0% found this document useful (0 votes)
95 views12 pages

Baumgartner 2008

The document discusses two types of future-oriented emotions: anticipatory emotions which are currently experienced due to the prospect of a future event, and anticipated emotions which are expected to be experienced in the future if certain events occur or don't occur. It provides definitions and examples of each type and examines their role in motivating goal-directed behavior through two studies. The results provide support for the distinctness of the two emotion types and their independent influence on goal-directed behavior.

Uploaded by

vladimir216
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
95 views12 pages

Baumgartner 2008

The document discusses two types of future-oriented emotions: anticipatory emotions which are currently experienced due to the prospect of a future event, and anticipated emotions which are expected to be experienced in the future if certain events occur or don't occur. It provides definitions and examples of each type and examines their role in motivating goal-directed behavior through two studies. The results provide support for the distinctness of the two emotion types and their independent influence on goal-directed behavior.

Uploaded by

vladimir216
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 12

European Journal of Social Psychology

Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)


Published online 19 September 2007 in Wiley InterScience
(www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.467

Future-oriented emotions: Conceptualization and behavioral effects

HANS BAUMGARTNER1,
RIK PIETERS2* AND RICHARD P. BAGOZZI3
1
Pennsylvania State University, USA
2
Tilburg University, The Netherlands
3
University of Michigan, USA

Abstract

We distinguish between two kinds of future-oriented emotions (anticipatory and anticipated) and investigate their
behavioral effects. Anticipatory emotions are currently experienced due to the prospect of a future event (e.g., hope or
fear). Anticipated emotions, on the other hand, are expected to be experienced in the future if certain events do or do not
occur (e.g., anticipated joy or regret). We discuss the theoretical differences between the two types of future-oriented
emotions and examine their role in motivating goal-directed behavior. The results of a longitudinal study (n ¼ 472) and a
separate control group analysis (n ¼ 340) provide consistent support for the convergent and discriminant validity of
positive/negative anticipatory and anticipated emotions and their independent influence on goal-directed behavior.
Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Sometimes people fear that an undesirable future event may happen, or hope that it will not, and they imagine the
behaviors that they can perform to avert the impending danger and visualize their relief or joy when the negative outcome
does not materialize. All these affective reactions to future events may influence goal-directed behavior.
This research is about the motivational effects of such future-oriented emotions, defined as affective reactions to future
events. Although several researchers have speculated about the role of emotions in the goal-action cycle (e.g., Carver &
Scheier, 2002; Lazarus, 1991), and even though some studies have investigated the motivational influence of specific
future-oriented emotions such as anticipated regret and disappointment (e.g., Abraham & Sheeran, 2004; Richard, van der
Pligt, & de Vries, 1996; Zeelenberg, 1999; Zeelenberg & Pieters, 2004), we still know little about the different kinds of
future-oriented emotions and their role in goal-directed behavior.
More specifically, the present research aims to make the following two contributions. First, we build on recent
proposals (e.g., Loewenstein & Lerner, 2003) to distinguish two types of future-oriented emotions. Although the
distinction is implicit in earlier writings (e.g., Atkinson, 1964), only recently have researchers begun to articulate the
differences between them. We will call the two kinds of future-oriented affective reactions anticipatory and anticipated
emotions. On the one hand, a person may currently experience an emotion due to the prospect of a desirable or undesirable
future event (i.e., hope or fear). These affective reactions are anticipatory emotions, because they are currently
experienced due to something that could happen in the future. On the other hand, a person may imagine experiencing
certain emotions in the future once certain desirable or undesirable future events have occurred (e.g., anticipated joy or
regret). These affective reactions are anticipated emotions. To our knowledge, the current study is the first attempt to study
anticipatory and anticipated emotions empirically and to establish their convergent and discriminant validity.

*Correspondence to: Rik Pieters, Tilburg University, Faculty of Economics and Business, Warandelaan 2, Tilburg, 5000 LE, The Netherlands.
E-mail: [email protected]

Received 22 March 2007


Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted 21 August 2007
686 Hans Baumgartner et al.

Second and most importantly, we consider the role of anticipatory and anticipated emotions in goal pursuit and seek to
establish their predictive validity. Although previous research has shown that each type of emotion can motivate future
behavior (e.g., Abraham & Sheeran, 2004; Bagozzi, Baumgartner, & Pieters, 1998; Gleicher, Boninger, Strathman,
Armor, & Ahn, 1995; van der Pligt, Zeelenberg, van Dijk, de Vries, & Richard, 1998; Zeelenberg & Pieters, 2004),
anticipatory and anticipated emotions have not been compared in a single study, and their relative effectiveness in
stimulating goal-directed behavior is thus uncharted.
We report the results of a study involving two large samples of respondents to assess the convergent and discriminant
validity of anticipatory and anticipated emotions, and to examine their predictive validity in goal-directed behavior. The
context for the study was a major societal event, the millennium change of 1999–2000.

ANTICIPATORY AND ANTICIPATED EMOTIONS ABOUT FUTURE EVENTS

Although at face value, anticipatory and anticipated emotions are clearly different, the two concepts are, in our view, fuzzy
sets for which no necessary and sufficient criteria of category membership are available (Fehr & Russell, 1984). However,
we posit that certain features are characteristic of the two types of future-oriented emotions, which we will discuss in this
section. These characteristics deal with (a) the role of uncertainty, (b) the phenomenology of the emotion, and (c) the range
of discrete emotions subsumed in each category.
First, anticipatory emotions are current affective responses to the prospect of future events that have positive or negative
consequences (Ortony, Clore, & Collins, 1988). Therefore, uncertainty about what is going to happen constitutes part of
the meaning of anticipatory emotions, in the sense that the feeling of uncertainty per se partially causes the emotion (i.e.,
anticipatory hope in the case of desired events and anticipatory worry in the case of undesired events; see Roseman,
Antoniou, & Jose, 1996; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). In contrast, anticipated emotions do not involve any uncertainty
because they are based on the assumption, through mental simulation, that the future event has already happened or will
not happen. The person imagines how good or bad it would feel to experience certain outcomes, given that the imagined
future event has actually occurred. To the extent that likelihood comes into play, it is distinct from the emotion itself (‘How
would I feel if X happened?’). This is similar to the distinction of valuation and expectation found in expectancy-value
models.
Second, anticipatory emotions are always currently experienced, phenomenologically real affective responses to
possible future events that have positive or negative implications for the self. For instance, one may worry about the
prospect of losing one’s job because of rumors of an impending plant closure. In contrast, anticipated emotions are based
on pre-factual thinking about imagined positive or negative consequences. Although they could be real emotional
experiences based on vivid visualizations of possible futures, often they are predictions, or affective forecasts (e.g.,
Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley, 1998; Wilson & Gilbert, 2003), about one’s future emotions based on
imagined states of the world and their personal implications. For instance, one may visualize having just won the lottery
and anticipate the joy one would feel in this situation, without experiencing other components of emotions such as
emotivational goals and action tendencies (Frijda, 2004; Roseman et al., 1996).
Third, the range of anticipatory emotions is smaller than the range of anticipated emotions. Anticipatory emotions are a
specific subset of all discrete emotions that people may experience, namely, those related to the prospect of future events
(expectant promises and impending dangers). In contrast, in the case of anticipated emotions, any discrete emotion that
can be experienced may be anticipated in advance, based on a mental simulation of future outcomes. For example, a person
may anticipate satisfaction when imagining a positive outcome, guilt when visualizing a blameworthy action, or regret
when imagining a decision gone awry.
Based on these distinctions, we propose that hope and fear are the prototypical categories of positive and negative
anticipatory emotions, respectively (Ortony et al., 1988). Hope reflects pleasure about the prospect of a desired future
event. Specific emotional facets are, for example, anticipatory excitement and feelings of optimism, confidence, or
relaxation (Carver & Scheier, 2002; Ortony et al., 1988; Plutchik, 1980; Snyder & Lopez, 2002). In contrast, fear indicates
displeasure about the prospect of an undesired future event (Lazarus, 1991). Specific facets include worry, anxiety,
nervousness, and tension or stress (Averill, Catlin, & Chon, 1990; Ortony et al., 1988; Plutchik, 1980; Shaver, Schwartz,

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Future-oriented emotions 687

Kirson, & O’Connor, 1987). Anticipation (hope) and fear are opposite in Plutchik’s (1980) circumplex model of primary
emotions.
Although in principle any experienced emotion can be anticipated, some emotions are more likely than others to
function as anticipated emotions in goal-directed behavior. Using the framework developed by Ortony et al. (1988), we
propose the following anticipated emotions as relevant for goal-directed behavior. First, some emotions are experienced
when a prediction about a desired or undesired future event is confirmed or disconfirmed. Although these emotions usually
occur when an event actually happens, based on pre-factual thinking such confirmations or disconfirmations can also be
anticipated. For example, anticipated satisfaction reflects confirmed expectations about a desirable future, anticipated
disappointment reflects disconfirmed expectations about a desirable future, anticipated fears-confirmed reflects confirmed
expectations about an undesirable future, and anticipated relief reflects disconfirmed expectations about an undesirable
future. Second, there are event-based emotions, such as joy and distress, for which prospects are not relevant but outcomes
are important. These emotional reactions may be anticipated as well. Third, since our interest is in the behavioral effects of
future-oriented emotions and since simulations of future events may include visualizing what one has or has not done to
bring about or avert certain consequences, affective responses indicating approval or disapproval of one’s actions, such as
pride or guilt, may be experienced or imagined as well.
In sum, with respect to anticipatory emotions, people at present experience emotions due to the likelihood that a
desirable or undesirable event may happen in the future. With respect to anticipated emotions, people at present imagine
the emotions they would experience in the future under the assumption that a desirable or undesirable event has happened.
Because of our aim to differentiate anticipatory from anticipated emotions, both conceptually and empirically, the focus in
the empirical part will be on positively and negatively valenced emotions, rather than on specific, discrete emotions.
However, instead of using general feelings measures such as semantic differential scales (e.g., Richard et al., 1996), we
will employ specific (unipolar) emotion terms, derived from the previous discussion, to cover the spectrum of affective
reactions that people may experience or anticipate due to future events.

MOTIVATIONAL EFFECTS OF FUTURE-ORIENTED EMOTIONS

The predicted effects of anticipatory emotions on behavior derive from the motivational tendencies associated with fear
and hope. With regard to hope, Lazarus (1991) argues that, although a clear action tendency cannot easily be specified for
hope, approaching and staying committed to the desired outcome might result from hope. Research by Averill et al. (1990)
on the action rules of hope supports this, indicating that ‘[p]eople who hope should be willing to take appropriate action to
achieve their goals, if action is possible’ (p. 34). Participants reported that the primary action taken as a consequence of
hope was working harder and being more persistent in one’s effort, and this was especially the case for participants who
experienced personal control over the situation. Based on this, we predict that positive anticipatory emotions will
encourage the formation of behavioral intentions aimed at bringing about a desirable future state.
With regard to fear, researchers agree that fear engenders a desire to escape from or avoid an aversive state (Frijda,
Kuipers, & ter Schure, 1989; Lazarus, 1991; Öhman, 2000; Plutchik, 1980; Roseman et al., 1996). In particular, the large
body of work on fear-arousing persuasive messages supports the notion that an increase in fear (as assessed by both
manipulations and measures of fear) prompts danger-control strategies, which have a positive influence on attitudes,
intentions, and behavior (for reviews see Boster & Mongeau, 1984; Milne, Sheeran, & Orbell, 2000; and Sutton, 1982).
Therefore, we hypothesize that negative anticipatory emotions will lead to the formation of behavioral intentions aimed at
avoiding undesirable future states.
Previous research is consistent with the idea that anticipated emotions can motivate goal-directed behavior as well.
Within the context of Fishbein and Ajzen’s (1975) theory of reasoned action and its variants (e.g., Ajzen, 1991), Parker,
Manstead, and Stradling (1995), Perugini and Bagozzi (2001), and Richard et al. (1995, 1996) have shown that anticipated
emotions increase intentions, behavioral expectations, or desires to act, even when controlling for other determinants of
behavior such as attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, or past behavior. For example, in an influential
study by Richard et al. (1995) participants who anticipated negative emotions about having had sex without using a
condom indicated significantly higher expectations to use condoms than those who anticipated less negative feelings. In a
similar fashion, but using cognitive appraisal theories of emotions as a theoretical guide, Bagozzi et al. (1998) presented

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
688 Hans Baumgartner et al.

evidence that both positive and negative anticipated emotions evoked by imagined success or failure experiences in goal
pursuit increased volitions (plans, intentions, and effort) to engage in weight control.1
Motivational effects of specific, discrete anticipated emotions have been documented as well, in particular for
anticipated regret. For example, Abraham and Sheeran (2004) showed that anticipated regret predicted intentions to
engage in health-enhancing behavior, even after controlling for other determinants.
Finally, the mental simulation literature also suggests that imagining events often evokes strong emotions and that the
action tendencies intrinsically linked to these emotions may be responsible for the beneficial behavioral effects of
visualizing the future. As Taylor and Pham put it, the emotions may ‘provide the fuel for bringing about effective action’
(1996, p. 232). One particular mechanism is based on what Gleicher et al. (1995) call prefactuals. Prefactuals are like
counterfactuals, except that they refer to imagined future events. For example, a person who plans to lose weight may
imagine having been unsuccessful in losing weight, and the anticipated negative emotions associated with this failure may
stimulate coping behaviors to prevent this letdown. Gleicher et al. (1995) argue that the particular structure of prefactuals
may give them some advantages over other types of (outcome) simulations. Similar to outcome simulations in general, the
consequent of a pre-factual refers to a desired or undesired outcome associated with affect, but in addition the antecedent
often specifies the behaviors responsible for the outcome (e.g., If I had done X, Y would have happened). In this way,
prefactuals combine a motivating outcome with a specific course of action that promises to lead to that outcome. Since
anticipated emotions include the required actions to produce them, they may increase the likelihood of performing them.
In summary, we argue for a conceptual distinction between two types of future-oriented emotions, anticipatory and
anticipated. The prototypical anticipatory emotions are hope and fear, which are ‘real’ emotions that are
phenomenologically experienced in the present due to the prospect of desired or undesired future events, with
uncertainty being a key ingredient in the experience of the emotion. In contrast, anticipated emotions can include any
discrete emotion that may be experienced in response to the assumed (certain) occurrence of a desired or undesired future
event. Uncertainty is not part of the experience of the emotion, and frequently the emotion is ‘virtual’ in the sense that it
consists of one’s forecast of an affective reaction to a future scenario only, without the thoughts, feelings, emotivational
goals, and action tendencies that accompany ‘real’ emotions (Frijda, 2004). We also argue that both types of emotions can
motivate future behavior and that they independently influence goal-directed behavior through their influence on
behavioral intentions. We now describe a study that was designed to establish the distinct empirical status of anticipatory
and anticipated emotions and test their separate effects on behavior.

METHOD

The data were collected in the context of the millennium transition in 1999–2000 (the so-called Y2K problem). Prior to the
date change, there was widespread concern and uncertainty about the consequences of rolling over the digits from ‘99’ to
‘00’ in the world’s computers, and people were advised to ‘[h]ope for the best, but plan for the worst’ (Hyatt, 1998, p. 208).
This made the millennium transition an ideal context for studying future-oriented emotions.

Participants

A total of 812 members of the CentER Data panel at Tilburg University participated in a two-wave longitudinal study.
Panel members, representative of the Dutch population, received and returned questionnaires on their personal computers.
The 812 participants were randomly assigned to a longitudinal group (n ¼ 472) and a control group (n ¼ 340). The control
group was used to establish the reliability as well as convergent and discriminant validity of positive and negative
anticipatory and anticipated emotions. Therefore, control-group respondents were included in the first wave only and
completed only the items about emotions, as described below. The longitudinal group was used to establish the predictive
validity of positive and negative anticipatory and anticipated emotions. Therefore, respondents in the longitudinal group
participated in both waves and completed all questions. Using two separate groups to establish convergent and
1
Bagozzi et al. (1998) do not draw a distinction between anticipatory and anticipated emotions and seem to use the two terms interchangeably. However,
their conceptualization and particularly operationalization of goal-directed emotions is most consistent with the present notion of anticipated emotions.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Future-oriented emotions 689

discriminant validity on the one hand and predictive validity on the other hand is conservative, and minimizes the
likelihood of overusing the data.

Design and Measures

The first wave of data collection was during the first week of November 1999, about 7 weeks prior to the millennium
change. After a brief description of the millennium transition and a screening question to make sure that everybody had
heard about the issue (all had), we collected data on the following constructs in the following order: likelihood of various
negative outcomes; anticipatory emotions; anticipated emotions; behavioral intentions to prepare for the millennium
change; and future (trait) anxiety. Control-group participants responded only to the emotion questions.

Likelihood of Negative Outcomes

Participants rated how likely it was that the millennium change would have various negative outcomes for the Netherlands
and for them personally during the first few days of 2000. This allows us to test the prediction that anticipatory emotions,
but not anticipated emotions, are dependent on likelihood appraisals. In addition, it allows us to examine whether
anticipatory and anticipated emotions motivate behavior, over and above the important cognitive factor of likelihood
estimates. A 7-point scale with endpoints of ‘very unlikely’ (1) and ‘very likely’ (7) was used for each of the likelihood
items. Based on a review of the Y2K literature, the likelihood ratings were collected for seven specific areas: (1) airplane,
car, train, tram, and ferry travel (e.g., traffic jams); (2) financial business (e.g., problems with withdrawing or depositing
cash); (3) government agencies and health care (e.g., unavailability of medical information about patients); (4) supply of
stores (e.g., shortages of certain products); (5) supply of natural gas, water, electricity, cable, etc. (e.g., black-outs); (6)
communication (e.g., problems with telephone); and (7) road travel (e.g., accidents). In addition, there were two overall
questions about the likelihood of negative outcomes, one for the Dutch economy and society in general and one for
participants personally.

Anticipatory Emotions

Participants rated how worried, anxious, and uncomfortable (for negative anticipatory emotions) and how optimistic and
confident (for positive anticipatory emotions; see Ortony et al., 1988; Shaver et al., 1987) they were that the millennium
transition would have (no) negative outcomes for them personally during the first days of 2000. The ratings were collected
using 7-point response scales with endpoints of ‘not at all’ (1) and ‘very much’ (7).

Anticipated Emotions

Participants were asked to imagine that it was the first week of 2000 and that the millennium change had caused certain
problems in the Netherlands. For positive anticipated emotions, participants were told to assume that they had succeeded
in avoiding or limiting the negative outcomes caused by the millennium change because of what they had done (thus
combining process and outcome simulation; see Taylor & Pham, 1996), and they indicated how relieved, satisfied, happy,
and proud they would feel (Ortony et al., 1988; Bagozzi et al., 1998). For negative anticipated emotions, participants were
told to assume that they had done nothing to avoid or limit the negative outcomes of the millennium transition, and they
indicated how disappointed, annoyed, regretful, stupid, guilty, and angry with themselves they would feel because of it
(Bagozzi et al., 1998; Ortony et al., 1988). The ratings were collected using 7-point scales with endpoints of ‘not at all’ (1)
and ‘very much’ (7).

Behavioral Intentions

Participants were asked to indicate, using 7-point scales ranging from ‘certainly not’ (1) to ‘certainly so’ (7), whether they
would do certain things in order to avoid or limit the possible negative outcomes caused by the millennium transition. The

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
690 Hans Baumgartner et al.

behaviors, derived from the Y2K literature (e.g., Hyatt, 1998), were: (1) getting extra cash to be able to make payments; (2)
building an extra supply of batteries, candles, fire wood, propane gas, and water; (3) building an extra supply of food
products, such as coffee, sugar, bread, potatoes, and cheese; (4) having someone check whether one’s computer or other
electronic equipment at home can cause trouble; (5) taking days off from work during the first week of January; (6) making
extra copies of important documents, such as medical information; (7) avoiding traveling by car, plane, or public
transportation during the first few days of 2000; and (8) checking whether one’s banks and insurance companies have taken
the necessary precautions. In addition, there was an overall behavioral intention question that asked how much participants
were planning to do in order to avoid or limit the expected problems caused by the millennium transition. The response
alternatives on this question ranged from ‘do nothing’ (1) to ‘do very much’ (7).

Future Anxiety

To control for individual differences in dispositional negative anticipatory emotions, we included a 12-items short-form
version of the Zaleski’s (1996) scale of future anxiety (e.g., ‘I am afraid that some catastrophe will soon occur’).
Participants provided the ratings on a scale from ‘decidedly false’ (1) to ‘decidedly true’ (7). The inclusion of trait anxiety
allows us to examine how anticipatory emotions are related to dispositional determinants, and whether future-oriented
emotional states motivate behavior over and above a theoretically related dispositional factor.
The second wave of data collection was administered in early January during the weekend following the millennium
change, 2 months after the first wave. The behavioral data were collected at this stage.

Behavior

Participants indicated how much they had done prior to January 1st in order to avoid or limit the negative outcomes of the
millennium transition. Participants considered their behavior in the same eight areas that had been used in the behavioral
intention questions in the first wave, and they provided their answers on 7-point scales ranging from ‘not at all’ (1) to ‘very
much’ (7). They also assessed their behavior overall, using a scale with endpoints of ‘done nothing’ (1) to ‘done very
much’ (7).

RESULTS

Structure of Anticipatory and Anticipated Emotions: Convergent and Discriminant Validity

We specified a confirmatory factor model, using the data of the control group (n ¼ 340), to investigate the validity and
reliability of measurement of the four future-oriented emotions factors: positive and negative anticipatory emotions, and
positive and negative anticipated emotions (see Figure 1). The analysis was conducted with LISREL 8.8, using the
Satorra-Bentler scaled chi-square statistic and robust standard errors to correct for non-normality of the data. The fit of the
four-factor model was adequate, with x2(84) ¼ 242.69, CFI ¼ .97, RMSEA ¼ .075 (90 per cent confidence interval from
.064 to .086), SRMR ¼ .038. These values exceed, or are close to, the standards commonly suggested in the literature (i.e.,
CFI should exceed .95, RMSEA should be below .06, and SRMR should be smaller than .08; see Hu & Bentler, 1999). All
loadings on target factors were highly significant (smallest t-value ¼ 12.90), and the average completely standardized
factor loading was .85 (smallest loading ¼ .72). The largest (absolute) non-target loading was .11 (based on the expected
parameter change statistic). The coefficient alpha reliabilities were as follows: positive anticipatory emotions .72; negative
anticipatory emotions .93; positive anticipated emotions .92; negative anticipated emotions .94. These findings support the
conclusion that the four types of future-oriented emotions were measured reliably.
The means of the items measuring each emotion factor show that participants, on average, were hopeful that nothing
would happen (5.50 on a 7-point scale) and did not worry much about possible negative outcomes (1.95). The intensities of
positive and negative anticipated emotions were around the midpoint of the scale (4.44 and 3.45, respectively).

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Future-oriented emotions 691

Figure 1. Factor model for future-oriented emotions. Note: Hypothesized constructs are shown as ellipses. Figures underneath
non-directed arcs between ellipses refer to correlations between the constructs, with robust standard errors in parentheses. Emotion
words below the ellipses refer to the items used to measure each construct. Figures in parentheses are factor loadings from the
completely standardized solution, in which both the factors and observed variables are standardized to a variance of 1

Importantly, the pattern of correlations between the four emotion factors further supports the distinction between the
two types of future-oriented emotions. Specifically, positive and negative anticipatory emotions were strongly negatively
correlated (.70, p < .001), demonstrating that respondents who were hopeful and confident that nothing would happen
also felt little fear or worry about possible negative outcomes of Y2K. On the other hand, the correlation between positive
and negative anticipated emotions was positive (.30, p < .001), revealing that these two emotions were coordinated, due to
the overall goal to attain either good outcomes or avoid bad outcomes in the mental simulations. In addition, we also found
that negative anticipatory and anticipated emotions were positively correlated (.35, p < .001), which suggests that the more
worried people were about Y2K (anticipatory), the worse they felt about the imagined negative consequences of the
millennium problem in case they were to act ineffectively (anticipated). Alternatively, people who forecast feeling bad
about assumed negative consequences were more worried. All other correlations were non-significant at the 5 per cent
level (see Figure 1 for the specific results).2 Overall, these findings demonstrate the reliability and convergent and
discriminant validity of the two types of future-oriented emotions.

Behavioral Effects of Future-Oriented Emotions: Predictive Validity

The structural model, tested on the longitudinal group (n ¼ 472), is shown in Figure 2. Of primary interest are the direct
effects of positive/negative anticipatory and anticipated emotions on behavioral intentions. The prediction is that all four
emotions will independently encourage the formation of intentions to engage in behavior aimed at averting possible
negative consequences of the millennium transition. We did not expect direct effects of the future-oriented emotions on
actual behavior since the effects of emotions on behavior are generally mediated by volitional variables such as behavioral
intentions (e.g., Bagozzi et al., 1998). However, because we do predict a positive effect of intentions on behavior, we
expect and test for indirect effects of future-oriented emotions on behavior (as mediated by behavioral intentions), and
explore the possibility of direct effects of future-oriented emotions on behavior.
2
Separate analyses for males (n ¼ 181) and females (n ¼ 159) revealed the same factor structure, loadings, correlations, and means for both genders.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
692 Hans Baumgartner et al.

Figure 2. Antecedents and consequences of future-oriented emotions. Note: Correlations between the four types of future-oriented
emotions (i.e., correlations between the error terms of the four emotions) are not shown for simplicity

In addition to the constructs of primary interest, the proposed model also included two additional constructs (likelihood
of negative consequences and future anxiety) that served as control variables. Since future anxiety is a stable emotional
trait, it was specified as an antecedent of the more temporary future-oriented emotion factors. Consistent with the earlier
discussion of the role of uncertainty in the experience of future-oriented emotions, we also expected that the intensity of
anticipatory emotions (but not the intensity of anticipated emotions, because uncertainty is not part of the meaning
structure for these emotions) would be a function of the judged likelihood of negative outcomes. Finally, likelihood of
negative consequences and future anxiety were allowed to directly influence behavioral intentions.3
Preliminary analyses indicated that the individual intention and behavior measures were internally consistent
(coefficient alphas of .83 and .72, respectively), so to simplify the analysis they were averaged to form overall intention
and behavior composites (Bagozzi & Heatherton, 1994). The reliabilities of likelihood of negative outcomes and future
anxiety were also adequate (coefficient alphas of .91 and .85, respectively). In addition, a confirmatory factor analysis of
the emotion measures for this longitudinal group confirmed the findings of the analysis for the control group that positive
and negative variants of the two types of future-oriented emotions can be assessed validly and reliably. The overall fit
measures were acceptable (x2 (84) ¼ 251.40; CFI ¼ .98; RMSEA ¼ .065, with a 90 per cent confidence interval from .056
to .074; SRMR ¼ .037), and the coefficient alpha reliabilities were as follows: positive anticipatory emotions .79; negative
anticipatory emotions .93; positive anticipated emotions .91; negative anticipated emotions .95. As in the control group
analysis, positive and negative anticipatory emotions were correlated negatively (.57, p < .001), and positive and
negative anticipated emotions were correlated positively (.38, p < .001). The correlations between the two positive
emotions (.18, p < .001) and the two negative emotions (.28, p < .001) were also significant.

3
Figure 2 shows the final model selected as the best representation of the data. Initially, we started with a model in which likelihood of negative outcomes
was freely correlated with future anxiety, the four emotion factors were allowed to correlate (by correlating their error terms), anticipatory emotions were
a function of the likelihood of negative consequences and future anxiety, behavioral intentions were a function of the four emotion factors, and behavior
was a function of behavioral intentions. We then respecified the model based on significant modification indices, provided the change made substantive
sense. In response to a suggestion by one of the reviewers, we also performed some additional validation analyses. Specifically, we randomly split the total
sample in half, developed a well-fitting specification using one half as the calibration sample, and then cross-validated the model using the other half. This
procedure confirmed the specification of the model shown in Figure 2.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Future-oriented emotions 693

Table 1. Antecedents and consequences of future-oriented emotions: path analysis results


Unstandardized Standardized
Path path coefficient t-value path coefficient

Likelihood of negative outcomes ! positive anticipatory emotions .27 4.13 .23


Likelihood of negative outcomes ! negative anticipatory emotions .50 11.52 .54
Likelihood of negative outcomes ! behavioral intentions .32 7.15 .31
Future anxiety ! positive anticipatory emotions .21 3.02 .14
Future anxiety ! negative anticipatory emotions .28 5.25 .24
Future anxiety ! negative anticipated emotions .57 7.45 .29
Future anxiety ! behavioral intentions .12 2.01 .09
Positive anticipatory emotions ! behavioral intentions .01 .20 .01
Negative anticipatory emotions ! behavioral intentions .16 2.73 .15
Positive anticipated emotions ! behavioral intentions .10 3.16 .12
Negative anticipated emotions ! behavioral intentions .18 6.50 .27
Behavioral intentions ! behavior .17 6.44 .36
Note: All t-values > 2.0 are significant at p < .05.

The final structural model presented in Figure 2, which includes only the paths that were significant (except for the
hypothesized path from positive anticipatory emotions to behavioral intentions) fit the data well: x2 (9) ¼ 12.08;
CFI ¼ .99; RMSEA ¼ .027; SRMR .031. More importantly, as shown in Table 1, the estimated path coefficients confirm
most of the hypotheses. To begin with, negative anticipatory emotions and positive and negative anticipated emotions all
have positive effects on behavioral intentions. This means that participants who were fearful about possible negative
consequences of the millennium transition and who experienced stronger emotional reactions to a mental simulation of the
positive and negative outcomes of the millennium change also indicated that they were more motivated to engage in
behaviors aimed at averting the threat. Only the predicted positive effect of hope on behavioral intentions was not
empirically supported.
Table 1 also shows that, as expected, the judged likelihood of negative outcomes was negatively related to positive and
positively related to negative anticipatory emotions. In contrast, the likelihood of negative outcomes had no impact on
anticipated emotions (both modification indices were non-significant). Future anxiety had a dampening effect on positive
and an intensifying effect on negative anticipatory emotions. Furthermore, future anxiety increased the intensity of negative
anticipated emotions. Jointly, this demonstrates that dispositional worriers are more likely to experience anticipatory
emotions and also that they are particularly good at imagining or even experiencing negative emotions in response to
simulated future events. Both the likelihood of negative outcomes and future anxiety had direct positive effects on behavioral
intentions, although the magnitude of the latter effect was small. Importantly, the predicted influence of positive and
negative future-oriented emotions emerged even while controlling for a strong cognitive factor (likelihood judgments) and
strong affective trait (future anxiety). Intentions also had the predicted positive effect on self-reported behavior.
The specified model accounts for 38 per cent of the variance in intentions and 13 per cent of the variance in behavior.
Although behavior has only one direct antecedent (behavioral intentions), the indirect effects of likelihood of negative
outcomes (.07, t-value ¼ 5.37), future anxiety (.05, t-value ¼ 3.60), negative anticipatory emotions (.03, t-value ¼ 2.57),
positive anticipated emotions (.02, t-value ¼ 2.94), and negative anticipated emotions (.03, t-value ¼ 4.42) are all
statistically significant. It should be emphasized that behavior was assessed about 2 months after the other variables, so
these relationships are unlikely to be due to consistency biases, which strengthens the findings. Likelihood of negative
outcomes had the largest total effect on behavioral intentions (.40), followed by future anxiety (.27), negative anticipated
emotions (.18), negative anticipatory emotions (.16), and positive anticipated emotions (.10).

DISCUSSION

The present research makes several contributions to the emotion literature, in particular concerning the structure and
motivational implications of emotions. It is, to our knowledge, the first empirical study to establish the construct validity of
positive and negative anticipatory and anticipated emotions, and the first attempt to assess their predictive validity for

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
694 Hans Baumgartner et al.

goal-directed behavior. We identified essential differences between the two types of future-oriented emotions, equating the
positively and negatively valenced forms of anticipatory emotions to hope and fear, respectively, and defining anticipated
emotions as any emotional reaction to mental simulations of future events and behaviors. We proposed that both
anticipatory and anticipated emotions would encourage the formation of intentions to engage in behavior aimed at
bringing about or averting the future events at which these emotions are directed. Most importantly, the empirical analyses
revealed that positively and negatively valenced anticipatory and anticipated emotions can be measured reliably, are
empirically distinct, and relate to other constructs in the hypothesized way. For example, the likelihood that the target
event will occur was shown to influence the experience of anticipatory emotions, but was unrelated, or much more weakly
related, to anticipated emotions. Anticipatory and anticipated emotions were shown to independently motivate
goal-directed behavior, and these effects emerged even while controlling for the likelihood of negative outcomes, an
important cognitive factor, and future anxiety, a relevant emotional disposition. These findings are new and support the
proposed framework of future-oriented emotions.
The analyses also indicated, first, that the motivational effects of anticipated emotions are stronger than those of
anticipatory emotions and, second, that negative emotions are more powerful determinants of behavioral intentions than
positive emotions. With regard to the first point, the combined (absolute) standardized effects of positive and negative
anticipatory emotions on behavioral intentions was .16 (.01 and .15, respectively), whereas the combined effect of
positive and negative anticipated emotions was .39 (.12 and .27, respectively), which is more than twice as large. Future
research will have to explore whether this is a general result or whether there are circumstances in which anticipatory
emotions are a stronger motivator of behavior. Concerning the second point, for both anticipatory and anticipated
emotions, negative affect led to stronger behavioral intentions than positive affect. On the one hand, this result is consistent
with the finding that negative information often has more powerful effects on information processing, persuasion, and
behavior than positive information (negativity effect; see Peeters & Czapinski, 1990; Tversky & Kahneman, 1981). On the
other hand, the future event studied in this research was a threat that people tried to avoid or limit by engaging in
anticipatory coping behavior. Perhaps negative emotions are more important motivators of behavior when the future event
is associated with undesired outcomes, whereas positive emotions might have more potent effects when the future
outcomes are desired, a speculation which follow-up research may test.
In the case of anticipated emotions, the behavioral effects of both positive and negative affect were significant. In
contrast, and different from our predictions, positive anticipatory emotions did not significantly influence behavioral
intentions. We speculate that this is due to the target of the positive anticipatory emotions. Specifically, a person may hope
that the future state of the world will be desirable, or that the outcomes of behavior to cope with the event are desirable
(coping potential or agency thoughts) (Snyder, Rand, & Sigmon, 2000). Hope is expected to have motivational effects if a
person is confident and optimistic that he or she can do something to achieve positive outcomes. In contrast, if a person
hopes that the desired outcomes will occur automatically, there is little incentive to take action (see Oettingen, Pak, &
Schnetter, 2001). To clarify this, consider a person who is ill and hopes that a cure will be found for the illness. This person
is unlikely to do much. On the other hand, a person who hopes that eating healthy food will cure the illness is likely to be
motivated to perform this instrumental behavior. Our present research could not distinguish between potentially different
targets of hope and their behavioral effects, since the emotion was linked to the final outcomes for the person, which might
result directly from the event not producing undesired outcomes or from the positive effects of the coping behavior.
Anticipated emotions, and the underlying process of mental simulation, are in some ways similar to subjective expected
utility models, such as expectancy-value models (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). Thus, one may argue that the utility or value
component in these models is analogous to anticipated emotions. However, while subjective expected utility models are
based on a rational calculus that combines estimates of the likelihood of certain outcomes with an assessment of the utility
of these outcomes, anticipated emotions are not focused on the likelihood that certain outcomes will occur and they are
potentially much more ‘hot’ than the ‘cold’ deliberations about value implicit in subjective expected utility models. In
support of this reasoning, research has shown that anticipated emotions, in particular anticipated regret, has incremental
effects on goal-directed behavior over and above the effects of attitudes and subjective norms, which should mediate the
subjective expected utility effects (Abraham & Sheeran, 2004; Richard et al., 1995, 1996; Zeelenberg & Pieters, 2004).
Our conceptualization of anticipatory and anticipated emotions assumes that the two types of future-oriented emotions
are fuzzy sets characterized by graded membership structure.4 This implies that there are certain emotions that do not fall

4
We thank the editor and reviewers for stimulating this discussion.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Future-oriented emotions 695

neatly into one of the two categories. Following Frijda (2004), we have argued that anticipated emotions are often virtual in
the sense that they capture forecasts of one’s future emotions. However, such mental simulations about future states may
lead to actually experiencing the future emotion at present, when the affective forecast leads to the feelings, thoughts,
emotivational goals, and action tendencies that accompany actual emotions. Consider the situation of contemplating the
joy that one will experience during one’s birthday party, before the first guests arrive. Such a mental simulation might
gladden us, and anticipated joy may lead to experienced joy. This joy about a future event is called ‘Vorfreude’ in German
and ‘voorpret’ in Dutch (literally pre-joy), and it may be translated as pleasant anticipation. Although this emotion shares
some similarities with anticipatory emotions because it is a phenomenologically ‘real’ affective reaction that is
experienced in the present, not an affective forecast of some hypothetical future scenario, we would not classify it as an
anticipatory emotion in our terminology (at least not as an unambiguous example of anticipatory emotions) because
uncertainty is not a defining characteristic of this emotional experience. As another example, consider the emotion of fear
or anxiety. In our conceptualization, fear is the prototype of negative anticipatory emotions. However, fear could also be
experienced as an anticipated emotion. For example, somebody could visualize that a dreaded event has actually occurred
and experience fear as a consequence (‘suppose that the doctor tells me in a week from now that I have a terminal form of
cancer, how much would I fear death?’). Ortony et al. (1988) use the terms fears-confirmed or worst fears realized to refer
to such situations. In cases where these affective reactions are forecast pre-factually, we would classify them as anticipated
emotions. Future research should investigate the conditions that turn affective forecasts into actual emotional experiences,
and whether some individuals are more prone to such transitions than others.
To return to our original inspiration, this research found that positive and negative anticipatory and anticipated
emotions are conceptually and empirically distinct, are driven by different mechanisms, and can independently motivate
goal-directed behavior, even while controlling for important cognitive and emotional trait factors. This reveals the
implications of the experienced and imagined affect about what the future holds in stock for us, and supports the construct
and predictive validity of these distinct types of future-oriented emotions.

REFERENCES

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–210.
Abraham, C., & Sheeran, P. (2004). Deciding to exercise: The role of anticipated regret. British Journal of Health Psychology, 9,
269–278.
Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
Averill, J. R., Catlin, G., & Chon, K. K. (1990). Rules of hope. New York: Springer.
Bagozzi, R. P., Baumgartner, H., & Pieters, R. (1998). Goal-directed emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 1–26.
Bagozzi, R. P., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). A general approach to representing multifaceted personality constructs: Application to state
self-esteem. Structural Equation Modeling, 1, 35–67.
Boster, F. J., & Mongeau, P. (1984). Fear-arousing persuasive messages. In R. N. Bastrow, & B. H. Westley (Eds.), Communication
yearbook (Vol. 8, pp. 330–375). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2002). Optimism. In C. R. Snyder, & S. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 231–243).
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 113, 464–486.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212–228.
Frijda, N. H. (2004). Emotion and action. In A. S. R. Manstead, N. Frijda, & A. Fischer (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam
symposium (pp. 158–173). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in
affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617–638.
Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., Strathman, A., Armor, D., & Ahn, M. (1995). With an eye toward the future: The impact of counterfactual
thinking on affect, attitudes, and behavior. In N. J. Roese, & J. M. Olson (Eds.), What might have been: The social psychology of
counterfactual thinking (pp. 283–304). Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new
alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
Hyatt, M. S. (1998). The millennium bug: How to survive the coming chaos. New York: Broadway Books.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
696 Hans Baumgartner et al.

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Loewenstein, G., & Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith
(Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 619–642). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Milne, S., Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000). Prediction and intervention in health-related behavior: A meta-analytic review of protection
motivation theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 106–143.
Öhman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: Evolutionary, cognitive, and clinical perspectives. In M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.),
Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 573–593). New York: The Guilford Press.
Oettingen, G., Pak, H., & Schnetter, K. (2001). Self-regulation of goal setting: Turning free fantasies about the future into binding goals.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 736–753.
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Parker, D., Manstead, A. S. R., & Stradling, S. G. (1995). Extending the theory of planned behaviour: The role of personal norm. British
Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 127–137.
Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational
negativity effects. European Review of Social Psychology, 1, 33–60.
Perugini, M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2001). The role of desires and anticipated emotions in goal-directed behaviors: Broadening and
deepening the theory of planned behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 79–98.
Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotions: A psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper & Row.
Richard, R., van der Pligt, J., & de Vries, N. (1995). Anticipated affective reactions and prevention of AIDS. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 34, 9–21.
Richard, R., van der Pligt, J., & de Vries, N. (1996). Anticipated affect and behavioral choice. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 18,
111–129.
Roseman, I. J., Antoniou, A. A., & Jose, P. E. (1996). Appraisal determinants of emotions: Constructing a more accurate and
comprehensive theory. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 241–277.
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirston, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061–1086.
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48,
813–838.
Snyder, C. R., & Lopez, S. (2002). Handbook of positive psychology. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Snyder, C. R., Rand, K. L., & Sigmon, D. R. (2000). Hope theory: A member of the positive psychology theory. In C. R. Snyder, &
S. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 257–276). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Sutton, S. R. (1982). Fear-arousing communications: A critical examination of theory and research. In J. R. Eiser (Ed.), Social
psychology and behavioral medicine (pp. 303–337). New York: John Wiley.
Taylor, S. E., & Pham, L. B. (1996). Mental simulation, motivation, and action. In P. M. Gollwitzer, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology
of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 219–235). New York: Guilford.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458.
van der Pligt, J., Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W. W., de Vries, N. K., & Richard, R. (1998). Affect, attitudes and decisions: Let’s be more
specific. In W. Stroebe, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology 8, 34–66.
Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 346–412.
Zeelenberg, M. (1999). Anticipated regret, expected feedback and behavioral decision-making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making,
12, 93–106.
Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (2004). Consequences of regret aversion in real life: The case of the Dutch postcode lottery.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 93, 155–168.
Zaleski, Z. (1996). Future anxiety: Concept, measurement, and preliminary research. Personality and Individual Differences, 21,
165–174.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 685–696 (2008)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp

You might also like