Academy of Management
Academy of Management
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Academy of Management is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Academy
of Management Review.
http://www.jstor.org
?
Academy ofManagement Review
2005,Vol. 30,No. 2, 269-287.
? THEORYOF TEAMCOACHING
J.RICHARD HACKMAN
Harvard University
RUTHW?GEMAN
Dartmouth College
After briefly reviewing the existing literature on team coaching, we propose a new
model with three distinguishing features. The model (1) focuses on the functions that
coaching serves for a team, rather than on either specific leader behaviors or lead
ership styles, (2) identifies the specific times in the task performance process when
coaching interventions are most likely to have their intended effects, and (3) expli
cates the conditions under which team-focused coaching is and is not likely to
facilitate performance.
study of 268 task-performing teams in 88 organi Wilson, 1991). Although varied, these models
zations, we Hackman, & Lehman, specify ways that team leaders can develop
(Wageman,
2004) asked team leaders and members to rank members' interpersonal skills, define members'
the amount of attention the team leader gave to roles and expectations, deal with conflict and
activities in each of the four categories listed interpersonal frictions, and help a team achieve
above (with a rank of "1" signifying the greatest a level of "maturity" that lessens the team's de
attention). For both leader and member reports, pendence on its leader (Eden, 1985; Fischer, 1993;
coaching the team as a whole came in last (the Geber, 1992; Manz & Sims, 1987; Patten, 1981;
combined mean ranks were as follows: structur Rees, 1991; Torres & Spiegel, 2000; Woodman &
ing the team and itswork, 1.75; running external Sherwood, 1980).
interference, 2.16; coaching individuals, 2.88;
and coaching the team, 3.02).
Process Consultation
The lesser attention given to team coaching
could simply mean that leaders underestimate The process consultation approach developed
the potential benefits of providing coaching as by Schein (1969, 1988)posits that competent in
sistance to their teams. More likely, perhaps, is terpersonal relations are essential for effective
that leaders do not coach their teams because task performance and that group members
they do not know how to do so, or they have themselves must be intimately involved in ana
ventured a coaching intervention or two that did lyzing and improving those relationships. The
not help and thereafter focused their behavior consultant engages team members in analyzing
on seemingly more promising team leadership group processes on two levels simultaneously:
strategies. By using existing research and the (1) the substantive level?to analyze how human
to of leader be are work on a specific organ
ory identify the kinds coaching processes affecting
haviors that do help teams operate more effec izational problem?and (2) the internal level?to
here not only to advance basic better understand the team's own interaction
tively, we seek
understanding aboutteam coaching but also to processes and the ways that team processes
provide practitioners with some of what they foster or impede effective group functioning
need to know to coach their teams competently. (Schein, 1988: 11-12). Decidedly clinical in orien
tation, this type of coaching requires the process
consultant first to directly observe the group as
EXISTING APPROACHES
itworks on a substantive organizational prob
In a review of existing research and theory, lem and then, once the group is ready, to intro
we identified three conceptually driven ap duce systematic or confrontive interventions in
to team coaching and one eclectic ap tended to help the team deal with its problems
proaches
proach that is largely atheoretical. These four and exploit previously unrecognized opportuni
approaches, described below, point the way to ties.
have three features. First, they are real groups. Because we believe that these dimensions also
That is, they are intact social systems, complete are consequential for any team's long-term or
with boundaries, interdependence among mem ganizational performance, we define team effec
bers, and differentiated member roles (Alderfer, tiveness using the following three-dimensional
1977). Members of real groups can be distin conception (adapted from Hackman, 1987).
guished reliably from nonmembers, they are in
terdependent for some common
purpose, and 1. The productive output of the team (i.e., its
roles within product, service, or decision) meets or ex
they invariably develop specialized
ceeds the standards of quantity, quality,
the group. Real groups can be either small or
and timeliness of the team's clients?the
large and either temporary or long-lived. people who receive, review, and/or use the
Second, work teams have one or more group output. It is the clients' standards and as
tasks to perform. They produce some outcome for sessments that count in assessing team
which members bear collective products?not those of the team itself (ex
responsibility
in rare cases where the team is the
and for which acceptability is potentially as cept
client of its own work) or those of the team's
sessable. The kind of outcome produced is not
manager (who only rarely is the person who
critical?it could be a physical product, a ser receives and uses a team's
actually output).
vice, a decision, a performance, or a written 2. The social processes the team uses in car
report. Nor is it necessary that the outcome ac rying out the work enhance members' capa
some larger social this social serve as sites for personal learning and can
system. Usually
spawn relation
system is the parent organization that created satisfying interpersonal
ships, but they also can deskill, frustrate,
the team, but it can be people or groups outside and alienate their members. We do not
that organization as well, such as opponents for count as effective any team forwhich the net
an athletic team or customers for a service impact of the group experience on members'
team. What is critical is that team learning and well-being is more negative
providing
than positive.
members be collectively responsible formanag
ing consequential transactions with other indi
Although the three criteria vary in importance
viduals and/or groups.
in different circumstances, effective teams bal
ance them over time, never completely sacrific
ing any one to achieve the others. In the pages
Team Performance Effectiveness that follow, we identify the coaching functions,
the temporal imperatives, and the contextual
Criterion measures in empirical research on
circumstances that affect the degree to which
team performance often consist
of whatever
coaching behaviors can help a work team
quantitative indicators happen to be available
achieve and sustain a high standing on all three
or are easy to obtain (e.g., production figures for of the criteria.
industrial workgroups or number of correct re
sponses for teams studied in experimental lab
oratories). Such criteria of convenience do not
FUNCTIONS
address other outcome dimensions, such as cli
ent assessments of a team's work, the degree to Over four decades ago, McGrath first sug
which a team becomes stronger as a performing gested that "[The leader's] main job is to do, or
unit over time, or the extent towhich individual get done, whatever is not being adequately han
members become more knowledgeable or dled for group needs" (1962: 5). If a leader man
skilled as a result of their team experiences. ages, by whatever means, to ensure that all
2005 Hackman and Wageman 273
functions critical to group performance are cient effort, use inappropriate strategies, and/or
taken care of, the leader has done his or her job apply inadequate talent in their work?are
well. Thus, a functional approach to leadership likely to fall short in one or more of the effective
leaves room for an indefinite number of ways to ness criteria.
proper coaching intervention in such cases is to cycle when they are made. Regularities in group
members their rela life cycles have been explored empirically for
help improve interpersonal
tionships. many decades, beginning with Bales and Strodt
In fact, research that, in some cir beck's (1951) classic study of phases in group
suggests
arrow points in the op problem solving. In a number of conceptual
cumstances, the causal
direction?that is, drives in frameworks, scholars have sought to summarize
posite performance
research findings about group development, the
terpersonal processes (or, at least, perceptions
most prominent being the "forming-storming
of those processes), rather than vice versa. For
Staw norming-performing" model proposed by Tuck
example, (1975) gave task-performing
man (1965). Almost all of these frameworks have
teams false feedback about their performance
treated group development as following a fixed
and then asked members to provide "objective" set of stages, with each successive stage being
descriptions of how members had interacted.
contingent on successful completion of the prior
Teams randomly to the high perfor
assigned one.
mance condition reported more harmonious and
In recent years, research on temporal issues in
better communications, among other differ has raised doubt about the gen
group behavior
ences, than did groups assigned to the low per
erality and validity of stage models (Ancona &
formance condition (see also Guzzo, Wagner, 1999; Gersick, 1988; Ginnett, 1993;
Chong,
Maguire, Herr, & Hawley, 1986). McGrath & Kelly, 1986; Moreland & Levine, 1988;
Doubt also is cast on interpersonal ap for a recent attempt to reconcile alternative tem
to coaching by action research that see Chang, Bordia, & Duck, 2003).
proaches poral models,
seeks to improve team performance by improv Gersick's findings are particularly relevant for
ing the quality of members' interactions. Some our purposes. In a field study of the life histories
of these studies use interventions based on the of a number of project teams whose performance
process consultation approach to coaching re periods ranged from several days to several
viewed earlier; others employ a broader set of months, Gersick (1988) found that each of the
interventions that generally are referred to as groups she tracked developed a distinctive ap
team building or group development activities. proach toward its task as soon as it commenced
interventions that address members' work, and stayed with that approach
each until
Although
between its first and
relationships and interaction can be quite en precisely halfway meeting
and do affect members' its project deadline. At the midpoint of their
gaging attitudes, they
do not reliably improve team performance (for
reviews, see Kaplan, 1979; Salas, Rozell, Mullen,
& Driskell, 1999; Tannenbaum, Beard, & Salas, 2Woolley's main effect finding was significantly condi
tioned by the timing of the intervention, and the Kernaghan
1992; Woodman & Sherwood, 1980). Moreover,
and Cook finding was obtained only for groups composed of
those experimental studies that have directly with ample task-relevant abilities. We discuss the
members
compared teams given task-focused and inter moderating effects of timing and of group design on the
personally focused interventions have found the impact of coaching interventions later.
2005 Hackman and Wageman 275
lives, almost all teams underwent a major tran likely to be helpful if they are provided at a time
sition. In a concentrated burst of changes, they in the life cycle when the team is not ready for
dropped old patterns of behavior, reengaged them. Indeed, ill-timed interventions may actu
with outside supervisors, and adopted new per ally do more harm than good by distracting or
spectives on their work.3 Following the midpoint diverting a team from other issues that do re
transition, groups entered a period of focused quire members' attention at that time. We next
task execution, which persisted until very near discuss the kinds of interventions that are most
the project deadline, at which time a new set of appropriate at the beginnings, midpoints, and
issues having to do with termination processes ends of work team life cycles.
arose and captured members' attention. Gersick
(1989) subsequently replicated these findings in
the experimental for that all Beginnings
laboratory groups
had the same amount of time to complete their When team members first come together to
task (foralternative views of temporal dynamics perform a piece of work, the most pressing piece
in task-performing groups, see Seers & Woo of business, both formembers and for the team
druff, 1997, and Waller, Zellmer-Bruhn, & Giam as a whole, is for them to get oriented to one
batista, 2002). another and to the task in preparation for the
Gersick's findings about the natural develop start of actual work. This involves establishing
mental processes of task-performing groups the boundary that distinguishes members from
raise the possibility, consistent with both the nonmembers, starting to differentiate roles and
process consultation and developmental ap formulate norms about how members will work
proaches to team coaching previously summa together, and engaging with (and,inevitably,
rized, that the readiness of work teams for coach redefining) the group task. These activities,
ing interventions changes systematically across which involve simultaneous engagement with
their life cycles. By "readiness for coaching" we the interpersonal and task issues that dominate
mean (1) the degree to which the issues to be the start-up of any social system, create a high
addressed are among those naturally on team state of readiness foranything that shows prom
members' minds at the time of the intervention, ise of reducing members' uncertainties and
coupled with (2) the degree towhich the team as helping them get off to a good start. A coaching
a whole is not at that time preoccupied with intervention that helps a group have a good
more pressing or compelling matters. "launch," therefore, can significantly enhance
We posit that coaching interventions are more members' commitment to the team and the task,
effective when they address issues a team is and thereby enhance their motivation to perform
ready for at the time they are made and, more the work of the team as well as they can.
over, that readiness varies systematically The power and persistence of coaching be
across the team life cycle. In contrast, even com haviors at the launch of a task-performing team
petently administered interventions are un are affirmed by Ginnett's (1993) study of the be
havior of airline captains during the first few
3
minutes of a newly formed crew's life. The struc
In Gersick's research itwas not clear whether the mid
tural "shell" within which cockpit crews work is
point transition was prompted externally (i.e., by reference
to a clock or calendar) or internally sense quite detailed: the aircraft to be flown, where it
(i.e., by members'
that about half their allotted time had elapsed). Mann (2001) is to be flown, the roles of each crew member,
investigated this question experimentally by having groups basic work procedures such as checklists, and
work in a room with a clock that ran normally, in one where much more all are prespecified and well under
the clock ran one-third faster than normal (i.e., when thirty stood by all crewmembers. Therefore, a new
minutes had passed, the clock showed that forty minutes
or in one where
crew should be able to proceed with its work
had elapsed), the clock ran one-third slower
than normal (i.e., at the thirty-minute mark it showed twenty
without spending time getting organized, which
minutes). Groups with the normal clock experienced a single is, in fact, what happens if a captain does not
midpoint transition, replicating earlier findings. But groups conduct a launch briefing when the crew first
with the faster and slower clocks exhibited two such transi
meets.
tions?one at the midpoint indicated by the clock and the
other at the actual midpoint of the allotted
Consistent with Gersick's results, Ginnett
time?showing
that groups pace their work in response to both internal and (1993) found that what happened in the first few
external cues about elapsed time. minutes of crewmembers' time together carried
276 Academy of Management Review April
forward throughout a significant portion of the groups did better than the "discuss your strat
crew's life. Crews led by captains who merely egy first" groups on the version of the task for
took the time in their preflight briefings to affirm which the obvious way of proceeding was the
the positive features of the crew shell fared bet optimum strategy, and the reverse was true
ter than those whose captains gave no briefing when the obvious way of proceeding was sub
at all or those whose captains undermined the optimal.
standard shell. Best of all were crews whose Perhaps the most significant finding of the
captains went beyond mere affirmation and ac study, however, is buried in the discussion sec
tively elaborated the shell?identifying, com tion of the research report?namely, that itwas
menting on, and engaging their crews in a dis nearly impossibleto get the experimental
cussion of the unique circumstances of the trip groups have a discussion
to actually of perfor
that was about to begin. These captains trans mance strategy at the start of the work period.
formed a highly competent set of individual pi Only by structuring the strategy intervention as
lots into a motivated flight crew. a "preliminary task" and explicitly requiring the
Because most work teams do not have struc team to check off each step as itwas completed
tures as detailed and elaborate as those of cock could the experimental groups be induced to
pit crews, what happens as members come to have more than a perfunctory discussion of their
gether and come to terms with their work should performance strategy at the beginning of the
shape their trajectories even more profoundly work period. Beginnings are not a good time for
than was the case for Ginnett's crews. Begin strategy discussions, but, as will be seen next,
nings provide a unique opportunity formotiva midpoints are.
tional coaching interventions that breathe life
into a team's structural shell?no matter how
or how elaborate that shell may Midpoints
rudimentary
be?and thereby help get a team off to a good A second window for coaching interventions
start with high motivational engagement by all opens around the midpoint of the team's work.
members. At that point the team has logged some experi
In contrast, and perhaps surprisingly, begin ence with the task, providing data formembers
nings are not good times to help teams formu to use in assessing what is working well and
late work strategy. When they are just starting poorly. Moreover, the team is likely to have ex
out, teams are not yet ready to address ques perienced an upheaval, driven in part by mem
tions of strategy, as Hackman, Brousseau, and bers' anxieties about the amount of work they
Weiss (1976) inadvertently discovered in an ex have accomplished relative to the time they
perimental study of team performance. These have remaining, that opens the possibility of
researchers asked a subset of participating significant change in how the team operates
teams to take a few minutes to reflect on their (Gersick, 1988, 1989). For these reasons, readi
performance strategy?that is, to consider vari ness for a strategy-focused coaching interven
ous ways of carrying out the task?before actu tion is high at the temporal midpoint of a team's
ally starting work on it. The investigators hy work.
pothesized that these teams would perform We posit that ongoing teams having no dead
better than teams that were encouraged to line, and therefore no temporal midpoint, also
plunge immediately into the work?but only on experience increased readiness for strategy
tasks for which the most obvious and natural focused interventions when they are about half
way of proceeding was not the optimum task way finished with example, when
the work?for
strategy. they have consumed half of their available re
performance
To test their hypothesis, the researchers struc sources, have progressed halfway to their goal,
tured their experimental task in two different or have arrived at some other natural break
ways. In one version of the task, the most obvi point in the work. At that time members are
ous and natural way of proceeding was, in fact, more likely than previously to welcome and be
optimum for team performance; in a second ver helped by interventions that encourage them to
sion, that way of proceeding would introduce assess their work progress, to review how they
inefficiencies and result in suboptimal perfor are applying members' efforts and talents to the
mance. As expected, the "plunge right in" work, and to consider how they might want to
2005 Hackman and Wageman Til
alter their task performance strategies to better how best to proceed with their work. This con
align them with external requirements and in clusion deals specifically with the timing of ex
ternal resources. ternal interventions intended to foster team
The increased receptivity to coaching inter strategy planning. When work teams spontane
ventions that encourage reflection on team work ously engage in planning activities in their ini
strategies by teams that have logged some ex tial team meetings, process management norms
perience, relative to those that are just starting sometimes emerge that are
subsequently help
out, is a tenet of developmental theories of team ful in pacing and coordinating team activities
coaching (e.g., Kozlowski, Gully, McHugh, Salas, (Janicik & Bartel, 2003).
& Cannon-Bowers, 1996) and is empirically sup In both the Woolley (1998) study and the ex
ported by the findings of Woolley (1998), men periment by Hackman et al. (1976) described ear
tioned earlier. Woolley created an experimental lier, itwas difficult but possible to introduce a
version of an architectural task that involved strategy-focused intervention at the beginning
constructing a model of a college residence hall of a task cycle. In other cases it is impossible to
out of LEGO? bricks. Groups were informed in do so. Total quality management programs, for
advance how the structures they created would example, involve use of such techniques as Pa
be evaluated (criteria included sturdiness, aes reto analyses, control charts, and cost-of-quality
thetics, and various technical indices). She de analyses to develop improved production strat
vised two coaching-type interventions?one in egies (for details, see Hackman & Wageman,
tended to improve members' interpersonal 1995). These techniques simply cannot be used
relationships and one that provided assistance until a record of experience with existing strat
to the team in developing a task-appropriate egies has been amassed?once again affirming
performance strategy. Each team received only that consultative coaching is more appropri
one intervention, administered either at the be ately provided around the middle of a task cycle
ginning or at the midpoint of its work period. than at its beginning.
Woolley's findings, shown in Figure 1, confirm
that strategy interventions are especially help Ends
ful when they come near the midpoint of a
team's work cycle. When the strategy interven The third special opportunity for coaching oc
tion was provided at the beginning of the work curs at the end of a performance period, when
period, it did not help, further affirming that the work is finished or a significant subtask has
members need to log some experience before been accomplished (Kozlowski, Gully, Salas, &
they are ready to have a useful discussion of Cannon-Bowers, 1996). The end of a task cycle is
FIGURE 1
Woolley's Findings About the Focus and Timing of Coaching
- - -
8 -:-
j--- -1
One such contribution is to help members co coaching interventions are abundant for such
ordinate their activities and thereby minimize teams, because they experience multiple begin
the risks of tacit coordination identified by Wit nings, midpoints, and ends.
tenbaum, Vaughan, and Stasser (1998). Leaders Still other teams operate continuously, with
who take on too much of the responsibility for out any official beginnings or ends whatsoever.
coordination do run the risk of becoming so in industrial production teams, for example,
Many
volved in the actual that they overlook
work keep on turning out the same products month
opportunities to help the team develop into an after month, into the future. Even
indefinitely
increasingly competent performing unit. And teams with continuous tasks, however, usually
team members may eventually abdicate to the have and ends. They
beginnings, midpoints,
leader their own responsibilities formanaging have them because the teams, or their manag
team performance processes. Still, when task ers, create them.
team performance processes that are (Bettenhausen & Murnighan, 1985; Gersick,
salient for a given task; those that ad 1988), and they tend to remain in place until and
dress nonsalient processes are, at best, unless something fairly dramatic occurs to force
ineffectual. a rethinking about what behaviors are and are
not appropriate (Gersick & Hackman, 1990; Louis
& Sutton, 1991). When those upfront norms of
Group Design conduct actively promote continuous scanning
Certain features of a team's design, including of the performance situation and proactive plan
properties of the social system context within ning of how members will work together, they
which it operates, can negate the impact of facilitate the development of task performance
coaching interventions, even those that are well strategies that are appropriate for the team's
executed and that address appropriate team task and (Hackman et al., 1976; Wool
situation
performance processes. Moreover, design fea ley, 1998). The appropriateness of a team's per
tures can exacerbate the effects of good and formance strategies also depends, however, on
poor coaching on team effectiveness, heighten the degree towhich the organizational informa
ing the benefits of good coaching and making tion system makes available to the team what
even worse the problems brought about by poor ever data and projections members may need to
coaching. select or invent ways of proceeding that are well
Each of the three performance processes that tuned to their circumstances (Abramis, 1990; Bik
form the core of our model?the level of effort the son, Cohen, & Mankin, 1999).
team expends on its task, the appropriateness of The level of Jcnowiedge and skill a team
its performance strategies, and the amount of brings to bear on its work is influenced by the
knowledge and skill it applies to the work?is composition of the team (a structural feature)
shaped not only by coaching interventions but and by the organizational education system (a
also by how well a team is structured and by the contextual support). Well-composed teams have
level of contextual support provided (Hackman, members who bring to the group a rich array of
Wageman, Ruddy, & Ray, 2000). task-relevant knowledge and skills, and they
The effort a team expends on its work is influ are structured so that members' talents can be
enced by the design of its task (a structural drawn on readily in pursuing team purposes.
feature) and by the reward system of the orga Such teams are as small as possible, given the
nization in which the team operates (a contex work to be accomplished, they include members
tual support). A motivating team task is a whole who have appropriate skills, and they have a
and meaningful piece of work forwhich mem good mix of members?people who are neither
bers share responsibility and accountability so similar to one another that they are like peas
and one that is structured so that members re from the same pod nor so different that they risk
ceive regular and trustworthy data about how having difficulty communicating and coordinat
they are doing. Well-designed team tasks foster ing with one another (Ancona & Caldwell, 1992;
high, task-focused effort by team members Campion, Medsker, & Higgs, 1993; Druskat, 1996;
(Hackman & Oldham, 1980). Team effort is en Goodman & Shah, 1992; Jackson, 1992).
hanced by organizational reward systems that Even well-composed teams, however, may not
recognize and reinforce team excellence?and have within their boundaries all of the talent
that avoid the common, if usually unintended, required for excellent performance. Organiza
problem of providing disincentives for collabo tional education systems can supplement inter
ration among team members by placing them in nal resources by making available to teams, at
competition with one another for individual re the teams' initiative, technical or educational
wards (Wageman, 1995). assistance for any aspects of the work forwhich
The task appropriateness of a team's perfor members are not already knowledgeable,
mance strategy is influenced by its core norms skilled, or experienced, including, if necessary,
of conduct (a structural feature) and by the or assistance honing members' skills in working
ganizational information system (a contextual together on collective tasks (Stevens & Yarish,
support). Collective expectations about accept 1999).
able behavior are either "imported" to the group Research evidence clearly establishes the pri
by members or established very early in its life ority of structural and contextual features over
282 Academy of Management Review April
coaching behaviors as influences on team per When, then, can coaching make a constructive
formance processes and outcomes (Cohen, Led difference in team performance processes? We
ford, & Spreitzer, 1996). For example, Wageman propose that coaching makes relatively small
(2001) found, in a study of field service teams at adjustments to an already defined trajectory.
the Xerox Corporation, that team design fea When a team's performance situation is favor
tures, including those described just above, con able, competent coaching can be helpful to
trolled significantly more variance in both the members in minimizing process losses and cre
level of team self-management and in perfor ating process gains. When a team's structure is
mance effectiveness than did team leaders' flawed and/or its context is unsupportive, how
coaching For team self-management,
behaviors. ever, even competent process-focused coaching
design features controlled 42 percent of the vari may do more harm than good.
ance, compared to less than 10 percent formea
Proposition 4: Competent coaching in
sures that assessed the quality of leaders'
terventions (i.e., those that foster col
coaching activities; for team performance, de
lectiveeffort, task-appropriate perfor
sign controlled 37 percent of the variance, com
mance strategies, and good use of
pared to less than 1 percent for coaching. These
member knowledge and skill) are
findings are consistent with other evidence,
more beneficial for groups that are
cited earlier, showing that even highly compe
well structured and supported than for
tent process-focused coaching by team leaders those that are not; poor coaching in
or consultants rarely generates substantial or
terventions (i.e., those that subvert
enduring improvements in team processes or
team performance processes) are more
performance, and with the more general finding detrimental for teams that are poorly
that coaching cannot prevail against strong structured and supported than for
structural and contextual forces (Hackman,
those that are well designed.
1987). It is nearly impossible to coach a team to
greatness in a performance situation that under Further findings from the Wageman (2001)
mines rather than supports teamwork. study described above provide evidence in sup
FIGURE 2
How Team Design and Leader Coaching Jointly Affect Team Self-Management
Effective coaching Ineffective coaching
3 t-1 3
2.5 2.5 +
So Well-designed team
O)
? Well-designed team
ti
?
S
2+
o
>
o 1.5 1.5 +
other scientific fields, such as medical research Ancona, D. G., & Chong, C. L. 1999. Cycles and synchrony:
and subatomic to first create one's phe The temporal role of context in team behavior. In
physics,
R. Wageman (Ed.), Groups in context: 33-48. Stamford,
nomenon of interest before conducting research
CT: JAI Press.
on its dynamics (Argyris, 1969). Research in lab
Argyris, C. 1969. The
incompleteness of social psychological
oratory settings, for example, could involve the
theory: Examples from small group, cognitive consis
construction and administration of model
tency, and attribution research. American Psychologist,
specified coaching interventions at times either 24: 893-908.
consistent or inconsistent with the model's prop Argyris, C. 1982. Reasoning, learning, and action. San Fran
ositions and then assessment of the conse cisco: Jossey-Bass.
quences for group dynamics and performance.
Argyris, C. 1993. Education for leading-learning. Organiza
And action research in field settings could as tional Dynamics, 21(3): 5-17.
sess the impact of educational programs in
Bales, R. F., & Strodtbeck, F. L. 1951. Phases in group problem
tended to help team coaches design interven solving. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46:
tions specifically tailored to the task and 485-495.
2005 Hackman and Wageman 285
Bettenhausen, K., & Murnighan, J.K. 1985. The emergence of Geber, B. 1992. From manager into coach. Training, Febru
norms in competitive decision-making groups. Adminis ary: 25-31.
trative Science Quarterly, 30: 350-372.
Gersick, J.G.C.1988. Time and transition in work teams:
Bikson, T. K., Cohen, S. G., & Mankin, D. 1999. Information Toward a new model of group development. Academy of
R. 2000. Why poor teams get poorer. Evidence from airplanes, orchestras, and hospitals.
Corn, The influence of
team effectiveness and design on the quality of Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24: 1-18.
quality
group diagnostic processes. Unpublished doctoral dis Hackman, J. R., Brousseau, K. R., & Weiss, J. A. 1976. The
sertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA. interaction of task design and group performance strat
Edmondson, A. E.
1999. Psychological safety and learning Hackman, J.R., & Oldham, G. R. 1980. Work redesign. Read
behavior in workteams. Administrative Science Quar ing, MA: Addison-Wesley.
terly, 44: 350-383. Hackman, J.R., & Wageman, R. 1995. Total quality manage
Edmondson, A. E., B?hmer,R. M., & Pisano, G. P. 2001. Dis ment: Empirical, conceptual, and practical issues. Ad
ministrative Science 40: 309-342.
rupted routines: Team learning and new technology im Quarterly,
plementation in hospitals. Administrative Science Quar Hackman, R., Ruddy, T. M., & Ray, C. R. 2000.
J.R., Wageman,
terly, 46: 685-716. Team in theory and practice.
effectiveness In C. Cooper
Ellis, S., Mendel, R., Nir, M., & Davidi, I. 2002. After-event & E. A. Locke (Eds.), Industrial and organizational psy
reviews: Drawing lessons from successful vs. failed ex chology: Theory and practice: 109-129. Oxford: Black
developing new team leadership skills. New York: tive work groups: 72-119. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
McGraw-Hill.
Hirokawa, R. Y. 1985. Discussion procedures and decision
Fournies, F. F. 1978. Coaching for improved work perfor making performance: A test of a functional perspective.
mance. Bridgewater, NJ: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Human Communication Research, 12: 203-224.
286 Academy of Management Review April
Hollingshead, A. B., Wittenbaum, G. M., Paulus, P. B., Hiro From habits of mind to active thinking. Human Rela
kawa, R. Y., Peterson, R. S., Jehn, K. A., & Yoon, K. In tions, 44: 55-76.
press. A look at groups from the functional perspective. for a change:
Mann, J.B. 2001. Time The role of internal and
In A. B. Hollingshead & M. S. Poole (Eds.), Theories of
external pacing mechanisms in prompting the midpoint
small groups: An interdisciplinary perspective. Thou
transition. Unpublished honors thesis, Department of
sand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.
Jackson, S. 1992. Team composition in organizations. In
Manz, C. O, & Sims, H. P. 1987. Leading workers to lead
S. Worchel, W. Wood, & J.Simpson (Eds.), Group process themselves: The external leadership of self-managing
and productivity: 138-173. London: Sage. work teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 32: 106?
Janicik, G. A., & Bartel, C. A. 2003. Talking about time: Effects 128.
of temporal planning and time awareness norms on M. A., Mathieu,
Marks, J.E., & Zaccaro, S. J.2001. A temporally
group coordination and performance. Group Dynamics, based framework and of team processes.
taxonomy
7(2): 122-134. Academy of Management Review, 26: 356-376.
Jehn, K. A., & Mannix, E. A. 2001. The dynamic nature of McGrath, J.E. 1962. Leadership behavior: Some requirements
conflict: A longitudinal study of intragroup conflict and for leadership DC: U.S. Civil Ser
training. Washington,
group performance. Academy of Management Journal, vice Commission.
44: 238-251.
McGrath, J. E., & Kelly, J.R. 1986. Time and human interac
Kaplan, R. E. 1979. The conspicuous absence of evidence that tion: Toward a social of time. New York:
psychology
process consultation enhances task performance. Jour Guilford Press.
nal of Applied Behavioral Science, 15: 346-360.
Moreland, R. L., & Levine, J.M. 1988. Group dynamics over
Kernaghan, J.A., & Cooke, R. A. 1990. Teamwork in planning time: Development and socialization in small groups. In
innovative projects: Improving group performance by J. E. McGrath (Ed.), The social psychology of time: New
rational and interpersonal interventions in group pro perspectives: 151-181. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
cess. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management,
Orlitzky, M., & Hirokawa, R. Y. 2001. To err is human, to
37: 109-116.
correct is divine: A meta-analysis of research testing the
Kinlaw, D. C. 1991. Developing superior work teams. San functional theory of group decision-making effective
Komaki, J. L. 1986. Toward effective supervision: An op?rant Osborn, A. 1963. Applied imagination (3rd ed.). New York:
contingent leader roles. In G. R. Ferris (Ed.), f?esearch in Seers, A., & Woodruff, S. 1997. Temporal pacing in task forc
and human resource vol. 14: es: Group development or deadline pressure. Journal of
personnel management,
Management, 23: 169-187.
253-305. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
research on organizations. Behavior and R., Hackman, J.R., & Lehman, E. V. 2004. Devel
Organizational Wageman,
Human Performance, 13: 414-432. opment of the Team Diagnostic Survey. Working paper,
Tuck School, Dartmoth College, Hanover, NH.
Steiner, I. D. 1972. Group process and productivity. New York:
Academic Press. Waller, M. J.,Zellmer-Bruhn, M. E., & Giambatista, R. C. 2002.
Watching the clock: Group pacing behavior under dy
Stevens, M. J.,& Yarish, M. E. 1999. Training for team effec
namic deadlines. Academy of Management Journal, 45:
tiveness. In E. Sundstrom (Ed.), Supporting work team
1046-1055.
effectiveness: 126-156. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Wasserman, N., Anand, B., & Nohria, N. 2001. When does
Tannenbaum, S. L., Beard, R. L., & Salas, E. 1992. Team
leadership matter? The contingent opportunities view of
building and its influence on team effectiveness: An
CEO leadership. Working paper No. 01-063, Harvard
examination of conceptual and empirical develop Business School, Boston.
ments. In K. Kelley (Ed.), Issues, theory, and research in
117-153. Amster Wellins, R. S., Byham, W. C, & Wilson, J.M. 1991. Empowered
industrial/organizational psychology:
dam: Elsevier. teams: Creating self-managing working groups and the
improvement of productivity and participation. San
Taylor, D., Berry, P., & Block, C. 1957. Does group participa Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
tion when using brainstorming facilitate or inhibit cre
Wittenbaum, G. M., Vaughan, S. I., & Stasser, G. 1998. Coor
ative thinking? Administrative Science Quarterly, 3:23-47.
dination in task-performing groups. In R. S. Tindale,
Torres, C, & Spiegel, J. 2000. Self-directed work teams: A & E. J.Posavac on
J.Edwards, (Eds.), Theory and research
primer. Jossey-Bass/Pfeiffer. small groups: 177-204. New York: Plenum Press.