CLIL Learning Theories-3
CLIL Learning Theories-3
Jerome Bruner (1986), drawing on Vygotsky’s work, promoted the idea of discovery
learning. For Bruner, the learning process is the acquisition of new knowledge, and
the checking of that new information against a new situation. Learning is therefore an
active process and requires active learners who learn by doing. For Bruner, and
other constructivists, the concept of scaffolding is extremely important within this
process of active learning. Scaffolding is the support given to a learner to help them
carry out activities and solve problems; it is gradually taken away so that learners
can eventually work without it. Examples of scaffolding include: simplifying tasks by
breaking them down into smaller steps, keeping pupils focused on completing the
task by reminding them of what the goal is, and showing other ways of doing tasks.
In a CLIL context, scaffolding also includes support strategies for language
production. Examples of this are the use of speaking and writing frames to support
spoken and written language production.
2
Activity 2.3: CLIL and learning theory
in the brain to connect with each other in order to create a network encoding new
learning. For this new learning to become deep learning it needs to be connected
with existing neural networks.
3
Activity 2.3: CLIL and learning theory
2. Bilingualism
However, as Shatz and Wilkinson (2013, pp. 33-34) point out, concepts underlying
language expressions in different languages can vary so that translation equivalents
do not always exists. This makes learning new vocabulary more demanding than
might be recognised by teachers. However, drawing attention to these differences
can also help learners to relate different concepts across languages.
Shatz and Wilkinson (2013) also give a helpful overview of recent research into
bilingualism. For example, some of this research has studied the time required by
bilinguals to process language. According to this research, whilst processing of
4
Activity 2.3: CLIL and learning theory
language takes longer for bilinguals, this is not a sign of language deficiency.
Likewise, research into code switching by bilinguals (the moving between two or
more languages, sometimes within a single sentence) is not a sign of language
impairment.
Shatz and Wilkinson (2013, pp. 34-38) also summarise recent research that points to
advantages of bilingualism:
5
Activity 2.3: CLIL and learning theory
Second Language Acquisition (SLA) theory refers to the study of individuals and
groups who are learning a second or additional language to their first language
(referred to as the L1). This second language is referred in SLA theory as the L2 or
target language.
A useful overview of four areas of SLA theory that are relevant for CLIL teachers to
know about is provided by Dale, van der Es, and Tanner (2010, pp. 20-21). These
areas are: input theory, intake theory, output theory, and social models of second
language acquisition.
Language input theories of SLA emphasize that the input given to learners, the
language they read and hear needs to be meaningful, relevant and realistic. It should
also be at a slightly higher level than the current level of the learner (i+1, with i
representing the current language level). Input theory also recognises that learners
will be able to understand more language than they are able to produce in the target
language. Intake theory also focuses on the input learners receive but places more
focus on the need for learners to encounter the L2 frequently, as well as the time
needed to process this language.
Output theory argues that learners have to produce the second language in order to
learn it. By speaking or writing learners can then notice the difference between what
they can produce and what they want to produce. Furthermore, output theory
emphasizes the need for learners to be creative and make mistakes with the second
language. Experimenting with language production is key to learning the L2
according to output theories of SLA.
In recent years there has been a shift in SLA research from focusing on individual
processes of language acquisition to a wider social and cultural perspective on
language development. According to this view, learning a second language is not a
process taking place within the individual mind of a learner but happens as a result
of the social interactions between learners and ‘the kinds of identities these activities
6
Activity 2.3: CLIL and learning theory
make possible’ (Llinares, Morton and Whittaker, 2012, p. 12). This area of SLA
theory sees language learning as a social process.
7
Activity 2.3: CLIL and learning theory
References
Bruner, J. (1986) Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Dale, L., van der Es, W. and Tanner, R. (2010) CLIL skills. Leiden: ICLON, Leiden
University.
Llinares, A., Morton, T. and Whittaker, R. (2012) The roles of language in CLIL.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Petty, G. (2014) Teaching today: A practical guide. Fifth Edition, Oxford: Oxford
University Press.