p2 Bul - 146 - 4 - 279
p2 Bul - 146 - 4 - 279
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 146, No. 4, 279 –323
ISSN: 0033-2909 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000226
This study reports the most comprehensive assessment to date of the relations that the domains and facets
of Big Five and HEXACO personality have with self-reported subjective well-being (SWB: life
satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect) and psychological well-being (PWB: positive relations,
autonomy, environmental mastery, purpose in life, self-acceptance, and personal growth). It presents a
meta-analysis (n ⫽ 334,567, k ⫽ 462) of the correlations of Big Five and HEXACO personality domains
with the dimensions of SWB and PWB. It provides the first meta-analysis of personality and well-being
to examine (a) HEXACO personality, (b) PWB dimensions, and (c) a broad range of established Big Five
measures. It also provides the first robust synthesis of facet-level correlations and incremental prediction
by facets over domains in relation to SWB and PWB using 4 large data sets comprising data from
prominent, long-form hierarchical personality frameworks: NEO PI-R (n ⫽ 1,673), IPIP-NEO (n ⫽ 903),
HEXACO PI-R (n ⫽ 465), and Big Five Aspect Scales (n ⫽ 706). Meta-analytic results highlighted the
importance of Big Five neuroticism, extraversion, and conscientiousness. The pattern of correlations
between Big Five personality and SWB was similar across personality measures (e.g., BFI, NEO, IPIP,
BFAS, Adjectives). In the HEXACO model, extraversion was the strongest well-being correlate.
Facet-level analyses provided a richer description of the relationship between personality and well-being,
and clarified differences between the two trait frameworks. Prediction by facets was typically around
20% better than domains, and this incremental prediction was larger for some well-being dimensions than
others.
Keywords: Big Five, HEXACO, personality facets, psychological well-being, subjective well-being
Decades of research shows that personality traits play a critical Schmidt, & Shultz, 2008). Many researchers assess the good life in
role in how we experience, approach, and appraise our lives terms of subjective well-being (SWB): a composite of life satis-
(DeNeve & Cooper, 1998; Headey & Wearing, 1989; Steel, faction, high levels of positive affect, and low levels of negative
This article was published Online First January 16, 2020. Data, scripts, materials, and supplemental analyses are available at
X Jeromy Anglim and Sharon Horwood, School of Psychology, Deakin https://osf.io/42rsy.
University; Luke D. Smillie, Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, We are grateful to Jessie Sun and Ingo Zettler for their valuable feedback
University of Melbourne; Rosario J. Marrero, Department of Clinical on an initial draft of this article.
Psychology, Psychobiology, and Methodology, Faculty of Psychology, Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jeromy
University of La Laguna; X Joshua K. Wood, School of Psychology, Anglim, School of Psychology, Deakin University, Locked Bag 20000,
Deakin University. Geelong, 3220 Australia. E-mail: [email protected]
279
280 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
affect (Diener, 1984). Whereas SWB largely avoids making as- research provides the most comprehensive assessment yet of how
sumptions about the causes of happiness, other conceptualizations personality traits are linked to indices of human flourishing.
of well-being draw more strongly on eudaimonic and humanistic
perspectives in conceptualizing well-being (Waterman, 1993). In Subjective and Psychological Well-Being
particular, the six-dimensional model of psychological well-being
(PWB) identifies a broader set of well-being dimensions, compris- Whereas previous studies have adopted a range of different
ing positive relations, autonomy, environmental mastery, personal perspectives on well-being (Diener & Choi, 2009; Diener, Oishi, &
growth, purpose in life, and self-acceptance (Ryff, 1989). Previous Lucas, 2003; Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999; Lucas & Diener,
research shows that major dimensions of personality are robustly 2008), we focus on the complementary perspectives of SWB and
associated with both SWB and PWB, along with other indices of PWB. Several decades ago, Ed Diener and colleagues operation-
human happiness (e.g., Anglim & Grant, 2016; Sun, Kaufman, & alized SWB as high life satisfaction combined with high levels of
Smillie, 2018). positive affect and low levels of negative affect (Deci & Ryan,
To date, most research examining the personality correlates of 2008; Diener, 1984; Lucas, Diener, & Suh, 1996). Contrastingly,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SWB has focused on the Big Five (DeNeve & Cooper, 1998; Steel Carol Ryff and colleagues have operationalized PWB using a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
et al., 2008). These five broad domains of personality emerged six-dimensional framework comprising positive relations, auton-
from decades of research seeking to identify the major lines of omy, environmental mastery, personal growth, purpose in life, and
covariation among trait terms, and provide a robust organizing self-acceptance (McGregor & Little, 1998; Ryan & Deci, 2001;
framework for personality psychology as a whole (Anglim & Ryff & Keyes, 1995). Definitions and example items for all of
O’Connor, 2019; John & Srivastava, 1999). However, the Big Five these dimensions are depicted in Table 1. Although all nine well-
domains do not provide—nor were they ever intended to pro- being dimensions have moderate to large intercorrelations, they
vide—a complete description of personality. Personality traits can each appear to capture discrete aspects of well-being (Anglim &
be hierarchically arranged at multiple levels both above (e.g., Grant, 2016; Sun et al., 2018).
Anusic, Schimmack, Pinkus, & Lockwood, 2009; DeYoung, 2006; Despite the influence of situational factors on short-term fluc-
Digman, 1997; Musek, 2007; Veselka et al., 2009) and below (e.g., tuation in mood, and the longer-term impact that significant life
events appear to have on well-being—for example, marital tran-
Costa & McCrae, 1995; DeYoung, Quilty, & Peterson, 2007;
sition (Lucas, Clark, Georgellis, & Diener, 2003), acquiring a
Mõttus, Kandler, Bleidorn, Riemann, & McCrae, 2017; Mõttus,
disability (Lucas, 2007), or approaching death (Gerstorf et al.,
McCrae, Allik, & Realo, 2014) the five broad domains. In addi-
2008)—measures of well-being otherwise appear very stable over
tion, a prominent alternative to the Big Five, the six-factor
time (Fujita & Diener, 2005; Schimmack & Oishi, 2005). For
HEXACO model (Ashton, Lee, & De Vries, 2014), has received
example, in a recent, large panel study, Anglim, Weinberg, and
increasing interest and support. Researchers have thus begun to
Cummins (2015) obtained 8-year test–retest correlations for life
expand knowledge of the relation between personality and well-
satisfaction approaching .80. Furthermore, twin studies suggest
being by shifting to different levels in the personality trait hierar-
that SWB is reasonably heritable (Weiss, Bates, & Luciano, 2008).
chy within the Big Five, as well as within the HEXACO frame-
For example, in a large sample of Norwegian Twins, Røysamb, et
work (Aghababaei & Arji, 2014; Anglim & Grant, 2016; Marrero
al. (2018) found the twin-cotwin correlations for life satisfaction
Quevedo & Carballeira Abella, 2011; Schimmack, Oishi, Furr, & for monozygotic twins (r ⫽ .31) was much larger than for dizy-
Funder, 2004; Sun et al., 2018). gotic twins (r ⫽ .15). Grounded in the idea of the “hedonic
To strengthen and consolidate this emerging research, we aim to treadmill” (Brickman & Campbell, 1971), various set-point theo-
address several fundamental gaps in the literature. First, despite ries have been proposed to explain these findings. From this
meta-analytic work relating the Big Five domains to SWB (De- perspective, well-being is a homeostatic process that fluctuates
Neve & Cooper, 1998; Steel et al., 2008), no equivalent meta- around a relatively stable set-point (Cummins, 2015; Headey &
analysis has examined how the Big Five relates to PWB, or how Wearing, 1989, 1992). People differ in their set-points, and per-
the HEXACO model relates to either SWB or PWB. Second, the sonality describes the dispositional mechanisms that influence how
meta-analysis of Steel et al. (2008) focused exclusively on the people experience and perceive the world, which in turn influences
NEO and the meta-analysis of DeNeve and Cooper (1998) largely set-point dynamics (Headey & Wearing, 1989, 1992).
relied on categorizing personality measures that predated the Big
Five. Third, existing research examining facets of the Big Five and
their incremental prediction of well-being above and beyond the Descriptive Models of Personality Traits
Big Five domains suffers from several methodological limitations, Personality traits describe relatively stable patterns of affect,
including small sample sizes, biased statistics, invalid meta- cognition, and behavior. The early history of research on person-
analytically derived correlation matrices, and incomplete reporting ality traits was characterized by a huge proliferation of trait con-
(see the section below on Incremental Prediction for details; for a structs and scales to measure them. Subsequently, emerging from
critical review, see Anglim & Grant, 2014). Fourth, there has been the lexical tradition in the United States, the Big Five traits of
no robust examination of how facets of the HEXACO model map neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscien-
to dimensions of well-being. To address these gaps, we present a tiousness has functioned as a powerful synthesizing framework
meta-analysis that synthesizes the existing literature, and a sys- (Costa & MacCrae, 1992; Goldberg, 1993; McCrae & John, 1992).
tematic examination of the data sets with the largest sample sizes However, the Big Five is not the only game in town. In particular,
that have examined facet-level associations of Big Five and the six-factor HEXACO model, derived from the same lexical
HEXACO frameworks with both SWB and PWB. We believe this approach but in different (European and East Asian) language
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 281
Table 1
Components and Sample Items for Personality, SWB, and PWB
Big Five
Neuroticism Facets: Anxiety, Hostility, Depression, Self-consciousness, Impulsiveness, Vulnerability to Stress
Aspects: Withdrawal, Volatility
Extraversion Facets: Warmth, Gregariousness, Assertiveness, Activity, Excitement Seeking, Positive Emotion
Aspects: Enthusiasm, Assertiveness
Openness Facets: Fantasy, Aesthetics, Feelings, Actions, Ideas, Values
Aspects: Openness/Creativity, Intellect
Agreeableness Facets: Trust, Straightforwardness, Altruism, Compliance, Modesty, Tendermindedness
Aspects: Politeness, Compassion
Conscientiousness Facets: Competence, Order, Dutifulness, Achievement Striving, Self-Discipline, Deliberation
Aspects: Orderliness, Industriousness
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
HEXACO
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
groups, has emerged as a prominent alternative to the Big Five (see negative emotions of anxiety and fearfulness as well as more
Ashton et al., 2004; De Raad et al., 2014; Lee & Ashton, 2004; neutral emotional tendencies such as dependence and sentimental-
Saucier, 2009). HEXACO is an acronym for the six broad traits of ity. In general, conscientiousness, openness, and extraversion in
honesty-humility, emotionality, extraversion, agreeableness, con- the HEXACO framework are notionally close analogues to their
scientiousness, and openness. Big Five equivalents (e.g., cross-correlations all above .75 for the
There are strong similarities but also important differences NEO-PI R, Gaughan et al., 2012).
between the Big Five and the HEXACO models (Ashton & Lee, Both Big Five and HEXACO models are hierarchical frame-
2005; Ashton et al., 2014; Gaughan, Miller, & Lynam, 2012; works, where each broad domain is characterized by a set of
Ludeke et al., 2019). In particular, Big Five agreeableness and narrower traits or facets (see Table 1; for discussion see Anglim &
neuroticism are repartitioned in the HEXACO model to form the O’Connor, 2019). In the context of the Big Five, a range of
three domains of honesty-humility, agreeableness, and emotional- facet-level frameworks have been proposed (e.g., Soto & John,
ity. Honesty-humility, characterized by integrity and modesty, is 2017), but the most popular hierarchical framework in research
negatively correlated with antisocial personality traits (e.g., within settings has been the NEO Model which characterizes the Big Five
the Dark Triad framework; Lee & Ashton, 2014) and positively in terms of 30 facets (Costa & McCrae, 1995). This model can be
correlated with the modesty and straightforwardness facets from measured using the NEO PI-R, NEO PI-3, or the IPIP NEO (a
Big Five agreeableness (Ashton & Lee, 2005). HEXACO agree- public domain equivalent). More recently, an intermediate level
ableness captures patience, forgiveness, and a disposition to not between facets and domains has been proposed, whereby each Big
experience anger toward others. Emotionality includes both the Five domain is divided into two trait ‘aspects’ (DeYoung et al.,
282 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
2007). Unlike the facets of the Big Five, the aspects were derived DeNeve and Cooper (1998). However, Steel and colleagues re-
empirically, informed by quantitative genetic models and other stricted their focus to NEO personality measures, which represents
considerations, and are thus purported to less arbitrarily cut nature only a fraction of the Big Five personality measures used in
at the joints. The HEXACO model also has a hierarchical repre- research. It is presently unknown whether the results of Steel et al.
sentation that includes 25 facets and six domains (four facets for (2008) generalize to a wider range of Big Five measures. Further-
each domain and one interstitial facet; Lee & Ashton, 2018). more, no meta-analysis exists relating the Big Five to the six
dimensions of PWB and no meta-analysis exists relating
Personality Traits and Well-Being: What We HEXACO domains to either SWB or PWB. Fortunately, as a result
of growing interest in these associations, there are now a sufficient
Know So Far
number of primary studies to make such a meta-analysis worth-
Most research on the relation between personality and well- while. Such an examination would complete the mapping of
being has focused on the Big Five and the three dimensions of HEXACO and Big Five domains onto the dimensions of SWB and
SWB (DeNeve & Cooper, 1998; Steel et al., 2008). The results of PWB and provide a more robust assessment of the relationship
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Steel et al. (2008) were a watershed in this literature, as by this between Big Five personality and SWB.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
found that sincerity and fairness tended to have slightly larger ased estimates of population variance explained is essential, and
correlations with PWB than the facets of greed-avoidance and one reasonable approach is to use an adjusted r-squared correction.
modesty. This is particularly important in the context of domain and facet
Although these studies have provided important insights, they regression comparison because of the large difference in the num-
have not satisfied the methodological requirements for a robust ber of predictors.
assessment of facet-level correlations and the incremental predic- Fourth, Steel and colleagues (Steel et al., 2008, 2019) have
tion of facets (Anglim & Grant, 2014; Anglim & O’Connor, 2019). conducted meta-analytic regression models to estimate facet-
First, facets and domains need to be measured reliably. In partic- level prediction. However, because researchers rarely report
ular, a valid assessment of incremental prediction by facets re- facet-level intercorrelations, these meta-analytic facet-level re-
quires reliable measurement of the variance in facets not shared gressions have to rely on sources other than the primary studies
with personality domains. This is best achieved through the use of (e.g., test manuals). Facet-level correlations vary from study to
long-form measures of personality such as the HEXACO 200, IPIP study and the inability to accurately represent multicollinearity
300, and NEO PI R 240. Second, large samples are also required.
can dramatically inflate or distort variance explained in regres-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Research Question 3: What is the relative prediction of broad Cooper, 1998; Heller, Watson, & Ilies, 2004; Lucas & Fujita,
and narrow personality traits in relation to SWB and PWB and 2000; Steel et al., 2008, 2019).
how does this vary across the Big Five and HEXACO? After merging the above sources and removing obvious dupli-
cates, the Combined Dataset consisted of 2472 articles. Based on
The Present Research title and abstracts screening, the full-text was examined for 60.5%
of these articles.
In seeking to answer these three research questions, the overall In addition to the articles that met the inclusion criteria, a further
objective of this research is to thoroughly describe relations that 249 articles were identified where relevant variables were mea-
the domains and facets of HEXACO and Big Five personality have sured but the correlations were not reported or not completely
with the dimensions of SWB and PWB. To achieve these aims, we reported. The corresponding author of each of these articles was
conducted a set of comprehensive analyses of published domain- sent an e-mail inviting them to provide either the correlation
level correlations and facet-level data sets. To understand domain- matrix or the data from which we could compute the correlation
level correlations (RQ1), we conducted a meta-analysis of the matrix. When a working corresponding author’s e-mail could not
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
domain-level correlates of HEXACO and Big Five personality be found, another author or Doctoral supervisor was emailed.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
with the dimensions of SWB and PWB. Contacted authors also provided several additional studies that met
To provide a systematic assessment of facet-level correlations the inclusion criteria of our meta-analysis. Several of these addi-
(RQ2) and incremental prediction of facets-over-domains (RQ3) tional studies were unpublished or from articles where the corre-
across well-being measures and various Big Five and HEXACO lations were not reported. This process of contacting authors
frameworks, we adopted a multipronged approach. This included resulted in 68 additional studies being included in the meta-
collecting new data, reanalyzing partially reported raw-data, merg- analysis (11 supplied data; 57 supplied correlation matrices).
ing data sets where equivalent measures were used, and analyzing Several additional sources of correlations were as follows: We
complete correlation matrices where these were reported. All of obtained correlations from six studies in which the correlation
the data sets involved included (a) the nine well-being variables, matrices were not otherwise published that were reported in the
(b) reliable, full-length personality measures, and (c) moderate to meta-analysis on personality and various forms of satisfaction by
large sample sizes. Importantly, the combined sample size of these Heller et al. (2004). We included the domain-level correlations
data sets is an order of magnitude larger than previous attempts to from the two facet-level studies reported in the current paper that
estimate incremental prediction of facets, and will thus provide the have not previously been reported (i.e., the Combined Dataset and
first robust examination of that question. the NEO Dataset). We also computed correlations for six studies
that did not report correlation matrices but included a dataset with
Method the publication (e.g., data on the OSF, PlosOne, other data repos-
itory).
All data, scripts, materials, and supplemental analyses are avail-
After collating the studies, 17 studies were excluded for one of
able on the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/42rsy.
the following reasons. First, studies were excluded if they reported
correlations that used a sample that overlapped with another study.
Meta-Analysis This was common with large panel studies such as the GSOEP,
Our meta-analysis served to estimate cross-sectional self-report HILDA, BHPS, and MIDUS as well as some individual small-
relations that the HEXACO and Big Five Domains have with SWB scale studies. In these cases, we sought to retain the article that
and PWB. provided the most comprehensive study in terms of sample and
measurement. Second, several studies were excluded because they
used nonstandard measurement of personality or well-being that
Literature Search
was not initially excluded by our exclusion rules, but were flagged
The literature search sought to identify any study that reported because they produced outlier correlations (e.g., IPIP HEXACO,
a correlation between Big Five or HEXACO Personality and the asking about life satisfaction in the past, etc.). Third, we excluded
dimensions of SWB or PWB. The final literature search reported studies that had outlier correlations combined with other concerns
in this paper was conducted in August, 2019. Keyword searches about data integrity. In several studies, there were strong indi-
were conducted in Scopus and PsycINFO, which included disser- cators that a large proportion of participants were not complet-
tations and foreign language articles. The primary search sought to ing the study conscientiously as evidenced by use of samples
identify articles that included (a) at least one personality-related such as Mechanical Turk, very large average correlations be-
keyword indicating that the Big Five or HEXACO was used, tween the Big Five (e.g., above .6), exclusion of large numbers
which included any personality domain name (e.g., extraversion, of participants due to failing attention checks combined with
neuroticism, honesty-humility) or a common test or framework attention checks that would not be sufficient to identify all
name (e.g., BFI, NEO, HEXACO, Big Five, Big 5, FFM, Five nonconscientious responders, and relatively undifferentiated
Factor Model, etc.), (b) the word personality, and (c) a well-being personality–well-being correlations. Other indicators of con-
related term (e.g., SWB, PWB, subjective well-being, life satis- cern included correlations close to zero between well-being
faction, satisfaction with life, positive affect, negative affect, etc.). variables and poorly written articles.
Second, a search for well-being related terms was performed on The final cleaned database consisted of 377 articles and 462
the more than 600 HEXACO-related references listed on http:// studies. Note that in six samples both HEXACO and Big Five
hexaco.org/references. Third, references from key meta-analyses personality were measured, and these were treated as two
on personality and well-being were included (i.e., DeNeve & separate studies. Likewise, some articles reported correlations
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 285
separately for different groups (e.g., males and females; pa- a few other focused scales. We excluded any life satisfaction
tients and controls), and these were also treated as separate measure which included a broader set of well-being indicators.
studies. Articles were retained if they reported a correlation To be included, positive affect and negative affect needed to be
between a relevant personality variable (i.e., HEXACO or Big measured as the sum of items asking about the frequency of
Five) and a relevant well-being variable. In order to focus our experiencing a set of positive and negative emotions, respectively.
primary meta-analytic estimates on studies that used reliable The vast majority (86%) of studies used the PANAS (Watson,
measures, we classified correlations into core and noncore. If Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) or a variant of the PANAS. We excluded
the personality trait was measured with eight or more items and studies that measured affect using experience sampling methods
the well-being dimension was measured with five or more because there was a lack of standardization in how affect was
items, the correlation was classified as core. For reporting measured and aggregated to the person-level. We also excluded
purposes, we classified a study as core if it had one or more core measures of affect that were obtained following experimental
correlations. Sixteen studies had a mix of core and noncore manipulation or that were in response to stimuli.
correlations. To be included, PWB needed to be measured using an official
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Importantly, in recent years there has been a proliferation of measure of Ryff’s conception of the six dimensions of PWB. This
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
short-form measures of personality (e.g., TIPI, BFI 10, Mini-IPIP, mostly included 42-, 54-, and 84-item versions of Ryff’s scales and
etc.). There are also a wide range of short-form adaptations used in their translations. We focused exclusively on the six scales and not
individual studies. In contrast, studies classified as core tended to overall measures of PWB.
use reliable, well-validated and well-established measures of per-
sonality and well-being. The focus on these core studies also
Data Extraction
makes results more comparable across the Big Five and HEXACO,
where HEXACO personality is typically measured with 60, 100 For each included study, we extracted the following study
and 200 item formats. It also enables more direct comparison with features: sample size, personality measure, life satisfaction
the meta-analysis by Steel et al. (2008) which focused exclusively measure, positive affect measures, PWB measure, proportion
on the NEO where the most common formats involve 12 (NEO female, mean age, country of sample, type of sample (e.g.,
FFI) and 48 (NEO PI R) items per factor, respectively. It also university students, Mechanical Turk, Workers, Community,
reduces the need to rely on problematic assumptions related to etc.), the source of the correlations (e.g., from the article,
estimating reliability and correcting for measurement error. None- provided following correspondence with author, etc.), reference
theless, we do report results for the full set of studies in the section details, and additional notes. Correlations were extracted by
on moderator analysis. copying the correlation matrix into Excel, extracting the corre-
lations in the order they appeared in the correlation matrix and
then using data transformations to convert into a standardized
Eligibility Criteria and Data Coding Procedures
order. All study feature and correlation extraction was per-
Several criteria needed to be satisfied for correlations to be formed by the first- and fifth-author of this paper. All correla-
retained in the meta-analysis. For consistency, the study needed to tions were extracted by one author and checked for accuracy by
involve self-report measurement of both personality and well- the other. To further identify data entry errors, reporting errors
being. Second, personality needed to be measured with either a by original authors, and problematic studies, we obtained
standard measure of the HEXACO (e.g., HEXACO 60, 100, 192, z-scores for all correlations by correlation type (i.e., there were
200, etc.) or a measure explicitly designed to assess the Big Five. 99 different types of correlations based on the 11 personality
We excluded the one study by Churchyard, Pine, Sharma, and traits and 9 well-being variables). We closely examined corre-
Fletcher (2014) that used the IPIP HEXACO, largely because this lations with absolute z-scores larger than 2.5. In a few cases,
is based on an early model of HEXACO that excluded social researchers had made an error in reporting their correlations
self-esteem. This also resulted in the exclusion of studies that used (e.g., omitting the minus sign on correlations with neuroticism)
the Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI) or the Eysenck Person- and this was corrected. In other cases, we examined the study
ality Questionnaire (EPQ). Detailed meta-analysis of the EPI and more carefully and identified indicators that the study was
EPQ are already available in Steel et al. (2008), and we wanted to problematic (nonconscientious participants; failure to exhibit
focus on measures that were explicitly designed to partition per- universal features of correlations in this area such as correla-
sonality trait variance into the Big Five or HEXACO. We similarly tions between well-being), and these studies were excluded as
excluded measures that can be scored to derive a Big Five measure described earlier.
but were not designed to measure the Big Five.
Third, the well-being measure needed to be designed to measure
Data Analytic Approach
satisfaction with life, positive affect, negative affect (i.e., SWB) or
the six scales of Ryff’s measure of PWB. In relation to life Meta-analytic correlations were estimated using a random-
satisfaction, we sought to only include pure measures of life effects model using the metafor package in R (Viechtbauer,
satisfaction. Life satisfaction was typically (82%) measured using 2010). The standard deviation of true effect sizes (i.e., ) was
Diener’s Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & estimated using restricted maximum-likelihood estimation.
Griffin, 1985). We also included single-item measures of life Meta-analytic estimates were obtained using both observed
satisfaction, composite measures of life satisfaction that sum sat- correlations and correlations corrected for measurement error.
isfaction with various life domains (e.g., Personal Well-Being Relatively few studies provided scale-level reliability informa-
Index), modified versions of the Satisfaction with Life Scale, and tion, so we relied on more general sources based on the test
286 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
used, and where this was not available we estimated reliability Combined dataset. We conducted a new study in which me
as the average reliability for tests in the database with equiva- measured the HEXACO PI R, the IPIP NEO, and both SWB and
lent numbers of items per factor. PWB. This enabled (a) the first rigorous estimate of HEXACO
correlates of SWB and PWB at the facet-level, (b) a more robust
assessment of the correlates of the IPIP NEO with SWB and PWB,
Facet-Level Analysis (c) clarity regarding the similarities and differences between the
Identifying data sets. To provide a comprehensive assess- HEXACO and IPIP NEO frameworks, and (d) an opportunity to
ment of facet-level correlates and incremental prediction, we examine the combined prediction of HEXACO and the IPIP NEO.
sought to identify all studies that had included a hierarchical The final sample consisted of 465 Australian university students
measure of personality that enabled reliable facet-level measure- (79% female; age in years M ⫽ 25.1, SD ⫽ 7.8, range: 18 to 56),
ment, and that included measurement of SWB and PWB. In order based on an initial sample of 578, from which 113 cases were
to estimate incremental prediction, we needed to have either (a) the dropped because of incomplete data. Because of the large number
raw data, (b) the full correlation matrix between facets, domains, of items, data was collected online over two sessions. In the first
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
weeks. The 20 items each concerned a different emotion and were rials, we report semipartial correlations that remove the shared
rated on a 5-point scale (1 ⫽ very slightly or not at all, 2 ⫽ a little, variance between the facet and the five domain-level personality
3 ⫽ moderately, 4 ⫽ quite a bit, 5 ⫽ extremely). Scales were factors. They provide an estimate of the unique prediction pro-
scored as the mean of items. The NEO Dataset used a version of vided by the facets over and above the domains. The square of the
the measure translated into Spanish by Marrero et al. (2016). The semipartial correlation is equivalent to the percentage of incremen-
Big Five Aspects Dataset measured positive and negative emotions tal variance explained by a regression model that adds the facet of
using six-items from the PERMA-Profiler (Butler & Kern, 2016). interest (e.g., gregariousness) as a predictor to one with only the
Psychological well-being. Ryff’s (1989) scales were used to domains (e.g., the Big Five). Incremental prediction of facets over
measure the six proposed dimensions of psychological well-being. domains was obtained by taking the difference in the adjusted
Items were rated on a 6-point scale (1 ⫽ strongly disagree, 2 ⫽ r-squared values for a regression model with domains as predictors
disagree somewhat, 3 ⫽ disagree slightly, 4 ⫽ agree slightly, 5 ⫽ to one with facets as predictors.
agree somewhat, 6 ⫽ strongly agree). The scale consisted of
positively and negatively worded items, and scale scores were the Results
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
mean after item reversal. The NEO Dataset used the 84-item
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Table 2
Summary of Studies Included in Meta-Analysis
Table 2 (continued)
Table 2 (continued)
Table 2 (continued)
Table 2 (continued)
Table 2 (continued)
Table 2 (continued)
Table 2 (continued)
Table 2 (continued)
openness exhibited similar correlations with well-being to their Big were reversed neuroticism with reversed negative affect (.14), and
Five analogues. The average correlations with well-being for honesty- personal growth (⫺.15); openness with personal growth (.22);
humility and HEXACO agreeableness were also similar to the corre- agreeableness with positive relations (.13) and autonomy (⫺.13),
lation for Big Five agreeableness. Results also showed that the vari- and conscientiousness with purpose in life (.13). For HEXACO,
ance in observed correlations was greater for the Big Five than for the these were reversed emotionality with reversed negative affect
HEXACO; this is consistent with the greater variability in question- (.19), positive relations (⫺.18), autonomy (.22), and purpose in life
naires used to measure the Big Five. (⫺.14); agreeableness with autonomy (⫺.13); conscientiousness
To assess which combinations of personality and well-being with purpose in life (.18); and openness with autonomy (.12) and
dimension were uniquely related, we performed a marginalization personal growth (.15).
procedure on the meta-analytic corrected correlation matrix (see Table 9 presents the meta-analytic estimate of the correlations
the online supplemental materials). Specifically, we reversed neg- between the Big Five and SWB across various moderators (i.e.,
ative affect, neuroticism, and emotionality so that all variables core and noncore studies, item length, and personality measure-
were positively aligned with well-being. We then subtracted the ment type) and compares results with past meta-analyses. It also
overall mean correlation, and the row and column marginal means reports the mean and standard deviation of correlations after re-
from the correlation matrix (for further details of the procedure versing the negative correlations (i.e., N with PA, N with SWL,
see, Anglim & Grant, 2016). Large residual cross-correlations and E, O, A, C with NA). The mean correlation indexes the extent
(e.g., above .10 or .15) highlight the unique profile of the to which personality is related to well-being. The standard devia-
personality-well-being relationship, where positive residuals indi- tion of correlations indexes the degree to which a nuanced profile
cate that the pair of variables is more related than expected, and of personality correlates is provided as opposed to a more homog-
negative residuals indicate that the pair of variables is less related enous set of correlations. Overall, the pattern of correlations is
than expected. Absolute residuals greater than .12 for the Big Five fairly robust across different types of measures and different item
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 297
Table 3
Combined Sample Sizes and Number of Studies Across Study Features
lengths. Nonetheless, consistent with reduced reliability of mea- sively on the NEO framework. The current meta-analysis found
surement and potentially validity, noncore studies and extrashort meta-analytic correlations between personality and well-being that
measures had weaker correlations with well-being. were slightly larger than Steel et al. (2008). Importantly, the
In general, there was a high degree of consistency across the current results indicate that this finding is not limited to the NEO
different personality frameworks, although the TIPI was notably framework, but is shared across a broad range of personality
less consistent. The BFAS had somewhat stronger average corre- measures that are intended to measure the Big Five.
lations and the TIPI had weaker average correlations. The NEO
The pattern of correlations in the current meta-analysis was
and BFAS had larger standard deviations. To quantify the consis-
almost identical to that obtained in Steel et al. (2008), but quite
tency across frameworks, we created a data frame that had 15 rows
different to that of DeNeve and Cooper (1998). To quantify this,
for the 15 absolute SWB correlations and seven columns for the
we first treated the 15 absolute correlations between Big Five
seven personality frameworks. We then computed the average
correlation each framework had with the other six frameworks. personality and SWB (i.e., SWL, PA, NA) for the three meta-
These correlations were .88 (NEO), .88 (IPIP), .90 (BFAS), .87 analyses (i.e., current study, Steel et al., and DeNeve & Cooper) as
(BFI), .74 (TIPI), .90 (Adjectives), and .84 (Other). a vector. The correlation between the 15 Big Five–SWB-absolute-
Table 9 also compares meta-analytic correlations of the current correlations was r ⫽ .991 (current study with Steel), r ⫽ .689
study with that of previous meta-analyses. A major conclusion of (current study with DeNeve), and r ⫽ .679 (DeNeve with Steel).
Steel et al. (2008) was that personality is more strongly related to Thus, it seems that categorizing historical measures of personality
well-being than was found in the meta-analysis of DeNeve and into Big Five frameworks as was done by necessity in DeNeve and
Cooper (1998). Whereas DeNeve and Cooper (1998) synthesized Cooper (1998) only provides an approximation of how Big Five
a mostly pre-Big Five literature, Steel et al. (2008) focused exclu- personality actually correlates with well-being.
298 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
Table 4
Meta-Analytic Correlations of Big Five and HEXACO Personality With SWB and PWB
Measure SWL PA NA PR AU EM PG PL SA M
NEO
Neuroticism ⴚ.39 ⴚ.34 .56 ⴚ.43 ⴚ.45 ⴚ.58 ⴚ.34 ⴚ.45 ⴚ.60 ⴚ.46
Extraversion .32 .44 ⫺.21 .47 .26 .38 .39 .39 .43 .37
Openness .08 .24 ⫺.05 .20 .24 .11 .44 .21 .16 .19
Agreeableness .20 .19 ⫺.25 .39 .10 .28 .31 .28 .28 .25
Conscientiousness .27 .35 ⫺.25 .32 .30 .51 .32 .50 .44 .36
HEXACO
Honesty-humility .11 .07 ⫺.15 .20 .19 .20 .21 .18 .14 .16
Emotionality ⫺.09 ⫺.12 .31 .01 ⴚ.36 ⫺.19 ⫺.11 ⫺.03 ⫺.24 ⫺.16
Extraversion .43 .55 ⴚ.39 .57 .39 .52 .45 .41 .61 .48
Agreeableness .17 .14 ⫺.25 .27 .02 .22 .16 .13 .23 .18
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Conscientiousness .22 .32 ⫺.17 .18 .23 .41 .31 .47 .23 .28
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Openness .10 .15 ⫺.01 .14 .25 .10 .34 .14 .18 .16
Note. PWB ⫽ psychological well-being; SWB ⫽ subjective well-being; SWL ⫽ satisfaction with life; PA ⫽ positive affect; NA ⫽ negative affect; PR ⫽
positive relations; AU ⫽ autonomy; EM ⫽ environmental mastery; PG ⫽ personal growth; PL ⫽ purpose in life; SA ⫽ self-acceptance. Absolute
correlations above .30 are bolded. M ⫽ mean correlation between the personality trait and well-being variables, where the correlation with negative affect
(NA) is reversed.
Finally, a publication bias analysis was conducted. There are sev- negative affect, none of the correlations examined exhibited
eral reasons to expect publication biases to be minimal in this context. significant positive asymmetry.
First, the majority of primary studies have a high degree of power to
detect the main correlations between personality and well-being. For
Well-Being Intercorrelations
example, a study with n ⫽ 200 has 99% statistical power to detect a
population correlation of .30 at a .05 significance threshold. Second, To contextualize the meta-analytic and facet-level analyses, we
many studies measure personality and well-being incidentally as part present estimates of the intercorrelations between dimensions of
of broader studies of individual differences and there is no obvious well-being. Table 10 presents correlations among the nine well-
incentive to show a specific pattern of correlations between person- being scales for the Combined and the NEO Data Sets. Reflecting
ality and well-being. Nonetheless, we examined funnel plots for the a general well-being factor, the average correlation between well-
99 correlation types (i.e., 11 personality traits by nine well-being being variables was .51 in the Combined Dataset. Consistent with
variables) and calculated the rank test for funnel asymmetry (Begg & the focus on the scale-level, when factor analysis is performed and
Mazumdar, 1994). After reversing neuroticism, emotionality, and two factors are extracted, loadings for the nine scales do not align
Table 5
Detailed Meta-Analytic Results for Big Five Domains and Subjective Well-Being
Table 6
Detailed Meta-Analytic Results for Big Five Domains and Psychological Well-Being
Agreeableness 16 6,102 .10 .11 .04 .16 .13 .14 .05 .20
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Conscientiousness 17 6,309 .30 .05 .27 .34 .36 .07 .32 .41
Environmental mastery
Neuroticism 16 6,160 ⫺.58 .11 ⫺.64 ⫺.52 ⫺.69 .13 ⫺.76 ⫺.63
Extraversion 16 6,160 .38 .14 .31 .45 .45 .16 .37 .53
Openness 15 5,953 .11 .11 .04 .17 .13 .15 .04 .21
Agreeableness 15 5,953 .28 .10 .22 .34 .35 .13 .27 .42
Conscientiousness 16 6,160 .51 .10 .45 .56 .61 .11 .55 .67
Personal growth
Neuroticism 16 5,920 ⫺.34 .11 ⫺.40 ⫺.28 ⫺.41 .15 ⫺.49 ⫺.33
Extraversion 16 5,920 .39 .09 .34 .44 .47 .12 .41 .54
Openness 15 5,713 .44 .10 .39 .50 .55 .12 .48 .61
Agreeableness 15 5,713 .31 .10 .25 .36 .38 .12 .31 .45
Conscientiousness 16 5,920 .32 .06 .28 .36 .40 .08 .35 .44
Purpose in life
Neuroticism 15 5,699 ⫺.45 .12 ⫺.51 ⫺.38 ⫺.53 .14 ⫺.61 ⫺.46
Extraversion 15 5,699 .39 .10 .33 .45 .47 .13 .40 .54
Openness 14 5,492 .21 .09 .15 .26 .25 .13 .18 .33
Agreeableness 14 5,492 .28 .06 .24 .32 .35 .09 .29 .40
Conscientiousness 15 5,699 .50 .10 .44 .55 .60 .10 .54 .66
Self-acceptance
Neuroticism 14 5,488 ⫺.60 .13 ⫺.67 ⫺.53 ⫺.69 .15 ⫺.77 ⫺.61
Extraversion 14 5,488 .43 .11 .37 .49 .50 .13 .43 .57
Openness 13 5,281 .16 .10 .10 .23 .19 .13 .11 .27
Agreeableness 13 5,281 .28 .06 .24 .32 .35 .09 .29 .41
Conscientiousness 14 5,488 .44 .05 .40 .47 .51 .08 .46 .56
Note. k is the number of studies. r is mean observed correlation estimated from random-effects model and inverse-variance weighting. is the equivalent
correlation estimated using correlations corrected for measurement error. r and are the estimated standard deviations of true unadjusted and corrected
correlations, respectively.
with higher-order PWB and SWB dimensions. Life satisfaction Zero-order correlations between personality facets and well-
shared the greatest overlap with self-acceptance, although corre- being are presented for NEO (see Table 11), IPIP NEO (see Table
lations were relatively large for most other well-being scales, with 12), and HEXACO (see Table 13). Domain-level correlations for
the exception of autonomy and personal growth. the NEO and IPIP NEO data sets are reported in the online
supplemental materials. Semipartial correlations that involved re-
Facet-Level Correlations moving overlap between each facet and the corresponding domain
scores are also reported in the online supplemental materials. For
We first examined the degree to which the domain correlations
the NEO, the strongest average correlations with well-being are
between personality and well-being in the facet-level data sets
were consistent with the core meta-analytic estimates. In general, seen for depression (⫺.46), vulnerability (⫺.44), and competence
there was very strong convergence with the pattern of domain (.41). For the IPIP NEO, semipartial correlations frequently high-
correlations for all the facet-level data sets: NEO (r ⫽ .94), IPIP lighted depression as an incremental predictor over and above the
(r ⫽ .95), HEXACO (r ⫽ .96), Big Five Aspects (r ⫽ .89) data Big Five. Positive emotions was also a prominent incremental
sets (see the online supplemental materials for details). Average predictor in relation to satisfaction with life, positive affect, and
correlations between personality and well-being were higher self-acceptance. Various other semipartial correlations emerged
(mean difference study and meta-analytic correlations in parenthe- consistent with the unique profile of the well-being variable (e.g.,
ses) than meta-analytic estimates for the IPIP (M ⫽ .06) and Big purpose in life with achievement striving and autonomy with angry
Five Aspects (M ⫽ .12), but similar for HEXACO (M ⫽ .03) and hostility [⫹], self-consciousness [⫺], and assertiveness [⫹]). For
NEO (M ⫽ ⫺.03). the HEXACO, social self-esteem and liveliness emerged as the
300 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
Table 7
Detailed Meta-Analytic Results for HEXACO Domains and Subjective Well-Being
Extraversion 8 3,834 .55 .04 .51 .58 .63 .05 .59 .67
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Agreeableness 8 3,834 .14 .09 .07 .21 .17 .10 .09 .25
Conscientiousness 8 3,834 .32 .10 .25 .40 .38 .12 .29 .47
Openness 8 3,834 .15 .04 .10 .20 .17 .05 .13 .22
Negative affect
Honesty-humility 9 4,134 ⫺.15 .05 ⫺.20 ⫺.11 ⫺.18 .06 ⫺.23 ⫺.13
Emotionality 9 4,134 .31 .09 .24 .37 .36 .11 .28 .44
Extraversion 9 4,134 ⫺.39 .11 ⫺.47 ⫺.32 ⫺.46 .13 ⫺.55 ⫺.37
Agreeableness 9 4,134 ⫺.25 .07 ⫺.31 ⫺.19 ⫺.30 .09 ⫺.36 ⫺.23
Conscientiousness 9 4,134 ⫺.17 .09 ⫺.24 ⫺.10 ⫺.20 .11 ⫺.28 ⫺.12
Openness 9 4,134 ⫺.01 .02 ⫺.04 .03 ⫺.01 .04 ⫺.05 .03
Note. k is the number of studies. r is mean observed correlation estimated from random-effects model and inverse-variance weighting. is the equivalent
correlation estimated using correlations corrected for measurement error. r and are the estimated standard deviations of true unadjusted and corrected
correlations, respectively.
strongest average predictors of well-being. Differential correla- prediction in the NEO sample. Overall, the greatest proportional
tions of emotionality facets highlight why emotionality correlated increase in variance explained by facets was seen for life satisfac-
much less with well-being overall. Specifically, anxiety and to a tion, autonomy, self-acceptance, and purpose in life.
lesser extent fearfulness had strong negative correlations with
well-being whereas dependence and sentimentality did not. Simi- HEXACO Versus Big Five Comparison
larly, with regards t conscientiousness, it was mostly diligence that
had the stand-out correlations. To contextualize the meta-analytic finding and frame a compar-
ison of HEXACO and Big Five, Table 15 presents the correlations
between HEXACO and Big Five domains using the Combined
Incremental Prediction of Facets Over Domains
Dataset. All analogous scales between HEXACO and Big Five
To examine the variance explained by broad and narrow traits correlated greater than .50. Interestingly—though unsurprisingly,
across the four data sets, regression models were estimated pre- given the rotational differences between the two models—
dicting each well-being variable from either the broad or the honesty-humility correlated more with Big Five agreeableness
narrow traits for the given personality measure. The variance than did HEXACO agreeableness. Of relevance to understanding
explained by broad and narrow traits (adjusted r-squared) for each correlations with well-being, HEXACO extraversion correlated
measure is shown in Table 14. Two measures of incremental more with neuroticism than did HEXACO emotionality.
prediction of narrow traits are also provided: raw incremental Table 16 presents the domain-level correlations for HEXACO
prediction by narrow over broad traits and proportional increase of and IPIP NEO Domains with well-being dimensions in the Com-
narrow traits relative to broad traits. bined Dataset. The pattern of correlations is broadly similar to the
On average, broad traits explained 46% of variance and narrow meta-analytic findings, albeit the correlations are slightly stronger
traits explained 53% for an average proportional increase of facets on average. This may reflect the use of particularly reliable per-
over domains of 18% (21% if you exclude the Big Five Aspects sonality and well-being measures in this study. We also computed
data). Despite differences in the overall magnitude of prediction the HEXACO Neuroticism domain score using the weighted facet-
(i.e., Big Five Aspects and IPIP NEO explained more than composite described in the Method section. This yielded a pattern
HEXACO and NEO), the general pattern of well-being predicted of correlations that was very similar to IPIP NEO Neuroticism.
by domains and facets/aspects was similar across NEO, IPIP NEO, To compare the HEXACO and Big Five models of personality
and HEXACO, but distinct for the Big Five Aspects. On average, in terms of the prediction of well-being dimensions, regression
PWB variables were better predicted by personality than SWB models were estimated (using the Combined Dataset) predicting
variables. IPIP NEO and HEXACO had larger incremental pre- each well-being variable from various sets of personality predic-
diction than the NEO and Big Five Aspects, although the differ- tors: that is, HEXACO Domains, NEO Domains, HEXACO Fac-
ence for the NEO was reduced when incremental prediction was ets, NEO Facets, and the different combinations of Domains and
defined as a proportion, due to the relatively lower levels of Facets from both instruments. The variance in well-being ex-
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 301
Table 8
Detailed Meta-Analytic Results for HEXACO Domains and Psychological Well-Being
Extraversion 5 2,033 .39 .00 .36 .43 .49 .02 .45 .53
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Agreeableness 5 2,033 .02 .07 ⫺.05 .10 .03 .09 ⫺.06 .12
Conscientiousness 5 2,033 .23 .05 .17 .29 .29 .06 .22 .36
Openness 5 2,033 .25 .05 .19 .32 .32 .07 .24 .39
Environmental mastery
Honesty-humility 5 2,033 .20 .02 .15 .25 .26 .06 .19 .32
Emotionality 5 2,033 ⫺.19 .09 ⫺.28 ⫺.10 ⫺.23 .10 ⫺.33 ⫺.13
Extraversion 5 2,033 .52 .08 .44 .61 .64 .09 .56 .72
Agreeableness 5 2,033 .22 .07 .14 .30 .27 .09 .18 .37
Conscientiousness 5 2,033 .41 .07 .34 .49 .51 .11 .41 .61
Openness 5 2,033 .10 .08 .01 .19 .12 .11 .01 .23
Personal growth
Honesty-humility 5 2,033 .21 .07 .13 .29 .27 .10 .17 .37
Emotionality 5 2,033 ⫺.11 .00 ⫺.15 ⫺.06 ⫺.14 .05 ⫺.20 ⫺.07
Extraversion 5 2,033 .45 .04 .40 .50 .56 .00 .53 .59
Agreeableness 5 2,033 .16 .04 .10 .21 .20 .05 .14 .26
Conscientiousness 5 2,033 .31 .02 .26 .35 .40 .05 .35 .46
Openness 5 2,033 .34 .05 .28 .41 .43 .09 .35 .52
Purpose in life
Honesty-humility 5 2,033 .18 .00 .13 .22 .24 .06 .17 .31
Emotionality 5 2,033 ⫺.03 .04 ⫺.09 .03 ⫺.03 .05 ⫺.10 .04
Extraversion 5 2,033 .41 .08 .33 .49 .52 .06 .46 .59
Agreeableness 5 2,033 .13 .07 .05 .21 .17 .09 .08 .27
Conscientiousness 5 2,033 .47 .00 .43 .50 .60 .04 .55 .64
Openness 5 2,033 .14 .00 .10 .19 .19 .02 .15 .24
Self-acceptance
Honesty-humility 5 2,033 .14 .02 .10 .19 .18 .03 .12 .23
Emotionality 5 2,033 ⫺.24 .00 ⫺.29 ⫺.20 ⫺.31 .06 ⫺.37 ⫺.24
Extraversion 5 2,033 .61 .03 .57 .64 .74 .03 .71 .78
Agreeableness 5 2,033 .23 .06 .17 .30 .29 .07 .21 .37
Conscientiousness 5 2,033 .23 .07 .15 .30 .27 .09 .18 .36
Openness 5 2,033 .18 .10 .08 .27 .22 .14 .09 .35
Note. k is the number of studies. r is mean observed correlation estimated from random-effects model and inverse-variance weighting. is the equivalent
correlation estimated using correlations corrected for measurement error. r and are the estimated standard deviations of true unadjusted and corrected
correlations, respectively.
Table 9
Meta-Analytic Correlations Between Big Five Personality and Subjective Well-Being by Study Type, Number of Personality Items,
Personality Measure Type, and Comparison With Past Meta-Analyses
SWL PA NA
Personality items N E O A C N E O A C N E O A C M SD
Study status
Core studies ⫺.39 .32 .08 .20 .27 ⫺.34 .44 .24 .19 .35 .56 ⫺.21 ⫺.05 ⫺.25 ⫺.25 .28 .13
Noncore studies ⫺.32 .24 .09 .18 .21 ⫺.36 .40 .27 .24 .26 .53 ⫺.20 ⫺.08 ⫺.14 ⫺.24 .25 .12
Personality items
Extra Short 1 to 3 ⫺.31 .22 .08 .15 .20 ⫺.34 .33 .20 .12 .23 .46 ⫺.20 ⫺.05 ⫺.13 ⫺.21 .22 .11
Short 4 to 7 ⫺.32 .27 .14 .19 .23 ⫺.32 .45 .36 .33 .28 .55 ⫺.18 ⫺.10 ⫺.12 ⫺.23 .27 .12
Standard 8 to 15 ⫺.38 .31 .09 .21 .26 ⫺.34 .43 .25 .22 .36 .57 ⫺.20 ⫺.07 ⫺.27 ⫺.26 .28 .13
Long 16 or more ⫺.42 .33 .06 .18 .29 ⫺.35 .46 .19 .11 .31 .57 ⫺.22 ⫺.01 ⫺.20 ⫺.22 .26 .15
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Measure type
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
NEO ⫺.42 .34 .05 .17 .28 ⫺.32 .44 .18 .10 .36 .56 ⫺.20 ⫺.02 ⫺.20 ⫺.21 .26 .15
IPIP ⫺.38 .28 .09 .19 .25 ⫺.36 .38 .20 .23 .33 .54 ⫺.21 ⫺.05 ⫺.23 ⫺.28 .27 .12
BFAS ⫺.43 .37 .06 .14 .31 ⫺.41 .57 .27 .24 .42 .65 ⫺.34 ⫺.12 ⫺.24 ⫺.27 .32 .16
BFI ⫺.34 .27 .09 .20 .23 ⫺.37 .43 .28 .24 .34 .57 ⫺.20 ⫺.06 ⫺.31 ⫺.29 .28 .13
TIPI ⫺.31 .22 .10 .14 .19 ⫺.32 .38 .27 .09 .19 .39 ⫺.26 ⫺.16 ⫺.01 ⫺.22 .22 .11
Adjectives ⫺.35 .26 .06 .21 .23 ⫺.29 .46 .33 .23 .33 .57 ⫺.22 ⫺.10 ⫺.19 ⫺.24 .27 .13
Other ⫺.34 .31 .17 .25 .25 ⫺.34 .46 .31 .26 .27 .58 ⫺.17 ⫺.09 ⫺.15 ⫺.12 .27 .13
Meta-analyses
Current (core) ⫺.39 .32 .08 .20 .27 ⫺.34 .44 .24 .19 .35 .56 ⫺.21 ⫺.05 ⫺.25 ⫺.25 .28 .13
DeNeve and Cooper (1998) ⫺.24 .17 .14 .16 .22 ⫺.14 .20 .14 .17 .14 .23 ⫺.07 .05 ⫺.13 ⫺.10 .15 .07
Steel et al. (2008) ⫺.38 .28 .03 .14 .22 ⫺.30 .44 .20 .12 .27 .54 ⫺.18 ⫺.02 ⫺.20 ⫺.20 .23 .14
Heller (2004) ⫺.48 .28 .08 .29 .31
Note. Current (core) k ⫽ 120 to 224, n ⫽ 39,023 to 158,934; Heller, Watson, and Ilies (2004) k ⫽ 19, n ⫽ 12,092; Steel et al. (2008) k ⫽ 22 to 57, n ⫽
6,040 to 16,764; DeNeve and Cooper (1998) k ⫽ 38 to 102, n is a subset of 42,171. M and SD is the mean and standard deviation of correlation after
reversing N with PA, N with SWL, and E, O, A, C with NA. NA ⫽ negative affect; PA ⫽ positive affect; SWL ⫽ satisfaction with life; BFAS ⫽ Big
Five Aspect Scales; BFI ⫽ Big Five Inventory; IPIP ⫽ International Personality Item Pool; TIPI ⫽ Ten-Item Personality Inventory.
The study also provides the first robust assessment of incre- works. Third, correlations with personality mirror the unique
mental prediction by facets across both SWB and PWB and two characteristics of different dimensions of well-being. For example,
major personality frameworks. notably strong correlations were observed between openness and
Several important findings emerged from this investigation. personal growth, between conscientiousness and purpose in life,
First, the research confirms that the overlap between basic person- and between neuroticism and negative affect. Fourth, examination
ality traits and well-being dimensions is substantial. Second, of facet-level correlates highlighted the unique importance of
whereas (lower) neuroticism is the strongest correlate of well- particular facets (e.g., depression and positive emotions in the Big
being within the Big Five framework, extraversion is the strongest Five framework and social self-esteem in the HEXACO frame-
correlate within the HEXACO framework. Conversely, conscien- work) as well as explaining differences between the HEXACO and
tiousness—which previous research has rarely highlighted in re- Big Five frameworks. Fifth, facets provided moderate levels of
lation to well-being—is a notable correlate within both frame- incremental prediction over and above domains when predicting
Table 10
Correlation Among Well-Being Scales for Combined Dataset (Lower Diagonal) and NEO
Dataset (Upper Diagonal)
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
SWB
1. Life satisfaction .36 ⫺.29 .41 .25 .51 .27 .52 .65
2. Positive affect .52 ⫺.09 .31 .23 .40 .32 .37 .36
3. Negative affect ⫺.44 ⫺.39 ⫺.32 ⫺.29 ⫺.43 ⫺.21 ⫺.33 ⫺.40
PWB
4. Positive relations .49 .53 ⫺.41 .45 .57 .53 .58 .63
5. Autonomy .16 .26 ⫺.42 .25 .55 .46 .48 .56
6. Environmental mastery .58 .60 ⫺.59 .61 .42 .47 .72 .74
7. Personal growth .36 .51 ⫺.38 .53 .44 .58 .53 .49
8. Purpose in life .55 .60 ⫺.49 .53 .38 .76 .69 .73
9. Self-acceptance .74 .63 ⫺.58 .60 .44 .77 .60 .77
Note. N ⫽ 903 for Combined Dataset; N ⫽ 1,673 for NEO Dataset; PWB ⫽ psychological well-being; SWB ⫽
subjective well-being.
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 303
Table 11
Correlations of NEO Facets With Well-Being Measures in NEO Dataset
Variable SWL PA NA PR AU EM PG PL SA M
N1. Anxiety ⫺.28 ⫺.16 .31 ⫺.21 ⫺.28 ⴚ.34 ⫺.06 ⫺.15 ⴚ.38 ⫺.23
N2. Angry hostility ⫺.23 ⫺.14 .35 ⴚ.39 ⫺.28 ⴚ.39 ⫺.20 ⫺.29 ⴚ.39 ⫺.29
N3. Depression ⴚ.48 ⴚ.32 .41 ⴚ.46 ⴚ.41 ⴚ.57 ⫺.27 ⴚ.49 ⴚ.66 ⫺.46
N4. Self-consciousness ⴚ.31 ⫺.27 .26 ⴚ.40 ⴚ.41 ⴚ.43 ⫺.22 ⴚ.34 ⴚ.50 ⫺.36
N5. Impulsiveness ⫺.15 ⫺.07 .19 ⫺.05 ⫺.14 ⫺.23 .04 ⫺.15 ⫺.21 ⫺.12
N6. Vulnerability ⴚ.39 ⴚ.35 .36 ⴚ.36 ⴚ.44 ⴚ.60 ⫺.28 ⴚ.48 ⴚ.59 ⫺.44
E1. Warmth .22 .27 ⫺.13 .59 .24 .32 .35 .31 .32 .33
E2. Gregariousness .19 .17 ⫺.07 .40 .04 .14 .24 .18 .18 .19
E3. Assertiveness .23 .28 ⫺.04 .31 .23 .28 .22 .23 .32 .26
E4. Activity .18 .29 .02 .22 .19 .25 .23 .30 .25 .24
E5. Excitement seeking .00 .12 .05 .07 ⫺.05 ⫺.06 .25 ⫺.07 ⫺.03 .03
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
E6. Positive emotions .34 .31 ⫺.14 .49 .22 .36 .42 .34 .40 .36
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
O1. Fantasy ⫺.02 .07 .06 .09 .03 ⫺.05 .30 .01 .00 .05
O2. Aesthetics .00 .10 .06 .10 .02 ⫺.02 .30 .01 ⫺.03 .06
O3. Feelings .07 .17 .04 .25 .14 .13 .41 .18 .12 .18
O4. Actions .08 .13 ⫺.03 .19 .12 .07 .43 .08 .12 .15
O5. Ideas .01 .19 ⫺.01 .09 .14 .08 .37 .09 .07 .13
O6. Values .02 .06 ⫺.11 .25 .23 .12 .40 .16 .13 .17
A1. Trust .22 .16 ⫺.15 .41 .12 .25 .17 .24 .27 .23
A2. Straightforwardness .02 ⫺.05 ⫺.15 .11 .13 .08 .05 .11 .07 .07
A3. Altruism .18 .14 ⫺.16 .43 .22 .28 .24 .30 .26 .26
A4. Compliance .05 ⫺.04 ⫺.15 .11 ⫺.06 .07 ⫺.03 .04 .08 .03
A5. Modesty ⫺.09 ⫺.13 ⫺.04 .05 .03 ⫺.06 .02 .00 ⫺.09 ⫺.03
A6. Tender-mindedness .07 .05 ⫺.11 .27 .22 .17 .27 .23 .18 .18
C1. Competence .37 .33 ⫺.24 .35 .35 .55 .28 .54 .51 .41
C2. Order .15 .14 ⫺.04 .06 .11 .30 .09 .30 .17 .17
C3. Dutifulness .17 .16 ⫺.15 .17 .31 .41 .17 .39 .28 .26
C4. Achievement striving .24 .33 ⫺.02 .18 .24 .39 .23 .46 .31 .30
C5. Self-discipline .28 .29 ⫺.19 .26 .34 .55 .19 .52 .43 .36
C6. Deliberation .15 .11 ⫺.14 .04 .09 .24 ⫺.04 .26 .18 .13
Note. N ⫽ 1,673; SWL ⫽ satisfaction with life; PA ⫽ positive affect; NA ⫽ negative affect; PR ⫽ positive relations; AU ⫽ autonomy; EM ⫽
environmental mastery; PG ⫽ personal growth; PL ⫽ purpose in life; SA ⫽ self-acceptance; Correlations .30 or above are in bold. Correlations equal to
or larger than .05, .07, and .09 are significant at .05, .01, and .001, respectively.
well-being. Across multiple measures of the Big Five and different well-being measures. In contrast, Steel et al. (2008)
HEXACO frameworks there were moderate levels of consistency focused on a small number of high-quality personality question-
in the degree of incremental prediction by facets. These findings naires such as the NEO and a limited set of reliable measures of
have fundamental implications for understanding well-being, in SWB. Similar to Steel et al. (2008), we focused the core meta-
terms of the role that both broad and narrow personality traits may analysis on a limited set of reliable personality and well-being
play in human flourishing. measures. Our research extends that of Steel et al. (2008) by
showing that the magnitude and pattern of correlations observed in
Personality and Well-Being Steel et al. (2008) is not limited to the NEO. A broadly similar
magnitude and pattern of well-being correlations was found across
According to effect size guidelines in individual differences a diverse range of Big Five measures. Second, the HEXACO and
research (e.g., Gignac & Szodorai, 2016), the relationship between the Big Five frameworks have a strong focus on affect, well-being,
personality and well-being is strong. The average correlation be- and psychological functioning. In general, it seems likely that
tween personality domains and well-being was r ⫽ .28, consider- measures based on the Big Five and related lexical approaches,
ably higher than the average correlation in individual differences such as the HEXACO, will generally exhibit strong correlations
research as a whole (i.e., r ⬃ .20). The strongest average correla- with well-being.
tions with well-being were ⫺.46 for Big Five neuroticism and .48
for HEXACO extraversion. Regression models indicated that Broad and Narrow Personality Traits of the Big Five
about half the observed variance in well-being scales can be
and HEXACO
explained by personality domains (46%) and facets (53%).
The domain-level correlations between Big Five personality and Overall, both the HEXACO and Big Five models are similarly
SWB were very similar to those reported in the meta-analysis by effective in predicting well-being. For the Big Five model, neu-
Steel et al. (2008) and larger and more nuanced than those reported roticism is a very strong predictor, extraversion and conscientious-
in the meta-analysis by DeNeve and Cooper (1998). There are ness are fairly strong, and openness and agreeableness are more
several reasons for this. First, DeNeve and Cooper (1998) included moderate. For the HEXACO model, extraversion is a very strong
many studies that predated the Big Five and also used a mixture of predictor (even stronger than Big Five neuroticism), conscientious-
304 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
Table 12
Correlations Between IPIP NEO Facets and Well-Being Measures in Combined Dataset
Variable SWL PA NA PR AU EM PG PL SA M
N1. Anxiety ⴚ.38 ⴚ.38 .59 ⴚ.33 ⴚ.43 ⴚ.56 ⴚ.31 ⴚ.36 ⴚ.53 ⴚ.43
N2. Angry hostility ⴚ.32 ⴚ.35 .54 ⴚ.32 ⴚ.29 ⴚ.45 ⴚ.30 ⴚ.33 ⴚ.43 ⴚ.37
N3. Depression ⴚ.65 ⴚ.58 .70 ⴚ.59 ⴚ.45 ⴚ.76 ⴚ.50 ⴚ.69 ⴚ.83 ⴚ.64
N4. Self-consciousness ⴚ.36 ⴚ.43 .49 ⴚ.45 ⴚ.56 ⴚ.56 ⴚ.42 ⴚ.44 ⴚ.55 ⴚ.47
N5. Impulsiveness ⫺.20 ⫺.22 .36 ⫺.13 ⴚ.34 ⴚ.36 ⫺.14 ⫺.27 ⴚ.31 ⫺.26
N6. Vulnerability ⴚ.41 ⴚ.43 .62 ⴚ.36 ⴚ.53 ⴚ.65 ⴚ.42 ⴚ.49 ⴚ.57 ⴚ.50
E1. Warmth .42 .50 ⴚ.40 .69 .25 .52 .44 .47 .53 .47
E2. Gregariousness .30 .36 ⫺.24 .46 .07 .33 .25 .24 .33 .29
E3. Assertiveness .34 .44 ⴚ.30 .42 .42 .47 .44 .46 .47 .42
E4. Activity .28 .41 ⫺.22 .29 .25 .49 .38 .51 .38 .36
E5. Excitement seeking .14 .23 ⫺.03 .17 .03 .09 .20 .04 .12 .12
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
E6. Positive emotions .50 .53 ⴚ.37 .59 .23 .48 .49 .47 .55 .47
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
O1. Fantasy .00 .11 .08 .09 .06 ⫺.06 .21 .03 .01 .04
O2. Aesthetics .08 .24 ⫺.06 .23 .15 .11 .42 .22 .16 .19
O3. Feelings .01 .09 .19 .19 .02 ⫺.04 .35 .20 .05 .08
O4. Actions .20 .30 ⫺.26 .27 .29 .29 .54 .32 .32 .31
O5. Ideas .12 .28 ⫺.17 .20 .41 .29 .48 .35 .26 .28
O6. Values ⫺.04 ⫺.04 .02 .01 .06 ⫺.08 .17 ⫺.04 ⫺.01 .00
A1. Trust .35 .32 ⴚ.37 .54 .10 .40 .34 .37 .42 .36
A2. Straightforwardness .08 .09 ⫺.25 .22 .15 .22 .21 .27 .17 .18
A3. Altruism .26 .36 ⫺.25 .52 .15 .34 .47 .43 .34 .35
A4. Compliance .13 .11 ⫺.21 .19 ⫺.04 .12 .17 .17 .15 .13
A5. Modesty ⴚ.30 ⫺.26 .16 ⫺.22 ⫺.18 ⫺.27 ⫺.17 ⫺.26 ⴚ.39 ⫺.25
A6. Tender-mindedness .10 .15 ⫺.07 .31 .07 .07 .33 .22 .14 .16
C1. Competence .41 .47 ⴚ.48 .42 .52 .66 .56 .68 .60 .53
C2. Order .10 .15 ⫺.14 .02 .12 .25 .10 .28 .13 .14
C3. Dutifulness .21 .23 ⴚ.34 .27 .30 .40 .34 .43 .32 .32
C4. Achievement striving .34 .45 ⫺.27 .29 .34 .54 .49 .67 .45 .43
C5. Self-discipline .34 .42 ⴚ.37 .26 .33 .61 .33 .58 .45 .41
C6. Deliberation .09 .06 ⫺.26 .09 .21 .26 .11 .30 .17 .17
Note. N ⫽ 903; SWL ⫽ satisfaction with life; PA ⫽ positive affect; NA ⫽ negative affect; PR ⫽ positive relations; AU ⫽ autonomy; EM ⫽
environmental mastery; PG ⫽ personal growth; PL ⫽ purpose in life; SA ⫽ self-acceptance. Correlations .30 or above are in bold. Correlations equal to
or larger than .07, .09, and .11 are significant at .05, .01, and .001, respectively.
ness is fairly strong, and honesty-humility, emotionality, agree- honesty-humility has a secondary correlation with Big Five con-
ableness, and openness are more modest. scientiousness, whereas HEXACO agreeableness has a secondary
Differences in well-being correlations between the Big Five and correlation with neuroticism, reflecting its content related to lower
HEXACO may largely result from how these models partition anger and hostility.
personality trait variance (for a review, see Ashton & Lee, 2019; Although organized differently across the Big Five and
Ashton et al., 2014). These differences can be readily appreciated HEXACO frameworks, the tendency to experience low levels of
by examining (a) the correlations between the HEXACO and the negative emotions and high levels of positive emotions accounts
Big Five (see Table 15 in the current paper and Table 1 in Gaughan for much of the effect of personality on well-being. In the Big Five
et al., 2012), (b) the item content of relevant HEXACO and Big model, neuroticism captures the broad set of tendencies to expe-
Five scales, and (c) the correlations between personality and well- rience negative emotions, whereas facets related to positive emo-
being at the facet-level for HEXACO and the Big Five. For
tions form only part of extraversion. Facets such as depression,
instance, HEXACO extraversion (a) correlates at ⫺.65 with IPIP
positive emotions, and social self-esteem are particularly strong
NEO neuroticism, (b) has many (reversed) items that relate to low
predictors of well-being. It is not surprising that these character-
self-esteem and depression (e.g., “I sometimes feel that I am a
istic ways of experiencing the world—viewing life through a more
worthless person”), and (c) shows correlations with well-being
most prominently for the facets of social self-esteem and liveli- negative lens, ruminating on negative experiences, and emphasiz-
ness. In contrast, HEXACO emotionality (a) correlated only .56 ing what’s wrong rather than what’s right with the world—trans-
with IPIP NEO neuroticism, and (b) combines traditional neurot- late into lower levels of well-being. On the other hand, Big Five
icism facet scales such as fearfulness and anxiety (which correlate extraversion may operate both through the tendency to experience
negatively with well-being) with more neutral emotional tenden- positive emotion as well as the more instrumental pathways paved
cies such as dependence (which is relatively uncorrelated with by the behavioral components of extraversion, such as facilitating
well-being) and prosocial tendencies such as sentimentality (which positive social connections and actively engaging with environ-
correlate positively with some aspects of well-being). HEXACO mental rewards (Smillie, Cooper, Wilt, & Revelle, 2012; Smillie,
honesty-humility and HEXACO agreeableness both correlate most Wilt, Kabbani, Garratt, & Revelle, 2015; Sun, Stevenson, Kabbani,
strongly with Big Five agreeableness, although HEXACO Richardson, & Smillie, 2017).
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 305
Table 13
Correlations Between HEXACO Facets and Well-Being Measures in HEXACO Dataset
Variable SWL PA NA PR AU EM PG PL SA M
H1: Sincerity .14 .10 ⫺.25 .21 .27 .24 .23 .19 .21 .20
H2: Fairness .19 .21 ⫺.22 .25 .16 .21 .18 .25 .23 .21
H3: Greed-avoidance .08 .04 ⫺.14 .11 .23 .03 .15 .07 .10 .11
H4: Modesty ⫺.05 .00 ⫺.09 .11 .03 .01 .10 .01 ⫺.06 .03
E1: Fearfulness ⫺.04 ⫺.16 .19 ⫺.15 ⴚ.37 ⫺.27 ⫺.22 ⫺.14 ⫺.17 ⫺.19
E2: Anxiety ⫺.26 ⫺.22 .47 ⫺.23 ⴚ.35 ⴚ.43 ⫺.23 ⫺.26 ⴚ.40 ⫺.32
E3: Dependence .09 .05 .25 .17 ⴚ.30 ⫺.19 .01 ⫺.08 ⫺.05 ⫺.06
E4: Sentimentality .13 .17 .11 .25 ⫺.14 .04 .22 .18 .07 .09
X1: Social self-esteem .57 .56 ⴚ.55 .62 .37 .70 .50 .62 .75 .58
X2: Social boldness .27 .35 ⫺.27 .39 .44 .38 .40 .38 .40 .36
X3: Sociability .27 .33 ⫺.20 .51 .09 .32 .30 .24 .31 .29
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
X4: Liveliness .52 .59 ⴚ.46 .60 .29 .66 .50 .58 .64 .54
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
A1: Forgiveness .21 .21 ⫺.18 .29 .09 .21 .19 .15 .23 .20
A2: Gentleness .17 .17 ⫺.15 .18 .06 .10 .13 .07 .13 .13
A3: Flexibility .14 .14 ⫺.19 .23 ⫺.02 .16 .14 .10 .17 .14
A4: Patience .22 .27 ⴚ.34 .20 .16 .27 .20 .19 .27 .24
C1: Organization .11 .19 ⫺.12 .07 .16 .33 .14 .31 .18 .18
C2: Diligence .26 .44 ⫺.29 .24 .36 .52 .44 .62 .41 .40
C3: Perfectionism ⫺.02 .13 ⫺.03 .02 .16 .15 .20 .27 .10 .12
C4: Prudence .17 .24 ⴚ.35 .15 .27 .34 .17 .33 .27 .25
O1: Aesthetic appreciation .09 .20 ⫺.06 .12 .22 .11 .33 .16 .13 .16
O2: Inquisitiveness .06 .21 ⫺.16 .10 .29 .21 .30 .16 .16 .18
O3: Creativity .05 .23 ⫺.06 .08 .25 .08 .28 .13 .17 .15
O4: Unconventionality .00 .14 .05 .02 .22 ⫺.04 .25 .05 .07 .07
I: Altruism .14 .21 ⫺.06 .28 .00 .12 .32 .25 .18 .17
Note. N ⫽ 465; SWL ⫽ satisfaction with life; PA ⫽ positive affect; NA ⫽ negative affect; PR ⫽ positive relations; AU ⫽ autonomy; EM ⫽
environmental mastery; PG ⫽ personal growth; PL ⫽ purpose in life; SA ⫽ self-acceptance. Correlations .30 or above are in bold.
Whereas most previous research has emphasized only neuroti- correlations with well-being. Each of these prosocial traits may
cism/emotionality and extraversion in relation to well-being (e.g., plausibly improve well-being by reducing interpersonal conflict
Diener et al., 1999; Schimmack et al., 2004; Smillie, Kern, & and helping to foster positive relations with others. Status seeking,
Uljarevic, 2018), the present research reveals that conscientious- manipulativeness, and greed (captured by honesty-humility and
ness is not far behind, and is perhaps even on par with extraver- some facets of Big Five agreeableness) may also create instability
sion. For instance, the average correlation for Big Five extraver- of social networks, with negative consequences for well-being.
sion was .37 versus .36 for Big Five conscientiousness (.28 for Although self-interest may bring short-term benefits, excessive
HEXACO conscientiousness). Conscientiousness emerged as par- self-interest may, in the long term, damage one’s reputation, social
ticularly important for purpose in life and environmental mastery, relationships, and sense of meaning in life. Furthermore, placing
although was somewhat less related to negative affect and positive substantial value on status symbols and power places more weight
relations. Several processes described by conscientiousness could on zero-sum aspects of life (Headey & Wearing, 1992). As a
account for its positive implications for well-being. First, consci- counterpoint, we note that the modesty facet in both the Big Five
entiousness is related to a sense of competence in life, and the and HEXACO models tended to be unrelated or negatively related
competence facet of conscientiousness was a particularly strong to well-being. This may suggest that an inability or unwillingness
predictor of well-being. Second, conscientiousness describes ef- to compare oneself favorably to others—whether this be in terms
fective self-regulation, as when one forgoes short-term pleasures of income, wealth, health, physical attractiveness, or even popu-
for the attainment of longer-term goals, whether they be related to larity on social media—may have negative implications for well-
family, education, finance, or health (Roberts, Lejuez, Krueger, being. Indeed, it is well-established that most people perceive their
Richards, & Hill, 2014). Third, achievement striving and diligence lives to be “better than average” (Alicke, Klotz, Breitenbecher,
can connect people with a sense of purpose and meaning, that can Yurak, & Vredenburg, 1995; Headey & Wearing, 1992), and that
facilitate a deeper sense of life satisfaction. However, as a small this rationalization may promote well-being.
counterpoint, we note that a desire for order and perfection gen- Finally, openness to experience was also a modest but never-
erally showed much weaker correlations with well-being. Consis- theless meaningful predictor of well-being, with correlations ap-
tent with highlighting the shortcomings of one’s achievements proximating the average effect size in individual differences re-
relative to demanding expectations, perfectionism showed small search. Openness comprises such characteristics as intellectual
negative semipartial correlations with some well-being dimensions curiosity, an ability to adapt to change, and the tendency to seek
after controlling for personality domains (for further discussion of novel experiences (Schmutte & Ryff, 1997). Consistent with this,
the benefits and costs of perfectionism, see Stoeber & Otto, 2006; the current study revealed that openness was particularly related to
Stoeber & Stoeber, 2009). personal growth, autonomy, and positive emotions. Whereas
Both the Big Five and HEXACO conceptions of agreeableness, Stephan (2009) found openness to feelings and ideas to be the most
as well as HEXACO honesty-humility, had relatively modest important facets in relation to life satisfaction, our current findings
306 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
Table 14
Variance Explained by Broad and Narrow Traits Across Measures
Measure SWL PA NA PR AU EM PG PL SA M
2
Broad: Adjusted R
NEO .25 .23 .21 .47 .27 .51 .41 .44 .50 .36
IPIP NEO .32 .43 .52 .50 .38 .65 .54 .58 .57 .50
HEXACO .25 .37 .35 .47 .39 .52 .39 .46 .45 .41
Big Five aspects .32 .54 .67 .44 .69 .53 .67 .61 .53 .56
M .29 .39 .44 .47 .43 .56 .50 .52 .51 .46
Narrow: Adj R2
NEO .30 .25 .24 .54 .38 .55 .48 .51 .56 .42
IPIP NEO .47 .48 .58 .59 .52 .71 .62 .70 .74 .60
HEXACO .38 .44 .44 .51 .44 .63 .45 .58 .61 .50
Big Five aspects .39 .59 .69 .52 .73 .55 .72 .65 .55 .60
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
M .39 .44 .49 .54 .52 .61 .56 .61 .61 .53
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Adj R2 change
NEO .06 .02 .03 .06 .11 .04 .07 .07 .06 .06
IPIP NEO .15 .05 .06 .10 .14 .06 .07 .12 .17 .10
HEXACO .13 .07 .09 .04 .05 .11 .06 .11 .16 .09
Big Five aspects .07 .06 .03 .08 .03 .02 .05 .04 .02 .04
M .10 .05 .05 .07 .08 .06 .06 .09 .10 .07
Adj R2 prop increase
NEO .24 .09 .16 .13 .43 .08 .16 .16 .13 .17
IPIP NEO .47 .12 .11 .19 .37 .09 .14 .21 .30 .22
HEXACO .51 .19 .26 .09 .13 .20 .14 .24 .36 .24
Big Five aspects .21 .11 .04 .18 .05 .03 .07 .06 .03 .09
M .36 .13 .14 .15 .24 .10 .13 .17 .20 .18
Note. SWL ⫽ satisfaction with life; PA ⫽ positive affect; NA ⫽ negative affect; PR ⫽ positive relations; AU ⫽ autonomy; EM ⫽ environmental mastery;
PG ⫽ personal growth; PL ⫽ purpose in life; SA ⫽ self-acceptance. Mean values are in bold.
varied somewhat across the different data sets. Openness to actions being, but the process through which a person achieves the good
was a salient predictor to emerge in our data, particularly in life. For those high on openness to experience, variety and growth
relation to personal growth. Openness appears to reflect an orien- are important, for those low in openness to experience, stability,
tation toward well-being that involves valuing novelty and non- safety and maintaining tradition may be more critical.
conformity, and viewing life as a process of growth and change.
This is reflected in the strong correlation between values and
Well-Being Dimensions
openness for the Big Five (Parks-Leduc, Feldman, & Bardi, 2015)
and the HEXACO (Anglim, Knowles, Dunlop, & Marty, 2017), One of the main insights revealed by the present study concerns
whereby people who are high on openness tend to value self- the differential patterns of correlations between personality and
direction, stimulation, and universalist values and are less inter- well-being as one shifts between SWB and PWB. Whereas SWB
ested in power and conformity. Given that openness is relatively focuses on the evaluation of the good life, PWB is more strongly
unrelated to life satisfaction, it may provide an example of a reflective of Eudaimonic perspectives. It is important to note,
personality trait that influences not just the experience of well- however, that this distinction is theoretical and conceptual,
Table 15
Correlations Among HEXACO and IPIP NEO Personality Domains From Combined Dataset
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
HEXACO
1. Honesty-humility
2. Emotionality .06
3. Extraversion .01 ⫺.21
4. Agreeableness .37 ⫺.18 .31
5. Conscientiousness .31 ⫺.11 .21 .22
6. Openness .13 ⫺.18 .19 .19 .17
IPIP NEO
7. Neuroticism ⫺.19 .56 ⴚ.65 ⫺.46 ⫺.36 ⫺.26
8. Extraversion ⫺.09 ⫺.08 .83 .17 .11 .13 ⫺.49
9. Agreeableness .67 .22 .12 .53 .26 .05 ⫺.17 .08
10. Conscientiousness .32 ⫺.14 .28 .19 .84 .09 ⫺.48 .19 .32
11. Openness .16 .06 .23 .14 .15 .71 ⫺.19 .30 .20 .14
Note. N ⫽ 465. Cross-correlations between personality measures greater than .50 are shown in bold.
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 307
Table 16
Correlations Between HEXACO and IPIP NEO Domains and Well-Being Measures for Combined Dataset
Measure SWL PA NA PR AU EM PG PL SA M
IPIP NEO
Neuroticism ⴚ.45 ⴚ.52 .69 ⴚ.46 ⴚ.55 ⴚ.70 ⴚ.47 ⴚ.56 ⴚ.68 ⴚ.56
Extraversion .42 .55 ⴚ.30 .63 .30 .53 .53 .49 .52 .47
Openness .09 .32 ⫺.04 .26 .28 .15 .57 .31 .24 .25
Agreeableness .15 .20 ⫺.21 .35 .04 .19 .29 .24 .19 .21
Conscientiousness .27 .39 ⴚ.37 .26 .39 .59 .38 .61 .45 .41
HEXACO
Honesty-humility .12 .12 ⫺.23 .22 .23 .16 .21 .17 .16 .18
Emotionality ⫺.03 ⫺.07 .37 .00 ⴚ.41 ⴚ.31 ⫺.09 ⫺.12 ⫺.20 ⫺.18
Extraversion .49 .56 ⴚ.45 .64 .37 .62 .52 .55 .64 .54
Agreeableness .24 .26 ⫺.28 .29 .10 .24 .21 .17 .26 .23
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Conscientiousness .17 .33 ⫺.26 .16 .30 .44 .31 .50 .31 .31
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Openness .07 .25 ⫺.08 .11 .31 .12 .37 .17 .17 .18
HEXACO Neuroticism ⴚ.48 ⴚ.49 .64 ⴚ.48 ⴚ.45 ⴚ.70 ⴚ.44 ⴚ.55 ⴚ.68 ⴚ.55
Note. N ⫽ 465; SWL ⫽ satisfaction with life; PA ⫽ positive affect; NA ⫽ negative affect; PR ⫽ positive relations; AU ⫽ autonomy; EM ⫽
environmental mastery; PG ⫽ personal growth; PL ⫽ purpose in life; SA ⫽ self-acceptance. Correlations equal to or larger than .10, .12, and .16 are
significant at .05, .01, and .001, respectively. M ⫽ mean correlation between the personality trait and well-being variables, where the correlation with
negative affect (NA) is reversed. Correlations .30 or above in bold.
whereas the empirical differences between these models are less Second, of the three components of SWB, life satisfaction was
clear cut. All nine dimensions of well-being are positively inter- less well predicted by personality compared with positive and
correlated (after reversing negative affect), despite each capturing negative affect. This is perhaps unsurprising given that the ten-
important unique variance. Additionally, the nine scales do not dency to experience positive and negative emotions is part of the
segregate into distinct SWB and PWB factors. Thus, it is important core content of personality scales (Pytlik Zillig, Hemenover, &
to consider both the broad and the scale-specific patterns of per- Dienstbier, 2002). In contrast, life satisfaction is a cognitive ap-
sonality correlates. praisal, influenced both by expectations and evaluations, and the
First, and in line with recent research (e.g., Anglim & Grant, individual’s choice of what factors are relevant to that judgment. It
2016), many PWB scales showed a much stronger overlap with is therefore a step removed from summaries of a person’s typical
personality compared with SWB scales. In the meta-analysis, behavior and experience. Such factors may help explain why life
correlations were larger for environmental mastery, personal satisfaction shows a much more modest overlap with personality
growth, and self-acceptance, and smaller for life satisfaction, al- compared to other dimensions of well-being. Interestingly, the
though the PWB scale of autonomy also had smaller correlations. facets of modesty and perfectionism showed negative semipartial
In the domain- and facet-level regression models this pattern was correlations with life satisfaction. Thus, whether through objective
also observed, although positive and negative affect were also circumstance, arrogance, or pleasant self-deception, very high life
predicted somewhat less well. These differences may partially be satisfaction is often related to seeing oneself and one’s life as
methodological. PWB is often measured with a 14-item per scale superior to those around you. Furthermore, perfectionism may lead
format whereas the standard life satisfaction measure (Diener et people to focus on ways that their life could conceivably be better.
al., 1985) involves only five items. Nonetheless, as we discuss At a more general level, it was apparent that each well-being
below, there are several theoretical reasons why some PWB scales dimension was characterized by a coherent pattern of personality
overlap more with particular personality traits. correlates. Specifically, positive affect, unsurprisingly, was well-
Table 17
Adjusted R Squared for Regression Models Predicting Well-Being Measures in Combined Dataset
Predictors k SWL PA NA PR AU EM PG PL SA M
HEXACO domains 6 .25 .37 .35 .47 .39 .52 .39 .46 .45 .41
NEO domains 5 .26 .43 .49 .50 .36 .63 .55 .55 .53 .48
HEXACO facets 25 .38 .44 .44 .51 .44 .63 .45 .58 .61 .50
NEO domains ⫹ HEXACO domains 11 .31 .45 .50 .53 .45 .64 .55 .57 .57 .51
NEO domains ⫹ HEXACO facets 30 .41 .50 .52 .59 .49 .70 .57 .64 .67 .57
NEO facets 30 .44 .50 .57 .59 .52 .70 .64 .70 .70 .60
HEXACO domains ⫹ NEO facets 36 .46 .50 .57 .59 .54 .71 .64 .71 .71 .60
HEXACO facets ⫹ NEO facets 55 .48 .52 .56 .61 .56 .72 .64 .70 .73 .61
Note. n ⫽ 465; k ⫽ number of predictors in regression model; M ⫽ the mean adjusted r-squared value averaged over well-being variables; NEO ⫽ IPIP
NEO; SWL ⫽ satisfaction with life; PA ⫽ positive affect; NA ⫽ negative affect; PR ⫽ positive relations; AU ⫽ autonomy; EM ⫽ environmental mastery;
PG ⫽ personal growth; PL ⫽ purpose in life; SA ⫽ self-acceptance. k is number of predictors. Mean represents the average variance explained for the
predictor set over the nine well-being measures.
308 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
predicted by extraversion and facets related to the tendency to though much of the perceived value of narrow traits is attributable
experience positive emotions. Negative affect was strongly related to the idea that facets might double prediction, more modest
to neuroticism, and most prominently with the facet of depression. incremental prediction is still of practical and theoretical impor-
Positive relations showed close connections with agreeableness tance. Facets also provide a richer profile of how and why different
and to some extent extraversion. Autonomy combined common domains correlate with relevant criteria, and provide a more nu-
well-being correlates with a fairly unique set of personality corre- anced picture of the personality–well-being interface.
lates that combine impulsiveness, noncompliance, and low trust, Interestingly, the HEXACO model was characterized by larger
with assertiveness and social boldness. Environmental mastery incremental facet prediction (as a proportion) than the Big Five,
correlated fairly uniformly across personality traits although it did both in terms of the NEO and IPIP NEO. This is striking, given
show some elevation for conscientiousness. Personal growth was that the NEO model has fewer domains and more facets than does
characterized most uniquely by openness with some amplification the HEXACO model, which should lead the NEO model to have
for diligence and achievement striving. Purpose in life was partic- stronger incremental prediction. The IPIP NEO also has more
ularly well characterized by conscientiousness and especially dil- items per facet, which should yield more reliable measurement of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
igence and achievement striving. Finally, self-acceptance showed the unique aspects of each facet. On the other hand, the HEXACO
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
a somewhat similar pattern of correlations to that of life satisfac- model incudes the interstitial trait of altruism, which is not used in
tion albeit at much greater levels. Although self-acceptance and scoring the domains, whereas all of the items of the Big Five
life satisfaction are highly correlated, self-acceptance places rela- facets/aspects are used to compute the domain scores. Critically,
tively less emphasis on the external conditions of life. This em- none of the HEXACO domains capture the general tendency to
phasis on liking or loathing oneself brings it very close to several experience negative emotions in the same way as Big Five neu-
dimensions of personality, as seen by the particularly large corre- roticism (Gaughan et al., 2012). Rather, the HEXACO model
lation with the facet of depression. Some of these cross- distributes content from Big Five neuroticism over various do-
correlations have already been noted in previous research (e.g., mains including extraversion (r ⫽ ⫺.50), emotionality (r ⫽ .52),
Anglim & Grant, 2016; Grant et al., 2009; Sun et al., 2018), and and agreeableness (r ⫽ ⫺.38; Gaughan et al., 2012). The most
the current study consolidates these observations through the first salient observation regarding incremental facet prediction within
comprehensive, large sample assessment. the HEXACO concerned the emotionality facet of anxiety and the
extraversion facets of social self-esteem and liveliness, all of
Incremental Prediction by Narrow Traits which seem to capture the most affect-related influences on well-
being.
One of the most critical contributions of the present study
concerns estimation of the proportional increase in variance ex-
plained by facets above and beyond domains. Average incremental Limitations and Future Research
variance explained by facets was 17%, 22%, and 24% for NEO,
IPIP NEO, and HEXACO taxonomies, respectively. The amount Because the current meta-analysis is based on self-report mea-
of incremental prediction showed some systematic variation across sures of personality and well-being, some care is required when
these three measures, although much less consistency was ob- generalizing the findings to the latent constructs. Participants vary
served for the Big Five Aspect Scales. In particular, life satisfac- in the degree to which social desirability influences their re-
tion, autonomy, and self-acceptance showed the greatest incremen- sponses, and items and scales vary in their degree of socially
tal prediction. These scales are not obviously broader or narrower desirable content (Anglim, Morse, et al., 2017; McCrae & Costa,
than other well-being dimensions. Rather they may exhibit a 1983; Wiggins, 1968). Person- and item-level variance in socially
complexity that means that several facets are important as is the desirable responding can lead to elevated correlations between
case with autonomy. Equally, there may be a particular facet that personality and well-being. This is particularly evident in the
aligns very closely, perhaps as can be seen with depression and minority of studies using low-paid participant samples where
social self-esteem in relation to self-acceptance. many participants engage in satisficing and semirandom respond-
A major focus of the literature on incremental facet prediction ing. We observed that in such studies, correlations between broad
has been on life satisfaction (Røysamb et al., 2018; Schimmack et personality traits were often elevated, which presumably translates
al., 2004; Steel et al., 2019), and this exhibited somewhat greater to elevated correlations between personality and well-being. As a
increases of between 24% and 51% depending on the personality consequence, care is needed when evaluating personality measures
framework. This estimate is broadly consistent with the largest in terms of how much variance they explain in self-reported
study to report incremental facet prediction to date, albeit limited well-being. One measure might predict self-reported well-being
to life satisfaction, which obtained 33% incremental prediction better because it has more socially desirable items. This may
(Røysamb et al., 2018). Steel et al. (2019) reported a 78% increase partially explain why the IPIP NEO predicted well-being better
based on a meta-analytic correlation matrix, but it is important to than the HEXACO PI R. Similarly, if one sample has more
note that meta-analytic regression is problematic. In particular, evaluative variance, then this may lead to elevated correlations
estimating a regression model with 30 highly correlated predictors, between personality and well-being. For example, the greater
where facet-level intercorrelations are not provided in the primary prediction of well-being in the Big Five Aspects dataset may
studies leads to unreliable and often inflated estimates of variance partially be explained by the use of a Mechanical Turk sample.
explained. Although several studies have examined other-reports of person-
More generally, we consider the proportional increase of 10% to ality and well-being (Dobewall, Realo, Allik, Esko, & Metspalu,
50% when using hierarchical instruments as noteworthy. Even 2013; Schimmack et al., 2004), more research is needed in this
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 309
area, particularly involving large samples, full hierarchical mea- of the role that personality traits play in pathways to the good
sures of personality, and multidimensional models of well-being. life.
Finally, it is worth considering the degree to which the
correlations between personality and well-being are attributable References
to artifactual measurement overlap (Anglim & Grant, 2016;
Schmutte & Ryff, 1997). Theoretically, the concepts of person- References marked with an asterisk were included in the meta-analysis.
ality and well-being can be distinguished in terms of temporal ⴱ
Agbo, A. A., & Ngwu, C. N. (2017). Aversion to happiness and the
frame-of-reference, implied stability, and degree of attribution experience of happiness: The moderating roles of personality. Person-
to the person versus the situation. Whereas personality is de- ality and Individual Differences, 111, 227–231. http://dx.doi.org/10
fined as relatively stable and originating more from the person, .1016/j.paid.2017.02.010
ⴱ
well-being captures the experience and appraisal of life at a Aghababaei, N. (2014). God, the good life, and HEXACO: The relations
given moment. Nonetheless, it is unsurprising that an individ- among religion, subjective well-being and personality. Mental Health,
ual’s general approach to acting in and experiencing the world Religion & Culture, 17, 284 –290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13674676
.2013.797956
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
experiences and evaluations of life. Importantly, the correla- of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 139 –142.
tions between personality and well-being index the extent and http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.037
nature of this relationship. So, for example, to remove negative ⴱ
Aghababaei, N., Błachnio, A., Arji, A., Chiniforoushan, M., Tekke, M., &
affect from neuroticism, or positive affect from extraversion is Fazeli Mehrabadi, A. (2016). Honesty– humility and the HEXACO
to fundamentally change the nature of these personality traits. structure of religiosity and well-being. Current Psychology, 35, 421–
However, many important research questions remain regarding 426. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12144-015-9310-5
ⴱ
the causal processes that relate personality and well-being. Aghababaei, N., & Tabik, M. T. (2013). Gratitude and mental health:
Facet-level analysis provides some perspective about which Differences between religious and general gratitude in a Muslim context.
aspects of a given trait are more or less important in predicting Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 16, 761–766. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1080/13674676.2012.718754
different dimensions of well-being. Nonetheless, the literature ⴱ
Ahadi, S. A., & Puente-Díaz, R. (2011). Acculturation, personality, and
would benefit from more experimental and experience sampling psychological adjustment. Psychological Reports, 109, 842– 862. http://
research exploring these questions (e.g., Jacques-Hamilton, dx.doi.org/10.2466/02.07.17.20.PR0.109.6.842-862
Sun, & Smillie, 2019). ⴱ
Albrecht, A. G., Dilchert, S., Deller, J., & Paulus, F. M. (2014). Openness
in cross-cultural work settings: A multicountry study of expatriates.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 96, 64 –75. http://dx.doi.org/10
Conclusion .1080/00223891.2013.821074
ⴱ
The current research reaffirms that personality is critical to the Albuquerque, I., de Lima, M. P., Matos, M., & Figueiredo, C. (2012).
experience of well-being. This is consistent with set-point theories Personality and subjective well-being: What hides behind global analy-
of well-being (Cummins, 2015; Headey & Wearing, 1989; Headey ses? Social Indicators Research, 105, 447– 460. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1007/s11205-010-9780-7
& Wearing, 1992), and the idea that well-being is relatively stable ⴱ
Alfonsi, G., Conway, M., & Pushkar, D. (2011). The lower subjective
despite short-term fluctuations in response to many transient social status of neurotic individuals: Multiple pathways through occu-
events. However, it is also important to remember that personality pational prestige, income, and illness. Journal of Personality, 79, 619 –
traits are not set like plaster but rather are malleable, with a wealth 642. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00684.x
of evidence that traits change across the life span (Ashton & Lee, Alicke, M. D., Klotz, M. L., Breitenbecher, D. L., Yurak, T. J., & Vre-
2016; McCrae et al., 1999; Soto, John, Gosling, & Potter, 2011), denburg, D. S. (1995). Personal contact, individuation, and the better-
after specific experiences (e.g., Zimmermann & Neyer, 2013) or than-average effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
interventions (e.g., Roberts et al., 2017), and even according to 804 – 825. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.804
ⴱ
one’s trait change-goals (e.g., Hudson & Fraley, 2015). It would Anand, S., Vidyarthi, P., Singh, S., & Ryu, S. (2015). Family interference
therefore be inappropriate to interpret the strong relation between and employee dissatisfaction: Do agreeable employees better cope with
stress? Human Relations, 68, 691–708. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
personality and well-being as indicative of the immutability of
0018726714539714
human happiness. Rather, efforts to improve well-being might Anglim, J., & Grant, S. L. (2014). Incremental criterion prediction of
target the most critical aspects of one’s habitual or characteristic personality facets over factors: Obtaining unbiased estimates and con-
patterns of behavior and experience, as reflected in basic person- fidence intervals. Journal of Research in Personality, 53, 148 –157.
ality traits. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.10.005
ⴱ
In summary, we have provided the most comprehensive Anglim, J., & Grant, S. (2016). Predicting psychological and subjective
assessment yet of the relations between personality traits and well-being from personality: Incremental prediction from 30 facets over
dimensions of well-being. Our study expands the mapping of the Big 5. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 59 – 80. http://dx.doi.org/
personality to well-being by encompassing both the Big Five 10.1007/s10902-014-9583-7
ⴱ
and the increasingly popular HEXACO model of personality, Anglim, J., & Horwood, S. (2019). HEXACO IPIP well-being data.
Unpublished dataset presented in the current article.
and also both Diener’s SWB perspective as well as Ryff’s PWB
Anglim, J., Knowles, E. R., Dunlop, P. D., & Marty, A. (2017). HEXACO
perspective on well-being. Moreover, our analyses span personality and Schwartz’s personal values: A facet-level analysis. Jour-
domain-level traits and narrower aspects and facets within the nal of Research in Personality, 68, 23–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
personality trait hierarchy, while contributing more broadly to .jrp.2017.04.002
methods for synthesizing facet-level research. Taken together, Anglim, J., Morse, G., De Vries, R. E., MacCann, C., & Marty, A. (2017).
the findings reported here expand and enrich our understanding Comparing job applicants to non-applicants using an item-level bifactor
310 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
ⴱ
model on the HEXACO personality inventory. European Journal of Baselmans, B. M. L., van de Weijer, M. P., Abdellaoui, A., Vink, J. M.,
Personality, 31, 669 – 684. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.2120 Hottenga, J. J., Willemsen, G., . . . Bartels, M. (2019). A Genetic
Anglim, J., & O’Connor, P. (2019). Measurement and research using the investigation of the well-being spectrum. Behavior Genetics, 49, 286 –
Big Five, HEXACO, and narrow traits: A primer for researchers and 297. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10519-019-09951-0
ⴱ
practitioners. Australian Journal of Psychology, 71, 16 –25. http://dx.doi Baudin, N., Aluja, A., Rolland, J.-P., & Blanch, A. (2011). The role of
.org/10.1111/ajpy.12202 personality in satisfaction with life and sport. Behavioral Psychology/
Anglim, J., Weinberg, M. K., & Cummins, R. A. (2015). Bayesian hier- Psicología Conductual, 19, 333–345.
archical modeling of the temporal dynamics of subjective well-being: A ⴱ
Bauer, J. J., & McAdams, D. P. (2010). Eudaimonic growth: Narrative
10 year longitudinal analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 59, growth goals predict increases in ego development and subjective well-
1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2015.08.003 being 3 years later. Developmental Psychology, 46, 761–772. http://dx
ⴱ
Antunes, A. C., Caetano, A., Pina, E., & Cunha, M. (2017). Reliability and .doi.org/10.1037/a0019654
construct validity of the Portuguese version of the Psychological Capital ⴱ
Beer, A., Watson, D., & McDade-Montez, E. (2013). Self-other agree-
Questionnaire. Psychological Reports, 120, 520 –536. http://dx.doi.org/ ment and assumed similarity in neuroticism, extraversion, and trait
10.1177/0033294116686742 affect: Distinguishing the effects of form and content. Assessment, 20,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Anusic, I., Schimmack, U., Pinkus, R. T., & Lockwood, P. (2009). The 723–737. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191113500521
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
nature and structure of correlations among Big Five ratings: The halo- Begg, C. B., & Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank
alpha-beta model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics, 50, 1088 –1101. http://
1142–1156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017159 dx.doi.org/10.2307/2533446
ⴱ
Anwar, C. M. (2017). Linkages between personality and knowledge ⴱ
Belsky, J., Crnic, K., & Woodworth, S. (1995). Personality and parenting:
sharing behavior in workplace: Mediating role of affective states. E⫹M: Exploring the mediating role of transient mood and daily hassles. Jour-
Ekonomie a Management, 20, 102–115. http://dx.doi.org/10.15240/tul/ nal of Personality, 63, 905–929. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494
001/2017-2-008 .1995.tb00320.x
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2005). Honesty-humility, the big five, and the ⴱ
Benet-Martínez, V., & Karakitapoğlu-Aygün, Z. (2003). The interplay of
five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 73, 1321–1354. http://dx.doi cultural syndromes and personality in predicting life satisfaction: Com-
.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00351.x
paring Asian Americans and European Americans. Journal of Cross-
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2016). Age trends in HEXACO-PI-R self-
Cultural Psychology, 34, 38 – 60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00220
reports. Journal of Research in Personality, 64, 102–111. http://dx.doi
22102239154
.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.08.008 ⴱ
Benotsch, E. G., Lutgendorf, S. K., Watson, D., Fick, L. J., & Lang, E. V.
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2019). How well do Big Five measures capture
(2000). Rapid anxiety assessment in medical patients: Evidence for the
HEXACO scale variance? Journal of Personality Assessment, 101,
validity of verbal anxiety ratings. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 22,
567–573.
199 –203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02895114
Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., & de Vries, R. E. (2014). The HEXACO Honesty- ⴱ
Bianchi, R., Rolland, J. P., & Salgado, J. F. (2018). Burnout, depression,
Humility, Agreeableness, and Emotionality factors: A review of research
and borderline personality: A 1,163-participant study. Frontiers in Psy-
and theory. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18, 139 –152.
chology, 8, 2336. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02336
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868314523838 ⴱ
Biderman, M. D., McAbee, S. T., Job Chen, Z., & Hendy, N. T. (2018).
Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Perugini, M., Szarota, P., de Vries, R. E., Di Blas,
Assessing the evaluative content of personality questionnaires using
L., . . . De Raad, B. (2004). A six-factor structure of personality-
bifactor models. Journal of Personality Assessment, 100, 375–388.
descriptive adjectives: Solutions from psycholexical studies in seven
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2017.1406362
languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 356 –366. ⴱ
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.356 Blatný, M., Květon, P., Jelínek, M., Šolcová, I., Zábrodská, K., Mudrák,
ⴱ
Augusto Landa, J. M., Martos, M. P., & López-Zafra, E. (2010). Emo- J., & Machovcová, K. (2018). The influence of personality traits on life
tional intelligence and personality traits as predictors of psychological satisfaction through work engagement and job satisfaction among aca-
well-being in Spanish undergraduates. Social Behavior and Personality, demic faculty members. Studia Psychologica, 60, 274 –286. http://dx.doi
38, 783–793. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2010.38.6.783 .org/10.21909/sp.2018.04.767
ⴱ
ⴱ
Austin, E., Saklofske, D., & Mastoras, S. (2010). Emotional intelligence, Blatný, M., Millová, K., Jelínek, M., & Osecká, T. (2015). Personality
coping and exam-related stress in Canadian undergraduate students. predictors of successful development: Toddler temperament and adoles-
Australian Journal of Psychology, 62, 42–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ cent personality traits predict well-being and career stability in middle
00049530903312899 adulthood. PLoS ONE, 10, e0126032. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal
ⴱ
Aykaç, B., Copuroğlu, C., Ozcan, M., Ciftdemir, M., & Yalnız, E. (2011). .pone.0126032
ⴱ
Postoperative evaluation of quality of life in lumbar spinal stenosis Bogin, L. (2018). A portrait of college success: Grit, theories of intelli-
patients following instrumented posterior decompression. Acta Ortho- gence, and cumulative life adversity (Doctoral dissertation). Pace Uni-
paedica et Traumatologica Turcica, 45, 47–52. versity, Pleasantville, NY.
ⴱ ⴱ
Balgiu, B. A. (2018). The psychometric properties of the Big Five Boland, A., & Cappeliez, P. (1997). Optimism and neuroticism as pre-
inventory-10 (BFI-10) including correlations with subjective and psy- dictors of coping and adaptation in older woman. Personality and
chological well-being. Global Journal of Psychology Research: New Individual Differences, 22, 909 –919. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-
Trends and Issues, 8, 61– 69. http://dx.doi.org/10.18844/gjpr.v8i2.3434 8869(96)00251-6
ⴱ ⴱ
Baltes, B. B., Zhdanova, L. S., & Clark, M. A. (2011). Examining the Bono, T. J. (2011). What good is engagement? Predicting academic
relationships between personality, coping strategies, and work–family performance and college satisfaction from personality, social support,
conflict. Journal of Business and Psychology, 26, 517–530. http://dx.doi and student engagement (Doctoral thesis). Washington University, Saint
.org/10.1007/s10869-010-9207-0 Louis, MO.
ⴱ ⴱ
Barr, P. (2018). Personality traits, state positive and negative affect, and Boudreau, J. W., Boswell, W. R., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Effects of
professional quality of life in neonatal nurses. Journal of Obstetric, personality on executive career success in the United States and Europe.
Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing, 47, 771–782. http://dx.doi.org/10 Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 53– 81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/
.1016/j.jogn.2018.08.003 jvbe.2000.1755
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 311
ⴱ ⴱ
Brailovskaia, J., Bierhoff, H. W., & Margraf, J. (2019). How to identify Carrillo, E., Prado-Gascó, V., Fiszman, S., & Varela, P. (2012). How
narcissism with 13 items? Validation of the German Narcissistic Per- personality traits and intrinsic personal characteristics influence the
sonality Inventory-13 (G-NPI-13). Assessment, 26, 630 – 644. http://dx consumer’s choice of reduced-calorie food. Food Research Interna-
.doi.org/10.1177/1073191117740625 tional, 49, 792–797. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodres.2012.09.006
ⴱ ⴱ
Brailovskaia, J., & Margraf, J. (2016). Comparing Facebook users and Castro Solano, A., & Cosentino, A. C. (2018). IVyF abreviado—
Facebook non-users: Relationship between personality traits and mental IVyFabre: Análisis psicométrico y de estructura factorial en Argentina
health variables: An exploratory study. PLoS ONE, 11, e0166999. http:// [sci Brief Form (sci-bf): Psychometric and factor structure analyses in
dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0166999 Argentina]. Avances en Psicología Latinoamericana, 36, 619 – 637.
ⴱ
Brailovskaia, J., & Margraf, J. (2018). What does media use reveal about http://dx.doi.org/10.12804/revistas.urosario.edu.co/apl/a.4681
ⴱ
personality and mental health? An exploratory investigation among Cellini, N., Duggan, K. A., & Sarlo, M. (2017). Perceived sleep quality:
German students. PLoS ONE, 13, e0191810. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/ The interplay of neuroticism, affect, and hyperarousal. Sleep Health, 3,
journal.pone.0191810 184 –189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2017.03.001
ⴱ ⴱ
Brajša-Žganec, A., Ivanović, D., & Lipovčan, L. K. (2011). Personality Chambers, B. J. (2004). Adjustment to career termination in professional
traits and social desirability as predictors of subjective well-being. hockey players (Doctoral thesis). Simon Fraser University, Vancouver,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Bratko, D., & Sabol, J. (2006). Personality and basic psychological needs Chan, B. C. L., Luciano, M., & Lee, B. (2018). Interaction of physical
as predictors of life satisfaction: Results of the on-line study [Osobine activity and personality in the subjective wellbeing of older adults in
ličnosti i osnovne psihološke potrebe kao prediktori zadovoljstva Hong Kong and the United Kingdom. Behavioral Sciences, 8, 71.
životom: Rezultati on-line istraživanja]. Drustvena Istrazivanja, 15, http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/bs8080071
ⴱ
693–711. Chen, C. (2015). Incremental validity of achievement goals in predicting
ⴱ
Brenner, K., St-Hilaire, A., Liu, A., Laplante, D. P., & King, S. (2011). subjective well-being among university students. Journal of Cognitive
Cortisol response and coping style predict quality of life in schizophre- Education and Psychology, 14, 38 – 62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/1945-
nia. Schizophrenia Research, 128, 23–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j 8959.14.1.38
ⴱ
.schres.2011.01.016 Chen, F. F., Hayes, A., Carver, C. S., Laurenceau, J.-P., & Zhang, Z.
Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning (2012). Modeling general and specific variance in multifaceted con-
the good society. In M. H. Apley (Ed.), Adaptation-level theory (pp. structs: A comparison of the bifactor model to other approaches. Journal
287–305). New York, NY: Academic Press. of Personality, 80, 219 –251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494
ⴱ
Burles, F., Guadagni, V., Hoey, F., Arnold, A. E., Levy, R. M., O’Neill, .2011.00739.x
ⴱ
T., & Iaria, G. (2014). Neuroticism and self-evaluation measures are Chen, M. M. (2011). A study of the criterion and incremental validity of
related to the ability to form cognitive maps critical for spatial orienta- the Structured Interview for DSM–IV personality, the revised neuroti-
tion. Behavioural Brain Research, 271, 154 –159. http://dx.doi.org/10 cism, extraversion, openness - personality inventory, and schedule for
.1016/j.bbr.2014.06.002 adaptive and nonadaptive personality (Doctoral thesis). St. John’s Uni-
ⴱ
Burton, C. M., Plaks, J. E., & Peterson, J. B. (2015). Why Do Conserva- versity, New York, NY.
ⴱ
tives Report Being Happier Than Liberals? The Contribution of Neu- Chen, S. X., & Carey, T. P. (2009). Assessing Citizenship Behavior in
roticism. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 3, 89 –102. http:// Educational Contexts. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27,
dx.doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v3i1.117 125–137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0734282908325146
ⴱ
Butler, J., & Kern, M. L. (2016). The PERMA-Profiler: A brief multidi- Choi, Y., & Lee, D. (2014). Psychological capital, Big Five traits, and
mensional measure of flourishing. International Journal of Wellbeing, 6, employee outcomes. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 29, 122–140.
6. http://dx.doi.org/10.5502/ijw.v6i3.526 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/JMP-06-2012-0193
ⴱ ⴱ
Bye, D., & Pushkar, D. (2009). How need for cognition and perceived Chopik, W. J., & Lucas, R. E. (2019). Actor, partner, and similarity effects
control are differentially linked to emotional outcomes in the transition of personality on global and experienced well-being. Journal of Re-
to retirement. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 320 –332. http://dx.doi.org/ search in Personality, 78, 249 –261. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018
10.1007/s11031-009-9135-3 .12.008
ⴱ
Cabrera-Darias, M., & Marrero-Quevedo, R. J. (2015). Motivos, person- Churchyard, J. S., Pine, K. J., Sharma, S., & Fletcher, B. C. (2014). Same
alidad y bienestar subjetivo en el voluntariado [Motives, personality and traits, different variance: Item-level variation within personality mea-
subjective well-being in volunteering]. Anales de Psicología, 31, 791– sures. SAGE Open, 4, 4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2158244014522634
ⴱ
801. http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.31.3.180921 Çikrikçi, Ö. (2019). An exploration of the associations among positivity,
ⴱ
Caprara, G. V., Alessandri, G., Eisenberg, N., Kupfer, A., Steca, P., general distress and internet addiction: The mediating effect of general
Caprara, M. G., . . . Abela, J. (2012). The positivity scale. Psychological distress. Psychiatry Research, 272, 628 – 637. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Assessment, 24, 701–712. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026681 j.psychres.2018.12.147
ⴱ ⴱ
Caprara, G. V., Fratte, A., & Steca, P. (2002). Determinanti personali del Clark, M. A., Lelchook, A. M., & Taylor, M. L. (2010). Beyond the Big
benessere nell’adolescenza: Indicatori e predittori [Personal determi- Five: How narcissism, perfectionism, and dispositional affect relate to
nants of well-being in adolescence: Indicators and predictors]. Psicolo- workaholism. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 786 –791.
gia Clinica dello Sviluppo, 6, 203–233. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.01.013
ⴱ ⴱ
Carciofo, R., & Song, N. (2019). The Chinese Morningness-Eveningness- Clifton, J. D. W., Baker, J. D., Park, C. L., Yaden, D. B., Clifton,
Stability-Scale improved (MESSi): Validity, reliability, and associations A. B. W., Terni, P., . . . Seligman, M. E. P. (2019). Primal world beliefs.
with sleep quality, personality, affect and life satisfaction. Chronobiol- Psychological Assessment, 31, 82–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
ogy International, 36, 1036 –1046. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07420528 pas0000639
ⴱ
.2019.1608225 Compton, W. C., Smith, M. L., Cornish, K. A., & Qualls, D. L. (1996).
ⴱ
Carmona-Halty, M. A., & Rojas-Paz, P. P. (2014). Rasgos de personali- Factor structure of mental health measures. Journal of Personality and
dad, necesidad de cognición y satisfacción vital en estudiantes univer- Social Psychology, 71, 406 – 413. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
sitarios chilenos [Personality traits, need for cognition and life satisfac- .71.2.406
ⴱ
tion in Chilean university students]. Universitas Psychologica, 13, 83– Correa, T., Hinsley, A. W., & de Zúñiga, H. G. (2010). Who interacts on
93. http://dx.doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY13-1.rpnc the Web?: The intersection of users’ personality and social media use.
312 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 247–253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-
j.chb.2009.09.003 analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psycholog-
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1995). Domains and facets: Hierarchical ical Bulletin, 124, 197–229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.2
personality assessment using the revised NEO personality inventory. .197
ⴱ
Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 21–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/ Denovan, M. A. (2018). Investigating student stress from a positive
s15327752jpa6401_2 psychology perspective (Doctoral thesis). Sheffield Hallam University,
ⴱ Sheffield, England.
Costa, P. T., & MacCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO personality inven-
tory (NEO PI-R) and NEO five-factor inventory (NEO-FFI): Profes- De Raad, B., Barelds, D. P., Timmerman, M. E., De Roover, K., Mlačić,
sional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. B., & Church, A. T. (2014). Towards a pan-cultural personality struc-
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2008). The Revised NEO Personality ture: Input from 11 psycholexical studies. European Journal of Person-
Inventory (NEO-PI-R). In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske ality, 28, 497–510. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.1953
ⴱ
(Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality theory and assessment (Vol. Deventer, J., Lüdtke, O., Nagy, G., Retelsdorf, J., & Wagner, J. (2019).
2, pp. 179 –198). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Against all odds - is a more differentiated view of personality develop-
ⴱ ment in emerging adulthood needed? The case of young apprentices.
Cotter, E. W., & Fouad, N. A. (2011). The Relationship Between Sub-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
jective Well-Being and Vocational Personality Type. Journal of Career British Journal of Psychology, 110, 60 – 86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Drezno, M., Stolarski, M., & Matthews, G. (2019). An in-depth look into jective indicators of successful aging. Psychology and Aging, 13, 531–
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
ⴱ
Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. Halama, P., Martos, T., & Adamovováú, L. (2010). Religiosity and
American Psychologist, 48, 26 –34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003- well-being in Slovak and Hungarian student samples: The role of per-
066X.48.1.26 sonality traits. Studia Psychologica, 52, 101–116.
ⴱ
Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality Harris, P. R. (2002). Constructive thinking as a mediator of the relation-
inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-factor models. ships between neuroticism, extraversion, and subjective well-being (the-
Personality Psychology in Europe, 7, 7–28. sis). ProQuest Information & Learning.
ⴱ
Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Hart, P. M. (1999). Predicting employee life satisfaction: A coherent
Cloninger, C. R., & Gough, H. G. (2006). The international personality model of personality, work, and nonwork experiences, and domain
item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. Journal satisfactions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 564 –584. http://dx.doi
of Research in Personality, 40, 84 –96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp .org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.4.564
ⴱ
.2005.08.007 Hayes, N., & Joseph, S. (2003). Big 5 correlates of three measures of
ⴱ
Goldberg, S. B., Flook, L., Hirshberg, M. J., Findley, D., Kesebir, P., subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 723–
Schaefer, S. M., & Davidson, R. J. (2017). Getting a grip on the handgrip 727. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00057-0
task: Handgrip duration correlates with neuroticism but not conscien- Headey, B., & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjective
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
tiousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1367. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/ well-being: Toward a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Person-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
ⴱ
Hossack, R. C. (1997). Salutogenic and pathogenic orientations to life: National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
ⴱ
Kim, H., Schimmack, U., Cheng, C., Webster, G. D., & Spectre, A. Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO
(2016). The role of positive self-evaluation on cross-cultural differences personality inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 329 –358.
in well-being. Cross-Cultural Research: The Journal of Comparative http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8
Social Science, 50, 85–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1069397115617902 Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2006). Further assessment of the HEXACO
ⴱ
Kirkland, T., Gruber, J., & Cunningham, W. A. (2015). Comparing Personality Inventory: Two new facet scales and an observer report form.
happiness and hypomania risk: A study of extraversion and neuroticism Psychological Assessment, 18, 182–191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-
aspects. PLoS ONE, 10, e0132438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal 3590.18.2.182
.pone.0132438 Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2013). Prediction of self-and observer report
ⴱ
Kjell, O. N. E., Nima, A. A., Sikström, S., Archer, T., & Garcia, D. scores on HEXACO-60 and NEO-FFI scales. Journal of Research in
(2013). Iranian and Swedish adolescents: Differences in personality Personality, 47, 668 – 675. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.06.002
traits and well-being. PeerJ, 1, e197. http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.197 Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2014). The dark triad, the big five, and the
ⴱ
Kluemper, D. H. (2008). Trait emotional intelligence: The impact of HEXACO model. Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 2–5.
core-self evaluations and social desirability. Personality and Individual http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.048
Differences, 44, 1402–1412. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.12 Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2018). Psychometric properties of the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Luhmann, M., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2014). Thinking about 0022-3514.82.1.112
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
one’s subjective well-being: Average trends and individual differences. McGregor, I., & Little, B. R. (1998). Personal projects, happiness, and
Journal of Happiness Studies, 15, 757–781. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ meaning: On doing well and being yourself. Journal of Personality and
s10902-013-9448-5 Social Psychology, 74, 494 –512. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
ⴱ
MacCann, C., Lipnevich, A. A., Burrus, J., & Roberts, R. D. (2012). The .74.2.494
best years of our lives? Coping with stress predicts school grades, life ⴱ
McKay, D. A. (2017). A facet and domain-level analysis of two trait
satisfaction, and feelings about high school. Learning and Individual models of personality: Relationship with subjective well-being (Doctoral
Differences, 22, 235–241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.08.004 dissertation). The University of Akron, Akron, OH.
ⴱ
MacInnis, C. C., Busseri, M. A., Choma, B. L., & Hodson, G. (2013). The McMahan, E. A., Renken, M. D., Kehn, A., & Nitkova, M. (2013).
happy cyclist: Examining the association between generalized authori- Individual conceptions of well-being predict psychological and subjec-
tarianism and subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differ- tive well-being: Beyond the big five. In F. Sarracino (Ed.), The happi-
ences, 55, 789 –793. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.06.018
ⴱ
ness compass: Theories, actions and perspectives for well-being (pp.
Mangino, A. M. (2018). Connectedness to nature, life satisfaction, and
3–16). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
personality: Humans, nature, and therapy (thesis). Alliant International ⴱ
Meléndez, J. C., Satorres, E., Cujiño, M. A., & Reyes, M. F. (2019). Big
University, Sacramento, CA.
ⴱ Five and psychological and subjective well-being in Colombian older
Marcionetti, J., & Rossier, J. (2016). Global life satisfaction in adoles-
adults. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 82, 88 –93. http://dx.doi
cence. Journal of Individual Differences, 37, 135–144. http://dx.doi.org/
.org/10.1016/j.archger.2019.01.016
10.1027/1614-0001/a000198 ⴱ
ⴱ Mellor, D. J., Cummins, R. A., Karlinski, E., & Storer, S. P. (2003). The
Margolis, S., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2019). Experimental manipulation of
management of subjective quality of life by short-stay hospital patients:
extraverted and introverted behavior and its effects on well-being. Jour-
An exploratory study. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 1, 39.
nal of Experimental Psychology: General. Advance online publication.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1477-7525-1-39
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000668 ⴱ
ⴱ Michel, J. S., & Clark, M. A. (2013). Investigating the relative importance
Margolis, S., Schwitzgebel, E., Ozer, D. J., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2019). A
of individual differences on the work-family interface and the moderat-
new measure of life satisfaction: The Riverside Life Satisfaction Scale.
ing role of boundary preference for segmentation. Stress and Health, 29,
Journal of Personality Assessment, 101, 621– 630.
ⴱ
Marrero, R. J. (2019). Dataset on NEO PI R Personality and well-being 324 –336.
ⴱ
partially based on Marrero et al (2016). http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/sjp Miciuk, Ł. R., Jankowski, T., Laskowska, A., & Oleś, P. (2016). Positive
.2016.95 Orientation and the Five-Factor Model. Polish Psychological Bulletin,
Marrero, R. J., Rey, M., & Hernández-Cabrera, J. A. (2016). Can Big Five 47, 141–148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/ppb-2016-0016
ⴱ
facets distinguish between hedonic and eudaimonic well-being? A dom- Miciuk, Ł. R., Jankowski, T., & Oleś, P. (2016). Incremental validity of
inance analysis. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 19, E84. http://dx positive orientation: Predictive efficiency beyond the five-factor model.
.doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2016.95 Health Psychology Report, 4, 294 –302. http://dx.doi.org/10.5114/hpr
ⴱ
Marrero Quevedo, R. J., & Carballeira Abella, M. (2011). Well-being and .2016.59895
ⴱ
personality: Facet-level analyses. Personality and Individual Differ- Mongrain, M., Barnes, C., Barnhart, R., & Zalan, L. B. (2018). Acts of
ences, 50, 206 –211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.030 kindness reduce depression in individuals low on agreeableness. Trans-
ⴱ lational Issues in Psychological Science, 4, 323–334. http://dx.doi.org/
Marshall, G. N., Wortman, C. B., Kusulas, J. W., Hervig, L. K., &
Vickers, Jr., R. R. (1992). Distinguishing optimism from pessimism: 10.1037/tps0000168
ⴱ
Relations to fundamental dimensions of mood and personality. Journal Montasem, A., Brown, S. L., & Harris, R. (2013). Do core self-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 1067–1074. http://dx.doi.org/ evaluations and trait emotional intelligence predict subjective well-being
10.1037/0022-3514.62.6.1067 in dental students? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 1097–
ⴱ 1103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12074
Martin, A. J., Nejad, H. G., Colmar, S., & Liem, G. A. D. (2013).
ⴱ
Adaptability: How students’ responses to uncertainty and novelty predict Morris, M. B., Burns, G. N., Periard, D. A., & Shoda, E. A. (2015).
their academic and non-academic outcomes. Journal of Educational Extraversion– emotional stability circumplex traits and subjective well-
Psychology, 105, 728 –746. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032794 being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 16, 1509 –1523. http://dx.doi.org/
ⴱ 10.1007/s10902-014-9573-9
Martínez-Molina, A., & Arias, V. B. (2018). Balanced and positively
ⴱ
worded personality short-forms: Mini-IPIP validity and cross-cultural Morrison, K. A. (1997). Personality correlates of the Five-Factor Model
invariance. PeerJ, 6, e5542. http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.5542 for a sample of business owners/managers: Associations with scores on
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1983). Social desirability scales: More Self-Monitoring, Type A Behavior, Locus of Control, and Subjective
substance than style. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, Well-being. Psychological Reports, 80, 255–272. http://dx.doi.org/10
882– 888. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.51.6.882 .2466/pr0.1997.80.1.255
318 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
ⴱ
Morsunbul, U. (2014). The validity and reliability study of the Turkish cial Psychological and Personality Science, 9, 364 –371. http://dx.doi
version of Quick Big Five Personality Test. Dusunen Adam. The Journal .org/10.1177/1948550617707016
ⴱ
of Psychiatry and Neurological Sciences, 27, 316 –322. Oishi, S., Krochik, M., Roth, D., & Sherman, G. D. (2012). Residential
Mõttus, R., Kandler, C., Bleidorn, W., Riemann, R., & McCrae, R. R. mobility, personality, and subjective and physical well-being. Social
(2017). Personality traits below facets: The consensual validity, longi- Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 153–161. http://dx.doi.org/10
tudinal stability, heritability, and utility of personality nuances. Journal .1177/1948550611412395
ⴱ
of Personality and Social Psychology, 112, 474 – 490. http://dx.doi.org/ Oken, B. S., Wahbeh, H., Goodrich, E., Klee, D., Memmott, T., Miller,
10.1037/pspp0000100 M., & Fu, R. (2017). Meditation in stressed older adults: Improvements
Mõttus, R., McCrae, R. R., Allik, J., & Realo, A. (2014). Cross-rater in self-rated mental health not paralleled by improvements in cognitive
agreement on common and specific variance of personality scales and function or physiological measures. Mindfulness, 8, 627– 638. http://dx
items. Journal of Research in Personality, 52, 47–54. http://dx.doi.org/ .doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0640-7
ⴱ
10.1016/j.jrp.2014.07.005 Olesen, M. H., Thomsen, D. K., & O’Toole, M. S. (2015). Subjective
ⴱ
Murray, G. (2002). The relationship between the Big-5 personality factors well-being: Above neuroticism and extraversion, autonomy motivation
and life satisfaction in two community samples. Unpublished data. matters. Personality and Individual Differences, 77, 45– 49. http://dx.doi
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Musek, J. (2007). A general factor of personality: Evidence for the Big One Ones, D. S., & Viswesvaran, C. (1996). Bandwidth–fidelity dilemma in
in the five-factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 1213– personality measurement for personnel selection. Journal of Organiza-
1233. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2007.02.003 tional Behavior, 17, 609 – 626. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-
ⴱ
Naukkarinen, S., Karkkola, P., Kuittinen, M., & Räty, H. (2016). Suomen- 1379(199611)17:6⬍609::AID-JOB1828⬎3.0.CO;2-K
ⴱ
kielisen MAAS-mittarin psykometrinen rakenne ja yhteydet persoonal- O’Rourke, N. (2004). Cognitive adaptation and women’s adjustment to
lisuuspiirteisiin sekä hyvinvointiin [The psychometric structure of the conjugal bereavement. Journal of Women & Aging, 16, 87–104. http://
Finnish MAAS measure and associations with personality and well- dx.doi.org/10.1300/J074v16n01_07
ⴱ
being]. Psykologia, 51, 343–359. O’Rourke, N. (2005). Personality, cognitive adaptation, and marital sat-
ⴱ
Navarro-Prados, A. B., Serrate-Gonzalez, S., Muñoz-Rodríguez, J. M., & isfaction as predictors of well-being among older married adults. Cana-
Díaz-Orueta, U. (2018). Relationship Between Personality Traits, Gen- dian Journal on Aging, 24, 211–224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/cja.2005
erativity, and Life Satisfaction in Individuals Attending University Pro- .0081
ⴱ
grams for Seniors. International Journal of Aging & Human Develop- Osma, J., Suso-Ribera, C., García-Palacios, A., Crespo-Delgado, E.,
ment, 87, 184 –200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0091415017740678 Robert-Flor, C., Sánchez-Guerrero, A., . . . Torres-Alfosea, M. Á.
ⴱ
Neff, K. D., Rude, S. S., & Kirkpatrick, K. L. (2007). An examination of (2018). Efficacy of the unified protocol for the treatment of emotional
self-compassion in relation to positive psychological functioning and disorders in the Spanish public mental health system using a group
personality traits. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 908 –916. format: Study protocol for a multicenter, randomized, non-inferiority
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.08.002 controlled trial. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 16, 46. http://dx
ⴱ
Ng, W. (2015). Processes underlying links to subjective well-being: .doi.org/10.1186/s12955-018-0866-2
ⴱ
Material concerns, autonomy, and personality. Journal of Happiness Panaccio, A., & Vandenberghe, C. (2012). Five-factor model of person-
Studies, 16, 1575–1591. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-014-9580-x ality and organizational commitment: The mediating role of positive and
ⴱ
Ng, W., Russell Kua, W. S., & Kang, S. H. (2019). The relative impor- negative affective states. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 80, 647– 658.
tance of personality, financial satisfaction, and autonomy for different http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2012.03.002
ⴱ
subjective well-being facets. The Journal of Psychology, 153, 680 –700. Parker, P. D., Martin, A. J., & Marsh, H. W. (2008). Factors Predicting
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2019.1598928 Life Satisfaction: A Process Model of Personality, Multidimensional
ⴱ
Nishimura, T., & Suzuki, T. (2016). Basic psychological need satisfaction Self-Concept, and Life Satisfaction. Australian Journal of Guidance and
and frustration in Japan: Controlling for the Big Five personality traits. Counselling, 18, 15–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/ajgc.18.1.15
Japanese Psychological Research, 58, 320 –331. http://dx.doi.org/10 Parks-Leduc, L., Feldman, G., & Bardi, A. (2015). Personality traits and
.1111/jpr.12131 personal values: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology
ⴱ
Novak, J. R., Anderson, J. R., Johnson, M. D., Hardy, N. R., Walker, A., Review, 19, 3–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868314538548
ⴱ
Wilcox, A., . . . Robbins, D. C. (2017). Does personality matter in Paulson, D., & Leuty, M. E. (2016). Dispositional coping, personality
diabetes adherence? Exploring the pathways between neuroticism and traits, and affective style relating to conflict between work and family
patient adherence in couples with type 2 diabetes. Applied Psychology: domains. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 37, 519 –539. http://
Health and Well-Being, 9, 207–227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/aphw dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10834-015-9470-3
.12087 Paunonen, S. V., & Ashton, M. C. (2001). Big five factors and facets and
ⴱ
Novakov, I., & Popovic-Petrovic, S. (2017). Personality traits as predic- the prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
tors of the affective state in patients after breast cancer surgery. Archive ogy, 81, 524 –539. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.3.524
of Oncology, 23, 3– 8. http://dx.doi.org/10.2298/AOO1701003N Paunonen, S. V., & Jackson, D. N. (2000). What is beyond the big five?
ⴱ
Novoa, C., & Barra, E. (2015). Influencia del apoyo social percibido y los Plenty! Journal of Personality, 68, 821– 835. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
factores de personalidad en la satisfacción vital de estudiantes universi- 1467-6494.00117
ⴱ
tarios [Influence of perceived social support and personality factors in Pavani, J.-B., Le Vigouroux, S., Kop, J.-L., Congard, A., Dauvier, B., &
vital satisfaction of university students]. Terapia Psicológica, 33, 239 – Denissen, J. (2017). A network approach to affect regulation dynamics
245. http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0718-48082015000300007 and personality trait-induced variations: Extraversion and neuroticism
ⴱ
Odacı, H., & Çikrikçi, Ö. (2018). Cognitive flexibility mediates the moderate reciprocal influences between affect and affect regulation
relationship between Big Five personality traits and life satisfaction. strategies. European Journal of Personality, 31, 329 –346. http://dx.doi
Applied Research in Quality of Life. Advance online publication. http:// .org/10.1002/per.2109
ⴱ
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11482-018-9651-y Pavot, W., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1998). The Temporal Satisfaction With
ⴱ
Oishi, S., Kohlbacher, F., & Choi, H. (2018). Does a major earthquake Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 70, 340 –354. http://dx
change attitudes and well-being judgments? A natural experiment. So- .doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa7002_11
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 319
ⴱ
Pazda, A. D., & Thorstenson, C. A. (2018). Extraversion predicts a predictions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 629 – 640.
preference for high-chroma colors. Personality and Individual Differ- http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167205284283
ⴱ
ences, 127, 133–138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.01.028 Robinson, O. C., Demetre, J. D., & Corney, R. (2010). Personality and
ⴱ
Petrides, K. V., Pita, R., & Kokkinaki, F. (2007). The location of trait retirement: Exploring the links between the Big Five personality traits,
emotional intelligence in personality factor space. British Journal of Psy- reasons for retirement and the experience of being retired. Personality
chology, 98, 273–289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/000712606X120618 and Individual Differences, 48, 792–797. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
ⴱ
Plopa, M., Plopa, W., & Skuzińska, A. (2017). Bullying at work, person- .paid.2010.01.014
ⴱ
ality and subjective well-being. Journal of Occupational Health Psy- Rodgers, J. D., Lodi-Smith, J., Hill, P. L., Spain, S. M., Lopata, C., &
chology, 22, 19 –27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0040320 Thomeer, M. L. (2018). Brief report: Personality mediates the relation-
ⴱ
Pollock, N. C., Noser, A. E., Holden, C. J., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2016). Do ship between autism quotient and well-being: A conceptual replication
orientations to happiness mediate the associations between personality using self-report. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48,
traits and subjective well-being? Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 307–315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3290-2
ⴱ
713–729. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9617-9 Romero, E., Gómez-Fraguela, J. A., & Villar, P. (2012). Life aspirations,
ⴱ
Pratt, A. K. (2006). Role of cognitive ability and personality factors in personality traits and subjective well-being in a Spanish sample. Euro-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
coping with work-family conflict (Doctoral thesis). Wayne State Univer- pean Journal of Personality, 26, 45–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
ⴱ ⴱ
Saeki, M., Oishi, S., Maeno, T., & Gilbert, E. (2014). Self–informant Sheu, H. B., Mejia, A., Rigali-Oiler, M., Primé, D. R., & Chong, S. S.
agreement for subjective well-being among Japanese. Personality and (2016). Social cognitive predictors of academic and life satisfaction:
Individual Differences, 69, 124 –128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid Measurement and structural equivalence across three racial/ethnic
.2014.05.018 groups. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 63, 460 – 474. http://dx.doi
ⴱ
Saiz, J., Álvaro, J. L., & Martínez, I. (2011). Relación entre rasgos de .org/10.1037/cou0000158
ⴱ
personalidad y valores personales en pacientes dependientes de la co- Shi, Y., Luo, Y. L. L., Liu, Y., & Yang, Z. (2019). Affective experience
caína [Relation between personality traits and personal values in on social networking sites predicts psychological well-being off-line.
cocaine-dependent patients]. Adicciones, 23, 125–132. http://dx.doi.org/ Psychological Reports, 122, 1666 –1677. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
10.20882/adicciones.155 0033294118789039
ⴱ ⴱ
Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., Mastoras, S. M., Beaton, L., & Osborne, Shulman, T. E., & Hemenover, S. H. (2006). Is dispositional emotional
S. E. (2012). Relationships of personality, affect, emotional intelligence intelligence synonymous with personality? Self and Identity, 5, 147–171.
and coping with student stress and academic success: Different patterns http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298860600586206
ⴱ
of association for stress and success. Learning and Individual Differ- Shyh Shin, W., Boon Ooi, L., Ang, R. P., Oei, T. P. S., & Aik Kwang, N.
ences, 22, 251–257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.02.010 (2009). Personality, Health, and Coping. Cross-Cultural Research: The
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
affect in individuals with spinal cord injuries. Spinal Cord, 51, 252–256. 10.1177/1069397109335729
ⴱ
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sc.2012.105 Sibley, C. G. (2011). The BIAS-Treatment Scale (BIAS-TS): A measure
Saucier, G. (2009). Recurrent personality dimensions in inclusive lexical of the subjective experience of active and passive harm and facilitation.
studies: Indications for a big six structure. Journal of Personality, 77, Journal of Personality Assessment, 93, 300 –315. http://dx.doi.org/10
1577–1614. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00593.x .1080/00223891.2011.559389
ⴱ
Schimmack, U., & Oishi, S. (2005). The influence of chronically and Sibley, C. G., Luyten, N., Purnomo, M., Mobberley, A., Wootton, L. W.,
temporarily accessible information on life satisfaction judgments. Jour- Hammond, M. D., . . . West-Newman, T. (2011). The Mini-IPIP6:
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 395– 406. http://dx.doi Validation and extension of a short measure of the Big-Six factors of
.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.395 personality in New Zealand. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 40,
ⴱ 142–159.
Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., Furr, R. M., & Funder, D. C. (2004). Person- ⴱ
Şimşek, Ö. F. (2011). An intentional model of emotional well-being: The
ality and life satisfaction: A facet-level analysis. Personality and Social
development and initial validation of a measure of subjective well-being.
Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1062–1075. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Journal of Happiness Studies, 12, 421– 442. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
0146167204264292
ⴱ s10902-010-9203-0
Schimmack, U., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2008). The influence of ⴱ
Şimşek, Ö. F., & Kocayörük, E. (2013). Affective reactions to one’s
environment and personality on the affective and cognitive component
whole life: Preliminary development and validation of the ontological
of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 89, 41– 60. http://
well-being scale. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14, 309 –343. http://dx
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11205-007-9230-3
ⴱ .doi.org/10.1007/s10902-012-9333-7
Schmutte, P. S., & Ryff, C. D. (1997). Personality and well-being: ⴱ
Şimşek, Ö. F., & Koydemir, S. (2013). Linking metatraits of the Big Five
Reexamining methods and meanings. Journal of Personality and Social
to well-being and ill-being: Do basic psychological needs matter? Social
Psychology, 73, 549 –559. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.3
Indicators Research, 112, 221–238.
.549 ⴱ
ⴱ
Singh, B., & Shejwal, B. R. (2017). Role of mental image, and personality
Schneider, T. R., Rench, T. A., Lyons, J. B., & Riffle, R. R. (2012). The
in emotions and affect among NCC cadets. Journal of the Indian
influence of neuroticism, extraversion and openness on stress responses.
Academy of Applied Psychology, 43, 276 –285.
Stress and Health, 28, 102–110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/smi.1409 ⴱ
Sirianni Molnar, D. (2011). The light and dark sides of perfectionism
ⴱ
Schoeps, K., González, R., & Montoya-Castilla, I. (2016). Personalidad, implications for health and well-being (Doctoral thesis). Brock Univer-
relaciones familiares ysatisfacción con la vida en parejas casadas [Per- sity, Ontario, Canada.
sonality, family relations and satisfaction with life in married couples]. ⴱ
Skomorovsky, A., & Sudom, K. A. (2011). Psychological well-being of
Revista Mexicana de Psicología, 33, 89 –100. Canadian Forces officer candidates: The unique roles of hardiness and
ⴱ
Schwartz, C. E., Michael, W., Zhang, J., Rapkin, B. D., & Sprangers, personality. Military Medicine, 176, 389 –396. http://dx.doi.org/10.7205/
M. A. G. (2018). Assessing reserve-building pursuits and person char- MILMED-D-10-00359
acteristics: Psychometric validation of the Reserve-Building Measure. ⴱ
Sliter, M., Withrow, S., & Jex, S. M. (2015). It happened, or you thought
Quality of Life Research: An International Journal of Quality of Life it happened? Examining the perception of workplace incivility based on
Aspects of Treatment, Care and Rehabilitation, 27, 423– 436. http://dx personality characteristics. International Journal of Stress Management,
.doi.org/10.1007/s11136-017-1694-2 22, 24 – 45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038329
ⴱ
Seder, J. P., & Oishi, S. (2012). Intensity of smiling in Facebook photos Smillie, L. D., Cooper, A. J., Wilt, J., & Revelle, W. (2012). Do extraverts
predicts future life satisfaction. Social Psychological and Personality get more bang for the buck? Refining the affective-reactivity hypothesis
Science, 3, 407– 413. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550611424968 of extraversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103,
ⴱ
Selnes, M., Marthinsen, K., & Vittersø, J. (2004). Hedonisme og eudai- 306 –326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028372
monia: To separate dimensjoner av livskvalitet? [Hedonism and eudai- Smillie, L. D., Kern, M. L., & Uljarevic, M. (2018). Description, devel-
monia: Two distinct quality-of-life dimensions?]. Tidsskrift for Norsk opment, and mechanisms. In D. P. MacAdams, R. L. Shiner, & J. L.
Psykologforening, 41, 179 –187. Tackett (Eds.), Handbook of personality development (pp. 118 –136).
ⴱ
Selvarajan, T. T., Singh, B., & Cloninger, P. A. (2016). Role of person- New York, NY: Guilford.
ality and affect on the social support and work family conflict relation- Smillie, L. D., Wilt, J., Kabbani, R., Garratt, C., & Revelle, W. (2015).
ship. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 94, 39 –56. http://dx.doi.org/10 Quality of social experience explains the relation between extraversion
.1016/j.jvb.2016.02.004 and positive affect. Emotion, 15, 339 –349. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
ⴱ
Sheu, H.-B., Liu, Y., & Li, Y. (2017). Well-being of college students in emo0000047
ⴱ
China. Journal of Career Assessment, 25, 144 –158. http://dx.doi.org/10 Sobocko, K., & Zelenski, J. M. (2015). Trait sensory-processing sensitiv-
.1177/1069072716658240 ity and subjective well-being: Distinctive associations for different as-
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 321
pects of sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 83, 44 – 49. life. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 530 –535. http://dx.doi
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.03.045 .org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.12.006
ⴱ ⴱ
Sodermans, A. K., & Matthijs, K. (2014). Joint physical custody and Stolarski, M. (2016). Not restricted by their personality: Balanced Time
adolescents’ subjective well-being: A personality ⫻ environment inter- Perspective moderates well-established relationships between personal-
action. Journal of Family Psychology, 28, 346 –356. http://dx.doi.org/ ity traits and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 100,
10.1037/a0036713 140 –144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.037
ⴱ ⴱ
Sorondo, B. M. (2017). Associations between affect, personality, and job Suh, E., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1996). Events and subjective well-being:
satisfaction among library employees: Efficient and ethical assessment Only recent events matter. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
of library staff. Advances in Library Administration and Organization, ogy, 70, 1091–1102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.1091
ⴱ
37, 35–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/S0732-067120170000037003 Sulaiman, W. S. W., Kadir, N. B. A., Halim, F. W., Omar, F., Latiff,
ⴱ R. A., & Sulaiman, W. S. W. (2013). Structural relations between
Soto, C. J., & John, O. P. (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2):
Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance personality traits, coping strategy, social support and well-being among
bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. Journal of Personality and adolescents. Pertanika Journal of Social Science & Humanities, 21,
Social Psychology, 113, 117–143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp 121–134.
ⴱ
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
0000096 Suldo, S. M. R., Minch, D., & Hearon, B. V. (2015). Adolescent life
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Soto, C. J., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2011). Age differences satisfaction and personality characteristics: Investigating relationships
in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains and facets in a large using a five factor model. Journal of Happiness Studies, 16, 965–983.
cross-sectional sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-014-9544-1
ⴱ
100, 330 –348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021717 Sun, J., Kaufman, S. B., & Smillie, L. D. (2018). Unique associations
ⴱ
Soto, C. J., & Luhmann, M. (2013). Who Can Buy Happiness? Social between big five personality aspects and multiple dimensions of well-
Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 46 –53. http://dx.doi.org/10 being. Journal of Personality, 86, 158 –172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
.1177/1948550612444139 jopy.12301
ⴱ
ⴱ
Soubelet, A., & Salthouse, T. A. (2011). Influence of social desirability on Sun, J., Stevenson, K., Kabbani, R., Richardson, B., & Smillie, L. D.
age differences in self-reports of mood and personality. Journal of (2017). The pleasure of making a difference: Perceived social contribu-
tion explains the relation between extraverted behavior and positive
Personality, 79, 741–762. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011
affect. Emotion, 17, 794 – 810. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000273
.00700.x ⴱ
ⴱ Szcześniak, M., Sopińska, B., & Kroplewski, Z. (2019). Big Five person-
Spörrle, M., Strobel, M., & Tumasjan, A. (2010). On the incremental
ality traits and life satisfaction: The mediating role of religiosity. Reli-
validity of irrational beliefs to predict subjective well-being while con-
gions, 10, 437. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/rel10070437
trolling for personality factors. Psicothema, 22, 543–548. ⴱ
ⴱ Tan, W. H., Sheffield, J., Khoo, S. K., Byrne, G., & Pachana, N. A.
Stamatopoulou, M., Galanis, P., & Prezerakos, P. (2016). Psychometric
(2018). Influences on psychological well-being and ill-being in older
properties of the Greek translation of the Trait Emotional Intelligence
women. Australian Psychologist, 53, 203–212. http://dx.doi.org/10
Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue-SF). Personality and Individual
.1111/ap.12297
Differences, 95, 80 – 84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.035 ⴱ
ⴱ Tanksale, D. (2015). Big Five personality traits: Are they really important
Stanton, K., Gruber, J., & Watson, D. (2017). Basic dimensions defining
for the subjective well-being of Indians? International Journal of Psy-
mania risk: A structural approach. Psychological Assessment, 29, 304 –
chology, 50, 64 – 69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12060
319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pas0000337 ⴱ
ⴱ
Tartaglia, S., Miglietta, A., & Gattino, S. (2017). Life satisfaction and
Stanton, K., Rozek, D. C., Stasik-O’Brien, S. M., Ellickson-Larew, S., &
cannabis use: A study on young adults. Journal of Happiness Studies,
Watson, D. (2016). A transdiagnostic approach to examining the incre-
18, 709 –718. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-016-9742-0
mental predictive power of emotion regulation and basic personality ⴱ
Teachman, B. A., Siedlecki, K. L., & Magee, J. C. (2007). Aging and
dimensions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 960 –975. http://dx symptoms of anxiety and depression: Structural invariance of the tripar-
.doi.org/10.1037/abn0000208 tite model. Psychology and Aging, 22, 160 –170. http://dx.doi.org/10
ⴱ
Steca, P., Capanna, C., Mecaroni, M., & Delle Fratte, A. (2005). La .1037/0882-7974.22.1.160
soddisfazione di vita e le sue relazioni con i cinque fattori di personalità ⴱ
Terracciano, A. (2003). The Italian version of the NEO PI-R: Conceptual
[Life satisfaction and its relation with the big five personality factors]. and empirical support for the use of targeted rotation. Personality and
Rassegna di Psicologia, 22, 21–33. Individual Differences, 35, 1859 –1872. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Steel, P., Schmidt, J., Bosco, F., & Uggerslev, K. (2019). The effects of S0191-8869(03)00035-7
personality on job satisfaction and life satisfaction: A meta-analytic ⴱ
Tett, R. P., Fox, K. E., & Wang, A. (2005). Development and validation
investigation accounting for bandwidth–fidelity and commensurability. of a self-report measure of emotional intelligence as a multidimensional
Human Relations, 72, 217–247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00187 trait domain. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 859 – 888.
26718771465 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167204272860
Steel, P., Schmidt, J., & Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the relationship ⴱ
Thingujam, N. S. (2011). Emotional intelligence and life satisfaction:
between personality and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, Re-examining the link and mediating role of affectivity and personality
134, 138 –161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138 in India. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology, 37,
Stephan, Y. (2009). Openness to experience and active older adults’ life 27–34.
satisfaction: A trait and facet-level analysis. Personality and Individual ⴱ
Thomas, E. A. (2011). Personality characteristics and behavioral out-
Differences, 47, 637– 641. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.025 comes associated with engagement in work-related roles (thesis). Alliant
ⴱ
Stimson, T. S. (2010). The precursors and outcomes of goal choice and International University, Los Angeles, CA.
attainment (Doctoral thesis). University of California Riverside, CA. ⴱ
Thoresen, C. J. (2000). Antecedents and consequences of coping with
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: setbacks at work: A theory-driven framework (PhD thesis). University of
Approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Psychology Iowa, Iowa City, IA.
ⴱ
Review, 10, 295–319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_2 Thorpe, K. (2015). The role of humor, mindfulness, coping, and person-
Stoeber, J., & Stoeber, F. S. (2009). Domains of perfectionism: Prevalence ality in psychological health (Doctoral thesis). St John’s University,
and relationships with perfectionism, gender, age, and satisfaction with New York, NY.
322 ANGLIM, HORWOOD, SMILLIE, MARRERO, AND WOOD
ⴱ
Tian, L.-L., & Zheng, X. (2007). Relationship between five personality of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 678 – 691. http://dx.doi.org/
factors and multi-dimensional life satisfaction in middle school students. 10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.678
ⴱ
Chinese Mental Health Journal, 21, 165–168. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and temperament: General
ⴱ
Tov, W. (2012). Daily experiences and well-being: Do memories of and specific factors of emotional experience and their relation to the
events matter? Cognition and Emotion, 26, 1371–1389. http://dx.doi five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 441– 476. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.660135 .org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00980.x
ⴱ Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali-
Trankle, S. A., & Haw, J. (2009). Predicting Australian health behaviour
from health beliefs. E-Journal of Applied Psychology, 5, 42979. dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS
ⴱ scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
Tuce, Ð., & Fako, I. (2014). Odrednice zadovoljstva životom kod ado-
lescenata [The determinants of satisfaction with life in adolescents]. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
ⴱ
Psihologijske Teme, 23, 407– 433. Watson, D., Hubbard, B., & Wiese, D. (2000). Self-other agreement in
ⴱ personality and affectivity: The role of acquaintanceship, trait visibility,
van Allen, Z. M., & Zelenski, J. M. (2018). Testing trait-state isomor-
phism in a new domain: An exploratory manipulation of openness to and assumed similarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1964. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/ 78, 546 –558. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.546
ⴱ
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
fpsyg.2018.01964 Watson, D., Klohnen, E. C., Casillas, A., Simms, E. N., Haig, J., & Berry,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Vázquez, C., Duque, A., & Hervás, G. (2013). Satisfaction with life scale D. S. (2004). Match makers and deal breakers: Analyses of assortative
in a representative sample of Spanish adults: Validation and normative mating in newlywed couples. Journal of Personality, 72, 1029 –1068.
data. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 16, E82. http://dx.doi.org/10 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00289.x
ⴱ
.1017/sjp.2013.82 Watson, D., O’Hara, M. W., Simms, L. J., Kotov, R., Chmielewski, M.,
Veselka, L., Schermer, J. A., Petrides, K. V., Cherkas, L. F., Spector, T. D., McDade-Montez, E. A., . . . Stuart, S. (2007). Development and vali-
& Vernon, P. A. (2009). A general factor of personality: Evidence from dation of the Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms (IDAS).
the HEXACO model and a measure of trait emotional intelligence. Twin Psychological Assessment, 19, 253–268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-
Research and Human Genetics, 12, 420 – 424. http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/ 3590.19.3.253
ⴱ
twin.12.5.420 Watson, D., Stanton, K., & Clark, L. A. (2017). Self-report indicators of
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor negative valence constructs within the research domain criteria (RDoC):
A critical review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 216, 58 – 69. http://dx
package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36, 1– 48. http://dx.doi.org/10
.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.09.065
.18637/jss.v036.i03 ⴱ
ⴱ Watson, D., Stasik, S. M., Chmielewski, M., & Naragon-Gainey, K.
Vilhena, E., Pais-Ribeiro, J., Silva, I., Pedro, L., Meneses, R. F., Cardoso,
(2015). Development and validation of the Temperament and Affectivity
H., . . . Mendonça, D. (2014). Optimism on quality of life in Portuguese
Inventory (TAI). Assessment, 22, 540 –560. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
chronic patients: Moderator/mediator? Revista da Associação Médica
1073191114557943
Brasileira, 60, 373–380. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1806-9282.60.04.017 ⴱ
ⴱ Watson, D., Suls, J., & Haig, J. (2002). Global self-esteem in relation to
Villieux, A., Sovet, L., Jung, S.-C., & Guilbert, L. (2016). Psychological
structural models of personality and affectivity. Journal of Personality
flourishing: Validation of the French version of the Flourishing Scale
and Social Psychology, 83, 185–197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
and exploration of its relationships with personality traits. Personality
3514.83.1.185
and Individual Differences, 88, 1–5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid ⴱ
Webb, C. A., Schwab, Z. J., Weber, M., DelDonno, S., Kipman, M.,
.2015.08.027
Weiner, M. R., & Killgore, W. D. S. (2013). Convergent and divergent
Visser, B. A., & Pozzebon, J. A. (2013). Who are you and what do you
validity of integrative versus mixed model measures of emotional intel-
want? Life aspirations, personality, and well-being. Personality and
ligence. Intelligence, 41, 149 –156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell
Individual Differences, 54, 266 –271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid .2013.01.004
.2012.09.010 ⴱ
Weber, M., & Huebner, E. S. (2015). Early adolescents’ personality and
ⴱ
Vittersø, J. (2001). Personality traits and subjective well-being. Person- life satisfaction: A closer look at global vs. domain-specific satisfaction.
ality and Individual Differences, 31, 903–914. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Personality and Individual Differences, 83, 31–36. http://dx.doi.org/10
S0191-8869(00)00192-6 .1016/j.paid.2015.03.042
ⴱ
Vollmann, M., Pukrop, J., & Salewski, C. (2016). Coping mediates the Weiss, A., Bates, T. C., & Luciano, M. (2008). Happiness is a personal(ity)
influence of personality on life satisfaction in patients with rheumatic thing: The genetics of personality and well-being in a representative
diseases. Clinical Rheumatology, 35, 1093–1097. http://dx.doi.org/10 sample. Psychological Science, 19, 205–210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
.1007/s10067-016-3215-z j.1467-9280.2008.02068.x
ⴱ
Vorkapić, S. T., & Lončarić, D. (2013). Posreduje li profesionalno sago- ⴱ
West, N. M. (2007). The relationship among personality traits, character
rijevanje učinke osobina ličnosti na zadovoljstvo životom odgojitelja strengths, and life satisfaction in college students (Doctoral thesis).
predškolske djece? [Does professional burnout mediate the effects of University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN.
personality traits on life satisfaction among preschool teachers?] Psi- ⴱ
Whisman, M. A., Uebelacker, L. A., Tolejko, N., Chatav, Y., & McKel-
hologijske Teme, 22, 431– 445. vie, M. (2006). Marital discord and well-being in older adults: Is the
ⴱ
Wahl, H. W., Heyl, V., & Schilling, O. (2012). Robustness of personality association confounded by personality? Psychology and Aging, 21,
and affect relations under chronic conditions: The case of age-related 626 – 631. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.626
vision and hearing impairment. The Journals of Gerontology Series B, ⴱ
White, G. A. (2011). Implications of relationship social comparison
Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67, 687– 696. http://dx.doi tendencies among dating and married individuals (Doctoral thesis).
.org/10.1093/geronb/gbs002 University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA.
ⴱ ⴱ
Wang, D., Hu, M., Li, S., & Tao, S. (2019). Psychometric properties of the Wicker, A. D. (2016). The relationship between workplace sexual identity
temperament and character inventory-revised in Chinese young adults. management and workplace satisfaction among lesbian, gay, and bisex-
Current Psychology, 38, 1276 –1284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12144- ual professionals (Doctoral thesis). Massachusetts School of Profes-
017-9643-3 sional Psychology, Newton, MA.
ⴱ
Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of Wigert, L. R. (2002). An investigation of the relationships among per-
personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal sonality traits, locus of control, religious orientation, and life satisfac-
PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING 323
tion: A path analytical study (Doctoral thesis). University of Nebraska, and affect via the structural equation model in young adults. Applied
Lincoln NE. Neuropsychology: Adult. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/
Wiggins, J. S. (1968). Personality structure. Annual Review of Psychology, 10.1080/23279095.2019.1576690
ⴱ
19, 293–350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.19.020168.001453 Zeidner, M., & Olnick-Shemesh, D. (2010). Emotional intelligence and
ⴱ
Williams, P. G., & Wiebe, D. J. (2000). Individual differences in self- subjective well-being revisited. Personality and Individual Differences,
assessed health: Gender, neuroticism and physical symptom reports. 48, 431– 435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.011
ⴱ
Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 823– 835. http://dx.doi.org/ Zellars, K. L., Perrewé, P. L., Hochwarter, W. A., & Anderson, K. S.
10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00140-3 (2006). The interactive effects of positive affect and conscientiousness
ⴱ
Williams, T. F., & Simms, L. J. (2018). Personality traits and maladap- on strain. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 11, 281–289.
tivity: Unipolarity versus bipolarity. Journal of Personality, 86, 888 – http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.11.3.281
ⴱ
901. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12363 Zhai, Q.-G., O’Shea, B., Mike, W., & Yang, Y.-W. (2010). Subjective
ⴱ
Wilt, J. A., Grubbs, J. B., Exline, J. J., & Pargament, K. I. (2016). well-being, personality and environmental satisfaction in Urban China.
Personality, religious and spiritual struggles, and well-being. Psychology 2010 International Conference on Management Science and Engineer-
of Religion and Spirituality, 8, 341–351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ ing, ICMSE, 2010, 879 – 885.
ⴱ
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
rel0000054 Zhai, Q., Willis, M., O’Shea, B., Zhai, Y., & Yang, Y. (2013). Big Five
ⴱ
Wong, W. S., Lam, H. M., Chen, P. P., Chow, Y. F., Wong, S., Lim, H. S., personality traits, job satisfaction and subjective wellbeing in China.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
. . . Fielding, R. (2015). The fear-avoidance model of chronic pain: International Journal of Psychology, 48, 1099 –1108. http://dx.doi.org/
Assessing the role of neuroticism and negative affect in pain catastro- 10.1080/00207594.2012.732700
ⴱ
phizing using structural equation modeling. International Journal of Zhang, J. W., & Howell, R. T. (2011). Do time perspectives predict
Behavioral Medicine, 22, 118 –131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12529- unique variance in life satisfaction beyond personality traits? Personality
014-9413-7 and Individual Differences, 50, 1261–1266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
ⴱ
Wood, D., Nye, C. D., & Saucier, G. (2010). Identification and measure- .paid.2011.02.021
ⴱ
ment of a more comprehensive set of person-descriptive trait markers Zhang, J., Mandl, H., & Wang, E. (2010). Personality, acculturation, and
from the English lexicon. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, psychosocial adjustment of Chinese international students in Germany.
258 –272. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.02.003 Psychological Reports, 107, 511–525. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/07.09
ⴱ
Woyciekoski, C., Natividade, J. C., & Hutz, C. S. (2014). Contributions of .11.17.PR0.107.5.511-525
ⴱ
personality and life events to the subjective well-being [As contribuições Zhang, R.-P., & Tsingan, L. (2014). Extraversion and neuroticism mediate
da personalidade e dos eventos de vida para o bem-estar subjetivo]. associations between openness, conscientiousness, and agreeableness
Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 30, 401– 409. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/ and affective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15, 1377–1388.
S0102-37722014000400005 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-013-9482-3
ⴱ ⴱ
Wu, R., Liu, Z., Guo, Q., Cai, M., & Zhou, J. (2019). Couple Similarity Zhu, X., Woo, S. E., Porter, C., & Brzezinski, M. (2013). Pathways to
on personality, moral identity and spirituality predict life satisfaction of happiness: From personality to social networks and perceived support.
spouses and their offspring. Journal of Happiness Studies. Advance Social Networks, 35, 382–393. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2013
online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-019-00108-8 .04.005
ⴱ
Xu, L., Liu, R. D., Ding, Y., Mou, X., Wang, J., & Liu, Y. (2017). The Zimmermann, J., & Neyer, F. J. (2013). Do we become a different person
mediation effect of coping style on the relations between personality and when hitting the road? Personality development of sojourners. Journal of
life satisfaction in Chinese adolescents. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 515–530. http://dx.doi.org/10
1076. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01076 .1037/a0033019
ⴱ
Yeo, P. L. (2015). Religiosity, personality, and subjective wellbeing
among Muslim adults in Indonesia (Doctoral thesis). Regent University,
Virginia Beach, VA. Received May 17, 2019
ⴱ Revision received December 4, 2019
Yilmaz, S., & Kafadar, H. (2019). Investigating the relationship between
decision-making processes and cognitive processes, personality traits, Accepted December 7, 2019 䡲