False Memory
False Memory
1998,5(4), 710-715
Roediger and McDermott (1995)rejuvenated interest in Deese's (1959) paradigm for producing re-
liable intrusions and false alarms. Using this paradigm in three experiments, we demonstrated that
visual study presentation dramatically reduces the rate of false memories. Only auditory study pre-
sentation resulted in equal production of studied and critical items. Correct recall and recognition
were unaffected. The suggestion that visual presentation provides a means for discriminating be-
tween false and true memories was supported by Experiment 3: Pleasantness rating of study items
significantly reduced the creation of false memories regardless of modality.
False memories can be created reliably by presenting the literature supported our suspicions that visual pre-
lists of words that are all strong associates of a nonpre- sentation was the cause of this reduction.'
sented word. The strong associate usually is recalled All published reports of created memories obtained
and recognized at the same rate as that for presented with the Roediger and McDermott/Deese paradigm, with
items. This laboratory technique for invoking predictable one exception, have used an auditory list presentation
intrusions or false alarms has received considerable at- (Johnson et aI., 1997; Mather, Henkel, & Johnson, 1997;
tention in the literature following Roediger and McDer- McDermott, 1996; Norman & Schacter, 1997; Payne,
mott's (1995) replication and extension of Deese's (1959) Elie, Blackwell, & Neuschatz, 1996; Read, 1996; Schac-
work. ter et aI., 1996; Schacter, Verfaellie, & Pradere, 1996).
The experiments that we report here were motivated The one exception to the use of auditory presentation is
by an interest in the effects of item-specific processing of a study by Robinson and Roediger (1997) in which only
list words on recall of the critical, nonpresented associ- visual presentation was used. The rates of critical item
ates. Item-specific processing is assumed to affect a dis- intrusions were noticeably lower in this study than the
criminative process of distinctiveness at retrieval. Thus, rates in other published reports. For instance, Robinson
item-specific processing of list words may facilitate dis- and Roediger reported probabilities of false recall of.31
crimination of those items from nonpresented items and and .30 for 15-word lists as compared with a false recall
thereby reduce the level of false memory. We began this rate of .55 for 15-word lists reported by Roediger and
research by using a powerful item-specific study task McDermott (1995).
that requires subjects to report one thing that is different Although the primary goal of our paper is to report the
about an item from all other items in the list (Hunt & empirical finding of a modality effect on false memories,
Smith, 1996). Unfortunately, we were unable to evaluate we shall speculate about a possible explanation for this
the proposed role of distinctive processing because we effect. This speculation is grounded in the framework
failed to create false memories at a rate equivalent to true that explains memorial phenomena in terms of the inter-
memories in the control condition of that experiment, a action of distinctive and relational processing. Specifi-
condition comparable to the critical conditions ofRoedi- cally, we propose that visual presentation provides a bet-
ger and McDermott (1995). As it happened, we had in- ter means for discriminating between studied items and
advertently changed an apparently important aspect of the related critical items than does auditory presentation.
the experimental paradigm: modality oflist presentation. If this is the case, we should be able to enhance discrim-
Our items were presented via a computer monitor, and ination through some other mechanism even when audi-
the results showed unusually low levels of intrusions in tory presentation is used. In the third experiment, we in-
conditions that normally yield critical intrusions at the vestigated this possibility and obtained favorable results.
same rate as that for actually studied items. A search of
EXPERIMENT 1
We thank Robert Crowder, Arthur Glenberg, David Payne, and Henry In the first experiment, we used a modified version of
Roediger for helpful comments on a previous version of the manu- the Deese paradigm, in which the different lists of asso-
script. This research was supported in part by funds from the Research ciated words were presented blocked in one list followed
Council of the University of North Carolina at Greensboro granted to by a recall test. Given previous research showing that false
the second author. Correspondence may be addressed to R. E. Smith
or R. R. Hunt, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina
memories can be created at a rate equal to that of true
at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, 27412 (e-mail: [email protected]. memories using such a technique (McDermott, 1996), we
edu or [email protected]). expected to replicate the effect with auditory presenta-
tion. However, if presentation modality affects the dis- the visual condition [t(18) = 2.76]. The same trend was
crimination of presented and critical items, we should evident in the noncritical intrusions, but insufficient
not find equivalent production of the studied items and power was available to detect differences. The differ-
critical items in recall following visual presentation, and ences in intrusion rates were not due to shifts in criteria,
the production of critical lures should interact with modal- given the lack of difference between visual and auditory
ity of presentation. conditions on studied items [t(18) = -.72].
The interaction of modality and item type could occur
Method because visual study presentation reduces the likelihood
Subjects and Design. Twenty introductory psychology students par- of thinking of the critical item either at study or at test or
ticipated for credit. The between-subjects manipulation of study modal-
ity, visual or auditory, was combined with the within-subjects manip- because visual presentation increases the subjects' abil-
ulation of item type, which also had two levels: studied (items actually ity to discriminate items that they thought of at study
seen or heard at study) or critical (the high associates of the studied from those that had actually been presented. If the inter-
items that were not actually presented at study). action of modality and item type is solely due to a re-
Materials and Equipment. We randomly chose 10 critical items
from the lists presented in Roediger and McDermott (1995). The 5
duced likelihood of production of an item at retrieval,
highest associates of each critical item made up the 50-item study list. then the interaction of modality and item type should
The studied items were presented blocked during study, always going disappear when a recognition test is used. Alternatively,
from the Ist to the 5th associate. The words were read aloud by an ex- if recognition performance yields the same interaction,
perimenter or presented on a computer monitor. the result would implicate a contribution ofstudy modal-
Procedure. The experiment consisted of two phases: a study phase
and a test phase, separated by a brief period in which the subjects com- ity to the discrimination between critical and studied items.
pleted credit forms. Study presentation rate was one word/I.5 sec in
both the auditory and the visual conditions. At test, 3 min were allowed EXPERIMENT 2
for free recall, with the instructions that the subjects should be rea-
sonably sure that the words were in fact ones that they had either heard
or seen. The primary purpose of the second experiment was to
generalize the finding of a modality effect to the use of
Results and Discussion longer lists of associates and a recognition test. In order
The mean proportions of words produced on the free to do so while also making a direct comparison with pre-
recall test are shown in Table I as a function of modality vious reports of created false memories, we replicated Ex-
and item type. Modality had no reliable effect [F( 1,18) = periment 1 of Roediger and McDermott (1995) with the
1.42, MS e = .01], and the effect of item type [F(1, 18) = additional manipulation of modality. This design in-
20.84, MS e = .01] was qualified by a reliable interaction cludes separate free recall tests after each individual list
of the two variables [F(I,18) = 6.43, MS e = .01] (ex = presentation and a final recognition test. The use oflonger
.05). Individual comparisons indicated that critical and lists is important, because false alarm rates increase
studied item recall did not differ reliably following audi- when more items related to the lures are presented at
tory presentation [t(9) = -1.63], but following visual study (see, e.g., Robinson & Roediger, 1997). The inter-
presentation, studied items were twice as likely to be re- action of modality and item type in our first experiment
called as critical items [t(9) = -4.54]. Moreover, criti- could be an artifact of the relatively short lists of associ-
cal intrusions were reliably higher in the auditory than in ates used.
Table I
Recall and Recognition of Studied and Nonstudied Items
Item Type
False Alarms" Noncritical
Intrusionst
---
Experiment Task Modality M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Auditory .26 .10 .21 .10 2.30 2.67
Visual .29 .09 .11 .06 1.l0 .88
More plausible is the possibility that the critical lures The proportion of studied items correctly recalled was
come to mind during presentation of the study list, much examined first. The manipulations ofmodality [F( 1,76) =
in the spirit ofUnderwood's (1965) concept of implicit as- 1.49, MS e = .01] and study task [F(1,76) = 0.18, MS e =
sociative responses. In the absence of differential process- .01] did not influence the production of studied items,
ing ofthe studied and critical items, the critical items will nor was the interaction of the two variables significant
not be discriminated from studied items if they come to [F(1,76) = 1.59, MS e = .01]. Similarly, critical item re-
mind at test. Modality of study presentation provides one call was not affected reliably by the interaction of modal-
such dimension of differential processing. Visual process- ity and study task [F(1,76) = 0.45, MSe = .03]. In con-
ing differs from auditory processing, and on the speculative trast, both modality [F(1,76) = 11.26, MS e = .03] and
assumption that visual perception is less like thought than study task [F(1,76) = 7.21, MS e = .03] influenced the
is auditory perception, critical items would be more eas- likelihood of producing critical items on the recall test.
ily discriminated from list items following visual pre- Thus, while studied items were unaffected by our ma-
sentation than following auditory presentation of list nipulations, critical item production was influenced by
items. The implication is that other forms of differential modality of presentation and by the type of instructions
study processing should reduce the level of false recall. given to the subjects at encoding.
We tested his hypothesis in the third experiment, by com- The exact nature of this influence was investigated
paring the effects of a pleasantness rating orienting task with one-tailed independent t tests, which showed that
with standard instructions. critical item production was significantly decreased by
In the third experiment, we followed the same proce- visual presentation relative to auditory presentation for
dures as those of the first experiment, but with the addi- both the pleasantness rating [t(38) = 1.85] and standard
tion of a pleasantness rating study condition to both the encoding [t(38) = 2.93] groups. Likewise, pleasantness
auditory and visual conditions. Since pleasantness rat- rating decreased critical item production relative to stan-
ing requires consideration of the specific meaning of dard encoding, regardless of whether presentation was
each word, and since that meaning is different for each auditory [t(38) = 1.95] or visual [t(38) = 1.97]. Intru-
word, pleasantness rating encourages item-specific pro- sion of related distractors also was higher following au-
cessing ofa form that should be useful for discrimination ditory presentation with standard instructions than in
among items at retrieval. Thus, we predicted a lower other conditions, but again the small number of related
level of false recall following pleasantness rating of the and unrelated item intrusions precluded meaningful
study list than following standard instructions. analysis.
Finally, within-group comparisons contrasted critical
Method item production and studied item production. No reliable
Subjects and Design. Twenty volunteers, participating for course difference in recall of studied and critical items [t( 19) =
credit, were randomly assigned to each of four experimental groups re-
sulting from the manipulation of study modality (auditory, visual) and
0.75] occurred following auditory presentation with stan-
study task (standard encoding, pleasantness rating). dard encoding instructions. In contrast, both the visual
Materials and Equipment. The word lists from Experiment 2 were presentation pleasantness rating group [t(19) = -7.04]
either shown on slides or read aloud by an experimenter. and the visual presentation standard encoding group
Procedure. The procedure matched that of Experiment I, except for
[t( 19) = -4.35] recalled reliably more studied items
the additional instructional manipulation and an increase in presenta-
tion time to 2 sec. Half of the subjects were given intentional study in- than critical items. Collapsing over modality, critical
structions and the other half were given incidental instructions. The items were less likely to be produced than studied items
incidental instructions requested that each word be rated for pleasant- following pleasantness rating than following standard in-
ness on a scale of I = unpleasant to 5 = pleasant. Recall instructions structions [t(19) = -2.73].
were the same as in Experiment I for all conditions.
Results Discussion
The mean proportions of words produced on the free The results of Experiment 3 replicated the modality effect of Ex-
periments I and 2. In comparison with auditory presentation, visual
recall test are shown in Table I as a function of modality, presentation significantly decreased the likelihood of producing criti-
study task, and item type. The reliable effects of modality cal items on a recall test. Furthermore, subjects who performed a pleas-
[F(1,76) = 6.00, MSe = .02], study task [F(1,76) = 4.78, antness rating task at study were less likely to produce critical items on
MSe = .02], and item type [F(1,76) = 34.15, MSe = .02] the recall test than subjects who intentionally tried to remember the
words. Only the control group, auditory presentation with standard in-
were qualified by reliable interactions of modality and
structions, produced critical items at a rate that was equivalent to the
item type [F(1, 76) = 13.52, MSe = .02] and of study task rate of correct recall of studied items. Both visual presentation and the
and item type [F(I,76) = 7.29, MSe = .02]. Neither the pleasantness rating task resulted in a critical intrusion rate below the
interaction between modality and study task [F(1,76) = rate of studied item production.
.0 I, MSe = .02] nor the three-way interaction [F(1, 76) = The lack of an effect of pleasantness rating on study list items
should not be seen as inconsistent with the enormous literature show-
1.39, MSe = .02] was significant. Separate analyses were ing benefits of semantic orienting tasks. Note that the comparisons in
conducted for the two item types to investigate the sig- this experiment were between pleasantness rating and intentional
nificant interactions. memory instructions. Previous research has found comparable perfor-
714 SMITH AND HUNT
mance following these two study conditions (e.g., Hyde & Jenkins, However, one result reported by Israel and Schacter directly contra-
1969). dicts our data. Subjects in a control condition were given visual pre-
sentation of words, and the false memory effect was not reduced. Im-
GENERAL DISCUSSION portantly, visual presentation was accompanied by simultaneous
auditory presentation. Auditory presentation may have truncated visual
processing of the word analogous to the reduction of sensory process-
The three experiments reported here demonstrate the existence and ing in the generation paradigm when semantic context is provided
reliability ofa modality effect on false memory in the Roediger and Me- (Blaxton, 1988; Jacoby, 1983). Pictures, with their more elaborate sen-
Dermott (1995) paradigm. In comparison with auditory presentation, sory processing requirements, would be immune to the influence ofsi-
visual presentation of study items reduces the level of false memory.
multaneous auditory presentation.
The fact that the modality effect occurs in recognition suggests that In summary, the Roediger and McDermott (1995) paradigm is de-
false memory is not just a product of associative activation at retrieval
signed to bring critical nonpresented items to mind at study. The cues
in recall, but rather that the critical item probably comes to mind at
provided by free recall instructions are then as likely to reinstate the
study as well as at test (Roediger & McDermott, 1995; Underwood,
critical items as the presented items unless some basis for differentia-
1965). If the critical item does come to mind at study, it then is part of
tion occurs at study. The existence of a modality effect suggests that
the encoding experience, much as categorical information is part of the
even subtle differences in processing of critical and presented items at
experience of encoding a block of category instances. Thus, mere pres-
study can service the distinctive processing in retrieval that is neces-
ence on the study list is not sufficient for subjects to discriminate be-
sary to reduce false memory.
tween a list item and a critical lure. Successful discrimination requires
processing along some dimension that differentiates the study item
from the critical item, and speculatively, visual processing of the study REFERENCES
items provides a better basis for differentiation than does auditory pre-
sentation. In support of this general idea, the results of Experiment 3 BLAXTON, T. A. (1988). Investigating dissociations among memory
indicate that other dimensions of differential processing-in this case, measures: Support for the transfer appropriate processing frame-
pleasantness rating of the study list-also will reduce the rate offalse work. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning. Memory, &
memory. Cognition, 15,657-668.
Our interpretation of the data derives from the more general view of BLAXTON, T. A. (1996). Distinguishing false from true in human mem-
memory as determined by the processing of similarity and differences ory. Neuron, 17,191-194.
among elements of an event (Hunt & McDaniel, 1993). In retrieval, DEESE, J. (1959). On prediction of occurrence of particular verbal in-
differences are presumed to be important for discriminating among el- trusions in immediate recall. Journal ofExperimental Psychology,
ements of an event, because the event is defined by similarity among 58, 17-22.
the elements. For example, critical items and studied items in this par- HASHTROUDI, S., FERGUSON, S. A., RAPPOLD, V. A., & CHROSNIAK,
adigm share the property of having been experienced at encoding, and L. D. (1988). Data-driven and conceptually driven processes in partial-
therefore, reinstating the similarity processing with the cue "Recall the word identification and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psy-
study items" is insufficient for distinguishing critical and studied chology: Learning, Memory. & Cognition, 14, 749-757.
items. Differential processing of the items at study facilitates discrim- HUNT, R R, & McDANIEL, M. A. (1993). The enigma of organization
ination at retrieval and reduces the recall and recognition of critical and distinctiveness. Journal ofMemory & Language, 32, 421-445.
items. HUNT, R. R., & SMITH, R E. (1996). Accessing the particular from the
Existing evidence is congruent with various aspects of our interpre- general: The power of distinctiveness in the context of organization.
tation. The assumption that visually driven processing is more dis- Memory & Cognition, 24, 217-225.
criminable from thought than is auditorially driven processing has not HYDE, T. S., & JENKINS, J. J. (1969). Differential effects of incidental
been directly tested in the reality monitoring literature, but Johnson, tasks on the organization of recall of a list of highly associated
Nolde, and De Leonardis (1996) have reported that source monitoring words. Journal ofExperimental Psychology, 82, 472-481.
is more highly correlated with visual detail than with auditory detail in ISRAEL, L., & SCHACTER, D. L. (1997). Pictorial encoding reduces false
the original event. Notice, however, that our assumption is that modal- recognition of semantic associates. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,
ity of presentation will be useful only to the extent that it differentiates 4,577-581.
among items. In the Roediger and McDermott (1995) paradigm, we JACOBY, L. L. (1983). Remembering the data: Analyzing interactive
would not predict a modality effect on studied items. This prediction processes in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behav-
is not only consistent with our results but also with those of Hash- ior, 22,485-508.
troudi, Ferguson, Rappold, and Chrosniak (1988), who found that au- JOHNSON, M. K., NOLDE, S. E, & DE LEONARDIS, D. M. (1996). Emo-
ditory study increased false alarms on a recognition test without influ- tional focus and source monitoring. Journal of Memory & Lan-
encing hits. guage,35,135-156.
Our assumption about the importance of processing similarities and JOHNSON, M. K., NOLDE, S. E, MATHER, M., KOUNIOS, J., SCHACTER,
differences among items is also consistent with existing evidence. Me- D. L., & CURRAN, T. (1997). The similarity of brain activity associ-
Dermott (1996) and Mather et al. (1997) found increased levels of crit- ated with true and false recognition memory depends on test format.
ical item recall with blocked list presentation relative to random list Psychological Science, 8, 250-257.
presentation at study. Blocked presentation encourages relational pro- MATHER, M., HENKEL, L. A., & JOHNSON, M. K. (1997). Evaluating
cessing, and in this case the relationship among items is the critical characteristics of false memories: Remember/know judgments and
item. Therefore this item is brought to mind more readily than with ran- memory characteristics questionnaire compared. Memory & Cogni-
dom presentation. tion, 25, 826-837.
Read (1996) reported that instructions to remember the order of pre- McDERMOTT, K. B. (1996). The persistence of false memories in list
sentation reduced the recall of critical items. Because information cor- recall. Journal ofMemory & Language, 35, 212-230.
responding to serial order is different for each item, successful serial NAIRNE, J. S., NEATH, I., SERRA, M., & BYUN, E. (1997). Positional dis-
recall has been attributed to distinctive processing (Nairne, Neath, Serra, tinctiveness and the ratio rule in free recall. Journal of Memory &
& Byun, 1997). Thus, Read's data are consistent with the importance Language,37,155-166.
of distinctive processing in reducing false recall. NELSON, D. L., REED, V. S., & McEvoy, C. L. (1977). Learning to order
Israel and Schacter (1997) discovered that false memory is less likely pictures and words: A model of sensory and semantic encoding.
ifthe study materials are pictures rather than words. Nelson, Reed, and Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory,
McEvoy (1977) argued that the picture superiority effect in memory is 3,484-497.
due to more elaborate sensory processing required by pictures, which NORMAN, K. A., & SCHACTER. D. L. (1997). False recognition in younger
in turn enhances the distinctiveness of pictures at retrieval. Thus, Israel and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories.
and Schacters data can be seen as congruent with our interpretation. Memory & Cognition, 25, 838-848.
FALSE MEMORIES AND STUDY MODALITY 715
PAYNE, D. G., ELIE, c.J., BLACKWELL, J. M., & NEUSCHATZ,J. S. (1996). SCHACTER, D. L., VERFAELLIE, M., & PRADERE, D. (1996). The neuro-
Memory illusions: Recalling, recognizing, and recollecting events psychology of memory illusions: False recall and recognition in am-
that never occurred. Journal of Memory & Language, 35, 261-285. nesic patients. Journal ofMemory & Language, 35, 319-334.
READ, J. D. (1996). From a passing thought to a false memory in 2 min- UNDERWOOD, B. J. (1965). False recognition produced by implicit ver-
utes: Confusing real and illusory events. Psychonomic Bulletin & bal responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70,122-129.
Review, 3, 105-111.
ROBINSON, K. J., & ROEDIGER, H. L., III (1997). Associative processes in NOTE
false recall and false recognition. Psychological Science, 8, 231-237.
ROEDIGER, H. L., III, & McDERMOTT, K. B. (1995). Creating false I. Blaxton (1996) compared several neuroimaging studies and de-
memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal ofEx- tected a pattern suggestive of the importance of modality of presenta-
perimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 803-814. tion.
SCHACTER, D. L., REIMAN, E., CURRAN, T., YUN, L. S., BANDY, D., Mc-
DERMOTT, K. B., & ROEDIGER, H. L., III (1996). Neuroanatomical
correlates of veridical and illusory recognition memory: Evidence (Manuscript received October 14, 1997;
from positron emission tomography. Neuron, 17, 267-274. revision accepted for publication February 24, 1998.)