Self Acceptance Scale - V3
Self Acceptance Scale - V3
(USAS-C)
Fung Ching-man
Background
Early empirical studies on Carl Rogers’ theory of personality focused mainly on his
observation of the positive relation between self-acceptance and acceptance of others and this
proposed relationship have garnered empirical support since 1950s (Berger, 1952; Fey, 1954;
Phillips, 1951; Suinn, 1961; Suinn & Hill, 1964; Durm & Glaze, 2001). Other research
related to self-acceptance include its negative association with psychopathology (e.g. Berger,
1955; Zuckerman & Monashkin, 1957; Richter, Richter, Eisemann & Seering, 1995), anxiety
(Ohnmacht & Muro, 1967; Pilsuk, 1963), and its applications in the mental health disciplines
(Baisden, Linstrom & Hector, 1982; Brandel, 1982; Kornblum & Anderson, 1982; Rudnick,
1982). Although research on self-acceptance have been abundant, it seems that each study
had their own interpretation of self-acceptance, and there have not been a single definition of
self-acceptance being accepted by all who use the term (Crowne & Stephens, 1961). Most of
the studies in the 1950s adopted the definition of self-acceptance deriving from the construct
concept is congruent with the individual’s description of his “ideal self.” For example,
Stephenson (1953)’s Q-sort technique, Bills (1958)’s Adjective rating scale, and Brownfain
(1952)’s Self-rating scale were using the measurement of discrepancy between real-self and
ideal-self to determine the level of self-acceptance (Crowne & Stephens, 1961). While these
demonstrate that their measures are equivalent and interchangeable (Crowne & Stephens,
1961). Therefore, the lack of an operational definition is the problem of the early studies of
person’s unconditional self-acceptance tendency (USAQ). They took on the idea of Ellis
(1962, 1995) that any self-rating process is dysfunctional as there is no objective basis for
determining the worth of a human being; therefore, they consider unconditional self-
3
acceptance as the condition where “the individual fully and unconditionally accepts himself
whether or not he behaves intelligently, correctly, or competently and whether or not other
people approve, respect, or love him” (Ellis, 1977, p. 101). Although Ellis and Rogers hold a
unconditional positive regard and unconditional self-worth (Chamberlain and Haaga, 2001;
Flett, Besser, Davis, & Hewitt 2003; Cucu-Ciuhan & Dumitru, 2017). Empirical findings
suggest that unconditional self-acceptance is positively associated with self-esteem and state
mood (Chamberlain and Haaga, 2001), negatively related to anxiety (Chamberlain and
Hagga, 2001; Popov, Biro, & Radanovic, 2015; Popov, Radanovic & Biro, 2016), depression
(Ccuc-Ciuhan & Dumitru, 2017), and perfectionism (Chang, 2006; Dixon, Earl, Lutz-Zois,
negatively related to narcissistic personality because narcissism originates from the tendency
to seek approval or recognition from others to maintain one’s self-worth (Chamberlain and
Hagga, 2001).
both Rogers (1951, 1961) and Ellis (1962, 1977, 1995) consider that conditional self-worth is
facilitates personal adjustment and well-being (Ellis, 1962; Rogers, 1951). Ellis even
suggested that people with extremely high self-esteem are vulnerable because they are
characterized by conditional self-worth and that their self-value is decided upon by their
comparison with others (Chamberlain & Haaga, 2001). As a result, unconditional self-
health on adolescents and that there is not a Chinese version of the questionnaire for this
specific population. Therefore, the aim of this study is to develop an adolescent version of
Methodology
Participants in this study were N=19 (Male: 5 (26.4%); Female 14 (73.6%). Their
ages ranged from 15-17 years old (mean age = 16). Subjects were recruited in a psychology
class; the aim of the study was explained and consent from each student was obtained at the
1.2. Measures
Chamberlain & Haaga, 2001). The original questionnaire is a 20-item 7-point Likert-scale
with 11 reversed items. The present Chinese version for adolescents has revised the
questionnaire into a 5-point Likert-scale. Participants responded to each item ranging from 1
(Extremely disagree) to 5 (Extremely agree). The internal consistency of the scale was
moderate (Cronbarch alpha = .69 while the original scale was .72).
administered to measure the participants’ self-esteem. The RSE is a widely used self-report
instrument consisting of 10-item 4-point Likert-scale. It has been translated and validated
into different languages including Chinese (Leung & Wong, 2008). High internal
consistency (Cronbach alpha = .92), 2-week retest reliability, convergent and discriminant
Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Hall, 1979). The Chinese children
version of NPI developed by Ang & Raine (2009) was administered to measure the
participant’s narcissistic tendency. The Chinese children version of NPI is a 12-item 5-point
and Exploitativeness. The 12-item scale has an average Cronbach alpha of .85 and adequate
Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979). BAI is widely
Participants were given a brief introduction to the study and consent was obtained
from each participant at the beginning of the questionnaire. Respondents participated in the
study voluntarily and they could withdraw anytime. No personal data was collected in this
study.
Results
Descriptive Statistic
Table 1 displays the descriptive statistics, and table 2 shows the inter-corrections of
Measure N Mean SD
Unconditional Self-Acceptance Questionnaire for 19 60 7.09
Adolescents (USAQ-C)
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale 19 26.11 4.56
Narcissistic Personality Inventory for Children 19 37.26 9.48
Beck’s Anxiety Inventory 19 37.16 9.74
6
Variable 2 3 4
**. Correlation is significant at the .0.1 level (2-tailed); *. Collection is significant at 0.05 level (2-tailed).
esteem, r (19) =.84, but was found unrelated to narcissistic personality. The present study
Discussion
The present study aims at developing a Chinese version of the USAQ for adolescent.
this population which is consistent with the original finding in Chamberlain and Haaga
(2001). The result suggests that adolescents who score high on both USAQ and the RSE
strongly avoid negative self-rating and the main distinction of the two constructs is that
that any self-rating is inherently irrational and there is no objective basis for determining
Unlike the original study, the present investigation fails to establish a significant link
between unconditional self-acceptance and anxiety and that the relationship between
References
7
Ang, R. P., & Raine, A. (2009). Reliability, validity and invariance of the narcissistic
Baisden, H. E., Lindstrom, V. J., Hector, J. V. (1982). Self-hate, self-esteem, and self-
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N. Brown, G. K., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression.
Berger, E. M. (1952). The relation between expressed acceptance of self and expressed
Bills, R. E. (1958). Manual of the Index of Adjustment and Values. Auburn: Alabama
Polytechnic Institute.
19(3), 163-176.
8
functioning, and academic achievement. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 677-
697.
Dixon, L. J., Earl, K. A., Lutz-Zois, C. J., Goodnight, J. A., & Peatee, J. J. (2014). Explaining
101-111.
Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy. New York: Lyle Stuart.
Ellis, A. (1977). Psychotherapy and the value of a human being. In A. Ellis & R. Grieger
85-89.
Fey, W. F. (1954). Acceptance of self and others, and its relation to therapy-readiness.
Flett, G. L., Besser, A., Davis, R. A., & Hewitt, P. L. (2003). Dimensions of perfectionism:
Kornblum, H., & Anderson, B. (1982). “Acceptance” reassessed: A point of view. Child
Leung, S. On, & Wong, P. W. (2008). Validity and reliability of Chinese Rosenberg Self-
memory of trait adjectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 704-
709.
Ohnmacht, F. W., & Muro, J. J. (1967). Self-acceptance: Some anxiety and cognitive style
Philips, E. L. (1951). Attitudes toward the self and others: A brief questionnaire report.
Popov, S., Biro, M., & Radanovic, J. (2015). Self-evaluation and mental health: an
236.
Popov, S., Radanovic, J. & Biro, M. (2016). Unconditional self-acceptance and mental health
Richter, J., Richter, G., Eisemann, M., & Seering, B. (1995). Depression, perceived parental
161-164
Nebraska Press.
Rogers, C. R., & Dymond, R. F. (Eds.). (1954). Psychotherapy and Personality Change.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
learning theory analysis. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63(1), 37-
42.
11
Suinn, R. M., & Hill, H. (1964). Influence of anxiety on the relationship between self-
119
1 別人的讚美令我感到自己更有價值。
2 我感到自己是有價值的,縱然我未能成功地達成學業目標。
3 當我受到負面評價,我會視之為改善我行為或表現的機會。
4 我感到有些同學更具價值。
5 犯了大錯可能是失望的,但它不會改變我對自己的看法。
6 有時我發現自己在思考究竟我一個好人還是壞人。
7 要感到自己有價值,我必須感到身邊重要的人愛我。
8 我為自己訂下目標,因為我希望它能使我高興。
9 我想如果我能做好每一件事,我便是一個好人。
10 我的自我價值感很大程度上是取決於我怎樣與其他人比較。
11 我相信我是有價值的,這完全因為我是一個人。
12 當我面對負面反應時,我發現我很難去開放地聽取那人說關於我的事情。
13 我為自己定下目標因為我希望這能証明我的價值。
14 某些方面的不好令我感到自己少了點價值。
15 我認為成功的人便是有價值的人。
16 我認為別人的讚賞能令我知道自己的強項。
17 我認為我是有價值的人,即使別人沒有認同我。
18 我避免用跟他人比較的方式去決定自己是否有價值。
19 當我被批評或遇到挫折時,我感到自己是一個很差勁的人。
20 我認為評價自己是否一個有用的人並不是一個好主意。
ii
Appendix II
1整體來說,我滿意自己。
2有時我會覺得自己一點好處都沒有。
3 我覺得自己有不少優點。
4 我能夠做到與大部份人的表現一樣好。
5 我認為自己沒有什麼可以值得自豪。
6 有時我十分覺得自己毫無用處。
7 我覺得自己是個有價值的人,最低限度我與其他人一樣的價值。
8 我希望我能夠多一些尊重自己。
9 從各方面看來,我是較傾向覺得自己是一個失敗者。
10 我用正面的態度看自己。
iii
Appendix III
Narcissistic Personality Inventory for Children – Chinese Version (NPI-C; Ang & Raine,
2009)
1 我經常知道自已在做什麼。
2 我是天生的領導者。
3 我將會成為一個偉大的人。
4 我覺得我是個特別的人。
5 我喜歡看自己的身體。
6 我是一個非凡的人。
7 我發現操縱別人是容易的。
8 如果讓我來統治世界,這個世界將會更好。
9 我很善於讓別人遵從我的方式行事。
10 我能讓任何人相信我想讓他們相信的事。
11 我天生就有一種能影響人們的本事。
12 面對懲罰時,我常有能力辯解而避免受罰。
iv
Appendix IV
Beck’s Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979)
1感覺麻木或刺痛
2 感覺炎熱
3 腳不自主的抖動
4 無法放鬆
5 擔心最壞的事情發生
6 頭昏腦脹
7 心跳突然加速
8 情緒不穩定
9 感覺驚嚇
10 感覺緊張
11 感覺難以下嚥
12 手部容易顫抖
13 感覺不安
14 不能自控
15 呼吸困難
16 對死亡有恐懼
17 感覺害怕
18 消化不良
19 暈眩
20 臉頰發紅
21出汗或冒冷汗