0% found this document useful (0 votes)
125 views9 pages

A Structured Approach To Strategic Decisions - Kahneman Et Al - 2019

The document discusses a structured approach called the Mediating Assessments Protocol (MAP) for reducing errors in strategic decision-making. MAP draws from research showing that structured interviews are more accurate than unstructured ones for evaluating job candidates. Similarly, MAP involves: 1) defining key assessments in advance, 2) independently scoring options on each assessment based on facts, and 3) making a final decision only after all assessments are complete. This structured process helps address cognitive biases and increases transparency, similar to how structured interviews improve on unstructured ones for evaluating job candidates.

Uploaded by

maikelbello
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
125 views9 pages

A Structured Approach To Strategic Decisions - Kahneman Et Al - 2019

The document discusses a structured approach called the Mediating Assessments Protocol (MAP) for reducing errors in strategic decision-making. MAP draws from research showing that structured interviews are more accurate than unstructured ones for evaluating job candidates. Similarly, MAP involves: 1) defining key assessments in advance, 2) independently scoring options on each assessment based on facts, and 3) making a final decision only after all assessments are complete. This structured process helps address cognitive biases and increases transparency, similar to how structured interviews improve on unstructured ones for evaluating job candidates.

Uploaded by

maikelbello
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 9

A Structured Approach to Strategic Decisions

Reducing errors in judgment requires a disciplined process.


BY DANIEL KAHNEMAN, DAN LOVALLO, AND OLIVIER SIBONY

Envision the following situations: A board of directors considers acquiring a competitor. A marketing team
decides whether to launch a new product. A venture capital investment committee chooses among an array
of startups to fund.
All those strategic decisions share a common feature: They are evaluative judgments. To make such tough
calls, people must boil down a large amount of complex information to either
(1) numerical scores for competing options or (2) a yes-no decision on whether to choose a specific path. Of
course, some management decisions are made without weighing quite so much information. But strategic
decisions tend to involve the distillation of complexity into a single path forward.
Given how unreliable human judgment is, all evaluations are susceptible to errors. These errors can stem from
known cognitive biases — or they can be random errors, sometimes called “noise.” Unreliability in judgment
has long been recognized and studied, particularly in the context of decision-making about hiring. We draw
inspiration from that body of research and experience to suggest a practical, broadly applicable approach to
reducing errors in strategic decision-making. We call it the Mediating Assessments Protocol (MAP), and we’ll
describe it here, after discussing the underpinning research.

Strategic Options Are Like Job Candidates


The body of research on the job interview, the most common tool for employee selection, contains a wealth
of information about the accuracy of evaluative judgments.1 Most companies still use traditional
(unstructured) interviews to make a global evaluation. The interviewer starts with an open mind, accumulates
information about the candidate, and then reaches a conclusion.

Unfortunately, a vast amount of evidence indicates that unstructured interviews lead to biased evaluations
that have very little predictive value.2 That’s because the interviewer forms a mental model (colloquially
known as an “impression”) of a candidate, a process that psychologists have shown has three specific
limitations:
1. Excessive coherence. Mental models are usually simpler and more coherent than the reality they aim
to assess. As interviewers, if we assume, for instance, that a particular candidate is an extrovert, we tend to
ask questions that confirm this hypothesis.
2. A “quick and sticky” quality. We form our mental models rapidly, often on the basis of limited
evidence at the start of the process, and we alter our models slowly as new facts emerge. That explains why,
as common sense would suggest (and research has confirmed3), first impressions have a disproportionate
effect on the assessments we make of people in general and on the outcome of job interviews.4
3. Biased weighting. Our mental models often don’t give each pertinent fact the weight it deserves. We
may discount important bits of information or, in contrast, give great weight to factors that should be entirely
irrelevant. For example, an interviewer may wrongly perceive that a male candidate has great leadership
qualities just because he is tall and has a deep voice.
Such challenges in hiring are easy to recognize. For that reason, we do not expect all interviewers to agree on
one candidate — and we often compensate by averaging several interviewers’ viewpoints.
What many people do not fully appreciate is that we use the same process of mental model formation in
strategic decision-making, with similar
limitations and results.5
Suppose, for instance, that you’re selecting a location for a new plant. You must assess multiple factors: labor
costs, technical feasibility, regulatory requirements, political stability in various regions, and so on. You
already have a mental image of the candidate countries and cities. As you learn new facts about each
prospective site, the bias toward excessive coherence leads you to confirm that image, which is likely to be
much less nuanced and less ambiguous than the reality.
This bias may be even stronger if a team is collaborating on the decision:6 As a favorite plant site emerges,
its perceived benefits are exaggerated and its perceived costs underestimated. Then, when the team
reviews the merits of possible locations, early impressions formed in the first minutes of the discussion are
likely to weigh heavily on the final decision. Once an impression is formed, you will tend to ask leading
questions that support your early views. Because of confirmation bias,7 you will interpret ambiguous facts in
light of preexisting attitudes. It takes a considerable amount of evidence to reverse an erroneous initial
judgment.
Finally, just as a candidate’s physical appearance may influence a hiring decision, certain attributes of a
manufacturing site may carry undue weight. Predictably, extra weight is often given to recent and salient
information (a case of “availability bias”).8 For example, you may overreact to recent news of political turmoil
or overemphasize shortterm considerations. And given that most of us tend to be optimistic and
overconfident in our forecasts, you may underestimate technical challenges in constructing the new plant.
In short, unstructured decision-making, whether in job interviews or in other, more strategic decisions, is
vulnerable to both bias and “noise.” The presence of noise explains why researchers find large variations
whenever they systematically investigate the reliability of judgment.9

What We Can Learn From Structured Interviewing


Dozens of studies on personnel selection have shown conclusively that decisions are more accurate when
interviews are structured rather than unstructured.10 Therefore, a growing number of organizations,
especially those that put a high premium on the quality of the talent they hire, have adopted structured
interviews.
An early form of structured interviewing was developed in 1956 by Daniel Kahneman while he served in the
Israeli Army, where he observed that holistic ratings given by interviewers were poor predictors of the future
success of recruits. He replaced these ratings with separate scores on six attributes: sense of duty, sociability,
energy level, punctuality, capacity for independent thought, and what was then called “masculine pride.” A
simple average of these scores proved to predict overall performance more accurately than did an intuitive
evaluation based on an unstructured interview. An intuitive prediction made after these separate structured-
interview ratings were assigned was also useful; the combination of the two was the best performance
predictor of all.
The significance of this finding, which Kahneman reiterated many years later,11 is that the accuracy of intuitive
judgment is much improved by delaying a global evaluation until the end of a structured process. The Israeli
Army still uses structured interviewing today, as do companies such as Google, Amazon, and McKinsey.12
In a structured interview, the interviewer must rate several key traits before making a final evaluation. Scores
on each attribute serve as mediating assessments: intermediary ratings, produced in a predetermined,
standardized manner in order to be as fact-based as possible. The final evaluations are then derived from
these ratings.
Surprisingly, although structured interviews are becoming increasingly common in hiring, the type of
structured decision-making they exemplify is rare in other contexts. Strategic decisions typically involve a
holistic evaluation of all available information, without explicitly scoring key attributes. We think structure
and mediating assessments can improve the quality of those decisions, as well.

Core Elements of
Structured Decisions
MAP is a structured approach to grounding strategic decisions, like structured interviews, on mediating
assessments. MAP has three core elements:
• Define the assessments in advance. The decision maker must identify a handful of mediating
assessments, key attributes that are critical to the evaluation. In the decision to acquire a company, for
example, the assessments could include anticipated revenue synergies or qualifications of the management
team. This process is similar to one a hiring committee would follow when creating a job description that
outlines attributes required for success in the position.
• Use fact-based, independently made assessments. People who weigh in on one aspect of a strategic
option should not be influenced by one another — or by other dimensions of the option. Their opinions should
be grounded in the evidence available. This approach is comparable to a wellorganized structured-interview
process, in which job seekers are scored on each key attribute solely on the basis of their answers to relevant
questions, calibrated using predefined scales.
• Make the final evaluation when the mediating assessments are complete. Unless a deal- breaker fact
is uncovered (for instance, evidence of accounting fraud at the acquisition target), the final decision should
be discussed only when all key attributes have been scored and a complete profile of assessments is available.
This is similar to having a hiring committee review all the evaluations made by each interviewer on each key
requirement of the job description before making a decision on a candidate.
The use of mediating assessments reduces variability in decision-making because it seeks to address the
limitations of mental model formation, even though it cannot eliminate them entirely. By delineating the
assessments clearly and in a factbased, independent manner, and delaying final judgment until all
assessments are finished, MAP tempers the effects of bias and increases the transparency of the process, as
all the assessments are presented at one time to all decision makers. For example, because salient or recent
pieces of information are not given undue weight, the process preempts the availability bias. MAP also
reduces the risk that a solution will be judged by its similarity with known categories or stereotypes (an error
arising from the representativeness bias13). When differentiated, independent facts are clearly laid out, logical
errors are less likely.
Some decision makers will have an initial dislike for MAP, just as many recruiters still resist structured
interviewing. The requirements may appear mechanical, and the limits it places on the role of intuition will
not appeal to leaders who have been rewarded for “trusting their gut.” Structured decision- making, based
on mediating assessments, will be adopted only if it is viewed as offering a substantial improvement in
decision-making quality.
Accordingly, we next examine MAP’s application and benefits in two types of strategy decisions: large one-
off decisions made by teams of executives or directors, and recurrent decisions made as part of formalized
processes that, in aggregate, shape a company’s strategy.

Structuring One-Off Strategic Decisions


Let’s return to the example of a large company that is contemplating a major acquisition. A deal team,
assembled to perform due diligence, begins by identifying key issues — revenue and cost synergies, hidden
liabilities, cultural compatibility, and so on. It prepares a report to the board, with chapters that cover each
issue. In the subsequent board meeting, the chair leads a thorough discussion in which members state their
view of the merger and explain their reasoning. Questions are asked and answered, and the concerns that
emerge are discussed in depth. Eventually, a vote is taken.
This approach is thorough and professional and seems unobjectionable. However, it resembles unstructured
interviewing in a critical respect: The conclusion is drawn directly from a global image of the case, an image
that emerges spontaneously and gradually as information is considered. In contrast, structured decision-
making requires leaders to make separate, explicit assessments of each aspect and to use those assessments
as the basis for a decision. In the case of the possible acquisition, it would proceed as follows:
• Due diligence is completed as usual. If the deal team has done a good job, the key issues it has
identified correspond to the required mediating assessments. The only novelty is that the deal team is
required to conclude each chapter of its report with a summary rating. Each rating answers the question:
“Leaving aside how important this topic is to the overall decision, how strongly does the evidence in this
chapter argue for or against the deal?” Like the scoring of specific traits in a structured interview, each of
these ratings should be based solely on the facts contained in the relevant chapter, not on data gathered
about other topics.
• The agenda of the board meeting follows the chapters of the report. Topics are reviewed in sequence
(unless a deal-breaker fact emerges that ends the discussion). Drawing on the data supplied in each chapter,
the board members, first individually and then as a group, consider whether to accept or amend the deal
team’s proposed rating on each assessment. In a hiring decision, the corresponding step would be for the
hiring committee to review interviewers’ scores on each requirement of the job description.
• Board members are asked to refrain from making general comments about the acquisition until the
mediating assessments are complete. At that point, ratings on all assessments are displayed. Only then does
the discussion turn to the holistic evaluation of the deal. Eventually, a vote is taken.
As this example illustrates, the implementation of MAP does not substantially increase the overall burden of
deciding. Trivial extra effort is required from the deal team to generate a summary assessment on each due
diligence topic. A board discussion that is structured around mediating assessments will be more organized
and focused than the usual process, but not necessarily longer or more contentious.
At this moderate cost, the rigor of formal structure in strategic decision-making has the benefit of sequencing
the process such that important facts are less likely to be overlooked and thoughtful, selfcritical consideration
of trade-offs is much more likely to occur. In this way, using MAP triggers conscious reflection. In typical
unstructured decision- making, by contrast, we (often unconsciously) weigh losses more than gains, the near
future more than the distant future, and vividly presented anecdotes more
than dull statistics.14
For instance, in the sequence of presentations and review meetings that lead up to an acquisition, the topics
discussed typically change as new information becomes available. As urgency mounts and enthusiasm about
the one-off deal builds, serious issues may be ignored or relegated, say, to an appendix of a presentation
document. Meanwhile, the target’s most appealing features gain increasing prominence, and the narrative
rationale for the deal gets more and more compelling. It is easy to see how these dynamics can lead to
mistakes in judgment. Using a structured decision-making process ensures that the checklist of key questions
is defined in advance and that, at the time of final decision, all its elements are given sufficient visibility. This
rigor limits the risk that a compelling narrative will sway the board.
Our advice is not to assign explicit weights to assessments, as some decision theorists would suggest.
Executives reject that type of framework because it mechanizes decisions and removes room for intuition,
including the possible interactions among the criteria. Unlike algorithmic decisionmaking, which aims to take
subjectivity out of the decision entirely, MAP values intuition, provided that it is informed. The holistic
judgment of experienced executives is valuable, but it must first be prepared by a profile of mediating
assessments.
Nor do we advise treating all assessments equally. Rather, some dimensions that are simply more important
than others should be framed as make-or-break assessments and evaluated first. For example, when
evaluating a new technology, considerations such as time to market and cost are important, but the
assessment of whether the technology will work at all is paramount.

Structuring Recurrent Decisions


Recurrent decisions, in aggregate, also produce strategic outcomes. Consider, for instance, launching new
products in the fast-moving consumer-goods market, advancing products along an R&D pipeline in
pharmaceuticals, or making small acquisitions in a broader roll-up strategy. Large organizations make
countless decisions like these, and their collective impact on the business can be critical.15
MAP provides the same rigor and discipline to recurrent decisions as to one-off choices. In addition, when
decisions of a particular type are repeated, a structured approach facilitates learning. Explicit mediating
assessments make it easy to conduct a postmortem of past decisions, examine the reasoning and use of facts,
learn which assessments may have been misguided, and revise the assessments or the scales accordingly. The
quality of judgments is likely to improve further if the assessments and the evaluation are expressed on a
relative scale. (See “Using Percentiles as a Rating Scale,” p. 72.)
MAP’s most important contribution to routine decisions is in providing true standardization. People who have
the same role in an organization and make similar decisions are assumed to be interchangeable, but
companies do not normally check whether this assumption is correct. However, when organizations test it
through a “noise audit,” evaluations often differ by 50% or more between supposedly interchangeable
professionals (including highly experienced ones).16 This level of noise, which is ordinarily invisible, is
obviously unacceptable.
To better understand how to structure recurrent decisions, consider the case of a venture capital firm we will
call VC Co., which has redesigned its investment process. Like many investors, VC Co. followed a thorough
evaluation protocol, focusing its attention in every case on the specifics that appeared most relevant to form
a mental model of the investment target. To improve the quality of its decisions, the firm decided to adopt a
structured decision approach. In doing so, VC Co. incorporated the key features of MAP.
First, to predefine the critical assessments, VC Co. engaged in a thorough discussion of the following question:
“What are the key factors we should consider when making investment decisions?” The output was a list of
criteria that would not necessarily be identical for another venture capitalist, because they reflected the
investment philosophy of this firm. For example, one of these criteria is a judgment on the target company’s
founding team. This judgment, in turn, breaks down into specific subassessments such as problem-solving
skills, technical expertise, resilience, and open-mindedness. Such qualities can be assessed on the basis of
interactions with the team, its track record, and reference checks.
Second, VC Co. now ensures that its assessments are made separately, do not influence one another, and are
summarized in a percentile-score rating. Organizing the data collection to make assessments independent of
one another (to the extent possible) minimizes the halo effect. For instance, having seen an impressive
product demo, VC Co.’s investment professionals may be tempted to rate the management team’s skills
favorably. MAP guides them to treat these as separate judgments.
Assigning a numerical rating to each assessment safeguards against the tendency to form overly coherent
mental models. For instance, when VC Co. formulated its assessment of a target company’s top team
qualitatively, the wording was sometimes ambiguous: Depending on the mental model that had emerged
from prior assessments, a word such as “strong” could be interpreted as either adding to a positive impression
or expressing doubt. With a numerical rating, this risk is reduced.
Using percentile scores to make qualitative assessments puts recurrent decisions into context. To evaluate
the caliber of a target company’s founders, for instance, VC Co. now uses a “slate” showing the names of the
dozens of founders to whom it has had exposure. Instead of asking whether a founder is “an A, a B, or a C,”
investment committee members ask: “On this particular subassessment — say, technical skills — is this person
in the same league as Anna and the other five founders we regard as the very best on this dimension? Or is
she comparable to Bob and the other entrepreneurs we view as belonging to the second quartile?” VC Co.
applies the same comparative approach to other qualitative assessments, such as the potential defensibility
of a company’s competitive advantage.
Third, the company has formalized conduct of its decision meetings. Just as in one-off decisions, the meeting
agenda follows the evaluation structure: VC Co. reviews each assessment separately before coming to a final
decision. Summary narratives that encapsulate a rationale for the proposed investment are banned until all
assessments have been reviewed one at a time and a profile of the ratings is displayed. A major benefit of
applying MAP to a recurrent decision is that investment committee members have multiple chances to
practice this new discipline and can continuously improve their skills and tools. For instance, each new
evaluation of a founder adds a name to the slate, making comparisons with future candidates richer.

VC Co.’s early experience using MAP did not slow down the firm’s decision-making. On the contrary, members
of the firm have told us that the orderly flow of work in the new protocol actually saves time, and they have
pointed to several investments where they believe it has led them to a distinctly better decision. Quantitative
effects will take years to assess.

Using Percentiles as a Rating Scale

How should decision makers express their assessments? Standard rating scales with verbal anchors (“very
good,” “good,” and so on) have the advantage of simplicity. However, the ambiguity of verbal labels is a major
source of noise, because different people use different words to convey the same underlying judgment.

Percentile scales offer a possible solution. Many leading companies already use them to express judgments
about the performance of employees on multiple dimensions. For example: “Jenny is in the top 10% of the
population of junior executives for

raw intellect, but in the third quartile for interpersonal skills.” We recommend extending the use of percentile
scales to assessments in other domains. When evaluating the quality of a target company’s go-to-market
capabilities, for instance, a venture capital firm can ask: “How does this company compare with other
companies in the same sector? Is it in the top 10%? In the top 25%?” The percentile scale requires a specified
frame of reference, understood and shared by all. In this example, “all the potential targets we have
evaluated” would be the most appropriate reference class.

Percentile scales have several advantages over other types of rating scales. First, they require the evaluator
to bring comparable cases to mind and to think of the case at hand as one particular instance of a broader
category. This approach, which has been called the outside view, is a powerful debiasing technique by itself.i

Second, percentile scales allow individual biases to be detected and corrected. For example, an overly lenient
or optimistic person who rates 40% of cases as belonging to the top 10% will eventually be identified and
trained to use the scale appropriately. When people

have learned to use a percentile scale well, they are said to be calibrated. Improving calibration is another
major step in reducing noise in judgments.
Third, percentile ratings can easily be translated into policy. If underwriters, for instance, rate risks in
percentiles, premiums can be priced on the basis of their ratings, and the company can decide at what
percentile in the distribution of risks it sets the limit of what it is willing to underwrite.

Percentile scales can be challenging to define, introduce, and administer. But the accuracy gains they bring
are worth the effort.

Whatever else it produces, any organization is a decision factory. Some of its decisions are made by people
following clear rules. But many of the decisions that shape the future of organizations require time-consuming
deliberation, analysis, and the balancing of multiple considerations. Such decisions cannot easily be “quality
checked.” To improve them, we must work on the processes by which they are made.
MAP is one way of doing so. By adding discipline to decision-making and limiting some well-known flaws, it
brings quality assurance to complex decisions. While other decision support approaches, such as decision
theory or advanced analytical models, share the same objective, MAP has some advantages. It is easy to learn,
involves a minimal amount of additional work, and leaves senior executives some freedom to exercise
intuitive judgment, albeit after a useful delay. As such, it should be a valuable tool for any leader who aims to
raise the quality of an organization’s decisions.
REFERENCES
1. J. Levashina, C.J. Hartwell, F.P. Morgeson, and M.A. Campion, “The Structured Employment
Interview: Narrative and Quantitative Review of the Research Literature,” Personnel Psychology 67, no. 1
(February 2014): 241-293.
2. J. Dana, R. Dawes, and N. Peterson, “Belief in the
Unstructured Interview: The Persistence of an Illusion,” Judgment and Decision-Making 8, no. 5 (September
2013): 512-520; and D.A. Moore, “How to Improve the Accuracy and Reduce the Cost of Personnel
Selection,” California Management Review 60, no. 1 (November 2017): 8-17.
3. M.R. Barrick, S.L. Dustin, T.L. Giluk, G.L. Stewart, et al., “Candidate Characteristics Driving Initial
Impressions
During Rapport Building: Implications for Employment Interview Validity,” Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology 85, no. 2 (June 2012): 330-352.
4. C.Y. Olivola and A. Todorov, “Fooled by First Impressions? Reexamining the Diagnostic Value of
Appearance- Based Inferences,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46, no. 2 (March 2010): 315-324.
5. D. Kahneman, Thinking, Fast and Slow (New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2011).
6. C.R. Sunstein and R. Hastie, Wiser: Getting Beyond Groupthink to Make Groups Smarter (Boston:
Harvard Business Review Press, 2015).
7. R.S. Nickerson, “Confirmation Bias: A Ubiquitous Phenomenon in Many Guises,” Review of General
Psychology 2, no. 2 (June 1998): 175-220.
8. A. Tversky and D. Kahneman, “Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases,” Science 185,
no. 4157 (Sept. 27, 1974): 1,124-1,131.
9. D. Kahneman, A.M. Rosenfield, L. Gandhi, and T. Blaser, “Noise: How to Overcome the High, Hidden
Cost of Inconsistent Decision-Making,” Harvard Business Review 94, no. 10 (October 2016): 36-43.
10. Levashina et al., “The Structured Employment Interview.”
11. Kahneman, Thinking, Fast and Slow.
12. L. Bock, Work Rules! Insights From Inside Google That Will Transform How You Live and Lead
(London: Hachette U.K., 2015).
13. Tversky and Kahneman, “Judgment Under Uncertainty.”
14. Kahneman, Thinking, Fast and Slow.
15. O. Sibony, D. Lovallo, and T.C. Powell, “Behavioral Strategy and the Strategic Decision Architecture of
the Firm,” California Management Review 59, no. 3 (May 2017): 5-21.
16. Kahneman et al., “Noise.”
i. D. Lovallo and D. Kahneman, “Delusions of Success: How Optimism Undermines Executives’ Decisions,”
Harvard Business Review 81, no. 7 (July 2003): 56-63.

You might also like