Pyc2602 Exam Prep1421
Pyc2602 Exam Prep1421
Most people show an underlying stability in aspects of personality and behaviour i.e. most shy children
generally display shyness to a moderate degree throughout their life.
Charles Darwin was the first theorist to emphasize the developmental nature of infant behaviour.
Physical Development: Growth of body and brain, the development of sensory capacities and motor
skills and health. All influence other aspects of development. Eg. Child with frequent ear infections may
develop language more slowly than a child without this physical problem.
Cognitive Development: Change and stability in mental abilities, such as learning, memory, language,
thinking, moral reasoning, and creativity. Closely related to physical, social and emotional growth.
Ability to speak depends on the development of mouth and brain. A child who has difficulties in
expressing herself in words may bring negative reactions in others, affecting her popularity and sense of
self worth.
Psychosocial Development: Change and stability in personality, emotions, and social relationships. Can
affect cognitive and physical functioning. Anxiety about taking a test can worsen performance. Social
support can help children cope with stress on physical and mental health. Physical and cognitive
capacities affect psychosocial dev by contributing to self-esteem and social acceptance.
Environment: The world outside of the self beginning in the womb and the leaning that comes from
experience – incl. socialisation, a child’s induction into the value system of the culture.
Research points to a blend of inheritance and environment in the development of specific traits. Thus,
even though intelligence is strongly affected by heredity, environmental factors such as parental
stimulation, education, and peer influence also affect it.
Many typical changes of infancy and early childhood, such as the emergence of the abilities to walk and
talk, are tied to maturation of the body and brain – unfolding of universal, natural sequence of physical
changes and behaviour patterns, including readiness to master new abilities such as walking and talking.
These maturational processes, act in concert with the influences of heredity and environment. Even in
maturational processes that all children undergo, rates and timing of development vary.
Contexts of Development
Family
Nuclear Family: is a two-generational kinship, economic, and household unit consisting of one or two
parents and their biological children, adopted children, and/or stepchildren.
Extended Family: a multi-generational kinship network of grandparents, aunts, uncles, cousins and more
distant relatives. This is the traditional family form.
Many people live in extended family households, where they have daily contact with kin. Adults share
breadwinning and child raising responsibilities, and children are often responsible for younger siblings.
Often these households are headed by women. Extended families are less typical in developing countries
due to industrialisation and migration to urban centres.
SES is related to developmental processes (such as mothers verbal interactions with their children) and to
developmental outcomes (such as health and cognitive performance). SES affects these outcomes
indirectly, through factors such as the kinds of homes and neighbourhoods children live in and the quality
of nutrition, medical care, supervision, schooling and other opportunities available to them.
Poverty is harmful to the physical, cognitive, and psychosocial well-being of children and families.
Threats to well-being multiply if several risk factors – conditions that increase the likelihood of a
negative developmental outcome – coexist.
Culture refers to a society’s or group’s total way of life, including customs, traditions, laws, knowledge,
beliefs, values, language, and physical products, from tools to artworks – all of the behaviour and
attitudes that are leaned, shared and transmitted among members of a social group. Culture is constantly
changing, often through contact with other cultures.
An Ethnic group consists of people united by a distinctive culture, ancestry, religion, language, and/or
national origin, all of which contribute to a sense of shared identity and shared attitudes, beliefs and
values.
Ethnic and cultural patterns affect child development by their influence on the composition of a
household, its economic and social resources, the way its members act toward one another, the foods they
eat, the games children play, the way they learn, how well they do in school, the occupations adults
engage in and the way family members think and perceive the world.
The term Race, an identifiable biological category, is now agreed to be a social construct. There is no
clear scientific consensus on its definition and it is impossible to measure reliably. Race as a social
category makes a difference in how individuals are treated, where they live, their employment
opportunities, the quality of their health care, and whether they can fully participate in their society.
Categories of culture, ethnicity and race are fluid, continuously shaped and redefined by social and
political forces.
Historical context: The time period in which people live and grow.
The historical context is an important part of the study of development. How certain experiences, tied to
time and place, affect the course of children’s lives.
Normative age graded influences are highly similar for people in a particular age group. They include
biological events (eg puberty) and social events (eg entry into formal education). The timing of
biological events is fixed (you don’t hit puberty at age 3). The timing of social events is more flexible
and varies in different times and places, within maturational limits.
Normative history-graded influences are significant events (such as the Great Depression or 9/11) that
shape the behaviour and attitudes of a historical generation: a group of people who experience the event
at a formative time in their lives.
A historical generation is not the same as an age cohort. A historical generation may contain more than
one age cohort, but not all cohorts are part of historical generations unless they experience major, shaping
historical events at a formative point in their lives.
Non-normative influences are unusual events that have a major impact on individual lives and may
cause stress because they are unexpected.
They are either typical events that happen at an atypical time of life ( eg marriage during teens, loss of a
parent when young) or atypical events (eg having a birth defect or being in a plane crash). They can also
be happy events (eg winning the lotto).
Young people may help create non-normative events - drinking and driving or applying for a scholarship
– and thus participate actively in their own development.
Imprinting: Instinctive form of learning in which, during a critical period in early development, a young
animal forms an attachment to the first moving object it sees, usually the mother. Konrad Lorenz believed
imprinting is automatic and irreversible.
Lorenz says imprinting is a result of a predisposition toward learning: the readiness of an organism’s
nervous system to acquire certain information during a critical period during life.
A critical period is a specific time when a given event, or its absence, has a specific impact on
development. If a necessary event does not occur during a critical period of maturation, normal
development will not occur, and the resulting abnormal patterns may be irreversible. However the length
of a critical period is not absolutely fixed.
Critical periods also occur in human development. Eg during gestation woman are too avoid X-rays,
certain drugs etc otherwise the fetus may show specific ill effects. Critical periods also occur early in
childhood. Eg if a muscle problem interfering with the ability to focus both eyes on the same object is not
corrected early in life, the brain mechanisms necessary for binocular depth perception probably will not
develop.
The concept of critical periods is controversial. Many aspects of development, even in the physical
domain have been found to show plasticity: modifiability of performance, so it may be more useful to
think about sensitive periods: when a childs development is especially responsive to certain kinds of
experiences, but later experience continues to influence development.
Erik Erikson (1902-1994), a German born psychoanalyst emphasised the influence of society on the
developing personality. Erikson was a pioneer in the life-span perspective, Erikson contended that ego
development is lifelong.Eriksons theory of psychosocial development covers 8 stages across the life span.
Each stage involves what Erikson originally called a crisis (now referred to as conflicting or competing
tendencies) in personality – a major psychosocial theme that is particularly important at that time but will
remain an issue to some degree throughout life. These issues which emerge according to a maturational
timetable, must be satisfactorily resolved for healthy ego development.
Each stage requires the balancing of a positive trait and a corresponding negative one. Although the
positive quality should predominate, some degree of the negative is needed as well. The successful
outcome of each stage is the development of a particular virtue or strength.
Eriksons theory is important because of its emphasis on social and cultural influences and on
development beyond adolescence. He is probably most widely known for his concept of the identity
crisis.
Piaget viewed development organismically, as the product of children’s efforts to understand and act on
their world. Piaget set out to standardise the test Alfred Binet had developed to assess the intelligence of
French schoolchildren. Piaget’s clinical method combined observation with flexible questioning. To find
out how children think, Piaget followed up their answers with more questions, and he designed tasks to
test his tentative conclusions.
Piaget suggested that cognitive development begins with an inborn ability to adapt to the environment.
Piaget described cognitive development as occurring in four qualitatively different stages, which
represent universal patterns of development. At each stage a child’s mind develops a new way of
operating. From infancy through asolescence, mental operations evolve from learning based on simple
sensory and motor activity to logical abstract thought. This cognitive growth occurs through three
interrelated processes: Organisation; Adaption; and Equilibrium.
Organisation: the tendency to create increasingly complex cognitive structures: systems of knowledge or
ways of thinking that incorporate more and more accurate images of reality. These structures called
Schemes: are organised patterns of behaviour that a person uses to think about and act in a situation. As
children acquire more information; their schemes become more and more complex.
Adaption: is how children handle new information in light of what they already know. Adaption
involves two steps:
(1) assimilation: taking in new information and incorporating it into existing cognitive structures and;
(2) accommodation: modifying one’s cognitive structures to include the new information.
Equilibration: a constant striving for a stable balance, or equilibrium – dictates the shifts from
assimilation to accommodation. When children cannot handle new experiences within their existing
cognitive structures, they experience an uncomfortable state of disequilibrium. By organising new mental
patterns that integrate the new experience, they restore equilibrium.
Example: A breast-or-bottle fed baby begins to suck on the spout of a sippy cup is showing assimilation
– using an old scheme to deal with a new situation. When the infant discovers that sipping from a cup
requires different tongue and mouth movements, she accommodates by modifying the old scheme. She
has adapted her original sucking scheme to deal with a new experience: the cup.
Thus, assimilation and accommodations work together to produce equilibrium. Throughout life, the quest
for equilibrium is the driving force behind cognitive growth.
Some contemporary psychologists believe Piaget underestimated the abilities of infants and young
children and question his distinct stages, pointing instead to evidence that cognitive development is more
gradual and continuous. They also challenge Piagets idea that thinking develops in a single, universal
progression leading to formal thought. Instead, children’s cognitive processes seem closely tied to
specific content (what they are thinking about) as well as to the context of a problem and the kinds of
information and thought a culture considers important.
Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory stresses children’s active engagement with their environment; he saw
cognitive growth as a collaborative process. Children learn through social interaction. They acquire
cognitive skills as part of their induction into a way of life. Shared activities help children internalise
their society’s modes of thinking and behaving and make those folkways their own. Vygotsky placed
special emphasis on language - not merely as an expression of knowledge and thought but as an essential
means to learning and thinking about the world.
According to Vygotsky, adults or more advanced peers help direct and organise a child’s learning before
the child can master and internalize it. This guidance is most effective in helping children cross the Zone
of Proximal Development (ZPD): the gap between what they are already able to do and what they are
not quite ready to do by themselves. Children in the ZPD for a particular task can almost but not quite do
the task on their own. With the right kind of guidance however they can do it. Responsibility for
directing and monitoring learning gradually shifts to the child
Scaffolding: is the temporary support that parents, teachers and others give a child doing a task until the
child can do it alone.
The Information Processing Approach attempts to explain cognitive development by analysing the mental
processes involved in perceiving and handling information. This is not a single theory but a framework
that underlies a wide range of theories and research.
Information processing researchers infer what goes on between a stimulus and a response.
Information processing theorists see people as actively thinking about their world. They generally do not
propose stages of development; instead they view development as continuous. They note age-related
increases in the speed, complexity, and efficiency of mental processing and in the amount and variety of
material that can be stored in memory.
The information processing approach has practical applications. It enables researchers to estimate an
infant’s later intelligence from the efficiency of his her sensory perception and processing. It enables
parents and teachers to help children learn by making them more aware of their mental processes and of
strategies to enhance them. Psychologists often use information processing models to test, diagnose and
treat learning problems.
RESEARCH METHODS
Developmental Research Designs
The two most common strategies used to study child development are:
Cross-sectional studies: which show similarities and differences among age groups; and
Longitudinal studies: reveal how children change or stay the same as they grow older.
Because each of these designs have drawbacks, researchers also devised Sequential designs.
Cross-Sectional study: children of different ages are assessed at one time. Eg. Asking a group of 3-,
4-,6-, and 7-year-olds the same question.
Longitudinal study: researchers study the same child or children more than once, sometimes years apart.
Designed to assess changes in a sample over time.
Researchers may assess a cross-sectional sample on two or more occasions in sequence to find out how
members of each age cohort have changed. This procedure permits researchers to separate age-related
changes from cohort effects.
Another sequential design consists of a sequence of longitudinal studies, running concurrently by starting
one after another. This design enables researchers to compare individual differences in the course of
developmental change.
A combination of cross-sectional and longitudinal sequences can provide a more complete picture of
development.
Microgenetic Studies
Microgenetic Study: study design that enables researchers to directly observe change by repeated testing
over a short time.
Over a short time span, participants are repeatedly exposed to a stimulus for change or opportunity for
learning, enabling researchers to see and analyse the processes by which change occurs.
CHAPTER 3 – FORMING A NEW LIFE
NATURE AND NURTURE: INFLUENCES OF HEREDITY AND
ENVIRONMENT
Studying the Relative Influences of Heredity and Environment
Measuring Heritability
Heritability is a statistical estimate of how great a contribution heredity makes toward variations in a
specific trait at a certain time within a given population. Heritability merely indicates the statistical extent
to which genes contribute to a trait.
Heritability is expressed as a percentage ranging from 0.0 to 1.0; the higher the number, the greater the
heritability of a trait, with 1.0 meaning that genes are 100% responsible for variances in the trait within
the population.
Researchers rely on 3 types of correlational research: Family, Adoption and Twin studies.
These studies are based on the assumption that immediate family members are more genetically similar
that more distant relatives, adopted children are genetically more like their biological families than their
adoptive families, and monozygotic twins are more genetically similar than dizygotic twin.
Family studies: researchers measure the degree to which biological relatives share certain traits and
whether the closeness of familial relationship is associated with the degree of similarity. If the correlation
is strong, the researchers infer a genetic influence. However, family studies cannot rule out
environmental influences so this is why researchers do adoption studies, which can separate the effects of
heredity from those of a shared environment.
Adoption studies: look at similarities between adopted children and their adoptive families and also
between adopted children and their biological families to determine which traits are inherited and which
are from an environmental influence.
Twin studies: compare pairs of monozygotic twins and same sex dizygotic twins. Monozygotic twins
are twice as genetically similar, on average, as dizygotic twins, who are no more genetically similar than
any other same sex siblings. When monozygotic twins are more concordant (have a statistically greater
tendency to show the same trait) than dizygotic, we see the likely effect of heredity. Concordance rates
which may range from 0.0 to 1.0, estimate the probability that a pair of twins in a sample will be
concordant for a trait. When monozygotic twins show higher concordance for a trait than do dizygotic
twins, the likelihood of a genetic factor can be studied further through adoption studies.
Reaction Range: is the conventional term for a range of potential expressions of a heredity trait. Body
size for example depends largely on biological processes which are genetically regulated. Even so a
range of sizes is possible, depending on environmental opportunities and constraints and a person’s own
behaviour.
Heredity can influence whether a reaction range is wide or narrow. Eg. A child born with mild retardation
is more able to respond to a favourable environment that a child born with more severe limitations.
Canalization: is how heredity restricts the range of development for some traits. Some human
characteristics, such as eye colour, are so strongly programmed by genes that they are said to be highly
canalized; there is little opportunity for variance in their expression.
Certain behaviours also develop along genetically dug channels; it takes an extreme change in
environment to alter their course. Behaviour that depends largely on maturation seem to appear when a
child is ready. Normal babies follow a typical sequence of motor development: crawling, walking,
running, in that order, at certain approximate ages. Still, this development is not completely canalized;
experience can affect its pace and timing.
Cognition and personality are more subject to variations in experience. Recently scientists have begun to
recognise that a usual or typical experience, too, can dig channels for development.
Genotype-Environment Interaction
Genotype-Environment Correlation
Passive Correlations: parents, who provide the genes that predispose a child toward a trait, also tend to
provide and environment that encourages the development of that trait. Eg musical parents usually
produce musical kids; they pass on the gene and the house/their activities/experiences are always
musically orientated. This type of correlation is called passive because the child does not control it.
Passive correlations are most applicable to young children, whose parents, the source of their genes; also
have a great deal of control over their early experiences.
Reactive, or Evocative correlations: children with differing genetic makeups evoke different responses
from adults. If a child shows interest and ability in music, parents who are not musically inclined; may
react by making a special effort to provide that child with musical experiences. This response in turn
strengthens the child’s genetic inclination toward music.
Active correlations: as children get older and have more freedom to choose their own activities and
environments, they actively select experiences consistent with their genetic tendencies. A child with a
talent for music will probably seek out musical friends, take music classes and go to concerts. A shy
child is likely to spend more time in solitary pursuits than and outgoing child. This tendency to seek out
environments compatible with ones genotype is called niche-picking; it helps explain why identical twins
reared apart tend to be quite similar.
One reason may be genetic differences which lead children to need different kinds of stimulation or to
respond differently to a similar home environment. In addition studies in behavioural genetics suggest
many of the experiences that strongly affect development differ for different children in a family.
These non-shared environmental effects result from the unique environment in which each child in a
family grows up. Children in a family have a shared environment but also one that is not shared by their
siblings. Parents and siblings may treat each child differently. Certain events and experiences outside the
home affect one child and not the other. Heredity accounts for most of the similarities among siblings
and the non shared environments accounts for most of the differences. There seems to be a balance
between the shared environment and the non-shared environment.
Genotype-environment correlations may play and important part in the non-shared environment.
Childrens genetic differences may lead parents and siblings to react to them differently and treat them
differently, and genes may influence how children perceive and respond to that treatment and what its
outcome will be. Children also mold their environments by the choices they make – what they do and
with whom – and their genetic makeup influences these choices.
Epigenesis (meaning “on the gene”): refers to chemical molecules attached to a gene, which alter the
way a cell reads the genes DNA. The epigenetic framework can be visualised as a code written in pencil
in the margins around the DNA. Because every cell in the body inherits the same DNA sequence, the
functions of these epigenetic markers is to differentiate types of body cells. They do so by switching
genes on or off during embryonic formation. Sometimes errors arise in the process, which may lead to
birth defects or disease. Epigenetic changes can occur throughout life in response to environmental
changes such as nutrition and stress.
Not only do monozygotic twins look alike, but they are also more concordant than dizygotic twins in their
risk for medical disorders such as high blood pressure, heart disease, stroke, rheumatoid arthritis, peptic
ulcers and epilepsy. Life span too seems to be influenced by genes. Obesity in twin, adoption and other
research studies suggest that 40-70% of the risk is genetic.
Intelligence
Heredity exerts a strong influence of general intelligence (as measured by intelligence tests) and, to a
lesser extent, on specific abilities such as memory, verbal ability, and spatial ability.
Evidence of the role of heredity in intelligence come from adoption and twin studies. Adopted children’s
IQ’s are consistently closer to the IQ’s of their biological parents and monozygotic twins are more like in
intelligence than dizygotic twins.
It is likely that the genes that affect one cognitive ability; also affect other cognitive abilities.
Furthermore, the genetic influence, which is primarily responsible for stability in cognitive performance,
increases with age. The shared family environment seems to have a strong influence on young children
but a diminishing influence on adolescents and adults. The non-shared environment, in contrast, is
influential throughout life and is primarily responsible for changes in cognitive performance.
Personality
Scientists have identified genes directly linked with specific personality traits, such as neuroticism, which
may contribute to depression and anxiety. Heritability of personality traits appears to be between 40 and
50%, and there is little evidence of shared environmental influence.
Psychopathology
There is evidence for a strong hereditary influence on such mental disorders as schizophrenia, autism, and
depression. All tend to run in families and to show greater concordance between monozygotic twins than
dizygotic twins. However heredity alone does not produce such disorders; and inherited tendency can be
triggered by environmental factors.
Schizophrenia is now widely considered a neurological disorder characterised by loss of contact with
reality and by such symptoms as hallucinations and delusions. It has multifactorial causes. The risk of
schizophrenia is ten times greater among siblings and offspring of schizophrenics than among the general
population. Twin and adoption studies suggest that this increased risk comes from shared genes, not
shared environments. Estimates of heterability are as high as 80-85%
Because not all monozygotic twins are concordant for the illness, its cause cannot be purely genetic.
Everything that affects a womans well being, from her diet to her moods, may alter her unborn childs
environment and influence its growth. Not all environmental hazards are equally risky for all foetuses.
Some factors that are teraogenic(birth-defect producing) in some cases have little or no affect in other.
The timing of the exposure the, the dose, duration, and interaction with other teratogenic factors may
make a difference.
The fetus has direct access to the maternal blood supply through the placenta so an expectant mother does
not have control over the amount of nutrients that she “loses” to her fetus. It is important then for an
expectant mother to take in enough nutrients to adequately feed herself and her fetus.
Being either overweight or underweight can be risky: among women having their first babies, those who
were overweight before pregnancy had the most risk of stillbirth or of losing their babies during the first
week of life. On the other hand, underweight women are more likely to have dangerously small babies.
Obese women risk habing children with neural-tube defects, as well as heart defects and other birth
defects. Obesity also increases the risk of other complications of pregnancy, including miscarriage,
difficulty inducing labour, and a greater likelihood of caesarean delivery.
What an expectant mother eats is important: Fish appears to be brain food for a fetus. Recently scientists
have learned of the critical importance of folic acid in a pregnant woman’s diet.
Malnutrition
Prenatal malnutrition may have long-range effects. Severe prenatal nutrition deficiencies in the first and
second trimesters affect the developing brain, increasing the risk of antisocial personality disorders at age
18. Children whose mothers had low vitamin D levels late in pregnancy had low bone mineral content at
age 9, potentially increasing their risk of osteoporosis in later life. Fetal under-nutrition is also linked
with schizophrenia.
Malnourished women who take dietry supplements while pregnant tend to have bigger, healthier, more
active, and more visually alert infants. Women with low zinc levels who take daily zinc supplements are
less likely to have babies with low birth weight and small head circumference.
Moderate exercise does not seem to endanger the fetuses of healthy women. Regular exercise prevents
constipation and improves respiration, circulation, muscle tone, and skin elasticity, all of which constitute
to a more comfortable pregnancy and an easier, safer delivery.
Employment during pregnancy generally entails no special hazards. However strenuous working
conditions, occupational fatigue, and long working hours may be associated with a greater risk of
premature birth.
Drug Intake
Vulnerability is greatest in the first few months of gestation, when development is most rapid. Some
problems resulting from prenatal exposure to drugs can be treated if the presence of a drug can be
detected early.
Medical Drugs: The effects of taking a drug during pregnancy do not always become apparent
immediately. The AAP Committee on Drugs (1994) recommends that no medication be prescribed for a
pregnant or breast-feeding woman unless it is essential for her health or her child’s. Pregnant women
should not take over-the-counter drugs without consulting a doctor.
Alcohol: Fetal Alcohol Syndrome (FAS): a combination of retarded growth, facial and bodily
malformation, and disorders of the central nervous system. Prenatal alcohol exposure is the most
common cause of mental retardation and the leading preventable cause of birth defects in the USA and is
a risk factor for development of drinking problems and alcohol disorders in young adulthood.
Even small amount of social drinking may harm the fetus, and the more the mother drinks, the greater the
effect. Moderate or heavy drinking during pregnancy seems to disturb an infant’s neurological and
behavioural functioning and this may affect early social interaction with the mother which is vital to
emotional development. Heavy drinkers who continue to drink after becoming pregnant are likely to
have babies with reduces skull and brain growth as compared with babies of non drinking woman or
expectant mothers who stop drinking. Because there is no known safe level of drinking during
pregnancy, it is best to avoid alcohol form the time a woman begins thinking about becoming pregnant
until she stops breast-feeding.
FAS-related problems can include, in infancy, reduced responsiveness to stimuli, slow reaction time, and
reduced visual acuity (sharpness of vision); and throughout childhood, short attention span, distractibility,
restlessness, hyperactivity, learning disabilities, memory deficits and mood disorders; as well as
aggressiveness and problem behaviour.
Some FAS problems recede after birth, but others, such as retardation, behavioural and learning
problems, and hyperactivity, tend to persist.
Nicotine: Women who smoke during pregnancy are more than one and half times as likely as non
smokers to bear low birth weight babies. Even light smoking(less than 5 a day) is associated with a
greater risk of low birth weight. Maternal smoking is the single most important factor in low birth weight
in developed countries. Tobacco during pregnancy also brings increased risk of miscarriage, growth
retardation, stillbirth, small head circumference, sudden infant death, colic in early infancy, hyperkinetic
disorder (excessive movement) and long term respiratory, neurological, cognitive and behavioural
problems.
The effects of prenatal exposure to second-hand smoke on cognitive development tend to be worse when
the child also experiences socioeconomic hardships, such as substandard housing, malnutrition, and
inadequate clothing during the first 2years of life. Women who smoke during pregnancy also tend to
smoke after giving birth, and each type of exposure seems to habe independent effects.
Smoking during pregnancy seems to have some of the same effects on children whe they reach school age
as drinking during pregnancy: poor attention span, hyperactivity, anxiety, learning and behaviour
problems, perceptual-motor and linguistic problems, poor IQ scores, low grade placement, and
neurological problems.
Caffeine for the most part caffeine does not cause trouble for the fetus. Caffeine is not a teratogen for
human babies. However, 8 or more cups of coffee a day may dramatically increase the risk of fetal death
and four or more cups a day, of sudden death in infancy.
Marijuana, Cocaine, and Methamphetamine: some evidence from marijuana studies suggests that
heavy marijuana use can lead to birth defects, low birth weight, withdrawal like symptoms (excessive
crying and tremors) at birth, and increase risk of attention disorders and learning problems later in life.
The drug may affect functioning of the brain’s frontal lobe.
Cocaine use during pregnancy has been associated with spontaneous abortion, delayed growth, premature
labor, low birth weight, small head size, birth defects, and impaired neurological development. In some
studies, cocaine exposed new born’s showed acute withdrawal symptoms and sleep disturbances. High
prenatal cocaine exposure was associated with childhood behaviour problems.
Methamphetamine exposed infants were more likely to have low birth weight and to be small for their
gestational age. Prenatal methamphetamine exposure is associated with fetal growth restriction.
HIV/AIDS
If an expectant mother has the virus in her blood, perinatal transmission may occur: the virus may cross
over to the fetus’s bloodstream through the placenta during pregnancy, labor, or delivery or, after birth
through breastmilk. The risk of transmission can be reduced by choosing caesarean delivery, especially
when a woman has not received antiretroviral therapy, and by promotion of alternatives to breastfeeding.
Prospective parents should try to prevent any infection – colds, flu, urinary tract and vaginal infections, as
well as sexually transmitted diseases. If the mother does contract an infection, it should be treated
promptly. Pregnant women should also be screened for thyroid deficiency, which can affect their
children’s cognitive performance.
Rubella (German measles) if contracted before her 11th week of pregnancy; is almost certain to cause
deafness and heart defects in her baby.
Offspring of mothers with diabetes are 2-5 times more likely to develop birth defects, especially of the
heart and spinal cord (neural tube defects). Research on mice suggest why: high blood glucose levels
deprive an embryo of oxygen, with resultant cell damage, during the first 8weeks of pregnancy when its
organs are forming. Women with diabetes need to make sure their blood glucose levels are under control
before becoming pregnant. Use of multivitamin supplements during the first 3months before conception
and the first 3months of pregnancy can help reduce the risk of diabetes associated birth defects.
An infection called toxoplasmosis, caused by a parasite harboured in the bodies of cattle, sheep and pigs
and in the intestinal tracts of cats, typically produces either no symptoms or symptoms like those of the
common cold. Inpregnant woman, however, especially in the 2nd and 3rd trimesters, it can cause fetal
brain damage, severely impaired eyesight or blindness, seizures, or miscarriage, stillbirth, or death of the
baby. Although 9 out of 10 of these babies may appear normal at birth more than half of them have later
problems including eye infections, hearing loss and learning disabilities. Treatment with 2 anti-parasitic
drugs during the first year of life can reduce brain and eye damage. To avoid infection, expectant mother
should not eat raw meat or very rare meat should wash hands and all work surfaces after touching raw
meat, should peel or thoroughly wash raw fruit and vegetables, and should not dig in a garden where cat
faeces are buried. Women who have cats should get them checked for the disease, should not feed them
raw meat and should have some else change the litter box often.
Maternal Stress
Some stress during pregnancy is normal and does not necessarily increase risks of birth complications.
Moderate stress may even spur organization of the developing brain. Unusual stress during pregnancy
may negatively affect the offspring. In one study women whose parteners or children died or were
hospitalized for heart attackes or cancer were at elevated risk of giving birth to children with
malformations, such as cleft lip, cleft palate, and heart malformations.
Maternal Age
The chance of miscarriage of stillbirth rises with maternal age. The risk for miscarriage reaches 90% for
women age 45 and older. Women over 30-35 are more likely to suffer complications due to diabetes,
high blood pressure, or severe bleeding. There is also more likelihood of premature delivery, retarded
fetal growth, birth defects, and chromosomal abnormalities such as Down Syndrome. Women over 40
are at an increased risk of needing operative deliveries. Women over 50 are 2-3 times more likely than
younger women to have babies who are very small, born prematurely, or still born.
Adolescents also tend to have premature or underweight babies – perhaps because a young girls still-
growing body consumes vital nutrients the fetus needs. These newborns are at heightened risk of death in
the first month, disabilities, or health problems.
Air pollution, chemicals, radiation, extremes of heat and humidity, and other hazards of modern life can
affect prenatal development.
Air that contains high levels of fine combustion-related particles: premature or undersized infants or have
chromosomal abnormalities.
High concentrations of disinfection by-products: low birth weight and slowed fetal growth.
Lead, mercury, dioxin, nicotine and ethanol: explain sharp rise in asthma, allergies, and autoimmune
disorders such as lupus.
Drinking chemically contaminated water and use of pesticides: childhood cancers, incl. Leukemia
In utero exposure to radiation 8 through 15weeks after fertilization has been linked to mental retardation,
small head size, chromosomal malformations, Down Syndrome, seizures, and poor performance on IQ
tests and in school.
Paternal Factors
A man’s exposure to lead, marijuana or tobacco smoke, large amounts of alcohol or radiation, DES,
pesticides, or high ozone levels may result in abnormal or poor quality sperm.
Men who smoke have an increase likelihood of transmitting genetic abnormalities. A pregnant woman’s
exposure to the father’s second hand smoke has been linked with low birth weight, infant respiratory
infections, sudden infant death, and cancer in childhood and adulthood.
Older fathers may be a significant source of birth defects due to damaged sperm. Advancing paternal age
is associated with increases in the risk of several rare conditions including dwarfism. Advanced age
could also be a factor in a disproportionate number of cases of schizophrenia and of autism and related
disorders.
Babies have an internal clock that regulates their daily cycles of eating, sleeping, and elimination and
perhaps even their moods. These periodic cycles of wakefulness, sleep and activity, which govern an
infant’s state of arousal, or degree of alertness seems to be inborn and highly individual. Changes in state
are coordinated by multiple areas of the brain and are accompanied by changes in the functioning of
virtually all body systems.
Most babies spend about 75% of their time – up to 18 hours a day – asleep, but awaken every 3-4 hours,
day and night for feeding. Newborns sleep alternates between quiet (regular) and active (irregular) sleep.
Active sleep appears rhythmically in cycles of about 1 hour and accounts for up to 50% of newborn’s
sleep time. The amount of active sleep declines to less than 30% of daily sleep time by age 3 and
continues to decrease steadily throughout life.
Beginning in the first month, night-time sleep period gradually lengthen and total sleep time diminishes
as babies grow more wakeful in the daytime. Some infant’s begin to sleep through the night as early as
age 3months. By 6months, an infant typically sleeps for 6hours straight at night, but brief waking is
normal even during late infancy and toddlerhood. A 2year old typically sleeps about 13hours a day,
including a single nap usually in the afternoon.
Although crying is usually more distressing than serious, it is particularly important to soothe low birth
weight babies as quiet babies maintain their weight better. Steady stimulation is the time-proven way to
soothe crying babies: by rocking or walking them, wrapping them snugly, or letting them hear rhythmic
sounds.
Researchers following the ethological approach consider behaviour in human beings to be biologically
determined and emphasizes critical or sensitive periods for development of certain behaviours.
A critical period for bonding does not exist in humans. Parents can form a bond with the child even if
they are not present at the birth (adoptive parents and/or fathers).
From an evolutionary perspective, parental bonding may be a mechanism to ensure that parents invest the
tremendous energy and resources needed to enable a helpless infant to survive and reproduce.
Evolutionary developmental psychologists point out that child rearing involves a balancing act between
needs of the parents and those of the offspring. Bonding helps ensure that the benefits to the parents are
worth the cost.
Feeding is not the most important thing babies get from their mothers. Mothering includes the comfort of
close bodily contact and the satisfaction of an innate need to cling. Human infants also have needs that
must be satisfied if they are to grow up normally. It is the task of parents to try meet those needs.
The fathering role is a social construction, having different meanings in different cultures. In some
societies, fathers are more involved in their young children’s lives – economically, emotionally, and in
time spent – than other cultures.
A father’s frequent and positive involvement with his child, from infancy on, is directly related to the
child’s well-being and physical, cognitive, and social development.
We need to remember that patterns of parent-infant interaction we take for granted may be culture-based.
CHAPTER 7
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT DURING THE FIRST THREE
YEARS
STUDYING COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT: SIX APPROACHES
1. Behaviourist Approach
Studies the basic mechanics of learning, which fall in the domain of cognitive development
Behaviourists are concerned with how behaviour changes in response to experience
2. Psychometric Approach
Seeks to measure quantitative differences in cognitive abilities by using tests that indicate or
predict these abilities
3. Piagetian Approach
Looks at changes, stages, in the quality of cognitive functioning.
Concerned with how the mind structures its activities and adapts to the environment
4. Information-Processing Approach
Focus on processes involved in perception, learning, memory, problem solving
Seeks to discover what children do with their information from the time they encounter it, until
they use it.
5. Cognitive neuroscience Approach
Examines the hardware of the central nervous system
Seeks to identify what brain structures are involved in specific aspects of cognition
Infant memory
→ Infantile amnesia – no memory of events prior to 2 years old
Developmental scientists have proposed various explanations for this common phenomenon:
→ Piaget – brain not sufficiently developing to store memories
→ Freud – early memories are stored, but repressed, as they are emotionally troubling
Recent research – infant’s memory process does not differ fundamentally from older
children / adults except retention time is shorter.
These studies have found that babies will repeat a learned action if periodically reminded of
the situation (operant conditioning)
Young infant’s memory of behaviour is linked specifically to the original cue
2 – 6 mths old repeated the learned behaviour only when seeing the original stimulus
9 – 12 mths old try out behaviour on similar stimulus if no more than 2wks had gone by since
training
A familiar context can improve recollection when a memory has weakened
→ From evolutionary developmental perspective: abilities develop as they can fulfil useful
functions in adapting to the environment
Infancy is a time of great change and retention of specific experiences in unlikely to be useful
for long – this could be why adults don’t’ remember events that happened in infancy.
Classic conditioning
Learning based on associating a stimulus that does not ordinarily elicit a particular response
with another stimulus that does elicit the response (Pavlov)
Learner is passive and reacting to stimulus
Operant conditioning
The learner operates, or acts, on the environment
Learning is based on reinforcement or punishment
Response produced a particular effect
Also we cannot be sure on the basis of HOME and correlational findings that parental responsiveness or
an enriched home life actually increases a child’s intelligence. These factors are just associated with high
intelligence and achievement. Intelligent, well-educated parents may be more likely to provide positive,
stimulating home environments and because they also pass on their genes to their children there may be a
genetic influence as well (passive genotype correlation)
Research identified 7 aspects of early home environment that enable cognitive and psychosocial
development and help prepare children for school:
1. Encouraging exploration of the environment
2. Mentoring in basic cognitive and social skills
3. Celebrating developmental advances
4. Guidance in practicing and extending skills
5. Protection from inappropriate disapproval, teasing and punishment
6. Communicating richly and responsively
7. Guiding and limiting behaviour
The consistent presence of all 7 aspects early in life is “causally linked to many areas of brain
functioning and cognitive development”
Fostering competence (Cognitive development) *Table 7-2 pg 184*
1. In early months, provide sensory stimulation, but avoid over stimulation and distracting noises
2. As babies grow older, create an environment that fosters learning (books, objects, places to
play)
3. Respond to babies signals – establishes a sense of trust that the world is a friendly place and gives
baby a sense of control over their lives
4. Give babies that power to affect changes – toys that can be shaken, moulded, moved; help
baby discover turning door knob opens a door, flick a switch = light, tap = water
5. Give babies freedom to explore! Baby proof an environment and let them go!
6. Talk to babies, they won’t pick up the language from radio / TV – they need adults
interaction
7. In talking / playing – enter into whatever they are interested in at the moment instead of
redirecting their attention
8. Arrange opportunities to learn basic skills – labelling, comparing, sorting size and colour
9. Applaud new skills and help baby practice and expand them
10. Read to baby in warm, caring atmosphere from an early age
11. Use punishment sparingly
PIAGETIAN APPROACH: THE SENSORIMOTOR STAGE
FIRST stage of Piagets stages of cognitive development = sensorimotor stage
SIX Sub Stages of the Sensorimotor Stage *Study Table 7-3 pg186*
SCHEMES – Piaget’s terms for organised patterns of thought and behaviour used in particular
situations
Stages flow from one to another as babies’ schemes become more elaborate
Stage 1 – 5: babies learn to coordinate input from senses and organise activities in relation to their
environment – by a process of organisation, adaption and equilibration
6 sub stage: progress form trial and error learning to the use of symbols and concepts to solve simple
th
problems
→ Early cognitive growth comes from CIRCULAR REACTIONS, in which infant learns to
reproduce pleasurable or interesting events originally discovered by chance.
Activity produces enjoyable sensation, therefore action is repeated (in which cause and
effect keep reversing) and the original chance behaviour becomes a new scheme
1. First sub-stage (birth – one month)
→ Neonates exercise some control over inborn reflexes i.e. sucking reflexively when lips touched,
such when not hungry
2. Second sub-stage (one – four months)
Baby learns to repeat pleasant bodily sensation first achieved by chance – primary circular
reaction
→ Baby turns towards sounds, showing ability to coordinate different sensory information (vision
and hearing)
3. Third sub-stage (four – eight months)
New interest in manipulating objects and learning about their properties - secondary circular
reactions:
→ Intentional actions repeated to get results beyond the infants own body. i.e. shake a rattle to hear
noise, coo to a friendly face to make it stay longer
4. Fourth sub-stage (eight – twelve months)
→ Coordination of secondary schemes – baby has built on schemes they were born with.
→ Learned to generalise from past experiences to solve new problems – they can distinguish
means from ends
Crawl to get what they want, grab it; push away a barrier to it
They try out, modify and coordinate previous schemes to find one that works
This sub-stage marks the development of complex goal-directed behaviour
5. Fifth sub-stage (twelve - eighteen months)
→ Baby experiments with new behaviour
Once they walk, they explore environment with more ease
→ Tertiary circular reactions = varying an action to get a similar result (rather than repeating
pleasing behaviour)
a toddler may squeeze a rubber duck that squeaked when stepped on to see if it will squeak again
→ for the first time children show originality in problem solving
By trial and error, they try out behaviours until they find the best way to attain a goal
6. Sixth sub-stage – mental combinations (eighteen months – two years) is a transition into
preoperational stage of early childhood.
→ Toddler develops REPRESENTATIONAL ABILITY – the ability to mentally represent
objects and events in memory, largely through symbols such as worlds, numbers, mental pictures
The ability to manipulate symbols frees children from immediate experience, they can pretend, and
they can think about actions before taking them – no more trial and error.
The OBJECT CONCEPT - the idea that objects have an independent existence, characteristics and
locations in space - is a later fundamental cognitive development to an orderly view of physical reality
Object concept is the basis for children’s awareness that they exist apart from objects, and other
people
When does object permanence develop?
Object Permanence – Piaget – understanding that an object/person will continue to exist even when out
of sight, develops in the sensorimotor stage
At first infants have no such concept but by the:
3rd sub-stage – 4 – 8 months – they will look for something dropped and if they can’t see it – will act as
if it no longer exists
4th sub-stage – 8 – 12 months – will look for object in place they first found it, after seeing it hidden,
even if they later saw it being moved to another place = A, not-B error
5th sub-stage – 12 – 18 months – will search in place object was last seen hidden, but will not search for
it in a place where they did not see it hidden
6th sub-stage – 18 – 24 months – object permanence achieved, toddler looks for an object even if they
did not see it hidden
Esther Thelen’s dynamic systems theory – is a new interpretation of the A, NOT B theory
The decision where to search for an object is about what babies do and why – not about what they know.
Infants reaching behaviour is influenced by vision, perception, attention, movement and memory
One factor is how much time has elapsed between the infants seeing the object hidden in a new place (B)
and the infants reaching for it. If the elapsed time is brief, the infant is more likely to reach for the object
in the new location. When the time interval is longer, however, the perceptual and motor memory of
having previously found the object in the old place (A) inclines the infant to search there again and that
inclination grows stronger the more times the object has been found in the same place.
Piaget believed ability of object permanence developed during sensorimotor period, over course of 6 sub-
stages and object concept is fully developed by 18 – 24 months
In contrast – recent research findings indicate that object concept develops at much earlier stage
INFORMATION-PROCESSING APPROACH: PERCEPTIONS AND
REPRESENTATIONS
Habituation
Visual preference – the amount of time a baby spends looking at different kinds of sights: based
on visual the ability to make visual distinctions
Novelty preference – new-born’s preference to look at new sights over familiar ones
Visual recognition memory – is the ability to distinguish a familiar visual stimulus from an
unfamiliar one, when shown both at the same. Depends on comparing incoming information with
existing information (ability to form and refer to mental representation)
Contrary to Piaget’s view:
Habituation and novelty preference studies suggest rudimentary representational ability exists at
birth and becomes more efficient.
→ Auditory discrimination studies – are based on attentional preference: New-borns can tell sounds
that they have already heard from new sounds.
Piaget: believed that senses are unconnected at birth and are only gradually integrated through experience
→ If so – the integration begins immediately
The fact that neonates will look at the source of sounds shows that they associate hearing and sight.
Language = communication system based on words and grammar, and cognitive development
CHAPTER 8 –
PSYCHOSOCIAL DEVELOPMENT DURING THE FIRST
THREE YEARS
Temperament
Temperament
A person characteristic biologically based way of approaching and reacting to people and situations
The “how” of behaviour
Temperament has an emotional dimension which is consistent and enduring
Temperament may also affect the way children regulate their mental, emotional, and behavioural
functioning
Individual differences in temperament form the core of the developing personality
Studying Temperamental Patterns (the New York Longitudinal Study – NYLS) *study table 8-2 pg
222*
Goodness of fit: the match between a child’s temperament and the environmental demands and
constraints the child must deal with
i.e. An active child forced to sit still for a long period of time or a slow to warm up child constantly
pushed into new situations; tension may occur
Caregiver’s response to children may reflect the amount of control the caregiver thinks they have over a
child’s behaviour
Parent’s who think they have little control, play directively with their babies - urging, reminding,
restraining, questioning and correcting them. They were more likely to consider their infant difficult
When caregiver recognises child acts a certain way due to inborn temperament, they will feel less out of
control and can anticipate the child’s reactions and help the child adapt
Developing Attachments
Attachment – reciprocal, enduring emotional tie between two people (infant & caregiver) each of whom
contribute to the quality of the relationship
From an evolutionary point of view, attachments have adaptive values for babies, ensuring their
psychosocial as well as their physical needs will be met
ETHOLOGICAL THEORY: behaviour is biologically determined. Infants and parents are
biologically predisposed to becoming attached to each other and attachment promotes a baby’s survival
On the basis of a baby’s interactions with the mother, baby builds a model of what can be expected from
her.
If mother continues to act in the same way – the model holds up
If behaviour is repeatedly changed, then baby might revise the model and security of attachment might
change.
Working model of attachment related to Erikson’s concept of basic trust
Secure attachment= trust; Insecure attachment = mistrust
Securely attached babies have learned to trust their caregivers and their own ability to get what they need.
Babies who cry a lot and whose mothers respond by soothing them tend to be securely attached.
Mothers of securely attached infants are sensitive and responsive; equally NB is mutual interaction,
stimulation, a positive attitude, warmth and acceptance, and emotional support.
Varied findings
Both mothers sensitivity and baby’s temperament influence attachment patterns
Neurological / physiological conditions may underlie temperamental difference in attachment
Eg. Variability of heart rate associated with irritability; and heart rate seems to vary more in insecurely
attached infants
A baby’s temperament may have not only an impact on attachment but also an indirect impact through its
effects on the parents.
Irritability on the infants part may prevent the development of secure attachment but not if the mother has
the skills to cope with the baby’s temperament
Stranger Anxiety (wariness of unknown people) and Separation Anxiety (distress when family
caregiver leaves)
Developing Autonomy
Erikson: Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt 2nd stage of psychosocial development
18 months – 3 years
Autonomy versus shame and doubt – if successfully resolved, results in the virtue of WILL
Marked by shift from external control to self control
Toddler substitutes their own judgement for their caregivers
Toilet training is an important step toward autonomy
Language allows their wishes to be expressed, making them more powerful and independent
Unlimited freedom is not safe / healthy – therefore shame and doubt necessary
Toddlers need appropriate limits set by parents; shame and doubt help them recognise the need for those
limits
Terrible Twos is a normal manifestation of the drive for autonomy – toddlers test the notion that
they are individuals who have control over their world, and that they new and exciting powers
Drive show’s itself in the form of negativism – the tendency to shout “NO” just for the sake of resisting
authority
Starts at 2, peaks by 3 ½ - 4 and declines by 6
Caregivers who view expression of self- will by children as normal, healthy and striving for independence
– and not as stubbornness, help them to learn self-control, contribute to their sense of competence, and
avoid excessive conflict
Dealing with Terrible two’s – How to discourage negativism and encourage socially acceptable
behaviour
*Table 8-4 pg234*
Socialisation:
Is the process by which children develop habits, skills, values and motives that make them responsible,
productive members of society. Compliance with parental expectations can be seen as the first step
toward compliance with societal standards
Socialization rests on:
Internalisation of these standards:
The process by which children accept societal standards of conduct as their own
Successfully socialised children don’t merely obey rules or commands to get rewards / avoid
punishments, but they have made society’s standards their own.
Developing self-regulation
Self-regulation: control of behaviour to conform to a caregivers demands or expectations, even
when the caregiver is not present / Control of a person’s behaviour to conform to understood
societal expectations
Self-regulation is the foundation of socialisation and links all domains of development – physical,
cognitive, social, emotional
Toddler sticking finger in plug – father says no – toddler stops and conforms future behaviour in the same
situation
By reading parents emotional response to their behaviour, children absorb information about what
conduct their parents approve of
Before they can control their behaviour, children need to regulate/control their ATTENTIONAL
PROCESSES and modulate negative emotions, enabling them to develop willpower and cope with
frustration
Growth of self-regulation parallels development of self-consciousness & evaluative emotions – empathy,
shame, guilt . It requires the ability to wait for gratification. It is correlated with measures of conscience
development, such as resisting temptation and making amends for wrong doing.
Full development of self-regulation takes at least 3 years in most children.
Conscience = internal standards of behaviour which usually controls one’s conduct and produce
emotional discomfort when violated
Before children can develop a conscience they need to have internalised moral standards. Conscience
depends on willingness to the right thing before a child believes it is right, not (as in self-regulatory) just
because someone else says so.
Constructive conflict over child’s misbehaviour - conflict involving negotiation, reasoning, resolution -
can help children develop moral understanding by helping them to see another point of view.
Receptive cooperation – goes beyond committed compliance
Child’s eager willingness to cooperate harmoniously with a parent in both disciplinary and daily
interactions
Enables child to be an active partner in socialisation
Boys
Longer, heavier, slightly stronger
More physically vulnerable from conception on
Brains at birth 10% larger (continues to adulthood)
Equal to girls in sensitivity of touch, tend to teeth, sit-up, walk – at the same age
Behavioural – play more aggressively than girls
Girls
Less reactive to stress and more likely to survive infancy
Brains at birth 10% smaller than boys
Behavioural – girls (2-3 years) say words pertaining to their gender
Infants perceive differences between male and female before behaviour is gender differentiated and even
before they can talk
6 months – respond different to male and female voice
9 – 12 months – tell difference in male versus female faces
24 – 36 months – infants associate gender typical toys (dolls) with the face of the correct gender
Boys slower than girls with this knowledge
US parents promote GENDER TYPING = socialisation process by which children learn behaviour
that their culture considers appropriate for each sex
Fathers treat boys and girls more differently than mothers do. During the first year fathers talk more and
spend more time with boys, and mothers with girls. Girls at this age tend to be more talkative than boys.
Fathers of toddlers play more roughly with their sons and show more sensitivity to daughters.
Negative effects on cognitive development at 15months – 3years was evident when mothers worked 30 or
more hours a week by the child’s 9th month. Maternal sensitivity, a high quality home environment, and
high-quality child care lessened, but did not eliminate these negative effects.
On the other hand, boys and girls in low-income families tend to benefit academically from the more
favourable environment a working mother’s income can provide.
The economic and social benefit s of maternal employment may outweigh any disadvantages resulting
from reduced time spent with the child.
Differences in time spent with infants were modestly related to maternal sensitivity but did not affect
social ort cognitive outcomes. Infants whose mothers spent more time with them did have more
stimulating home environments, but so did infants whose mothers spent more time at work. It seems
then, that mothers who are temperamentally prone to be sensitive and to provide stimulating, warm home
environments may find ways to do so whether or not they are employed.
CHAPTER 10 – COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT IN EARLY
CHILDHOOD
PIAGETIAN APPROACH: THE PREOPERATIONAL CHILD
Piaget: the pre-operational stage (Age 2 – 7 years) – so called because children at this age are not ready
to engage in logical mental operations
Characterised by great expansion of symbolic thought, or representational ability
Growing understanding of causality, identities, categorisation and number
Until age 3, children do not grasp relationship between pictures, maps, models and the larger / smaller
objects they represent
Older preschoolers can use simple maps, and they can transfer the spatial understanding gained from
working with models to maps and vice versa
Understanding of Causality
Piaget: Preoperational children cannot reason logically about cause and effect – they reason by
transduction = they mentally link two events whether or not there is a logically causal relationship
When tested on situations they can understand, young children do grasp cause and effect
In naturalistic observations of 2 ½ - 5 year olds everyday conversations with their parents, they show
flexible causal reasoning, appropriate to the subject
Pre-schoolers view all causal relationships as equally and absolutely predictable
Number
Piaget: main characteristic of preoperational thought = CENTRATION = tendency to focus on one aspect
of a situation and neglect others
DECENTER – ability to think about several aspects of a situation simultaneously; causes children to
come to illogical conclusions
EGOCENTRISM
Example of centration – two things / objects that are equal remain so if their appearance is altered, so long
as nothing is added or taken away. Preoperational children cannot consider height and width at the same
time – they centre on one aspect and therefore cannot think logically
IRREVERSIBILTY – failure to understand that an operation can go in two or more directions.
Preoperational children think as if they were watching a slide show with a series of static frames – they
focus on successive states and don’t recognise the transformation from one state to another
AGE Knowledge
3 – 5 years Understand that thinking occurs in the mind;
that it can deal with either real or imaginary
things; that someone can be thinking of one
thing while doing something else; that a person
whose ears and eyes are covered can think about
objects; that someone who looks pensive is
probably thinking; and that thinking is different
from seeing, talking, touching, and knowing.
Pre-schoolers Believe that mental activity starts and stops
Little / no awareness that they think in words, or
think while listening, reading, talking
Believe they can dream about anything they
wish
Middle childhood Children realise the mind is continuously active
5 years More understanding that physical experiences,
emotions, knowledge, and thoughts can affect
the content of dreams
11 years Realise they cannot control their dreams
Social Cognition– recognition that others have mental states – accompanies decline of egocentrism and
development of empathy
AGE Understanding
3 years Lacks understanding that people can hold false
beliefs, which flow from the realisation that
people hold mental representations of reality,
which can sometimes be wrong (crayons in a
candy box)
May stem from egocentric thinking – belief that
everyone else knows what they know and have
trouble understanding that their beliefs can be
false
4 years Understand that people who see / hear same
event may come away with different belief
6 year s Realise 2 people can have different
interpretations of the same event
Deception
Deliberate effort to plant false believe in someone’s mind
Suppression of impulse to be truthful
Piaget: children regard all falsehoods as lies
Recent research: 3 year olds have an understanding of the role of intent in deception
Piaget: 5/6 years old distinguish between what seems to be and what is – research confirms this.
3 year olds – confuse appearance with reality. However, putting the task in the context of deception helps
children realise that an object can be perceived as other than what it actually is.
Between 18 months and 3 years – children learn to distinguish between real and imagined events
3 years old:
Knows the difference between a real dog and a dog in a dream and between something invisible (air) and
something imaginary
They can pretend and they know when someone is pretending
Knows the difference between trying to do something and pretending to do something
Magical thinking: is a way to explain events that don’t have obvious realistic explanations or simply to
indulge in pleasures of pretending (imaginary friends). They are aware of the magical nature of such
fantasy figures but are more willing to entertain the possibility that they may be real
Magical thinking tends to decline near end of preschool period.
Some children develop theory of mind abilities earlier than others – reflects brain maturation and
improvements in cognition.
Social competence and language development contribute to an understanding of thoughts and emotions
Children whose teachers and peers rate them high on social are better able t recognise false beliefs, to
distinguish between and real and feigned emotion, and to take another person’s point of view. These
children also tend to have strong language skills.
The kind of talk a child hears at home may affects the child’s understanding of mental states
Families that encourage pretend play – stimulate the development of theory of mind skills
Bilingual children know an object can be represented linguistically in more than one way, this may help
them see that different people have different perspectives
Bilingual children also recognise the need to match language to their partner, therefore they are more
aware of others mental state
Bilingual children tend to have better attentional control and this may enable them to focus on what is
true and real rather then what seems to be so.
Early childhood memories rarely deliberate – children remember events that made a strong impression
GENERIC MEMORY
Age two – produces a script of familiar routines to guide behaviour
A general outline of a familiar repeated event without details of time or place
EPISODIC MEMORY
LTM of specific experiences or events linked to a time and place
Early episodic memories enable young children to build a mental picture of their world by organising
their experience around events.
Young children remember more clearly events that are new to them
Young children have limited memory capacity therefore episodic memories are temporary – unless they
recur several times in which case they are transferred to generic memory
AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY
Specific and long-lasting memories that from a person’s life history
Only memories that have special personal meaning are stored
Emerges age 3 – 4, continuous by 4 ½
Late arrival possibly due to the fact that children cannot store memories about themselves until the
required development of a concept of self emerges, around which to organise those memories
IQ score= measure of how well a child can do at certain tasks, at a certain time, in comparison with
others of the same age.
Factors that affect how well a child does on an IQ test:
Temperament
Social and emotional maturity
Ease in the testing situation
Pre-literacy or literacy skills
Socioeconomic status – affects health, stress, parenting practice and home atmosphere
Ethnicity and culture
Match between child’s cognitive style and tasks proposed
Influence of home environment is questionable – parental IQ influences home environment which
impacts child
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
Piaget:
Private speech is a sign of cognitive immaturity
Young children are egocentric, they cannot recognise others viewpoints and therefore cannot
communicate meaningfully, therefore simply vocalise what’s on their mind
Young children cannot symbolise (distinguish between words and the actions the words stand for)
By end of pre-operational stage, cognitive maturation and social experience result in children becoming
less egocentric and more capable of symbolic thought – therefore discard private speech
Vygotsky
Private speech helps young children integrate language with thought (Piaget and Vygotsky)
Not egocentric but a special form of communication, conversation with the self
It serves important function between early social speech and inner speech – a transition towards the
internalisation of socially derived control of behaviour
Private speech stimulated by social experience
Tends to increase when child is trying to perform difficult tasks
Emergent Literacy: Pre-schoolers development of skills, knowledge and attitudes that underlie
reading and writing
Pre-reading:
Oral Language skills – vocabulary, syntax, narrative structure.
Understanding that language used to communicate
Specific skills that help in decoding the printed word
Phonological skills
Phonemic awareness – realisation that words are composed of distinct phonemes (sounds) and phoneme
grapheme correspondence – the ability to link sounds with corresponding letters
Self-Esteem
Conflict arises from a sense of purpose which spurs a child to plan and carry out activities, and the
growing consciousness / moral reservations that may prevent them from carrying out plans
When the desire to do is reconciled with the desire for approval, the virtue of PURPOSE results – the
courage to envision and pursue goals without being unduly inhibited by feeling so guilt / fear of
punishment
If the conflict is not resolved – the child may grow into an adult that who continually strives for success
or shows off, is inhibited and unspontaneous or self righteous and intolerant; or suffers from impotence or
psychosomatic illness.
With ample opportunities to do things on their own – but under guidance and consistent limits – children
can attain a healthy balance and avoid the tendency to overdo competition and achievement and the
tendency to be repressed and guilt ridden.
GENDER
Gender Differences
Gender Differences – Psychological & behavioural differences between male and female
Gender Roles: Behaviours, interests, attitudes, skills and personality traits that a culture
considers appropriate for each sex – differs for males and females
Gender Typing: Socialisation process taking place in early childhood whereby children learn
appropriate gender roles
Gender Stereotypes: Overgeneralisations about male and female behaviour “all males are aggressive”
Cognitive Approaches on Gender Development
GENDER STABILITY
Comes with a girls realisation she will grow up to be a woman and a boy will grow up to be a man –
gender does not change
NB – they may base judgements about gender on superficial appearances (cloths/hair) and stereotyped
behaviour
GENDER CONSISTENCY
3 – 7 years or later
Realisation that a girl remains a girl even if her hair is short and she wears trousers and;
Boy remains boy even if his hair is long and he wears earings
Once children realise that their behaviour or dress will not affect their sex, they become less rigid in their
adherence to gender norms.
Gender-Schema = a mentally organised network of information about gender that influences behaviour
Develops with age in response to experiences
As knowledge about gender increases it influences what they do, what they pay attention to and what they
remember.
The role of Socialisation
Family Influence
Experiences in the family seem to reinforce gender typical preferences and attitudes
Difficult to separate environment and genetic influences
Boys more strongly gender socialised concerning play preferences than girls (dolls vs. trucks)
Girls have more freedom with toys, clothes, games
Egalitarian household – father’s role in gender socialisation important: children whose fathers did
more housework and child care were less aware of gender stereotypes and engaged in less gender-typed
play
Siblings influence gender development – the 2nd born child is more like older sibling
Peer Influences
By age 3, pre-schoolers play in sex-segregated groups which reinforces gender typing
Peer groups show more disapproval of boys who act like girls, than of girls who act like boys
Cultural Influences
TV is a major channel for transmission of cultural attitudes towards gender
Books – source of gender stereotype – woman in domestic roles, not as pilots!
Biosocial Theory
Psychological aspects of gender arise from interaction between physical characteristics of sexes, their
developmental experiences and character of societies they live in.
PARENTING
Forms of Discipline
Discipline – methods of moulding children’s character and of teaching them to exercise self-control and
engage in acceptable behaviour
Children usually learn more from being reinforced for good behaviour
EXTERNAL reinforcement = Tangible – sweets, money, gold stars or Intangible – smile, hug, praise,
special privilege
Should be received consistently after showing desired behaviour
Eventually behaviour should provide its own INTERNAL reward – a sense of pleasure /
accomplishment
When the parent ignores when the child behaves well and scolds and spanks when bad – they are actually
reinforcing misbehaviour with attention
Still punishment such as isolation or denial of privileges, is necessary
Must be consistent, immediate and tied to the offence
Must be administered calmly and in private and aimed at eliciting compliance, not guilt
Most effective when accompanied with short explanation
Harsh punishment can be counterproductive
CORPORAL PUNISHMENT = The use of physical force with the intention of causing pain, but not
injury, so as to correct / control behaviour - this has NEGATIVE consequences
PSYCHOLOGICAL AGGRESSION – verbal attack by a parent – results in psychological harm to the
child
Parental Styles
3 parenting styles and typical behaviour patterns of children raised according to each
AUTHORITARIAN
Value control and unquestioning obedience
Make children conform rigidly to a set standard of conduct
Punishment for violation of standards using power assertive techniques
Detached, less warm parents
Children = discontented, withdrawn, mistrusting
PERMISSIVE
Value self-expression and self-regulation
Make few demands and allow children to monitor their own activities
Consult children about policy decisions and rarely punish
Warm, non-controlling, undemanding
Preschool children – immature, least self-controlled and least exploratory
AUTHORITATIVE
Values child individuality but also stress social constraints
Confidence in their ability to guide children
Respect children’s independence decisions, interests, opinions and personalities
Loving and accepting and demand good behaviour – firm in maintaining standards
Impose limited, judicious punishment within context of warm loving relationship
Favour inductive techniques in discipline, explaining the reasoning behind their stands and encouraging
verbal negotiation / give and take
Children secure in knowing they are loved and what is expected of them
Pre-schoolers most self-reliant, self-controlled, self-assertive, exploratory and content
Children – perform well, fulfil commitments
Neglectful/ Uninvolved
Sometimes because of stress or depression, focus on own needs rather than those of children.
Neglectful parenting been linked with a variety of behavioural disorders in childhood and adolescence
BOYS GIRLS
More physically & verbally aggressive –
apparent by age 2
Engage in OVERT DIRECT aggression – Engage in RELATIONAL, SOCIAL aggression
aggression openly directed at its target – more subtle, damaging or interfering with
relationships, reputation or psychological well-
being (teasing, manipulating, rumours, name
calling) It can be either overt or covert(indirect)
Influences on aggression
Children who are more intense emotionally and low in self-control tend to express anger more
aggressively
Physical aggression genetically inherited
Social aggression environmentally influenced
Insecure attachment and lack of maternal warmth in infancy predict early aggression in childhood
Manipulative behaviours such as withdrawal of love and making a child feel guilty or ashamed may foster
social aggression.
Negative parent-child relationships may set the stage for prolonged, destructive sibling conflicts, in which
children imitate their parent’s hostile behaviour.
Aggression may be bred from a stressful, un-stimulating home environment, harsh discipline, lack of
maternal warmth and social support, exposure to aggression and violence and transient peer groups which
prevent stable friendships
Fearfulness
Young children’s fear stems from mostly intense fantasy life and tendency to confuse appearance with
reality
Older children fears more realistic and self-evaluative and stem from personal experience or hearing
about something
Triggered by appraisals of danger
Parents allay children’s fears by instilling a sense of trust and normal caution without being too protective
Reassure and encourage open expression of feelings
SYSTEMATIC DESENSITISATION = Therapeutic technique
Child is exposed to gradually increasing amounts of feared object / situation
Sibling / peer relationships strengthen social cognition or ‘mind reading ‘ – the ability to understand
others intentions , feelings
Relationships provide a measuring stick for self-efficacy = child’s sense of capability to master
challenges and achieve goals
CHAPTER 12
PHYSICAL DEVELOPMENT AND HEALTH IN MIDDLE
CHILDHOOD
(6-12YEARS)
Recess time Play - promotes growth in agility and social competence and fosters adjustment to
school
Organised sports
Girls tend to spend less time than boys on sports and more time on housework, studying, personal
care
Regular physical activity has immediate and long term health benefits:
It improves motor skills
Weight control
Lower blood pressure
Improved cardio-respiratory functioning
Enhanced self-esteem and well being
@ age 7 = stage of concrete operations: uses mental operations to solve concrete (actual) problems
Think logically and take multiple aspects of a situation into account
Real situations in the here and now
Cognitive Advances
Advances in Selected Cognitive Abilities during Middle Childhood *Table 13-1 pg 351*
Ability Example
Spatial Thinking Use map/model to search for hidden object
Give someone else directions to find object
Estimate distance – find their way to and from
school
Judge how long it will take to get from A to B
Cause and Effect Understands physical attributes of an object will
affect weight (number of objects matter – not
their colour)
DOESN’T know that position and placement of
objects will make a difference
Categorization Sort objects into their categories – shape, colour,
subclass (rose) has fewer members than the class
of which it’s a part (flower)
Seriation and Transitive Inference Arrange sticks from shortest to longest
Insert intermediate stick correctly in place
1st stick longer than 2nd & 2nd longer then 3rd –
can deduce that 1st is longer than 3rd
Inductive and Deductive Reasoning Can solve inductive and deductive problems
Knows that certain inductive conclusions (based
on a particular premise) are less certain than
deductive ones based on a general premise
Conservation @ 7 knows that a ball of clay in different shapes
still hold the same amount of clay
@ 9 knows ball and sausage shape of clay weigh
the same
Early adolescence – know they displace the
same liquid if dropped into water
Number and Mathematics Count in their head
Add by counting from the smallest number
Do simple story problems
Space and Causality (1 + 2)
Categorisation (3)
Helps children think logically and includes:
SERIATION: arrange objects in a series according to a dimension (length – short to long / colour – light
to dark)
TRANSITIVE INFERENCE: Ability to infer a relationship between 2 objects, from the
relationship
between each of them and a 3rd object
CLASS INCLUSION: Ability to see relationship between whole and it’s part (rose is smaller
part of flowers)
Inductive Reasoning:
Children in concrete operations stage move from particular observation to a general conclusion
“My dog barks, so does yours
Deductive Reasoning
Develops in adolescence – general statement or premise to a specific statement
Conservation
Understands principle of identity – clay is the same clay despite a diff. shape – 7/8 years
PRINCIPLE OF REVERSIBILITY Change sausage clay back into ball of clay – 7/8 years
DECENTER can focus on length and width (preoperational can only focus on one
dimension)
CONSERVATION OF WEIGHT 9 – 10 years - solve the problem of whether the ball and sausage
weigh the same
CONSERVATION OF VOLUME 12 years – sausage or ball displace equal amounts of liquid
HORIZONTAL DECALAGE: Piaget’s term for child’s inability to transfer learning about one type of
conservation to other types – very concrete thinking
The shift from rigid illogical thinking of younger children to the flexible logical thinking of older children
Depends on neurological development and experience in adapting to the environment
Children who achieved conservation of volume tasks had different brain wave patterns
Moral Reasoning
Younger than 7 years - Immature moral judgement centre on degree of offence (large ink stain)
More mature judgements consider intent
Piaget – moral reasoning develops in 3 stages
Age 2 – 7: Preoperational stage = Rigid obedience to authority
Young children are egocentric therefore they cannot imagine more than one way of looking at a moral
issue
Rules cannot be bent / changed
Behaviour is either right or wrong
Any offense deserved punishment, regardless of intent
Age 7 – 12: Concrete operations = increasing flexibility
More interactions and more viewpoints
Develop a sense of justice, fairness
Consider multiple aspects of a situation therefore can take intent into consideration
@ 12 – capable of formal reasoning = Equity
Belief that everyone should be treated alike and take specific circumstances into account
Selective Attention: ability to direct one’s attention deliberately and shut out distractions (due to
neurological maturation)
School age children can summon from memory the meaning of a word and suppress others whose
meaning does not fit the context
Growth in selective attention may hinge on the executive skill of INHIBITORY CONTROL =
voluntary suppression of unwanted responses
Conventional intelligence tests tap only 3 intelligences (linguistic, Logical / mathematical, Spatial)
Gardner’s theory = each person has 8 distinct forms of intelligence
*Table 13-3 pg362*
LINGUISTIC: Ability to use and understand words and nuances of meaning, writing, editing, translating
LOGICAL / MATHMATICAL:
Ability to manipulate numbers and solve logical problems, science, business, medicine
SPATIAL:
Ability to navigate an environment and judge relationships between objects in space, architecture,
carpentry, city planning.
MUSICAL:
Ability to perceive and create patterns of pity and rhythm, musical composition, conducting,
BODYILY KINESTHETIC:
Ability to move with precision, dancing, athletics, surgery
INTERPERSONAL:
Ability to understand and communicate with others, teaching, acting, politics
INTRAPERSONAL:
Ability to understand the self, counselling, psychiatry
NATURALIST: Ability to distinguish species and their characteristics, hunting, fishing, farming,
cooking
High intelligence in one area does not necessarily accompany high intelligence in any others
TACIT KNOWLEDGE = Information not formally taught, or openly expressed, but necessary to get
ahead
Practical knowledge needed to use language for communication purposes – include conversational skills
There are gender differences:
boys use more controlling statements and make more negative interruptions
Girls – more tentative remarks, conciliatory way.
Communication is more collaborate when working with a partner of the same sex.
Literacy
Reading, writing – no longer rely on face to face communication
Allows children access to ideas and imaginations to others
Reading
DECODING: Process of phonetic analysis by which printed word is converted to spoken, before retrieval
from long term memory
Visually Based Retrieval – looks at the word and retrieves it from LTM
Phonetic / Code-Emphasis approach – teaching reading that emphasises decoding of unfamiliar words
Whole-language approach – emphasise on visual retrieval and use of contextual cues.
Children learn to read with better comprehension if they experience written langue as a way to
gain information and express ideas and feelings – not as a system of isolated sounds and syllables
(teacher directed tasks)
Instruction in both phonetic strategies to decode unfamiliar worlds and visual retrieval for familiar words
– forms better more versatile readers
As word identification becomes more automatic and capacity of working memory increases, children can
focus on meaning of what they read and can adjust their speed and attentiveness to the importance and
difficulty of the material
METACOGNITION: awareness of one’s own thinking processes helps children monitor their
understanding of what they read and enables them to develop strategies to clear up any problems – such
as reading slowing or rereading passages.
Having students recall, summarise, and ask questions about what they read can enhance comprehension.
Writing
Unlike conversation, which offers constant feedback, writing requires the child to judge independently
whether the communication goal has been met. The child must also keep in mind a variety of other
constraints: spelling, punctuation, grammar, and capitalization, as well as the basic physical task of
forming letters.
Self-efficacy beliefs:
High self-efficacy: believe in themselves and their ability – more likely to succeed
Self-regulated learner, set’s goals, tries hard and plans, seeks help when necessary
Low self-efficacy: does not believe in their own ability, become frustrated and depressed
Gender:
Girls:
Do better than boys in school; less likely to repeat grades, have fewer school problems and outperform
boys in national reading and writing assessments.
In their approach to school work – they aim for mastery of subject matter
Better classroom behaviour and more efficient strategies for learning BUT less confidence in their
abilities
Boys
Concerned with how smart they look in class
Have an advantage is spatial skills –but this is affected by SES – higher SES boys are more likely to
engage in spatially oriented activities – Lego.
Parenting Practices
Create an environment for learning and routine
Show interest in children’s lives – Children whose parents are involved in their schools do better in
school
Parents perceived self-efficacy – their belief in their ability to promote child’s academic growth - affects
their success in doing so
Parents who are economically secure and who have high aspirations for their children and a strong sense
of parental efficacy tend to have children with high academic goals and achievement
Parents motivate using:
Extrinsic (external means) – money, treats
Intrinsic (internal means) – praising ability and hard work = more effective
Peer acceptance
Industry versus inferiority – children must learn productive skills required by their culture or face feelings
of inferiority
With successful resolution- virtue developed = COMPETENCE = a view of self as able to master skills
and complete tasks
Unsuccessful resolution will result in feelings of inadequacy – which will result in a return to the familiar
or if they become too industrious they may turn into workaholics
Parents strongly influence children’s beliefs about competence
Emotional Growth
@ 7 / 8 years children:
Aware of shame and pride
Have a clearer idea between guilt and shame – which affects opinion of self
Understand conflicting emotions (boys are yucky but I love my brother)
Middle childhood - aware of cultural rules for emotional expression
Knows what makes them angry / sad and how to adapt behaviour accordingly
Emotional self-regulation involves effortful (voluntary) control of emotions, attention & behaviour
Low in effortful control = visibly angry / frustrated
High in effortful control = able to stifle impulse to show negative emotion at inappropriate times – this
ability increases with age.
Pro-social behaviour
School age children generally become more empathic and pro-social.
Act appropriately in social situations and control negative emotions and cope with problems
constructively.
Parents who respond to children’s distress help them to foster empathy, pro-social behaviour, and social
skills.
When parents respond with disapproval or punishment, emotions such as anger and fear my become more
intense and may impair social adjustment, or the child may become more secretive and anxious about
these negative feelings. As children approach adolescence, parental intolerance of negative emotion may
heighten parent-child conflict.
Family Structure
Children do better in families with 2 continuously married people
Happily married – children have higher standard of living, effective parenting, closer relationships with
parents, and fewer stressful events
Parent’s relationship Affects child’s adjustment more than parents
Quality of parenting marital status
Ability to create favourable family atmosphere
Father’s frequent and positive involvement with child is directly related to child’s well-being and
physical, cognitive and social development
Adoptive families
Adoption is usually closed, no contact between birth mother and child
OPEN ADOPTION: allows contact / share info
Few significant differences between adopted and biological children
Adopted children score higher on IQ – perform better in school than siblings / peers who remain in
institutions
Interracial adoption – no problems psychologically adjusting
Living with Grandparents
Age difference can be a barrier and because of their age they cannot keep up with a child
If they don’t get custody / or just foster – they have no legal status
Sibling Relationships
In some cultures – older girls care for the younger and teach them as part of the social system in some
cultures
Non-industrialised society – larger number of siblings allows work to be done and assist with caring for
old.
Industrialised – few siblings and further apart in age
Changes in sibling relationships most likely to occur when one sibling is between the ages of 7 – 9 years.
Siblings motivated to make up after quarrels if they see each other every day – and learn that expressing
anger does not end a relationship
Same sex siblings – more squabbles
Siblings influence gender development = 1st born is influenced by the parents, the 2nd born is more
like the older sibling
Siblings have a direct influence on each other and indirect influence through impact on each other’s
relationship with parents - parents experience with an older sibling influences their expectations and
treatment of a younger sibling.
Behaviour patterns a child establishes with parents tend to spill over into the child behaviour with
siblings.
Males and females differ in body size, strength and energy therefore natural segregation
Boys need more space and physical exercise by sex
Same sex peer groups help socialise children for future and help children learn gender appropriate
behaviour and incorporate gender roles into self-concept
Popularity = more important in middle childhood
Well-liked by peers = better adjusted
Socio-metric popularity = measured by asking children which peers they like the most and least
Perceived popularity = measured by asking children which children are best liked by their peers
Socio-metrically popular children – good cognitive abilities, high achievers, good at solving social
problems, helpful and assertive without disruptive / aggressive behaviour, kind and trustworthy, loyal,
and self-disclosing and provide emotional support; superior social skills
Perceived popular children – dominant, arrogant, aggressive, tend to be physically attractive and athletic
Authoritative parents tend to have popular children possibly because parents reason with them and try
help them understand how another person might feel
Children of authoritarian parents who punish and threaten are likely to do the same to other children; they
are less popular
Friendship
Individuals form friendship
Learn to communicate and cooperate with friends – helps children feel good about themselves, and cope
with stressful situations
The inevitable quarrels help children resolve conflicts
Peer rejection and no friends = low self-esteem
Children cannot be / have true friends until they achieve cognitive maturity to consider other people’s
views and needs
School age children have 3 – 5 “best” friends
Girls care about having few close friends they can rely on
Boys have more friendships – less intimate and affectionate
Stage 1: One-way assistance Unilateral level – a good friend He’s my friend because he
4 – 9 years does what the child wants them always says yes when I want to
to do. borrow his toy
Stage 2: two-way fair weather Reciprocal level overlaps stage A friend will play with you
cooperation 1, involved give and take but when you don’t have anyone
6 – 12 years still serves many self-interests else to play with
rather than common interest
Stage 3: Intimate, mutually Mutual level friendship has life It takes a long time to make a
shared relationship of it’s own. On-going, close friend, when you do , you
9 – 15 years systematic, committed, doing feel bad if you find out your
things for each other, becomes friend is trying to make other
possessive and demand friends.
exclusivity
Stage 4: Autonomous Interdependent stage, respect Support and give and trust but
interdependence beginning @ friends needs for dependency also let go
12 years and autonomy
MENTAL HEALTH
Resilient children weather adverse circumstances, function well despite challenges / threats or bounces
back from traumatic events
These children simply hold onto basic systems and resources that promote positive development
Protective factors (influences that reduce impact of early stress)
Good family relationship
Cognitive functioning - tend to have high IQ’s – superior information processing skills (good problem
solvers)
Resilient children are likely to have good relationships and strong bonds with at least one
supportive parent or caregiver or other caring competent adult. Their superior info processing
skills may help them cope with adversity, protect themselves, regulate their behaviour, and learn
from experience. They may attract the interest of teachers, who can act as guides, confidants, or
mentors.
Other protective factors cited are:
Temperament / personality: adaptable, friendly, well liked, independent and sensitive to others. They are
competent and have high self-esteem. They are creative, resourceful, independent, and pleasant to be
with. When under stress they can regulate their emotions by shifting attention to something else.
Reduced risk: Children who have been exposed to only one of a number or risk factors for psychiatric
disorder (such as parental discord, low social status, a disturbed mother, a criminal father, and experience
in foster care or an institution) are often better able to overcome stress than children who have been
exposed to more than one risk factor.
CHAPTER FIFTEEN:
PHYSICAL DEVELOPMENT AND HEALTH IN
ADOLESCENCE
GONADARCH
Girls ovaries step up output of estrogen which stimulate growth of female genitals, breast growth and
pubic/underarm hair
In boys – testes increase production of androgens – testosterone – which stimulates growth of male
genitals, muscle mass and body hair
Boys and girls have both types of hormones , but girls have more estrogen and boys more testosterone
(which in girls influences growth of clitoris, bones, pubic, auxiliary hair)
Level of body fat reached for necessary reproduction will dictate rush of hormonal activity
Girls with a higher percentage of body fat in early childhood and who experience unusual weight gain
between 5 – 9 tend to show earlier pubertal development
Leptin – hormone that has a role in over-weight, may trigger onset of puberty by signalling the brain that
sufficient fat has accumulated.
Heightened emotionality and moodiness of early adolescence can be attributed to these hormonal factors,
as well as negative emotions such as distress and hostility as well as symptoms of depression in girls, do
tend to rise as puberty progresses.
Other influences such as sex, age, temperament, and the timing of puberty may moderate or even override
hormonal influences.
The adolescent growth spurt: a rapid increase in height, weight, muscle and bone growth that precedes
sexual maturity
Girls – 9 ½ - 14 ½ (2 years earlier than boys)
Boys – 10 ½ - 16
Lasts approx. 2 years
Girls at 11/12/13 are taller, heavier and stronger than boys of the same age
Girls reach full height at 15 and boys at 17
Fat accumulates twice as rapidly in girls
Girls are generally happier if their timing is about the same as that of their peers.
Early maturing girls tend to be:
Less sociable
Less expressive
Less poised
More introverted and shy
And more negative about menarche
Perhaps because they feel rushed into confronting the pressures of adolescence before they are ready, they
are more vulnerable to psychological distress.
More likely to associate with antisocial peers
May have poor body image and lower self-esteem
They are also at an increased risk of anxiety and depression, disruptive behaviour, eating disorders, early
smoking, drinking and substance abuse, precocious sexual activity, early pregnancy and attempted
suicide.
However this is less true of girls with no history of behaviour problems.
Among both girls and boys, early matures tend to be vulnerable to risky behaviour and the influence of
deviant peers.
Effects of early or late maturation are most likely to be negative when adolescents are much more or less
developed than their peers; when they do not see they changes as advantageous; when several stressful
events such as the advent of puberty and the transition to junior high occur at the same time.
Contextual factors such as ethnicity, school and neighbourhood can make a difference. Also early
maturing girls are more likely to show problem behaviour in mixed gender schools than in all girls
schools and in disadvantaged urban communities than in rural or middle-class communities.
CHAPTER SIXTEEN: COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT IN
ADOLESCENCE
ASPECTS OF COGNITIVE MATURATION
Functional Change
Processes for obtaining, handling, retaining information are functional aspects of cognition – learning,
remembering, reasoning
Most NB functional changes:
Continued increase in processing speed
Further development of executive function which include such skills as:
Selective attention, decision making, inhibitory control of impulsive responses and management of
working memory
MORAL DEVELOPMENT
Adolescent’s tendency toward altruism and empathy increases, they are better able to take another
person’s perspective, to solve social problems, deal with interpersonal relationships, and to see
themselves as social beings – all foster moral development.
Kohlberg later added a Transitional level between II and III – when people no longer feel bound by
societies moral standards, but not yet reasoned out own principles of justice, instead moral decisions
based on personal feelings
The reasoning underlying a person’s response to a moral dilemma, not the answer itself, which indicates
stage of moral development
Before people can develop a fully principled (level III) morality they must realise the relativity of moral
standards. Very few people reach a level where they can choose among differing moral standards.
Kohlberg even went as far as to question the validity of his 6 th stage because so few people attain it. Later
he proposed a seventh, “cosmic” stage where people consider the effect of their actions not only on other
people but on the universe as a whole.
CHAPTER SEVENTEEN:
PSYCHOSOCIAL DEVELOPMENT IN ADOLESCENCE
Adolescents seek to develop a coherent sense of self, including the role he/she has to play in society.
IDENTITY: coherent conception of self made up of goals, values, beliefs to which a person is committed.
Psychosocial moratorium
Time out period provided by adolescence – allow the search for commitments – period necessary to build
a stable, inner direct self.
Resolution of identity crisis results in the virtue of FIDELITY = sustained loyalty, faith, sense of
belonging. Fidelity can also mean identification with a set of values, an ideology, a religion, a political
movement, a creative pursuit or an ethnic group. Fidelity is an extension of trust and commitment.
Erikson saw the prime danger of this stage as identity confusion, which can greatly delay reaching
psychological adulthood. Some degree of confusion is normal. According to Erikson it accounts for the
seemingly chaotic nature of much adolescent behaviour and for teenager’s painful self-consciousness.
Defences against identity confusion = Cliquishness and intolerance of differences
Erikson
Male identity development = the norm
Man not capable of real intimacy until after he achieves a stable identity
Woman define themselves through marriage and motherhood – develop identity through intimacy
Marcia: Identity Status – Crisis and Commitment
James E Marcia distinguished four types of identity statuses, states of ego (self) development. These four
categories differ according to the presence or absence of crisis and commitment.
Identity status = state of ego development that depend on the presence / absence of crisis and
commitment.
Crisis = period of conscious decision making related to identity formation
Commitment = personal investment in an occupation or ideology
SEXUALITY
Warm close relationship with mother likely to delay sexual activity, so are those who perceive that their
mothers disapprove of such behaviour. Another reason given is premarital sex is against religion or
morals or that they don’t want to get/ make a girl pregnant.
Adolescent rebellion = emotional turmoil, conflict within family, reckless behaviour, rejection of adult
values
Only 1 in 5 teenagers fits this pattern
First formal theory – G. Stanley Hall – in an effort to adjust to changing bodies to the imminent demands
of adulthood – ushers in a period of ‘storm and stress’ that produces conflict between the generations
Freud: storm and stress is universal and inevitable, growing out of a resurgence of early sexual drives
towards the parents.
Margaret Mead – when a culture provides serene, gradual transition from childhood to adulthood, storm
and stress not typical.
Full-fledged rebellion now appears to be relatively uncommon even in Western societies –
Most young people feel close to and positive about their parents, share similar opinions on major issues
and value parent’s approval.
34 year long longitudinal study of boys – vast majority adapted well
Deeply troubled adolescents tended to come from disrupted families, and as adults, continued to have
unstable family lives and to reject cultural norms
Those raised in two parent homes with a positive family atmosphere tended to sail through adolescence
with no serious problems as adults, have solid marriages and lead well-adjusted lives
Family conflict, depression and risky behaviour are more common than during other parts of the lifespan
Negative emotionality and mood swings are most intense during early adolescence - and more stable in
later adolescence
Mixed feelings for parents, want children to be independent and find it hard to let go
Parenting styles can influence conflict
Effective parental monitoring depends on how much children let parents know, which in turn depends on
atmosphere at home.
Teenagers lives affected by parents work / marital status / SES
Most arguments between adolescents and parents concern mundane personal matters – chores, school
work, dress, money, curfew dating, friends – rather than issues of health and safety or right and wrong.
The emotional intensity of these conflicts – out of all proportion with the subject matter – may reflect the
underlying processes of Individuation.
Individuation = adolescents struggle for autonomy and differentiation or personal identity
An important issue of individuation is carving out boundaries of control between self and others
Family conflict most frequent in early adolescence and most intense in mid-adolescence, lesser extent of
conflict in late adolescence
Frequency of strife in early adolescence due to strains of puberty, and need to assert autonomy
The reduced frequency in later adolescence signifies adjustment and balance of authority between parent
and child.
Family conflict during adolescence
Authoritative parenting encourages disclosure: warm responsive family environment in which adolescents
were encouraged to speak openly and when parents provided clear expectations without being overly
controlling.
Growing autonomy of teenager and shrinking parental authority redefine the types of behaviour
adolescents are expected to disclose.
Both adolescents and parents saw the following as subject to disclosure:
Prudential behaviour – related to health and safety - smoking, drinking, drugs
Moral issues – lying
Conventional issues – bad manners and swearing
Multifaceted / borderline issues– seeing R rated movies
Personal issues – how time and money is spent – least subject to disclosure
Each type of behaviour parents were more inclined to expect disclosure than adolescents were to do it.
Girls – closer more personal relationship with mother
Adolescents living with continuously married parents have less behaviour problems
Father’s involvement is important (in all family structure – non-resident and living at home)
Changes in marital distress / marital conflict – for the better or worse – predict corresponding changes in
adolescent’s adjustment
Adolescent boys and girls whose parents later divorced showed more academic, psychological and
behavioural problems BEFORE the break up
Adolescents living with continuously married parents tend to have significantly less behavioural problems
than those in any other family structure
Adolescents in cohabiting families – tend to have greater behaviour and emotional problems than those
living in married families
These effects are independent of economic resources, parental well-being, and effectiveness of parenting,
suggesting that parental cohabitation may be more troublesome for adolescents than for younger children
Multi-ethnic study of children of single mothers - no negative effects – what mattered most were the
mother’s education level and ability, family income and the quality of the home environment – suggests
that negative effects of single parent home can be offset by positive factors.
Peer group = source of affection, sympathy, understanding and moral guidance; a place for
experimentation; and a setting for achieving autonomy and independence from parents. It’s a place to
form intimate relationships that serve as rehearsals for adult intimacy.
Influence of peers peaks @ 12/13 years and declines during middle and late adolescence
13 /14 years – try drugs, sneak in without paying – to demonstrate to peers their independence from
parental rules
Risk taking is higher in peer groups than alone – more true for younger participants
Dyadic peer interaction – one to one – in childhood
Larger groupings form in middle childhood
Adolescence – CLIQUES – structured groups of friends who do things together become more important
CROWDS – large groups, based on reputation, image or identity – not personal interaction. Crowd
membership is a social construction, a set of labels by which younger people divide the social map based
on neighbourhood, ethnicity, SES etc.
Friendships
Amount of time spent with friends greatest in adolescence
Friends with similar academic attitudes may influence each other towards pro-social activity or risky
behaviour
Friends influence on binge drinking and sexual activity is fairly weak.
A stress on intimacy, loyalty and sharing marks a transition toward adult-like friendships
Adolescents rely more on friends than parents for intimacy and support
Girls friendships more intimate than boys and they share more confidences
Intimacy with same sex friends increases during early to mid-adolescence, then typically declines
Increased intimacy results from cognitive and emotional development and reflects early adolescents
concern with getting to know themselves
Confiding in a friend helps young people explore their feelings, define their identity, and validate their
self-worth
Adolescents with stable, close, supportive friendships have high opinion of themselves, do well in school,
are sociable and are unlikely to be hostile, anxious or depressed. They also tend to have strong bond with
parents
Bidirectional process – good relationships foster adjustment, which in turn, fosters good friendships
Cliques
Prominent in early adolescence
Consist of young people of the same age, gender and ethnicity
Membership is based on popularity, social status as well as personal affinity
Person can belong to more than one clique, with a shifting or stable membership
Membership in pre-adolescence – high status based – more so with girls
Highest status = leader who chooses members and forms a hierarchy
The higher status cliques are most desirable and most controlled.
The clique structure can seems harsh to outsiders, but it effectively serves the purpose of “redirecting
young people’s priorities from childhood to adolescent social norms. It sends a blunt message as to who
is in charge of the peer social system (peers rather than adults) and provides unequivocal information
about how to proceed within that system”. It also can create emotional distress among those who are less
than successful in negotiating the system.
Crowds
Crow labels – cognitive designations for a feature that members of the crowd have in common
Neighbourhood = west-siders
ethnic background – Italians
Peer status = snobs
Abilities, interests or lifestyle =brains, jocks druggies
Crowds
Help adolescents establish their identity
Reinforce allegiance to the behavioural norms of ethnic or socioeconomic groups
Make it easier to establish relationships with peers in the same crowd
Crowd affiliation tends to become looser as adolescence progresses (as with cliques )
Romantic Relationships
Central part of social world of adolescent
Contribute to development of intimacy and identity
More intense and more intimate across adolescence
Early adolescents think about how romantic relationships may affect status in peer group. They pay little
or no attention to attachment or support needs, such as help, caring, and nurturing, and their attention to
sexual needs is limited to how to engage in sexual activity and which activities to engage in.
Not until late adolescence or early adulthood do romantic relationships begin to meet full gamut of
emotional needs that such relationships can serve and then only in relatively long-term relationships.
Relationships with parents and peers may affect the quality of romantic relationships. The parent’s
marriage or romantic relationship may serve as a model for their adolescent child.
The peer group forms the context for most romantic relationships and may affect an adolescent’s choice
of a partner and the way the relationship develops.
PAGE \* MERGEFORMAT 52