languages. Among the
reduced structures present in several languages, Reduced Relative Clauses/RRCs are the case
which will be the focus of the discussion here, with special reference to Italian. Reduced
relative clauses are (past) participial clauses available in various languages, including Italian.
In (1) two examples illustrate the construction in Italian and in the English equivalent
translation:
The position corresponding to the thematic interpretation of the noun phrase head of the
reduced relative clause is indicated with <__>, highlighting the fact that the head of the relative
corresponds to the internal argument of the verb, expressed in the past participle form in the
reduced relative. Such past participle corresponds to a passive past participle as witnessed by
the (possible) presence of a by-phrase expressing the external argument of the verb.
The term ‘reduced’ refers here to the intuitive analysis that any native speaker would informally
and naturally provide, according to which the sentences in (1) are the reduced form of complete
relative clauses such as those in (2), in which the deleted part is not pronounced:
2
(2) a Il libro che è (stato)/viene letto <__> dallo studente
The book that is (has been) read by the student
Both structures mentioned, RI and RRCs, are instances of clausal constructions that use a
limited portion of the clause structure, be this the result of a truncation operation on the already
grown tree2, or else the output of a different type of reduction operation as the one informally
pictured in (2). In what follows some items of the empirical background that led to truncation-
type proposals in the late 19-nineties will be briefly reviewed in the aim of proposing a
comparison with the different reduction operation occurring in reduced relative clauses of the
type in (1). The comparison is inspired by developmental results over a long period of time
concerning on the one side the truncation stage, leading to use of Root-Infinitives in early
grammars, as has been documented in several languages, and, on the other side, more recent
such as object relative clauses and RRCs in particular. It appears that the incomplete clausal
structures are not treated in the same way in the two cases during development: truncated
structures belong to an early stage, whereas RRCs belong to a later stage. The early vs late
appearance of the different expressions both using limited portions of the clause structure
strongly indicates that something more than just the ‘small size’ of the clause must be at stake
and play a crucial role. What the relevant factor(s) may be is the focus of the following pages.
Section 2 provides the essential background defining the truncation stage and its interaction
with growing tree; in section 3 the further theoretically relevant background on the locality
2
Or possibly in very early stages, it could be the consequence of the incomplete growth of the syntactic tree, as
mentioned.
3
illustrated. In Section 4 the main empirical observation from developmental results is described
concerning the late appearance of RRCs; a proposal is outlined of the main features of the
assumed analysis of RRCs from which their fundamental different status w.r.t. truncated
structures clearly emerges. This difference may be at the core of the reason why the two
incomplete structures have a different developmental time line. Some related consequence of
the proposal are sketched out in section 5. Some reflections will conclude this work focusing
As mentioned in the introductory section 1, young children typically undergo a stage in their
development known as the Root Infinitive stage in which they produce root declarative
descriptive clauses with no specification of tense nor expression of features of finite agreement
inflection (i.e. including the person feature). (3) below is an example from French where the
This stage can be described as one in which a somewhat impoverished sentence structure is
consistently properly mastered by young children, in contrast with one expressing a full-
fledged functional structure. Cross-linguistic differences have been detected in the way the
stage can manifest itself in different languages (e.g. for Italian through use of past participles
or other not richly inflected forms such as the imperative, Antinucci & Miller 1976; Salustri &
Hyams 2003, 2006). In all cases the impoverished expression of phi-features characteristically
4
Following Rizzi’s original proposal and its earlier and later developments, the RI stage may be
similar operation of pruning has been assumed to occur in the domain of aphasia, as proposed
by Friedmann 2002). Such truncation option is assumed to be to some extent favoured in early
stages as it allows for an overall less complex syntactic computation, less costly for the child’s
developing grammar (Rizzi 2006): A smaller portion of the complete clause structure is
computed by truncating it. Truncation may occur at the CP level, as illustrated in the sketchy
diagram in (4); in (4) the possibility that truncation may occur at a lower level e.g. vP, is also
illustrated:
(4)
CP
1
1
C TP
1
1
T vP
5
As Rizzi’s term RI emphasizes, the impoverished structure is solely present in the children’s
embedded contexts nor in contexts in which a richer structure and its components are present,
In (5) and in all embedded clauses clearly containing the CP level, which are produced by
children during the RI stage, the verb always appears fully inflected in its finite form; the
subject is also consistently overtly realized in the non-null subject French. This RI option
correlates with the concomitant Child Null Subject stage, in which the specifier of the root, i.e.
Spec/TP in (4), can remain unpronounced. The Child Null Subject stage in turn is known to
5
also be a root phenomenon, as confirmed by the overt realization of the subject in (5).3
Crucially, productions like (5) in the RI infinitive stage indicate that children during this stage
do have access to the complete functional structure of the clause. They may just not use it in
root declarative contexts. This would allow them to perform a less complex computation
In a cartographic articulated CP as in the map in (6) from Rizzi and Bocci (2017), truncation
(6) Force > Int > Top > Q/Foc > Mod > Fin > IP
Topic drop constructions mentioned earlier, of the type found in German (Cardinaletti 1990,
1990a for a first discussion), can be analysed (Rizzi 2006) as one such instance of truncation
available in adult grammars, in which the operation occurs at the TopP level. Spec/TopP thus
becomes the specifier of the root and as such it can remain unpronounced (dropped, as in the
option that young children (may) exploit during an early stage of their development, which,
According to the growing tree logic recently presented in Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi (2021),
the syntactic tree of the clause grows incrementally during development. The main
consequence of this stage-by-stage growth is that the clausal structure can be only partly
3
This is the crucial property differentiating the null subject of this stage from the null subject of null subject
languages, a consequence of the positive setting of the null subject parameter (Rizzi 1993/94 reconsidering the
first grammatical approach to this important developmental stage presented in Hyams’ 1986 first pioneering and
influential work).
4
An instance of CP projection up to the FocP layer may be instantiated by the case of the CP of cleft sentences
(Belletti 2008, 2012), which will be taken up again in section 5.1 See footnote 15 also relevant to the point made
in the text.
6
accessible to the developing child in earlier stages. The growing tree approach is a recent
revisitation of a family of proposals formulated during the 19-nineties5, grounded on the results
from work in syntactic cartography, which provides refined maps and detailed levels of the
functional structure of the clause. This drives detailed investigations, which allow for subtle
discoveries that may otherwise remain unnoticed through less fine-grained lenses. As the
analysis of the acquisition of the Hebrew left periphery has revealed, the acquisition of the
clausal structure progresses in three fundamental stages, with different constructions involving
the relevant portion of structure appearing at different pace. Each stage only allows for those
syntactic constructions and computations that involve the portion of the syntactic tree available
to the child at that point, and excluding those that involve bigger, i.e. higher, portions of the
The growing tree logic is crucially implicational: If the tree has grown up to CP, all structures
belonging to the lower TP (and vP) level will also be available. In the cartographically analyzed
CP the implications are detailed: set of constructions realized in low positions of the clause
belong to a certain Stage1, and thus all appear during this stage, whereas constructions
involving higher positions of the left periphery belong to the later Stage2 and are not available
yet in Stage1, i.e. they do not appear in children’s productions. In contrast, if structures of Stage
2 are present, structures of Stage 1 are all also available and thus appear in children’s
productions. In a simplified representation as (4), the implicational chain implies that if the CP
level is available, the TP level must be available as well (and a fortiori vP as well). The precise
manifestation of the stages in the acquisition of the Hebrew left periphery are presented in
detail in Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi (2021). Here, it should just be kept in mind that the stage
at which relative clauses are acquired is the last: the stage in which the highest positions of the
5
Dating from Radford (1990) on; see Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi (2021) and references cited there in this
connection.
7
CP space are present. Given the map in (6), the left periphery is present up to the highest ForceP
level.
The implicational flavor of growing tree also characterizes the truncation stage.
Thus, similarly to what just noted in terms of the growing tree, truncation in an already grown
tree can occur at the CP level, hence the available structure will contain both the TP and the vP
layers; if it occurs at the TP level the available layer will be vP. Crucially, truncation cannot
only affect the TP level without also affecting the higher CP. As observed in the introduction,
work on truncation has determined that this reduction operation takes place on completely
grown syntactic trees. This is so as truncation appears to be an option that children may exploit
in Root clauses, in the same stage in which they master constructions involving full-fledged
clauses, with the expression of the complete functional structure up to the CP layer, such as
A rich bulk of experimental findings coming from a mass of work on the acquisition of A’-
dependencies (e.g. Relative clauses), that appeared over the last ten years or so, has reached a
(7) i. children can master complex structures such as relative clauses from relatively early
on (3;6-4)
ii. Subject Relatives/SRs are mastered better and earlier than Object Relatives/ORs
(confirming results from classical literature).
iii. not all ORs are hard for children (a finding also holding in types of impairment).
As for (7)iii, , the main finding has been that the only hard ORs, in both development and
impairment in fact (Friedmann, Yachini, Szterman 2015), are those showing a situation of
6
By truncating the whole CP layer, the remaining structure would be a (non-finite) TP projection, whence the
RI stage.
8
featural intervention expressed according to the featural Relativized Minimality/fRM
grammatical principle (Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi 2009). These are the hardest structures,
which are not mastered by children until late in development (not up until around age 9).
Specifically, according to the system developed in Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi (2009 and
and related subsequent literature) the hardest ORs (and object A’-dependencies more
generally) are those in which a feature inclusion situation is created between an intervening
lexical subject and the lexical head of the relative clause. Assuming a nominal feature dubbed
[+NP] to be among the features to which the principle fRM is sensitive, inclusion of the
computed by young children (in the relevant age range). This holds crosslinguistically, as
should be expected given the principled nature of the account. The account is schematized in
(8):
*
The relation between X (target) and Y (origin) cannot be established if there is
a Z that structurally intervenes and X and Z share relevant features.
(adapted from: Rizzi 1990, 2004; Starke 2001; Friedmann, Belletti and
Rizzi 2009)
In elicited production experiments, various ways have been found that children adopt to handle
the hard object dependency: they systematically tend to resort to alternative constructions
which do not involve the intervention configuration in (8). They do so either maintaining the
9
target meaning or just producing non-target sentences (most typically: a SR in place of the
results over the years have shown that adults as well tend to avoid the hard intervention
configuration illustrated in (8). This point is taken up in the following subsection which
3.2.1 Other findings from development: The case of RRCs and the comparison with
adults.
Results from production experiments in Italian have shown that both adults and children tend
not to produce elicited ORs containing the hard intervention configuration of feature inclusion.
The first crucial observation is that adults tend to produce passive past participial Reduced
Relative Clauses/RRCs; i.e. structures of the type presented in (1) in the introduction. Indeed,
they do so in an ample proportion. Considering the results presented in Contemori and Belletti
(2014) for instance, in about half of their answers in the passive, adults resort to RRCs (48%);
the other half of their productions are full relative clauses using copular or venire passives. As
in the quoted references and related ones, I refer to this type of productions as PORs – Passive
Object Relatives –, i.e. relative clauses in the passive answering a question eliciting an active
object relative. PORs may come in various guises: the reduced form is the one largely exploited
by adults (a result also found in Belletti & Chesi 2014). In contrast, in the same experimental
conditions, children tend to produce different types of structures. They also resort to some
PORs, and more and more so as they grow older and the passive computation is better mastered
by them. Interestingly, however, the passive clauses that they use as their PORs are not RRCs,
which are virtually absent from their productions. Their PORs are clearly unreduced clauses.
Moreover, most of their passives are ‘longer’ passive as in the case of si-causative passives, a
10
much-preferred structure by children from their early productions.7 The comparison between
children and adults in this respect is discussed in detail in Belletti (2017). Here the focus is
going to be on the absence of RRCs in children production. Specifically, the following new
Q: What makes the reduced relative clause structure of RRCs somewhat hard,
so that children do not appear to be ready to master it and they do not resort to
RRCs in their PORs, differently from what adults do in the same elicitation
contexts?
The question is taken up in the following section 4. It will be addressed by viewing it from the
with the smaller structure that young(er) children access, in contrast, through the truncation
4. Reduction in RRCs and the comparison with truncation: New questions from the
perspective of language development.
The question that adults’ PORs realized as RRCs raise can in turn be addressed from the angle
of the issue of what reduction amounts to in RRCs, in particular in light of the insights coming
from the RI stage and the truncation analysis with which our discussion began.
Q’: What makes the reduction implemented in passive past participial RRCs harder
and different from the reduction derived through truncation that young(er) children
often select in early(-er) stages of their development?
7
Results from Contemori and Belletti (2014): in the age range 3;4-8;10 up to 40% of children’s PORs are realized
with si-causative passives in one experiment, and up to 35% are realized as copular/venire passives in another. In
all tasks the si-causative passive is the first and only one type of passive to appear in the youngest groups.
8
A complement question is the following: Why do children opt for an apparently more complex structure (like a
full passive or a si-causative passive); what makes it more readily accessible to them? Some possible hypotheses
to answer this question are presented in Manetti and Belletti (2015), Belletti (2020); for space reasons they will
not be addressed here. The attention here is rather on the lack of children’s use of RRCs. It is worth noticing that
since the experimental conditions are always very informal, the fact that adults resort to RRCs in their answers to
the elicitation questions suggests that such structure is productive and not limited to special formal registers in
standard Italian.
11
From this angle, the reduction occurring in the POR realized as RRCs must not be simple for
the young children; in fact, it appears to be harder than the computation of some complete full
clauses, such as the PORs in the si-causative passive or even in the copular/venire passive,
which children produce to some extent in the elicitation experiments reviewed (footnote 7).
We note first of all that the difficulty with RRCs cannot be a consequence of not having yet
available the relevant position in the CP involved in relativization, in a growing tree type spirit.
The same young children are able to compute subject relatives at the same time in which they
do not resort to RRCs (up to more than 80% SRs in the same experimental results from
Contemori & Belletti 2014 are produced by the youngest group, 3;4-3;11). And some complete
PORs are also produced, as mentioned. Hence, it cannot be an issue having to do with the
process of relativization per se, in general and specifically in an incomplete tree. The syntactic
tree is fully available to them as is the relativization process (and passive as well, to some
extent).
As discussed in previous work (see also Belletti 2014), PORs in which relativization is
disturbing anymore, precisely due to passivization. The hypothesis is based on the assumption
that the derivation of passive involves smuggling (Collins 2005; Belletti 2014; Belletti and
Collins 2021), moving a chunk of the verb phrase containing the verb at the past participle and
the Internal argument pass the External argument sitting in the specifier of the highest vP.
Form this smuggled position, the object moves into the position of the left periphery hosting
the relative head, Spec-Force as in Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi (2021)9; in this step of the
9
The relative CP is embedded in a D projection, as in Bianchi’s (1999) analysis, assumed in Friedmann, Belletti
and Rizzi (2021).
12
derivation, it does not encounter any intervener on the way. The assumed derivation of a POR
is schematized in (9):
Il bambino che [(__) è [VP pettinato <il bambino>] da [vP la mamma <VP>]
Passivization step/smuggling
The child that (__) is combed <the child> by the mother
(8) schematically illustrates the derivation of a full POR, namely a POR with a complete clausal
structure and a full-fledged morphological manifestation of tense, agreement and the passive
voice, including the inflected passive auxiliary, the past participle and the by-phrase. Given
this background analysis, the question to ask is: how does it compare to the analysis of a
RRCs are small clauses that contain the CP layer, hosting the position into which the noun
phrase head of the relative clause is attracted; the complement of such CP is an impoverished
clausal projection. It is a non-finite complement, containing the past participle and other
components of the passive morphology (Siloni 1995; Harwood 2018, for proposal along similar
lines; Sleeman 2017 for overview). The CP layer is not a complete left periphery in turn: it
contains the higher Force projection hosting the relative head in its Spec, but does not contain
the lowest Fin head, consistently with the non-finite nature of its small clausal complement.
Lack of Fin is the way in which absence of the overt realization of the complementizer in RRCs
can be expressed, under the assumption that the finite complementizer always originates in the
lowest Fin head of the structure in (6) and then raises up into the Force head in full embedded
clauses, such as complement clauses and relatives (see 5.1 for more on this point). The ForceP
layer takes as its complement (at least) a past participial projection in the passive voice,
13
possibly embedded under an aspectual head.10 This analysis is illustrated in (10)a, b. (10)a
presents the low portion of the functional structure of past participial RRCs in the passive voice
indicating the smuggling step of the derivation; in (10)b the high portion of the functional
structure of the past participial RRC is presented in which the relativization step occurs:
(10)
10
This analysis may also extend to past participial small clauses in general, such as the Absolute small
clauses/ASC discussed in Belletti (1992); Cecchetto and Donati (2020) for recent revisitation. One salient
difference between RRCs and the ASCs is lack of the relativization step in the latter.
14
4.2. The source of the difficulty with RRCs in (early stages of) development
Given the analysis of RRCs sketched out in (10), a natural reason is provided for the late
appearance of passive past participial RRCs in the detected developmental stage discussed: It
in a stage in which the syntactic tree is fully grown, up to the highest Force layer. In particular,
since past participial RRCs are subordinate clauses, they imply presence of the CP level up to
Force. A fully-grown syntactic tree should then contain all positions lower than the highest
Force. However, the structural representation in (10) precisely realizes a syntactic structure in
which the highest position is present, but neither lower portions of the Left periphery nor
positions of the TP area are fully present. In other words, the derivation in (10) occurs in a
structure that is incompatible with the incremental growth of the syntactic tree as well as with
the discussed implicational logic of both growing tree and truncation, according to which if a
higher structural position is present, all positions below it are present as well. Indeed, the
truncation option only operates at the edge of the clausal projection. As discussed, truncation
cannot affect intermediate layers of the structure. This is the crucial property illustrated in (4).
In contrast, the clausal representation of RRCs precisely instantiates a structure that cannot be
spotted by the results, which indicate total lack of RRCs in the elicited productions of young
children in contrast with adults, since such structures cannot be derived through some form of
truncation, they simply cannot be derived at all: a fully projected CP up to Force, should
express a fully projected sentence structure at this stage11. Note that this is consistent with the
results from children’s elicited productions, in which, in the same experiment, when they
resorted to PORs, children always produced full clausal unreduced PORs, as mentioned in
11
This is an updated way of phrasing Rizzi’s principle according to which all clauses are CPs (Rizzi 1993/94).
15
3.2.1. The reduction instantiated by RRCs is thus not a parsimonious computation at young
ages, in contrast with truncation, which may be accessed (also) in very early stages, truncating
5.1. Lack of Fin and the absence of the complementizer in past participial RRCs
According to the proposed analysis, the reduced structure of past participial RRCs only
contains the upper Force layer of the CP left periphery. Specifically, the lowest Fin head
closing the CP domain is not present (Rizzi 1997 and structure in 6).
This expresses the non-finite/untensed nature of past participial RRCs.13 If the finite
expected (and similarly in past participial Absolute small clauses, see footnote 9).
The idea that the finite complementizer originates in Fin, and then raises up to Force – thus
satisfying selection in case of complement clauses – is not new (see e.g. Belletti 2008, 2013
and references cited therein). Here I will just mention the case presented in the quoted previous
work providing evidence for the low location of the finite complementizer, lower than the
higher left peripheral (corrective) focus position. The relevant evidence comes from clefts
sentences in Italian (and also the finite complement of perception verbs in pseudorelatives)
where location of the finite complementizer che in Fin is visible. This is illustrated in (11)a, b
(for an object cleft), where the complementizer displays a distribution parallel to that of the
12
The more general issue of the acquisition of small clauses in development goes beyond the scope of the present
discussion. The prediction is that small clauses could be early acquired only if their analysis is compatible with
the incremental growth of the syntactic tree or with truncation at the edge. Hence, embedded small clauses may
in fact be late acquired. A result in need of closer investigation, but which is consistent with results on the late
acquisition of raising (Mateu and Hyams 2021 and references cited therein; also considering other properties at
play interacting with locality) and also on the late acquisition of ECM/Raising to Object, on which see section
5.3.
13
The proposal shares insights with Pesetsky’s (2021) Exfoliation proposal, according to which non-finiteness
correlates with smaller clausal structure.
16
Japanese complementizer no, the lowest complementizer of the Japanese right periphery, also
Thus, in RRCs lack of the Fin layer, leads to no overt expression of the complementizer.
5.2. Past participial adjectival modification in Italian child spontaneous speech and in
elicited production
Belletti and Chesi (2014) reported that some past participial modifications are present in young
children’s spontaneous productions. The corpus study undertaken in that work (files from
CHILDES database), has revealed the presence of expressions like the following:
These expressions should not be considered RRCs but are rather best analyzed as
such, they do not involve the kind of clausal reduction that young children do not appear to be
Similarly, recent experimental findings in Martini (2020), have documented apparent past
participial reduced PORs in children’s answers to eliciting questions (in both French and
Italian). Most of these productions (87%), however, do not contain the overt expression of the
by-phrase, suggesting again an adjectival nature. Moreover, the age factor may also play a role
as these elicited productions mainly came from older children (e.g.: Quella sgridata/ the one
14
In Japanese a dedicated form of the low complementizer, i.e. no, is available, differently from Italian where the
same form che also used in full declaratives appears in clefts. See references quoted for further details.
15
In (11) the complement of the copula is a FocP, due to the selecting properties of be; hence the structure of
the CP does not reach the Force layer; the complementizer may be assumed to remain in Fin in this construction.
See references quoted for further details.
17
punished. Quella punita dalla maestra/ the one scolded by the teacher), suggesting a gradual
Similar conclusions on late access to reduced structures can be drawn from results on the late
European Portuguese/EP, discussed in Santos, Hyams and Gonçalves (2016). According to the
results presented, young children (3 to 5 yo) do not properly master use of reduced (defective)
sentential complements in RtO until relatively late. Interestingly, as the authors note,
sometimes they also over-extend use of Inflected infinitival complements in contexts in which
these forms are not possible in the adult target grammar (e.g. in object control), as illustrated
In the authors’ words, children have an early preference for ‘complete functional
which they opt for a structure including a complete rich person agreement. These results from
EP are also in line with the observed later acquisition of RtO with the verb want in English
presented in Landau & Thornton (2011). This observation is especially significant when
compared the one on subject control sentences, which are assumed to involve a more richly
articulated clausal structure, and which, all things being equal, appear to be more easily
16
Ages tested 3 to 9.yo Distribution of the relevant productions is as follows: 1 case at 5yo; 9 cases at 7yo, of
which only 1 with by-phrase; 14 cases at 8yo; 25 cases at 9yo, of which 5 with by-phrase.
18
6. General concluding remarks
The developmental data reviewed, strongly suggest an overall general conclusion, which is
particularly significant if one looks at it from the perspective of the issue of complexity. The
conclusion is that young children do not necessarily favor shorter expressions involving ‘less’
words, affixes…., over longer ones, and they do so sometimes in contrast with what adults
favor instead. We have seen this happen in the Italian example reviewed concerning the late
access to RRCs. In the same elicitation condition, the typical answer provided by adults is a
‘shorter’ RRC. Children’s answers are different and, when realized in the passive, they are
typically ‘longer’. This is a type of result that also the cross-linguistic findings briefly
mentioned in the previous section indicate, in different domains. Then, the conclusion is that
it all depends on how the ‘size’ of the clause structure is obtained. And it may so happen, as
in the case reviewed, that shorter does not count as simpler for the developing grammar. It can
in fact count as harder, thus in fact more complex at the appropriate level of analysis. This in
turn provides a reason as to why the construction appears later in development. The proposal
here has been that the reduction of the sentential structure of RRCs may be one such case, as
its late mastering suggests, in clear contrast with the early process of truncation at the root,
which young(er) children can exploit on complexity grounds. According to the proposal
developed here, the difficulty with RRCs comes from the incomplete projection of the syntactic
structure in the intermediate area between the CP and the TP layers. The proposal here has
been that this type of reduction of the clause structure is at odds both with the incremental
growth of the syntactic clausal structure and with a truncation process that could only occur at
unexpected behaviors during development such as the one emerged from the reviewed data
discussed in these notes, whereby adults opt for ‘less’ where children go for ‘more’. In turn,
19
developmental stages may be illuminating of relevant steps of linguistic analyses and the
theoretical assumptions behind them, thus contributing to reveal their deepest significance.
These notes are thus a contribution to the productive ongoing dialogue between formal
References
Antinucci, F. & R. Miller. 1976. How children talk about what happened. Journal of Child
Language 3. 167–189.
Belletti, Adriana. 2015. The Focus map of clefts: Extraposition and Predication. In: U.Shlonsky
ed. Beyond Functional Sequence, Cartography of Syntactic Structures volume 10,
Oxford University Press, 42-59
Belletti, Adriana. 2014. Notes o Passive Object Relatives. In P.Svenonius ed. Functional
Structure from Top to Toe, New York:Oxford University Press, 97-114
Belletti, A. 2013. On Fin: Italian che, Japanese no, and the selective properties of the copula in
clefts, in Miyamoto Y. et al. eds. Deep Insights, Broad Perspectives. Essays in Honor
of Mamoru Saito, Kaitakusha Co., Ltd, Tokyo, p 42-55
Belletti, A. 2012. Revisiting the CP of Clefts. In: Discourse and Grammar. From Sentence
Types to Lexical Categories. Berlino:de Gruyter, 91-114
Belletti, A. 2008. The CP of Clefts. RGG. Rivista di Grammatica Generativa, vol. 33, 191-204
Belletti, A. 1992. Agreement and Case in Past Participial Clauses in Italian. In: T.Stowell
and E.Wehrli eds. Syntax and Semantics, vol.26, p. 21-44, New York: Academic Press
Belletti, A. & C. Chesi 2014. A syntactic approach toward the interpretation of some
distributional frequencies: Comparing Relative clauses in Italian corpora and in elicited
production. In Rivista di Grammatica Generativa, 36, 1-28
Belletti, A. & C. Manetti 2018. Topics and passives in Italian speaking children and adults.
Language Acquisition, (2019, 26:2, 153-182) DOI: 10.1080/10489223.2018.1508465
20
Bianchi, V. 1999. Consequences of antisymmetry: Headed relative clauses. Berlin: Mouton de
Gruyter.
Collins, C. 2005. A smuggling approach to the passive in English. Syntax 8(2). 81–120.
Contemori, C. & A. Belletti 2014. Relatives and Passive Object Relatives in Italian speaking
children and adults: Intervention in production and comprehension. Applied
Psycholinguistics 35(6), 1021-1053
Friedmann, N., A. Belletti, L.Rizzi 2021. Growing trees: The acquisition of the left periphery.
accepted for publication in Glossa.
Friedmann N., A. Belletti, L.Rizzi 2009. Relativized Relatives: Types of intervention in the
acquisition of A’-dependencies. Lingua 119: 67-88
Friedmann, N., Yachini, M., and Szterman, R. 2015. “Relatively easy relatives: children with
syntactic SLI avoid intervention,” in Structures, Strategies and Beyond. E. Di
Domenico, C. Hamann, and S. Matteini eds.Amsterdam: John Benjamins; Linguistik
Aktuell series, 303–320.
Harwood, W.2018. Reduced Relatives and Extended Phases: A Phase-Based Analysis of the
Inflectional Restrictions on English Reduced Relative Clauses. Studia Linguistica,
72(2), 428-471
Hyams, N. 1986. Language acquisition and the theory of parameters. Dordrecht: D. Reidel
Landau, I. and R. Thornton 2011. Early Child Control. Lingua 121, 920-941.
Manetti, C. & Belletti, A. 2015. Causatives and the acquisition of the Italian passive. In
Language Acquisition and Development. C.Hamann and E. Ruingendijk eds.,
Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 282-298
Martini, K. 2020. Aspects of the acquisition of locality. Comprehension and production studies
21
in Italian and French. Doctoral dissertation, University of Geneva
Mateu, V. & N. Hyams 2021. On children’s late acquisition of raising seem and control
promise: Is a unified account possible? In: C.Collins and A.Belletti eds. Smuggling in
Syntax ,Oxford Studies in Comparative Syntax, OUP, 222-254
Radford, A. 1990. Syntactic theory and the acquisition of English syntax: The nature of early
child grammars of English. Blackwell: Oxford.
Rizzi, L. 2004. Locality and Left Periphery. In: Structures and Beyond – The Cartography of
Syntactic Structures, vol 3, A. Belletti ed., Oxford University Press, Oxford, New
York, 223-251
Rizzi, L.1993/94. Some Notes on Linguistic Theory and Language Development: The Case of
Root Infinitives. Language Acquisition, 4 (3), 371-393.
Rizzi, L. & G.Bocci 2017. The left periphery of the clause – Primarily illustrated for Italian.
In: The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Syntax, Second Edition, M. Everaert & H. C.
van Riemsdijk eds., John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 2017, DOI:
10.1002/9781118358733.wbsyncom104
Saito, M. 2012. Sentence type and the Japanese right periphery. In E.Zimmermann and
G.Grewendorf eds. Ten years after. Studies in Generative Grammar 112, De Gruyter
Salustri, M. & N.Hyams 2003. Is there an analogue to the RI stage in the Null Subject
languages? Proceedings BUCLD 29. Sommerville: Cascadilla Press. 692-703
Salustri, M. & Hyams, N. 2006. Looking for the universal core of the RI stage. In V. Torrens
& L. Escobar, eds. The Acquisition of Syntax in Romance Languages, John Benjamins,
Amsterdam
Santos A.L., Gonçalves A. & N. Hyams 2016. Aspects of the acquisition of object control
and ECM-Type verbs in European Portuguese, Language Acquisition, 23:3, 199-233.
Siloni, T. 1995. On participial relatives and complementizer D°: A case study in Hebrew and
French. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 13(3), 445-487.
Starke, M. 2001. Move dissolves into merge: A theory of locality. Geneva, Université de
22
Genève dissertation.
Wexler, K. 1994. Optional Infinitives, head movement and the economy of derivations in child
grammar. In D. Lightfoot & N. Hornstein (Eds.) Verb movement. Cambridge University
Press: 305-350
23