Hahn, A., Cohen, M. Cap. Mujeres Golpeadas PDF
Hahn, A., Cohen, M. Cap. Mujeres Golpeadas PDF
Edited by
K AR N AC
ISBN: 978–1–78220–434–3
www.karnacbooks.com
Introduction
Margaret Cohen & Alberto Hahn 1
3 Point–line–surface–space:
on Donald Meltzer’s concept of one- and
two-dimensional mental functioning in autistic states
Suzanne Maiello 35
4 Autism reconsidered
A response
Jeffrey L. Eaton 75
11 On having ideas:
the aesthetic object and O
Meg Harris Williams 166
12 Degrees of entrapment:
living and dying in the claustrum
Pamela B. Sorensen 176
Concluding thoughts
on the nature of psychoanalytic activity
Alberto Hahn 233
references 237
index 249
S
ince it was founded in 1920, the Tavistock Clinic has developed
a wide range of developmental approaches to mental health
which have been strongly influenced by the ideas of psycho
analysis. It has also adopted systemic family therapy as a theoretical
model and a clinical approach to family problems. The Clinic is now
the largest training institution in Britain for mental health, provid-
ing postgraduate and qualifying courses in social work, psychology,
psychiatry, and child, adolescent, and adult psychotherapy, as well
as in nursing and primary care. It trains about 1,700 students each
year in over 60 courses.
The Clinic’s philosophy aims at promoting therapeutic methods
in mental health. Its work is based on the clinical expertise that is
also the basis of its consultancy and research activities. The aim of
this Series is to make available to the reading public the clinical,
theoretical, and research work that is most influential at the Tavistock
Clinic. The Series sets out new approaches in the understanding and
treatment of psychological disturbance in children, adolescents, and
adults, both as individuals and in families.
It is a kind of publishers’ lore that collections of conference
papers seldom make a good book. Doing Things Differently: The Influ-
ence of Donald Meltzer on Psychoanalytic Theory and Practice, however,
is in itself “different”. As this conference proceeded and with no
ix
I
n February 2015, the Donald Meltzer Development Fund organized
an international conference at the Tavistock Clinic in London with
the attendance of 120 participants. This conference was the response
to a frequently expressed wish for a reunion in which Meltzer’s ideas,
teachings, and clinical insights could be pored over, where exchanges
could be—as indeed they were—stimulating, and which could provide
a place in which his work could be celebrated among colleagues from
all over the world.
In organizing this conference, our motivations were several. We
wanted to meet up with old friends and colleagues, but we were also
aware that, whereas when we were training, Meltzer was a lively force
in our thinking, people training now receive his teaching because it is
implicit in so many of the older child psychotherapists’ backgrounds.
Furthermore, we realized that many of the papers came from people
who had been supervised or taught by him.
Although there is systematic teaching of Meltzer’s thought at the
Tavistock Clinic, there is only an occasional mention of his work at the
British Psychoanalytical Society, with no systematic reading. One of
our motivations was therefore to set up an event that would interest
the trainees and younger members of our profession. Also, from the
beginning we wanted to continue the work of harvesting the rich crop
that has come from his teaching and written work, and the papers
Margaret Rustin
T
he title of this chapter is intended to draw attention to aspects
of Donald Meltzer’s ways of working which characterized his
practice as a psychoanalyst and which, I think, are important
in appreciating his originality. Of course, such observations arise from
one’s own particular perspective and may not be in accord with the
recollections or understanding of others, and it is obvious that doing
things differently—which I am interpreting, in part, as Meltzer’s char-
acteristic commitment to doing things in his own way—means that
there will be conflicting views about whether such differences have a
good outcome. This chapter is not going to address the institutional
conflicts that were part of the historical picture—in fact, I am sure that
I am quite ignorant of much of this history. Instead, I hope to describe
things that I have observed both in the years of some personal contact
with Meltzer and in reading his books and papers over time, things
that have struck me as enlightening and interesting, or sometimes
maddening and frustrating features of his work, and which arise from
his personal style as a writer and analyst. Perhaps, also, I am going to
be doing something rather different from other writers who address
his ideas, since their focus is more usually on his clinical contributions.
The themes I want to follow are these: the central relevance of child
analysis and the unity of child and adult analysis; the value of work-
ing in a clinical group with colleagues; the revision of psychoanalytic
sented in clinical detail, and the chapter that describes the process in
an individual session with a 4½-year-old girl seen in analysis from the
age of 3 is a very powerful example of Meltzer’s approach. The case
was presented for detailed discussion in a seminar of child analysts
and psychotherapists (reminding us, incidentally, of how closely these
two groups worked together at that time), and the clinical material
is presented in a carefully descriptive form, recording all the child’s
activities, verbalizations, facial expressions and so on, but, strikingly,
including nothing at all about the countertransference impact on the
analyst. Indeed, Meltzer is explicit in stating that he “leaves problems
of countertransference aside as private to the supervisee”. I think
this method, even though it is clearly different from contemporary
practice, where a group discussion of the clinician’s feelings about the
patient, and indeed contributions from other group members about
their own emotional reaction, would be expected, draws attention to a
fundamental idea underlying Meltzer’s conception of psychoanalysis.
This is that if the therapist is truly to be working at the appropriate
depth to deal with the infantile transference, an analytic process also
has to be ongoing for him or her, either in the form of continuing
personal analysis or in the self-analysis that has to be the outcome
of a good-enough analytic experience. The model of training as a
psychoanalytic therapist which is based on personal analysis, clinical
experience, and intensive supervision is thus seen also as the neces-
sary cornerstone of an ongoing professional life. I think it is important
to remind ourselves of this, because although professional bodies now
demand evidence of continuing professional development (CPD) as
part of continuing fitness to practise, the question of what this really
requires of us is a serious one. The tremendous intensity of the training
years is often seen as exhaustingly demanding, something from which
people can crave a rest even if they are simultaneously aware of how
much the quality of their clinical work is linked to that intensity. The
Psychoanalytical Process makes a strong case that this level of engage-
ment is integral to the capacity to continue to work psychoanalytically.
Meltzer uses the clinical material presented to clarify what he
thought actually constituted psychoanalysis. From the patient’s per-
spective, it is a question of the evolution of the unconscious in the
context of the availability of a transference relationship. From the
analyst, the contribution required is the provision and maintenance
of the setting (that is, the psychoanalytic setting in the sense just dis-
cussed about the dedicated state of mind of the therapist in relation
to the task) and a steady commitment to “working through”, Freud’s
ing project to explore whether the ideas they proffer about family
functioning might map onto the totally different discourses of family
therapy in some ways, or whether the psychoanalytic recognition of
the centrality of the unconscious makes for quite divergent perspec-
tives. One could make a similar suggestion about the more recent but
psychoanalytically much closer discipline of couple psychotherapy.
Has this benefited from this earlier work? In James Fisher’s writing
about work with couples, we have an excellent example of the poten-
tial for imaginative integration (Fisher, 1999).
There is not space here for a full presentation of their typology of
family cultures, but to remind us or whet the appetites of those unfa-
miliar with the report, I will pick out some examples. The structures
described are being tested against the crucial functions that Meltzer
and Harris ascribe to the family. These are set out as pairs:
not cope with Adam, whereas he could come in and calm things down.
Mother’s difficult family background, in contrast to his own, was sug-
gested as the reason for her vulnerability to becoming overwhelmed,
and she agreed with this. Gradually, this somewhat grandiose pater-
nalistic stance—the patriarchal family, as described by Meltzer and
Harris, including the harshly scathing and belittling scolding of Adam,
and the denigration of his wife—began to show cracks. The criticism of
the other adults in Adam’s life who were not felt to be good enough at
their jobs—including his teacher, cricket coach, and babysitter—gave
way to a realization that these others were offering the boy a great
deal of support and that the view of things that only he as the father
of the family really knew what was what and could be relied upon
was a distorted one. At this point the work with the couple felt quite
different, in that both now seemed more on a level with each other
and both were able to voice their worries and despondency about their
frequent failures to contain the children. As the unbalanced dynamic
between mother and father shifted, it was also interesting to note that
the younger sister began to feature differently, not only as the victim
of her brother’s aggression and greed for space, but as having her own
difficulties in being so easily able to set him up to be in the wrong.
The transference manifestation of all this was an initial idealiza-
tion of their therapist in which he was seen as possessing something
akin to patriarchal authority and omnipotence. I think he was prob-
ably also secretly feared for the harsh criticism that was forestalled
by concealing the extent of their helplessness and rage in the face of
Adam’s distress. Later came some of the secret denigration, behind a
polite exterior, of anything offered by anyone else, and a difficulty in
attending sessions regularly. However, it was possible for the therapist
to speak to the couple about the impact it had when there were longer
periods of time between sessions, whether planned or unplanned,
and for them to see that these tended to coincide with an escalation
of trouble in the family. This awareness that these meetings had some
meaning for them led to the shift I have briefly described. The couple
of parent worker and child therapist—not always an easy one to hold
together—can, of course, be a potent representation of the couple func-
tioning to which one hopes parents may aspire, and this was both an
area of potential splitting and of its modification in this case.
There are natural connections between the work on family func-
tioning and Meltzer’s major study, “Sincerity” (1971), in which he uses
three of Harold Pinter’s early plays to tackle the topic of truthful emo-
tionality and its perversions. As some readers will know, I too have
Lucy, the girl described to me, spoke one day of how she had had
an individual music lesson at the end of the day, thus managing
to avoid a disliked final class lesson. The music department was
in the basement of her school, but her form room was right at the
top, so in preparing to leave school and come to her session she
had to go up to collect her belongings. On the way to the tube
station, she realized she had forgotten her purse and Oyster card,
so she ran back to look for them. She located the card, though not
the purse, and then hurried back to the station, managing to arrive
only seven minutes late for her session. The time was filled with
complaints and real misery about how hungry and thirsty she was,
because she had had no time or money to buy her usual snack.
Her dry mouth and her empty tummy filled her mind, and she
made her discomfort seem absolutely visceral. Her therapist felt
it was really hard not to offer at least a drink of water. This was a
post-weekend session not long after a holiday break, so the thera-
pist spoke to her about how painful it was to find that what she
wanted was not within her reach. This led to more details about
the lost purse. Lucy wondered at first about it having been stolen,
but because they had previously been able to work on her paranoid
fear of things being stolen from her, which in reality were things
she herself had lost, her main idea was that she had left it in the
lunch room. She then explained that she had chosen the” extra”
choice at lunch, one not covered by the already-paid-for dish of
the day, and in fact she found she hated it and therefore hardly
touched it, hence now being terribly hungry, and hence having had
to take out her purse to pay for it. “If only”, she said, in an infinite
regress, “if only I had had the ordinary lunch, I wouldn’t have lost
my purse, I wouldn’t have been late in arriving here”, and so on.
More associations about things she loses or damages followed this.
ness of her efforts to escape reality, both internal and external, became
starkly visible in her recognition as the session proceeded that the ease
of not having to get up in the morning and go to school and instead
to stay luxuriously at home in bed also meant that she was bored and
lonely, missing her friends, and dying to get to school tomorrow, just
as she had been very keen to get to her session despite exceptionally
nasty weather. She was, in fact, both provocative and persuasive in
her delighted seductive tricks to ensure that she continue to occupy a
princess position, and her therapist struggled to resist colluding with
this. Her sessions in earlier years were absolutely filled with princesses
and their amazing wardrobes and the undoubted superiority of vari-
ous imaginary realms. At times it was tempting to shoot her down,
and both therapist and supervisor could feel this desire to puncture
Lucy’s narcissism harshly. But giving in to this pressure would doubt-
less have served to make her redouble her efforts to protect herself
from a very cruel and superior superego, of which we got an occa-
sional glimpse in her extreme anxiety about being in quite ordinary
trouble with adults.
Now, to return to the 1970 paper about mental space: it is fascinat-
ing to find that Meltzer’s final example is from architecture itself, in
the form of an architect patient’s description of his planned building.
This vignette provides us with a glimpse into the workings of Melt-
zer’s clinical imagination: he sees the building the patient describes
as showing him the shape of his patient’s inner world, and simultane-
ously he alerts us, his readers, to view the buildings of our external
world anew. He reminds us that a sensitive appreciation of both natu-
ral and man-made worlds, as of the riches of art and literature, is what
makes for an imaginative response to the language of the unconscious.
While a grasp of theory and technique is needed for good analytic
work, it is surely clear that psychoanalytic education should embrace
a serious engagement with much that is studied in other disciplines,
particularly the arts and humanities, if it is to flourish.
Meltzer’s interest in so many spheres of human endeavour is, of
course, linked to his enjoyment of collaborative work, including his
work with non-clinicians. One of the ideas implicit in his model of
atelier training was that an atelier could have more porous boundaries
than the clinical trainings within professional training bodies and thus
could benefit from freer intellectual interdisciplinary dialogue, as in
the Imago group I mentioned earlier. Certainly, it was explicit that he
favoured non-traditional scientific backgrounds as a good basis for
psychoanalytic training.
work unless one can see, in the mind’s eye, the baby put down after
a feed? How to make sense of the existential anxiety and confusion
of identity stirred by the awareness of there being more than just one
patient unless we think of what we learn in infant observation about
the terrifying loss of identity involved for a young child when a new
baby appears? “Who am I if I am not mother’s baby?” is a shatter-
ing question. Meltzer’s sensitivity to these early terrors, which were
so much at the heart of his close collaboration with Esther Bick, has
influenced technique greatly and engenders the capacity for analytic
kindness in the face of the depth of infantile dependence that is evoked
by the transference relationship (Meltzer, 1960).
Alongside these broad themes is the interest in dimensionality
and its special importance in working with autistic children and
autistic phenomena. The particular early development seen, I believe,
in autistic children with their vulnerability to too-early depressive
anxiety (too early for them to manage, that is) is also linked with his
later revised model of child development and the introduction of the
concept of aesthetic conflict. Here he suggests a pre–paranoid-schizoid
phase in which the pain of the early encounter with mother is what
pushes the infant into a more schizoid state as a defence against the
too-muchness of the beginning of life outside the womb (Meltzer &
Harris Williams, 1988).
I think we do see some babies in infant observation or everyday
life of whom this description rings true, and we certainly encounter
similar moments in clinical work, but I would suggest that the range of
states of being and states of mind at the start of life is quite consider-
able. One account of the beginnings of life is not likely to encompass
the complexity we might reasonably expect.
The distinction between different forms of identification—projec-
tive, introjective, intrusive, adhesive—to which Meltzer devoted much
attention remains vital, and this takes us to the investigation of claus-
trophobic anxieties and the phenomena of the claustrum (Meltzer,
1992a). In supervision of child psychotherapy with the many quite
disturbed children now seen in clinics, this is, I believe, alongside the
idea of the “gathering” of the transference, the single most useful of
Meltzer’s concepts. I find myself explaining his theory to bewildered
therapists who are suffering the experience of being with a child who
seems inside something while they remain irrelevant outsiders. His
technical suggestions about how to find a position from which one
can make useful observations, muse aloud, make contact with a child
who feels inside his or her object have often proved useful. Of similar
Note
This chapter is based on a version previously published in the Journal of Child
Psychotherapy, Vol. 42 (No. 1, 2016), pp. 4–17. Copyright © Association of Child
Psychotherapists, reprinted by permission of Taylor & Francis Ltd, http://www.
tandfonline.com on behalf of the Association of Child Psychotherapists.
Maria Rhode
M
y aim in this chapter is to explore the bearing of Donald
Meltzer’s concept of nipple-penis confusion, firstly on selec-
tive mutism, and secondly on the articulation of words. I
suggest that this concept has great explanatory power, as it seems to
be capable of subsuming phenomena that Meltzer (1986a) described
in connection with the Theatre of the Mouth as well as the traumatic
experiences that can be implicated in selective mutism. It also provides
a framework for linking these two areas to the child’s character.
I begin by outlining the concept of nipple-penis confusion and then
refer to Meltzer’s proposed conditions for language development,
supplementing this by discussing the child’s ability to take psycho-
logical ownership of the organs of the mouth. This is necessary for the
production of speech and is a process that Frances Tustin’s work on
autistic children’s experience of the mouth allows us to understand to
some extent. I shall distinguish different ways in which the paternal
part-object can be implicated in traumatic experiences, as these often
seem to be the trigger for selective mutism, and contrast various
identifications that seem to influence how children may respond to
such experiences. I shall also suggest that it may be useful to extend
some of Meltzer’s formulations on the motives in play in nipple-penis
confusion by considering its manifestations on these more primitive
levels of trauma and adhesive identification.
21
of internal objects, rendering them separate and silent” (p. 205). I sug-
gest later that the concept of nipple-penis confusion provides a way
of approaching this issue of “the interference with verbal coition” on
the concrete level of the organs involved.
Some children understand language well and can read and write
fluently but do not feel that the organs of their mouth are theirs to
use. Morton Gernsbacher (2005) has called this “oral dyspraxia”. For
example, Tito Mukhopadhyay, beginning when he was eight (Muk-
hopadhyay, 2000), has written a series of books in which he describes
in sensitive, lyrical detail both his relationship to other people and
his imaginative construction of the world. And yet he continued to
present as a classical severe autistic who never really acquired a voice.
Tellingly, one of his books is called How Can I Talk If My Lips Don’t
Move? (Mukhopadhyay, 2008).
Selective mutes are capable of “moving their lips” in specific situa-
tions and in the presence of particular people, but are quite incapable
of doing so with others. For this reason, they provide the perfect exam-
ple of the importance of strictly emotional factors in the production
of language. Non-analytic authors have described selective mutism
as a social phobia and have recommended a gradual and progressive
de-sensitization strategy. A traumatic incident can often be the trig-
gering factor, as it was with many victims of “shell-shock” in the First
World War. Judith Trowell and Israel Kolvin (1995) have carried out an
unpublished retrospective study in which adults who had been selec-
tively mute as children were interviewed and assessed on a variety
of instruments. These adults ranged from those functioning normally,
through those with a neurotic character structure, to a few who were
schizophrenic: clearly, selective mutism in childhood does not imply
a single pathway (though Jeanne Magagna, 2012a, 2012b, has sug-
gested that early problems with language development always point
to an impaired “bridge” between child and parents). But every one
of the interviewees stressed how overwhelmingly helpless they had
felt as children—a very different perspective from that of the adults
surrounding selective mutes, who tend to feel helpless themselves
and to see the child as controlling, withholding, and extremely power-
ful. Writing from a psychoanalytic perspective, Weininger (1992) has
emphasized selectively mute children’s fear of their own aggression,
particularly on the anal level, while Staehle (2007) has stressed the
importance of traumatic separation and Truckle (2006) that of trauma
more generally.3 My own anecdotal impression is that these children
often show the same intense, primitive bodily anxieties that we meet
I understood the fact that there was no one outside the house as point-
ing to his own identification with his dead father, who should have
been inside. As far as his mutism was concerned, we could say that
his voiceless or tongueless mouth was identified with a mother-house
that contained no nipple-penis, and that Harry was frightened that it
was his activity and initiative that had removed the nipple-penis from
the breast. Again like Ricardo, this child was transformed when a man
came into his mother’s life.
I want to repeat that neither of these boys seemed to be relating
to the nipple-penis in a way that was hostile to the maternal breast-
object, and that both showed a considerable degree of depressive feel-
ing.4 This is likely to be important in terms of who it is that the child
predominantly identifies with—whether with the damaged parental
figure or, like Anthony, with the traumatizing giant. I shall return to
this question later.
The nipple-penis
and problems surrounding articulation
So far, I have tried to explore what the concept of the nipple-penis
may contribute to our understanding of selectively mute children
in relation to phantasies concerning the possession of the tongue
or the voice. I will now discuss several consecutive sessions with
6-year-old Andrew in order to trace Meltzer’s proposed sequence of
nipple=penis=faeces in connection with Andrew’s capacity for correct,
articulated pronunciation.
made him feel worried. “Broken,” he said, “I broke it” (notice the
correct syntax and pronunciation). I agreed that he had, though I
repeated that I thought he hadn’t meant to and had been shocked
when it happened.
Andrew now fitted his mouth around the rim of the lamp, and
produced considerable quantities of saliva that spread over the
lamp’s surface. Then he repeated, “broken” and fiddled with his
mouth, his chin, and particularly his tongue. I said that he was
worried about breaking the bulb inside the lamp, and that now, if
his mouth wasn’t joined up to the lamp or to me by means of his
own spit, he seemed to feel that his tongue and chin were broken
too, and it was hard to speak properly. He responded with some
more fingering of his chin and the organs of his mouth.
He then curled up on the couch and whispered “Mummy” in a
tiny baby voice, pretending to be a baby who was crying. I said
that he was showing me a baby who was missing his mummy and
was feeling very sad. Towards the end of the session, he became
absorbed in switching the lights off and being able to switch
them back on, saying, “Dark”. I said how important it was to feel
he could stop things being dark inside me. In a later session, he
fingered his mouth while doing this, and I said that he needed to
make the light come back on: that if my eyes seemed dark, perhaps
it made him feel his mouth was broken.
Concluding remarks
To sum up my argument: I have tried to illustrate the power of Melt-
zer’s nipple-penis confusion as a conceptual tool in the attempt to
understand two cases of selective mutism as well as the way in which
the clarity of one child’s pronunciation varied with his state of mind.6
Meltzer’s original formulation applied, I suggest, to a level of experi-
ence where weaning might be a source of pain that the child rebelled
against, with various characterological consequences. In contrast, the
children I discuss seem to be preoccupied with the loss of the nipple
as an existential catastrophe, as delineated by Frances Tustin, and I
think that it is useful to extend Meltzer’s concept so that it includes
this adhesive level. This allows concrete experiences in the mouth to
be linked with such ideas as “the verbal coition of internal objects”
and to be situated within the context of the child’s developing char-
acter. I have also proposed that the concept of the nipple-penis can
encompass the fact that trauma of one kind or another, which is often
a trigger of selective mutism, can be construed as being mediated by
the paternal function.7
The rivalrous appropriation of the nipple-penis (as Meltzer
described it and as shown in Andrew’s material) can be contrasted
with the identification with damaged parental figures (as with Ricardo
and Harry). The question of whether the child identifies with a dam-
aged parent or with a phallic aggressor appeared to be linked in two
autistic children with the degree to which of the mother’s own internal
father was supportive and could serve as a “buttress” (S. Klein, 1980;
Houzel, 2001) to the maternal function. This may well apply to differ-
ing identifications in selectively mute children.
Juliet Mitchell, who has stressed the identification with a dead
mother figure in mute, shell-shocked soldiers of the First World War
(Mitchell, 1996), has also reminded us that the hysterical aphonia with
which Freud’s patient Dora came to treatment is often overlooked
(Mitchell, 1998). She points out that it returned temporarily after the
end of treatment and suggests that Dora was identified with a dead
father who was therefore mute: “in Dora’s dream, it is her mother who
writes to Dora in Dora’s dream-absence, telling her daughter that she
can come home, as her father is dead” (p. 129). In fact, Dora’s second
episode of aphonia came on after she had seen Herr K knocked over
by a carriage—an obvious parallel to Ricardo. This implies, I think,
that descriptions on the level of the nipple-penis can be relevant to
patients such as hysterics whose functioning is apparently much more
advanced than that of many children with selective mutism. Again, it
is the degree to which Meltzer’s concept is capable of unifying many
different phenomena, conditions, and levels of experience that makes
it so useful.
Notes
1. Klein thought that premature erotization led to a harmful confusion
between oral and genital experience and, indeed, to confusion more generally
(Klein, 1957, pp. 195, 197–198). A body of literature exists on the relationship
between the male and female elements of the primitive combined object, which
falls beyond the scope of this chapter. This literature includes S. Klein’s suggestion
that the baby first experiences the masculine element as a necessary support to the
feminine element before it comes to be seen as an intrusion into the mother–baby
couple (S. Klein, 1980). Similarly, Houzel (2001) has emphasized the necessary
“buttressing” function of the masculine nipple; he differentiates the “nipple link”,
which he thinks is operative “proximally”, from the “penis link”, which operates
over longer distances. Birksted-Breen (1996) distinguishes between the narcissistic
phallus and the “penis as link”, which provides triangulation and promotes the
structuring of mental space, while I have suggested (Rhode, 2000) that primal,
essential splitting can be interfered with when it is confused with damage to the
developmentally necessary early conjunction of male and female elements.
2. Lubbe (2011) has posted a helpful review of Kleinian and post-Kleinian
theories of sexuality.
3. Since the paper on which this chapter is based was first written, Pozzi
Monzo, Micotti, & Rashid (2015) have published an account of selective mutism
that includes individual and family approaches as well as a literature review.
4. In contrast, I have heard from a colleague about a girl who seemed con-
cerned to hide her capacity for speech as well as her interest in sexual mat-
ters—themes that appeared together in her sessions—as though trying to avoid
retaliation by her female therapist, who was also pointedly excluded from the
child’s close relationship with her father.
5. This is a feature that I have seen in a number of children; Geneviève Haag
(2004) has called it “démutisation en voyelles” [demutization in vowels].
6. Carlos Tamm (2014) has recently documented the parallel between the
capacity of two boys with autistic or psychotic features to use language correctly
and the nature of their “mental landscape”.
7. It can also encompass cases in which words are felt to be produced by
objects inside the mother (by babies as well as by a father), whose existence the
child wishes to suppress by magical means (Rhode, 2013): these are beyond the
scope of this chapter.
Point–line–surface–space:
on Donald Meltzer’s concept of
one- and two-dimensional mental
functioning in autistic states
Suzanne Maiello
“In the early days, there was great pleasure in doing what my
teachers taught me to do and finding out that they were right. . . .
But then . . . there comes a time when you cast off from the pier
and into the open sea and are on your own . . .”
Donald Meltzer, “A Review of My Writings” (2000)
T
owards the end of the nineteenth century, Edwin A. Abbott
published a satirical narrative with the title Flatland—A Romance
of Many Dimensions (1884). This modest little book did not
achieve great success at the time. It was discovered almost forty
years later after Einstein’s formulation of the theory of relativity and
the introduction of the concept of time as the fourth dimension of
three-dimensional space. The story is about a two-dimensional world
referred to as Flatland. Its inhabitants are geometric shapes. The main
character is a square. He receives the visit of a sphere who takes him
to three-dimensional Spaceland. The revelations of Spaceland open the
Flatlander’s mind to new and unexplored lands. His research is ori-
ented both forwards towards increasing dimensions, and backwards
to Lineland and Pointland.
35
After his initiation into the third dimension, the native Flatlander
explores in his dreams the one-dimensional and even a-dimensional
world. The one-dimensional country of Lineland is inhabited by a
“multitude of small Straight Lines . . . of the nature of lustrous
points—all moving to and fro in one and the same Straight Line” (p.
54).1 For the Linelander, “the Straight Line . . . constituted the whole
of the world, and indeed the whole of Space. Not being able either
to move or to see, save in his Straight Line, he had no conception of
anything out of it. . . . Outside his World, or Line, all was a blank to
him; nay, not even a blank, for a blank implies Space; say, rather, all
was non-existent” (p. 55).
Eventually, Abbott’s dream-explorer is taken to Pointland, “the
Abyss of No dimensions” (p. 92), whose sole inhabitant is a point:
That Point is . . . confined to the non-dimensional Gulf. He is himself
his own World, his own Universe; of any other than himself he can
form no conception; he knows not Length, nor Breadth, nor Height,
for he has had no experience of them; he has no cognizance even of
the number Two; nor has he a thought of Plurality; for he is himself
his One and All, being really Nothing. [p. 92]
Abbott was a philosopher and mathematician, not a clinician, but his
was a free, critical, and creative mind. When he published Flatland,
he did not intend to write a psychological treaty. At that time, Freud
had not yet developed his interest in psychopathological phenomena,
but it seems that thoughts around the functioning of the human mind
were in the air.
Donald Meltzer does not seem to have come across Abbott’s Flat-
land. He never refers to this satirical little book. Although the intentions
of the two authors were different, there are striking similarities in their
descriptions of the functioning of the mind in terms of dimensionality:
they both explore the narrowing effects of two-dimensional mental
functioning and the ultimate breakdown of mental dimensionality
altogether. Today, Abbott’s description of Pointland evokes the notion
of the astronomic black hole, which had yet to be discovered when
he wrote his Romance of Many Dimensions. An astronomic black hole is
described as what remains after the catastrophic breakdown of a dying
star with great mass. Space-time collapses over the star which then
disappears from our universe (Kaufmann, 1977). In Abbott’s prophetic
vision, Pointland is close to this absolute unthinkable nothingness.
Autistic mental functioning seems very close to that of the inhabit-
ants of two-dimensional Flatland and one-dimensional Lineland, and
autistic children even seem to know about the ultimate a-dimensional
spoon-feeding, which Emilio refused. From that time, the baby would
close or avert his eyes and cover his ears if anybody approached him.
Around that time, mother had to resume work. Every morning, she
took the local train with baby Emilio on her lap. The mother left him
with her parents, who lived almost an hour away, and brought him
back home on the same train in the evening.
When he was 15 months old, mother decided that it was time to
wean him, but she seemed unable to imagine how to manage this
change. So, she covered her nipples with Band-Aids. From one day to
another, the potentially ever-present breast became a no-access breast.
Emilio touched the Band-Aid strips and said, “Bua–bua” (“sore–sore”),
but after a week the breast seemed “gone” from the child’s memory,
and Emilio did not search for it again.
It was only when he started nursery school, at the age of 3, that the
parents began to worry seriously about his development. Emilio was
withdrawn; he did not play with other children, he flapped his hands,
and his speech was echolalic. For one year, Emilio had two-weekly
sessions of psychomotor therapy. After a follow-up consultation, the
clinic suggested psychotherapy.
From the beginning of his life, Emilio seemed to have experienced
the nipple as an integral part of his mouth. Had he managed to exclude
any awareness of differences between presence and absence, and any
perception of otherness from his experience? When partial weaning
was started, Emilio covered his eyes and ears, obliterating the distance
senses—sight and hearing—that open the mind to undeniable three-
dimensional experiences in the external world. He may have clung to
the exciting tactile sensation of the nipple-in-the-mouth, which still
continued at night. It is possible that he had shut out any awareness
of the existence of in-betweenness in the dimensions of both space and
time and had withdrawn into a pathological delusion of uninterrupted
adhesive union with the mother:
In an attempt to re-establish the primary situation of flowing-over-
at-oneness, which had been agonisingly disturbed by experiences of
sensuous disconnection from a sensation-giving “mother” who had
been experienced as part of the body, auto-sensual reactions are set
in train which bring about the delusion of fusion with a sensation-
object. [Tustin, 1981, p. 22]
When Emilio entered the therapy room for the first time, he acknowl-
edged neither the separation from his parents nor the therapist’s
presence. His behaviour was robotic and so was his speech, yet, in
that very first session, he was able to represent the catastrophe of the
Figure 3.1
Emilio first drew a small blue square at the bottom of the sheet,
then a large pink shape, a “bubble”. Around the bubble, were
knobs, which he called “clicks”, and various other protruding
shapes. On the upper side of the bubble, he added a larger blue
bulge, which he called “shoulder”, and to the left a big long purple
protrusion, which looked like a tongue, but he said that it was a
“throat”.
Emilio had a notion of the body parts that he had drawn, but neither
their position nor their shape had a consistent formal identity nor felt
meaningful. What he drew were part-objects, unrelated both to one
another and to the body of the bubble. The clicks look like sticking-out
nipples—might they correspond to Tustin’s patient John’s buttons?—
over which Emilio may have needed command. The sticking-out
shoulder corresponds, more or less, to its anatomic shape, but with
the tongue-shaped throat everything seemed to have become more
confused: there had been a breakdown of differentiation between
the convexity of tongue and nipple, and the concavity of mouth and
throat. With this drawing, Emilio seemed to have found a way of rep-
resenting an even deeper level of the primary catastrophe that he had
shown with the archway and the train, where what had collapsed had
been “only” the potential of three-dimensional mental functioning.
In this drawing, the breakdown seemed to have occurred at an
even more primitive mental depth. If nipple and mouth become
Emilio added two long thin (reddish) shapes to the drawing, which
pointed downwards to the low edge of the sheet of paper, without
reaching it (Figure 3.2). To explain what they were, he said “reggi,
reggi”. “Reggere” means to support, bear, hold, sustain. The child
had felt that the big pink bubble seemed no longer able to sustain
itself, but it was unlikely that those two fragile added supports
would really be strong enough to carry its weight.
Figure 3.2
that searches for the convex nipple to fill the empty space and create
the condition for the arrival of the nourishing milk.
Emilio’s upside-down drawing showed not only his spatial confu-
sion, but also conveyed his dawning feelings of utter helplessness,
which the omnipotent illusion of the initial magical clicks was no
longer able to keep at bay. The drawing of the following session seems
to confirm this (Figure 3.3).
It is black to begin with. No soft pastel colours any more. Emilio
produced a shape reminiscent of the pink bubble. The little blue square
of the first drawing was now replaced by a sharp black prick. Emilio
Figure 3.3
started colouring the inside of the bubble, but soon gave up. The hap-
hazard black scribbles seem to say something about the child’s utter
disorientation and hopelessness.
There were no clicks any more, only the sheer weight of primary
depression that Emilio had shown in the first assessment session with
the bridge that had collapsed under the impact of the train and buried
it under its broken parts. Could Emilio’s experience of catastrophe
even be linked back to mother’s anxiety around the voracity of her
new-born baby? A ferocious predator meeting a fragile bubble-breast
resulting in the breakdown of the baby’s primary experience of dimen-
sionality? Or even further back to the birth scene, with the mother
imagining that the baby was sleeping and did not want to be born?
Tustin (1994) writes about autistic disorder in terms of a two-stage
pathology, the unbearability of separation being the second stage of a
preceding state of undue adhesive unity.
Emilio seemed to know that this was where a reparation of the catas-
trophe needed to begin. He placed two “trains”, which consisted of
one thicker building-block each, on the two arms of the cross. Each
train moved on its own track. No crashes occurred at the cross point,
because the two trains never moved simultaneously, but there were
no encounters either. The trains were identical, but Emilio accepted to
identify them as the Emilio-train and the Francesca-train, respectively.
One day, his train was about to fall off at the far end of his rail. Emilio
was precipitated into a state of panic. The terror of endless falling into
bottomless space, which had been counteracted by transforming the
Figure 3.4
Figure 3.5
The rail-cross, the trains, and the shelters were arranged in every
single session over many months in an almost identical configuration,
but occasionally with minimal variations. There were also endless
stand-stills and, again and again, the risk of stereotyped repetitions
that denied meaning to what had been represented, with the child’s
mind withdrawing from the communication with his therapist and
from his train and rail-cross activities. In those moments, he escaped
physically to the window and stood there, sometimes for most of the
session time, lost in an unreachable no-man’s land. From the window,
he could see the local trains arriving, stopping at the nearby station,
and moving on again. He seemed to lull himself and to dissolve in
their sound and repetitive motion, to-and-fro, in both directions. No
contacts, no crossings. There was an addictive quality to his with-
drawal at the window. The therapist was reminded of the time when,
from the age of 9 months, and for almost a year, Emilio had been
taken on daily train journeys with his mother. At that time, his autis-
tic manoeuvres may have allowed him to shut out the experience of
separation from his mother, who left him with his grandparents for
the day; he would take refuge in the rocking continuity of a delusional
sensuous union, enveloping himself in the sounds, noises, and vibra-
tory rhythms of the moving train.
The movement of change was slow but eventually undeniable.
Physical three-dimensionality had become part of Emilio’s experience;
the shelters offered a reliable protective function, the trains occasion-
ally met at the cross point, and the opening and closing of the shelter
doors, with the trains going in and coming out, had brought about
basic differentiations in Emilio’s mental configuration. The scene was
set for the discovery of the vicissitudes around traumatic experiences
in the mouth and oral aggression, which, as suggested by Tustin, are
invariably connected in some ways with autistic withdrawal. The
first intentional representations and the first moments of pretend play
appeared in Emilio’s activities.
He took the kitchen-pot and made the gesture and the hissing
sound of lighting the gas flame under the pot. He put the little
mummy-figure in the pot and “cooked” her. When she was “done”,
he gave the doll to the therapist to eat, but warned her that the
mummy was very hot and would burn her mouth. He also cooked
the children, including the baby, and occasionally banged the
mummy doll on the table with rage. He took a piece of string that
had represented a strip of Band-Aid in an earlier session and tied
it around the boy-doll’s neck, saying that his neck or his throat (it
was not clear) was sore.
After a long time, the child seemed to have taken up the confused
and confusing throat/tongue issue of his first drawing again, but
at a new, emotionally more intense and meaningful level. At this
point, “an internal space within the mind” seemed to have devel-
oped in the child, “in which phantasy as trial action, and therefore
experimental thought, could take place (Meltzer, 1975b, p. 225). Dur-
ing this “cannibalistic” kitchen period, in which mothers and babies
alike were cooked and eaten, Emilio had become able to project
his emerging, burning-hot, oral phantasies into the therapist, who
was made to feel the scorching emotions connected with his abrupt
weaning at the age of 15 months. From one day to another, mother’s
nipples, which had filled his passionately sucking mouth since he
was born, were gone. By that time, he must have had a mouth full
of teeth. Was he terrified that his voracious mouth had bitten off
mother’s nipples? At the sight of the Band-Aid strips on her breasts,
Emilio had said at the time that they were sore. The disappearance
of the nipples had caused the abrupt rupture of the baby’s highly
eroticized delusional continuity of the breast-mouth connection,
leaving him with the “black hole with the nasty prick” feeling in
his mouth. Had the sight of those mutilated flattened breasts, which
had lost their powerful, dark protruding part, contributed to the
final breakdown of the third dimension in the child’s mind, a pro-
cess that had started much earlier, at the latest at the time of partial
weaning when the baby covered his eyes and ears when approached
by a person perceived in his or her otherness?
There must have been a mixture of unbearable pain, despair, and
guilt, which entailed not only the breakdown of any differentiation
between mouth and nipple, but at the deepest levels of spatial experi-
ence, the collapse of the preconception of the encounter of opposed
and complementary concave and convex shapes. Mother’s nipples
and the child’s mouth may have disappeared in one cosmic existential
wound, a great black hole.
In the first drawing (Figure 3.6), Emilio began by outlining the big
duck’s beak, then he drew its head, eye, neck, and body. Next came
a duckling. The child was pleased with what he had produced
and said, “mummy and son”. Two more ducklings followed in a
row and, finally, a fourth one was added underneath the mummy
Figure 3.6
duck. Only then did he notice that he had not drawn the waterline.
He drew it from left to right, in the direction of the ducks’ swim-
ming motion. The line began at a low level and moved upwards
towards the birds with the clear intention of supporting their
bodies. However, he did not quite succeed in his intention, and
the ducks remained floating above the water line. Emilio noticed
this and vigorously filled the gap between the ducks and the water
with black pencil. He clearly wanted them to be supported by the
water line. Pointing to the fourth duckling, he said that that one
was “under water” and added “eyes burning”. (Was the experi-
ence of the distance between him and the mother duck too painful
to be looked at? Or did he simply remember summer holidays and
the salty sea water? In either case, there was a mental association.)
He coloured the water carefully, paying attention to the contours
of the under-water-duckling. When the therapist asked him, hav-
ing intuitively grasped that Emilio’s new capacity for orientation
in space allowed new meanings to emerge, if that duckling had
fallen into the water, Emilio replied: “Drowned.”
The first thing Emilio did this time was to trace the waterline,
which was straighter and less hesitant than in the first draw-
ing. Space was clearly divided horizontally into an upper and a
lower part. There were again five ducks, the big mother and four
ducklings (Figure 3.7). On the whole, they looked more solid than
in the initial drawings, and their bodies were in contact with the
water. Their necks were coloured in black and the mother’s neck,
in particular, was quite big. Emilio added little wings to each duck,
but there was not enough space for the last duckling of the row. Its
Figure 3.7
body was nothing but a tiny annex to its black throat, and it looked
rather miserable. It seemed to be lagging behind and was not really
part of the family. From its position, it could not see the mother
duck properly, because the duckling in front of it was too big.
But in this drawing Emilio added a new element: two little fish
swimming under the surface. Their shape and colour reminded
me of Nemo, the cartoon fish who gets lost in the ocean after its
mother’s death but is found in the end by its father. Both little fish
in the drawing have a big round eye with a clearly drawn pupil
and an eyebrow and, under the eye, a smaller, uncoloured circle.
Emilio explained that this was the mouth. The eyes are quite elabo-
rated, and the mouth is empty. This drawing, too, was replicated
almost identically for many sessions.
Under the water, where in the first drawing there had been a
drowned duckling, there were now two colourful living fish. Did
Emilio gradually come to life? And did two-ness become more and
more thinkable? Were there signs of the primary split, of a primary
differentiation whose first signs had appeared with the intersected
train rails and had developed further with the horizontal line on
the paper, which structured space by separating the upper from the
lower part? Separation and differentiation seemed no longer to coin-
cide inevitably with trauma and death. The duck family above the
water line is rather stereotyped, guaranteeing possibly an illusion
of continuity, but in the under-water-world new elements became
visible. Fish have no beaks. They have mouths like people. In this
drawing the mouths are empty. But the eyes are quite elaborate.
New explorations became possible as shown in the last of the cho-
sen duck drawings (Figure 3.8).
Figure 3.8
first time, their mouths are not empty, but coloured in red and
protruding from their faces. Emilio said that they were wicked
monster-fish. They ganged up to attack the mummy-duck from
underneath and wanted to eat her. They were sharks, Emilio said,
“mother and son, a big shark and a small shark”. He then drew a
green and a red circle in front of the open mouths of the two fish.
These circles were lanterns, he explained. (In a parent meeting,
the therapist was told that he had been to the zoo and had been
impressed by so-called lantern fish, which have a large luminous
spot on top of their head.) In his drawing, Emilio had moved the
“lanterns” down from their heads and placed them in front of
their mouths. Was it there that something had to be illuminated?
He commented that the bulging red shapes that emerged from the
mouths were “the tongue”.
But under that surface, a very different, but more genuine internal
world began to be representable: voracious sharks, mother and son,
were conspiring against the unsuspecting duck mother. Emilio had
developed an internal world with internal objects, which could inter-
act with each other. The primary split allowed for idealization and
denigration, and for a vengeful alliance against the formerly idealized,
but emotionally indifferent duck mother.
There was a story with a dramatic plot. But this was only part of
it. Thanks to the fish lanterns’ illuminating function, Emilio could
begin to explore and sort out the confusions caused by the break-
down of three-dimensionality in his traumatized baby mind. Now he
could observe and notice the difference between concave and convex
objects and also acknowledge their different locations and functions.
The lantern was placed so that it could illuminate the oral cavity, the
tongue in the mouth and the implicit teeth of the mother–child couple
of sharks. The acknowledgement of the difference between tongue and
nipple—the two similar protruding shapes that were confused initially
at the part-object level—became the object of a realistic quest once
Emilio had realized that the fullness of the tongue could not replace
the emptiness caused by the disappearance of the nipple. In fact, the
mother-son gang of sharks was about to attack the unsuspecting duck
mother from underneath. She did not know, but the shark mother-and-
son knew and had a plan.
The deceptive idea of the oneness of mouth, tongue, and nip-
ple, which had protected Emilio from the catastrophic experience of
his weaning, could be abandoned and his oral rage overtly directed
towards the maternal object. Oral aggression, hitherto submerged,
could now come to the surface, not only in his representation of cruel
oral phantasies in the cannibalistic kitchen scenes, but also, at a more
abstract level, in his drawings and in the accompanying meaningful
verbal narrative.
In conclusion
The core of autistic one- and two-dimensional mental functioning
lies in the inability to conceive of separation and separateness. But
the dream of a primal one-ness must be abandoned for two-ness to
become possible. Two-ness implies an internal migration: it implies
leaving behind, at the most primary level, what Meltzer has called the
“romantic object”. This migration is the prerequisite for the emergence
of a space/time between self and other. It is this very betweenness
Notes
1. All page numbers for quotations from Flatland refer to the version available
online at www.feedbooks.com
2. I thank Emilio’s psychotherapist, Francesca Bevilacqua, Rome, for allowing
me to discuss her patient’s material and drawings.
Autism reconsidered
Suzanne Maiello
A
t the age of 8 years, Donald Meltzer accompanied his parents
on a trip to Europe. The young boy was deeply impressed by
the beauty of historical buildings on the old continent and
wanted to become an architect. Later, as a psychoanalytic thinker,
Meltzer continued to use his gift for spatial design by conceptualizing
and interpreting psychic phenomena at the meta-psychological level
in terms of dimensionality. Following on from Klein’s notion of an
internal world inhabited by internal objects and, later, Bion’s concept
of container/contained, the scene was set for the emergence, in 1967
(well before Explorations in Autism) of Meltzer’s spatial notions of the
geography of the mind, of geographical and zonal confusions, and, later, of
the compartments of the internal object and the claustrum.
In Explorations in Autism, the idea of dimensionality of mental
functioning became the central notion from which Meltzer’s theo-
retical formulations were to spring and expand. He introduced the
chapter on “Dimensionality as a Parameter of Mental Functioning”
by stating:
It is of interest with regard to the psycho-analytical method that
altered views of life-space found expression in the interpretative
work long before they came as theoretical realizations. [1975b, p.
223]
56
Tommaso:
the discovery of linear movement
and three-dimensional space
Tommaso1 was a severely autistic 4-year-old child without lan-
guage when he began psychotherapy. He spent the first months of
his treatment running aimlessly around the room. At some point,
however, the corridor that led from the waiting room to the therapy
room became a meaningful space of transition. The child began to
experience between-ness. He wanted the therapist to walk beside him,
but he allowed her to do so at her own pace. Differences began to be
noticed and accepted. During the sessions, he started hiding behind
an armchair and then re-emerge, and he spent months engaged with
repeatedly placing a small box inside a larger box and taking it out
again. One day, he discovered the reflection of his face on the win-
dow pane. He had seen it for a brief moment, although he blurred
his image immediately with saliva.
One day, he was lying on the floor at the therapist’s feet, looking
towards a corner of the room. He then crawled to that corner.
Lying on his back, his eyes moved upwards from the floor, all the
way to the ceiling, along the line where the two walls meet, and
then followed the horizontal lines along the ceiling. Suddenly, he
turned on his stomach, made masturbatory movements and then
got to his feet. He stood there motionless, his eyes lost, while his
urine began to form a puddle on the floor.
Rosetta:
the dimension of time—from endlessness
to finitude, from dismantling to rhythmic coordination
Rosetta began three-times-a-week psychotherapy just before the age
of 5 years. Her verbal language was echolalic. In the fourth year of
our work, she began to be aware of the alternation of presence and
absence. The reality of rhythmically structured time began to dawn
upon the little girl’s mind. The alternation of yes-days, when she came
to therapy, and no-days, when she did not, could be acknowledged.
When differences can be experienced, three-dimensional mental func-
tioning is on the way, as a conquest, but also as a threat of renewed
existential catastrophe. The little girl’s material shows her struggle to
overcome the autistic need for sameness but, at the same time, the
ever-lurking temptation to deny endings and dismantle consensuality,
in order to prevent the catastrophe of the emergence of overwhelming
emotions.
Rosetta was 8, and was going to school, when she began to sing
nursery rhymes in the session and needed me to sing them with
her. She no longer used our unison to obliterate separateness, but to
reinforce her capacity to bear it. She particularly liked a rhythmical
song about the days of the week, which were named in sequence
and accompanied by clapping. This is how she learned to count. Her
learning process was imbued with emotional turmoil around pres-
ence and absence, meeting and parting, beginnings and endings. She
had known the names of numbers before, but in the past these had
rarely been connected with the here-and-now experience of reality.
She would start to count real objects, but then lost touch with them,
continuing her counting until she would lose herself in the high range
of senseless numbers. Stopping would have represented a boundary,
an ending, including the end of the one-dimensional illusion of infinite
continuity and sameness, where all-ness and nothingness coincide.
In the entrance hall of my office there are five spotlights in a row.
During that same period, Rosetta noticed them for the first time. She
started counting them: one, two, three, . . . and raised her fingers as
we walked to the therapy room. However, at the end of the corridor
she noticed that the last number that she had pronounced did not cor-
respond to the number of her raised fingers. There had been no shared
rhythm between voice and hand. When she noticed the discrepancy,
she used to give up.
Meltzer writes: “It seems likely that . . . the first leaps of imagi-
nation were of a myth-making variety, enacted in song-and-dance”
(1986a, p. 184). When Rosetta first tried to count the spotlights, there
was no connection yet at the level of song-and-dance between her
“singing” voice and her “dancing” fingers. However, something in
Rosetta needed to explore this most primitive proto-symbolic myth-
making level of three-dimensional mental functioning. At this point in
time, dismantling was no longer an unconsciously sought-for strategy
against consensuality, but was experienced as a failure. The day came
when Rosetta’s voice and fingers met in a shared rhythm: the vocal
five coincided with the raised five fingers of her open hand. She was
beaming. She took my hand to lift both our arms in triumph, and we
had to sing Beethoven’s Ode to Joy together.
João:
a haphazard mental topography2
In one- and two-dimensional mental functioning, shapes are both
reversible in their formal configuration and unreliable as to their posi-
tion in space. Autistic children often seem to ignore the basic physical
laws of gravity. They may walk on tiptoes and feel to be floating in
unstructured space, where not only does nothing have volume or
boundaries, but where up and down, front and back, left and right
are interchangeable. Meltzer illustrates the two-dimensional mental
functioning of an autistic little boy by describing his drawing of a
house with a door on one side of the sheet, and another house with a
door in the same position on the other side of the sheet:
. . . the child demonstrated his experience of a two-dimensional
object: when you enter by the front door you simultaneously exit
by the rear door of a different object. It is in effect an object without
an inside. [1975e, p. 18]
Front and back coincide.
A drawing by an autistic child will illustrate the interchangeability
of spatial vertices in this child’s mind: up/down, left/right, as well as
the indifferentiation of shapes themselves—convexity/concavity, and
the haphazard quality of movement in and through space. Nothing is
grounded anywhere. Every potential space-structuring and meaning-
conveying shape or movement is reversible. Since there is no mental
container, there is no meaningful memory and, therefore, no base for
anticipatory phantasy or expectation.
João was born prematurely and spent the first three weeks of
his life in an intensive care unit. He was not breast-fed. He vomited
after feeds and underwent gastric surgery shortly after his arrival
home. At the beginning of his five-times-a-week treatment, he was
2 years old. He had no verbal language and seemed fused with
his mother’s body. From the age of 4, he started to produce draw-
ings which were replicated endlessly throughout the sessions. At the
beginning, they were just scribbles. Later, some became more struc-
tured. The child was powerfully attracted by the steep, high moun-
tains that rise from the city of Rio de Janeiro: the Corcovado with
the Christ figure on top, and the Sugar Loaf. João used to repeat
their names and draw their shapes endlessly. Figure 4.1 is the last of
an endless series of identical drawings produced during one session.
João was now 9 years old.
Figure 4.1
Figure 4.2
Janeiro, with natural geological formations that look like eyes, nose
and mouth. Maintaining the sheet of paper in this second position,
João now drew a roller coaster which ran downhill on the steep slope
of the first Corcovado, which was now upside down. Thin lines on the
vehicle represented the arms held up by excited people.
Comment
Considering the drawing in its first position: there is a steep, high
mountain shape with a rounded top and a short vertical line point-
ing upwards. It seems difficult to reach that top. (In reality there is a
cogwheel train that goes up.) João’s town is a cluster of stone blocks,
which lies under the horizon. At the part-object level, might we won-
der whether at the deepest depths of this child’s unconscious mind
there could be a surviving preconception of an unreachable breast
and nipple, of which he had experienced no positive realizations.
As a baby, he had vomited the milk contained in the bottle that had
replaced the absent breast.
Considering the inverted drawing: the animal-like shape is a pet-
rified creature that adheres tightly to “mother earth”. There is no
nipple on this second mountain, but, according to the child, it is the
Corcovado again. A roller-coaster runs down the steep (inverted)
slope of the first Corcovado. What would have been uphill in the first
position of the sheet of paper is now downhill. The saving nipple of
the first mountain is unreachable once more, this time because it is
buried under the inverted horizon line. What is visible of the people
on the roller coaster are just the very thin lines of their arms rising up
in excitement. There are no passages, no openings leading from one
side to another, no orifices with their both separating and connecting
function. The awareness of orifices, as Meltzer suggests, ushers in
three-dimensional mental functioning.
If we tentatively turn the sheet back to its original position (which
João did not do), the Gávea stone is upside down and stuck to what
is now the skyline (Figure 4.3). Perhaps he is at risk of falling off?
The second Corcovado is no longer a mountain, but has turned into a
deep pit. And the roller-coaster on the first Corcovado would now be
hanging over an abyss, with the people head down, at risk of hurtling
into nothingness, unless they were glued, from underneath, onto the
surface of the slope.
João seems to represent dramatically the non-orientation and the
disorientation of his movements in space. He tries to protect himself
Figure 4.3
Concluding remarks
Meltzer’s meta-psychological metaphor of the dimensionality of the
mind is a precious legacy, which not only maintains its meaningfulness
forty years after the publication of Explorations in Autism, but, being
an unsaturated concept with much wider implications, maintains its
Notes
1. I thank Patrizia Ercolani for permission to present some considerations on
her patient’s clinical material.
2. I thank Marisa Helena Monteiro for allowing me to present some considera-
tions on an aspect of her patient’s material.
Didier Houzel
T
he scientific exploration of a domain of experience supposes
the possibility of locating this domain within a space whose
internal structure can be described. Such an approach was ren-
dered impossible within the field of psychic phenomena by Cartesian
dualism for almost two centuries. Descartes (1641) opposes the bio-
logical body—which he defines as an “extended substance”, that is to
say, occupying a space and, thus, capable of being explored scientifi-
cally—and the non-extended soul, which occupies no space and, conse-
quently, escapes any form of scientific study. A century later, Kant still
affirmed that the soul was indivisible. At the very most, he admitted
a temporal dimension in the internal sense but rejected any possibility
of a “science of the soul”:
The theory of the soul can never become anything more than an
historical doctrine of nature, and, as such, a natural doctrine of inner
sense which is as systematic as possible, that is, a natural doctrine of
the soul, but not a science of the soul . . . [1787, 4:470–471]
66
The more common non-orientable manifolds are the Moebius strip, the
Klein bottle, and the projective plane.
I have noticed that some patients become disorientated when
there has been a discontinuity in the psychoanalytic frame and may,
for example, experience difficulty finding their car after a session.
Working with autistic children has inspired me to hypothesize a non-
orientability or a loss of orientability of psychic space, which I hope
to illustrate with the following extracts from my work with an autistic
boy.
A clinical illustration
Cyril’s analysis began when he was 3 years old. For the first two years,
I saw him three times per week; then, when he was 5, this increased
to four weekly sessions. He had been diagnosed with autism. Cyril is
the elder of two boys: his brother is two years younger. His parents are
from a cultured background: his father is in an intellectual profession,
while his mother had been in higher education but, currently, does
not work. A deformity of the uterus made her pregnancies difficult,
and she only conceived Cyril after three miscarriages. This pregnancy
was a time of great anxiety, during which she stayed mainly resting in
bed, and the baby moved very little in utero. Her impression was that,
by remaining very still, he was protecting himself from the uterine
contractions that she experienced as being threatening to him, given
the very little space that he had. So, from the beginning, she found it
extremely difficult to conceive of herself as a good container for her
baby and even sensed herself as a threat from which he had to protect
himself.
Cyril was a quiet baby. His sleep soon settled into a regular pat-
tern. He was breast-fed for six weeks, but this was very brusquely
interrupted due to an infection of the mother’s breast. Nothing was
noticed at the time of this sudden weaning. Cyril’s motor development
was delayed: he only sat up when he was a year old and did not walk
until he was 22 months old.
Mother’s second pregnancy began when Cyril was 15 months
old. Complications during the final trimester threatened to provoke a
premature birth, so she remained in bed until being hospitalized for
the last month.
Cyril’s parents first became concerned by his delayed motor devel-
opment, but they soon worried about his failure to develop language.
“nest of babies” fantasy. This is associated with the notion that there
are “special babies” who are given “special food”. [Tustin, 1972a,
pp. 177–178]
After our first summer holiday break, Cyril inaugurated a new activity:
he emptied his box of toys, threw away the pens and paper from the
table where I had put them, climbed triumphantly onto this table and
from there onto my lap, and exclaimed “big, big, big”. I first interpreted
this as his wish to grow into a big boy coming to see me and drawing
support from me. But then I realized that he was expressing rivalry
with the objects he had thrown away, representing the rival babies who
(in his view) had stayed with me all through the long summer break.
In a subsequent session, this aspect became clearer, as the following
extract shows:
Cyril threw the pens to the floor, then played with the water,
attempting to flood the whole room, in spite of my forbidding him
to do so. Then he threw all the sheets of paper from the little table
to the floor, and climbed onto the table saying in a triumphant
tone of voice “Grown up”. From there, he climbed onto my lap,
then back to the table. He picked up the pens, gave them to me to
hold for him, then threw them back onto the floor. With one of the
pens, he drew several long lines on some sheets of paper, saying
they were “little cats”. He then threw these sheets of paper to the
floor and went through the whole sequence again: climbing onto
the table, then onto my lap, then back to the table. He also made
as if to bite me.
Cyril then had to face the paternal figure as a powerful rival—the big-
gest baby, said Tustin—who threatened to separate him definitively
from the maternal figure. One day he arrived slightly early and saw
me walk in accompanied by a woman. Once in the therapy room, he
seemed furious, asking, “Why was there a lady? Where did the lady
go?” Then he told me, “You know, I could break your feet, your legs,
your knees?” all the way up to my head and my hair.
During the following years Cyril’s fantasies evolved into more
and more orientable scenarios, but with two kinds of preoccupation
according to the direction of the action. On a horizontal axis, the ques-
tion was this: how might he find the right way to join me in the trans-
ference? He expressed this through fantasies of the route he might
follow with the taxi driver who drove him to the clinic: which way
out of the ring road did they have to choose? How could they avoid
traffic jams or accidents caused by other cars crashing into the back of
the taxi? I interpreted these fantasies in the transference as connected
to his mother’s second pregnancy and his little brother’s birth which
he had felt as a violent intrusion in his maternal containing object
(the taxi). When he had such a fantasy he called for a policeman to
recover his own space in his containing object. The policeman seemed
to represent a good paternal protective figure.
On a vertical axis, the question was this: how could he slide down
from the top to the bottom, but not too fast and without too much anxi-
ety? To illustrate this, he used the scenario of sliding on a toboggan in
a swimming pool, which made me think of a precipitated birth. Cyril
put lights at the top of the toboggan to force people to wait before
sliding. I link this fantasy with an adult patient’s account of being at
the birth of his children: he noticed that, during the delivery, it was
necessary to slow down the progression of the baby’s head to prevent
the risk of a perineal tear. I interpreted this as the reclamation of a
protective paternal figure in the transference to prevent the psycho-
analytic process, which resembles a kind of birth, from going too fast
and roughly. The patient replied that sometimes he felt lost in a huge
space, without knowing which direction he might follow. I understood
Cyril’s scenario in the same way: he needed to reclaim a good pater-
nal object to help him get out of the maternal womb in order to be
born and to grow up without destroying his maternal container (the
perineal tear) or falling endlessly through space. I suppose that the
orientability of the psychic space relies both on the paternal elements
of the container and on the child’s identification with a paternal figure.
By contrast, non-orientability could be a defence against the primitive
agonies the child has to face when leaving the womb.
Conclusion
One of the most original features of Meltzer’s work is its spatial refer-
ence. He describes the psychic world in terms of spaces and the differ-
ent states of mind as linked to a shift from one space to another. This
spatial reference leads him to describe autism as a state with no dif-
ferentiation between an inner world an outer world. Absence of differ-
entiation hinders any possibility for projection outside or introjection
inside any external object and the chance of communication with it.
To describe this state of mind, Meltzer uses a geometrical model based
on the concept of dimensionality. But scrutinizing his clinical illustra-
tions of this model, one might think that a topological model would
better suit clinical work with autistic children or with patients having
an autistic enclave. The topological concept of non-orientability allows
for the description of spaces—whatever their dimension—in which
there can be no differentiation between interior and exterior.
In his introduction to Explorations in Autism, Meltzer writes:
In fact it will soon be clear to a discerning reader that we are in the
business of locating problems rather than of solving them. This is
probably really the fundamental truth about the human sciences in
general and psycho-analysis in particular. Thus we believe we have
located some very mysterious phenomena of the mind by recogniz-
ing them operative in very condensed form in the children treated.
[Meltzer, 1975c, pp. 4–5]
It seems to me that, by emphasizing this locating function of psycho
analysis, Meltzer introduces the issue of a topological model of mental
apparatus. Topology was first called by Leibniz analysis situs, which
means analysis of the location of the elements studied in mathematics.
Autistic children teach us that we have first to clarify the problem of
location if we are to help them develop their own psychic space.
Jeffrey L. Eaton
F
orty years ago, in Explorations in Autism, Donald Meltzer offered
eloquent testimony to the complexity of autistic experience
(Meltzer et al., 1975). His keen formulations remain as valuable
today as when introduced in 1975. It was his capacity to capture and
describe complexity that I want briefly to highlight as a context for the
specific developments offered by Suzanne Maiello and Didier Houzel.
With his colleagues, Meltzer noted a number of specific features
about the children with whom they worked, including the child’s
often high intelligence; the factor of speed (they were fast to register
and slow to process experience); their openness to sensation (Meltzer
characterized it as “naked to the wind”; 1975e, p. 9); their sensitivity
to the therapist’s state of mind; and, finally, a primitive permeability
to the emotions of others.
Meltzer also described other features, including what he called
“an uncompromising possessiveness of the maternal object” (1975e,
p. 10), hyper-sensuousness, and a hatred of transience, leading to a
feeling of being persecuted by the awareness of time. All of these
observations, and many others that I have not noted here, help to
clarify the autistic and post-autistic states as witnessed in analytic
experience with children who are often difficult to comprehend.
With these features in mind, one can better appreciate the rich con-
tributions of Maiello and Houzel regarding the specific theme of
psychic dimensionality that is a particular instance of the complexity
Meltzer started to investigate.
75
This is highly condensed material and the particular details are very
important. Meltzer continues:
One child showed us the answer in a single stroke of creative
intensity. For months he had drawn doors and gates, usually with
complex wrought-iron grills. Then gradually rather Victorian gothic
houses took shape. One day he painstakingly drew an ornate house
seen from the front on one side of the page, a house in Northwood,
while on the other side he drew a back view of a pub in Southend.
Thus the child demonstrated his experience of a two-dimensional
object; when you enter by the front door you simultaneously exit
by the rear door of a different object. It is in effect an object without
an inside. [Meltzer, 1975e, p. 18]
Houzel takes issue with the conclusion that this describes an object
without an inside. In working with autistic states, one does encounter
descriptions of flat, adhesive states that are without an inside and that
seem to present purely sensation-dominated surfaces. This vignette,
however, does not seem like a good example, because, as Houzel points
out, you cannot enter a two-dimensional space. There is no door to a
surface or a plane.
We should take such facts literally. Meltzer emphasized the con-
creteness of psychic reality. If you take the drawing literally, the child
gives much attention to doors and gates. These figures are drawn over
and over for months. If one looks at sameness, then one may feel there
is a repetition. If one looks at novelty, then one may feel that something
is being constructed, something is trying to be realized.
What kind of complex space allows the passage from a house in
Northwood to a pub in Southend? Is this an object without an inside?
Alternatively, can it be seen as an object with such a complex inside
space that it cannot be easily mapped or represented? I have observed
in my work with autistic children the ubiquitous presence of “por-
tals” (doors and gates) in many children’s material, which suggests
the challenge of managing experience both within and between very
complex spaces.
To describe the extraordinarily complex space that some autistic
children struggle to express, Houzel offers the imaginative conjecture
of what movement in such a complex space, represented here by a
three-dimensional torus, would be like:
Look at the back wall and the line of sight passes through that wall
and returns from the opposite point on the front wall. What you see
is a copy of yourself from behind. Look to the right and you see a
copy of yourself from the left; look down at the floor and you see
the top of your head. [Thurston & Weeks, 1984, p. 108]
Meltzer used the concept of confusional anxiety in relationship to the
capacity for thinking. Here, Houzel offers another crucial point of
view: the necessity to consider the function of “orientability” and “non-
orientability” as part of the challenge of the autistic child’s subjective
experience.
This idea has large implications for themes like the “presence” of
the object and the absence of the object. In what kind of space does the
child meet the object? What kind of space is created by the awareness
of the object’s presence and its absence? What kind of experience of
cause and effect is created when space (and time) are distorted and
disoriented? And, how is the child’s perception of an object influenced
by the nature of its space? These ideas offer practical help in following
the phenomenological experience of a patient’s material.
I would like to describe an experience with a post-autistic patient,
Steven, I have seen in analysis for ten years. The vignette I present
Steven, aged 17 at this time, enters the session and greets me cheer-
fully. Then he says, “Long time since I’ve seen you, Jeff.” “Yes,” I
say, “it’s been a long time.” He then asks me if I have heard about
the Charlie Hebdo shootings in Paris. Steven considers himself a
cartoonist and graphic artist. Over the years he has produced many
clever drawings and cartoons both inside and outside his sessions.
Several have been published in his school newspapers. Now he
tells me that he is starting to create a graphic novel, a project begun
over the break.
This story seems to evoke, in all its details, something powerful about
Steven’s feelings when he is separated from me. Our sessions provide
him with a kind of orientation that can be suddenly lost over the long
holiday break, but now, rather than concretely living out this disorien-
tation and withdrawing into autistic and omnipotent defences, Steven
Louise Allnutt
“Adoption is outside. You act out what it feels like to be the one
who doesn’t belong. And you act it out by trying to do to others
what has been done to you. It is impossible to believe that anyone
loves you for yourself.”
Jeanette Winterson, Why Be Happy
When You Could Be Normal? (2011)
Being at home
Dimensionality, identity, and security are concepts that I would
like to draw together and house descriptively as the experience of
“being at home”. In my view, one needs to have developed a clear
sense of the dimensions of space and your own place in it alongside
a strong and secure-enough relationship to oneself and others to
have the opportunity to feel at home, either in terms of “joining the
human family”, as Maria Rhode has described it (2008), or in terms
of being capable of making transitions and crossing thresholds into
different spaces without a sense of losing one’s own coherence. My
experience of working with one particular patient has highlighted
these issues at both a developmental and emotional level, but the
absence or presence of “at-home-ness” is something that I find lies
at the heart of many clinical relationships, sometimes in relation
81
little sense of purpose. He made eye contact with neither parent nor
staff at the centre and had little language. He did not cry but was heard
to grunt and moan. He did not play and sought neither comfort nor
the company of either of his parents or any other adult. The initial
assessment concluded that parental neglect and emotional abuse were
having a profound impact on Joseph’s development.
Following this assessment and his foster placement, Joseph returned
to the assessment centre for some therapeutic sessions with his new
carer. My first contact with Joseph was during these six sessions with
a social work colleague, aimed at supporting him and his carer with
his transition into care in order to prepare him for adoption as well as
serving as an assessment for further therapeutic work.
Joseph did not speak during these sessions. Rather than play, he
tended to wander around the room aimlessly, locating objects in a
blank way. Initially these objects appeared to have little communica-
tive meaning. As in the assessment, Joseph was observed, during the
first couple of sessions with his foster carer, to take a small toy kitchen
pan and a wooden spoon and beat the two together repeatedly, the
spoon stabbing the inside of the pan with a thud. There was no modu-
lation in this activity, nor did Joseph seek the interest of the adults
present in what he was doing. This “mindless” and “one-dimensional”
activity (Meltzer, 1975b) continued throughout the first two sessions
despite our attempts to make contact with him.
As the sessions developed, so did Joseph’s curiosity, and during
the third session he turned his attention to the dressing-up box, where
there were a few small plastic clothes hangers. Joseph appeared quite
enlivened by these hangers, gathering up a few and handing one
to each person in the room. His foster carer and the clinicians were
surprised and equally enlivened by this process, and our discussion
in the room then centred upon how this was his first use of play as
communication. Despite Joseph’s high level of need and wide range
of difficulties, it was encouraging to see him engaged and beginning
to make contact with us.
Joseph’s foster carer was getting to know him, and this was also
having a positive influence upon his development. The hangers
became a regular feature in the sessions, accompanied by increased
enjoyment and contact with each person to whom he handed one. It
was almost as if he were deliberately giving us something from which
to hang him in our minds. It certainly had the desired effect, as Joseph
was a child whom you remembered and thought about between each
Joseph took a car to the sink and rolled the wheels along its edge.
He then let it fall from the edge into the sink. He said, “Fall over.”
Joseph looked down into the sink at the car.
Joseph removed it from the sink and took it to the table, where
he placed it on the edge. He rolled it back and forth along the
edge a couple of times. He then abandoned the car and returned
to the box. [Following this, Joseph struggled to focus; he located a
couple of objects—a rocker stack and then a teapot—which he ended
up throwing away; he then returned to the car.] The car on the table
caught Joseph’s eye. He wandered back to the table and with
his head down to one side he rolled the car back and forth. . . .
He returned with the car to the sink and again placed it on the
edge. He again let it drop and stated, “Fall over.” He repeated
this several times. At one point he then turned, leaving the car
in the sink and said, “One, two, three!” at the same time jump-
ing with both feet in the air. I remarked Joseph was counting and
he shouted, “Three, four, five!” He then turned and banged on
the door.
During this moment at the door, I held Joseph’s head and spoke to
him echoing the rhythm of his rocking. As Joseph calmed down,
he looked around the room—for the first time—and I identified
that he was showing interest in the inside of the room. At this
point, he placed his fingers inside his mouth. Both physical hold-
ing during the transition and rhythmical attunement during the
emotional transition from distress to calm provided the conditions
for Joseph to look into the physical space of the room and imme-
diately equate this interior with the inside of his own mouth. He
was, at this moment, held together by the relationship between
inside and outside.
into the room for the first time. Looking into the room implies an
evolved capacity for noticing, as distinct from the first session in
which Joseph appeared to move from one activity to the next with-
out leaving much space for noticing. There is little reference to the
therapist in the first session, whereas the additional support sought
out for the transition from his foster carer and then experienced in
relation to his therapist appears to create a bit more space for Joseph
to take in the environment. This seems to be an example of what
Rey (1979) described as the experience of “marsupial space”. The
shift between collapsing to looking into the room seems to be facili-
tated by the experience of being held rhythmically. This is very dif-
ferent from Joseph’s experience of the “fridge” which followed the
perception of a frightening occupant in the mirror.
This experience in infancy is explored by Sorensen in “Observa-
tions of Transition Facilitating Behaviour” (2000). Through her obser-
vations of nursing care provided to premature infants on a Neonatal
Intensive Care Unit (NICU), Sorensen identified that the function of
such behaviour is to “create a bridge from one state to another or
one experience to another” (p. 49). In this sense, the discontinuity
expressed by Joseph’s collapse is subsequently bridged by the support
offered by me as I physically held him and talked to him. His need
for a concrete experience of holding was significant and reflected the
infantile level of his anxiety, what Winnicott (1960) describes as the
level of “absolute dependency” (p. 113). The rhythmical quality of
my vocalizations are in this respect more important than the content
and are reminiscent of Tustin’s “rhythm of safety” (1986a) and what
Canham (1999) identifies as often lacking in fostered and adopted
children. Canham noted:
Many of these children come from backgrounds where their lives
have not been characterised by rhythm, but rather by its oppo-
site. Many children who end up being taken into care have been
neglected for very long periods of time—left unfed, unwashed and
forgotten. [1999/2012, p. 62]
Separation
With these new realizations came an increased awareness and explora-
tion of the experience of separateness. In Session 26 Joseph explored
what he described as the “snap snap scissors”; he drew a line down
the centre of a piece of paper and proceeded to use the blades of the
scissors to mark along the line, saying, “snap snap scissors,” as he did
so. I felt Joseph was seeking to identify how the scissors could be used
effectively to define separateness.
Interestingly, in the next sequence Joseph used a black crayon to
draw two separate circles:
This was the first time Joseph had drawn two separate objects, in
contrast with a “cut” or “split” singular object. The developments
in the material emerged alongside my interest in Joseph’s preoccu-
pations and, as he identified “one–two” in response to my enquiry
about his drawing, we engaged in a game of identifying different
pairs.
Final remarks
With these brief vignettes of the first few weeks of Joseph’s treatment,
I hope I have illustrated how psychoanalytic work with very young
children can attend to difficulties in spatial organization and orienta-
tion and further contribute to stability and security in object relations
and identity. My sense is that, similar to what Meltzer described in
terms of the developmental process of spatial organization, the experi-
ence of “being at home” in both an internal sense as well as an external
one is developmentally driven.
Psychoanalytic psychotherapist June Campbell (2006) highlighted
these issues in relation to the adult homeless population in Edinburgh.
Note
This chapter is based on a version previously published in the Journal of Child
Psychotherapy, Vol. 42 (No. 1, 2016), pp. 18–29. Copyright © Association of Child
Psychotherapists, reprinted by permission of Taylor & Francis Ltd, http://www
.tandfonline.com on behalf of the Association of Child Psychotherapists.
Carlos Tabbia
A
t all ages, involuntary and persistent isolation is a matter for
concern. At the end of infancy, it may be worrying, but the
isolation that appears in adolescence is frequently a symptom
of emotional disturbance and can feel deeply alarming for the fam-
ily. This conflicting developmental period can be better understood
psychoanalytically but remains a common topic in the media, which
often focus particularly on the influence of electronic games in causing
adolescent isolation.
After infancy and latency, puberty emerges with a vigour that
can surprise the young person, the family, and his or her friends.
Particularly unsettling is the imbalance created by the unintegrated
movement of the personality, prone to splitting while sustained by
obsessional defences. Puberty also seems to destabilize the physical
“centre of gravity”. This is reminiscent of the physical experience that
takes place with ice skaters. The Olympic ice skater Yulia Lipnitskaia
won a gold medal when she was 15 years old, at the Olympic Games
in Sochi, Russia, in 2014. One year later, she had to modify her skating
technique because her bodily changes had shifted the centre of gravity
she had established over many years of training. While her coaches
refer to her physical loss of equilibrium, we might also imagine a
loss of emotional balance in the face of the changing states of internal
objects.
95
This dynamic can alter when the group becomes more depressive
and less paranoid-schizoid, and at that point the isolated adolescent
may be able to obtain more understanding and containment for his
or her suffering.
somite thirty: Keep warm in bed. Your King and Country want
you—you stay in bed. [1991, p. 443]
This staying in bed, locked in a world without stimulation, is the
consequence of relinquishing the external world, because it feels too
dangerous and frustrating. Meltzer suggests that possessive jealousy
is a motivation for projective identification, observing how it is seen
“in the autistic children and in children whose drive to maturity is
very low, so that they wish either to remain infantile or to die. This
means in their unconscious to return-to-sleep-inside-mother” (1967,
p. 15). This omnipotent phantasy is at the core of states of retraction
and somnolence.
We can differentiate between a wish to sleep inside the mother and
a state in which some “parts may be left behind in the womb, produc-
ing states of withdrawal quite different in phenomena from those of
projective identification” (1992a, p. 127). I think that when there is a
predominance of intrusiveness—as in the head/breast compartment
of the internal mother—we find states of Oblomovian lassitude, where
the subject has no desire to leave the object because he enjoys indo-
lence. In the case of isolation due to unborn parts of the personality,
however, the baby shows an absence of stimulation or incentive to
come out into the world and appears drowsy. According to Meltzer,
some parts have not been born due to a failure in the aesthetic con-
flict between baby and mother. He relates this to the possibility that
puerperal depression may be more common than is reported; if a baby
does not find a passionate containing object, it is likely that it will fail
to be born complete.
Whether it is returning to the womb or not wanting to abandon it,
there is a universal phantasy that there is no experience of need in the
womb because a constant provision makes the notion of lack or desire
impossible. Nevertheless it is possible:
to experience unpleasantness, sometimes violence linked to sudden
alterations of the biochemistry of the environment as it is also possi-
ble to feel comfortable in a stable environment protected from exter-
nal interference. These differences in intrauterine life will surely
have an influence on growth, strength and vitality, on the capacity
to take an interest in the objects that are present in the womb and
in the last instance on the way in which he can be prepared for the
delivery and life. [Caccia, 2007, p. 60]
The phantasy of lost paradise, together with the fear of conflicts in the
external world, can attract and sustain mental states characterized by
Gerard
Sixteen-year-old Gerard had been a “good boy”, adapting well to
school and belonging to a latency group. Once he reached puberty, he
isolated himself after getting into trouble with the police: he had been
caught stealing from supermarkets and touching-up girls while steal-
ing their mobile telephones. For Gerard, the external world was full of
dangers, and he experienced his sessions as approaching the horizon
and being about to fall into hell. His parents were confused profession-
als with a problematic relationship and were very annoyed with their
frustrating adolescent. It seemed that his entry into adolescence was
not supported by objects capable of containing him; his parents found
it unbearable to see him too isolated to sustain a therapeutic process,
and they decided to interrupt his therapy and give him medication.
The “case or sheath as an exo-skeleton” (Bion, 1991, p. 431) that Gerard
chose for himself was a caricature of a Wild West bandit: his thin arms
always looked ready to draw his pistols. He was so terrorized that he
could find refuge only within the four walls of his room.
Certain ways of locking oneself in a room can be as serious as
those found in the psychotic states linked to addictions and video
games (Rosenfeld, 2001). Some isolated adolescents with a borderline
psychotic psychopathology manage to go through life appearing to
be well-adjusted, enjoying successful academic or sporting lives but
enduring great difficulties in establishing intimate relationships. Their
painful isolation surprises those who know them. Gerard was an
educated, studious, and well-adjusted child, and we can assume that
he left latency in a pseudo-mature state; while his brothers played
football, he studied philosophy and discussed politics. This enabled
him to avoid juvenile emotional turbulence and allowed him to look at
the world with arrogance and judge his friends, relatives, and neigh-
bours. In some sense, this arrogance is characteristic of adolescents,
because it is the consequence of confusional states. In the same way,
however, just as all adolescents go through confusional states, this can
become chronic when one tries to escape the limitations of the self and
an attempt is made to “seize an object’s identity by intrusion into it”
(Meltzer, 1973a, p. 53).
Alexander
Another young man, Alexander, has been living an isolated life for
many years. At first, he was the idealized object of his parents’ hopes,
but this changed when his brothers were born. Each new baby became
their idealized love object while, at the same time, they kept bundling
the older siblings together as if to confirm the infantile phantasy that,
“Adults are fascinated when they have babies, but later they drop
them, and it all boils down to the fact that what they really like is mak-
ing babies, simply the product” (Meltzer, 2002, p. 30). Jealousy and
resentment stimulated a wish in Alexander not to become excluded,
and he split his world in such a way that, in relation to his mother, he
developed a narcissistic identification and became her counsellor and
support. With his father, on the other hand, he became a dutiful son,
and, while he tolerated his father’s contempt by placating his brutality,
he managed to stay near him and become the manager of the family
business, without feeling that he had earned it because of his intellect
or his managerial skills. He was unable to have a relationship with his
sisters and had a sadomasochistic relation with his older brother. His
life took place in the greatest solitude.
During adolescence he tried to establish friendly relations, but
these, somehow, wore off. Internet pornography became his refuge
from isolation in which women would get undressed and praise his
penis. But, terrified by the prospect of a physical encounter, he never
went beyond the internet. Only intrusive identification allowed
him to overcome his isolation, but these invaded objects, far from
improving his feelings of safety, undermined his personality and
made him increasingly distrustful and fearful of meeting other peo-
ple. Through his therapy, he has been able, partially, to restore his
internal objects, in which he has come to trust. This has allowed
him to give up his masturbatory activity and his use of marijuana,
diminish his total isolation, and become increasingly dedicated to
other children. He has, laboriously, managed to create a group of
friends from whom he still feels the pain of being excluded, from
time to time. All this does not mean that he has become able to
tolerate emotional pain; when this increases, he flees, preferring to
placate his persecuting paternal object rather than rebelling: being
left alone is more persecuting than being hurt or going out into the
external world. As long as the pleasure from the different compart-
ments of the internal mother protect and gratify him, he will con-
tinue to avoid the external world in which his parental objects are
not very interested in discovering their son’s individuality.
In the same way that Alexander forced his way into his objects
through masturbatory intrusiveness and, in doing so, acquired a
pseudo-identity, there are situations in which adolescents become
vulnerable to the intrusiveness of other people. This is the equivalent
of being swallowed up by an “aspired projective identification”, as
occurs with the psychopathology of the folie à deux, where mixed
projective identifications make it difficult to create an emotional link
with one member of the collusive couple.3 For the adolescent girl, this
might entail an inseparable, twin-like alliance with her mother, almost
sharing an identity. This might revolve around their beauty, expressed
by their referring to themselves in terms of “we, we, we” (Meltzer &
Harris, 1998). These perverse4 alliances can have different objectives,
such as exploiting others with the bait of their beauty or intelligence
or delinquent plotting. In these cases, the family group has become the
adolescent gang, for whom transgression offers shared excitement. It
becomes very difficult for an adolescent immersed in this delinquent
culture to abandon the group and join the real adolescent community;
the adolescent may feel that he or she is betraying his or her family by
rejecting the erotization of orgiastic family life.
Hector
This is what Hector believed when he struggled to prevent his
bipolar mother walking around the house wearing only her knickers
and feeling free to touch his body, something that his father did noth-
ing to prevent. He was frequently sandwiched naked between his pro-
miscuous parents—three polymorphous “adolescents”—in a state of
shared excitement. This did not prevent Hector from feeling isolated;
with his excessive greed and jealousy, he struggled to make friends.
Hector’s imprisonment in his family gang only offered temporary
relief that could not protect him from urgently seeking any object that
would fill the vacuum. It seemed as if Hector would not have to worry
about his future because he would inherit the small business, as well
as the various family properties. The family pact was “us against the
world”, but this did not soothe his sense of isolation and helplessness.
His compulsive eating, drinking, promiscuity, and cocaine abuse was
not enough to fill his emptiness. For Hector, other children were not
so much rivals but objects for consumption. As the geographical con-
fusions and the symbiosis in the family were dissolved, his anxiety
increased, and he expressed a great desire to possess the analyst as a
transference object. He said in a session: “I get annoyed about having
to pay €2 for parking even when I have a nice car, I get annoyed about
paying €2.5 for a beer in a nice place. I hate mankind, it is desperate.
I would like to eat you up whole so that you become only mine and
nobody else’s.”
Once he recovered from his anaesthetized state, born out of the
confusion with his family gang, his needs increased as well as his
understanding of his mental states, and he could recognize his wish
that everything should be his for free. There was no hunger attached to
the erotized family sandwich, but the moment he separated and had to
pay for himself, he became furious. Not so long ago, Hector dreamed
that he was robbing a bank. When he reached the safe, he noticed it was open
and that other thieves were already present. At this point, he lost interest and
left because he wanted the loot just for himself, he wanted to monopolize the
object. After a lot of work, Hector is recognizing that he cannot relate
or love in that state of mind. “I feel as if I am in a big continent, in the
shape of a bowl or a cone, and that everything that falls into it is for
me, but I cannot feel love, I feel I have no love for others, or for you.”
Conclusion
Adolescence is a time when maturational processes are synthesized
and when the gates open towards adult life. If the adolescent arrives
at this point with a faulty centre of gravity, the passage towards adult-
hood will require a restructuring that is likely to have an uncertain
outcome.
I believe that many of the disturbances suffered by isolated ado-
lescents fall into the category of borderline psychotic states that are
always linked to geographical confusions. In my opinion, overcoming
these states of isolation requires going through stages that have been
avoided out of fear of emotional turbulence. The itinerary that leads
to maturity was clearly described by Meltzer when he wrote:
the resolution of this configuration of object relations stands as the
border between mental illness (psychosis) and mental health, just as
the resolution of the obstacles to the dependent introjective relation
to the breast traverses the border between mental instability and
mental stability, and as the passing of the oedipus complex leads
from immaturity to maturity. [1967, p. 22]
In order for the isolated adolescent to resolve conflicts at every stage,
he or she must rely on the containing capacity of the analyst, because
“if an analyst can bear to persevere when geographical confusions are
in the forefront of the transference he will certainly be rewarded with
progress” (p. 22).
Notes
1. “Introjections that are linked to the figure of an unconditional friend are of
great importance as the basis that is used for the choice of a loved object, and we
could almost say that it is the most important identificatory substratum” (Rios,
1985, p. 508).
2. “When a particular infantile part, or organization of them, seizes upon
consciousness and dominates a person’s behaviour, temporarily, say, the sense-
of-identity is bound to be oppressed by the loneliness, however defiant, of the
child-in-the-adult-world” (Meltzer, 1971, p. 202).
Clara Nemas
“So these are my ideas on supervision. You can see [it] is not like
a master class in music. It is more of a participation—more like
playing in the orchestra; just contributing . . .”
C
ontact with the work and the person of Donald Meltzer has
produced in all who met him not only a strong conviction
in the value of the psychoanalytic method and a most vivid
approach to our clinical work with children, adolescents, and adults,
but also changes to our view of the world, of life, and of the human
being. All this made of each contact with Meltzer an emotional learn-
ing experience: a K-link, to use Bion’s term. But it was in his super-
visions—“eye openers”, as Francis Tustin once called them—that one
could feel the passion, the capacity for observation, and the “real proof
of his experience” and creativity full at work. I must say that none of
this would have been possible without the generosity of Benito López,
our teacher for so many years, who first introduced us, in Buenos
Aires, to the work of Meltzer and, later, to his dear friend, the man
himself in person.
108
Vignette 1
A young colleague brings to the supervision her difficulties with a
patient who has a history of abandoning analytic treatments in the
past. The patient received psychotherapy at the age of 4 years after her
mother’s death, but all she could recall was not liking the therapist.
At the age of 17 years, she was referred to a group for eating disor-
ders, but she stopped going because she found it too invasive and not
containing. More recently, she started another treatment which soon
ended when she felt insulted by the therapist; she did not feel under-
stood, attributing this to the fact that she came from a small provincial
town whereas the therapist came from the big city.
The supervisee is not Argentinian; she has recently come from
another country and has a distinctive Central American accent. When
the patient contacting the analyst by phone and heard her accent, she
expressed doubt whether the analyst, being from another country,
would be able to understand her. The analyst suggested they have an
initial meeting and see how she felt, which the patient accepted.
In the first interview, the patient was very hostile. She would not
allow the analyst to finish her phrases, interrupted her whenever she
tried to say something, and, on the occasions when she did listen to
her, denigrated her comments. Nevertheless—and not surprisingly—
she agreed to start therapy. When the analyst tried to describe the
hostile atmosphere present in the sessions, the patient would reply, “I
don’t know what are you talking about! This does not happen to me!
This is your problem!”
In supervision, the analyst commented: “In my countertransfer-
ence I felt uncomfortable with the way she was treating me; I tried
to get close to her, but each time I felt that my interpretations were
rebuffed and even attacked. I felt doomed to join the list of her failed
therapists, and I realized that this had become like a challenge I had
to overcome, which complicated my analytic attitude and neutral-
ity.” We were aware that, despite her good reputation, the analyst
had struggled to get referrals; unfairly, people may have wondered
whether she might one day have to interrupt treatment and return to
her own country. This had caused her pain, as she was a committed
and responsible analyst. As her supervisor, I felt that this situation
was weighing heavily in the countertransference, creating feelings of
annoyance and exhaustion while, at the same time, the analyst was
becoming preoccupied with keeping the patient in treatment. On the
patient’s side, we knew that her mother had died when she was little,
Vignette 2
Ana, an 11 year-old patient, has been in analysis for five years now.
The treatment started at the time the parents were in the middle of a
turbulent divorce. The school suggested the consultation. The patient
was a first-class student who also excelled in sport and art, but her
communication was impeded by her persistent thumb-sucking, which
had already provoked a degree of malformation in her palate and
teeth. From the beginning, her face showed no emotion and she wore
a fixed smile. In her sessions, she built elaborate constructions and
small-scale models that she kept tidily in her box, together with the
strips of paper she had used to make them.
Years passed before Ana showed any emotion in the sessions; she
could cry and express feelings of anger and frustration, but her thumb
remained in her mouth, despite continued efforts to understand this
fixed symptom. In the supervision, I proposed talking to the thumb-
in-the-mouth as if it were another character in the session. How did
the thumb feel inside the mouth, and how did it think it might feel if
it went outside the mouth? Was it scared to leave its sanctuary? Was
it curious about what went on outside? Would it be able to get back
inside if it felt too scared outside? All of a sudden all these questions
started coming up, until we could both empathize with the fear of the
thumb getting out of its refuge as a personification of the unborn baby
part of the personality, which did not dare to come out into the world.
The analyst started playing this game in the sessions, and it took
some time for the girl to engage in it. The therapist would personify
the finger, talking in the first person, and Ana would speak for the
other characters in the mouth: the teeth, tongue, and lips. Sometimes
they changed roles. They started making up stories in the session
about the thumb who had been adopted by this mouth family. Mostly,
the thumb felt safe but sometimes it felt threatened by foreign things
coming in, such as the moving tongue or the biting teeth.
The thumb-in-the-mouth became a subject of shared interest
between analyst and patient. It was also an invariant picture of a part
of the personality in the way that sometimes happens with dreams,
as Melanie Klein points out in her wonderful text “Personification in
the Play of Children” (1929), becoming a shorthand not only between
analyst and patient but also throughout the supervision.
Vignette 3
Mark, who had been in analysis for six years, was a very disturbed
young man who demanded a strong psychoanalytic conviction from
the analyst. The latter has been well contained by her supervisor in
all the years of this treatment, finding the courage to treat Mark with
firmness and commitment. She had one supervision with me when
she visited Buenos Aires, after presenting the same patient to me at a
clinical seminar some months earlier in New York. I shall focus on one
particular aspect of this encounter. This is what she brought:
position and the presence of the good lost object as the nucleus of the
self. Remembering had an active quality that, in this patient, seemed
to be closer to a superego demand in response to the fear of losing
the object. The supervisee shared thoughts that our conversation
had evoked in her about the Christian faith (which were new to me),
and she brought in a new perspective from which to understand the
patient and his violent defences.
Vignette 4
This vignette describes a situation in which a disruption in the atmos-
phere of the supervision created a new way to understand clinical
material. The work between supervisor and this intuitive and experi-
enced supervisee was usually characterized by good communication
and receptivity on both sides.
The analyst brought a session of a patient who was having difficul-
ties becoming pregnant. She had consulted doctors at a fertility clinic,
who could find no organic cause, but the patient insisted on the idea of
being ill. The analyst was caught between a description of the medical
procedures involved in the consultation and the problems the patient
had relating to the doctors. The analyst felt that it had been difficult
to keep the analytic focus in the session, but she did not realize the
enactment that was taking place. Each time I tried to draw her atten-
tion to this fact, I felt that she, uncharacteristically, seemed to defend
herself and her approach to the material. I found myself feeling that I
was facing an aspect of the analyst I had not encountered before, won-
dering if I had neglected to realize that there had been some mutual
idealization going on.
I remained silent until it occurred to me that we were having a
fertility problem in our supervision. I told the analyst that it was as if
there was a condom that was preventing the sperm from penetrating
the ovum in the session with her patient and that, somehow, the same
situation was repeating itself in the supervision. The atmosphere in
the supervision changed, and the analyst became aware of the weight
this situation had in her countertransference.
Conclusion
In “Constructions in Analysis” (1937d), Freud introduces the notion
of constructions, as distinct from interpretation, wherein the analyst
may need, in certain situations, to reconstruct a part of the patient’s
for a mind of one’s own, an identity, and a style of life. Only by tak-
ing into account the anxieties elicited by a three-dimensional creative
growing—as opposed to socializing or fakeness of projective identifi-
cation or pseudo-maturity—we may develop the true capacity to bear
uncertainties and difficult questions.
The challenge we meet in supervision is to generate a space in the
thin line where imagination may develop at the same time that our
work takes anchor in the basic analytic technique and theory.
Bion, in Taming Wild Thoughts (1997), talks about this apparent
paradox:
Unless the analyst allows himself the exercise of his speculative
imagination he will not be able to produce conditions in which a
germ of scientific ideas can flourish. . . . So while I say that it is
extremely important to exercise your imagination, to let it go, to give
it a chance to flourish, at the same time keep it under some sort of
discipline. [pp. 46, 48]
Working in other contexts where psychoanalysis is still developing, we
are more overtly confronted with the problem of colonization: how to
help somebody to develop a mind of his or her own and, at the same
time, understand what is truly analytic beyond more cultural and
historical facts. It is important to be able to understand how much of
our identifications, how much of our more conscious elections, enter
into the process of developing a mind of one’s own. Perhaps it is the
work of a lifetime.
Note
1. I am very grateful to Nancy Moreno Dueñas, Adela Vinocur, and Alison
Bruce for allowing me to use material from their supervision for this presentation.
Monica Vorchheimer
A memory
I
t was during Donald Meltzer’s last visit to Buenos Aires that I had
the privilege of being supervised by him on a very complicated
clinical case that I was treating at that time. A young homosexual
woman had been coming for treatment and had developed a very
open and intense transference love: “A very complicated woman;
very sensual, very erotic, and very homosexual. What is going to
happen with her rude demand of and erotic and sensual gratification
towards her analyst? We will see. But she seems armed to the teeth
against parental care from her analyst.” In this way Meltzer opened
his comment and shared with us his usual very interesting ideas; but
there was one in particular that I kept in mind, echoing down time.
He said: “This could be thought of as resistance, but it is not. I guess
that she has opened the door to her pornographic concern and to her
masochism and she wants you to feel guilty for having profited from
the opened door which allowed you to investigate what has been
revealed. . . . Everything is opened here, there is no resistance.” And
119
later he added, “All the material is now present for Monica to explore
as slowly or as fast as she would wish . . .”.
This was going to be a challenge, a real test of the courage to
analyse—the courage that one can find over and over again through-
out his work. I always considered that the image described in The
Psychoanalytical Process (1967) regarding the analytic attitude as a ten-
sion close to the limit would acquire new shades in the “Routine and
Inspired Interpretations” paper (1973c). I have tended to go back to
the latter more than once, whenever I feel the need to shake off the
drowsiness that sometimes hampers my interest or my creativity, in
order to recover an awakened state that allowed me to return to the
analytic listening “close to the limit”.
Exploring as slowly or as fast as I would wish—is it merely a mat-
ter of wishing? This question becomes especially crucial with certain
patients, such as the so-called protracted latents who get settled in
analysis with a phantasy of an endless process, in a kind of mother–
baby secret love affair “until death do us part”.
Etchegoyen suggests that all phenomena that fall under these head-
ings might coalesce into a psychoanalytic impasse, which must be
differentiated from the non-coercible resistance of the patient and the
technical mistakes of the analyst. The true impasse is described as
neither of these two but, rather, as a total halt of the analytic process
achieved by the converging forces of acting out, reversible perspective,
and NTR. As we know, destructive emotions are the basis of these
anti-process manoeuvres, which I view as quite distinct from the kind
of features I am trying to convey with the patients I am describing.
It is clinically important to distinguish the stasis of an impasse
from the motionlessness of protracted latency within the analytic
situation. The cause for the latter can be found in a detention in the
threshold of adolescence which is actualized in the transference situa-
Dream-life as experimentation
After a long time, however, movement begins to find representation
in dream-life, in spite of what appears to be immobilized in daily life.
Dreams flourish during the night and can be expanded in the sessions
during the exploratory phase, opening the door to the roots of infantile
sexuality. At first, the analyst’s enthusiasm cannot be satisfactorily
transformed into clear interpretations; although conjectures might be
plausible, it is not yet clear to whom they can be communicated. A
sense of artificiality permeates the consulting room, while the thera-
pist can be suspected of nymphomania or promiscuity, insofar as his
or her curiosity is excited by these dreams. Paraphrasing Meltzer, it
could be said that “all the material is there”, but due to projective
identification—with powerful effects on the countertransference—it
is difficult to find a way of approaching the material without falling
into interpretative stereotypes or formulations inducing the patient
to action. There is a strong feeling of sterility despite the richness of
the oneiric production, as the material only confirms what has been
examined on numerous occasions.
With the patient mentioned above, in the transference, I was either
the mother who would teach her how to be a woman and please her
husband or the laboratory medical-father who would fecundate her
without sex. I remember fighting against my own lethargy, trying to
stay awake and alert, and always being grateful when she brought
dreams that could enliven the dull atmosphere in the consulting room.
Once, she dreamt that
she was in a little town in the countryside with her daughter who was
learning how to ride on horseback; they passed by a church; people were
going inside for Mass, and the girl wanted to go inside with the horse
but they passed by. There was a woman standing on the pavement, and
the patient shouted at her daughter that she should be careful.
As we can see, from time to time, the problem of learning and expe-
riencing novelty and voluptuousness came up in her dreams, and the
transference scenario moved to the outside—the countryside—where
it is possible to achieve speed in development, although experimenta-
tion and exploration could still be dangerous and castration or a cut
could be discovered. The temptation to go back to religious–ascetic–
church life remained present.
In these patients, the question about how passions are aroused
seems to be a central enigma, mirroring the analyst’s search for a way
to reach the self while modulating temperature, distance, rhythm, and
speed. Fortunately, however, difficulties in analytic communication
can be also recollected in the oneiric scenario: the search for the penis
can be suspicious, introjection might be cheating, and something may
elude the analyst; corruption of the analytic method can be reported
in dream features like feeding the children rotten food, telephones that
don’t work, not being able to bring together the feminine, the mas-
culine, the feeding-creative function, keeping the objects apart, and
maintaining bisexuality split off. If infantile polymorphism cannot be
integrated, neither can bisexuality, and the mystery of how passions
are aroused cannot be accessed either.
In the transference and countertransference situation, this has been
created by the diminished creativity of interpretative activity, as a
result of the dissociation of the combined object in the analyst’s mind.
Instead, the analyst provided corrupted interpretations—out-of-date
food—desexualized and devitalized, in which the fertile and potent
penis (and testicles with good “milk”) was kept at a distance, unable to
communicate with the bitch-sensual-woman. In this way, the desexu-
alization of the parental figures of latency was reproduced, and no
passionate quality could emerge in the transference. But in dreams,
this problem finds expression.
As time passes, and dreams help to clarify the transference–coun-
tertransference field, there are indications that something new is being
mobilized in the oneiric scenario; new experiences (rejected while
awake) come to life while sleeping. Surprisingly, in dream-life, a new
capacity to experience excitement and attraction towards the other sex
becomes more vivid, in contrast to what seems to be a small, pale flame
during waking life or in the transference relationship. “It looks as if,
in dreams, I dare to live what I cannot while awake”, a patient vividly
commented, as if quoting the epigraph: “The dream that invents me
has its eyes wide open. / And I close my eyes to look at the world”
(Pontalis, 2011, p. 16). In the same session, he had realized that he had
Discussion
During a conference of the Asociación Psicoanalítica de Buenos Aires
in 1995, Meltzer suggested that emotions are first experienced as body
states and it is only gradually that those states find their way to expres-
sion through symbols that can be dreamed. However, sometimes
things seem to follow an inverse direction. Body states and sensations
appear initially in dreams, allowing the experimentation that had been
avoided in development to unfold, as found in clinical cases such as
the ones described above (Meltzer, 1997).
The oneiric space, the “real and vital” dream experience, is the
space where confusions can be sorted out and knowledge can be
accessed. Although for many years patients, like children, had drawn
upon rather conventional symbols in dreams, it is unclear whether
those symbols could transmit more than “the shade of emotions” or
uncoloured metaphors, as Meltzer suggested on that occasion. How-
ever, after years of repetition with weak and subtle transformations,
something starts to move. It is reminiscent of how children enjoy lis-
tening to the same stories thousands of times over; perhaps patients
provide dream material for the analyst to tell them their stories over
and over again, never quite sure whether the value is in the lyrics
or the music. As children begin to prefer writing their own stories,
there comes a time when patients appropriate their own dreams as a
Jeanne Magagna
T
he structure of time is inextricably linked with the concept of
hope. In the beginning of a baby’s life there is hope, hope for
a communion with the mother and father as loving, protective,
caring figures. Alongside this hope is a preconception that the breast
will meet the baby’s requirements to be nourished. As the baby grows
and matures, holding on to this hope is both difficult and dangerous;
hope can be filled with too much greed to possess all of a mother’s and
father’s life. Hope can involve a constitutional incapacity to tolerate
the frustration of waiting for mother’s reappearance, filled with rage
at mother for not being attuned to the baby’s rhythm of communicat-
ing needs. Hope is then submerged beneath rage and disappointment
with mother. Hope, which promised a future of contentment, then
becomes despair. Without hope, there is no sense of the future, just the
disappointing present or the yearning for a moment in the past that
was experienced as good.
In meeting a child for the first time with his or her parents, I am
acutely attuned to the sense of the child’s expectation in meeting me.
In that first look, I see hope, curiosity, and anticipation that, perhaps,
with this new person, some different experience may occur. Alterna-
tively, I see fear or even terror that something dreadful will certainly
happen in my presence. I, too, have a sense of suspenseful anticipation
as I get to know the child. In this first meeting I am the recipient of
127
Upon entering the therapy room, I give Mia a box of play materials
and explain that the materials are for her to use during her time
with me. Mia ignores the toys and hastily grabs the sellotape and
all the crayons. Immediately she begins a detailed drawing of the
room and all its contents (Figure 9.1). She draws the chairs, the
tables, the sellotape and then a detailed picture of me. I occupy
the same space as the desk and the chair.
and I return Mia to her mother. A few minutes later, the door bursts
open. Mia is there shouting “pig” once again. She subsequently
races to the exit where her mother is standing.
The plasticine cover, the felt-tip colour, and the “make-up mummy
beautiful face” are part of Mia’s attempt to retreat from any experi-
ence of being a kind of stick figure without much inner substance,
flesh, or even human shape. These covers are attempts to “hold
herself together” in the face of some terrible sense of fragmentation
and potential sense of loss of the mother-therapist (Rhode, 1997).
Mia’s unconscious aim is to obliterate time, a sense of waiting and
the psychic terror and pain she feels when she experiences herself
as a separate person.
Around the third and fourth week, it is clear that Mia is more vehe-
mently using me as a kind of “dump” into which she evacuates parts
of her blistered self (Tustin, 1990). For example:
Fourth week
At the beginning of one session she immediately rushes to the jug
of water. Pouring water on the couch, she declares, “Pouring water
on your bed.” She then takes off her shoes and socks. Racing to
the desk, she grabs her box and dumps all its contents on the floor
saying, “I’m messing up your room again.” She pours glue all over
and attempts to kick me. When I restrain her, she bites me.
But then she steps on a pieces of fence and cries, complaining, “It
pricked me.” When I describe her feeling that she has messed the
inside of the mummy and now she feels she is being pricked and
made to cry, Mia responds, “Yes, it hurts me.” This is a sobering
moment, for she has put her pain into words.
In the next session, Mia puts the fence pieces in a bucket and places
them under the couch along with other hard toys. She explains, “It
Fifth week
During the fifth week, Mia spends some of the sessions curled up
under the blanket on the couch. I feel she is beginning to become
painfully aware as we part of her sense of loss, her feeling of bod-
ily damage. She feels helpless without a sturdy internal psychic
structure enabling her to consider that there will be another session
and another. This internal rhythm of safety could hold her flood of
despair and terror arising through fragmentation of the self in the
time apart. I talk about Mia wanting a “blanket-mummy” to hold
her together and make her feel safe. At the end, Mia cries, pleading
that she does not want to go. Later in the corridor, as she meets
her mother, she becomes very subdued and begins sobbing. Mia
complains that her mother has not brought her scarf. This is the
very first day I have seen Mia crying. As she cries in front of her
mother, I feel that “Now there is a crying baby, a baby crying for
‘a mummy’ to hold her pain and protect her.”
I remember mother saying in the first interview that Mia didn’t cry
much as a baby. Now Mia is having a different experience with her
mother, who is supported by a therapist and me. For a moment, the
obliteration of time through sensuousness ceases. There is an acute
experience of an emotional point that is held still. There is a tie and
some internal space in which Mia feels sadness and fear of being with-
out a protective cover. Emerging in Mia is a new concept of “mother”,
the concept of a mother who can experience and understand her sad-
ness and fear of loss of a protective mother.
Mia’s new concept of mother transforms her basic structure of
time. There is now the possibility of a sense of time followed by a
Sixth week
Mia arrives for the first time with a watch on. She requests that
I take care of her “ticker” in my drawer. Shortly afterwards she
asks me to keep her large red button earrings safe in my drawer
during the session. I feel that I am to be a kind of “special secure
place” for those saner parts of herself that she wishes to protect
from the chaos created by her confusion and continual messing of
the room. There is just a hint of Mia’s developing internalization
of a “talking mummy with a protective space inside her”. Mia
alternates between messing the entire room and lying completely
under the couch cover using my words as an additional blanket
to wrap round herself.
Mia has little capacity for symbolic thought; she makes concrete her
notion of time passing and a future time—when we will meet again—
by asking me to keep safe her watch and her earrings. She cannot keep
the notion of our time together safely inside her, and, at the beginning
of each session, she recapitulates the internal process that has occurred
during the separation from me. Rage at separation and subsequent
fragmentation of the introjected experience of feeling understood by
me creates internal chaos. Upon entering the room, Mia regularly feels
compelled to throw toys and water all over the room, thrusting her
furious chaos into me. I must say I feel very confused and disoriented
when her mind is in this state, but after I receive this chaos and lend
thought to it, Mia can begin to elaborate on other experiences:
over to me, and explains that she wants to “push the button on my
head”. I describe how she wants to push the button on my head in
order to take control of my speaking and thinking with her. I say
she wishes she could turn me on and turn me off as she wished.
This is primitive omnipotent control, showing me that she cannot tol-
erate the terror of being dependent on a good figure. When she experi-
ences me as someone who is helpful, she feels compelled to hold on to
me in this way. This increased dependence on the mother-therapist is
heralded by more severe anxieties about feeling helpless outside the
sessions. Mia cries plaintively when she leaves this session.
At this time of intensifying dependence on me, Mia also begins
to experience sleeping difficulties. She says she likes sleeping in my
room and would like to sleep there every night, because my room does
not have cracks in it like her room. The external object (the therapist)
gives her the feeling of a soothing protective space, a space without
cracks. However, with her depressed mother outside the therapy, Mia
feels she is left with a faulty, cracked, internal psychic container that
does not protect her.
the good but idealized mother in a good internal space. The second
possibility is that Mia slips into being projectively identified with a
mother looking after a baby whom she is wheeling in a pram rather
than a mother preoccupied with her appearance, as at the beginning
of therapy. The third possibility is that, following a destructive attack
on the disappearing “mother-therapist”, there is an upsurge of loving,
reparative wishes mitigating Mia’s hate. When this predominance of
Mia’s love for a good internalized mother-therapist mitigates her jeal-
ous attack, she produces these more symbolic drawings. The flow of
the lines suggests that her thoughts and feelings are becoming inte-
grated in a more spontaneous and creative manner (Jackson, 2015).
Thus it is possible to envisage separation from a loved mother-
therapist as a crucial moment for Mia to regress or to develop.
Transformations in time
Assessment of a child’s therapeutic progress involves examining the
changing nature of the time-limiting role of the father, which permits
the mother-therapist to have a creative space to join the father and
other babies. The role of the father also ensures a regular rhythm of
therapy sessions, at a particular time, for a particular period. Near the
end of treatment, there may be a resurgence of love and a capacity to
appreciate the notion of time; the child may begin to feel supported
by the father’s protective role in setting and maintaining limits. There
is also, often, a resurgence of hate and the potent, perilous feelings
aroused by the time limit proposed by the end of therapy.
For this reason, I shall now describe the end of our psychotherapy
to show the fluctuations in Mia’s concept of time.
does another drawing, transforming “tree trunk neck” into a tree with
flowing branches (Figure 9.10).
In a subsequent session, Mia says, “You have a child already lined
up to be with you when I leave . . . and you will see this child forever
and take her go home with you.” A series of sessions then occur in
which Mia tries to break the window, and she hits me. Regression has
occurred in the face of the ending.
Mia seems to allow me to have my therapeutic capacities when
apart from her, but she wants to imprison me in my consulting room
Mia asks me, “Do you remember yesterday when I put you in a
prison with me?” I acknowledge her wish to stay with me. She then
wants to imprison me in the corner of the room furthest from the
door, saying, “I am going to lock you in. I can get out.” I say, “You
can put into words now what you feel.” She adds, “I am going
to leave you all the mess and you can clean up and I am going
to have a fantastic feast and you are not invited!” Mia covers me
with the couch blanket, declaring, “Your eyes are to see beautiful
things with and you can use your mouth to talk, but the rest of
your body is old, mouldy, and rotting and your head is covered
with old grey hairs.”
I respond, describing how Mia sees the future: “Yesterday you
talked about the mother with lovely white mummy breasts who
took care of babies in a good, loving way. Today you are impris-
oning me so that I can have no future with the daddy, no babies,
no other patients. I am to be your therapist and only a therapist.
It seems this is the only way you can hold the mummy-therapist
forever good inside you.”
Mia subsequently recounts two dreams: “First, I had a dream of
your being a prisoner locked up with only bread and water. Only your
head was showing in the dream. Then I had a dream of your dying
and ending up with the devils who burn you up. Afterwards you are
only ashes.”
I describe how it is very hard to keep me good inside her, saying,
“The baby’s rage is spoiling me leaving nothing good inside.” I
suggest, “We can keep each other in mind forever, you know.”
as she says, “Do you remember when I was seven? Do you remem-
ber when I was eight?” She implies by her calmness that she has
outgrown all her wild messing of me and the room. “Yes, now you
can think about and talk about your feelings. It feels very different
inside. It feels safer now when you hold onto the good-mummy-
therapist who thinks with you.”
she imprisons the mother and burns her mother with her rage; then
there is no sense of the future, only the death of the object. Hate and
possessiveness are feelings easier to experience than the vulnerability
and pain of losing the therapist.
In retrospect, I would say that a much longer time in therapy is
needed for such a very ill child in such an uncontaining family setting
to consolidate any internal developments made in the course of ther-
apy. The question of how the patient will live in the future is always
uncertain, although I do know that, by the end of her psychotherapy,
Mia had gained an appreciation of her responsibility for trying to
protect her good internal parents from her possessiveness and rage.
Mia wrote a note suggesting that I had given three and a half years
of my life to help her to grow up. She also acknowledged that in some
ways she had grown to be nice and in some ways she was still nasty.
The future is determined by how Mia will be nurtured and understood
and how she will allow me to have my freedom away from her. Most
important is the question of how she can forgive me for what we were
not able to do together to foster her psychological development.
Hope for the future is really based upon recognizing and protecting
the goodness of the internal mother. This comes through having suf-
ficient love to forgive the internal mother and father and allow them
the possibility of being a procreative couple.
Time for the ending of therapy should arise in the therapist’s mind
when he or she senses that the possibility of preserving the good
internal parents has arisen within the child’s mind. As in the ending
of Mia’s therapy, however, when such total internal destruction of
the good object occurs and there is no possibility of going through
the mourning process, the therapist needs to reconsider the notion of
“time for the ending of therapy”. Whatever external crises arise, hope
for the future can exist only through the taming of destructive feelings
so that the internal parents can be restored to goodness and remain
understanding and protective throughout life.
Final hypothesis
Until there is a perception of a good object, separate from the external
good object, life is emotionally hazardous with unpredictable out-of-
control feelings. The introjection of the good object, and the maintain-
ing of its goodness while allowing the primary object to be absent,
produces three profoundly important transformations in the child’s
experience of time (Colarusso, 1979).
Jeffrey L. Eaton
F
or Donald Meltzer, dreaming is at the centre of the psycho-
analytic experience. He links the exploration of dreams with the
efficacy of the psychoanalytic process. Exploring dream-life helps
to glimpse the way dreaming contributes to what Meltzer calls “the
question of growth”. Here, I offer some personal reflections, inspired
by Meltzer, on the fruitful harvest of dreams and dreaming.
153
a fabulous lost beast. The dream itself can never be recovered, relived,
or represented in any complete way.
Dreaming creates what can be poetically called “a country of the
mind”, a place where we can go, usually while asleep, to yield full
attention to psychic reality. In this new country, we can live a second
life while dreaming. This second life may become a laboratory where
unconscious experiments in living and feeling are carried out each
night. While dreaming, problems are pictured, solutions are sought,
alternatives are realized, and emotions and memories are tested in
ways that may not be available during waking life.
Dreaming may be seen as a form of unconscious play; watching
children play is, perhaps, as close as we can come to observing a
dream being made. The child playing makes a scene populated with
characters, actions, backgrounds, intentions, conflicts, and rapid trans-
formations of anxieties, ideas, fantasies, and emotions. Something
similar takes place in dreaming. The concreteness of the experience of
entering a dream scene lends the experience emotional power, while
the transience of dreaming makes possible the realization of myriad
circumstances.
Even violent impulses can be safely expressed in play and dream-
ing because there is a mysterious process of symbolization develop-
ing—an atmosphere that children call “just pretend” and that adults
call “just a dream”.
Dreaming, then, like playing, opens up a space of psychic freedom.
Somehow, something troubling, or something new, or even something
reaching towards us from the future makes its way into the dream
scene and, from there, sometimes proceeds on into waking conscious-
ness.
Through dreaming and the production of dream thoughts and
events, there is contact with a continuous unfolding unconscious con-
versation between the self and its own experiences. As one continues
to dream, night after night and year after year, autonomous symbols
naturally emerge, and the result is what the poet Shelley (1891) calls
an “expression of the imagination”.
Meltzer emphasizes that we import meaning from one world (the
dream laboratory) into another (the world of our waking lives): dream-
ing leaves the gate to the psyche open every night, and the emotions
generated by dreaming wander back through and linger into the day,
shaping our perceptions.
The many experiences accumulated in the second life of dreaming
influence our choices in waking life. Successful dreaming means that
less pain must be hidden in the body, projected into the group, or
split off, denied, and located in other objects or dimensions. Through
the intimate exploration of the patient’s dreams, shared in the ana-
lytic encounter, waking and dreaming processes begin to cooperate.
Contact with dream-life, paradoxically, helps one to develop a greater
tolerance and deeper respect for the facts of external reality.
The figures and events, the objects and emotions that emerge in dreams
like this one are painful to register and intimidating to behold. The
intensity of the dream strains both the analyst and the patient. Yet at
the same time such dreams are really gifts if one can find the psychic
space necessary to welcome their intensity; if it can become possible to
turn towards the dream, it can become a shared object that takes on its
own life between the patient and the analyst. The shared dream (and
the pain) can then be explored against a new background of gradually
expanding spacious awareness generated by a dreaming couple.
To me, this is the heart of the therapeutic action of psychoanalysis:
the discovery of a symbolizing couple and its growth in the inter-
action between analyst and analysand through a process that can
become repeatedly internalized and elaborated within the patient’s
own dream-life.
Survival
I was driving a truck, a real beater, and even though I was not going over
the speed limit, everything felt really shaky. Then, all of the sudden, the
road collapsed and I and all the others cars plunged into a gigantic hole.
I just kept falling, there was no bottom. I woke up and I couldn’t go back
to sleep.
Meltzer has compellingly described. But that is only one possible fate.
I believe dreams in this domain convey the beginnings of a process
that attempts to make thinkable the unthinkable through picturing
experiences of catastrophic background events.
As an individual’s dream-life evolves in the analytic process,
we may discern plausible patterns that suggest how the person has
unconsciously coped with his or her own personal primal catastrophe.
Dreams such as this one suggest that the patient feels he or she is miss-
ing what I call “a reliable floor” for experience. Without such a floor,
the very possibility of survival is called into question.
I believe that identifying this dimension of the dream background
can be crucial in giving the patient not only a feeling of being under-
stood, but also belief that it is possible and worthwhile for the analytic
couple to face such intimidating pains and go forward together. It
is through sharing these disturbing images and the sensations and
affects that they carry that a gradual trust in the reality of an emotional
floor for experience can be discovered and built.
Adaptation
I’m standing on a stage with a group of men with a giant Nazi flag behind
us. I’m wearing a Nazi uniform. I’m giving the Heil Hitler salute with
everyone else. I’m terrified people will see that my heart’s not really in it.
The theme of adaptation has, of course, a long and complex history. The
concept is central in biological and evolutionary sciences as well as in
some streams of psychoanalysis, especially in ego psychology. As I am
using the term, the dreams in this domain have to do with the struggle
to create personal meaning versus complying with (or being domi-
nated by) the family and the group. Obviously, such a theme involves
a kaleidoscope of issues: obstacles to intimacy and sincere expression
are often pictured, or patients may describe feelings of helplessness
and frustration that coexist with thwarted initiative. There is often the
feeling that the patient may be selling out, settling for less, cutting his
or her losses, being a coward, playing the fool, and running out of time.
Once the issue of survival has been adequately addressed in the
analytic process by successfully creating an emotional floor for expe-
rience, the next domain often involves entering the profound realm
of adaptation to the family, the group, the community, and even to a
particular moment in history. The environment can then be explored
as a multitude of variables that both constrain and facilitate the devel-
opment of the self. Within it, emotions are patterned and processed
through the challenges of adaptation and through increasingly per-
sonal interaction with the environment, which sponsors learning and
maturation across the life span. Dreams help show the history of this
process as well as its progress.
An important aim of the analytic work is to help the patient dif-
ferentiate between contractual interactions (dominant in the adaptive
domain) from opportunities for real intimacy and the development of
sincerity in relationships.
Thriving
I’m in a beautiful place in the mountains with a woman I am in love with.
I have some kind of arithmetic problem I’m trying to solve. I’m trying to
write it out on a large piece of paper on the ground. The woman offers
to help me and I let her. I wake up with a feeling of incredible happiness.
ness opens a new window through which one can observe the evolu-
tion of the self and its circumstances within a psychoanalytic process.
In Meltzer’s view, a dream is a construction that reveals an evolv-
ing continuity, not necessarily within one dream, but over the course
of many dreams strung together in time. This crucial time dimension
can be measured over a span of nights, weeks, months, years, or even
decades. In particular, Meltzer speaks of dreams being able to depict
a problem that the patient is unconsciously trying to work out. If this
can be noticed, he suggests, then a problem can be investigated more
clearly, and with greater depth and nuance. If this psychic complexity
can be tolerated, rather than evaded, then it can be thought about and
reflected upon. This opens another dimension of experience, which
brings with it not only different forms of anxiety, but also, over time,
evidence of transformation and even, in the best instances, novel solu-
tions proposed through the unconscious creativity of the dreamer’s
inner work.
What I think should be highlighted is the way that the background
of the dream evolves into an inner workspace for the organ of con-
sciousness. Dream exploration starts with the analyst getting to know
the patient’s dream-life but may soon become a shared task. The
psychoanalytic setting is a workspace for exploring dream-life, and,
if it is successfully elaborated, a kind of apprenticeship in exploring
psychic reality can unfold. Gradually, dream-life reveals the construc-
tion of an inner workspace, a space capable of welcoming, tolerat-
ing, investigating, and learning from dream events. The patient’s
capacity to recognize this transforming inner workspace is one factor
that makes the creativity of analysis an aesthetic experience for both
participants.
One obstacle to this kind of process would seem to be the discom-
fort that patients, as well as many analysts, feel in handling dream
material. There is a mistaken conviction that one must be able to
decode the dream, rather than simply getting to know it and explor-
ing its details. If an analyst can assume a humble role of curiosity
about the dream and all its particulars, a spontaneous force field of
emergent meaning may be detected. Over time, patterns with linking
power become clearer. Meltzer felt he had a gift for opening dreams
as a generative process, not as a source of fixed meaning.
The process of exploring dreams promotes a deep sense of intimate
cooperation and can inspire an evolving appreciation for the process
of expanding self-awareness. Gradually the patient begins to take
an interest in and develops a desire to actively explore, learn from,
sustain, and protect the analytic project because she realizes that the
analyst is not trying to do something to her, but, instead, is present to
help her discover herself as a complex subject in process.
I met Meltzer only briefly towards the end of his life. He seemed
both vibrantly intelligent as well as irascible. His physical presence
and communicative intensity made a strong impression that still lin-
gers many years later. Meltzer, not unlike some Buddhist teachers,
transmitted something simply through his way of being. I remember
him rather like an old Alaskan miner somewhat hardened by life in the
deep backwoods. I like to picture him in the Alaskan wild, standing
hip deep in the stream of the unconscious, panning for gold. As an
experienced dream explorer, Meltzer would patiently sift the contents
of each dream, taking in all the details before deciding which specific
elements to examine more deeply. He did not seem to be looking for
a single golden nugget (of meaning) but, rather, allowed himself to
become fascinated by all that the dream stream produced and left in
his tin pan (the clinical hour).
In his gruff but often humorous way, Meltzer showed a remark-
able freedom to lend his agile attention to patterns that spontaneously
emerged to him. Once he had described them, the patterns seemed
somehow obvious. He was patient, relaxed and receptive as he repeat-
edly dipped his attention and sifted the material he gathered from the
river of the unconscious (including the natural history of the transfer-
ence). He gave me conviction that it was necessary (not grandiose) to
seek to contact the heart of the matter in any given moment.
For Meltzer, the dream is neither debris nor pure gold but is the
raw material for important inner work. One sensed that Meltzer rel-
ished getting dirty, bending close to the muddy river, and that he was
inspired by its changing conditions across the seasons of life. His inti-
macy with the energy and the landscape of the unconscious showed
him the continuity of the dream process, the unity of the dream itself,
and revealed what he called “the grammar of the dream”. I admired
his profoundly disciplined imagination, and he offered a model of
clinical intuition that remains inspiring.
Psychoanalysis sponsors an improbable conversation, not just
between the analyst and the analysand, but also between the patient’s
discovery of psychic reality and the many complex links to daily life
and the external world. Of immediate interest is the dream’s linking
function in the interaction between the external world and the internal
world, between the body and symbolization, between relationships
with others and how our fantasies unfold, between thinking and feel-
ing, and between getting to know and the illusion of always already
knowing.
There is no such thing as a dream, only a dreaming process. Dream-
ing is a kind of river. You cannot show someone only a part of the river,
except as an abstraction. You can only really speak about moments in
the flow of the river, just as you can only really speak about moments
in the process of dreaming. This is why the focus on process is so
important in understanding the sensibility that Meltzer brought to
understanding dream-life. The habits of language create a distortion of
this larger truth. The better we can describe the actuality of dreaming,
the more we may be open to question the mystery of the experience of
becoming a self. Is there a self that creates a dream and then narrates
it? Or, is dreaming a process that creates a feeling of self which, later,
becomes aware of living moments in the river of dreaming?
In a deepening analytic process the patient will also learn how to
wade out into the river of dreaming. The patient will not idealize the
analyst and wait for him to deliver his knowledge, but will join along-
side him, increasingly able to share the task of gathering and sifting
the dream material for fragments of emotional gold. Meltzer left me
with a visceral sense of the task shared between patient and analyst,
both at work together, waist deep in the river of dreaming, opening
to whatever arrives, surrounded by the wilderness of O.
Notes
This chapter is dedicated to Franco Scabbiolo with gratitude for the many conver-
sations we have shared regarding dreaming and dream-life.
Much of the text in this essay appeared as a chapter in my book A Fruitful
Harvest: Essays after Bion published by The Alliance Press, Seattle, WA, in 2011 and
used by permission.
On having ideas:
the aesthetic object and O
I
should like to talk about what we mean by “having ideas”, using,
as reference points, Donald Meltzer and three of the Kleinian
thinkers who had an influential bearing on his own ideas—Wilfred
Bion, Adrian Stokes, and Roger Money-Kyrle—to sketch a composite
picture of their idea of how we have ideas. This is equivalent to ask-
ing how we learn from our internal objects without being overawed
or inhibited by them. These objects are those advanced aspects of the
mind that are formed progressively from a complex mixture of exter-
nal influences, intimate relationships, and innate internal qualities.
Psychoanalysis, Meltzer said, works through holding “conversa-
tions between internal objects” (see Williams, 2003, p. 219)—those of
analyst and analysand working together to form a symbol for the idea
of the current emotional experience. As the definition of mental health
shifted to one of self-knowledge, the goal of psychoanalysis changed
from one of cure to one of facilitating the formation of ideas, through
the creation of symbols. This endeavour is not unique to psychoa-
nalysis: it runs through all artistic disciplines, as it does through life.
166
Meltzer, especially in his later talks and writing, tried to convey the
“simplicity” of this process, by which he meant its complexity. But the
message itself is a simple one. To quote Martha Harris:
Introjection remains a mysterious process: how do involvement and
reliance upon objects in the external world which are apprehended
by the senses (and, as Wilfred Bion has pointed out, described in
language which has been evolved to deal with external reality)
become assimilated and transformed in the mind into what he calls
“psychoanalytic objects” which can contribute to the growth of the
personality? This is a process about which we have almost every-
thing to learn. [Harris, 1978b, p. 176]
The great psychoanalytic thinkers are well aware that psychoanalysis
has, to date, added only a little to our knowledge about the mind.
This is not surprising when we consider the awesome global cultural
heritage. Meltzer saw the cultural significance of psychoanalysis as
contributing a new aesthetic object—the psychoanalytic method—a
way of using the transference that is a fundamental feature of human
nature and that already exists and operates in other human relation-
ships. Meltzer, Bion, Money-Kyrle, all repeat that the “new idea” we
are considering is really an old idea in a new context. It is the new link,
the new context, that brings it to life, so that it begins to mean some-
thing, either to a particular person or to a whole school of thinking or
an artistic–scientific methodology such as psychoanalysis. “Meaning
something” means it becomes “known”, not just “known about”.
At the end of his final paper, “Making the Best of a Bad Job”
(1979), Bion cites Plato’s Theaetetus on the difficulty of ensuring that a
young man’s education fill his head, not with “phantoms”, but with
“live thoughts”. Bion made it clear that we don’t invent our ideas:
we discover them, after they have somehow made their way into our
mind—what the poets call inspiration, or psychoanalysts introjective
identification. In response to Roland’s question in A Memoir of the
Future—“Why can’t you make up your mind?”—Bion’s heroine Rose-
mary replies that she doesn’t make up her mind: “I let my mind make
me up” (Bion, 1991, p. 407), or, as Meltzer has said, “I haven’t done
psychoanalysis my way—I’ve been done its way” (quoted in Cassese,
2002, p. 104). Ideas “make up” the mind. Having ideas is a “passive”
if strenuous process (with its connotations of turbulence and suffer-
ing), accompanied by a sense of dependency—not on external objects
or persons but on internal objects.
The Platonic quasi-theological system, adopted by all these think-
ers, is the philosophical bedrock for object-relations psychoanalysis,
“emotional storm” and the chance to form (or deny) a symbol of the
emotional experience: either to dovetail in conversation, or to split or
cover over with dogma or denial.
Bion called the dovetailing “intersecting with O” (1970, p. 32) the
ultimate source of knowledge; it is something that happens through-
out life, not only in psychoanalysis, and not only between mother–
baby or analyst–analysand, but between any two minds that spark a
contact, or any two parts of a mind (such as prenatal and postnatal),
any two art forms, or any two fields of knowledge of any sort (amongst
these Bion emphasizes the three basic vertices of art, science, religion).
Following Socrates, we can know nothing without the knowledge of
our ignorance. The formulation “O” (as Bion uses it) has two levels of
reference: the unknowable world of absolute reality, and the unknown,
underlying idea or meaning of an emotional encounter. The greater
O is like the godhead, whose function is to emphasize dimensions
beyond time and space that we can never penetrate, hence giving us
the concept of infinite possibility, beyond our comprehension. The
smaller or lesser O refers to the underlying but abstract idea of a
specific conflict or situation at a specific time; we become aware of
its existence through an emotional experience; this is Money-Kyrle’s
(1968) conceptual “base” of the mouth-and-nipple (p. 218).
Hence Bion concludes Attention and Interpretation with the dec-
laration that we need both the “restoration of god (the mother)”
and “the evolution of god (. . . the ineffable)” (Bion, 1970, p. 129).
God the mother is the first object, the conceptual base; god the inef-
fable is the larger O, the guarantee of the evolutionary quality that
will extend that first oneness (“at-one-ment”) to othernesses in the
future: unbounded, unknowable, infinite in potential. (We note that,
to Money-Kyrle’s three preconceptions, Bion seems to add another: the
infinite, although this may actually be a sub-category of the idea of
death.) The object’s own capacity to develop depends upon this belief
in the infinite source of all qualities, a source that cannot be tapped
directly but only mediated. Its existence, though unknowable, is an
essential feature of our innate worldview.
The two levels of O are both necessary to picture learning from
experience. As Bion says, “the Absolute . . . is not so biting as a tooth-
ache” (1991, p. 574); O does not impinge upon human experience or
sensation in any direct way. What “bites” is the emotional experience,
which becomes fictionalized in the form of a symbol, something that
is an incomplete or slanted version of the abstract or absolute truth
but is nonetheless truthful and therefore mind-building. According to
tradition, Truth enters the world not as a whole or perfect shape but in
pieces, as in Milton’s description in Areopagitica (1644): “The Absolute
enters the human world in many pieces, these pieces are perceived
by our senses . . .” (p. 143). A symbol is produced by the intersection
of O with the life of sense. The mother is knowable on the sensuous
level, yet contains the abstract or unknowable god-principle that is
infinite and ineffable, hence is the source and guide of vital curiosity
(the K-link). In Meltzer’s version of the myth, this idea takes the form
of the aesthetic conflict and the tension between the mother’s beauti-
ful exterior and enigmatic interior. Where the traditional neo-Platonic
system envisaged a ladder ascending from sensuous to divine beauty,
stepping towards the heart of meaning, the aesthetic conflict describes
the interaction of outside and inside, in which the search for the inside
is stimulated by the contrast between the sensuously known and the
unknown meaning or truth.
Every symbol demonstrates the intersection of the abstract noume-
non with a sensuous phenomenon—as described by Coleridge (1816)
when he says that a symbol, as distinct from an allegory, always “par-
takes of the reality which it renders” (p. 30). The abstract or noumenal
level allows for the evolution of the internal object as it searches in
the world of Platonic forms for the next little piece of truth required
by the infant-self in its current phase. This mediated communication
with the object via different levels of intersection (towards godhead)
thus allows the self to introject not only the object’s understanding
of its present emotional experience (via the symbol), but the object’s
capacity to think, which entails its dependence on a greater object
or O. For although the object knows more than the self, it must be
simultaneously learning from a further O, a fount of truth that has no
logical endpoint, since mental development is not circumscribed by
bodily growth or decay.
For this reason it does not make sense to talk about “becoming O”,
any more than “being O”. Bion likes to speak of “becoming” but in
the context of becoming oneself, not becoming one’s object or object-
source. Instead, to designate what happens when we have ideas, he
suggest alignment with O, or “at-one-ment”—“transformations in O”
not transformations into O. (Milton pictures this as the earth hang-
ing from heaven by a golden chain.) It is more like iron filings in a
magnetic field, or “consensuality” as all the senses come together and
point in the same direction in an act of recognition. Alignment is not
the same as fusion or unity in the primitive oceanic sense but, rather,
indicates a sense of direction and proportion. Alignment with truth
Degrees of entrapment:
living and dying in the claustrum
Pamela B. Sorensen
I
n his evocative and difficult book, The Claustrum (1992a), Donald
Meltzer offers an investigation and description of claustrophobic
phenomena. I suggest that these phenomena might be viewed on
a continuum from relatively ordinary, with potential to yield to the
developmental momentum of object relations both internal and exter-
nal, to so severely disturbed that the life of the mind hardens into a
death of the soul. To illustrate this continuum, I use two films: Coraline,
directed by Henry Selick (2009) and based on the 2002 novella by Neil
Gaiman, and The Talented Mr. Ripley, directed by Anthony Minghella
(1999) and based on the 1955 novel by Patricia Highsmith. These films
characterize the predicament at either end of the continuum; they
show how escape from the non-life of the claustrum is made possible
at the more benign end and how the possibility of exit is foreclosed
in the most extreme form of pathology. Studying the films through a
psychoanalytic lens brings into focus the critical factor in determining
the degree of entrapment suffered by the self caught in a claustropho-
bic world, devoid of emotional intimacy and filled with dread.
The concept of the claustrum is an unconscious phantasy of a space
inside the body of the internal mother that has been forcibly entered
and occupied. With this concept, Meltzer has elaborated Melanie
Klein’s (1957) idea of projective identification in three ways. First, he
emphasizes that this phantasy space is inside the internal maternal
176
visual metaphors of the films illustrate this, but a brief review of the
characteristics of the claustral compartments may be useful.
There is no trust, only obedience. Meltzer says that this is the world
of the concentration camp, where survival is all. Deviousness and
self-idealization serve to postpone the dreaded reckoning with the
phantasied sadistic paternal object that rules this world. There is no
escaping him, only becoming him.
These descriptions of compartments represent the emotional geog-
raphy of the claustrum. Inhabiting this world means living not in
relationships, but in transactions. These phantasied spaces are like
commercials where cliché substitutes for thought, and where there
can be no mysterious ambiguity of another because the other is
never beheld within the context of separateness— the other has been
invaded so as to obliterate separateness and thus obliterate what can
never be known, only contemplated. Such states of mind afflict us all
from time to time with loneliness and fear. We need the help of other
people to overcome them, and sometimes we cannot.
The films
These two films make an unlikely pair. The first, Coraline, is an ani-
mated film that is as whimsical and funny as it is thought-provoking.
The main character is a strong-willed little girl who is quite endearing.
The second, The Talented Mr. Ripley, is a tale of murder and intrigue
with a sumptuous Italian setting. The main character is capable of
chilling manipulation, exploitation, and murder. Yet both protagonists
suffer forms of entrapment in their respective claustra, skilfully con-
veyed through plot and visual detail. Coraline escapes her entrapment
to find the opportunity for repair and restoration of her good objects,
while Ripley sinks deeper into hopelessness and despair. The critical
factor that allows for development in the one and death in the other
is discussed below.
Coraline
Coraline is the story of a girl aged about 9 or 10 years, who is very
angry with her parents. They have moved from a neighbourhood
with many friends to a new home that we see through Coraline’s
eyes as a drab and desolate house, ironically and provocatively called
the Pink Palace. This house has other tenants at the top and bottom
of the building who will become significant in filling out the picture
of Coraline’s inner world. Coraline is contemptuous of her mother’s
lack of home-making skills and resentful of her preoccupation with
the work she does with her husband from home, which is creating a
seed catalogue business. Although her father takes a bit more notice of
her and shows an amused affection, he too, from her point of view, is
unwilling to take proper care of her—for instance, offering disgusting,
slimy vegetables as the only food. The refrigerator never has anything
good in it.
Defiant, Coraline marches out of the house to explore the barren,
empty landscape beyond her home. The tilted void of the animated
drawing conveys the feeling of being nowhere, just as Meltzer describes
the fundamental fear of the claustral dweller. Coraline unknowingly
plucks a dousing stick from a poison oak to assist her furious foray
into the unknown in search of water, when she is surprised by the
noisy arrival of a helmeted boy on a motorcycle named YB (short for
Why Born). This name, Why Born, suggests that he too has known the
feeling of falling into nothingness—perhaps finding no welcome in the
eyes of his mother at birth.
Fellow sufferer or not, Coraline is contemptuous of anything YB
might have to say and rejects his overtures of friendship, only to
retreat into her lonely house. She has noticed a small door in the down-
stairs hall, but her mother has shown her that there is only a brick wall
behind it and places the key where Coraline cannot find it. Of course,
late at night, when dreams conspire to reveal emotional truth, Coraline
follows the helpful scuttling of a guiding mouse to discover that the
door does, indeed, open into another world—the claustral world of the
other mother, where Coraline is treated as the special child she longs to
be and where she is offered all the wonderful food she could possibly
eat, in abundance and without waiting, and all the adoration and com-
fort she could possibly want. There is only one hitch. In exchange for
being the idealized and spoiled daughter of the other mother and father
she must agree to allow them to replace her eyes with buttons. Why?
Button eyes do not see. Thus, there can be no authentic connection
between two people, which necessarily includes ambivalence, both
love and hate, only the flat acquiescence to the lie of idealization. No
let downs, no waiting, no uncertainties. Initially, Coraline declines the
invitation and returns to the exigencies of her disappointing daily life
with her disappointing, though hard-working, parents.
Meltzer suggests that intrusive projective identification may be
accompanied by secret masturbatory phantasies that propel entry into
the object in a damaging way. The depiction of the little door as a hid-
den opening into a spiralling chute leading to the delusional world
of the other mother shows how such phantasies may rev up the manic
has become entangled. Here is the first image that carries the meta-
phor of the good combined object needed to provide the grounding
for emotional growth and development. The process of reclamation
and restoration of her good objects now begins with all the pluck and
energy with which Coraline marched away from them in her yellow
boots in the first place. Coraline’s greed for adulation has yielded to an
appreciation of the love she has been given, and her struggle has led
to a greater capacity to endure depressive pain, allowing it to enrich
her mind so that she can tell truth from falsehood in both herself and
others.
What is it that allows Coraline to resist the temptations of the
claustral world and struggle to find her real parents and her real self?
It is a combination of internal and external factors. Coraline has the
good fortune to have loyal friends who tell her the truth about herself
and, in so doing, strengthen her hold on her good objects. One of these
friends is an analyst in the form of a cat who is reliably available at
critical moments, disappearing at the end, as analysts do, to leave
Coraline to live her life with greater confidence in her own capacity
for love. He has assisted her by pointing out her arrogance, helping
her recognize what is false, bearing with her in facing the cruelty of
her own projections in the form of the other mother, and offering the
tenderness of friendship—a guide, philosopher, and friend, as Roger
Money-Kyrle defined the role of the analyst (personal communica-
tion).
Coraline’s first friend, YB from the earlier scene, has persisted
despite her contempt. He is loyal, protective, and funny, offering Cora-
line the possibility of being part of a creative couple at the end of the
film, when, together, having vanquished the other mother, murderous
in her jealous rage, they make a garden of their own, using the seeds
and tools Coraline’s parents have given them. But most important,
Coraline has parents who can withstand her jealous rage and hold on
to family life. Amidst the destruction wrought by the other mother’s
violent attempt to capture Coraline and prevent her from finding her
real parents, Coraline notices an object over the hearth in her home.
It is a broken snow globe from a family trip, dripping water like tears
onto the floor. Inside it, the distressed faces of her parents peer out.
Coraline now knows that she has hurt them through her dismissive
contempt and realizes that they love her, nevertheless, and can forgive
her. The reliance on an internal couple who can withstand attacks
through lies and distortion, yet persist in the task of building some-
to his son, Tom thinks the game is up. But to his astonishment and
despite the protestations of Marge, who knows Tom is guilty, his guile
succeeds in convincing Mr Greenleaf and the authorities that he is
innocent. Only at the very last minute, as Tom is leaving Italy with his
musician lover, Peter, who knows him as Tom Ripley, is he spotted by
another passenger on the ship, a girl he has feigned interest in, who
knows him as Dickie. He fobs her off with a kiss. When Tom returns
to his cabin, Peter is hurt because he has seen the kiss. Tom, know-
ing now that his ruse will be exposed, pretends to embrace Peter and
strangles him with a necktie.
The life that Tom Ripley steals from Dickie displays all the claus-
tral characteristics Meltzer describes: the elitist and entitled display
of material riches in the head/breast compartment, as seen in the
luxurious lifestyle of Dickie and his friends; the sexual frenzy of the
genital compartment, seen in the heady scene in the jazz club, where
Tom jumps in to mimic the louche singing of Dickie’s friends; and the
ultimate tyranny of the rectal compartment, seen in the three murders
Tom commits. Although there are brief moments when Tom views the
beauty of the maternal object from the outside, as when he wants to
go sightseeing in Rome and when he listens to music, his utter despair
and fear of abandonment drive him to force his way into the maternal
space, represented by the gorgeous landscape of Italy, and steal the
identity of Dickie Greenleaf through violence, trickery, and lies. The
fear of being found out as an impostor fills the atmosphere with dread.
Tom’s homosexual ardour for Dickie—his longing to merge with
him in every way, through dressing in his clothes, singing his songs,
joining their reflections in the window glass of trains, sharing his
bath—is an expression of his utter loneliness and fear of being nothing
and no one, as he tells Peter at the end of the film, “I’m going to be
stuck in the basement, alone in the dark. No one will ever find me”.
Degrees of entrapment
Both Coraline and Tom Ripley are trapped in inner worlds where
greed and self-delusion dominate: Coraline for a while, and Tom for-
ever. In this world, sorrow cannot yield the fruit of remorse. Gratitude
cannot soften the pain of separation. Hope cannot allow the unknown
to unfold. Coraline develops in her capacity to bear psychic pain and
in her capacity to face the truth about herself through struggle with
her demons and with the help of a good internal couple and a cat. Tom
cannot escape his claustral world, but is driven deeper in with every
Note
This chapter is based on a version previously published in the Journal of Child
Psychotherapy, Vol. 42 (No. 1, 2016), pp. 45–53. Copyright © Association of Child
Psychotherapists, reprinted by permission of Taylor & Francis Ltd, http://www.
tandfonline.com on behalf of the Association of Child Psychotherapists.
Mary Fisher-Adams
I
n this chapter I look at Ulysses as a description of a claustrum world
and how fear and dread can produce, in the so-called “replacement
child”, a proleptic imagination that keeps the claustrum dweller
imprisoned and paralysed. A link is made with Shakespeare, who is
a presence throughout Ulysses and was himself a replacement child.
I had been struck by similarities between two of my patients and
James Joyce. They had all lost siblings in early childhood and seem
to have felt emotionally cut adrift by the mother’s grief at her loss—
a feeling marginalized that had extended into adulthood. They all
showed particular sensitivity as children and were possessed of a
highly active imagination and exceptional literary creativity. Above
all, they seemed tormented by fear in the extreme. My two patients
came into analysis in their forties still plagued by nightmares of
lions, monsters, and dead babies. Most strikingly, both expressed
the fear that they had murdered someone and that they remain
lethal and shouldn’t be allowed out into the world. James Joyce also
suffered great fears and nightmares. He spoke of “that skull” that
came to torment him at night (Ellmann, 1982, p. 178). He was highly
188
of guilt and ill equipped to engage with him, makes Mulligan the cruel
one and walks away. There is talk of fathers throughout Ulysses, as
though seeking someone to lift Stephen out of himself and his resent-
ments, but, in his wish to deny generational reality, Stephen proclaims
himself his own father, “made not begotten” (p. lxix).2 Joyce’s biogra-
pher, Richard Ellmann, comments that “paternity is a more powerful
motif in the book than sexual love” (1982, p. 371).
Guilt was also a major feature for my two patients. But this was not
depressive-position guilt seeking forgiveness. They did not expect or
even seek compassion in life, only condemnation. The “guilt” seems
more an expression of fear and anger. It is a persecutory anxiety
based on a fantasy rather than an actual crime or hurt the person has
caused. “I know it is all my fault”, we hear patients say, even though
intellectually they know that is not the case. It is one of those “end of
story!” statements—a state of mind fearful of exploring what might
be their fault and what isn’t. The “I know” becomes the certainty with
which they try to hold themselves together, but it conveys, “I don’t
really want to know.” It is as though the act of imagining has become
a dangerous act that might lead to new awareness and pain.
Feelings my patients had were intense and hard to control and
often produced behaviour that one might well condemn. It was almost
as though they wanted to prove their badness, but any attempt by me
to address this seemed to touch on real fragility and fear. One patient
described her life as being tirelessly available for all and sundry, but
at the same time she was telling me about behaviour that was clearly
unethical and hurtful to others.
Ulysses is full of Joyce getting revenge on the many people he felt
had wronged him. Leon Edel (1980) called Joyce an “Injustice Collec-
tor”, and Hugh Kenner points out that Joyce imprisoned himself and
had to remain in exile because of all the lawsuits he would have had
against him had he returned to Ireland: “Joyce’s revenge on Oliver
Gogarty [Buck Mulligan in the book] was to shut him into a book for
all to see” (Kenner, 1962, p. 49).3
In real life, Joyce’s sense of entitlement—born of fear and guilt—
particularly with his wife and brother, made him notoriously ruthless.
It is a curious dynamic: the way some who lost siblings latch onto
another sibling in a most intense and controlling way. Van Gogh and
Thomas Mann are two examples. It becomes a sadomasochistic folie à
deux. Julian Barnes refers to Van Gogh as the “flat-sharer from hell, an
insistent, overbearing presence, needy, demanding, free with advice
and always knowing better” (Barnes, 2015, p. 8). One of my patients
But for my patients, and for Joyce, the intensity of feelings from their
early experience felt, I believe, barely survivable. They did not have
the space and security to let things take their course. They were poised
ready for disaster, wishing to “jump the life to come” (Macbeth).
The claustrum
One way to control the object is to get inside, and Ulysses seems a strik-
ing example of an author entering right into his characters in a way
that goes beyond mere imaginative identification and becomes the
intrusive identification characteristic of the claustrum dweller. Molly’s
monologue in the final scene could be seen as a particular example
of this—a kind of intrusion by Joyce himself. At one point Joyce has
Molly say, “Oh Jamesy let me up out of this pooh”, as though Joyce
is controlling things (18: 1128). There seems to be a subtle difference
between conveying what the character is like and thinks and Joyce
bringing to it his own fantasies, denying the woman, in this case, her
separateness and essential unknowability. Writers talk about their
At the age of 40, Joyce had a dream that has elements of inevitability
of a proleptic and persecuted state of mind in a compartmentalized
claustrum world where there is no escape. It is set in a luscious Persian
pavilion:
There were sixteen rooms, four on each floor. Someone had commit-
ted a crime, and he entered the lowest floor. The door opened on
a flower garden. He hoped to get through but when he arrived at
the threshold a drop of blood fell on it. I could know how desperate
he felt, for he went from the first floor all the way up to the fourth,
his hope being that at each threshold his wound was not capable
of letting fall another drop. But always it came, an official discovered
it, and punctually at the sixteen rooms the drop fell. There were
two officials in brocaded silk robes, and a man with a scimitar who
watched him. [Ellmann, 1982, p. 547]
Joyce, disparaging of psychoanalysis and opting for self-analysis,
interpreted the rooms as the twelve signs of the zodiac; the three doors
are the Trinity, the man who committed the crime was himself, and the
man with the scimitar was his “wife next morning”. The pavilion with
light-blue lattices was “like a box”, he said (Ellmann, 1982, p. 547). We
do not know the context for this dream, except that he had seen the
Russian ballet the night before and was writing Finnegans Wake, but
as well as a “criminal” entering the lowest floor trying to evade being
discovered, there is a distinct sense of nameless dread and “ineluc-
tability”—Joyce’s favourite word—meaning inevitable and inescap-
able, which has the ring of proleptic certainty: “I could always know”
and “always it came”. Most poignantly, the man in the dream was
wounded and felt desperate with no way of escape.
Perhaps the desperate wish to “escape” from some obscure guilt
and getting stuck, instead, in a luscious palace with erotic overtones
gives a glimpse of the state of mind in which Joyce wrote Ulysses. The
dream pales in comparison with the wild nightmarish pantomime
“Circe” section in Ulysses, in which lonely Bloom is hauled before a
court of judges and accused of everything under the sun in front of a
baying mob. While full of Joycean hilarity, it is also the extended heart-
felt outcry of someone caught in the bogus persecutory world of the
claustrum. It is a world replete with all kinds of “sluts and ragamuf-
fins”, bishops and ghosts, and a seductive Molly Bloom “in Turkish
costume, her opulent curves filling out the scarlet trousers and jacket
slashed with gold” (p. 570). Stephen’s dead mother also appears, as
do Macbeth and the three witches (p. 682).
James Fisher sees Macbeth as a moving portrayal of the proleptic
imagination in action. He emphasizes the depth of emotion expressed
in the play while describing Macbeth’s proleptic state of mind as an
attempt to control the emotions. He points out that there is no fool
or “truthsayer” in Macbeth—no Paulina, for example, who, in the
inter’s Tale, could speak the truth to Leontes. Instead, there are a few
W
moments when Macbeth himself briefly reflects on what was happen-
ing—moments, Fisher says, that Shakespeare marks “with some of
the most unforgettable poetry of the play, indeed of any of his plays”:
Macbeth senses that his conscious action can never match this
strange experience where wanting and having appear to be one. Per-
haps as a warrior hardened in battle he distrusted the omnipotence
of action, although he had never given up his infantile omnipotence
of thought, at least not when it seems thrust upon him. It is remarka-
ble that Shakespeare chooses to have Macbeth give eloquent expres-
sion to an emotion we can only suppose threatens to overwhelm as
he thinks feelingly about the murder of Duncan. Pity, seemingly an
emotion as fragile as a “naked new-born babe”, is pictured “strid-
ing the blast”, blowing “the horrid deed in every eye”. It is as if
Macbeth, for the moment at least, sees a link between pity and an
infant, not as helpless but “striding the blast” of the heavens. It is a
picture of the power of the emotions, the kind of thing a Fool might
say to Macbeth, reminding him of reality, the reality of emotional
experience—tears that “drown the wind”. [2009, p. 15]
Rather than reflecting on reality, Ulysses seems more reality-evading,
or reality-trashing. Virginia Woolf described it as the work of a man
who “in order to breathe had to break all the windows” (Kiberd, 1992,
p. xviii). The “Circe” section in Ulysses has the same manic, unintel-
ligible, unstoppable quality as Lucky’s outpouring in Beckett’s Waiting
for Godot (2006b) and the character Mouth in his play Not I (2006a).
Leopold Bloom as a character seems quite extraordinarily strange.
Joyce tries to lift him into a realm of great humanity, saintliness even,
as though this “womanly man” might be the ideal. But at the same
time he is portraying a grieving, cuckolded, marginalized Jew who
distracts himself with endless bits of information, inhabits a world of
masturbation, and seems unable to cry out his pain. Joyce, perhaps
feeling the absence of a mature combined couple, may want to save
this bereaved couple, Molly and Bloom, who lost a child, and bring
them to life again. But it doesn’t happen. At the end, they remain lying
topsy-turvy, with Bloom kissing Molly’s buttocks, unable to overcome
their grief.
Intrusive identification
Sabbadini describes the replacement child who lost a sibling as “treated
more as the embodiment of a memory than as a person in its own
right”. Being allowed a life of their own was problematic, given their
sense that they should not have survived when their siblings had died
(1988, p. 530). Desperate to prove their worth, and feeling angry at
their situation, can make them very competitive. (See Meltzer, 1992a,
p. 66, for a discussion of the Odyssey and the “Telemachus part of the
infantile personality vis-à-vis the bad and naughty brothers and sisters,
fraught . . . with savage infantile competitiveness”.) However, success
immediately brings new fears. Whatever survivors do, they feel that
being competitive risks causing the demise of the other. The proleptic
imagination pictures only one possible outcome, a devastating one,
whatever is wished for.
One of my patients is an accomplished writer, but, afraid of pub-
lishing in her own name, she works as a ghost writer instead, disguis-
ing her own identity. Anisfeld and Richards give a description of this
dynamic in their paper on the “replacement child”:
He could never be certain that he was loved for who he was or for
the genuineness of his achievements. When he performed an action
from which he reaped a reward at someone else’s expense, he was
convinced that he had initiated it; if his deeds were in any way altru-
istic, he doubted their sincerity. He believed that he should have been
able to do the impossible and save the lives of his half-sisters even though
he had not yet been born. This grandiose fantasy paradoxically made
him scorn his actual accomplishments as worthless and even led him
to be taken advantage of by others for their own glorification. [2000,
p. 314; emphasis added)
The virtuoso set pieces are less acts of creation than of parody; and
parody is the act of a trapped mind which, realizing that it cannot
create anew, takes its revenge by defacing the masterpieces of the
past. [p. xlviii]
Conclusion
It has been pointed out by Stephen Greenblatt (2004) that Shakespeare
found it difficult to portray or even imagine fully achieved marital
intimacy in his plays. Joyce’s own experience of a parental couple
was marred by the breakdown of his parents’ relationship. Perhaps it
is no surprise that in Ulysses Stephen proclaims that only the imagi-
nation can be the source of rebirth and regeneration (Schwarz, 1987,
p. 113). James Fisher has shown that if the imagination is a paralysing
proleptic one, defending against emotion, there can be no creative
intercourse. In discussing the K-state-of-mind that makes possible the
experience of the full range of human emotions, he states:
As Bion emphasized numerous times, it is the hatred of emotion
that lies at the heart of psychotic phenomena (Bion, 1959, p. 311).
Paradoxically certain emotions, such as anxiety, envy and hatred,
attack and make impossible the experiencing of other emotions.
Actually, rather than envy, perhaps we should put fear at the head
of the list of the –K factors, the fear that emotional experience is not
survivable. [Fisher, 2006, p. 1233]
Notes
1. For Joyce, life was all coincidences and superstition. He referred to Finne-
gans Wake as having a “prophetic and magical nature”; he knew the superstitions
of most of Europe “and adopted them all” (Ellmann, 1982, pp. 517, 525). For Joyce,
reality was “a paradigm . . . the perception of coincidence . . . reality, no matter
how much we try to manipulate it, it can only assume certain forms; the roulette
wheel brings up the same numbers again and again” (p. 551).
2. “James Joyce once remarked that Ulysses was for him essentially a way of
capturing the speech of his father and his father’s friends: “. . . so much literature
stems from a wish to assuage homesickness . . . [T]he modernist is one who is
likely to use his intimate life as material for his art, shaping the ordinary into the
extraordinary” (Oates, 2015, p. 84).
3. For all his seeming dismissal of Oliver Gogarty, the two books left on his
desk when he died were a Greek Lexicon and Gogarty’s I Follow Saint Patrick
(Ellmann, 1982, p. 742).
4. One night in the pub he had drunk one glass of wine when someone pointed
out a rat running down the stairs. Joyce said “Where? Where? That’s bad luck”
and a minute later lost consciousness. They took him home, and “Nora was about
to scold him but became tender when she saw he was suffering from fright rather
than alcohol” (Ellmann, 1982, p. 517).
5. In Molloy (1955), Beckett recalls his own unwanted birth: “My mother, I
don’t think too harshly of her. I know she did all she could not to have me, except
of course the one thing, and if she never succeeded in getting me unstuck, it was
that fate had earmarked me for less compassionate sewers” (p. 15).
6. “The shift to writing in French may have been an important way of escaping
the influence of Joyce—greater simplicity and objectivity—could concentrate more
on the music of the language, its sounds and its rhythms” (Knowlson, 1996, p. 357).
7. Joyce asked Beckett to read this original ending to Finnegans Wake, and Beck-
ett was very moved by it. Interestingly, in one of his own last works, Company, some
forty years later, Beckett describes walking with his own mother, thinking back to
the skies of his childhood: “You make ground in silence hand in hand through the
warm still summer air. It is late afternoon and after some hundred paces the sun
appears above the crest of the rise. Looking up at the blue sky and then at your
mother’s face you break the silence asking her if it is not in reality much more
distant than it appears. The sky that is. The blue sky” (in Johns, 2010, p. 27).
David Mayers
L
et me begin by stressing that I totally agree with Mary Fisher-
Adams about the importance of both Meltzer’s claustrum and
Fisher’s proleptic imagination. My difficulties lie with the way she
brings these concepts to her reading of Joyce.
The relation between a writer’s work and his life and states of mind
is a vast and vexed topic—one that I shall not address here. I want to
confine myself to the interpretation of Ulysses. I think that one differ-
ence between Adams’ and my vision relates to the personal attitudes
that we bring to understanding Joyce: I have the impression, based
on her descriptions of his undoubted and manifold personal faults,
that she rather dislikes him; whereas I find his life and work so heroic
that I will largely forgive him everything. I recognize that his demand
of the reader that he will devote his whole life to studying the work
can seem omnipotently self-important; but I, and countless others, are
grateful for having been given the chance to do so.
Adams’ claim that Stephen, Bloom, and Molly are claustrum-
dwellers implies that their lives are tunnel-visioned and lacking in
development. I cannot avoid remembering that the Dublin of Blooms-
day was in many respects a narrow-minded philistine place in which
artists’ wings were relentlessly clipped. The atmosphere is poignantly
captured in the stories “A Mother” and “The Dead” from Dubliners
(Joyce, 1914): also in Yeats’s poem “September 1913” (in Yeats, 1989).
Joyce and Stephen had to escape to be free to think. Similarly, Molly
had to fight for her capacity to be a woman who enjoyed orgasmic
sex. Adams sees her “Ah Jamesy get me out of this pooh” as Joyce
206
intrusively identifying with his character (“pooh” was not at that time
a euphemism for excrement but, rather, a generalized term of disdain
or disgust). She does not mention that Molly had just realized she was
beginning to menstruate; nor that in demotic Dublin “Ah Jamesy” was
a mock-polite version of the ubiquitous “Ah Jaysus!”.
To be sure, a psychological reading is, in principle, quite compat-
ible with the one I offer. So let me give examples of why I find the
claustrum interpretation a bed of Procrustes that lops off vital parts,
whereas I see Joyce mobilizing a variety of points of view reminiscent
of Bion.
One feature of Ulysses that I cannot square with the claustrum
interpretation is that it is constantly side-splittingly funny. Now there
can be humour in the claustrum: in Beckett’s Endgame (1958), as vivid
an evocation of the anal claustrum as I could hope to find, Clov says
“If I don’t kill that rat it’ll die.” But this is gallows humour. In Ulysses
(1922a) and Finnegans Wake (1939), Joyce offers a spectrum of laughter
much more generous and life-enhancing, running from metaphysical
wit to slapstick.
Consider the Cyclops scene: a working-class pub; a place of hos-
pitality, bonhomie, meanness, pretentiousness, spite, gossip; a place
where life is down-to-earth or heroic, celebrated, ridiculed, reviled,
mourned; where food and drink offer the chance to feast with the gods
or make a pig of oneself; where singing can be uplifting and beautiful
or a social embarrassment; and so on.
Against this background, Bloom is attacked for his Jewishness by
the Citizen and counters triumphantly by pointing out that Jesus was
a Jew. He then flees the pub, pursued by the Citizen/Cyclops and his
barking dog, hurling an empty biscuit tin after him (think of the din
on the cobbles). Heroic, mock-heroic, triumphant, a rout, magnificent,
ridiculous? The lot.
Or think of the scene where Gertie MacDowell, crippled and iso-
lated, drowns herself in sentimentalized romantic fancy and shows
her knickers to a masturbating Bloom while the Host is elevated at
Benediction in a neighbouring chapel. Grubby, pornographic, blas-
phemous, of course: but also heart-warmingly accepting and sympa-
thetic—what else has the poor girl got? The claustrum interpretation
doesn’t leave any room for Joyce’s essential warmth and breadth of
vision.
When it comes to Finnegans Wake and its altered ending, I’m not
sure I understand what Adams thinks Joyce essentially discarded. I
think of two passages: the first from Anna Livia Plurabelle: where
Shem (Irish for James) has been renamed Mercius (the grateful, the
merciful, the forgiven, the messenger and how many more?).
With a beck, with a spring, all her ringringlets shaking, rocks drops
in her tachie, tramtokens in her hair, all waived to a point and
then all inuendation, little old-fashioned mummy, little wonderful
mummy, ducking under bridges, bellhopping the weirs, dodging
by a bit of bog, rapid-shooting round the bends, by Tallaght’s green
hills and the pools of the phooka and a place they call it Blessing-
ton and slipping sly by Sallynoggin, as happy as the day is wet,
babbling, bubbling, chattering to herself, deloothering the fields on
their elbows leaning with the sloothering slide of her, giddy-gaddy,
grannyma, gossipaceous Anna Livia.
He lifts the lifewand and the dumb speak.
Quoiquoiquoiquoiquoiquoiquoiq! [p. 194]
Hardly claustral: rather, mummy’s little duck, full of questions, luxu-
riating in the beauties of mummy’s body. And, some 200 pages later:
Three quarks for Muster Mark!
Sure he hasn’t got much of a bark
And sure any he has it’s all beside the mark. [p. 383]
King Mark, cuckolded like Bloom; Mr. Mac, the eponymous Irishman,
Everyman, to be given three cheers. Because we’re all sons of Anna
and all loved, despite our manifest failures and shortcomings. This last
is a reminder of the judgement passed on Bloom in the Circe scene
that Adams mentions:
You’ve made your second-best bed, and others must lie in it.
Bloom is a cuckold. He is not a hero with magic weapons, so rather
than coming home to kill the suitors, he lives with them. Yet he gets
three quarks and a Shakespearean mention. And how many ways do
we interpret “lie”?
I once had a patient in work with whom I was much helped by
Donald Meltzer’s thought. In the beginning he brought a dream where
he was in an underground kitchen with a tribe of cannibal brothers who were
cutting their wrists, dropping blood into a cauldron of mother’s shit and eat-
ing it. He was expected to join in, but was too scared.
He spent many years dreaming of trying to escape this anal claus-
trum, but was so afraid of the outside that he went back in. Gradually,
he was able to dream of living outside and learning to survive there.
In his last session with me he brought this dream: he was coming to have
lunch with me in a college that was mine but not his; when he arrived, the
Tara Harrison
T
his chapter chronicles the first years of therapy with a patient
contending with both external and internal intrusive forces,
leaving her struggling to form an authentic, thinking mind. I
show how Meltzer’s concepts of intrusive identification and claustrum
phenomena have provided a crucial framework to help understand
this patient’s internal system.
Donald Meltzer initially used the term “massive projective identifi-
cation” (1966, p. 17) to describe the excessive use of projective identifi-
cation as conceptualized by Melanie Klein. He shifted to a qualitative
view with the term “intrusive identification”—that is, an omnipotent
phantasy of forceful intrusion into an external or, crucially, an internal
object in order to control it, and eliminate its “otherness” (Meltzer,
1986b, p. 69, quoted in Fisher, 1999, p. 233). The aggressive quality
of the projections has a significant impact on the internal containing
function:
. . . the violent nature of intrusive identification destroys the con-
tainer, the place for maternal alpha function. The container is now
transformed into a claustrum, a rigid prison brought about by the
intrusive process either of active penetration or of being sucked in.
[Gosso, 2004, p. 21]
The work with the patient described here demonstrates the severe
impact of intrusive identification on internal object relations. It depicts
210
Intrusive identification
in relation to the patient’s external objects
Olivia’s intrusive relationships were most clearly demonstrated in her
presentation of two important people in her life: her mother and her
fiancé.
Authoritative and charismatic, her mother, as described by Olivia,
invaded Olivia with expectations and assumptions about who Olivia
was. Olivia felt she had grown to fit the space imagined for her by
her mother; if she revealed aspects of herself outside that shape, she
felt the response from her mother was terrifying. Olivia came from
seemed certain about who she is and what goes on inside her. Olivia,
it appeared, perceived herself as a passive receptacle, condemned to
accept the tyrannical management of an intrusive object.
From Olivia’s point of view, these characters, so intrusive with their
communications and projections, were impervious to any attempt from
her to reach them. She reported that she would become increasingly
more desperate, employing tears and tantrums to try and impact them,
until she gave up and locked herself in her room with headaches and
in despair. James Fisher describes the “not-good-enough mother who
can neither recognize nor respond to the gesture of the infant”, and he
suggests that “the substitution of her own gestures for those of the infant’s
is a tyrannical move” (1999, p. 46; emphasis added). He goes on to
describe the potential impact of this tyranny on the subject when he
observes how “every false-self personality structure hides in it the
tyrant, and one only has to observe such compliant persons when the
tables can be turned” (p. 46). Olivia’s reported tantrums and demands
seemed to reflect Fisher’s observation. I imagined Olivia’s migraines
as the pain of her banging her head against a brick wall—and I often
had that feeling myself throughout the work!
Despite these colourful stories, the experience of actually being
with Olivia in the consulting room was strangely hollow and empty. I
often felt blank and dazed, followed by a feeling of panic that I would
never be able to understand or help her.
Significantly, the assessor who first met with Olivia noted a similar
experience, ending her report with this insightful note: “In case all this
sounds a bit sunny and easy, I would like to put down a few thoughts
about the countertransferential feelings I had with this patient. I nor-
mally see patients for assessment for an hour and ten minutes. With
this young woman, I quite soon found my eyes straying to the clock
and had an increasingly uneasy feeling that we were going to get
through it all too quickly, that we would grind to a halt well before
the ‘end’, and I wouldn’t be able to think of anything more to say to
her.” Like the assessor, I would find myself looking at the clock wor-
ried that time was moving so slowly and wondering how would the
session be filled.
Fisher, now reflecting on a paper by Michael Feldman (1989), con-
tinues thus:
The infant confronted with an impenetrable mother or parental
couple becomes increasingly desperate and this gives rise to two
alternative patterns. Either the patient makes increasingly violent
attempts to get through to this impenetrable object, or it withdraws
This snippet shows how hard it was to make contact with Olivia. She
has an alternative system already established, with jargon in place of
language and certainty instead of exploration. Olivia “knows” she
should be disrupted by the break and that this should be “diagnosed”
by an assured and certain therapist. Meltzer (1976a) called this kind
of false-self version of therapy the “delusion of clarity of insight”,
and he linked it to intrusion into the head/breast compartment of
the internal maternal object. Meltzer describes the internal mother’s
head/breast compartment as the source of richness, generosity, and
insight. If in phantasy the internal object has been invaded and con-
quered, there is a shift in perspective—the resources, as Meltzer put
it, get “vulgarized”. Genuine search for knowledge becomes a rather
grandiose possession of information. Through the intrusive processes,
the patient is fused with the internal object. She has done away with
Internal situation
These relationships seemed to reflect Olivia’s internal object relations,
which also seemed to be characterized by tyranny and compliance.
We started to understand more about this through Olivia’s posi-
tioning of her handbag in the consulting room. At the start of every
session, she would place her handbag right next to my bag, even
though it was quite awkward for her to get to and it meant we had
to cross each other en route to the couch or chair: “It’s as if you expe-
rience where I place my bag as an instruction for you. This is how
you find a place for your bag.” Olivia was initially puzzled by my
observation. She could see that we crossed each other in an awkward
way because of the bag, but she could not see that there could be
somewhere more convenient for her to put her bag, a place that might
suit her better. Already in the consulting room she felt like the passive
receptacle for my instructive gesture. I wondered whether her bag
stuck to mine indicated an attempt to attach adhesively. When projec-
tive identification fails, Meltzer hypothesizes that intrusive identifica-
tion or adhesive identification can take its place. I wondered if there
were times when Olivia unconsciously presumed that her objects, so
unreceptive to her communications, had no space inside them, and so
she would try to attach adhesively to the outside layer. It is possible
that she would oscillate between attempts to get inside and attempts
to attach to the outside of her object.
I think it was highly significant that, around this time, Olivia had
started talking about two of her friends who had become pregnant. She
had been quite scathing about their decision to have a baby and was
quite sure that pregnancy was not for her. Yet just a few weeks later,
Olivia told me that she, herself, was newly pregnant. She described the
conception as accidental and quite a shock. I found myself wondering
if she had, quite unconsciously, experienced her friends’ pregnancies
as an instruction to become pregnant herself. And yet this would
indicate a seriously confused internal system.
A little later in the therapy as Olivia’s pregnancy developed, there
seemed to be more evidence to support this theory. One of her friends
suffered a miscarriage, and she told Olivia about this over a weekend.
On the Monday morning, Olivia felt unwell: she felt “tightness” in
her abdomen and strong sensations in her pelvic area. She became
alarmed, got off the bus, and went to hospital, cancelling our session.
During the course of the day at the hospital, perhaps as a response
to her anxiety, Olivia underwent three internal examinations. At this
point, she started to bleed, and by the end of the day it was Olivia who
was dealing with a possible miscarriage. Eventually the light bleeding
stopped and everything settled back down, but the pregnancy seemed
to have been potentially endangered by the day’s events. I was starting
to understand this as a process whereby Olivia herself perceives an
intrusive and instructive quality in her object’s communications. She
could not yet find her way to a kind of therapy in which we might
be able to think together and make a distinction between her friend’s
pregnancy and her own. It is quite literally a con-fusion: a fusion with
the identity and experience of the object, and there seems to be no
choice about it. Any attempt to understand her is misconstrued as an
instruction, and she is condemned to adjust to and even “suck in” the
hostile communications. It is a very distorted version of introjection.
It was a core issue in Olivia’s psychopathology and put her, I think,
in considerable physical and psychic danger.
Technical issues
During her pregnancy, Olivia became very frightened that she was
going to lose her mind. Her anxieties seem to be connected to her
partner Nick’s history. His mother had become extremely disturbed
during her pregnancy with him, and Nick was born in a psychiatric
setting. During Olivia’s pregnancy, he frequently described her as
psychotic and requiring sectioning. Olivia experienced this as a sure
prediction, and it caused her a huge amount of panic. Nick’s exces-
sive projections and Olivia’s claustrum psychopathology are knotted
together. I would present the possibility that an object, as James Fisher
puts it, can “protect itself against intrusion, closing its orifices against
an intrusive attempt to penetrate” (Fisher, 1999, p. 228), but Olivia
found this concept difficult to really engage with.
Just as difficult for Olivia to acknowledge was the notion that her
own lively, creative, and aggressive aspects—indeed, some of the
much needed aspects that might propel her forwards—get split off
and projected into her fiancé. She and Nick seemed to be caught in
a “projective gridlock” (Morgan, 1995) that felt to Olivia like a ver-
sion of togetherness and intimacy but is closer to collusion. It seemed
immensely exciting at some level for Olivia to be at the mercy of such
dominating objects, but the fixedness of their gridlock hampered pro-
gression in the therapy. The teratoma that Olivia imagines inside her
seems to be a mass of fragments of identity. What belongs to whom?
I suspect it is still too early to attempt unravelling, as that which is
truly Olivia is still embryonic.
There were other challenges to the therapy. Olivia missed many
sessions, and in this way the fragile link between us was broken. Any
corresponding internal linking was impaired too, I think. Language
would become jargon, and I would find my comments repeated back
to me, with meaning drained. The insight was hijacked in the system
of the claustrum, and in this way Olivia maintained control of the
culture within the consulting room. Like Olivia, I felt unable to expand
and develop outside preconceived expectations. I tried to find ordi-
nary ways of putting things, but my non-psychological vocabulary
caused suspicion that I must be underqualified and ineffective, rather
than actually having a capacity to not know and to discover. Olivia
was able to confess, at least, that Nick thought I was rather ineffectual.
I featured in a significant dream: I am a small, dreamy “London-
type” and Olivia discovers that I am a lesbian. I have a large, bossy wife
from the countryside who dominates me. I put it to her that she ima-
gines I have an internal world like hers, dominated by a larger,
managing aspect who comes from the countryside—a reference to
“mother’s village” and the intrusive maternal object, I think. As
Meltzer observed, the patient assumes that “this interior world is
all there is and the analyst is as much caught up in its net as is the
patient” (1992a, p. 102). This would indicate that, stuck in the same
swamp, I would be able to be of very little help to Olivia. She strug-
gles to perceive me as an object that could help her, and I feel like I
am banging my head against a brick wall to try to reach her. It is an
important countertransferential situation, however difficult it is to
bear. Perhaps the most hopeful aspect of the dream is that there is a
“small me” who has a capacity for dreaming. I might be able to be
uncertain, to explore, and to symbolize. Olivia may need to feel that
I can enter her claustral world with her and for her, but that I can
also exit and be able to think creatively. It might help her with the
idea that she, too, could leave the claustrum.
tect a private internal space, she may be able to relate to her internal
objects in a less intrusive, controlling way too. This work, done deep
in the unconscious, relies upon a respectful, reciprocal, imaginative
intimacy. Crucially, it must be intimacy established from the outside
of her internal objects. If she can ease the rigid, intrusive culture in
her unconscious life, her internal objects may be “free to come and
go”, to develop and combine—so that she would have access to more
mature and creative internal resources (Cassese, 2002, p. 11). Olivia
may discover she could bear to be uncertain, to have mixed feelings
and to develop a mind of her own.
T
he material presented here comes from a three-year research
project conducted by the Psychology Faculty of Javeriana Uni-
versity, Colombia, which took place in a shelter for battered
women and their children, in Bogota. The authors of this chapter were
supervisors of the project and thesis directors and were responsible
for a research group of five students. The dissertations included the
presentation of three individual psychotherapeutic process record-
ings, which were supervised and analysed using the meditative review
method described by Bion (1963, pp. 99–100).
In total, we observed 40 women whose abusive relationships since
childhood and currently with their spouses were described in detail in
these protocols. As the stories of these battered women were reviewed,
the research team had the impression that they were entering a terri-
tory of human misery and dehumanization that was common ground
for these women, their aggressors, and their families, as well as for
the neighbourhood they lived in. An atmosphere of despair and rage
could be sensed by the research group, as if the psychic weave of the
chronicles permeated the way they listened to the distressing stories
born out of the presentations. The same thing happened when we
reviewed the material for a second time, long after the original dis-
cussion.
222
Magdalena
Magdalena’s breakdown
“Magdalena” was a 29 year-old prostitute who had taken refuge in
a shelter for battered women and their children in Bogota. It was the
second time she had sought therapeutic help. From her first session,
she confronted us with the cruelty she had suffered and exerted since
she was little. She told us how she had been abused by her brother-
in-law, the father of her older sister, and a carpenter who used to give
her money. She told the story of her relationship with a paramilitary
man that made her the most wanted and expensive woman in the
brothel. Things went sour when he demanded that she abort the
child they had conceived “by mistake”, arguing that he did not like
to “leave a trace of his actions”. When she refused, he sent hit-men to
beat her up. They hurt her badly, but—as always—she felt nothing,
alone with her “clouded mind”. She was simply the woman who was
abused by everyone, who felt nothing, because “it is not as terrible as
people may think”.
She described her current situation as the result of multiple expe-
riences of work exploitation, poverty, starvation, and sexual abuse.
When she reported her brother for raping her daughters and niece,
she described him to the police as an “ill man, criminal, thief, and
rapist”. Her mother cursed her for this. The material of her stories was
composed of dramatic adventures lived with indifference; her own
experiences were felt to be someone else’s. Her accounts were made up
of scattered stories. Nevertheless, amid this violence, abuse, maltreat-
ment, and attempted murder, two good figures appeared: her father,
who had always taken care of her and loved her, and Miguel, her cur-
rent boyfriend, who bewildered her with his unconditional kindness
because she thought that, “in the end, all men are women abusers”.
She would go from being the bad woman, condemned to poverty
and filth, to being the great queen of the brothel, the one with the best
income and largest number of clients. She claimed that she “never
drank or sniffed anything” but told how she would “come home com-
pletely destroyed” or stopped by at her friend Miguel’s place, so that
other people would not see her like that: showing her worst face to
the loved man and her best face to her abusers was a constant pattern.
As she shifted from being good, rich, and powerful to being bad,
ugly, and poor, so her objects changed from good to bad at a glance:
from great helpers to great abusers, from caring figures to battering
figures. Her bad sister could suddenly become her good sister. The
only constant was the endless sexual abuse: fathers and daughters,
brothers-in-law and sisters-in-law, adults and children. The shelter
and protection that she acknowledged they could give her could
become the nastiest prison, with the worst conditions of abuse and
maltreatment. She even thought of going back to the brothel, despite
the death threats of her former client, because there she could at least
make a living for herself and her daughters.
For Magdalena, neither sensations nor emotions helped her to dif-
ferentiate between her experiences. Her stories lacked symbolization
and meaning and were not organized in time and space; her only
tense was present continuous and she alternated between idealized
and denigrated persecutory places. In repeating herself, she conveyed
the absence of an object capable of making sense of these contrasting
experiences, with no one to mediate them.
Her disorganized sensory system seemed to have been damaged by
violent intrusions, which may have ruptured her perceptive sensory
matrix, making it difficult for her to discriminate, differentiate, relate,
or transform her experiences. In such cases, the end result is a broken
sensuality that makes women unable to feel, integrate, and make sense
of their experiences and to generate meanings. Magdalena’s internal
reality, suffused with trauma, threats, and concrete images of finding
a way out to a better future, oscillated, for no obvious reason, between
being persecuted to persecuting, from abused to abusing. It was as
though her body disappeared along with the possibility of perceiving
and remembering her experiences or making any sense of them; she
lost all sense of pleasure (Michel de M’Uzan, quoted by Green, 2002).
Her sexuality became her profession, disassociated from her body,
where the trauma was re-experienced time after time, masked by her
attempts to listen to the men who visited her.
This sensory breakdown became more evident through the
six sessions. The general feeling was that her discourse was fall-
ing to pieces and the association between events and words was
unclear; no time, space, sensation, emotion, image, or bonds with
objects remained long enough to become an organizing element. In
her mind, everything appeared and disappeared: nothing was sta-
ble and nothing remained. For this reason, she would turn to her
enemies for help, as if they could be her friends. Not even the scars
of physical violence remained in her mind: she would see them and
then forget them the next minute. One minute, she was a beauty
queen; the next, she was bruised and bloody. With these alternating
images, the essence of her existence was lost.
Final considerations
From what we can see, the non-mental functioning of battered women
is the result of early experiences that damage the body and hinder the
construction of an adequate psychic apparatus. These women were
bombarded, from very young, with sensory experiences that exceeded
their physical and mental capacities, and they had nobody to prevent
this from happening. They had no object that could discriminate, dif-
ferentiate, and set boundaries for this sensory and bodily experience,
resulting in sensory dismantling and de-mentalization. All of them
lacked an object that might act as a protective screen from the intrusive
stimulation to which they were exposed: an object with the function
of a boundary for the overflowing impulses, so they were not forced
to eliminate their sensorial and mental apparatus.
The functions of Klein’s “good object” and those of Winnicott’s
“good-enough mother” became clearer with Bion and Meltzer’s work
on the maternal function and its relation to the psychic apparatus,
and the findings on infant observation published by Bick and Harris.
The mother—or her replacement—receives the anxiety of the baby,
contains it, thinks it, and returns it to the baby digested, creating the
possibility of developing a thinking apparatus. This makes it possible
for the sensory impressions of emotional experiences to be trans-
formed into alpha elements, thoughts, connectors, and organizers
of the experience of the world and its events as well as loving and
destructive impulses.
It became clear that we were facing serious thinking disorders
in the repetition of unrelated events in these women’s stories. In
Explorations in Autism, Meltzer differentiates between the secondary
obsessional mechanisms related to motivational or defensive splitting,
which aim for omnipotent control of objects to reduce anxiety, and the
primary obsessional mechanisms that are more related to a “breach in
the logic of cause and effect that leaps from the wish to its fulfillment
without pause, to achieve the means of the transformation” (p. 186).
He also focuses on the relation between the use of autistic and obses-
sional mechanisms and the impossibility of using introjection as “a
means to establish identity” and for “coping with the separation from
external objects”. Existing merely in the sensory realm, dismantled
and with a motor action–reaction ruled by attraction and repulsion,
the self finds it impossible to develop or to differentiate between itself
and the object. This highlights the absence of an internal world and
the fragility of existence in the external world.
isturbance. These mothers are like the mothers in the Ik tribe, who
d
push their children into the world before they have been able to build
the psychic apparatus to perform the functions of perception, atten-
tion, consciousness, judgement, and reflection in relation to the world
and to themselves. These mothers do not assist in the introjection of a
protective object, one that can comfort and alert them, enabling them
to differentiate child from adult, good from bad, and self from object.
They may tolerate, even encourage, aggression and incestuous sexual
abuse. They also fail to provide the kind of integration that performs
differentiating and protective functions of awareness and alertness.
The absence of these objects and their functions leaves these battered
women as sighted but deaf, in some cases, and able to listen but blind
in others. They do not manage to articulate their experiences, a failure
that further impedes a helpful building of their own mind. As a result,
they remain trapped in an abusive situation, especially in a society in
which maltreatment and destructiveness are part of daily life.
Alberto Hahn
T
he content of our conference, as reflected in this book, bore
witness to Donald Meltzer’s enduring relevance, his inspiring
ideas, and his attitude of profound respect for patients’ relation
to their internal and external world—in particular to the infantile
parts of the self. During the conference, we heard repeated mention
of Meltzer’s legacy as a teacher and a thinker. The breadth of papers
showed how important this influence remains, both in our clinical
work and in our motivation to explore further the potential and the
boundaries of his psychoanalytic insights.
It is in this spirit that I want to address myself briefly to some
issues that have found their way into my own clinical work, the super
vision of colleagues, and the process of teaching Meltzer’s life’s work,
for over four decades. Meltzer wrote widely about the nature of the
psychoanalytic processes he was observing, their theoretical connota-
tions, and their clinical and technical implications, and I think that the
foundations laid down in The Psychoanalytical Process (1967) 50 years
ago remain a beacon that throws a shaft of light on the worlds of the
two parties involved in every analytic process we engage in. This
view, which argues that our internal world determines our outlook
and creates the possibility of deploying our observational and analytic
function on the process, seems to remain a permanent fundamental
233
237
Williams (Ed.), Collected Papers of Martha Harris and Esther Bick. Strath
Tay: Clunie Press, 1987.
Bion, W. R. (1959). Attacks on linking. International Journal of Psychoanalysis,
40: 308–315. Reprinted in: Second Thoughts (pp. 93–109). London Heine-
mann; reprinted London: Karnac, 1984.
Bion, W. R. (1961). Experiences in Groups. London: Tavistock Publications.
Bion, W. R. (1962a). Learning from Experience. London: Heinemann; reprinted
London: Karnac, 1984.
Bion, W. R. (1962b). The psychoanalytic theory of thinking. International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 43: 306–310. Reprinted as “A Theory of Think-
ing” in: Second Thoughts (pp. 110–119). London Heinemann; reprinted
London: Karnac, 1984.
Bion, W. R. (1963). Elements of Psycho-Analysis. London: Heinemann;
reprinted London: Karnac, 1984.
Bion, W. R. (1965). Transformations: Change from Learning to Growth. London:
Heinemann; reprinted London: Karnac, 1984.
Bion, W. R. (1970). Attention and Interpretation: A Scientific Approach to
Insight in Psycho-Analysis and Groups. London: Tavistock Publications;
reprinted London: Karnac, 1984.
Bion, W. R. (1979). Making the best of a bad job. In: Clinical Papers and Other
Works (pp. 321–331). London: Karnac, 1994.
Bion, W. R. (1980). Bion in New York and São Paulo. Strath Tay: Clunie Press.
Bion, W. R. (1991). A Memoir of the Future. London: Karnac.
Bion, W. R. (1997). Taming Wild Thoughts. London: Karnac.
Birksted-Breen, D. (1996). Phallus, penis and mental space. International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 77: 649–657.
Bloom, H. (1999). Shakespeare: The Invention of the Human. London: Fourth
Estate.
Brentano, F. (1874). Psychology from an Empirical Standpoint. London: Rout-
ledge, 1995.
Briggs, A. (Ed.) (2012). Waiting to Be Found: Papers on Children in Care. Lon-
don: Karnac.
Britton, R. (1989). The missing link: Parental sexuality in the Oedipus
complex. In: R. Britton, M. Feldman, & E. O’Shaughnessy, The Oedipus
Complex Today: Clinical Implications. London: Karnac.
Britton, R. (1994). The blindness of the seeing eye: Inverse symmetry as a
defence against reality. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 14: 365–378.
Caccia, O. (2007). La nascita della mente. In: S. Latmiral & C. Lombardo
(Eds.), Pensieri premature. Rome: Borla.
Campbell, J. (2006). Homelessness and containment: A psychotherapy
project with homeless people and workers in the homeless field. Psy-
choanalytic Psychotherapy, 20 (3): 157–174.
Canham, H. (1999). The development of the concept of time in fostered
and adopted children. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 19 (2): 160–172. Reprinted
in: A. Briggs (Ed.), Waiting to Be Found: Papers on Children in Care (pp.
61–71). London: Karnac, 2012.
Cassese, S. F. (2002). Introduction to the Work of Donald Meltzer. London:
Karnac.
Cassirer, E. (1995). The Philosophy of Symbolic Forms. Oxford: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Cecchi, V. (1990). Analysis of a little girl with an autistic syndrome. Interna-
tional Journal of Psychoanalysis, 71: 403–410.
Colarusso, C. (1979). The development of time sense: From birth to object
constancy. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 60: 243–251.
Coleridge, S. T. (1816). The Statesman’s Manual, ed. R. J. White. London:
Routledge, 1972.
de Sola, B. M. (1991). El amigo intimo de la adolescencia. Su lugar en el
proceso psicoanalitico. Psicoanalisis APdeBA, 13 (3): 565–583.
Descartes, R. (1641). Méditations métaphysiques. Paris: PUF, 2004.
Edel, L. (1980). The Genius and the Injustice Collector: A memoir of James
Joyce. The American Scholar, 49 (4): 467–487.
Ellmann, R. (1982). James Joyce. New York: Oxford University Press.
Emanuel, R. (2001). A-void: An explanation of defence against seeing noth-
ingness. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 82: 1069–1084.
Emanuel, R. (2012). An application of Bion’s theory of thinking and trans-
formation in hallucinations: An experience of supervision with Donald
Meltzer. Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 38 (3): 282–301.
Etchegoyen, H. (1991). The Fundamentals of Psychoanalytic Technique. Lon-
don: Karnac.
Feldman, M. (1989). The Oedipus complex: Manifestations in the inner
world and the therapeutic situation. In: R. Britton, M. Feldman, & E.
O’Shaughnessy, The Oedipus Complex Today: Clinical Implications (pp.
103–128). London: Karnac.
Fisher, J. V. (1999). The Uninvited Guest: Emerging from Narcissism towards
Marriage. London: Karnac.
Fisher, J. V. (2006). The emotional experience of K. International Journal of
Psychoanalysis, 87: 1221–1237.
Fisher, J. V. (2009). The Macbeths in the consulting room. Fort Da, 15 (2):
33–55.
Fisher, J. V. (2011). The Proleptic Imagination. Unpublished paper.
Meltzer, D., & Harris, M. (2011). Adolescence: Talks and Papers by Donald
Meltzer and Martha Harris, ed. M. Harris Williams. London: Karnac.
Meltzer, D., & Harris Williams, M. (1988). The Apprehension of Beauty: The
Role of Aesthetic Conflict in Development, Art and Violence. Strath Tay:
Clunie Press.
Milton, J. (1644). Areopagitica: A Speech for the Liberty of Unlicensed Printing.
London: Hunter, 1819.
Minghella, A. (Dir.) (1999). The Talented Mr. Ripley [Film]. Hollywood, CA:
Paramount.
Mitchell, J. (1996). Sexuality and psychoanalysis: Hysteria. British Journal of
Psychotherapy, 12: 473–479.
Mitchell, J. (1998). Trauma, recognition and the place of language. Diacrit-
ics, 28: 121–133.
Money-Kyrle, R. (1968). Cognitive development. In: Man’s Picture of His
World and Three Papers (pp. 209–228). London: Harris Meltzer Trust,
2014.
Morgan, M. (1995). The projective gridlock: A form of projective identifica-
tion in couple relationships, In: S. Ruszczinski & J. Fisher (Eds.), Intru-
siveness and Intimacy in the Couple. London: Karnac.
Mukhopadhyay, T. R. (2000). The Mind Tree. New York: Arcade.
Mukhopadhyay, T. R. (2008). How Can I Talk if My Lips Don’t Move? New
York: Arcade.
Muñoz Vila, C. (2011). Reflexiones psicoanalíticas. Bogotá: Pontificia Univer-
sidad Javeriana.
Muñoz Vila, C. (2014). Clínica psicoanalítica. Doce estudios de casos y algunas
notas de técnica. Bogotá: Pontificia Universidad Javeriana.
Nemas, C. (2000). Development is beauty, growth is ethics. In: M. Cohen
& A. Hahn (Eds.), Exploring the Work of Donald Meltzer. London: Kar-
nac.
Nemas, C. (2016). Courage and sincerity as a base for reverie and interpre-
tation. In: D. Blue & C. Harrang (Eds.), From Reverie to Interpretation:
Transforming Thought into the Action of Psychoanalysis (pp. 131–144).
London: Karnac.
Oates, J. C. (2015). Inspiration and obsession in life and literature. New York
Review of Books, 13 August.
Oelsner, M., & Oelsner, R. (2005). About supervision: An interview with
Donald Meltzer. British Journal of Psychotherapy, 21 (3): 455–461.
Ogden, T. H. (2005). This Art of Psychoanalysis: Dreaming Undreamt Dreams
and Interrupted Cries. London: Routledge.
O’Shaughnessy, E. (2015). The absent object. In: Inquiries in Psychoanalysis:
autism. In: Autistic Barriers in Neurotic Patients (2nd revised edition, pp.
76–93). London: Karnac, 1994.
Tustin, F. (1990). The Protective Shell in Children and Adults. London: Karnac.
Tustin, F. (1994). The perpetuation of an error. Journal of Child Psychotherapy,
20 (1): 3–23.
Weininger, O. (1992). Elective mutism in children: A Kleinian approach.
In: Melanie Klein: From Theory to Reality (pp. 133–150). London: Karnac.
Wilkinson, B. (2012). Phil Brown, Il Avalit [Book review]. Times Literary
Supplement, 14 September.
Williams, G. (1997). Internal Landscapes and Foreign Bodies. London: Duck-
worth.
Williams, M. H. (2003). Conversations with internal objects: Family and
narrative structure in Homer’s Odyssey. British Journal of Psychotherapy,
20 (2): 219–235.
Williams, M. H. (Ed.) (2008). A post-Kleinian model for aesthetic criticism.
PsyArt. An Online Journal for the Psychological Study of the Arts. Available
at: http://psyartjournal.com/article/show/harris_williams-a_post_
kleinian_model_for_aesthetic_critl
Williams, M. H. (Ed.) (2010). A Meltzer Reader. London: Karnac.
Williams, M. H. (Ed.) (2015). Art and Analysis: An Adrian Stokes Reader. Lon-
don: Harris Meltzer Trust.
Winnicott, D. W. (1951). Transitional objects and transitional phenomena.
In: Through Paediatrics to Psycho-Analysis. London: Tavistock Publica-
tions, 1958; reprinted London: Karnac, 1984.
Winnicott, D. W. (1952). Anxiety associated with Insecurity. In: Through
Paediatrics to Psycho-Analysis. London: Tavistock Publications, 1958;
reprinted London: Karnac, 1984.
Winnicott, D. W. (1960). The theory of the parent–infant relationship. In:
The Maturational Processes and the Facilitating Environment. London:
Hogarth Press, 1965; reprinted London: Karnac, 1990.
Winnicott, D. W. (1963). The mentally ill in your caseload. In: The Matura-
tional Processes and the Facilitating Environment. London: Hogarth Press,
1965; reprinted London: Karnac, 1990.
Winnicott, D. W. (1974). Fear of breakdown. In: Psycho-Analytic Explora-
tions, ed. C. Winnicott, R. Shepherd, & M. Davis. London: Karnac, 1989.
Winterson, J. (2011). Why Be Happy When You Could Be Normal? London:
Vintage.
Woolf, V. (1929). A Room of One’s Own. New York: Harcourt Brace, 1989.
Yeats, W. B. (1989). Poems. London: Macmillan.
249