Learning and Visual Communication
Learning and Visual Communication
net/publication/331492219
CITATIONS READS
4 6,110
1 author:
D. Sless
Communication Research Institute
65 PUBLICATIONS 370 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by D. Sless on 09 April 2019.
by David Sless
First published in 1981
by Croom Helm Ltd.,
This edition first published in 2018 by Routledge
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon, OX14 4RN
and by Routledge
711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 1981 David Sless
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or
utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now
known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any
information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the
publishers.
Publisher’s Note
The publisher has gone to great lengths to ensure the quality of this reprint but
points out that some imperfections in the original copies may be apparent.
Disclaimer
The publisher has made every effort to trace copyright holders and welcomes
correspondence from those they have been unable to contact.
A Library of Congress record exists under LCCN: 81006417
Preface
Acknowledgements
2. Communication 23
Introduction 23; The Two Faces o f Communication 24; The
Discovery o f Pulsars 25; The Communication Schema 26;
Inference and Visual Communication 27; Communication and
Information 28; The Case o f the Wink/Twitch 28; Meaning 30;
4What is the Meaning o f this Message?* 31; A Note on Effects 32;
Behaviourism and Rules 33; Postscript to the Pulsar 34; The
Author/Message Relation 35; The Inferred Audience 35; Know
ledge o f the Audience 38
References 192
Index 200
To Toni, Justine, Hannah, Eva and Georgia
PREFACE
David Sless
Adelaide
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Introduction
15
16 The Thinking Eye
explain the way the mind’s eye worked and so on in an endless regress.
Another problem with the analogy is that the eye has a very poor
lens and the cone of clear vision is only 2°wide. (You can test this by
holding a coin out in front of you, fixating the centre and at the same
time trying to read the date at the edge. It cannot be done unless the
angle between the eye, centre and edge of the coin is less than two
degrees.)
Yet another complication arises as a consequence of the character
istic movement of the eye. The eye is never at rest. It moves in a series
of jumps and between jumps it has a constant tremor, so that the
focused light is continually moving over the retinal field. A photo
graphic plate under such conditions would simply be a blur. If the
image is stabilised under experimental conditions the retinal cells
become satiated and the object on which the eye is focused appears to
merge or disappear into the background (Pritchard, 1961).
This blurred retinal image has two other technical defects. At the
point where the optic nerve leaves the eye there are no light-sensitive
cells so that effectively there is a hole in the retinal image; and the
image is inverted. Yet none of these facts is registered in our
perception which is of a panoramic, stable, unperforated, upright
world. So the retinal image explains nothing but in its turn generates a
whole series of difficulties that require explanation.
If this were not enough, the retina itself, far from being a simple
array of light-sensitive receptors, is an intricate network of cells which
organise the incoming visual information before transmitting it along
the optic nerve fibre. There are over 100 million light-sensitive cells in
the retina. There is also a series of cells, called bipolar cells, which
interconnect across groups of light-sensitive cells, and connect these
to the ganglion cells that make up the optic nerve fibre. There are only
800,000 ganglion cells which of itself suggests that some condensation
or recoding of the retinal information takes place. In fact with the
development of microelectrode techniques it has been possible to
compare the information being presented to the eye with the
neurological activity in the brain, and it has been discovered that singe
neurons are ‘feature specific’; that is, each one responds to aparticular
line or a particular direction of movement rather than to a particular
focus on the retina. The analogy between eye and camera must finally
be laid to rest. The eye’s constructive integrative ability must cast
serious doubt on the idea that vision is a sensory process pure and
simple.
However the attempt to maintain the innocence of vision finds
18 The Thinking Eye
we can predict what the information available to the eye will be from
any point of regard because light behaves consistently. This coherence,
or set of rules, is there as the basis for any organism’s orientation
in space. It is called the optic array.
So far we do not need Kant’s revolutionary notions of the way the
mind structures experience. An ordered structure exists in the
environment. That however is only part of the explanation since the
organism musteithersleamthe rules which govern the transformations
of the optic array as either the organism or objects in the world move,
or else those rules must be built into the visual system. Either way,
there does have to be a mental ability to ‘read’ the optic array. At the
very minimum it is necessary to postulate some readiness on the part
of an organism to learn about the optic array. However even if it is
assumed that some satisfactory account can be given of this and that
the overall framework of perception is guaranteed by the coherent
structure of light, it still remains necessary to explain how it is that an
organism selects certain features of the environment for attention
rather than others. All the optic array tells us is that whatever we look
at will behave consistently with respect to our own movement through
space, and that if it moves we will be able to read its movement in a
consistent way. W hat it does not tell us is which detectable feature
within the enormous range of detectable features within any optic
array we should attend to. For that we need some other explanation.
apparent what the figure represents, that is you may not have the
necessary schema to hand that will enable you to read it. If I provide
you with the necessary schema by telling you that is a picture of a cow
you may now be able to read the picture. If its form still eludes you
look at Figure 2 and you will find an outline that will give you the
correct schema. It should now be possible to make sense of the
picture. The transformation that has occurred is a dramatic demon
stration of the mutability of vision and it bears close analysis.
First, the schema is a framework which enables us to select winch
fragments are to be pieced together and which are to be ignored.
Secondly, once the schema is established it is remarkably stable, so
much so that it is difficult if not impossible to look at the picture in the
way we did before the schema was formed. Thirdly and most
interestingly the change that occurred seemed to do so before one’s
eyes.
I say ‘before one’s eyes’ because that is the impression we had. We
know that the picture has not changed and yet there is a strong
impression that a change has taken place outside ourselves. The only
The Thinking Eye 21
Visual Literacy