0% found this document useful (0 votes)
112 views52 pages

Kitty Balazadeh Josefin Hansson Bachelor Thesi PDF

This study aimed to investigate the relationship between the Dark Triad personality traits (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy), psychological resilience, and the moderating effect of locus of control. An online survey was completed by 57 students measuring these variables. Results showed significant associations between the full Dark Triad construct, narcissism individually, and resilience. Locus of control was found to moderate the relationships between the full Dark Triad and Machiavellianism with resilience. Narcissism predicted resilience regardless of locus of control, while psychopathy showed no association. All Dark Triad traits had unique relationships with locus of control and resilience.

Uploaded by

Adri Espinillo
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
112 views52 pages

Kitty Balazadeh Josefin Hansson Bachelor Thesi PDF

This study aimed to investigate the relationship between the Dark Triad personality traits (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy), psychological resilience, and the moderating effect of locus of control. An online survey was completed by 57 students measuring these variables. Results showed significant associations between the full Dark Triad construct, narcissism individually, and resilience. Locus of control was found to moderate the relationships between the full Dark Triad and Machiavellianism with resilience. Narcissism predicted resilience regardless of locus of control, while psychopathy showed no association. All Dark Triad traits had unique relationships with locus of control and resilience.

Uploaded by

Adri Espinillo
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 52

 

Institution of psychology  

The Dark Triad and Psychological Resilience: 


The Moderating Effect of Locus of Control  
 
Den Mörka Triaden och Psykologisk Resiliens:  
Den Modererande Effekten av Kontroll-lokus 

Kitty Balazadeh & Josefin Hansson  


Bachelor thesis in psychology, AT 2020 
 
 
 
 
Supervisor: Katarina ​Veličković 
Examiner: Yunhwan Kim
Abstract

The relationship between the Dark Triad and psychological resilience is understudied and
literature on the subject is insufficient. This study aimed to investigate the association between
the Dark Triad and psychological resilience, and the possibility of locus of control acting as a
moderator of this relationship. The Dark Triad was investigated both as a single construct, as
well as the triad traits (Narcissism, Machiavellianism and Psychopathy) individually. An
electronic survey consisting of the study variables and a total of 81 items was distributed to
students from Lund and Malmö University (​N ​= 57). The results showed significant associations
between the Dark Triad as a whole, narcissism and psychological resilience. Further, Locus of
control was found to moderate the relationship between the Dark Triad and resilience, as well as
Machiavellianism and resilience. Narcissism was the only construct in the Dark Triad that
predicted resilience regardless of Locus of control. Psychopathy had no association with
resilience. In conclusion, all traits had unique associations to Locus of control and resilience.
Larger samples and equal gender distributions are needed for more certain conclusions regarding
these relationships. Finally, since all three Dark Triad traits had unique associations to Locus of
control and resilience, future research could explore the mechanisms and factors that underlie
these differences.

Keywords: ​dark triad, psychological resilience, locus of control, narcissism, machiavellianism,


psychopathy.

1
Sammanfattning

Tidigare forskning och litteratur kring förhållandet mellan den mörka triaden och psykologisk
resiliens är begränsad. Denna studie syftade i huvudsak på att undersöka sambandet mellan den
mörka triaden och psykologisk resiliens, samt möjligheten kring kontroll-lokus som en potentiell
moderator för detta förhållande. Den mörka triaden undersöktes både som ett sammanhållet
konstrukt, likaså även triadegenskaperna Narcissism, Machiavellianism och Psykopati
individuellt. En webbenkät bestående av nämnda variabler och totalt 81 testfrågor distribuerades
till studenter från Lund och Malmö universitet (​N ​= 57). Resultaten visade signifikanta samband
mellan den mörka triaden som helhet, Narcissism och resiliens. Vidare föreföll kontroll-lokus
moderera förhållandet mellan den mörka triaden som helhet och resiliens, likaså
Machiavellianism och resiliens. Narcissism var det enda konstruktet i den mörka triaden som
förutspådde resiliens oavsett nivå av kontroll-lokus. Psykopati hade ingen signifikant koppling
till resiliens. Sammanfattningsvis hade alla tre triadegenskaper unika kopplingar till
kontroll-lokus och resiliens. Större urval och jämnare könsfördelning behövs för säkrare
slutsatser kring dessa förhållanden. Avslutningsvis kan framtida forskning undersöka de
mekanismer och faktorer som står till grund för skillnaderna i triadegenskapernas relation till
kontroll-lokus och resiliens.

Nyckelord: ​mörka triaden, resiliens, kontroll-lokus, narcissism, machiavellianism, psykopati.

2
Preface
First and foremost, a special thanks to our supervisor Katarina, who gave us her unlimited
support and guidance throughout the whole process. Your dedication and competence have been
priceless and we are beyond grateful for your help. We would also like to thank all the
participants of the study for their time and effort, without you this thesis would not be possible.

3
​The Dark Triad and Psychological Resilience: The Moderating Effect of Locus of Control
The good might come with the bad; at least if we examine the many dimensions and facets of
individuals with Dark Triad traits. Is it possible for phenomena that are conventionally thought to
be malicious and destructive to aid with or result in positive abilities and strategies that can help
with overcoming difficulties in life? Literature made on the subject states that among all the
toxic traits of the Dark Triad (further referred to as DT), indeed there are positive sides too:
adaptive strategies, successful crisis management and even being loyal friends are some of them
(​Jonason et al., 2009​; Lyons, 2019; Papageorgiou et al., 2017; Smith et al., 2018, Watts et al.,
2013).
What we found intriguing is that although DT traits and psychological resilience (further
referred to as resilience) at first glance seem to be very distinct, they actually might have
characteristics and behavioural processes in common. Effective coping strategies, stress
management, and an ability to remain calm when needed are some processes that are normally
seen in people that are resilient (Luthar et. al., 2000; Riopel, 2020; Türk-Kurtça & Kocatürk,
2020). However, some studies claim that these kinds of behaviours are seen in some or all of the
DT traits as well (​Furtner et al., 2017; ​Hart et al., 2018; Lyons, 2019; Patton et al., 2018; Smith
et al., 2018; Watts et al., 2013).
​To our knowledge, ​research conducted on the particular relationship between DT traits and
resilience is narrow and deficient (Grover, 2018). To understand the relationship between DT
and resilience better, we will examine the potential moderating effect of Locus of control (further
referred to as LoC). Internal LoC; a sense of being in control of your surroundings and not
internalizing negative emotions and events (Landström et al., 2008; Rotter, 1966) has been
identified as a predictor of resilience (Gamezy, 1993; Werner & Smith, 1982).
Since DT and resilience might share similar adaptive processes, and internal LoC seems to
be a predictor of resilience, we wonder if LoC could have a moderating role on the relationship
between DT and resilience. Regardless of results, we hope that our study might shed a light on
the exact relationships between these constructs and can contribute to a relevant gain of
knowledge in this field of psychology.

4
Theoretical background
The Dark Triad
DT consists of three components: Machiavellianism, subclinical Narcissism and subclinical
Psychopathy. These personality traits are three overlapping traits that share a common core with
each other (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). The shared features among the three traits (although the
level of manifestation of the characteristics might vary) typically are manipulation, selfishness,
callousness and deceitfulness (Jones & Figueredo, 2013; Lyons, 2019; Paulhus & Williams,
2002). The correlation and overlap between all three constructs are ascertained by several studies
(Paulhus & Williams, 2002). Machiavellianism has an overlap with Psychopathy (Fehr et al.,
1992; McHoskey et al., 1998), Narcissism has an overlap with Psychopathy (Gustafson & Ritzer,
1995) and Machiavellianism with Narcissism (McHoskey, 1995).
However, despite the similarities and overlapping features of the constructs they all have
their own unique and differentiating attributes (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). Hereby we will
describe and elucidate the three components of DT to better illustrate their own distinctive
properties.
​ achiavellianism. T
M ​ he emergence and origin of the concept of Machiavellianism (from a
psychological perspective) was influenced by the political strategist Niccolo Machiavelli and
stems from the 1970 publication by Richard Christie and Florence L. Geis (Lyons, 2019). The
authors portray Machiavellianism as consisting of two main characteristics: a sceptical
perspective on human nature and interpersonally manipulating strategies (Christie & Geis, 1970).
Further research has been able to establish that individuals that score high on Machiavellianism
have an increased ability in reaching their own personal goals due to their tactical manipulation,
avoidance of impulsive behaviors and strategic decision making (Lyons, 2019; Paulhus & Jones,
2015).
Nevertheless, the definition and distinction of Machiavellianism from the remaining DT
traits has not always been clear and there has been some uncertainty concerning the essence of
Machiavellianism. Linda Mealey (1995) suggests that Machiavellianism might not be different
from Psychopathy, but alternatively just a less severe and subclinical form of it. Other
researchers argue that Machiavellianism differs from Psychopathy by one important aspect:
Machiavellian individuals care about a positive reputation and seek to build alliances, whereas

5
psychopathic people lack these characteristics and pay little attention to the consequences of
their behaviour (Paulhus & Jones, 2015). However, modern research more often than not
indicates there are many dissimilarities and contrasts between the two traits and that they should
be examined and treated as separate constructs (​Furtner et al., 2017; ​Lyons, 2019; Paulhus &
Jones, 2015​)​.
In this study, Machiavellianism will be treated as an independent construct that, along with
the other traits in DT, will be measured in the Short Dark Triad test (SD3.1.1) where the key
aspects are manipulativeness, callous affect, strategic-calculating orientation (​Jones & Paulhus,
2014).  
​Narcissism. ​Narcissism as a personality phenomenon can exist on both a clinical and
subclinical level. When discussed in the context and as a component of DT, Narcissism is
conceptualized as a subclinical concept and not as a pathological personality disorder (Balaji &
Balasundaram, 2014; Paulhus & Williams, 2002).
Individuals high on Narcissism are characterized with traits such as a grandiose image of
the self, sense of superiority, low levels of empathy, self-sufficiency, authority and a need for
social admiration - among other features (Balaji & Balasundaram, 2014; Lyons, 2019; Miller et
al., 2011). The results of possessing these kinds of traits might lead narcissistic individuals to
have a sense of power over people and use their interpersonal relationships to achieve their goals,
while still maintaining their grandiose self-view (Brunell & Campbell, 2011; Lyons, 2019).
Miller and his colleagues (2011) suggest however, that Narcissism can be divided into two
subtypes: ​grandiose​ and ​vulnerable.​ Grandiose narcissists have an inflated level of confidence
and self-esteem and a dominant interpersonal style, whereas the vulnerable type is described as
insecure and using fake grandiosity as a strategy to mask feelings of inadequacy (Lyons, 2019;
Miller et al., 2011). In this study we will use the definition of SD3.1.1 (​Jones & Paulhus, 2014)
where the key aspects of Narcissism are distinguished from the other two traits: a never-ending
quest for ego-reinforcement and grandiosity.
​ imilar to Narcissism, in the context of DT, Psychopathy is seen as a
​Psychopathy. S
subclinical condition and not a pathological occurrence. There are some discussions and debates
about which features should be included in the definition of Psychopathy. One subclinical
definition is identified as containing high impulsivity, low levels of anxiety, thrill-seeking and
lack of empathy (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). On the other hand, a triarchic model suggests

6
Psychopathy to mainly consist of boldness, meanness and disinhibition (Patrick et al., 2009). A
third model divides Psychopathy into primary and secondary, where primary is seen as a
successful type that can achieve power in society thanks to low levels of guilt and empathy,
whereas secondary Psychopathy is seen as unsuccessful, often ending up in criminality (Lyons,
2019). Psychopathy, when tested with SD3.1.1 (​Jones & Paulhus, 2014)​, is defined by the key
aspects which separates Psychopathy from the other traits: deficits in affect (i.e., callousness) and
deficits in self-control (i.e., impulsivity).
​Psychological resilience. ​ Resilience has been defined in various ways over the years.
Luthar et. al. (2000) addresses the problem regarding the way it differs across theoretical
literature and also that resilience should be understood as an individual characteristic that
presents itself while the individual is being exposed to significant risks. Luthar et. al. (2000)
argue that difficulties could arise while comparing resilient people’s levels of experienced
adversity. What is a “significant risk” and what determines the individual's level of resilience?
One way of operationalizing the concept of resilience is according to the Connor-Davidson
Resilience scale (CD-RISC); originally developed by Kathryn M. Connor and Jonathan R.T.
Davidson (2003). According to this scale, resilience is a trait measured by the individual’s ability
to recover after facing tragedy, trauma or other stressful events. Thus, resilient people have an
ability to get through and move past life traumas, but also to grow and flourish despite adversity.
Türk-Kurtça and Kocatürk (2020) define resilience as “the ability to maintain a high level of
psychological function and to adapt healthily after being exposed to trauma or serious stress”
(p.105). In this thesis, the Connor-Davidson Resilience (CD-RISC) scale will be used for
measuring resilience. The CD-RISC is measuring several components of resilience such as
effective adaptation to change and adversity, efficient coping skills in regards to stress, emotion
regulation and the ability to stay focused and motivated despite hardships or sudden changes
(Riopel, 2020).
​Locus of control. ​LoC is a way of describing one's feelings of being in - or out of - control
of events. Rotter (1966) originally proposed LoC as a “generalized and enduring belief about
how responsive and controllable our environment is” (p.1). People with an external LoC explain
events like personal success and failure as things out of their control, while those with an internal
LoC see themselves as being in control of these events (Landström et al., 2008). This causes
individuals with an internal LoC to feel more at ease and in control despite unfamiliar and

7
uncomfortable situations while those with an external LoC naturally are not able to feel the same
sense of inner control. An external LoC has also been connected with increased feelings of
helplessness as well as poorer well-being and job satisfaction (Näswall et. al, 2008) while an
internal LoC has been linked to both empowerment and resilience (Kieffer, 1984). LoC is often
confused with closely related concepts like self-efficacy, attribution styles and explanatory
styles. While these are similar, what distinguishes LoC from others is how it refers to “the degree
to which a person believes that he or she can influence his or her life through his/her own
actions” (Rotter, 1966, p.1). Furthermore, Rotter (1966) emphasizes that the individual's
perception of whether ​reinforcement​ is - 1. A result of personal behavior or characteristics or 2.
A result of chance or other unpredictable forces - ultimately is what determines if the LoC is
more internal or external.
The background for the LoC theory can be found in Rotter’s Social learning theory (1954,
1955, 1960), hence why the individual’s beliefs about reinforcement is crucial in understanding
the concept of LoC. Having the social learning theory in mind, Rotter stated that an individual's
personal history and beliefs about reinforcements is what makes people differ in locus of control;
in other words, the way they attribute reinforcement as a result of personal behavior or as
something out of their control (Rotter, 1966). In this thesis, the Internal-External LoC scale,
developed by Rotter (1966) will be the chosen measure of LoC.
Previous research
Positive aspects of the Dark Triad
Despite the negative qualities of DT, there is also research highlighting the positive sides.
Machiavellianism in particular has been associated with self-control (Furtner et al., 2017) and
emotion regulation (Lyons, 2019), perhaps leading to a sense of control, power or calmness
(Gustafson & Ritzer, 1995; Lau & Marsee, 2013). Further studies have shown high-macs to be
pragmatic, sensitive to social context, and possessing high emotional intelligence (Czibor &
Bereczkei, 2012; Furtner et al., 2017; Lyons, 2019). Compared to the other two traits of DT,
Machiavellianism has been linked to a more cautious approach to life with less impulsivity and
risk taking (Lyons, 2019; Malesza & Ostaszewski, 2016).
Some characteristics​ of Narcissism are also related to charm and leadership (​Furtner et al.,
2017; ​Lyons, 2019) and studies done on former US presidents high on grandiose type of
Narcissism show successful achievements in regards to crisis management, popularity and

8
initiativeness (Watts et al., 2013). Other studies have also found that Narcissism, in contrast to
Machiavellianism and Psychopathy, is not necessarily linked to low empathy (Jonason & Kroll,
2015; Lyons, 2019; Lyons et al., 2019) and narcissists do in fact also possess high emotional
intelligence and are sensitive to social cues (​Furtner et al., 2017; ​Petrides et al., 2011).
Narcissism was also found to negatively correlate with alexithymia, which suggests the ability to
use own emotions as social cues and thus perhaps also understanding others better (Cairncross et
al., 2013; Lyons, 2019). These positive characteristics and features of Narcissism are also in line
with the findings of Lyons, Evans and Helle (2019) who concluded that Narcissism could act as
a buffer against negative health outcomes and high stress-levels.
Some of the more advantageous sides of Psychopathy is the ability to read negative
emotions (Demetrioff et al., 2017). Demetrioff et al. found that individuals scoring high on
Psychopathy had an increased ability to recognize emotional states such as sadness. This instance
might possibly lead to an advantage in situations when offering support might be strategic to
gain personal needs. Dutton (2012) also describes some of the positives of Psychopathy: high
confidence and self-esteem, low levels of anxiety and the ability to stay calm during stressful
situations.
When examining some of the positive aspects of DT traits, one might ponder whether
some of these individuals can be psychologically resilient; e.g. if their confidence, self-control
and/or low anxiety can buffer against adversity. Hereby the relationship between DT and
resilience will be examined.
​Dark triad and Resilience. ​As previously stated, the research connecting resilience to DT
by these specific constructs is quite limited (Grover, 2018). What we know today about this
relationship is ambiguous and needs further clarification since research on wellbeing and DT has
utilized concepts such as anxiety, depression, stress, coping, mental toughness and not resilience
per se​ (Aghababaei & Blachnio, 2015; ​Grover, 2018;​ ​J​onason et al., 2015; ​Lyons, et al., 2019;
Rudich et al., 2014). With this remark in mind, we will proceed to clarify research that implies
the relationship between resilience and DT should be examined more closely.
​Lyons, Evans and Helle (2019), examined the relationship between DT and the effects of
adversity and concluded that Narcissism in fact could act as a buffer against stressful events and
negative health outcomes such as anxiety and depression. However, this correlation was not
found in regards to Machiavellianism or Psychopathy. They found that Machiavellianism

9
(especially) instead of acting as a buffer, may predispose individuals to mental distress after
negative events and cumulative stress, whereas narcissistic traits might aid in coping with
hardships in life.
Another perspective is elucidated by Kajonius & Björkman (2018) when examining the
relationships between DT traits and perceived stress. The researchers found perceived stress to
have a strong positive correlation with vulnerable Narcissism, whereas Psychopathy and
grandiose Narcissism had a weak negative association with perceived stress. Machiavellianism
did not seem to relate to perceived stress to a significant degree. Overall, Kajonius and Björkman
(2018), concluded that the only trait that had a strong association with perceived stress was
vulnerable Narcissism.
Psychopathy has also been linked to very low levels of anxiety (Cleckley, 1976) which in
turn has been proposed to be an important resilience factor (Sandvik et al., 2015). Additionally,
more recent research implies that psychopathic traits might buffer individuals from the negative
effects on mental health caused by chronic stress (Anestis et al., 2017; Sandvik et al., 2015).
The research above investigates the relationship between DT and factors in some way
closely related to and included in the definition of resilience. When further examining the
construct of resilience as a whole, in relation to constructive aspects of DT, we find some, at
least superficial, similarities. As previously mentioned, individuals scoring high on resilience
share the ability to manage stress effectively, regulate overwhelming feelings, stay focused
during adversity and efficiently adapt to change (Luthar et. al., 2000; Riopel, 2020; Türk-Kurtça
& Kocatürk, 2020). These processes are also at times seen in some -or all - of DT traits (Hart et
al., 2018; Lyons, 2019; Lyons et al., 2019; Patton et al., 2018; Smith et al., 2018; Watts et al.,
2013). A study conducted by Grover (2018) indicates that the individual’s level of emotional
intelligence, another predictor of resilience (Schneider et al., 2013), was able to regulate the
negative effects of DT traits, as well as increase its positive effects. This is interesting in regards
to previous research stating that narcissists might possess high emotional intelligence (Petrides et
al., 2011; ​Furtner et al., 2017​). Further, the hypothesized moderator, LoC, and its associations
with resilience and DT will be explored, starting with examining its associations to resilience.
​ armezy (1993) and Werner and Smith (1982)
Psychological resilience and LoC. G
established internal LoC as one of the main features that affected resilience. Edwards, Catling
and Parry (2016) found that an internal LoC was an independent predictor of resilience. These

10
findings were also supported by Kieffer (1984) and in line with recent research showing a
significant, negative correlation between external locus of control and resilience (Türk-Kurtça &
Kocatürk, 2020). Furthermore, LoC has been associated with an improved ability to tolerate
external pressure, inner feelings of independence and more effective response style while facing
adversity (Ören, 1991 as quoted by Türk-Kurtça & Kocatürk, 2020), which could protect the
individual while experiencing setbacks (Juby & Rycraft, 2004). Agaibi and Wilson (2005)
additionally showed that an internal LoC can work as a protective factor against PTSD and other
kinds of problems regarding one's mental health that could arise in face of adversity. On the
contrary, external LoC has been associated with a significantly higher risk of experiencing these
kinds of mental health problems (Stewart & Yuen, 2011).
​ oC may work as a moderator of the proposed relationship between
​Dark triad and LoC. L
DT and resilience in the way that the strength of the relationship depends on whether one’s sense
of control is internal or external. ​Below, the concept of LoC will be defined and elucidated in the
context of the three DT traits.
​ ​review of meta-analyses made by Mudrack (1990), stated that
Machiavellianism. A
Machiavellianism was related to an external LoC throughout several studies. This is also in line
with recent research: ​Stanescu and Mohorea (2016) found that ​Machiavellianism​ had a positive
significant correlation with LoC in professional environments. These results also replicate the
findings of ​Gable, Hollon and Dangello (1990) as quoted by Singh, Rita and Suri (2017), as well
as Marolodo et. al (1976), who found that Machiavellianism correlated significantly with an
external LoC.
Another study examining the relationship between DT, LoC and positive/negative affect
conducted on entrepreneurship and psychology students found ​Machiavellianism​ having a
positive correlation with external LoC ​in the student group of entrepreneurs, however this
relationship was not found in psychology students (Rapp-Ricciardi et al, 2018).
​Narcissism. ​Mathieu & St-Jean (2013) found that narcissists had a high sense of personal
control over their life, which indicates internal LoC. However, this kind of measurement might
have limitations and O’Reilly, Doerr, Caldwell and Chatman (2013) discuss narcissists'
emotional vulnerability and way of defending themselves while belittling others seems like an
external LoC. ​Furthermore,​ ​as previously mentioned, the construct of Narcissism can be divided
into two subcategories: grandiose and vulnerable ​(Lyons, 2019; Miller et al., 2011)​. Sullivan

11
(2020) explored both types and their connection to LoC and found that only the vulnerable type
of Narcissism had a correlation with external LoC ​(Sullivan, 2020).
​ esearch regarding Psychopathy in relation to LoC is limited and there ​has
​Psychopathy. R
not been much research elucidating the relationship​ as compared to the other two DT traits.
Rapp-Ricciardi et al. (2018) found a significant correlation between Psychopathy and internal
LoC in a sample of entrepreneurship students, but further stated that LoC did not have any
significant associations when compared to the partial constructs of the groups. Earlier research
showed a significant positive link between external LoC and psychopathic tendencies (​Helode &
Kapai 1986),​ ​however the scale used for measuring psychopathic tendencies is not used for
testing Psychopathy in the context of DT. Further ​Turkcapar et. al (2002) found that an external
LoC was related to antisocial personality disorder (ASPD). ASPD and Psychopathy have many
traits in common (Blackburn, 2007), however they are still two separate constructs on different
clinical levels.
​Having examined the LoC of all three traits in DT, indeed it seems like the style of LoC
varies depending on which of the traits is being investigated, or the context of the conducted
research (sample size, demographics, clinical/non clinical etc). Nevertheless, it seems like
Narcissism might have stronger associations with an internal LoC, and Machiavellianism and
Psychopathy with external LoC. However, these relations are not ideally clear. Subsequently, it
is relevant to investigate this matter further and explore whether LoC could act as a moderator
between DT and resilience.
This study
The main aim of this study is to understand ​whether DT traits are associated with resilience,
despite their collection of negative and malevolent characteristics. Another aim is to clarify
whether this relationship is dependent on the level of LoC. Finally, ​this study aims to elucidate
the relationship between resilience and DT as a whole, as well as its three constituents
separately, namely Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy.​ ​Thus, the research questions
are as follows:
1. Are individuals who score high on DT traits (Narcissism, Machiavellianism, Psychopathy)
more psychologically resilient?
2. Does LoC moderate the relationship between DT traits and resilience?
- If so, in what way does LoC moderate the relationship?

12
Hypotheses
H1:​ Dark Triad as a whole, as well as Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy
separately, will have a positive association with resilience.
H2: ​This relationship will be stronger in case of internal LoC, and weaker in case of external
LoC.

Figure 1

Moderating model of the proposed relationship between DT, resilience and LoC.

Method
Design
This study​ ​implemented a cross-sectional design with a predictive, however exploratory
approach. The independent variables were DT as a whole and three DT traits, whereas the
dependent variable was resilience. LoC was hypothesized as a moderator of the relationship
between the variables.
Participants
The participants in this study were university students studying at Lund University and Malmö
University. The choice to conduct this study on university students was made since students are
an easily accessible population for the aim of the research. Lyons (2019) also underlines that

13
students are a suitable population if research needs to be done on a non-clinical population. The
rationale behind choosing to include both students from Lund and Malmö is that we aimed for
having an as heterogeneous student population as possible. Generalization is an important and
relevant matter when examining DT traits since previous empirical evidence in this field usually
has been conducted on WEIRD (white, educated, rich, industrialized and democratic) samples
(Henrich et al., 2010; Lyons, 2019).
Measures
In this study, a total of three instruments were used:
​Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale.​ (​CD-RISC;​ ​Connor & Davidson, 2003​)​. For
measuring resilience, a Swedish, validated version of the CD-RISC scale was used (see appendix
A). The instrument consists of 25 items in total, with no reversed items or fillers. Every item in
the test has a 5-point rating, ranging from not true at all (0) to true nearly all the time (4), thus a
total score can range between 0 to 100 on this scale (Riopel, 2020). The internal consistency of
the scale was considered good (α = .89).
​Short Dark Triad.​ (​SD3;​ ​Jones & Paulhus, 2014​)​. SD3 was developed with the goal to be
able to test all three DT traits in the same study. A Swedish, validated version (SD.3.1.1; ​Lindén
et al., 2013) ​of the test was implemented (see appendix B). The scale has a total of 27 items, nine
items for each trait. A total of five items (indicated with “R”) in the scale are reverse coded.
Every item corresponds to a statement to take a stand on, with each item consisting of a
five-point Likert-type format, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). A mean
value of the total scores for each participant are further calculated and range from 1 to 5. Internal
consistency of the subscales was fair for Narcissism (α = .71) and good for Machiavellianism (α
= .82) and Psychopathy (α = .82).
It should be noted that scores above the normal range of this scale are simply subclinical,
thus not able to indicate any occurrence of personality disorders or pathology.
​The Internal/External LoC scale.​ ​(Rotter, 1966). This scale, used for measuring LoC,
consists of 29 items, six of them being so-called filler items (see appendix C and D). Each item
has two answering options where the participant selects the statement that they agree with more.
After the participant completes the test, the sum of the scores is calculated. The final score
ranges from 0 to 23, with a higher score indicating external LoC, and lower score indicating
internal LoC. ​The ​Internal/External LoC scale was translated from English to Swedish using

14
back-to-back translation, with the help of individuals fluent in both languages. Internal
consistency of the scale was found to be fair (α = .73).
​Demographic variables.​ ​The demographic data included age, gender and the university
one attends.
Procedure
The data were collected using a survey method through the web-based survey tool Qualtrics
(https://www.qualtrics.com) during the period of 23/11/20-6/12/20. A survey link was posted on
personal Facebook accounts and in student groups in Malmö and Lund. The three questionnaires
consisting of DT, resilience and LoC were integrated and a total of 81 items were included in the
test. The first page consisted of demographic questions, followed by three sections testing the
constructs individually. The demographic items were obligatory, however for gender there was a
possibility to refuse to answer and this question was open-ended. The estimated time for
finishing the survey was roughly ten minutes.
Criteria for participating in the research were being a student at Lund or Malmö University
and being of age 18 or above. The sampling method used was a convenience sample, however
the participants were also encouraged to share the survey with other students studying in Lund or
Malmö University, thus implementing the snowballing technique.
Statistical analysis
Data screening
The data analyses such as descriptive statistics, t-tests, correlations and moderation analysis were
performed through the statistical programme jamovi (1.2.27.0).
A total of 82 responses from the survey were originally recorded, however, not all were
considered valid. Unfinished reports that had been automatically sent in through qualtrics were
removed from the study and considered invalid responses. Responses made by individuals under
the age of 18 were also withdrawn. A total of 25 responses were excluded from the data. Thus,
57 valid responses remained.
After filtering out the unfinished responses, the data were coded according to the original
format of the three scales (see measures). SD.3.1.1 contained five reversed items and the
internal/external locus of control scale included six filler items. The alpha level was set on 0.05
(two tailed) which is in line with previous research conducted on similar types of constructs and
testing variables.

15
​ he analysis conducted in this study initially explored descriptive statistics
Data analysis. T
for all variables. No outliers that affected the mean scores on any of the study variables were
found. Using independent t-tests, gender differences on DT values were explored.
The normal distribution of the variables of the study population was examined, however
due to the sample size these calculations were not considered fully reliable. Thus, the distribution
of the scales was investigated using the standardized skewness and kurtosis values, further
comparing them to the benchmark for the 95% confidence interval.
Additionally, alpha levels and the internal consistency of the three study scales were
calculated (see measures). Correlations between all variables included in the study were also
explored, and moderation analyses were performed using the medmod package​1​ in jamovi.
The moderating effect of LoC on the relationship between DT and resilience was explored
both with DT as a whole, as well as the three traits separately. Both moderation estimates and
simple slope estimates were performed and analyzed.
Ethical considerations
This study did not violate any of the ethical aspects of psychological research. However, the
purpose of the study was not entirely disclosed. When the survey was sent out, it was described
as testing “psychological processes linked to self image” instead of disclosing what constructs
were tested. The participants were informed that their participation was completely voluntary
and that it was entirely acceptable to discontinue the participation and drop out of the study at
any time without having to explain themselves. To ensure that participation was voluntary and
that the age and university requirements were met, the participants had to check a box to give
their informed consent to be a part of the study based on this.
The use of an online survey is advantageous from an ethical perspective since the
respondents remain anonymous, can ignore or cancel the questionnaire at any time and
participate at their own convenience (Miner & Jayaratne, 2014). Also, to guarantee that the
ethical requirement of confidentiality was met, all answers of the questionnaire were anonymized
and only handled by the authors and the supervisor of this thesis. The debriefing process took
place when a facebook status was created stating that the thesis was done and if anyone who

1
​The moderation analysis provided by this package runs a Wald test and computes standardized Z-scores,
comparing them to a Chi-square distribution, as opposed to a student’s t distribution.

16
participated in the study would like to see the results of the research, they could get in touch to
get a copy of the thesis.
Results
Descriptive statistics
A total of 57 participants were included in the study, whereas 40 of them were women, and 17
men. 0 participants stated any other gender identity. Age of the sample ranged between 19-51 (​M
= 24, ​SD ​= 4.9). Mean scores for all DT variables (DT as a whole and traits separately) were
overall found to be higher for men in comparison with women (see Table 1). Regarding DT as a
whole, the difference in mean scores between men and women was significant (​t(​ 55) = -4.25, ​p​ <
.001, ​d​ = 1.12). The difference between genders in Machiavellianism was also found to be
significant (​t(​ 55) = -4.27, ​p​ < .001,​ d​ = 1.18). Further, there was no significant difference
between scores on Narcissism for men and women (​t​(55) = 1.53, ​p​ = .13). The difference in
Psychopathy split by gender was found to be significant (​t​(55) = -4.64, ​p ​< .001,​ d​ = 1.19).
Women in the study population scored significantly lower than norm values (​M ​= 2.4) on
Psychopathy in comparison with men. The mean scores for men and women on CD-RISC were
comparable to the mean scores found by a recent study conducted on a large non-clinical sample
in Sweden (Velickovic et al., 2020). No significant differences between men and women were
found in mean scores on resilience (​t(​ 55) = 1.33, ​p​ = .19), nor LoC (​t(​ 55) = 1.11, ​p​ = .27). It
should be noted that the gender distribution within the sample was unequal, so gender
differences on measures should be interpreted with caution.
​DT as a whole did not have a normal distribution on a 95% confidence level.
Machiavellianism was positively skewed, however with a normal kurtosis. Psychopathy deviated
from a normal distribution on both skewness and kurtosis. Narcissism, resilience and LoC were
normally distributed.

17
Table 1

Mean scores (M), Standard deviations (SD) for women (n = 40) and men (n = 17),
Standardised Skewness and Kurtosis for All Variables (N = 57)

Men Women
Standardised Standardised
Measures
skewness kurtosis
M SD M SD

Dark Triad 2.93 0.62 2.34 0.41 4.21* 4.30*

Machiavellianism 3.08 0.70 2.35 0.53 2.47* 0.81

Narcissism 3.19 0.61 2.94 0.54 0.41 -0.78

Psychopathy 2.54 0.84 1.74 0.45 5.75* 7.98*

Resilience 63.8 14.9 68.9 12.6 -0.93 -0.39

Locus of Control 11.5 3.24 12.8 4.30 0.91 -0.87

*p < .05

Main analyses
Correlations
The correlation matrix is presented with Pearson's product moment correlation coefficient ​r.​
Table 2 contains correlations between DT as a whole, DT traits separately (Machiavellianism,
Narcissism, Psychopathy), resilience and LoC.

18
Table 2

Correlations between All Study Variables (N = 57)

DT M N P R

DT —

M .87*** —

N .74*** .42** —

P .91*** .76*** .52*** —

R .31* .06 .65*** .15 —

LoC -.22 -.17 -.21 -.19 -.30*

Note. Dark Triad (DT), Machiavellianism (M), Narcissism (N), Psychopathy (P), Resilience (R)
and Locus of Control (LoC).

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

DT as a whole correlated significantly with resilience. When examining the DT traits separately,
only Narcissism was found to have a significant positive correlation with resilience, however this
association was strong. Resilience and LoC had a significant negative correlation with each
other. Furthermore, all separate DT traits significantly correlated with each other and with DT as
a whole, with the strongest relationship being between Machiavellianism and Psychopathy. No
significant correlations between DT traits and LoC were found.

19
Moderation Analysis
Dark triad
To test the moderating effect of LoC on the relationship between DT as a whole and resilience, a
regression model was specified in which DT, LoC and their interaction term were entered as
predictors of resilience (​F​(3, 53) = 4.49, ​p​ = .007, ​R2​​ ​= 0.20). ​As indicated in Table 3, there was a
main effect of LoC on resilience, whereas the main effect of DT was not significant. The
interaction between DT and LoC was only marginally significant.

Table 3

Unstandardized Estimates, Standard Errors, z-values and p-values for Dark Triad and Locus of
Control Predicting Resilience

Estimate SE z p
DT 4.25 3.01 1.41 0.16
LoC -1.05 0.39 -2.66** 0.01

DT*LoC -1.69 0.88 -1.92 0.055

**p ​< .01.

Although the moderation effect was not significant, given its marginally significant value, a
simple slopes analysis and plot were performed to illustrate the pattern of effects of LoC. As
shown in Table 4 and Figure 2, for low values of LoC (i.e. internal LoC), the relationship
between DT and resilience was positive. For average and high values of LoC, this relationship
was not significant.

20
Table 4

Simple Sl​ ope ​Estimates for Different Levels of LoC

Estimate SE z p
Average 4.25 3.15 1.35 0.18
Low (-1SD) 11.01 4.07 2.70** 0.01
High (+1SD) -2.50 5.37 -0.47 0.64

Note.​ ​Shows the effect of the predictor (DT) on the dependent variable (resilience) at different
levels of the moderator (LoC)

**​p​ < .01

Figure 2

Effect of DT on resilience at different levels of the moderator (LoC)

​ he regression model with Machiavellianism, LoC and their


Machiavellianism. T
interaction term as predictors on resilience was further performed (​F(​ 3,53) = 3.71, ​p​ < .05, ​R​2​ ​=
0.17). There was a significant main effect of LoC, as well as of the interaction between LoC and
Machiavellianism (Table 5), suggesting a moderating effect of LoC.

21
Table 5

Unstandardized Estimates, Standard Errors, z-values and p-values for Machiavellianism and
Locus of Control Predicting Resilience

Estimate SE z p
M -1.01 2.43 -0.42 0.68
LoC -1.20 0.40 -3.01** 0.003

M*LoC -1.68 0.69 -2.41* 0.02

*​p < .05, **p​ ​ < .01

Investigating the moderating effect further with simple slopes (Table 6 and Figure 2), results
indicated that for high levels of LoC (i.e. external LoC), the relationship between M and
resilience was negative. This relationship was marginally significant. For low levels of LoC (i.e.
internal LoC), this relationship had a positive pattern, although it was not significant.

Table 6

Simple Slope Estimates for Different Levels of LoC

Estimate SE z p
Average -1.01 2.59 -0.30 0.70
Low (-1SD) 5.69 3.56 1.60 0.11
High (+1SD) -7.71 4.12 -1.87 0.06

Note.​ ​Shows the effect of the predictor (M) on the dependent variable (resilience) at different
levels of the moderator (LoC)

22
Figure 3

Effect of Machiavellianism on resilience at different levels of the moderator (LoC)

Narcissism. ​The regression model was also conducted with Narcissism, LoC and their
interaction as predictors of resilience (​F​(3,53) = 14.8, ​p ​= <.001, ​R​2​ ​= 0.46). Only the main effect
of Narcissism was significant (Table 7). This indicates that the higher the levels of Narcissism,
the higher the levels of resilience, regardless of LoC. The main effect of LoC was marginally
significant.

Table 7

Unstandardized Estimates, Standard Errors, z-values and p-values for Narcissism and Locus of
Control Predicting Resilience

Estimate SE z p
N 13.80 2.383 5.79*** <.001
LoC -0.61 0.324 -1.89 0.058

N*LoC -0.52 0.709 -0.74 0.46

***​p​ < .001

23
​ inally, the regression model for Psychopathy, LoC and their interaction as
Psychopathy. F
predictors of resilience was specified (​F​(3,53) = 2.74, ​p​ = 0.053 , ​R2​​ ​= 0.13). As shown in Table
8, the only significant effect in this model was the main effect of LoC. The main effect of
Psychopathy was not significant and neither was the interaction between Psychopathy and LoC,
indicating no moderating effect.

Table 8

Unstandardized Estimates, Standard Errors, z-values and p-values for Psychopathy and Locus of
Control Predicting Resilience

Estimate SE z p
P 0.59 2.47 0.24 0.81
LoC -1.16 0.41 -2.84** 0.005

P*LoC -1.09 0.70 -1.56 0.12

**​p​ < .01.​

Discussion
Main findings
One of the research questions in this thesis was to find out whether individuals high on DT traits
are more resilient than those low on DT traits. Another aim was to understand if LoC could act
as a moderator between this relationship. The correlation analysis indicates that DT as a single
construct, as well as Narcissism, has a significant positive correlation with resilience, which
implies that the first hypothesis is partially supported. These findings are in line with previous
research, indicating that there might be similarities in characteristics between individuals high on
DT and resilient individuals (​Furtner et al., 2017; Hart et al., 2018; Lyons, 2019; Patton et al.,
2018; Smith et al., 2018; Watts et al., 2013)​.​ The finding regarding Narcissism having a stronger
correlation with resilience compared to Machiavellianism and Psychopathy, is also coherent with
research stating that Narcissism might be different from the other triad traits (Lyons et al., 2019;
Rauthmann & Kolar, 2013; Volmer et al., 2016), and that Machiavellianism and Psychopathy are
more closely linked to each other (Mealey, 1995). The close relationship between

24
Machiavellianism and Psychopathy is further confirmed by the correlation analysis showing that
these two had the strongest correlation of all variables.
The results from the moderation analysis shows that the relationship between DT and
resilience, however marginally significant, is stronger and is positive for low levels of LoC. For
those with internal LoC, more DT indicates higher resilience.
The relationship between Narcissism and resilience is not moderated by LoC. The
individuals in the sample with narcissistic traits seem to be resilient, independently of their level
of LoC. This finding might also confirm the uniqueness of Narcissism in the Dark Triad. Thus,
one might wonder why Narcissism is so strongly correlated to resilience. Previously mentioned
research (see theoretical background) shows that Narcissism is not necessarily linked to low
empathy ​(Jonason & Kroll, 2015; Lyons, 2019; Lyons et al., 2019) and that these individuals
possess high emotional intelligence, are sensitive to social cues (​Furtner et al., 2017; ​Petrides et
al., 2011), and that these qualities might buffer against high stress-levels and negative health
outcomes (Lyons, Evans & Helle, 2019). Perhaps these characteristics, that people high on
Psychopathy and Machiavellianism not necessarily possess, is what makes narcissists resilient
despite their level of LoC.
However, for the model consisting of Machiavellianism and resilience, LoC undoubtedly
works as a moderator between the relationship. For Machiavellianism, level of resilience is
moderated by LoC; Machiavellians are psychologically resilient if possessing an internal LoC.
Thus, the second hypothesis is supported in regards to Machiavellianism​. Also, interestingly, for
those with an external LoC, machiavellianism was negatively associated with resilience. A
possible explanation for this finding might be due to the manipulative and strategic
characteristics of machiavellians. If they perceive their environment to be out of their control
(i.e. having external LoC), they might feel less capable to cope with stress and adversity since
they are then less reliant on their main strategies (such as manipulation) to handle setbacks.
However, if they feel like their environment is within their control and power, they might be
more confident in the effectiveness of their preferred strategies.
Further, the moderation analysis does not find LoC to moderate the relationship between
Psychopathy and resilience to any degree. The findings from the simple slopes analysis indicates
that the level of resilience is the same no matter the scores on Psychopathy. Considering there

25
was also no association between Psychopathy and resilience, Psychopathy does not seem to serve
as a buffer against stress and adversity.
Finally, the finding that the mean scores for men were higher than for women on all the
DT traits, including DT as a whole, is contingent with previous research (Chiorri et al., 2017;
Jonason et al., 2012). However, both sample size and gender stereotypes could have affected
these results. Jones and Paulhus (2010) argue that the DT traits are associated mainly with
qualities that are desirable for men, such as independence and self-assertion. Rudman et al.
(2012) further states that these traits are considered less desirable for women, who instead are
more highly valued when displaying traits such as interpersonal sensitivity.
Ethical discussion
When the survey was distributed, instead of disclosing the actual study variables, it was
described as testing “psychological processes linked to self image”. One of the motives for this
approach is partly because the Dark Triad in general might have a negative connotation, which
could cause the participants to respond based on social desirability or reactivity, rather than truth.
Secondly, since scores differing from the mean on the SD3.1.1 are only used on a subclinical
level and therefore not an indication of personality disorders, for a person without specialized
knowledge within the field of psychology, it could be a bit misleading and perhaps even
psychologically harmful if the respondents were to find out that they were scoring high on any of
the traits. For that reason, it would also be unethical to give any kind of debriefing on the test
results themselves.
Certainly, there is no guarantee that the risk of answers influenced by social desirability or
any other kind of response bias was completely avoided. It is also not entirely clear whether or
not any of the participants were emotionally affected when the true purpose of the study was
disclosed. However, since the participation was completely anonymous, there was nothing in the
final thesis that could tie their personal answers to the test results as a whole. No questions or
complaints regarding the survey were received.
​Limitations.​ ​A challenge that comes with doing online surveys, especially surveys
consisting of many items, is keeping the participants engaged enough to complete the entire
questionnaire. Out of 82 responses recorded, 57 of them were valid, fully completed. Thus, one
limitation of this study can be non-response bias. The individuals who chose not to continue the
study might be individuals with high scores on DT, or participants that could have affected the

26
results in another interesting way. While investigating the responses further a trend to drop out
after finishing 20% of the survey was detected. No questions around the 20% mark were
considered disturbing or unethical. Perhaps one of the reasons for dropping out had to do with
the length of the survey. Using shorter and effective versions of the scales could be more
favourable, especially when several measures are being merged into one test. If the questionnaire
would have been handed out in person, supervision of the participants would have been possible
and perhaps also a higher completion rate.
Another limitation is the sample size and the population examined. Larger samples and
more equal distributions are needed to draw more accurate conclusions. The gender distribution
was unequal and could have affected the results. The restrictions on avoiding physical contact
during the ongoing pandemic made it difficult to reach out to as many participants as desired. If
handing out surveys in public places would have been possible, larger and more equal samples
could have been used.
Further, a convenience sample was used, making a generalization to a broader population
not possible. The snowballing technique could also have led to somehow misleading results since
there is a possibility participants distributed our survey to like-minded individuals etc.
Another potential issue regarding the consistency of the study could be the translation of
the tests. An issue with a non-valid translation is its potential threats to the internal validity of the
study, since it could lead to inaccurate results, and thus misleading interpretation. However,
since the alpha level for the translations was calculated and all of them at minimum met the
requirements for a fair internal validity (see measures) this indicates that the translations
hopefully did not affect the results to a significant degree.
​Strengths. ​On the contrary,​ b​ y choosing to create an online survey, the risk of research
leader effects was significantly reduced. One of the biggest strengths of the chosen method is its
ability to avoid experimenter bias, reactivity and demand characteristics. Surveys are an effective
method for figuring out how extensive a phenomena is (Barmark & Djurfeldt, 2015) and also
have the convenience advantage; the respondent can participate whenever and wherever (Miner
& Jayaratne, 2014). Additionally, physical contact was completely avoided which made it safe to
participate in the study despite the ongoing pandemic.
Finally, the uniqueness of the research topic is another strength of the study, both in the
sense that the relationship between DT and resilience has not been sufficiently studied and also

27
by proposing a moderating model to explain how high scores on seemingly negative traits
actually could work in positive ways, given that there is a moderating relationship by LoC.
Constructs like good and bad are not one-dimensional and there can indeed be positive aspects
with constructs that might be socially considered as malicious and negative. This study sheds a
light on the complexities regarding dark traits in a way that has not been done before.
Future research
Since research on constructs like DT and resilience in relation to each other is an understudied
subject, further studies are required to draw more accurate conclusions about their associations.
The construct of Narcissism is something that needs a closer examination. Since Narcissism
predicted resilience alone regardless of LoC, and since previous research has pointed it out as a
unique trait in the DT (Lyons et al., 2019; Rauthmann & Kolar, 2013; Volmer et al., 2016),
exploring this matter further can be out of great value. Perhaps, the answers to why Narcissism
was the only trait in the DT that predicted resilience alone, lie in examining its subcategories
further to get a more nuanced image of how Narcissism and resilience are connected.
Further investigation into Psychopathy is also needed in order to gain deeper insights into
the correlation between Psychopathy and resilience, and the role of LoC in this relationship. In
this study, Psychopathy and resilience had no association with each other. Psychopathy seems to
also be the trait that mostly contributed to an unequal distribution for DT as a whole; do the
women in this particular sample answer based on social desirability, are more prone to
participate in studies, or were they simply very low on Psychopathy by chance? Larger and equal
samples are needed to draw more certain conclusions about these findings.
Also, since machiavellians in this study were found to be more resilient if possessing an
internal LoC and less resilient if attributing an external LoC, more research is needed to both
confirm our findings, but also gain deeper insight into why this could be the case with
Machiavellianism.
Finally, although there are strong correlations between the Dark Triad traits separately and
research stating that these traits should be examined as a whole, we found the traits to have
different associations to resilience when LoC was entered as moderator. Future research should
perhaps focus on which mechanisms underlie these differences between Machiavellianism,
Narcissism and Psychopathy and which factors that could be most important for resilience in
individuals with Dark Triad traits.

28
Conclusions
The findings of this study demonstrate that individuals with DT traits (DT as a whole, narcissists,
and machiavellians under certain conditions) seem to be psychologically resilient. All DT traits
have unique associations to LoC and resilience, which implies that LoCs’ effect on resilience
depends on which component of DT is being examined. For DT as a whole, a marginally
moderating effect was found. For Narcissism, resilience was not affected by the level of LoC.
The resilience level of Machiavellianism was moderated by the level of LoC and finally,
Psychopathy had no association with resilience, neither alone nor with LoC as a proposed
moderator. In conclusion, in this study, LoC had a moderating effect on resilience for individuals
with some DT traits, while others seem to be more resilient independent of constructs like LoC.
Further, this study highlights the differences and similarities between the three DT traits in the
context of variables and relationships that have not been sufficiently examined before.

29
References

Agaibi, C & Wilson, J. (2005). Trauma, PTSD, and resilience: A review of the literature.
Trauma, violence & abuse,​ ​6​(3), 195-216.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838005277438

Aghababaei, N., Błachnio, A. (2015). Well-being and the Dark Triad. ​Personality and
individual differences,​ 86, 365-368.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.043

Ahmed, S. U. (1985). NAch, risk-taking propensity, locus of control and entrepreneurship.


Personality and individual differences,​ ​6(​ 6), 781–782.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(85)90092-3

Balaji, V. (2014). A study on sub-clinical narcissistic personality score and its relationship
with academic performance-an indian experience. ​Asian social science, 11​(2), 96.
https://doi.org/​10.5539/ass.v11n2p96
Barmark, M., & Djurfeldt, G. (2015). ​Statistisk verktygslåda.​ Lund: Studentlitteratur.

Barry, C. T., & Malkin, M. L. (2010). The relation between adolescent narcissism and
internalizing problems depends on the conceptualization of narcissism. ​Journal of
research in personality,​ ​44(​ 6), 684–690.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.09.001

Barry, C. T., Loflin, D. C., & Doucette, H. (2015). Adolescent self-compassion:


Associations with narcissism, self-esteem, aggression, and internalizing symptoms in
at-risk males. ​Personality and individual differences​, 77, 118–123.
https://doi-org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.036

Basar, U. (2020). A multilevel study of relationships between leaders’ Dark Triad and
employee burnout: Mediating role of perceived dark leadership​.​ ​İşletme Araştırmaları
Dergisi,​ ​12​(3), 2407–2423.
https://doi.org/10.20491/isarder.2020.983

Blackburn, R. (2007). Personality disorder and antisocial deviance: Comments on the debate
on the structure of the psychopathy checklist-revised. ​Journal of personality disorders.
21, 142-59.
https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2007.21.2.142

30
Blair, R. (2003). Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. ​British journal of psychiatry, 182​(1),
5-7.
https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.182.1.​5

Brunell, A. & Campbell, W. K. (2011). Narcissism and romantic relationships:


Understanding the paradox. ​The handbook of narcissism and narcissistic personality
disorder,​ 344-350. ​John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Cairncross, M., Veselka, L., Schermer, J., & Vernon, P. (2013). A behavioral genetic
analysis of alexithymia and the Dark Triad traits of personality. ​Twin research and
human genetics,​ ​16(​ 3), 690-697.
https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2013.19

Chiorri, C., Garofalo, C. & Velotti, P. (2019). Does the Dark Triad manifest similarly in
men and women? Measurement invariance of the dirty dozen across sex. ​Current
psychology, 6​ 59-675.
https://doi-org.ludwig.lub.lu.se/10.1007/s12144-017-9641-5

Christie, R., & Geis, F. (1970). ​Studies in machiavellianism​. New York: Academic press,
2013.

Cleckley, H.M. (1976). ​The mask of sanity​ (5th ed.). Mosby: St. Louis, MO.

Connor KM, Davidson JR. (2003). Development of a new resilience scale: the
Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC)​.​ ​Depress anxiety,​ ​18(​ 2), 76-82.
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.10113

Czibor, A. & Bereczkei, T. (2012). Machiavellian people’s success results from monitoring
their partners. ​Personality and individual differences​. 53, 202-206.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.03.00​5

Demetrioff, S., Porter, S. & Baker, A. (2017). I know how you feel: the influence of
psychopathic traits on the ability to identify micro-expressions. ​Psychology, Crime &
Law,​ ​23​(3), 274-290.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1068316X.2016.1247159

Dutton, K. (2012) ​The wisdom of psychopaths: Lessons in life from saints, spies and serial
​ illiam Heinemann, London.
killers. W

31
Edwards, T., Catling, J. C., & Parry, E. (2016).​ I​ dentifying predictors of resilience in
students​. Psychology teaching review,​ ​22(​ 1), 26–34.
https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1146583.pdf

Fehr, B., Samsom, D. & Paulhus, D. (1992). The construct of machiavellianism: twenty
years later. ​Advances in personality assessment, 9​ , 77–116​.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/284691713_The_construct_of_Machiavellian
ism_twenty_years_later_in_Advances_in_Personality_Assessment_eds_C

Furtner, M. R., Maran, T., & Rauthmann, J. F. (2017). Dark leadership: The role of leaders'
Dark Triad personality traits. ​Annals of theoretical psychology: Leader development
deconstructed​, 15, 75–99. Springer International Publishing.
​https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-64740-1_4

Garcia, D., Adrianson, L., Archer, T., & Rosenberg, P. (2015). The dark side of the affective
profiles: Differences and similarities in psychopathy, machiavellianism, and narcissism.
​https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244015615167

Garmezy, N. (1993). Vulnerability and resilience. ​APA science vols. Studying lives through
time: Personality and development,​ 377–398. American Psychological Association.
https://doi.org/10.1037/10127-032

Grover, H. K. (2018). ​The upside to the dark side: an empirical investigation into the
moderators and mediators of the Dark Triad and work related outcomes​ [​ Doctoral
thesis, University College London]. University College London.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/330994308_The_Upside_to_the_Dark_side_A
n_empiric_investigation_into_the_moderators_and_mediators_of_the_Dark_Triad_and_
work_related_outcomes

Gustafson, S. B., & Ritzer, D. R. (1995). The dark side of normal: A psychopathy-linked
pattern called aberrant self-promotion. ​European journal of personality​,​ ​9, 147–183.
https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2410090302

Hart, W., Richardson, K. & Tortoriello, G. K. (2018). Meet your public relations team:
People with dark traits may help you manage your image. ​Personality and individual
differences​, 134, 164-173.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.019​.

32
Helode, R. D., & Kapai, N. (1986). A study of psychopathic tendency, locus of control and
socio-economic-status among adolescents. ​Indian journal of applied psychology, 23(​ 2),
91–97.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6208875/

Henrich, J., Heine, S. & Norenzayan. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. ​Nature,​ 466, 29.
https://doi.org/10.1038/466029a

Hmieleski, K. M., & Lerner, D. A. (2016). The Dark Triad and nascent entrepreneurship:
An examination of unproductive versus productive entrepreneurial motives. ​Journal of
small business management,​ 54, 7–32.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jsbm.12296

Jonason, P., Li, N., Webster, G. & Schmitt, David. (2009). The Dark Triad: Traits that
facilitate short-term mating in men. ​European journal of personality,​ 23, 5-18.
https://doi.org/10.1002/per.698

Jonason, P., Slomski, S. & Partyka, J. (2012). The Dark Triad at work: How toxic
employees get their way. ​Personality and individual differences, 52(​ 3), 449-453).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.008​.

Jonason, P., Strosser, G., & Kroll, C., Duineveld, J. & Baruffi, S. (2015). Valuing myself
over others: The Dark Triad traits and moral and social values. ​Personality and
individual differences,​ 81, 102-106.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.045

Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2010). Differentiating the Dark Triad within the
interpersonal circumplex​.​ ​Handbook of interpersonal theory and research,​ 249–267.
New York: Guilford Press.

Jones, D & Figueredo, A. (2013). The core of darkness: Uncovering the heart of the Dark
Triad. ​European journal of personality,​ ​27(​ 3), ​521-531.
https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1893

Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A brief
measure of dark personality traits. ​Assessment,​ ​21(​ 1), 28–41.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191113514105

33
Juby, C. & Rycraft, J. (2004). Family preservation strategies for families in poverty.
Families in society. ​The journal of contemporary human services,​ 85, 581-587.
https://doi.org/10.1606/1044-3894.1847

Kajonius, P. J., Björkman, T. (2018). Dark malevolent traits and everyday perceived stress.
Current psychology,​ ​39​(6), 2351-2356.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9948-x

Korber, S. & McNaughton, R. B. (2018). Resilience and entrepreneurship: a systematic


literature review. ​International journal of entrepreneurial behavior & research, 24​(7),
1129–1154.
https://doi.org/10.1108/IJEBR-10-2016-0356

Landström, H.​, Crijns, H., Laveren, E., & Smallbone, D. (2008). ​Entrepreneurship,
sustainable growth and performance: Frontiers in European entrepreneurship
research.​ Edward Elgar Publishing.

Lau, K. & Marsee, M. (2013). Exploring narcissism, psychopathy, and machiavellianism in


youth: Examination of associations with antisocial behavior and aggression. ​Journal of
child and family studies,​ 22, 355-367.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-012-9586-0

Luthar, Suniya & Cicchetti, Dante & Becker, Bronwyn. (2000). The construct of resilience:
A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. ​Child development,​ ​71​(3),
543-562.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00164

Lyons, M. (2019). ​The dark side of personality: Narcissism, machiavellianism and


psychopathy in everyday life.​ Academic press.

Lyons, M., Evans, K., & Helle, S. (2019). Do “dark” personality features buffer against
adversity? The associations between cumulative life stress, the Dark Triad, and mental
distress. ​SAGE open, 9​(1).
https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244018822383

Malesza, M. & Ostaszewski, P. (2016). Dark side of impulsivity — Associations between


the Dark Triad, self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity. ​Personality and
individual differences​, 88, 197-201.
​https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.016

34
Mathieu, C. & St-Jean, E. (2013). Entrepreneurial personality: The role of narcissism.
Personality and individual differences,​ ​55(​ 5), 527-531.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.04.026

McHoskey, J. (1995). Narcissism and machiavellianism. ​Psychological reports​, ​77​(3),


755–759.
https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995.77.3.755

McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and psychopathy.
Journal of personality and social psychology,​ ​74(​ 1), 192–210.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.192

Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary model.


Behavioral and brain sciences,​ ​18​(3), 523–599.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00039595

Miller, J., Hoffman, B., Gaughan, E., Gentile, B., Maples-Keller, J. & Campbell, W. K.
(2011). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: A nomological network analysis. ​Journal
of personality​, ​79(​ 5), 1013-1042.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/230245594_Grandiose_and_Vulnerable_Narci
ssism_A_Nomological_Network_Analysis

Miner, K., & Jayaratne, T. (2014). Feminist survey research​.​ In Hesse-Biber, S.J.
Feminist research practice a primer, ​296-329. SAGE.

Mudrack, P. (1990). Machiavellianism and locus of control: A meta-analytic review.


Journal of social psychology,​ ​130(​ 1), 125-126.
​https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1990.9922944

Näswall, K., Hellgren, J. & Sverke, M. (2008). ​The individual in the changing working life​.
Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511490064.001

O'Reilly, C., Doerr, B., Caldwell, D. & Chatman, J. (2013). Narcissistic CEOs and executive
compensation. ​The leadership quarterly, 25​(2), 218-231.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2013.08.002

35
Papageorgiou, K., Wong, B. & Clough, P. (2017). Beyond good and evil: Exploring the
mediating role of mental toughness on the Dark Triad of personality traits. ​Personality
and individual differences,​ 119, 19-23.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.031

Patrick, C. J., Fowles, D. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2009). Triarchic conceptualization of


psychopathy: developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness.
Development and psychopathology,​ ​21​(3), 913–938.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000492

Patton, C. L., Smith, S. F., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2018). Psychopathy and heroism in first
responders: Traits cut from the same cloth? ​Personality disorders: Theory, research,
and treatment,​ ​9​(4), 354-368.
https://doi.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Fper0000261

Paulhus, D & Jones, D. (2015). Measures of dark personalities. ​Measures of personality and
social psychological constructs,​ 562-594.
https://doi.org/​10.1016/B978-0-12-386915-9.00020-6

Paulhus, D. & Williams, K. (2002). The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism,


machiavellianism, and psychopathy. ​Journal of research in personality,​ ​36(​ 6),
556-563.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6

Petrides, K., Vernon, P., Schermer, J. & Veselka, L. (2011). Trait emotional intelligence and
the Dark Triad traits of personality. ​Twin research and human genetics: the official
journal of the International Society for Twin Studies​, ​14​(1), 35-41.
https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.14.1.35

Rapp-Ricciardi, M., Widh, J., Barbieri, B., Amato, C. & Archer, T. (2018). Dark Triad,
locus of control and affective status among individuals with an entrepreneurial intent.
Journal of entrepreneurship education​, 21.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323445763_Dark_Triad_Locus_of_Control_a
nd_Affective_Status_among_Individuals_with_an_Entrepreneurial_Intent

Riopel, L. (2020). ​The Connor Davidson + Brief Resilience Scales.​ Collected November
2020.
​https://positivepsychology.com/connor-davidson-brief-resilience-scale/

36
Rotter, J. B. (1954). ​Social learning and clinical psychology.​ Prentice-Hall.
https://doi.org/10.1037/10788-000

Rotter, J. B. (1955). The role of the psychological situation in determining the direction of
human behavior. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), ​Nebraska symposium on motivation,​ 245–269.
University of Nebraska Press.

Rotter, J. B. (1960). Some implications of a social learning theory for the prediction of goal
directed behavior from testing procedures. ​Psychological review,​ ​67(​ 5), 301–316.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0039601

Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of


reinforcement. P​sychological monographs: general and applied​, ​80​(1), 1–28.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0092976

Rudman, L. A., Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Nauts, S. (2012). Status incongruity
and backlash effects: Defending the gender hierarchy motivates prejudice against
female leaders. ​Journal of experimental social psychology, 48(1), 165-179).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.10.008

Sabouri, S., Gerber, M., Sadeghi Bahmani, D., Lemola, S., Clough, P. J., Kalak, N., Shamsi,
M., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., & Brand, S. (2016). Examining Dark Triad traits in relation
to mental toughness and physical activity in young adults. ​Neuropsychiatric disease
and treatment​, 12, 229–235.
https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S97267

Sandvik, A., Hansen, A., Hystad, S. W., Johnsen, B. H. & Bartone, P. (2015). Psychopathy,
anxiety, and resiliency – Psychological hardiness as a mediator of the psychopathy –
anxiety relationship in a prison setting. ​Personality and individual differences,​ 72,
30-34.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.009​.

Schneider, T., Lyons, J. & Khazon, S. (2013). Emotional intelligence and resilience.
Personality and individual differences,​ ​55(​ 8), 909-914.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.460​.

Singh, S., Rita & Suri, K. (2017). Locus of control and machiavellianism: A study on
college undergraduates. ​International journal of indian psychology,​ ​4(​ 3).
https://ijip.in/wp-content/uploads/ArticlesPDF/article_80e9c044d0c874de05bae2b9c5f
147e6.pdf

37
Smith, M. B., Hill, A. D., Wallace, J. C., Recendes, T., & Judge, T. A. (2018). Upsides to
dark and downsides to bright personality: A multidomain review and future research
agenda. ​Journal of management,​ ​44(​ 1), 191–217.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206317733511

Stanescu, D. F., & Mohorea , L. (2016). Welcome to the dark side: A correlational study of
the Dark Triad of personality with counterproductive work behavior and work of locus.
Pannon Management Review​, ​5​(1-2), 47-57.
https://pmr.uni-pannon.hu/articles/5_1_stanescu_mohorea

Stewart, D. & Yuen, T. (2011). A systematic review of resilience in the physically ill.
Psychosomatics,​ ​52​(3), 199-209.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psym.2011.01.036

Sullivan, S. S. (2020). ​The Relationship between trait narcissism and perceptions of control.
In BSU Honors Program Theses and Projects. Item 345.
https://vc.bridgew.edu/honors_proj/345/

Turkcapar, H., Ozel, A., & Guriz, S. & Isik Canpolat, Banu. (2002). Locus of control in
antisocial personality disorder. In: Scrimali T., Grimaldi L. (eds). ​Cognitive
psychotherapy toward a new millennium.​
​https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0567-9_67

Türk-Kurtça, T., & Kocatürk, M. (2020). The role of childhood traumas, emotional
self-efficacy and internal-external locus of control in predicting psychological resilience.
International journal of education and literacy studies,​ ​8(​ 3), 105–115.
http://dx.doi.org/10.7575/aiac.ijels.v.8n.3p.105

Velickovic, K., Rahm Hallberg, I., Axelsson, U., Borrebaeck, C.A.K., Ryden, L., Johnsson,
P., & Månsson, J. (2020). Psychometric properties of the Connor Davidson Resilience
Scale (CD-RISC) in a non-clinical population in Sweden. ​Health and quality of life
outcomes,​ ​18(​ 1).
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-020-01383-3

Vize, C., Miller, J. & Lynam, D. (2019). ​The Handbook of Antagonism.​ Academic Press.
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-814627-9.00017-7​.

38
Watts, A. L., Lilienfeld, S. O., Smith, S. F., Miller, J. D., Campbell, W. K., Waldman, I. D.,
Rubenzer, S. J., & Faschingbauer, T. J. (2013). The double-edged sword of grandiose
narcissism: Implications for successful and unsuccessful leadership among U.S.
presidents. ​Psychological science,​ ​24(​ 12), 2379–2389.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613491970

Werner, E., & Smith, R. (1982). ​Vulnerable but invincible: A longitudinal study of resilient
children and youth.​ Adams, Bannister and Cox.

39
Appendix A

Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC)

1. Jag kan anpassa mig när det sker förändringar.

2. Jag har minst en nära och trygg relation som hjälper mig när jag är stressad.

3. När det inte finns några tydliga lösningar på mina problem kan ödet eller Gud ibland hjälpa.

4. Jag kan hantera allt som kommer i min väg.

5. Tidigare framgångar ger mig självförtroende att klara nya utmaningar och svårigheter.

6. Jag försöker se den humoristiska sidan av saker när jag möter problem.

7. Att tvingas hantera stress kan göra mig starkare.

8. Jag tenderar att komma igen efter sjukdom skada eller andra vedermödor.

9. Jag tror det mesta sker av en anledning oavsett om det är bra eller dåligt.

10. Jag gör mitt bästa oavsett vad resultatet kan bli.

11. Jag tror jag kan nå mina mål även om det finns hinder.

12. Även när saker ser hopplöst ut ger jag inte upp.

13. Under tider av stress/kriser vet jag vart jag skall vända mig för att få hjälp.

14. Under press blir jag fokuserad och tänker klart.

15. Jag föredrar att ta ledningen när det gäller att lösa problem framför att låta andra fatta alla
beslut.

16. Jag blir inte lätt avskräckt av misslyckanden.

17. Jag ser mig själv som en stark person när jag hanterar livets utmaningar och svårigheter.

40
18. Jag kan fatta impopulära eller svåra beslut som påverkar andra människor om det är
nödvändigt.

19. Jag kan hantera obehagliga eller smärtsamma känslor som ledsenhet rädsla och ilska.

20. När man handskas med livets problem måste man ibland handla på en ingivelse utan att veta
varför.

21. Jag har en stark målmedvetenhet i livet.

22. Jag känner mig som om jag har kontroll över mitt liv.

23. Jag tycker om utmaningar.

24. Jag arbetar för att nå mina mål oavsett vilka hinder jag möter längs vägen.

25. Jag är stolt över mina prestationer.

Response categories:

0 – Inte alls sant

1 – Sällan sant

2 – Sant ibland

3 – Ofta sant

4 – Nästan alltid sant

41
Appendix B

SD3.1.1

I detta formulär finns ett antal påståenden​2​ om hur du upplever och ser på dig själv. Din uppgift
är att svara på hur väl du instämmer med varje påstående, enligt följande riktlinjer.

1: Instämmer inte alls 2: Instämmer inte 3: Varken instämmer eller inte instämmer 4: Instämmer
5: Instämmer helt och hållet

1. Det är inte så begåvat att berätta sina hemligheter för någon.

2. Jag gillar att använda smarta sätt att manipulera andra för att få som jag vill.

3. Man måste få de viktiga människorna på sin sida, oavsett vad det kostar.

4. Undvik direkta konflikter med andra personer, eftersom de kan vara användbara i framtiden.

5. Det är smart att hålla koll på saker som man senare kan använda mot andra människor.

6. Man bör vänta på rätt tillfälle att ge igen på någon.

7. Det finns saker man bör dölja för andra, eftersom de inte behöver veta.

8. Försäkra dig om att dina planer ger fördelar till just dig och inte någon annan.

9. De flesta människor kan bli manipulerade.

10. Andra ser mig som en naturlig ledare.

11. Jag avskyr att vara den som är i uppmärksamhetens mittpunkt.

2
​Påståendena i formuläret är utformade 2013 av Daniel N. Jones (University of Texas at El Paso) och
Delroy L. Paulhus (University of British Columbia). Svensk version, inklusive ”backtranslation”, gjordes
av Magnus Lindén f.d. R. Larsson (Lund universitet), Anna M. Dåderman (Högskolan Väst) och George
Farrants (Billericay AB).

42
12. Många gruppaktiviteter tenderar att bli tråkiga utan mig.

13. Jag vet att jag är speciell, eftersom alla hela tiden talar om det för mig.

14. Jag gillar att lära känna viktiga personer.

15. Jag känner mig bortgjord när någon ger mig en komplimang.

16. Jag har blivit jämförd med kända människor.

17. Jag är en ordinär person.

18. Jag insisterar på att få den respekt jag förtjänar.

19. Jag gillar att hämnas på auktoriteter.

20. Jag undviker farliga situationer.

21. En hämnd måste ske fort och utföras på ett fult sätt.

22. Andra talar ofta om för mig att jag har tappat kontrollen.

23. Det stämmer att jag kan vara elak mot andra.

24. Människor som bråkar med mig får alltid ångra det.

25. Jag har aldrig kommit i klammeri med rättsväsendet.

26. Jag njuter av att ha sex med personer jag knappt känner.

27. Jag säger vad som helst för att få som jag vill.

43
Appendix C

Rotter's Locus of Control Scale ​(Swedish translation)

Välj det påstående som du instämmer mest med på varje fråga.

1.
a. Barn hamnar i trubbel för att deras föräldrar straffar dem för mycket.
b. Problemet med de flesta barn nuförtiden är att deras föräldrar är för slappa med dem

2.
a. Många olyckligheter i människors liv är delvis på grund av otur
b. Människors otur är ett resultat av misstag de begått

3.
a. En av de största anledningarna till att vi har krig i världen är för att människor inte intresserar
sig tillräckligt mycket för politik.
b. Krig kommer alltid att finnas, hur mycket människor än försöker förhindra dem.

4.
a. I det långa loppet får människor den respekt de förtjänar i den här världen.
b. Tyvärr erkänns en individs värde ofta inte hur mycket han eller hon än försöker.

5.
a. Idén om att lärare är orättvisa mot studenter är nonsens.
b. De flesta studenter inser inte till vilken grad deras betyg influeras av olyckshändelser eller
slumpen

6.
a. Utan de rätta bromsarna kan man inte vara en effektiv ledare
b. Människor som är kunniga och kapabla men misslyckas med att bli ledare har inte tagit vara
på sina möjligheter.

7.
a. Hur mycket du än försöker kommer vissa människor ändå aldrig tycka om dig.
b. Människor som inte kan få andra att gilla dem förstår inte hur man kommer överens med
andra.

44
8.
a. Arv spelar störst roll i vad som avgör en människas personlighet
b. Det är individens livsupplevelser som avgör hur de är som person.

9.
a. Jag har ofta upptäckt att det som är menat att hända, kommer hända.
b. Att lita på ödet har aldrig slutat bra för mig när det kommer till att ta beslut för en definitiv
handlingsplan

10.
a. Så länge en student är väl förberedd finns det sällan eller aldrig några orättvisa tester/kan man
sällan eller till och med aldrig säga att ett test/prov är orättvist.
b. Många gånger tenderar provfrågor att vara så orelaterade till kursen att det är meningslöst att
studera.

11.
a. Att bli framgångsrik handlar om hårt arbete, tur har mycket lite eller inget att göra med det.
b. Att få ett bra jobb beror först och främst på om du är på rätt plats vid rätt tillfälle.

12.
a. En vanlig medborgare kan ha en påverkan på/möjlighet att påverka regeringsbeslut.
b. Världen styrs av få människor med mycket makt och det finns inte så mycket vanliga
människor kan göra åt det.

13.
a. När jag planerar saker är jag nästan helt säker på att jag kan få dem att funka.
b. Det är inte alltid klokt att planera för långt i förväg, eftersom många saker visar sig avgöras av
tur eller otur ändå.

14.
a. Det finns vissa människor som bara inte är goda alls.
b. Det finns något bra i alla.

15.
a. I mitt fall har tur lite eller inget att göra med att få det jag vill ha.
b. Många gånger kan vi lika gärna besluta om vad vi ska göra genom att singla slant.

45
16.
a. Vem som får bli chef har ofta att göra med vem som hade turen att vara på rätt plats vid rätt
tillfälle
b. Att få människor att göra rätt saker har med förmåga att göra. Tur har lite, eller inget alls med
det att göra.

17.
a. När det kommer till världsliga angelägenheter är många av oss offer för högre krafter som vi
varken kan förstå eller kontrollera.
b. Genom att vara en aktiv del i politiska och sociala affärer kan människor kontrollera
världshändelser.

18.
a. De flesta människorna inser inte till vilken grad deras liv kontrolleras av slumpmässiga
händelser
b.”Tur” finns egentligen inte.

19.
a. Man bör alltid vara villig att erkänna sina misstag
b. Det är oftast bäst att dölja sina misstag.

20.
a.Det är svårt att veta om en person verkligen tycker om en eller ej.
b. Hur många vänner du har beror på hur trevlig du är som person.

21.
a. I det långa loppet balanserar de goda ut de dåliga sakerna som händer oss.
b. De flesta olyckliga händelserna är ett resultat av oförmåga, ignorans, lathet eller alla tre.

22.
a. Med tillräcklig ansträngning kan vi göra oss kvitt med politisk korruption.
b. Det är svårt för människor att ha särskilt mycket kontroll över vad politikerna gör på sitt
kontor.

23.
a. Ibland förstår jag inte hur lärare landar i de betyg de bestämt sig för att ge mig.
b. Det finns en direkt koppling mellan hur mycket jag studerar och vilka betyg jag får.

24.
a. En bra ledare förväntar sig att andra tar sina egna beslut om vad de bör göra.
b. En bra ledare gör det klart för alla vad deras jobb är/innebär.

46
25.
a. Jag har många gånger känt att jag inte har så mycket påverkan/inverkan på/möjlighet att
påverka saker som händer mig.
b. Det är omöjligt för mig att tro på att slumpen eller tur spelar en viktig roll i mitt liv.

26.
a. Människor är ensamma för att de inte försöker vara vänliga.
b. Det är inte någon idé att försöka för mycket för att göra folk nöjda. Om de tycker om dig,
tycker de om dig.

27.
a. Det läggs för stor vikt vid idrott i gymnasiet.
b. Lagsport är ett utmärkt sätt att bygga karaktär på.

28.
a. Det som händer mig är ett resultat av mina egna handlingar
b. Ibland känner jag att jag inte har tillräcklig kontroll över riktningen mitt liv går i/tar.

29.
a. För det mesta kan jag inte förstå varför politiker beter sig som de gör.
b. I det långa loppet är folket ansvariga för en dålig regering - på en nationell såväl som lokal
nivå.

47
Appendix D

Rotter's Locus of Control Scale ​(original version)

For each question, select the statement that you agree with the most.
1.
a. Children get into trouble because their parents punish them too much.
b. The trouble with most children nowadays is that their parents are too easy with them.

2.
a. Many of the unhappy things in people's lives are partly due to bad luck.
b. People's misfortunes result from the mistakes they make.

3.
a. One of the major reasons why we have wars is because people don't take enough interest in
politics.
b. There will always be wars, no matter how hard people try to prevent them.

4.
a. In the long run people get the respect they deserve in this world
b. Unfortunately, an individual's worth often passes unrecognized no matter how hard he tries

5.
a. The idea that teachers are unfair to students is nonsense.
b. Most students don't realize the extent to which their grades are influenced by accidental
happenings.

6.
a. Without the right breaks one cannot be an effective leader.
b. Capable people who fail to become leaders hive not taken advantage of their opportunities.

7.
a. No matter how hard you try, some people just don't like you.
b. People who can't get others to like them don't understand how to get along with others.

8.
a. Heredity plays the major role in determining one's personality
b. It is one's experiences in life which determine what they're like.

48
9.
a. I have often found that what is going to happen will happen.
b. Trusting to fate has never turned out as well for me as making a decision to take a definite
course of action.

10.
a. In the case of the well prepared student there is rarely if ever such a thing as an unfair test.
b. Many times exam questions tend to be so unrelated to course work that studying in really
useless.

11.
a. Becoming a success is a matter of hard work, luck has little or nothing to do with it.
b. Getting a good job depends mainly on being in the right place at the right time.

12.
a. The average citizen can have an influence in government decisions.
b. This world is run by the few people in power, and there is not much the little guy can do about
it.

13.
a. When I make plans, I am almost certain that I can make them work.
b. It is not always wise to plan too far ahead because many things turn out to- be a matter of good
or bad fortune anyhow.

14.
a. There are certain people who are just no good.
b. There is some good in everybody.

15.
a. In my case getting what I want has little or nothing to do with luck.
b. Many times we might just as well decide what to do by flipping a coin.

16.
a. Who gets to be the boss often depends on who was lucky enough to be in the right place first.
b. Getting people to do the right thing depends upon ability. Luck has little or nothing to do with
it.

49
17.
a. As far as world affairs are concerned, most of us are the victims of forces we can neither
understand, nor control.
b. By taking an active part in political and social affairs the people can control world events.

18.
a. Most people don't realize the extent to which their lives are controlled by accidental
happenings.
b. There really is no such thing as "luck."

19.
a. One should always be willing to admit mistakes.
b. It is usually best to cover up one's mistakes.

20.
a. It is hard to know whether or not a person really likes you.
b. How many friends you have depends upon how nice a person you are.

21.
a. In the long run the bad things that happen to us are balanced by the good ones.
b. Most misfortunes are the result of lack of ability, ignorance, laziness, or all three.

22.
a. With enough effort we can wipe out political corruption.
b. It is difficult for people to have much control over the things politicians do in office.

23.
a. Sometimes I can't understand how teachers arrive at the grades they give.
b. There is a direct connection between how hard 1 study and the grades I get.

24.
a. A good leader expects people to decide for themselves what they should do.
b. A good leader makes it clear to everybody what their jobs are.

25.
a. Many times I feel that I have little influence over the things that happen to me.

50
b. It is impossible for me to believe that chance or luck plays an important role in my life.

26.
a. People are lonely because they don't try to be friendly.
b. There's not much use in trying too hard to please people, if they like you, they like you.

27.
a. There is too much emphasis on athletics in high school.
b. Team sports are an excellent way to build character.

28.
a. What happens to me is my own doing.
b. Sometimes I feel that I don't have enough control over the direction my life is taking.

29.
a. Most of the time I can't understand why politicians behave the way they do.
b. In the long run the people are responsible for bad government on a national as well as on a
local level.

Score one point for each of the following: 2. a, 3.b, 4.b, 5.b, 6.a, 7.a, 9.a, 10.b, 11.b, 12.b, 13.b,
15.b, 16.a, 17.a, 18.a, 20.a, 21. a, 22.b, 23.a, 25.a, 26.b, 28.b, 29.a.

A high score = External Locus of Control A low score = Internal Locus of Control

51

You might also like