Language Acquistion
Language Acquistion
1. Context:
Community College Child Growth and Development Course
• Infant –Toddler Development
• Language and Literacy Development
2. Goals /Objectives
Learners will:
a. Understand the developmental theories behind language acquisition
b. Describe the basic elements of speech and language development in early childhood
c. Exam the precursors of language infants and toddler develop before they are able to
speak
d. Observe infants and toddler to integrate their understanding of prelinguistic
development in infants and toddler
e. Utilize the CA Infant Toddler Learning Foundations to guide their observations and
apply their understanding of language acquisition in young children [ Research to
Practice]
3. Key Points:
• Language acquisition is the process by which humans acquire the capacity to perceive,
produce and use words to understand and communicate.
• How is Language Acquired? Nature vs Nurture OR Both…It is the intersection where
nature meets nurture
• Research has demonstrated the importance of quality child care in the cognitive and
language development of young children
• The development of language (communication) is uniquely tied to all domains of
development, including sensory (vision, hearing, tactile), motor, cognition, and
social/emotional
• Understanding the infant precursor developmental skills to communication, language and
speech will help guide our observations of infant, toddlers and their caregivers
4. Contents
• There are four major theories of language development.
o The behaviorist theory, proposed by B. F. Skinner suggests that language is
learned through operant conditioning (reinforcement and imitation). This
perspective sides with the nurture side of the nature-nurture debate.
o The nativist theory, proposed by Noam Chomsky, argues that language is a
unique human accomplishment. Chomsky says that all children have what is
called an LAD, an innate language acquisition device that allows children to
produce consistent sentences once vocabulary is learned.
o The empiricist theory suggests, contra Chomsky, that there is enough information
in the linguistic input that children receive, and therefore there is no need to
assume an innate language acquisition device (see above).
o The interactionist perspective, consists of two components. This perspective is a
combination of both the nativist and behaviorist theories. The first part, the
information-processing theories, tests through the connectionist model, using
statistics. From these theories, we see that the brain is excellent at detecting
patterns. The second part of the interactionist perspective, is the social-
interactionist theories. These theories suggest that there is a native desire to
understand others as well as being understood by others.
1
• OTHERS: How do other theories of development play into language acquisition:
o Piaget:
Object permanence - related to naming performance
Causality - requirement for communication, and related to verb
comprehension
Deferred imitation - related to naming performance
o Vygotsky:
His interest in language and literature blended with his interest in
psychology and lead him to theorize about language/ cognitive
development in children. He hypothesized that development of inner
speech in children developed in the same manner as all other mental
processes. His theory can be used as a map of cognitive development in
children. Vygotsky’s theory leads one to believe that cognitive
development takes place out of a need to communicate our needs to
others.
2
o Infants begin to understand language long before they can talk. At birth they can
distinguish speech from other kinds of sounds and recognize the voices of
significant adults. In the second year of life they understand word and phrase.
o Early in life, babies communicate through crying, making noises, smiling,
gesturing, and pointing.
o Near age 1, babies can speak words. Vocabulary grows exceedingly quickly
during the first 2 years of life. The first words babies learn are often names of
things they can act upon, or words that have social meaning.
5. Key Terminology:
o Cognition - Thinking skills that include perception, memory, awareness, reasoning,
judgment, intellect, and imagination
o Language - System for communicating ideas and feelings using sounds, gestures,
signs, or marks
o Language acquisition is the process by which humans acquire the capacity to
perceive, produce and use words to understand and communicate.
o Speech - Making definite vocal sounds that form words to express thoughts and
ideas
Others:
o Language Acquisition Device (LAD): Proposed biologically-based mental
structure that theorists believe plays a major role in children's language learning.
Linguist Noam Chomsky revolutionized the idea that an infant's innate ability to
understand a language structurally, before actually being able to speak it, allows
for the possibility that children can learn any language intuitively before a certain
age.
o Phonology involves the rules about the structure and sequence of speech
sounds.
o Semantics consists of vocabulary and how concepts are expressed through
words.
o Grammar involves two parts. The first, syntax, is the rules in which words are
arranged into sentences. The second, morphology, is the use of grammatical
markers (indicating tense, active or passive voice etc.).
o Pragmatics involves the rules for appropriate and effective communication.
Pragmatics involves three skills:
o using language for greeting, demanding etc.
o changing language for talking differently depending on who it is you are
talking to
o following rules such as turn taking, staying on topic
Preverbal infants communicate through eye contact, facial expressions, gestures, and
sounds. Understanding language precedes speaking it (Bloom and others 1996). In
addition, before being able to use language effectively, infants acquire some
3
understanding of the social processes involved in communication. They learn about the
social aspects of communication through engaging in turn-taking behavior in proto-
conversations with their parents or infant care teachers. In proto-conversations, the adult
usually says something to the preverbal infant, and the infant responds by making eye
contact, cooing, smiling, showing lip and tongue movements, or waving arms. These
“conversation-like” conversations go back and forth between the adult and the infant for
several turns.
There is broad variability in language development in its pattern and pace (Bloom and
Capatides 1987). However, the process of early language development is fundamentally
the same across cultures and languages. In describing early language development, Kuhl
(2002, 115) states: “One of the puzzles in language development is to explain the orderly
transition that all infants go through during development. Infants the world over achieve
certain milestones in linguistic development at roughly the same time, regardless of the
language they are exposed to.”
Infants are learning about the prosodic or sound characteristics of their native language:
by nine months of age, English-speaking infants demonstrate a preference for the sound
stress pattern characteristic of words in the English language (Jusczyk, Cutler, and
Redanz 1993). Kuhl (2002, 112) concludes: “At age one—prior to the time infants begin
to master higher levels of language, such as sound-meaning correspondences,
contrastive phonology, and grammatical rules—infants’ perceptual and perceptual-motor
systems have been altered by linguistic experience. Phonetic perception has changed
dramatically to conform to the native-language pattern, and language-specific speech
production has emerged.”
Receptive Language
Infants excel at detecting patterns in spoken language (Kuhl 2000). The literature
indicates that infants’ speech perception abilities are strong. Not only do infants
understand more vocabulary than they are able to produce, but they also demonstrate
awareness of the properties of the language or languages they are exposed to before
they acquire words (Ingram 1999). During the first six months of life, infants are better
than adults at perceiving various types of contrasts in speech (Plunkett and Schafer
1999). Infants improve in their ability to discriminate the sounds characteristic of their
native language while losing their abilities to discriminate some sounds characteristic of
languages other than their native language (Cheour and others 1998). According to Kuhl
(2004), the way in which the infant’s brain processes repeated experiences with speech
explains language acquisition in a social and biological context. According to this view,
from early infancy young children use a mental filter to orient, with greater efficiency and
accuracy, to the speech sounds characteristic of their native language. This strategy
enables infants to identify the phonemic units most useful to them in their native language
and serves as a building block to later word acquisition (Kuhl 2004).
Expressive Language
Infants use their expressive language skills to make sounds or use gestures or speech to
begin to communicate. Even preverbal infants use vocalizing or babbling to express
themselves. They also imitate the sounds and rhythm of adult speech. As they develop,
infants generate increasingly understandable sounds or verbal communication. They
demonstrate their expressive language abilities by asking questions and responding to
them and repeating of sounds or rhymes. Children typically acquire their first 50 words
between the ages of one and two (Ingram 1999). Kuczaj (1999, 145) notes: “The 24-
4
month-old child with a productive vocabulary between 50 and 600 words will easily
quadruple or quintuple her vocabulary in the next year, and then add between 3000 and
4000 words per year to her productive vocabulary until she graduates from high school.”
Interest in Print
Infants show an interest in print at first through physically exploring, such as putting
books in their mouths, handling books, or focusing on print in the environment around
them. Turning the pages of books, looking at books or pictures, asking for a favorite book
or telling a favorite story with an adult are other indicators of interest in print. As infants
grow older, making intentional marks on paper with a crayon or marker, pretending to
read and write, repeating stories, repeating rhymes, recognizing images in books,
noticing common symbols and words, and enjoying books are all related to interest in
print. Interest in print can be considered one aspect of emergent literacy, the idea that
literacy develops from early childhood rather than something that becomes relevant only
upon school entry (Whitehurst and Lonigan 1998). Because early experiences with print
contribute to later literacy, shared book reading is recommended as a valuable way to
promote emergent literacy (Whitehurst and Lonigan 1998).
5
Zero to Three: Tips on How Infants Learn to Talk
http://www.zerotothree.org/child-development/early-language-literacy/helping-learn-
to-talk.html
8. Evaluation procedures
a. Match the Behavior to the Developmental Skill Using the Infant/Toddler Desired
Results Profile, or other Assessment Tool (ie: Age and Stages) Cross-match
assignment
b. Midterm and/or Final Questions
c. Toddler Language Observation Assignment
Describe the toddler's ability to perceive sensory stimuli. What language skills does
this toddler possess? What words does he use to communicate his needs? Give
examples. How does he respond to commands and directions? What gestures does
he use? Describe two of them. Is he using single holo-phrases or telegraphic
speech? Give examples. Describe how he imitates sounds, movements, and words
that he hears or sees. What about simple games like "peek a boo." Does he
understand simple directions? Give 2 examples for each category. Make sure you
describe the setting in which the toddler used his/her words to communicate to
another person.
9. Resources
• Books:
o Beyond Baby Talk: From Sounds to Sentences, A Parent's Complete Guide
to Language Development by Kenn Apel Ph.D., Julie Masterson Ph.D.
o Early Communication Skills for Children with Down Syndrome: A Guide for
Parents and Professionals by Libby Kumin, Ph.D., CCC-SLP
o How Babies Talk: The Magic and Mystery of Language in the First Three
Years of Life, by R. M. Golinkoff Ph.D., and K. Hirsh-Pasek
o Nobody Ever Told Me (or my Mother) That: Everything from Bottles and
Breathing to Health Speech Development by Diane Bahr, MS, CCC-SLP
o Talk to Me, Baby”: How You Can Support Young Children’s Language
Development, by Betty. S. Bardige
o What’s Going On In There?: How the Brain and Mind Develop in the First
Five Years of Life by Lise Eliot, Ph.D.
o The Scientist In the Crib: What Early Learning Tells Us About the Mind by A.
Gopnik, A. Meltzoff, P. Kuhl
• Internet Sites:
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Language_acquisition
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Language_development
http://www.nichd.nih.gov/health/topics/seccyd.cfm
http://www.cde.ca.gov/sp/cd/re/itf09langdev.asp
http://speech-language-therapy.com
http://www.urmc.rochester.edu/speech-pathology/glossary.cfm
http://www.sk.com.br/sk-vygot.html
6
California Infant/Toddler Learning & Development Foundations.
7
• Quiet
down
when
gesture
to
match
the
own
nose
when
the
hearing
the
infant
care
verbal
request,
such
as
infant
care
teacher
says,
teacher’s
voice.
Turn
pointing
to
the
blanket
“Where’s
your
nose?”
toward
the
window
when
asking
the
child
and
then
touching
the
when
hearing
a
fire
to
get
it.
infant
care
teacher’s
truck
drive
by.
• Look
up
and
nose
when
he
says,
• Quiet
down
and
focus
momentarily
stop
“And
where’s
my
nose?”
on
the
infant
care
reaching
into
the
• Follow
two-‐step
teacher
as
he
talks
to
mother’s
purse
when
requests
about
the
child
during
a
she
says
“no
no.”
unrelated
events,
such
diaper
change.
• Show
understanding
of
as,
“Put
the
blocks
away
• Look
at
or
turn
toward
the
names
for
most
and
then
go
pick
out
a
the
infant
care
teacher
familiar
objects
and
book.”
who
says
the
child’s
people.
• Answer
adults’
name.
questions;
for
example,
communicate
“apple”
when
a
parent
asks
what
the
child
had
for
snack.
Foundation: Expressive Language
The developing ability to produce the sounds of language and use vocabulary
and increasingly complex utterances
8
months
18
months
36
months
At
around
eight
months
of
age,
At
around
18
months
of
age,
At
around
36
months
of
age,
children
experiment
with
sounds,
children
say
a
few
words
and
children
communicate
in
a
way
practice
making
sounds,
and
use
use
conventional
gestures
to
tell
that
is
understandable
to
most
sounds
or
gestures
to
others
about
their
needs,
wants,
adults
who
speak
the
same
communicate
needs,
wants,
or
and
interests.
language
they
do.
Children
interests.
combine
words
into
simple
sentences
and
demonstrate
the
ability
to
follow
some
grammatical
rules
of
the
home
language.
For
example,
the
child
may:
For
example,
the
child
may:
For
example,
the
child
may:
• Vocalize
to
get
the
• Look
at
a
plate
of
• Use
the
past
tense,
infant
care
teacher’s
crackers,
then
at
the
though
not
always
attention.
infant
care
teacher,
correctly;
for
example,
• Repeat
sounds
when
and
communicate
“Daddy
goed
to
work,”
babbling,
such
as
“da
da
“more.”
“She
falled
down.”
da
da”
or
“ba
ba
ba
ba.”
• Point
t o
a n
a irplane
i n
• Use
the
possessive,
• Wave
to
the
infant
care
the
sky
and
look
at
the
though
not
always
teacher
when
he
waves
infant
care
teacher.
correctly;
for
example,
and
says,
“bye-‐bye”
as
• Use
the
same
word
to
“That’s
you
car”
or
“Her
he
leaves
for
his
break.
refer
to
similar
things,
Megan.”
• Lift
arms
to
the
infant
such
as
“milk”
while
• Use
a
few
prepositions,
care
teacher
to
indicating
the
pitcher,
such
as
“on”
the
table.
communicate
a
desire
to
even
though
it
is
filled
• Talk
about
what
she
will
be
held.
with
juice.
do
in
the
future,
such
as
• Use
two
words
“I
gonna
get
a
kitty.”
together,
such
as
• Use
300–1000
words.
“Daddy
give.”
• Use
the
plural
form
of
• Shake
head
“no”
when
nouns,
though
not
offered
more
food.
always
correctly;
for
• Jabber
a
string
of
example,
“mans,”
and
8
sounds
into
the
toy
“mouses.”
telephone.
• Express,
“Uncle
is
• Gesture
“all
gone”
by
coming
to
pick
me
up.”
twisting
wrists
to
turn
hands
up
and
down
when
finished
eating
lunch.
• Use
made-‐up
“words”
to
refer
to
objects
or
experiences
that
only
familiar
adults
will
know
the
meaning
of;
for
example
“wo-‐wo”
when
wanting
to
go
next
door
to
visit
the
puppy.
Behaviors
leading
up
to
the
Behaviors
leading
up
to
the
Behaviors
leading
up
to
the
foundation
(4
to
7
months)
foundation
(9
to
17
months)
foundation
(19
to
35
months)
During
this
period,
the
child
may:
During
this
period,
the
child
During
this
period,
the
child
may:
• Squeal
when
excited.
may:
• Tend
to
communicate
• Make
an
angry
noise
• Babble
using
the
about
objects,
actions,
when
another
child
sounds
of
his
home
and
events
that
are
in
takes
a
toy.
language.
the
here
and
now.
• Make
a
face
of
disgust
• Consistently
use
• Use
some
words
to
refer
to
tell
the
infant
care
utterances
to
refer
to
to
more
than
one
thing;
teacher
that
she
does
favorite
objects
or
for
example,
“night-‐
not
want
any
more
food.
experiences
that
only
night”
to
refer
to
familiar
adults
know
bedtime
or
to
describe
the
meaning
of;
for
darkness.
example,
“ba
ba
ba
ba”
• Use
many
new
words
for
blanket.
each
day.
• Express
“Mama”
or
• Begin
to
combine
a
few
“Dada”
when
the
words
into
mini-‐
mother
or
father,
sentences
to
express
respectively,
enters
the
wants,
needs,
or
room.
interests;
for
example,
• Say
a
first
word
clearly
“more
milk,”
“big
enough
that
the
infant
doggie,”
“no
night-‐
care
teacher
can
night”
or
“go
bye-‐bye.”
understand
the
word
• Have
a
vocabulary
of
within
the
context;
for
about
80
words.
example,
“gih”
for
give,
• Start
adding
articles
“see,”
“dis”
for
this,
before
nouns,
such
as,
“cookie,”
“doggie,”
“uh
“a
book”
or
“the
cup.”
oh”
and
“no.”
• Use
own
name
when
• Name
a
few
familiar
asked
favorite
objects.
• Ask
questions
with
• Change
tone
when
raised
intonations
at
the
babbling,
so
that
the
end,
such
as
“Doggy
child’s
babbles
sound
go?”
more
and
more
like
• Communicate
using
adult
speech.
sentences
of
three
to
• Use
expressions;
for
five
words,
such
as
example,
“uh
oh”
when
“Daddy
go
store?”
or
milk
spills
or
when
“Want
more
rice.”
9
something
falls
off
the
table.
• Say
“up”
and
lift
arms
to
be
picked
up
by
the
infant
care
teacher.
Foundation: Communication Skills and Knowledge
The developing ability to communicate nonverbally and verbally
8
months
18
months
36
months
At
around
eight
months
of
age,
At
around
18
months
of
age,
At
around
36
months
of
age,
children
participate
in
back-‐and-‐ children
use
conventional
children
engage
in
back-‐and-‐
forth
communication
and
games.
gestures
and
words
to
forth
conversations
that
contain
communicate
meaning
in
short
a
number
of
turns,
with
each
back-‐and-‐forth
interactions
and
turn
building
upon
what
was
said
use
the
basic
rules
of
in
the
previous
turn.
conversational
turn-‐taking
when
communicating.
For
example,
the
child
may:
For
example,
the
child
may:
For
example,
the
child
may:
• Put
arms
up
above
head
• Respond
to
the
infant
• Persist
in
trying
to
get
when
the
infant
care
care
teacher’s
initiation
the
infant
care
teacher
teacher
says,
“soooo
of
conversation
to
respond
by
repeating,
big.”
through
vocalizations
speaking
more
loudly,
• Try
to
get
the
infant
care
or
nonverbal
expanding
on
what
the
teacher
to
play
peek-‐a-‐ communication.
child
said,
or
touching
boo
by
hiding
her
face
• Initiate
interactions
the
infant
care
teacher.
behind
a
blanket,
with
the
infant
care
• Repeat
part
of
what
the
uncovering
her
face,
and
teacher
by
touching,
adult
just
said
in
order
laughing.
vocalizing,
or
offering
a
to
continue
the
• Pull
the
infant
care
toy.
conversation.
teacher’s
hands
away
• Jabber
into
a
toy
phone
• Make
comments
in
a
from
his
face
during
a
and
then
pause,
as
if
to
conversation
that
the
game
of
peek-‐a-‐boo.
listen
to
someone
on
other
person
has
• Try
to
clap
hands
to
get
the
other
end.
difficulty
understanding;
the
infant
care
teacher
• Shake
head
or
express
for
example,
suddenly
to
continue
playing
pat-‐ “no”
when
the
infant
switch
topics
or
use
a-‐cake.
care
teacher
asks
if
the
pronouns
without
• Make
sounds
when
the
child
is
ready
to
go
making
clear
what
is
infant
care
teacher
is
back
inside.
(18
mos.
being
talked
about.
singing
a
song.
• Respond
to
the
infant
• Answer
adults’
• Interact
with
the
infant
care
teacher’s
questions,
such
as
care
teacher
while
comment
about
a
toy
“What’s
that?”
and
singing
a
song
with
with
an
additional,
but
“Where
did
it
go?”
Begin
actions
or
while
doing
related,
action
or
to
create
finger
plays.
comment
about
the
understandable
topics
same
toy;
for
example,
for
a
conversation
make
a
barking
sound
partner.
when
the
infant
care
• Sometimes
get
teacher
pats
a
toy
dog
frustrated
if
the
infant
and
says,
“Nice
care
teacher
does
not
doggie.”
understand
what
the
child
is
trying
to
communicate
• Participate
in
back-‐and-‐
forth
interaction
with
the
infant
care
teacher
10
by
speaking,
giving
feedback,
and
adding
to
what
was
originally
said.
Behaviors
leading
up
to
the
Behaviors
leading
up
to
the
Behaviors
leading
up
to
the
foundation
(4
to
7
months)
foundation
(9
to
17
months)
foundation
(19
to
35
months)
During
this
period,
the
child
may:
During
this
period,
the
child
During
this
period,
the
child
may:
• Respond
with
babbling
may:
• Ask
and
answer
simple
when
the
infant
care
• Copy
the
infant
care
questions,
such
as
teacher
asks
a
question.
teacher
in
waving
“bye-‐ “What’s
that
• Laugh
when
a
parent
bye”
to
a
parent
as
he
• Say,
“huh?”
when
nuzzles
her
face
in
the
leaves
the
room.
interacting
with
the
child’s
belly,
vocalizes
• Purse
lips
after
hearing
infant
care
teacher
to
expectantly
when
she
and
seeing
the
infant
keep
interaction
going.
pulls
back,
and
laugh
care
teacher
make
a
• Repeat
or
add
on
to
when
she
nuzzles
again
sputtering
sound
with
what
she
just
said
if
the
• Move
body
in
a
rocking
her
lips.
infant
care
teacher
does
motion
to
get
the
infant
• Repeat
the
last
word
in
not
respond
right
away.
care
teacher
to
continue
an
adult’s
question
in
• Engage
in
short
back-‐
rocking.
order
to
continue
the
and-‐forth
interactions
• Babble
back
and
forth
conversation;
for
with
a
family
member
with
the
infant
care
example,
saying
“dat”
by
responding
to
teacher
during
diaper
after
the
infant
care
comments,
questions,
change.
teacher
asks,
“What
is
and
prompts.
that?”
• Respond
almost
• Respond
with
“yes”
or
immediately
after
a
“no”
when
asked
a
parent
finishes
talking
in
simple
question.
order
to
continue
the
• Hold
out
a
toy
for
the
interaction.
infant
care
teacher
to
• Get
frustrated
if
the
take
and
then
reach
infant
care
teacher
does
out
to
accept
it
when
not
understand
what
the
infant
care
teacher
the
child
is
trying
to
offers
it
back.
communicate.
• Show
an
understanding
• Attempt
to
continue
that
a
conversation
conversation,
even
must
build
on
what
the
when
the
adult
does
not
other
partner
says;
for
understand
him
right
example,
expressing,
away,
by
trying
to
use
“bear”
when
the
infant
different
words
to
care
teacher
points
to
communicate
the
the
stuffed
bear
and
meaning
asks,
“What’s
that?”
• Sustain
conversation
• Initiate
back-‐and-‐forth
about
one
topic
for
one
interaction
with
the
or
two
turns,
usually
infant
care
teacher
by
about
something
that
is
babbling
and
then
in
the
here
and
now.
waiting
for
the
infant
• Respond
verbally
to
care
teacher
to
adults’
questions
or
respond
before
comments.
babbling
again.
• Say
“mmm”
when
eating,
after
a
parent
says,
“mmm.”
11