Intro - Cognitive Approach
Intro - Cognitive Approach
www.pmt.education
The Cognitive Approach
Part 1— An Introduction to The Cognitive Approach
• The cognitive approach sees mental processes as being separate from the brain.
• The invention of the computer in the 1960s was crucial in the development of cognitive
psychology, as psychologists now had a metaphor for the mind.
Schemas:
• Schemas are ‘packages’ of ideas and knowledge about a certain person, place, object
or time. They are generated through experience, becoming more sophisticated through
time.
• They also act as mental frameworks, providing us with ‘mental shortcuts’ so we can
process large volumes of data quickly and efficiently, thus avoiding sensory overload.
• However, since schemas are ‘pre-conceived’, they may lead to perceptual distortions
due to having an already established mental framework e.g. James Potter et al (2009)
showing that when watching TV, 1 “although viewers may share the same story schema,
they appear to make different judgements on the schema elements, and hence their
judgements about violence vary”.
1
W. James Potter, K. Pashupati, R.G. Pekurny, E.Hoffman and K.Davis, Perceptions of Television:
A Schema Explanation, Media Psychology, 4(1), 2002.
www.pmt.education
1. Brain Mapping in the 1870s = Carl Wernicke, based on case studies of patients who
all had damage to a specific area of the brain and all suffered from the same type of
aphasia (Wernicke’s), inferred that Broddman’s area 22 must be involved in language
comprehension.
A03 Evaluation:
+ Scientific Methods and Rigour = The emergence of cognitive neuroscience has
substantially increased the scientific credibility of psychology, bringing it closer to that
of biology, physics etc. This is due to the emphasis on objectively collecting reliable
data through direct observation of the neural processes underlying cognition, as seen in
PET, CT, MRI and fMRI scans.
+ Soft Determinism = The cognitive approach sees humans as being able to reason and
make conscious decisions within the limits of what they know or their ‘cognitive
system’, and so adopts a soft deterministic approach. This is more flexible than the
behaviourist hard determinism stance because it allows for humans to have some
conscious insight into their behaviour: a complexity which differentiates us from
animals, and so provides a better explanation for human behaviour than behaviourism.
CBT aims to identify and challenge irrational thoughts, replacing them with more productive
behaviours, and thus treating depression. Beck’s CBT aims on identifying the patient’s thoughts
and challenging them as irrational. This can be guided by the cognitive triad of automatic negative
www.pmt.education
thoughts (negative thoughts about the self, the future and the world), faulty information
processing and negative self-schemas. Cognitive therapy also aims for patients to test the reality
of their beliefs. For example, a patient may record each time someone was nice to them for the
past week. Next time they say that everyone hates them, the therapist can point towards the
journal as counter-evidence, thus proving the patient’s beliefs as irrational. This demonstrates the
idea of ‘patient as scientist’. Ellis’s rational emotive behaviour therapy aims to identify the
patient’s thoughts and challenge them as irrational, leading to a vigorous argument. This may be a
logical argument (i.e. the belief doesn’t follow on logically from the facts) or an empirical argument
(there is no evidence to support the irrational belief). Thus, this aims to change the irrational belief
and to break the link between negative life events and depression. Through behavioural activation,
patients are encouraged to engage in enjoyable activities, to provide further counter-evidence for
their irrational beliefs.
+ = Supporting research evidence - March followed a group of 327 adolescents with a main
diagnosis of depression. After 36 weeks - 81%, 81% and 86% were the respective
improvement rates for each of the three experimental conditions (CBT, antidepressants, CBT+
antidepressants). Therefore, this is compelling evidence for the idea that antidepressants are
effective in treating depression and are based on accurate biological explanations of depression
i.e. linked to the role of serotonin and noradrenaline in the development of depression.
— = CBT may not be an appropriate treatment for all cases of depression, and particularly the
most severe cases. This idea could also have been reflected in the evidence provided by March et
al, where a combination of CBT and antidepressants is the most effective combination. This is
because those with severe depression may not be able to attend the regular CBT sessions, due to
a lack of motivation/ an inability to get out of bed in the morning, and also may feel completely
hopeless i.e. that they are beyond help. This means that CBT cannot be used to address all cases
of depression, and arguably is not suitable for cases which need help the most!
— = The focus of the cognitive approach is on present life and the present challenges which life
presents. It is then assumed that the patient’s current circumstances are responsible for their
depression. However, a considerable number of patients may be aware of specific past events
which may be responsible for their depression, such as a traumatic life event or the death of a
loved one. Therefore, since CBT therapists are unwilling to ‘dwell on the past’, patients may
become frustrated that they have such little input or say into how their therapy is brought about.
• Eyewitness testimony refers to the information recalled about a crime by an eyewitness. The
accuracy of such an account can be reduced through the influence of misleading (incorrect)
information in the form of leading questions and post-event discussions.
• The effects of leading questions on the accuracy of EWT was investigated by Loftus and Palmer
(1974) where participants watched a film clip of a car crash and then gave speed estimates of
the cars based on the leading question of “About how fast were the cars going when they x into
each other?”, with each group being exposed to a different critical verb. Those exposed to the
verb “smashed” gave a speed estimate 8.7 mph greater than those who’d heard “contacted”.
Therefore, this shows that leading questions, because of the way they are phrased, suggest that
there is a correct answer.
• The effects of leading questions can be explained using the idea of response bias (i.e. these
questions only influence the participants to give
a certain answer), whereas Loftus and Palmer’s
study supports the substitution explanation (i.e.
leading questions change the eyewitness’
memory of the crime), as those who’d heard the
word “smashed” were more likely to report
having seen broken glass 2 weeks after the
crime (despite there being no broken glass)
compared to those who’d heard the word
“contacted”.
www.pmt.education
— A key methodological criticism for studies of EWT (including that of Loftus and Palmer) is that
they often use the same, young target to identify. This, as argued by Anastasi and Rhodes, may
be affected by own age bias, which describes the tendency to recall others from your own age
group with a high degree of accuracy, with a lower accuracy rate for those from other age groups.
This means that participants aged 55-78 years may be inaccurately represented as having a lower
accuracy of EWT, due to the frequent use of young targets.
— Demand characteristics may also reduce the reliability of the findings, as argued by Zaragosa
and McCloskey, who suggest that participants often want to be as helpful and attentive as
possible. This means that, through the mechanism of social desirability bias and the ‘Please-U’
effect, when in doubt over their answer to a question, they are likely to give an answer which
seems most beneficial or expected of the researcher, thus biasing the results and reducing the
likelihood that the same results will be demonstrated again.
— The artificial tasks and stimuli used by both Loftus and Palmer, alongside Gabbert, reduces the
ecological validity of the findings and the mundane realism of the methodology. For example, the
film clips of the car crashes do not expose participants to the anxiety of experiencing a real-life
car crash. This anxiety may either have a negative (Johnson and Scott) or positive (Yuille and
Cutshall) effect on the accuracy of EWT, thus biasing the findings.
www.pmt.education