Richard Ryan - The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation-Oxford University Press (2019)
Richard Ryan - The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation-Oxford University Press (2019)
Second Edition
Oxford Library of Psychology
Area Editors:
Clinical Psychology
David H. Barlow
Cognitive Neuroscience
Kevin N. Ochsner and Stephen M. Kosslyn
Cognitive Psychology
Daniel Reisberg
Counseling Psychology
Elizabeth M. Altmaier and Jo-Ida C. Hansen
Developmental Psychology
Philip David Zelazo
Health Psychology
Howard S. Friedman
History of Psychology
David B. Baker
Neuropsychology
Kenneth M. Adams
Organizational Psychology
Steve W. J. Kozlowski
1
2019
1
Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford.
It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship,
and education by publishing worldwide. Oxford is a registered trade mark of
Oxford University Press in the UK and certain other countries.
Published in the United States of America by Oxford University Press
198 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10016, United States of America.
© Oxford University Press 2019
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced,
stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means,
without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press,
or as expressly permitted by law, by license, or under terms agreed with
the appropriate reproduction rights organization. Inquiries concerning
reproduction outside the scope of the above should be sent to the
Rights Department, Oxford University Press, at the address above.
You must not circulate this work in any other form
and you must impose this same condition on any acquirer.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Names: Ryan, Richard M., editor.
Title: The Oxford handbook of human motivation / edited by Richard M. Ryan.
Description: Second Edition. | New York : Oxford University Press, [2019] |
Revised edition of The Oxford handbook of human motivation, c2012.
Identifiers: LCCN 2018053782 | ISBN 9780190666453 (hardcover : alk. paper)
Subjects: LCSH: Motivation (Psychology)
Classification: LCC BF503 .O94 2019 | DDC 153.8—dc23
LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2018053782
9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Printed by Sheridan Books, Inc., United States of America
S H O RT C O N T E N T S
Contributors ix
Chapters 1–532
Index 533
v
A B O U T T H E E D I TO R
vii
C O N T R I B U TO R S
ix
E. Tory Higgins Erika A. Patall
Department of Psychology University of Southern California
Columbia University Los Angeles, CA, USA
New York, NY, USA Eva M. Pomerantz
Martin grosse Holtforth University of Illinois, Urbana–Champaign
University of Bern Champaign, IL, USA
Bern, Switzerland Tom Pyszczynski
Pelin Kesebir University of Colorado, Colorado Springs
University of Wisconsin Colorado Springs, CO, USA
Madison, WI, USA Lili Qin
Lindsay E. Kipp National University of Singapore
Texas State University Singapore
San Marcos, TX, USA Markus Quirin
Julius Kuhl Stanford University
University of Osnabrück Stanford, CA, USA
Osnabrück, Germany Johnmarshall Reeve
Kyle F. Law Australian Catholic University
The State University of New York at Albany Sydney, Australia
Albany, NY, USA K. Ann Renninger
Woogul Lee Department of Educational Studies
Korea National University of Education Swarthmore College
Cheongju, South Korea Swarthmore, PA, USA
McKenzie Lockett Michael Richter
University of Colorado, Colorado Springs Liverpool John Moores University
Colorado Springs, CO, USA Liverpool, UK
Johannes Michalak Tomi-Ann Roberts
Witten/Herdecke University The Colorado College
Witten, Germany Colorado Springs, CO, USA
Mark Muraven Richard M. Ryan
The State University of New York at Albany Australian Catholic University
Albany, NY, USA Sydney, Australia
Kou Murayama William S. Ryan
University of Reading Department of Psychology
Reading, UK University of Toronto, ON, CA
Maida Mustafić Michael F. Scheier
School of Applied Psychology Department of Psychology
University of Applied Sciences and Arts Carnegie Mellon University
Northwestern Switzerland Pittsburgh, PA, USA
Olten, Switzerland Abigail A. Scholer
Jeanne Nakamura University of Waterloo
Quality of Life Research Center Waterloo, ON, CA
Claremont Graduate University Dale H. Schunk
Claremont, CA, USA School of Education
Gabriele Oettingen University of North Carolina, Greensboro
Department of Psychology Greensboro, NC, USA
New York University Constantine Sedikides
New York, NY, USA Center for Research on Self and Identity
x Contributors
Psychology Department University of Kentucky
University of Southampton Lexington, KY, USA
Southampton, UK Anja van den Broeck
Shannon Shankland Department of Work and Organization
Quality of Life Research Center Studies
Claremont Graduate University KU Leuven
Claremont, CA, USA Leuven, Belgium;
Paul J. Silvia Opentia Research Program
Department of Psychology North West University
University of North Carolina at Greensboro Vanderbijlpark, South Africa
Greensboro, NC, USA Stefan Vermeent
Yoon Young Sim Utrecht University
Department of Psychology Utrecht, the Netherlands
College of William and Mary Lena M. Wadsworth
Williamsburg, VA, USA Department of Psychological Sciences
Cecilia Sin-Sze Cheung College of William and Mary
University of California, Riverside Williamsburg, VA, USA
Riverside, CA, USA Xiaoqing Wan
Stephanie Su Department of Psychological Sciences
Apollo English Vietnam College of William and Mary
Hai Phong, Vietnam Williamsburg, VA, USA
Todd M. Thrash Patricia L. Waters
Department of Psychology The Colorado College
College of William and Mary Colorado Springs, CO, USA
Williamsburg, VA, USA Maureen R. Weiss
Mattie Tops University of Minnesota
Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam Minneapolis, MN, USA
Amsterdam, the Netherlands Stefan Westermann
Dwight C. K. Tse University of Bern
Quality of Life Research Center Bern, Switzerland
Claremont Graduate University Allan Wigfield
Claremont, CA, USA University of Maryland
Lara Turci Faust College Park, MD, USA
University of Maryland Rex A. Wright
College Park, MD, USA University of North Texas
Ellen L. Usher Denton, TX, USA
College of Education
Contributors xi
TA B L E O F C O N T E N T S
xiii
Part Four • Goals and Motivation
13. Achievement Goals 229
Kou Murayama and Andrew J. Elliot
14. Goal Attainment 247
Peter M. Gollwitzer and Gabriele Oettingen
15. Does Goal Pursuit Require Conscious Awareness? 269
Ruud Custers, Stefan Vermeent, and Henk Aarts
16. On Gains and Losses, Means and Ends: Goal Orientation and
Goal Focus Across Adulthood 285
Alexandra M. Freund, Marie Hennecke, and Maida Mustafić
Index 533
Table of Contents xv
P A R
T
1
Introduction
CH A PT ER
Richard M. Ryan
Abstract
Historical trends in the study of human motivation reveal a movement away from an exclusive focus on
external reinforcements and punishments in shaping behavior and toward the understanding, first, of
internal mediating mechanisms and processes, and second, of the internal sources of motivation
reflecting our evolved propensities and natures. The contributions in this volume are especially strong
regarding these internal sources of motivation and mediational processes, with contributions focused
on biological underpinnings, the dynamics of intrinsic and extrinsic motivations, implicit and explicit
cognition and self-appraisals, the roles of expectancies and goals, and the environmental and social
factors that influence or moderate these motivation-relevant mechanisms and processes. Contributions
also highlight the applied significance of contemporary motivation research, which is influencing
practice and outcomes in domains such as parenting, education, physical activity, sport, work and
organizations, and psychotherapy.
Keywords: human motivation, behaviorism, cognitive mediation, intrinsic motivation, neuroscience,
applied psychology
Just over a century ago a revolution was happening b ehaviorism that would dominate, and indeed
in the science of motivation. It was clamorous and define, the nature of motivation science for most of
controversial, and it reshaped the entire field. This the remaining 20th century. Watson, and later other
Handbook of Human Motivation, Second Edition, behaviorists, famously specified that psychology
reflects a quieter turn of events, but one just as must become an objective science. In this view
revolutionary. It reflects a strong turn within our events inside the organism were not particularly rel-
21st-century motivational science toward a detailed evant to a science of behavior. Even more strongly,
understanding of the internal psychological processes some behaviorists denigrated the world of ideas and
and biological mechanisms underpinning behavior. experiences as determinative of action. As Skinner
It is a direction of research that would send some of (1953) put it, “No matter how defective a behavioral
our forerunners reeling, but one that is nonetheless account may be, we must remember that mentalis-
essential to our continuing progress as a science. tic explanations explain nothing” (p. 246). It was not
In what follows, the outlines of these historical trends only psychological constructs that were eschewed;
are highlighted as they relate to the ideas of the neurological and physiological explanations were
field-leading theorists and researchers who have also sidelined. Instead, the focus was on the func-
contributed to this volume. tional control of behavior by external events and
reinforcements.
The 20th Century’s Behaviorist Revolution Behaviorism was not only a concept of what not
Just over 100 years ago, John Watson (1917) pub- to study, but also a guide to where the important
lished a book introducing a new approach called action is—in the environment. Outside the organism
3
are the contingencies of reinforcement of which all between inputs and outputs (Skinner, 1953). There
complex behavior was understood to be a function. was simply little need to enter the black box, either
Because any behaviors that recurred were posited to through phenomenology or through mechanistic
be “under the control of” reinforcement processes, the research, to understand and control behavior.
focus of research was on the most effective schedules
of reinforcement for increasing and maintaining Behavioral Science Today: Motives,
response rates over time. In this behaviorist world- Mechanisms, and Mediation
view, the inner workings of self and personality One hundred years after Watson, motivational
represent “at best a repertoire of behavior” that is science no longer embraces that black box view.
“imparted by an organized set of contingencies” In fact, motivational science in the early 21st century
(Skinner, 1953, p. 164). Accordingly, the search for is very much occupied with what is going on inside
motivation was focused almost exclusively on the the box. The major frameworks for understanding
nature of the contingencies that affect the probabil- behavior and its motivations are all about under-
ity of behavior. standing people’s goals, purposes, and meanings
One explanation for the long-term dominance and pinpointing the mediators and mechanisms,
of behavioristic theories in the field of motivation cognitive, emotional, and biological, underpinning
lies in the relative ease in which that model can be complex behaviors. These trends are richly expressed
applied. Manipulating facets of the environment in the explanatory and empirical frameworks to be
and looking for changes in output are operations that found in the chapters that follow.
are readily doable in laboratory settings. Internal
variables and mediators are not so easily isolated and Cognitive Mediation
varied. Thus, even researchers who may not techni- The revolt against the black box model most certainly
cally be behaviorist in orientation find the presenta- began with, and continues to be undergirded by, a
tions of external rewards and punishments a practical concern with cognitive mediators between environ-
inroad into motivational processes. Unfortunately, mental inputs and behavioral outputs. The harbinger
too often theorists unwittingly have generalized of this approach was in fact a contemporary of
this capacity of strong contingencies to change be- Watson and Skinner, namely Edward Chase Tolman,
havior in controlled settings to the idea that this is a brilliant experimenter who was often ignored in
how organismic motivational processes normally his time. But his work came to be reflected in subse-
work (McCall, 1977; Ryan, Legate, Niemiec, & quent thinking by researchers like Rotter (1954),
Deci, 2012). who helped seed this revolution by asserting that it
is the expectancy of reinforcement that determines the
The Black Box View probability of responding. Rotter showed that changes
This behavioristic orientation that dominated most in expectancy can alter probabilities of response
of the 20th century of psychological science has independent of actual histories of reinforcement.
been characterized as black box thinking. In fields Further, Rotter emphasized that reinforcement value
such as computing and engineering, a black box also matters. Indeed, both expectancies and values
model is one in which a system is analyzed exclu- represented critical cognitive mediators between the
sively in terms of its inputs and outputs. In behav- environment and behavioral responses.
ioral science, this model was applied not because of Several frameworks in this volume are anchored
any lack of curiosity about how organisms work, by a focus on these mediators of expectance and
but rather because of a more principled idea. Watson value. Most notably, Schunk and Usher (2019,
(1917) for example, believed that the contents of Chapter 2) begin the major theories section by
mind were simply inaccessible to proper scientific reviewing the social cognitive approach to motivation
methods. Skinner (1953) not only echoed Watson’s pioneered by Albert Bandura. Although Bandura’s
dismissal of mentalisms, but also described such in- work itself dates well back into the 20th century,
terests as having the appeal of the “arcane, the occult, (e.g., Bandura & Walters, 1963), the importance of
the hermetic, the magical” (p. 178). Furthermore, expectancies of reinforcement and value for outcomes
beyond concern with the difficulties of observing continues to be apparent in basic and applied
psychological events, even the physical mechanisms motivational research. Especially emphasized is the
of the brain, which presumably mediate behavior, import of feelings of self-efficacy in sustained behavior.
were seen as largely irrelevant to a functional view of This emphasis on expectancies and value as media-
behavior, which could focus simply on the relations tors of behavior is also explicitly at the center of the
RYAN 5
Kuhl (2019, Chapter 22) apply personality systems It is my hope that the varied contributions within
interaction theory to focus on autonomy and volition this second edition of the Oxford Handbook of
as supported by both mechanisms in central reward Human Motivation further facilitate these advances,
systems and the executive functions of the prefrontal serving as guideposts for this incredible journey into
cortex. This new emphasis on the neuropsychology of the inner workings of people as they act within this
autonomous and intrinsic motives in humans clearly material world.
represents a new frontier in motivational studies
(Di Domenico & Ryan, 2017).
Although the brain is the most celebrated organ References
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1963). Social learning and
of human motivation, it cannot by itself support
personality development. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart &
behavior. It is thus significant that included herein Winston.
is a review by Gendolla, Wright, and Richter (2019, Carton, J. S. (1996). The differential effects of tangible rewards
Chapter 21) emphasizing the cardiovascular supports and praise on intrinsic motivation: A comparison of cognitive
for effortful action, seen as dynamically affected by evaluation theory and operant theory. Behavior Analyst, 19,
237–255.
cognitive expectancies and beliefs about goal attain-
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2019). A self-regulatory viewpoint
ment and task difficulty. on human behavior. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford
handbook of human motivation (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Motivation: The Most Important Oxford University Press.
Variable in Applied Studies Custers, R., Vermeent, S., & Aarts, H. (2019). Does goal pursuit
require conscious awareness? In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The
Translational science attempts to bring findings
Oxford handbook of human motivation (2nd ed.). New York,
from basic research into practical usage in societies. NY: Oxford University Press.
Within translational sciences, the role of motivation Di Domenico, S. I., & Ryan, R. M. (2017). The emerging
is especially critical. There is no persistent action neuroscience of intrinsic motivation: A new frontier in self-
without motivation, making the frameworks articu- determination research. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11,
145. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00145
lated in this handbook particularly important to any
Freund, A. M., Hennecke, M., & Mustafić, M. (2019). On gains
attempt to harness and apply new knowledge and and losses, means and ends: Goal orientation and goal focus
constructively intervene in human behavior. In major across adulthood. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook
life domains such as parenting (Pomerantz, Cheung, of human motivation (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford
& Qin, 2019, Chapter 19); education (Wigfield University Press.
Gendolla, G. H. E., Wright, R. A., & Richter, M. (2019).
et al., 2019, Chapter 24); sports, physical activity,
Advancing issues in motivation intensity research: Updated
and exercise (Hagger, 2019, Chapter 25; Weiss, insights from the cardiovascular system. In R. M. Ryan
Kipp, & Espinoza, 2019, Chapter 26); psychotherapy (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of human motivation (2nd ed.).
(Westermann, grosse Holtforth, & Michalak, 2019, New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Chapter 23); and work (van den Broeck, Carpini, & Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2019). Goal attainment. In
R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of human motivation
Diefendorff, 2019, Chapter 27), authors from varied
(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
perspectives present state-of-the-art reviews of how Hagger, M. S. (2019). Advances in motivation in exercise and
to motivate engagement and behavior change and physical activity. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook
advance the effectiveness of interventions and of human motivation (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford
institutional policies aimed at optimizing people’s University Press.
McCall, R. B. (1977). Challenges to a science of developmental
performance and wellness. These chapters show
psychology. Child Development, 48, 333–344.
why motivational science is not just an ivory tower Muraven, M., Buczny, J., & Law, K. F. (2019). Ego depletion:
affair—its validity and utility are continuously being Theory and evidence. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford
critically tested in real-world settings. handbook of human motivation (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Murayama, K., & Elliot, A. J. (2019). Achievement goals. In
We Have Come a Long Way . . .
R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of human motivation
As a field of scientific inquiry, motivation research has (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
been ever evolving in its explanations of behavior, Nakamura, J., Tse, D. C. K., & Shankland, S. (2019). Flow: The
becoming more complex, as well as moving more experience of intrinsic motivation. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The
and more from the outside in. In the early 21st cen- Oxford handbook of human motivation (2nd ed.). New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
tury, empirical research is, it appears, resoundingly
Patall, E. A. (2019). Flow: The complex role of choice in human
focused on what is inside the black box. We have motivation and functioning. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The
traveled beyond mere functionalism to a science Oxford handbook of human motivation (2nd ed.). New York,
that has explanatory power, depth, and consilience. NY: Oxford University Press.
RYAN 7
PA RT
2
General Theories of
Human Motivation
CH A PT ER
Abstract
Social cognitive theory is a theory of human behavior that emphasizes learning from the social
environment. This chapter focuses on Bandura’s social cognitive theory, which postulates reciprocal
interactions among personal, behavioral, and social/environmental factors. Persons use various
vicarious, symbolic, and self-regulatory processes as they strive to develop a sense of agency in their
lives. Key motivational processes are goals and self-evaluations of progress, outcome expectations,
values, social comparisons, and self-efficacy. People set goals and evaluate their goal progress. The
perception of progress sustains self-efficacy and motivation. Individuals act in accordance with their
values and strive for outcomes they desire. Social comparisons with others provide further information
on their learning and goal attainment. Self-efficacy is a critical influence on motivation and affects task
choices, effort, persistence, and achievement. Recommendations are made for future research.
Keywords: social cognitive theory, vicarious processes, symbolic processes, self-regulatory processes,
goals, self-evaluations of progress, outcome expectations, values, social comparisons, self-efficacy
11
behavior (Schunk, 2012). This social cognitive among three sets of influences: personal (e.g.,
framework often is employed by researchers to cognitions, beliefs, skills, affect), behavioral, and
explore the operation and outcomes of cognitive social/environmental factors (Figure 2.1). These
and affective motivational processes. reciprocally interacting influences are illustrated
The next section describes the conceptual frame- using self-efficacy—a personal factor. With respect
Behavioral
Factors
Abstract
This chapter describes a set of ideas bearing on the structure of action and emotion and how they are
regulated. Some of the ideas are associated with the terms feedback control and cybernetics. Those
ideas have roots in many sources, including the concept of homeostasis and the creation of mechanical
devices to serve as governors for engines. With respect to motivation, the ideas yield a viewpoint in
which action is goal directed and reflects a hierarchy of control processes. The creation and reduction
of affect are seen as reflecting another set of feedback processes. The portion of the model devoted to
affect is of particular interest because it generates two predictions that differ substantially from those
deriving from other theories. The first prediction is that both approach and avoidance can give rise to
both positive and negative feelings; the second is that positive affect leads to coasting, a reduction in
effort regarding the pursued goal. The latter suggests a way in which positive affect is involved in
priority management when many goals are in play concurrently. Recent interest in dual process models,
which distinguish between top-down guidance of goal pursuit and reflexive responses to cues of the
moment, has caused a reexamination of some previous assumptions and consideration of the possibility
that behavior emerges in two distinct ways. This line of thought has also recently been applied to
conceptualizing diverse types of psychopathology. The chapter closes with brief consideration of how
the ideas considered here might be compatible with other viewpoints on motivation.
Keywords: cybernetic, feedback loop, control theory, affect, dual process models
This chapter describes several aspects of a viewpoint electronic, or living systems) regulate some current
on the guidance of behavior that we have used condition so as to stay on course or keep the current
throughout our careers in psychology (for a partly condition at a level that is acceptable. The idea that
overlapping, but differently focused discussion, see such systems also underlie the behaviors we engage
Carver & Scheier, 2017). This viewpoint has roots in throughout our waking lives is one theme of this
in several places. One source is the broad concep- chapter. We amplify this idea shortly, but first pro-
tion of homeostatic mechanisms, mechanisms that vide a bit more background.
regulate diverse aspects of the body’s physiological Cybernetic ideas had a brief heyday in motiva-
functioning (Cannon, 1932). Another source is tional psychology in the 1950s through the 1970s.
ideas about mechanical governors and computing Probably the best known example of this viewpoint
machines (e.g., Ashby, 1940; Rosenblueth, Wiener, & was an engaging book by Miller, Galanter, and
Bigelow, 1943; Wiener, 1948). In the middle of the Pribram (1960). This book introduced the acronym
20th century, Wiener (1948) coined the term cyber- TOTE, which stands for test–operate–test–exit, a
netic (from the Greek word meaning “steersman”) to sequence of events that describe the behavior of a
characterize the overall functioning of this type of cybernetic control system. Miller et al. (1960) were
system. Cybernetic systems (whether mechanical, not the only people to use cybernetic concepts
27
during this period (ideas with a similar character Feedback Control
were proposed, for example, by MacKay, 1956, What is a feedback control system? The term feedback
1966), but Miller et al. (1960) received the most control is abstract and can seem forbidding. An easy
attention from psychologists. To some extent this point of entry into the logic behind it, however, is
may be attributable to the fact that the operation the goal concept. People have many goals, at varying
of a TOTE unit paralleled the operation of the levels of abstraction and importance. Goals energize
basic element of a computer. Computers (which were and guide activities. Most goals can be reached in
fairly new at the time) were starting to influence many ways, creating the potential for complexity in
people’s thinking about the nature of cognition. the organization of action. The idea that goals guide
Thus, Miller et al.’s book was very much in the spirit action is a view that is easy and familiar for most
of its time. people, and it is part of the conceptual landscape of
Today when people use the word cybernetic, they contemporary motivational psychology. From there,
generally are referring either to robotics or to the the transition to thinking about cybernetic control
World Wide Web. It is also common to associate the is relatively straightforward.
ideas we describe here with the discipline of engi-
neering, partly because of its heritage in devices that Feedback Processes
govern engines and partly because of the usefulness The basic unit of cybernetic control is the feedback
of control theory in engineering applications. It loop. A feedback loop can be broken into four
is important, though, to keep in mind that the ideas pieces (Miller et al., 1960; MacKay, 1966; Powers,
have ties that extend well beyond engineering. As 1973; Wiener, 1948): an input function, a reference
noted, they pertain to homeostatic controllers of the value, a comparison process, and an output. Think of
body, which accounts for their prominent position the input function as perception. The input brings
in medical textbooks. They also pertain to complex in information of some sort about present circum-
systems in nature. stances. Think of the reference value as a goal. The
Thirteen years after Miller et al.’s (1960) intriguing perceived input is compared to this value to deter-
volume came another book that had a particularly mine whether a difference exists. If this comparison
strong impact on our thinking. This book, written detects a discrepancy, what is called an error signal
by William Powers (1973), was an extremely ambi- arises. The output function is a response to any
tious undertaking. Powers set out to portray how detected error (we treat the output as behavior, but
human behavior might reflect a hierarchy of cyber- sometimes the behavior is an internal signal rather
netic control processes. That is, he tried to account than a physical movement).
for how the nervous system creates the physical If the comparison detects no discrepancy, the
movements by which intentions and even abstract output remains as it was. If the comparison detects
human values are expressed in action. At center a discrepancy, the effect on output depends on
stage in his account was the feedback loop, the basic what kind of loop it is. There are two kinds of loops.
unit of cybernetic control. Powers set out to map In a discrepancy-reducing loop (also called negative,
several layers of feedback processes to aspects of for negating), the output acts to reduce (or eliminate)
the nervous system. Perhaps even more than Miller the discrepancy. Homeostatic systems are examples
et al. (1960), Powers made a compelling case for the of discrepancy-reducing loops. For example, if your
idea that the feedback construct was up to the chal- internal temperature sensors detect that your body
lenge of accounting for the complexity of behavior, temperature is elevated above normal, processes are
not as one loop, but as an interwoven network of engaged that reduce body temperature so that it
loops, dealing with regulation of diverse properties returns to that reference value. For example, sweat
simultaneously. might be released, which cools the body as it evapo-
Years ago we took up the Powers (1973) model rates. If the sensors detect a deviation below normal,
as a conceptual heuristic (Carver & Scheier, 1981). rather than above, you shiver, which generates heat
It helped us interpret a literature in personality and via muscle contractions.
social psychology in which we were immersed at Discrepancy-enlarging feedback loops also
the time (Carver & Scheier, 2012). And it provided exist, in which the output serves not to counter a
a reference point for us for the next nearly 40 years. discrepancy but to enlarge it. One might think of
In some ways it is the conceptual backbone of the reference value in this kind of loop as an anti-
this chapter. goal. Discrepancy-enlarging loops of some kinds
Extent of
engagement
or effort
Figure 3.4 Hypothesized approach-related affects as a function of doing well versus doing poorly compared to a criterion velocity.
A second (vertical) dimension indicates the degree of behavioral engagement posited to be associated with affects at different degrees
of departure from neutral.
Adapted from C. S. Carver, 2004, “Negative Affects Deriving From the Behavioral Approach System,” Emotion, 4, 3–22.
downgrade in priority. As described earlier, both the speed is needed (as when the situation is emotionally
upgrade and the downgrade have adaptive functions charged). The rational system evolved later, providing
in the appropriate situations. a more cautious, analytic, planful way of proceeding.
Operating planfully has important advantages, pro-
Shifts in the Theoretical Landscape: Two vided there is sufficient time and freedom from
Modes of Functioning pressure to think things through. Both systems are
We now turn to an entirely different issue. During presumed to be always at work, jointly determining
the past 2 decades or more, changes have occurred in behavior, though the extent of each one’s influence
how people view cognition and action. The implicit can vary by situation and disposition.
assumption that behavior is generally managed in Dual process thinking has also been influential
a top-down, directive way has been challenged. in other aspects of psychology. One theory that has
Questions have been raised about what role is influenced our thinking comes from developmental
played by consciousness in many kinds of action. psychology. Rothbart and her colleagues argue for
Interest has arisen in the idea that the mind has the existence of three temperament systems: two for
both explicit and implicit representations. These reactive approach and reactive avoidance and a third
various issues have also influenced how we think termed effortful control (e.g., Derryberry & Rothbart,
about ideas we have been using. 1997; Rothbart, Ahadi, & Evans, 2000; Rothbart &
Posner, 1985; see also Nigg, 2000). Effortful control,
Dual Process Models which is superordinate to approach and avoidance
Several literatures have developed around the idea temperaments, concerns (in part) the ability to sup-
that there exist two distinguishable modes of proc- press approach when it is situationally inappropriate.
essing (Carver, Johnson, & Joormann, 2008). In This view of effortful control greatly resembles
personality psychology Epstein (e.g., 1973, 1994) depictions of the deliberative or reflective mode of
long advocated such a view. He said that what he other dual process models.
called a rational system operates mostly consciously,
uses logical rules, is verbal and deliberative, and thus Hierarchicality Reexamined
is fairly slow. In contrast, an experiential system is Several sources of theory, then, suggest that the
intuitive and associative in nature. It provides a quick mind functions in two ways (for broader discussion
and dirty way of assessing and reacting to reality. see Carver & Scheier, 2017). They all promote the
It relies on salient information and uses shortcuts view that a deliberative mode of functioning uses
and heuristics. It functions automatically and quickly. symbolic and sequential processing and thus is
It is considered nonverbal and emotional (or at least relatively slow; all suggest that a more impulsive or
very responsive to emotions; see Metcalfe & Mischel, reactive mode of functioning uses associationistic
1999, for a similar view). processing and is relatively fast. Many of the theories
The experiential system is presumably older and suggest that the two modes are semiautonomous in
more primitive neurobiologically. It dominates when their functioning, competing with each other to
(B)
(A)
Ideal self
physical connection
Principles
health to others
Programs go to gym
Sequences
Figure 3.6 Attainment of a goal at a relatively lower level of abstraction often can contribute to more than one goal at the next
higher level.
An example, discussed in text, is that going to the gym can contribute to the maintenance of physical health, and it can also be a way
of making friends, thus enhancing connection to others.
Abstract
This chapter explores the motivational dynamics of the promotion and prevention systems outlined
in regulatory focus theory (Higgins, 1997). It includes a review of the core tenets of the theory—
identifying and responding to important and frequently asked questions—in discussing significant
research of the past two decades since the theory made its debut. In particular, the chapter includes a
discussion of what defines each system, how regulatory focus orientations are commonly measured
and manipulated, what differentiates promotion and prevention motivation from approach and avoidance
motivation, what characterizes the trade-offs of each system, and newer developments in research on
regulatory fit, group dynamics, and motivational flexibility. Throughout, avenues for future research
are suggested.
Keywords: regulatory focus, promotion, prevention, motivation, self-regulation, regulatory fit
Any stroll across the Internet or down the self-help 1997; Ryan & Deci, 2000). In this chapter, we
aisle in a bookstore makes it obvious that people explore how regulatory focus theory (Higgins, 1997)
are hungry to understand motivation. People want addresses these issues and discuss how the motiva-
to know how to motivate themselves, their part- tional distinctions it introduces contribute to a richer
ners, their children, their employees. People want understanding of the dynamics of goal pursuit.
answers to big questions: Why do I engage in In the 20 years since regulatory focus theory
behaviors that go against long-term interests? Why was introduced (Higgins, 1997, 1998), it has gen-
is change so hard? Why does she see the glass as half erated many new studies on motivation science.
full and I see it as half empty? Motivation science The discoveries from these studies have led to de-
researchers have grappled with these same ques- velopments in the theory itself, provided evidence
tions and addressed these issues from many valua- to support novel implications of the theory, and
ble perspectives: investigating how people navigate suggested new questions to explore. On this 20th
conflicts between fleeting desires and long-term anniversary of the theory, this chapter provides
goals (e.g., Fujita, 2011; Mischel, Cantor, & Feldman, an opportunity to take stock, look back at what
1996), studying factors that increase goal commit- research has revealed over this period, and probe
ment and performance (e.g., Locke & Latham, new developments and future horizons in testing
1990; Zhang & Huang, 2010), examining what the theory. We have organized our discussion by
makes an effective parent (e.g., Sears, Maccoby, & responding to the fundamental and frequently
Levin, 1957) or leader (e.g., Bass, 1985; Burns, 1978), asked questions that people often have when en-
and exploring the fundamental motivations that countering this theory, highlighting in each section
make us tick, effectively or otherwise, across the what we see as exciting opportunities for future
many challenges we face (Gray, 1970; Higgins, research.
47
What Is Regulatory Focus Theory? 1997; Higgins, Roney, Crowe, & Hymes, 1994;
Regulatory focus theory distinguishes between two Liberman, Molden, Idson, & Higgins, 2001; Molden
coexisting motivational systems (promotion, pre- & Higgins, 2005; Wang & Lee, 2006). Promotion-
vention) that serve essential but different survival related eagerness is reflected in many types of tactics
needs (Higgins, 1997). Although the systems differ and behaviors, such as considering multiple alterna-
in multiple ways, as we will discuss, the two core tives (Liberman et al., 2001), emphasizing positive
differences between the systems concern differences possibilities (Scholer, Ozaki, & Higgins, 2014; Zhang
in what fundamentally motivates goal pursuit (growth & Mittal, 2007), focusing on the forest over the trees
and advancement for promotion versus s ecurity and (Semin, Higgins, de Montes, Estourget, & Valencia,
safety for prevention) and which regulatory strategies 2005), prioritizing speed (Förster, Higgins, & Bianco,
are preferred in goal pursuit (eagerness for promotion 2003), and general openness to change (Cornwell &
versus vigilance for prevention). Psychological theo- Higgins, 2013; Higgins, 2008).
ries have long recognized growth (i.e., to develop
and be nurtured) and security as fundamental needs What Is Prevention Motivation?
(Bowlby, 1969, 1973; Higgins, 1987; Mowrer, 1960). Individuals with a prevention focus are sensitive to
Thus, people need both systems to be maximally security-related concerns such as maintaining safety
effective in the world. However, as we will discuss and upholding duties and responsibilities. This sen-
in more detail, in any given moment, the concerns sitivity to the absence and presence of negative out-
of one system may predominate over the other comes is reflected in greater assigned significance to
because of either chronic or situational differences the difference between 0 and −1 than to the difference
in accessibility. between 0 and +1 (Brendl & Higgins, 1996; Higgins,
1997; Higgins & Tykocinski, 1992). In other words,
What Is Promotion Motivation? prevention-focused individuals are maximally sensi-
Individuals with a promotion focus are sensitive to tive to nonlosses versus losses. Important nonlosses are
growth-related concerns such as advancement and those related to maintaining safety and doing what
progress. They dream big and broadly, aspiring for you ought to do (fulfilling duties and responsibilities).
the next big leap, pursuing ideals. Given these con- Therefore, success for a prevention-focused individ-
cerns, promotion-focused individuals are maximally ual is captured by maintaining a satisfactory state,
sensitive to two different outcomes: gains versus by holding onto 0 (a nonloss). Failure is reflected in
nongains. This difference between gains and non- the inability to maintain 0, being less than 0 at −1 (a
gains reflects what counts as success and failure loss). Importantly, as we discuss in more detail in
within the promotion system. Success is reflected in the section on system level differences, this means
gains, positive deviations from the status quo or neu- that a satisfactory status quo of 0 has a very different
tral state—the difference between 0 and +1. Thus, meaning within the prevention system than the
even when all is going well, promotion-focused promotion system; it is a positive nonloss in preven-
individuals are looking around to see how things tion but a negative nongain in promotion (Higgins
could go better. Promotion-focused individuals are & Cornwell, 2016).
less sensitive to negative deviations from the status These prevention-focused concerns with moving
quo or neutral state—the difference between 0 and −1 toward nonlosses and away from losses are best
(Brendl & Higgins, 1996; Higgins, 1997; Higgins served using vigilant avoidance strategies in goal
& Tykocinski, 1992). Instead, failure for a promotion- pursuit—avoiding mismatches to desired end
focused individual is captured simply by remain- states or nonlosses and avoiding matches to unde-
ing at 0 and failing to advance. Even if 0 is a satis- sired end states or losses (Crowe & Higgins, 1997;
factory state, it is not enough to simply hold onto Higgins et al., 1994; Liberman et al., 2001; Molden
it within the promotion system. It does not repre- & Higgins, 2005; Wang & Lee, 2006). Prevention-
sent the gains that define success (Higgins & related vigilance is generally reflected in many
Cornwell, 2016). types of tactics and behaviors, such as carefully vet-
These promotion-focused concerns with moving ting the alternatives one considers (Liberman et al.,
toward gains and away from nongains are best served 2001), emphasizing the possibility that things
using eager approach strategies in goal pursuit— might go wrong (Scholer et al., 2014), focusing on
enthusiastically approaching matches to desired the trees over the forest (Förster & Higgins, 2005),
end states or gains and approaching mismatches to prioritizing accuracy (Förster et al., 2003), and
undesired end states or nongains (Crowe & Higgins, generally embracing norms and the status quo
Abstract
The capacity for self-reflection, which plays an important role in human self-regulation, also leads
people to become aware of the limitations of their existence. Awareness of the conflict between one’s
desires (e.g., to live) and the limitations of existence (e.g., the inevitability of death) creates the potential
for existential anxiety. This chapter reviews how this anxiety affects human motivation and behavior in
a variety of life domains. Terror management theory and research suggest that transcending death and
protecting oneself against existential anxiety are potent needs. This protection is provided by an
anxiety-buffering system, which provides people a sense of meaning and value that function to shield
them against these concerns. The chapter reviews evidence regarding the role of death and other
existential concerns in four domains of existence: physical, personal, social, and spiritual. Because
self-awareness is a prerequisite for existential anxiety, escaping or changing the nature of self-awareness
can also be an effective way to manage the problems of life and death.
Keywords: terror management theory, experimental existential psychology, death anxiety, existential
anxiety, motivation
Unlike any other animal, we humans live our lives The motives for meaning and self-esteem through
starkly aware that, despite our fervent desires, death which people transcend death are uniquely human.
will sooner or later come to us. This knowledge, Although other animals react with fear to clear and
which is an inevitable consequence of our uniquely present dangers to their continued existence, only
human sophisticated cognitive capacities, pro- humans have the cognitive sophistication and self-
foundly influences the answers that people gener- awareness that make them aware of the inevitability of
ate for questions about the meaning, value, and death. This awareness of death, which resulted from
purpose of existence. Terror management theory the evolution of sophisticated intelligence in response
(TMT; Greenberg, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 1986; to other adaptive challenges, changed the way moti-
Solomon, Greenberg, & Pyszczynski, 2015) is an vational systems operate. Existential motives operate
attempt to explain the role that awareness of death on other more basic motive systems—co-opting
plays in diverse aspects of human life. The central them to meet new needs and changing the way other
tenet of TMT is that the desire to transcend the needs are pursued. We start by considering how the
fragility of human existence by construing oneself emergence of the capacity for self-awareness changed
as a valuable contributor to a meaningful universe the human condition.
plays an important role in most other human
motives. In this chapter, we discuss what TMT Self-Awareness: A Blessing and a Curse
and research have revealed about existential anxi- Awareness of self is a tremendously adaptive cognitive
ety and its effects on human motivation, behavior, capacity that exponentially increases the flexibility
and experience. of the system through which humans regulate their
67
behavior (Becker, 1971; Duval & Wicklund, 1972; conceptualization of a hierarchy of standards that
Leary, 2004; Pyszczynski, Greenberg, Solomon, & integrates concrete physical actions and the even
Hamilton, 1990). Self-awareness refers to the capac- more concrete biological, chemical, and electrical
ity of the human self to become an object of its own changes through which these actions are accom-
attention. Although some other species are capable plished, with the more abstract goals, identities, and
of a rudimentary form of self-recognition, they lack sense of self-worth that these actions (and their lower
the linguistic abilities to conceive of an abstract self level components) are oriented toward achieving.
and use it to structure their experiences and behavior From this perspective, all behavior functions to
(Mitchell, 2003). Self-awareness enables humans to simultaneously meet multiple hierarchically organ-
step back, reflect on their current state and circum- ized goals, and this organization gives coherence
stances, weigh multiple options for how to meet and flexibility to human action. The standard at any
their needs and the chances of each one succeeding, given level of abstraction is simultaneously a behavior
and then select the option they judge to be most through which the standard at the next higher level
likely to enable them to achieve their goals. It of abstraction is met. For example, writing a paper
greatly expands one’s options for how to behave and for a college class is a behavior through which the
gives one greater executive control over one’s actions. standard of getting a good grade in the class is met;
Along with other uniquely human capacities, such as getting a good grade in the class is the behavior
language, symbolic thought, causal thinking, and through which the more abstract standard of getting
imagination, self-awareness has been critical to the a college degree is met; getting a college degree is the
formation of complex human society and culture as behavior through which the more abstract standard
we know it today. As Leary put it, “science, philos- of getting a good job is met; getting a good job is the
ophy, government, education, and health care would behavior through which the more abstract standard
all be impossible if people could not consciously of having a successful career is met; and having a
self-reflect” (2004, p. 12). successful career is the behavior through which the
Contemporary thinking about the role of self- even more abstract goal of being a valuable person is
awareness in human behavior was stimulated by met. One could also move down the hierarchy to
Duval and Wicklund’s (1972) objective self-awareness consider the component behaviors through which
theory. They pointed out that conscious attention writing a paper, gathering information, reading arti-
can be directed either externally, toward the envi- cles to provide that information, and moving the
ronment, or internally, toward the self. Directing focus of one’s eyes across the words on the page are
attention toward the self instigates a self-evaluative accomplished, and so on, down to the biological
process, in which one’s current state on whatever and chemical changes underlying these actions.
dimension is currently salient is compared with sali- Flexibility in behavior is provided by the fact
ent standards for that dimension. The detection of that there are usually multiple behaviors through
discrepancies between current state and standards which any given standard can be met. For example,
produces the potential for affect, which motivates self-esteem can be achieved by means of success in
people to either reduce any discrepancies detected one’s career, relationships, community activities, or
or escape the self-focused state. Research has been family. And there are many ways to succeed in any
highly supportive of these propositions (for reviews, of these more specific endeavors, just as there are
see Carver & Scheier, 1981 Duval & Silvia, 2001). many routes through which any particular success
Carver and Scheier (1981) integrated these ideas with could be attained. Self-awareness sets in motion a
a very general cybernetic model of self-regulation in variety of executive processes through which choices
which this process of comparing the self ’s current among these multiple routes to goals at these various
state to standards, which increases efforts to reduce levels of abstraction are met. This is a very complex
any discrepancies that are detected, is viewed as the system and we can provide only a brief overview
central process through which the self regulates its here. For a more thorough presentation, see Carver
own actions. Self-awareness thus adds multiple layers and Scheier (1981). For the present purposes, our
of sophistication and flexibility to the simple system goal is to make clear the central role and adaptive
of comparing and matching to standards through utility that self-awareness and hierarchical organiza-
which all self-regulating systems operate. tion of standards and behavior play in human
One of the most important innovations that motivation and behavior. Put simply, self-awareness
Carver and Scheier (1981) brought to their synthe- increases the human capacity for freedom and will-
sis of self-awareness and self-regulation was their ful self-determined behavior.
Abstract
Human beings have fundamental psychological propensities toward growth, integrity, and wellness.
Yet, historically, many approaches to motivation have ignored these inner propensities, focusing instead
on how external contingencies shape expectancies and behaviors. This chapter reviews recent work
in self-determination theory, an organismic approach in which people’s intrinsic, growth-oriented
propensities are a central focus. Self-determination theory argues that people have basic psychological
needs to experience competence, autonomy, and relatedness to others. Satisfaction of these basic
needs facilitates autonomous motivation and wellness, whereas the frustration of these needs contributes
to ill-being and is associated with lower quality, and often highly controlled, forms of motivation.
Autonomous and controlled forms of motivation differ in their antecedents, neurological underpinnings,
and outcomes. Although most of the experimentation and evidence base of self-determination theory
has focused on proximal relationships (e.g., families, dyads, classrooms, teams, or workgroups), recent
research is extending self-determination theory to address pervasive contexts (e.g., cultural or economic
systems) and how they both directly and indirectly affect need satisfaction and motivation, thereby
impacting people’s development and wellness. Pervasive contexts also influence people’s aspirational
horizons and the life goals they pursue, further influencing both individual and community wellness.
More need-supportive contexts conduce to more authentic living and intrinsic aspirations, which in
turn promote more prosocial attitudes and actions and greater personal and societal wellness.
Keywords: self-determination theory, autonomy, motivation, control, intrinsic motivation, life goals,
aspirations, political contexts, economic contexts
As living entities, we are born to flourish. From the from outside the organism using potent contin-
standpoint of organismic theories within all indi- gencies of reinforcement (see R. M. Ryan, Bradshaw,
viduals there is a natural propensity to actively engage, & Deci, in press). Because this approach could
assimilate, and master one’s environments. There is produce powerful experimental results in controlled
a desire to learn, to grow, and, where possible, to laboratory conditions, it led to an emphasis on
develop and express capacities, talents, and interests. the malleability of behavior and contributed to a
Finally, as social creatures, humans have a need to “standard social science model” (Tooby & Cosmides,
connect with others in sharing, love, work, and 1992), in which behavior was seen as something
meaning. stamped in by social environments and cultures,
Many of the major theories and traditions in rather than being grounded in basic or inherent
behavioral science have grappled poorly with these needs or strivings. Often denigrating the significance
clearly built-in human propensities. In part, this is of inner experience (e.g., Skinner, 1953, 1971), the
the heritage of decades of behaviorism, in which theoretical focus of behaviorists thus enabled
the primary focus was on how to control behavior decades of scientific neglect of natural human
89
ropensities toward growth and research on what
p Ryan, 2018). But having proximal social supports,
supports and hinders their expression. especially in family, friendship, and community
Self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, settings, for being oneself is critical to wellness in
1985; R. M. Ryan & Deci, 2017) stands in contrast everyday life (e.g., Legate, Ryan, & Weinstein, 2012).
to behaviorist and, more generally, standard social Beyond these proximal interpersonal settings, how-
science models in its strong assumptions about the ever, autonomy can be supported or undermined by
nature of humans. As an organismic perspective, larger institutional, economic, and cultural forces.
SDT begins by assuming an active human nature For example, societal institutions and laws can
that entails a specific direction of movement toward support or hinder people’s rights (Rawls, 2001) and
growth, coherence, and wellness. People are under- capabilities (Sen, 1999) to pursue what matters most
stood as inherently prone to develop toward greater to them, impacting autonomy, competence and
capacities for self-regulation and integrity, and much relatedness, and ultimately psychological and physical
empirical evidence supports SDT’s claims concerning wellness (DeHaan, Hirai, & Ryan, 2016; R. M. Ryan
the spontaneity and pervasiveness of these hypoth- & Deci, 2017). Across history people have fought to
esized tendencies. Yet even more important within escape oppressive rule and to protect or gain rights
SDT is the idea that these propensities toward growth and resources and, thus, to better actualize valued
and integrity are not automatic. Instead, SDT argues aims and ideals (Welzel, 2013). Such struggles con-
that propensities toward flourishing are more or less tinue in the early 21st century, with respect to both
robustly expressed as a function of particular social totalitarian regimes and the controlling forces of
conditions. Social contexts that support basic psy- wealth, power, and ideology wherever they subju-
chological needs foster flourishing, whereas those in gate or disenfranchise individuals or groups.
which these needs are thwarted derail the integrative
trajectory of human development and move indi- Contexts and Basic Psychological Needs
viduals toward defense and self-protection. Thus, in Social contexts exert their impact on individual
SDT self-actualization is understood as dynamic, with motivation and wellness by facilitating versus
propensities to flourish catalyzed by some social impairing satisfaction of basic psychological needs.
environments and suppressed by others, a dynamic Within SDT, needs are defined as organismic neces-
that can be studied at levels of analyses from inter- sities for health. Basic psychological needs are a
personal settings to broader cultural, political, and subset of these necessities, those that are essential for
economic contexts. psychological growth, integrity, and wellness. Three
specific needs have been widely researched with the
Motivation and Wellness Within theory, namely, the need for competence, or to feel
Self-Determination Theory capable and effective in negotiating important life
Again, the organismic perspective is that in healthy tasks and experiences; the need for relatedness, or to
development, individuals move in the direction of feel connected to, significant, and cared for by
greater autonomy and self-regulation. This entails others; and the need for autonomy, or to experience
internalizing and integrating external regulations of one’s behavior as self-determined and volitional. To
behavior and learning to manage drives and emo- the extent that these needs are satisfied, people
tions, while staying connected within a social world. thrive, but to the extent that the need satisfactions
As well, it means maintaining intrinsic motivation are blocked or frustrated, negative experiential and
and interest, which are vital to assimilating new ideas functional outcomes accrue. Self-determination
and experiences. When people are more autono- theory focuses on how social contexts at each level
mously regulated, they exhibit greater engagement of proximity, from interpersonal interactions to per-
and vitality in activities, relationships, and life vasive cultural conditions, facilitate versus impede
projects. Thus, autonomy is an integral aspect of satisfaction of basic psychological needs.
full functioning and wellness at every age. It is worth noting that these three postulated
Yet autonomy is not just an individual affair. needs—competence, relatedness, and autonomy—
Across the life span, autonomy develops within were not simply assumed or formulated based on
the context of family and other social institutions, “armchair” theorizing, but were instead derived
which may or may not be supportive. Indeed, living both empirically and deductively. Though SDT has
autonomously is often a struggle, as individual always been open to considering additional basic
authenticity can require courage (W. S. Ryan & psychological needs (e.g., see Martela & Ryan, 2016),
EXTRINSIC INTRINSIC
Motivation AMOTIVATION
MOTIVATION MOTIVATION
Regulatory External
Style Regulation Introjection Identification Integration
Internalization
100 THE NATURE AND THE CONDITIONS OF HUMAN Autonomy and FLOURISHING
Ryan, 2007; Vansteenkiste, Ryan, & Deci, 2008). collectivistic value such as filial piety can be differently
Further, countries typically create laws that are con- defined and transmitted, with differential impact on
gruent with the economic and/or political systems need satisfaction.
and that either constrain or support the growth of
competencies, the abilities of citizens to affiliate and culture, aspirations, and well-being
connect, and the exercise of people’s autonomy. The issue of broader contextual effects and well-
These laws thus affect the wellness and effectiveness being can also be addressed with respect to aspira-
of the citizens. Because of these relations, SDT tions or life goals. The goals of wealth, fame, and
research has been increasingly examining how
image are very central to what we think of as the
cultural, economic, and political contexts and values American dream—that is, the set of values U.S. culture
affect and characterize individual motivation and and its economic system promotes (Kasser et al.,
behaviors. 2007). The advertising and marketing industry
within America has become enormous and its pri-
cultural values mary purpose is to get people to buy more goods
Chirkov et al. (2003) examined the relations of and services that represent external indicators of
cultural values to the well-being of individuals in worth—namely, appearing attractive, wealthy, and
four cultures, South Korea, Russia, Turkey, and the popular, values generally associated with dimin-
United States. In focus were differences in individ- ished basic psychological need satisfactions.
ualism or collectivism and whether the societies Twenge et al. (2010) examined cultural trends
were more horizontal or vertical authority struc- in the mental health of U.S. college students over
tures. The researchers found first that, even though 7 decades. They found a rather disconcerting pattern:
the cultures differed in terms of the values placed Over this period, U.S. students evidenced increases
on individualism versus collectivism, in each cul- in symptoms of depression, anxiety, and antisocial
ture the degree to which people had internalized tendencies. Examining factors that might account
the values predicted individual wellness. Chirkov for this negative trend, they ruled out numerous
et al. (2003) further found that vertical structures issues, from prosperity to family structure. These
were, on average, more difficult to internalize than authors concluded that the increasing shift toward
horizontal structures, suggesting that a vertical or poorer mental health and psychopathology may be
hierarchical system is likely (though not invari- “due to an increased focus on money, appearance,
antly) to be experienced as more controlling than a and status rather than on community and close
horizontal system. This is merely an on-average relationships” (p. 153). That is, the slide toward
finding, and it remains for the issue of hierarchy to more extrinsic life goals appears to have led, at least
be disentangled from the experience of autonomy in this U.S. cultural subgroup, to greater distress and
versus control at the cultural level. In principle, lower wellness.
according to SDT, one could congruently assent to
some hierarchical arrangements without losing a cultural values and need satisfaction
sense of autonomy (R. M. Ryan & Deci, 2017). Together, these findings about cultures and values
This kind of integrated identification with a hier- are important because they suggest that cultures play
archical structure would be facilitated by authori- a role in whether people can experience satisfaction
ties who behave in need-supporting ways. of their basic needs. This research further suggests
Such nuances were illustrated in research in by that having the need for autonomy satisfied in any
Pan, Gauvain, and Schwartz (2013), who investigated culture, regardless of the culture’s specific values,
the phenomenon of filial piety, or respect, care, and contributes to psychological wellness. Conversely,
honor for one’s parents, a common attribute in studies show that people feel more estranged from
many of the Chinese families they studied. The and less accepting of their cultures to the degree that
researchers found that when filial piety was empha- their basic needs are thwarted (e.g., Chirkov, Ryan, &
sized in terms of respecting and caring for parents, Willness, 2005). Thus, one basis for alienation and
it was associated with autonomous motivation, an instability within any culture may be the extent to
outcome mediated by parental autonomy support. which the culture’s ambient values and behavioral
In contrast, when the emphasis of filial piety was regulations fail to support or facilitate opportunities
on upholding parents’ honor and reputation, it was for basic need satisfaction among its constituents.
associated with less autonomy, a relation mediated by It can be treacherous for social scientists to
greater parental control. It seems that a hierarchical, evaluate others’ cultural practices and value systems.
102 THE NATURE AND THE CONDITIONS OF HUMAN Autonomy and FLOURISHING
and higher well-being. Our analysis also points violent content. Even engagement with television
to the embedded nature of contexts and suggests drama seems in part driven by need satisfactions
that the varied levels of analysis that support versus (Adachi, Ryan, Frye, McClurg, & Rigby, 2018).
thwart human needs must be considered. At the These examples show how SDT research is always in
broadest level, cultural, economic, and political sys- search of sources, moderators, and obstacles to basic
tems have substantial influences on individuals both need satisfactions across the varied activities of life.
directly and as mediated by basic need satisfactions. Although need satisfaction supplies the essential
nutriments for growth, integrity, and wellness, active
Basic Psychological Needs thwarting of needs produces a range of negative
In support of the assertion that there are three outcomes, from defensiveness and aggression to
universal psychological needs, we have reviewed psychopathology (Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, Ryan,
evidence drawn from diverse cultures and settings & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, 2011; Ryan et al., 2016).
indicating that when social contexts support peo- Indeed, from an SDT view, behaviors such as aggres-
ple’s basic psychological needs, people tend to thrive, sion and violence are not themselves inherently
whereas when needs are thwarted, distress and motivated (Przybylski et al., 2009; R. M. Ryan &
even psychopathology can result (Ryan, Deci, & Deci, 2017), but are instead typically consequences
Vansteenkiste, 2016; Vansteenkiste & Ryan, 2013). of need thwarting. That is, people are prone to aggres-
In line with this, various studies have examined sion whenever needs for autonomy, competence, or
need satisfaction as a mediator both between relatedness are frustrated or threatened, rather than
autonomy-supportive contexts and well-being and because it is an inherent drive or need. More gener-
between the pursuit and attainment of intrinsic ally, the dark sides of human behavior can typically
versus extrinsic aspirations and positive outcomes. be traced to persistent or severe need frustration
Using multilevel modeling, studies of within- and the substitute needs or compensatory activities
person need satisfaction over time (e.g., Reis, Sheldon, related to it (R. M. Ryan & Deci, 2000b, 2017).
Gable, Roscoe, & Ryan, 2000; Sheldon, Ryan, &
Reis, 1996) have further found that in addition to Awareness as an Important Part of
between-person relations of need satisfaction to Autonomy and Well-Being
well-being, daily fluctuations in satisfaction of each Reviewing the powerful effects of proximal and
of the needs predict unique variance in daily distal contexts on human motivation, performance,
well-being. In moments when people experience and wellness, as mediated by basic psychological need
satisfaction of their basic psychological needs, they satisfaction, might seem to suggest a very determin-
feel happier and even physically healthier. For istic or even passive view of human nature. But it
example, Ryan, Bernstein, and Brown (2010) demon- bears repeating that SDT assumes that people have
strated that the weekend effect—in which U.S. workers an active, growth-oriented, challenge-seeking nature.
experienced greater vitality and positive mood on It is only when they experience pervasive conditions
weekends—is primarily a function of the low auton- of threat and need thwarting that defensive behav-
omy and relatedness satisfactions most workers iors, need substitutes, and controlled orientations are
experience in their Monday-through-Friday jobs. catalyzed. When needs are satisfied, the inherent,
This brings us back to our earlier point that although active, and growth-oriented processes flourish.
capitalism offers its constituents choices and options, Part of the active nature involves the development
many of the vocations that are available and that of integrative awareness (Hodgins & Knee, 2002).
people “have to” adopt involve jobs that thwart According to SDT, autonomy is facilitated by
psychological needs. awareness, which entails the authentic attempt to
Need satisfaction also turns up in odd places. For experience what is occurring within and around
example, having identified that exposure to natural oneself. It is a relaxed and interested attention to
elements engenders greater vitality (Ryan, Weinstein, what is happening both within and without. One
et al., 2010), Weinstein, Przybylski, and Ryan concept closely aligned with such awareness is
(2009) showed that these positive effects were at mindfulness, which refers to an open, receptive stance
least partially a result of the increased autonomy regarding what is occurring in any given moment
and connectedness people feel when nature is salient (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Research has shown that
to them. Przybylski, Ryan, and Rigby (2009) found mindfulness is associated with enhanced autono-
that psychological need satisfaction could derive from mous functioning—that is, people are more likely
certain elements in video games, but not from their to act in accord with abiding values and interests
104 THE NATURE AND THE CONDITIONS OF HUMAN Autonomy and FLOURISHING
Assor, A., Cohen-Malayev, M., Kaplan, A., & Friedman, D. positive psychology of indigenous thriving and reciprocal
(2005). Choosing to stay religious in a modern world: research partnership model. Contemporary Educational
Socialization and exploration processes leading to an Psychology, 47, 32–43.
integrated internalization of religion among Israeli Jewish Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal
youth. In M. L. Maehr & S. Karabenick (Eds.), Advances in experience. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
motivation and achievement: Motivation and religion. (Vol. 14, de Charms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal affective
pp. 105–150). Oxford, UK: Elsevier. determinants of behavior. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Baard, P. P., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Intrinsic need Deci, E. L., Connell, J. P., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Self-
satisfaction: A motivational basis of performance and well- determination in a work organization. Journal of Applied
being in two work settings. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 74, 580–590.
Psychology, 34, 2045–2068. Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R. (1994).
Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., Ryan, R. M., & Thøgersen- Facilitating internalization: The self-determination theory
Ntoumani, C. (2011). Psychological need thwarting in the perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119–142.
sport context: Assessing the darker side of athletic experience. Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic
Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33, 75–102. review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards
Benware, C., & Deci, E. L. (1984). Quality of learning with an on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668.
active versus passive motivational set. American Educational Deci, E. L., La Guardia, J. G., Moller, A. C., Scheiner, M. J., &
Research Journal, 21, 755–765. Ryan, R. M. (2006). On the benefits of giving as well as
Black, A. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). The effects of student self- receiving autonomy support: Mutuality in close friendships.
regulation and instructor autonomy support on learning in a Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 313–327.
college-level natural science course: A self-determination Deci, E. L., Olafsen, A. H., & Ryan, R. M. (2017). Self-
theory perspective. Science Education, 84, 740–756. determination theory in work organizations: The state of a
Boiché, J. C., Sarrazin, P. G., Grouzet, F. M., & Pelletier, L. G. science. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and
(2008). Students’ motivational profiles in physical education Organizational Behavior, 4, 19–43.
and achievement outcomes: A self-determination theory Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The General Causality
perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 688–701. Orientations Scale: Self-determination in personality. Journal
Bromberg-Martin, E. S., Matsumoto, M., & Hikosaka, O. of Research in Personality, 19, 109–134.
(2010). Dopamine in motivational control: Rewarding, Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of
aversive, and alerting. Neuron, 68, 815–834. goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of
Brown, K. W., Kasser, T., Ryan, R. M., & Konow, J. (2016). behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.
Materialism, spending, and affect: An event-sampling study Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2016). Optimizing students’
of marketplace behavior and its affective costs. Journal of motivation in the era of testing and pressure: A self-
Happiness Studies, 17, 2277–2292. determination theory perspective. In W. C. Liu, J. C. K.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being Wang, & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Building autonomous learners:
present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Perspectives from research and practice using self-determination
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848. theory (pp. 9–29). New York, NY: Springer.
Burton, K. D., Lydon, J. E., D’Alessandro, D. U., & Koestner, R. Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagné, M., Leone, D. R., Usunov, J.,
(2006). The differential effects of intrinsic and identified & Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). Need satisfaction, motivation,
motivation on well-being and performance: Prospective, and well-being in the work organizations of a former Eastern
experimental and implicit approaches to self-determination Bloc country. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27,
theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 750–762. 930–942.
Chang, Y., Leach, N., & Anderman, E. M. (2015). The role of Deci, E. L., Schwartz, A. J., Sheinman, L., & Ryan, R. M. (1981).
perceived autonomy support in principals’ affective organi An instrument to assess adults’ orientations toward control
zational commitment and job satisfaction. Social Psychology versus autonomy with children: Reflections on intrinsic
of Education, 18, 315–336. motivation and perceived competence. Journal of Educational
Chirkov, V. I., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Parent and teacher Psychology, 73, 642–650.
autonomy-support in Russian and US adolescents: Common DeHaan, C. R., Hirai, T., & Ryan, R. M. (2016). Nussbaum’s
effects on well-being and academic motivation. Journal of capabilities and self-determination theory’s basic psychological
Cross-cultural Psychology, 32, 618–635. needs: Relating some fundamentals of human wellness.
Chirkov, V., Ryan, R. M., Kim, Y., & Kaplan, U. (2003). Differen Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 2037–2049.
tiating autonomy from individualism and independence: DeHaan, C. R., Ryan, R. M., & Curren, R. R. (2018). Conditions
A self-determination theory perspective on internalization of of living well: Three models. Manuscript in preparation.
cultural orientations and well-being. Journal of Personality and DeYoung, C. G. (2013). The neuromodulator of exploration:
Social Psychology, 84, 97–110. A unifying theory of the role of dopamine in personality.
Chirkov, V. I., Ryan, R. M., & Willness, C. (2005). Cultural Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 762.
context and psychological needs in Canada and Brazil: Di Domenico, S. I., & Fournier, M. A. (2014). Socioeconomic
Testing a self-determination approach to the internalization status, income inequality, and health complaints: A basic
of cultural practices, identity, and well-being. Journal of psychological needs perspective. Social Indicators Research,
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 423–443. 119, 1679–1697.
Choi, J., & Medalia, A. (2010). Intrinsic motivation and learning Di Domenico, S. I., & Fournier, M. A. (2015). Able, ready, and
in a schizophrenia spectrum sample. Schizophrenia Research, willing: Examining the additive and interactive effects of
118, 12–19. intelligence, conscientiousness, and autonomous motivation
Craven, R. G., Ryan, R. M., Mooney, J., Vallerand, R. J., on undergraduate academic performance. Learning and
Dillon, A., Blacklock, F., & Magson, N. (2016). Toward a Individual Differences, 40, 156–162.
106 THE NATURE AND THE CONDITIONS OF HUMAN Autonomy and FLOURISHING
La Guardia, J. G., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C. E., & Deci, E. L. Moran, C. M., Diefendorff, J. M., Kim, T. Y., & Liu, Z. Q.
(2000). Within-person variation in security of attachment: (2012). A profile approach to self-determination theory
A self-determination theory perspective on attachment, need motivations at work. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 81,
fulfillment, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social 354–363.
Psychology, 79, 367–384. Münster Halvari, A. E., Halvari, H., Bjørnebekk, G., & Deci,
Landry, R., Whipple, N., Mageau, G., Joussemet, M., Koestner, E. L. (2010). Motivation and anxiety for dental treatment:
R., DiDio, L., . . . Haga, S. M. (2008). Trust in organismic Testing a self-determination theory model of oral self-care
development, autonomy support, and adaptation among behavior and dental clinic attendance. Motivation and Emotion,
mothers and their children. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 34, 15–33.
173–188. Murayama, K., Matsumoto, M., Izuma, K., & Matsumoto, K.
Laurin, J. C., & Joussemet, M. (2017). Parental autonomy- (2010). Neural basis of the undermining effect of monetary
supportive practices and toddlers’ rule internalization: A reward on intrinsic motivation. Proceedings of the National
prospective observational study. Motivation and Emotion, 41, Academy of Sciences, 107(49), 20911–20916.
562–575. Ng, J. Y., Ntoumanis, N., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Deci, E. L.,
Lee, W., & Reeve, J. (2017). Identifying the neural substrates of Ryan, R. M., Duda, J. L., & Williams, G. C. (2012). Self-
intrinsic motivation during task performance. Cognitive, determination theory applied to health contexts: A meta-
Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 17, 939–953. analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 325–340.
Legate, N., Ryan, R. M., & Weinstein, N. (2012). Is coming out Niemiec, C. P., Brown, K. W., Kashdan, T. B., Cozzolino, P. J.,
always a “good thing”? Exploring the relations of autonomy Breen, W. E., Levesque-Bristol, C., & Ryan, R. M. (2010).
support, outness, and wellness for lesbian, gay, and bisexual Being present in the face of existential threat: The role of
individuals. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, trait mindfulness in reducing defensive responses to
145–152. mortality salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Levesque, C. S., Zuehlke, N., Stanek, L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). 99, 344–365.
Autonomy and competence in German and U.S. university Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The path
students: A comparative study based on self-determination taken: Consequences of attaining intrinsic and extrinsic
theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 68–84. aspirations in post-college life. Journal of Research in
Li, T., & Feng, F. (2018). Goal content, well-being, and Personality, 43, 291–306.
psychological needs satisfaction in Chinese adolescents. Social Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F.,
Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 46, 541–550. Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential
Litalien, D., Morin, A. J., Gagné, M., Vallerand, R. J., Losier, processing in our brain—a meta-analysis of imaging studies
G. F., & Ryan, R. M. (2017). Evidence of a continuum on the self. Neuroimage, 31, 440–457.
structure of academic self-determination: A two-study test Ntoumanis, N., Ng, J. Y., Barkoukis, V., & Backhouse, S.
using a bifactor–ESEM representation of academic motivation. (2014). Personal and psychosocial predictors of doping use
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51, 67–82. in physical activity settings: A meta-analysis. Sports Medicine,
Lonsdale, C., Hodge, K., & Rose, E. (2009). Athlete burnout in 44, 1603–1624.
elite sport: A self-determination perspective. Journal of Sports Ntoumanis, N., & Standage, M. (2009). Morality in sport:
Sciences, 27, 785–795. A self-determination theory perspective. Journal of Applied
Lorgelly, P. K., Lorimer, K., Fenwick, E. A., Briggs, A. H., & Sport Psychology, 21, 365–380.
Anand, P. (2015). Operationalising the capability approach Nussbaum, M. C. (2011). Creating capabilities: The human
as an outcome measure in public health: The development of development approach. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of
the OCAP-18. Social Science & Medicine, 142, 68–81. Harvard University Press.
Martela, F., & Ryan, R. M. (2016). The benefits of benevolence: Oliver, E. J., Markland, D., Hardy, J., & Petherick, C. M.
Basic psychological needs, beneficence, and the enhancement (2008). The effects of autonomy-supportive versus controlling
of well-being. Journal of Personality, 84, 750–764. environments on self-talk. Motivation and Emotion, 32,
Martela, F., Ryan, R. M., & Steger, M. F. J. (2017). Meaningfulness 200–212.
as satisfaction of autonomy, competence, relatedness, and Pan, Y., Gauvain, M., & Schwartz, S. J. (2013). Do parents’
beneficence: Comparing the four satisfactions and positive collectivistic tendency and attitudes toward filial piety
affect as predictors of meaning in life. Journal of Happiness facilitate autonomous motivation among young Chinese
Studies, 19(5), 1261–1282. adolescents? Motivation and Emotion, 37, 701–711.
McHoskey, J. W. (1999). Machiavellianism, intrinsic versus Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of
extrinsic goals, and social interest: A self-determination theory human and animal emotions. New York, NY: Oxford
analysis. Motivation and Emotion, 23, 267–283. University Press.
Menon, V. (2015). Salience network. In A. W. Toga (Ed.), Brain Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008). The effects
mapping: An encyclopedic reference (Vol. 2, pp. 597–711). of choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes:
Cambridge, MA: Academic Press. A meta-analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin,
Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention 134, 270–300.
and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Pelletier, L. G., Séguin-Lévesque, C., & Legault, L. (2002).
Structure and Function, 214, 655–667. Pressure from above and pressure from below as determinants
Moller, A. C., Buscemi, J., McFadden, H. G., Hedeker, D., & of teachers’ motivation and teaching behavior. Journal of
Spring, B. (2014). Financial motivation undermines potential Educational Psychology, 94, 186–196.
enjoyment in an intensive diet and activity intervention. Pelletier, L. G., Tuson, K. M., & Haddad, N. K. (1997). Client
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 819–827. motivation for therapy scale: A measure of intrinsic
108 THE NATURE AND THE CONDITIONS OF HUMAN Autonomy and FLOURISHING
on high-stakes testing. Theory and Research in Education, 7, Tricomi, E., & DePasque, S. (2016). The role of feedback in
224–233. learning and motivation. In S. Kim, J. Reeve, & M. Bong
Ryan, R. M., Weinstein, N., Bernstein, J., Brown, K. W., (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: Recent
Mistretta, L., & Gagne, M. (2010). Vitalizing effects of being developments in neuroscience research on human motivation
outdoors and in nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology, (pp. 175–202). Bingley, UK: Emerald.
30, 159–168. Twenge, J., Gentile, B., DeWall, C. N., Ma, D., Lacefield, K., &
Ryan, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (2018). Toward a social psychol Schurtz, D. R. (2010). Birth cohort increases in psycho
ogy of authenticity: Exploring within-person variation in pathology among young Americans, 1938–2007: A cross-
self-disclosure, congruence, and genuineness using self- temporal meta-analysis of the MMPI. Clinical Psychology
determination theory. Review of General Psychology. Advance Review, 30, 145–154.
online publication. doi:10.1037/gpr0000162 van den Broeck, A., Carpini, J., & Diefendorff, J. (2019). Work
Salamone, J. D., & Correa, M. (2016). Neurobiology of effort motivation: Where do the different perspectives lead us? In
and the role of mesolimbic dopamine. In S. Kim, J. Reeve, & R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of human motivation
M. Bong (Eds.) Advances in motivation and achievement: (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Recent developments in neuroscience research on human van Hiel, A., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2009). Ambitions fulfilled?
motivation (pp. 229–256). Bingley, UK: Emerald. The effects of intrinsic and extrinsic goal attainment
Schultz, P. P., Ryan, R. M., Niemiec, C. P., Legate, N., & on older adults’ ego-integrity and death attitudes. The
Williams, G. C. (2015). Mindfulness, work climate, and International Journal of Aging and Human Development,
psychological need satisfaction in employee well-being. 68, 27–51.
Mindfulness, 6, 971–985. Vansteenkiste, M., & Ryan, R. M. (2013). On psychological
Séguin, C., Pelletier, L. G., & Hunsley, J. (1999). Predicting growth and vulnerability: Basic psychological need satisfaction
environmental behaviors: The influence of self-determination and need frustration as a unifying principle. Journal of
and information about environmental health risks. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 23, 263–280.
Applied Social Psychology, 29, 1582–1604. Vansteenkiste, M., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). Self-
Sen, A. (1999). Development as freedom. New York, NY: Knopf. determination theory and the explanatory role of psychological
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (2008). Psychological threat and needs in human well-being. In L. Bruni, F. Comim, &
extrinsic goal striving. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 37–45. M. Pugno (Eds.), Capabilities and happiness (pp. 181–223).
Sheldon, K. M., & Krieger, L. S. (2014). Service job lawyers are Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
happier than money job lawyers, despite their lower income. Wang, F. (2008). Motivation and English achievement: An
The Journal of Positive Psychology, 9, 219–226. exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis of a new measure
Sheldon, K. M., Osin, E. N., Gordeeva, T. O., Suchkov, D. D., for Chinese students of English learning. North American
& Sychev, O. A. (2017). Evaluating the dimensionality of Journal of Psychology, 10, 633–646.
self-determination theory’s relative autonomy continuum. Wang, J. C., Morin, A. J., Ryan, R. M., & Liu, W. C.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43, 1215–1238. (2016). Students’ motivational profiles in the physical
Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Kasser, T. (2004). education context. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
The independent effects of goal contents and motives on 38, 612–630.
well-being: It’s both what you pursue and why you pursue it. Weinstein, N., Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2009). A multi-
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 475–486. method examination of the effects of mindfulness on stress
Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., & Reis, H. T. (1996). What makes attribution, coping, and emotional well-being. Journal of
for a good day? Competence and autonomy in the day and in Research in Personality, 43, 374–385.
the person. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, Weinstein, N., Przybylski, A. K., & Ryan, R. M. (2009). Can
1270–1279. nature make us more caring? Effects of immersion in nature
Soenens, B., Neyrinck, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Dezutter, J., on intrinsic aspirations and generosity. Personality and Social
Hutsebaut, D., & Duriez, B. (2012). How do perceptions of Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1315–1329.
God as autonomy supportive or controlling relate to Weinstein, N., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). When helping helps:
individuals’ social-cognitive processing of religious contents? Autonomous motivation for prosocial behavior and its
The role of motives for religious behavior. International influence on well-being for the helper and recipient. Journal
Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 22, 10–30. of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 222–244.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York, Welzel, C. (2013). Freedom rising: Human empowerment and
NY: Macmillan. the quest for emancipation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York, NY: University Press.
Knopf. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of
Standage, M., & Ryan, R. M. (2019). Self-determination theory competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.
in sport and exercise. In G. Tenenbaum & R. C. Eklund Williams, G. C., Cox, E. M., Hedberg, V., & Deci, E. L. (2000).
(Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (4th ed.). Hoboken, Extrinsic life goals and health risk behaviors in adolescents.
NJ: Wiley. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 1756–1771.
Tapscott, D., & Williams, A. D. (2006). Wikinomics: How mass Williams, G. C., Freedman, Z. R., & Deci, E. L. (1998).
collaboration changes everything. New York, NY: Penguin Books. Supporting autonomy to motivate glucose control in patients
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations with diabetes. Diabetes Care, 21, 1644–1651.
of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), Williams, G. C., Grow, V. M., Freedman, Z., Ryan, R. M., &
The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the Deci, E. L. (1996). Motivational predictors of weight loss
generation of culture (pp. 19–136). New York, NY: Oxford and weight-loss maintenance. Journal of Personality and
University Press. Social Psychology, 70, 115–126.
110 THE NATURE AND THE CONDITIONS OF HUMAN Autonomy and FLOURISHING
P A R T
3
Motivational Processes
CH A PT ER
7 Ego Depletion
Theory and Evidence
Abstract
Self-control all too often fails. Despite people’s best intentions and considerable negative outcomes,
people often find themselves at the losing end of resisting temptation, combating urges, and changing
their behavior. One reason for these failures may be that exerting self-control depletes a limited
resource (ego depletion) that is necessary for the success of self-control. Hence, after exerting
self-control, individuals are less able resist temptations, fight urges, or stop a behavior that results in
a loss of self-control. This chapter reviews the evidence for this theory in a wide variety of domains
and examines what behaviors appear to deplete ego strength and how depletion affects behavior.
A comprehensive theory that examines how depletion operates is put forth and used to examine
some factors that might moderate the depletion effect.
Keywords: self-control, ego depletion, willpower, motivation, conflicting goals
Introduction more desired state in the future. That is, the organism
As most people can attest, dieting, quitting smoking, is seeking to gain a larger but delayed reward over
controlling one’s temper, and working instead of a smaller but more immediate reward. To do so,
playing are not easy. In fact, it often feels quite diffi- the organism must resist the temptation to take the
cult to avoid immediate, pressing, or easy behaviors immediate reward. Self-control is the process that
to follow self-imposed or externally imposed rules, allows this to happen (Kanfer & Karoly, 1972;
get along with others, or reach long-range goals. Mischel, Shoda, & Rodriguez, 1989).
Moreover, such self-control efforts fail all too often. The ability to exert self-control is one of the
The point of the strength model of self-control critical features that differentiates humans from
(Muraven & Baumeister, 2000) is to describe how other organisms (Baumeister, 1998, 2005). Although
people resist such temptations and understand why other animals can exert self-control (for instance,
fails (and why it succeeds) and what can be done to squirrels burying nuts for the winter), the self-control
improve self-control. demands on humans are much greater than the
Self-control is the process that enables organisms self-control demands on these other animals. Indeed,
to override, inhibit, or stop urges, emotions and it has been argued (Sedikides & Skowronski, 1997)
moods, thoughts, or behaviors to reach a long-term that the growth in the ability to exert self-control
goal. These long-term goals can be personally set, drove the development of human cognition, society,
such as losing weight or succeeding in school, or can and the expansion of the self. Hence, understanding
be moral, interpersonal, or societal rules such as not how self-control operates can give us insight into
having premarital sex or not gossiping. Regardless many critical features of the human experience.
of the type of goal, it typically requires the individual Moreover, understanding self-control has im-
to forgo an immediate pleasure or desire to reach a mense practical benefits. Self-control is critical to
113
preventing the initiation and the cessation of addictive the noise that depleted the individual so that he or
behaviors (e.g., Brown, 1998; Wills, Sandy, & she was less able to deal with future demands.
Yaeger, 2002). Other research has illustrated the Such a depletion model can be contrasted with a
importance of self-control in dieting (Heatherton, constant resource or skill model. These models would
Striepe, & Wittenberg, 1998), overspending (Faber, predict that self-control should not be affected by
1992), relationship problems (Finkel & Campbell, previous demands or may even get better as the
2001), violence (Stucke & Baumeister, 2006), and individual warms up and gains knowledge of the
crime (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). Given that tasks. The depletion model specifically predicts an
many health problems can be linked to a lack of after-effect of exerting self-control. That is, even after
exercise, smoking, and poor eating habits, it is the initial self-control demand has been removed
apparent that a lack of self-control is a major
and a new situation introduced, there should be a
contributor to morbidity and mortality. Likewise, carry-over effect that leads to poorer self-control.
because many economic problems at both the Moreover, to be a unique prediction, this decline in
personal and the societal level follow from over- performance should not arise from changes in mood,
spending, lack of consideration of future demands, arousal, frustration, self-efficacy, or other well-
and educational underachievement, a better un- established psychological processes.
derstanding of how self-control operates is critical Extensive research has strongly suggested that
to our prosperity as well. the depletion model is the best fit for the observed
There have been many fruitful paths to under- data on self-control. In experimental studies, indi-
standing the nature of self-control. For example, viduals who exert self-control perform more poorly
Mischel and colleagues’ early research on the on subsequent tests of self-control compared to in-
marshmallow test (e.g., Mischel, Ebbesen, & Zeiss, dividuals who initially worked on a task that did not
1972) illustrated the developmental and cognitive require self-control. For example, Muraven, Collins,
underpinnings of self-control. Other researchers and Nienhaus (2002) had social drinkers either sup-
examined self-control from a decision-making press the thought of a white bear (a difficult thought
framework and tried to understand how people inhibition exercise; Wegner, Schneider, Carter, &
consider future actions and outcomes (e.g, Fujita, White, 1987) or solve addition problems. These tasks
Trope, Liberman, & Levin Sagi, 2006; Loewenstein did not differ in perceived unpleasantness, effort,
& Prelec, 1992). There also has been an extensive or difficulty; the only reported difference was the
literature on the relationships among executive amount of self-control required. Subsequently, par-
functioning and self-control (Barkley, 1997; Nigg, ticipants were given the chance to drink alcohol, with
2016) that is particularly useful for understanding the caveat that afterward they would take a driving
the neurological underpinnings of self-control. simulator test and those who did well would win
a prize. Compared to those who solved addition
Ego Strength problems, individuals who had to suppress their
Related to these areas of research, additional thoughts drank more and became more intoxicated.
theorizing on the motivational underpinnings of This suggests that the exercise of suppressing thoughts
self-control suggested that people’s ability to resist led to poorer control over alcohol intake subse-
temptations should worsen over time (Baumeister, quently. Indeed, participants’ reports of the amount
Heatherton, & Tice, 1994). An examination of prior of self-control they exerted on the first task were
research found considerable support for that idea related to the amount of alcohol they consumed.
(Muraven & Baumeister, 2000). That is, after exerting However, reports of mood, arousal, frustration, and
self-control, subsequent attempts at self-control suffer. displeasure were not related to the amount drank.
For instance, research on the effects of environmental This strongly suggests that the loss of control over
stress (Glass, Singer, & Friedman, 1969) found that alcohol intake is being driven by the amount of
individuals who were exposed to uncontrollable or self-control exerted in the first part of the experiment.
unpredictable noise subsequently performed more The initial exertion of self-control only affects tasks
poorly on a test of persistence and frustration toler- that require self-control, further giving evidence to
ance, after being moved to a quiet location, than did the specificity of the depletion model. For instance,
individuals who had been exposed to a controllable people who strongly desire to drink alcohol must
or predictable noise. These researchers argued that it exert greater self-control not to drink than people
was not the noise per se that affected performance, who desire alcohol less. Hence, in the alcohol study
but rather the process of adapting and coping with (Muraven et al., 2002), individuals who were not
Erika A. Patall
Abstract
Years of research have implicated a complex set of motivational causes and consequences of choice.
Psychological theory has often prescribed the benefits of choosing, though limitations to this view
of choice as being ubiquitously positive are apparent. In this chapter, the relation between choice,
motivation, and variety of psychological and behavioral outcomes is examined. The role of choice in
human functioning is examined from a variety of perspectives, including psychosocial, cognitive,
neurological, and sociocultural perspectives. Next, the complex and often contradictory findings
regarding the relation between choice and motivation are discussed in light of various critical
characteristics of choices, choosers, and environments likely to influence those effects. The directions
that future research might take are briefly discussed.
Keywords: choice, choosing, decision-making, autonomy, motivation
135
and sociocultural perspectives is highlighted to pro- relevant to the effects of choice from psychosocial
vide a broad understanding of the potential benefits perspectives, cognitive perspectives, neuroscience
and detriments of choice. Next, in an attempt to perspectives, and sociocultural perspectives.
understand the complex and often contradictory re-
lations that have been revealed between choice and Psychosocial Perspectives
various outcomes, factors that may influence choice Many theories focused on the environmental
effects are discussed. Finally, I discuss the implica- precursors of motivation have implicated choice,
tions of this theory and research and make recom- suggesting that choosing satisfies important psy-
mendations for future research. chological and social needs and leads to positive
affective experiences. Self-determination theory
The Motivation to Choose provides one of the more well-defined perspectives
Before we discuss how choice is itself often a regarding the role of choice in motivation. According
motivator (and sometimes a demotivator), it is im- to self-determination theory, people are naturally
portant to briefly note that in much psychological inclined to interact with the environment in ways
theory and research, choice is an outcome of some that promote learning and mastery (Deci & Ryan,
motivational process. That is, choice is the end 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000). The theory posits that
result of a decision-making process in which an autonomy, competence, and relatedness are three
individual has the freedom to select objects or fundamental needs that underlie people’s motiva-
behavior. From these perspectives, choice is both tion and well-being, particularly intrinsic motivation
the end result of some motivational process and or the propensity to engage in a behavior for its
an index of motivation—a barometer of where own sake (or out of enjoyment; Deci, 1971). Social
people’s motivation lies. When looking at choice as contexts that satisfy these needs will enhance mo-
an outcome, virtually any theory of human behav- tivation and well-being (Ryan & Deci, 2000). Thus,
ior might be conceived to have something to say people function better when the context supports
regarding the relation between motivation and a feeling that one is in control of his or her behavior,
choice. From early drive (e.g., Maslow, 1954; can endorse that behavior, and can competently
Murray, 1938) and behaviorist (e.g., B. F. Skinner, navigate the environment. In contrast, when the
1953; Thorndike, 1913) theories to cognitive theo- environment is experienced as controlling and
ries that acknowledge the role of peoples’ biases obstacles block a person’s ability to competently
(e.g., Kahneman & Tversky, 1979), personal beliefs navigate it, motivation and well-being are diminished
and orientations (e.g., Ames, 1992; Dweck, 1999; (Deci, Connell, & Ryan, 1989).
Higgins, 2000), expectancies and values (e.g., The provision of choice is one context that is ex-
Ajzen, 1985; Bandura, 1997; Eccles & Wigfield, pected to support that experience of autonomy and
2002; Vroom, 1964), or goals (e.g., Carver & motivation (e.g., Patall, Cooper, & Robinson, 2008;
Scheier, 2002; Markus & Nurius, 1986), the list of Deci & Ryan, 1987) because having the opportu-
factors that influence people’s choices is long. nity to make choices should theoretically lead to the
More limited in scope are theory and research that experience that one has self-authored and endorsed
have studied choice as a motivating experience behavior. However, choice has also been tied to the
that has cognitive, affective, and behavioral conse- other two needs: People feel competent when they
quences, which is the focus of this chapter. have control over and have chosen the tasks they
engage in (e.g., Henry, 1994; Henry & Sniezek, 1993;
Choice as a Motivator Langer, 1975) and feel related to others to the extent
The presumption that having choice can be a pow- that providing a choice and supporting autonomy
erful motivator is pervasive in theory and research. communicates caring and respect (Deci & Ryan,
Kurt Lewin (1952) provided one of the earliest 2014; Williams, Wallace, & Sung, 2016). In turn,
proposals of the notion that perceiving choice was self-determination theory holds that having choice
a powerful motivator, demonstrating that people will enhance intrinsic motivation, engagement,
would be more likely to engage in an activity if performance, and well-being (Deci, 1980; Deci &
they believed they had freely chosen it. Since then, Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000).
research inspired from a variety of perspectives has The importance of providing choices in the envi-
demonstrated that having and making choices can ronment is also emphasized in theories of academic
influence the way we think, how we feel, and what achievement motivation. For example, achievement
we do. Next, we will review theory and research goal orientation theory suggests that the provision
PATALL 137
participants were given a choice of ticket in the effects are driven in part by enhanced perceptions of
lottery, even though outcomes in a lottery are deter- control, competence, and positive affect that alter
mined by chance. the appraisal and response to the treatment (e.g.,
Moreover, the motivational consequences of Geers et al., 2013).
choosing for feelings of autonomy, competence, and In sum, several psychological theories that em-
perceived control go beyond influencing psycholog- phasize the environmental precursors of motivation,
ical experiences to even affect people’s physical learning, and well-being have suggested that choice
health. A rather dramatic example is provided by may be an influential motivator. According to these
Langer and Rodin (1976), who conducted a field perspectives, having, making, and especially per-
study in a nursing home in which a group of pa- ceiving choices motivate greater engagement in be-
tients were given choices about their routines and haviors that were chosen and lead to a variety of
living environment, such as when to watch a movie psychological and even physical benefits. These ef-
or how to arrange their bedroom furniture, or had fects are assumed to occur because the experience of
these same decisions made for them by the nursing choosing allows people to fully endorse the behavior
home staff. The results showed that in addition to and tasks they engage in and supports a sense of
an increase in choosers’ happiness, they also had competence and control over one’s outcomes.
higher activity levels relative to nonchoosers, as well
as better physical health and even lower death rates Cognitive Perspectives
in the long run. More recent research has also linked Making choices also motivates various cognitive
choosing with healthy behavior or health benefits processes, including the way we think or what we
in the context of medical treatment (e.g., Geers & remember and learn. Choice making plays an im-
Rose, 2011). For example, Rohlfs Dominguez and portant role in cognitive dissonance (Festinger,
colleagues (Rohlfs Dominguez et al., 2013) found 1964). Making a difficult choice or perceiving that
that children increased their vegetable intake when one had a choice in a situation that led to inconsistent
they were allowed to make choices about what to thoughts or behavior can elicit an aversive state known
eat. Relative to having just one vegetable to eat, as dissonance (Elliot & Devine, 1994). In turn, in-
children ate more vegetables when they either had dividuals experiencing dissonance are motivated to
the opportunity to select a vegetable at the begin- resolve those inconsistencies in one’s thoughts and
ning of a meal or had two vegetables on their plate behavior that triggered the dissonance in the first
to choose between throughout the meal. These choices place (e.g., Harmon-Jones & Mills, 1999). This dis-
had benefits even though children’s prechoice ratings sonance and attempts to resolve it arise because
indicated that all the vegetable options were liked people dislike inconsistency between their beliefs
on average across children in the sample, suggesting and the behavior they engage in, and therefore,
that the health benefits extend beyond simply giving people strive for their thoughts to be consistent with
people their preference. each other and with their behavior (Aronson, 1999;
Similarly, a number of studies have suggested Brehm, 1962; Festinger, 1957).
that in comparison to the standard medical ap- In response to difficult choices in particular, dis-
proach of simply prescribing a treatment, allowing sonance can arise because people readily experience
people to choose among treatment options leads to their thoughts about the options and their decisions
greater symptom reduction and satisfaction with about what to choose as potentially inconsistent.
the treatment (e.g., Handelzalts & Keinan, 2010; For example, in Brehm’s (1956) classic study exam-
McCaffery et al., 2011; Rokke & Lall, 1992). ining the role of choice making, after receiving a
Importantly, the possibility that treatment choices chosen household appliance among several options,
may be effective exclusively because they allow participants who chose between appliances similar
patients to find the best match of treatment given in desirability, according to their own initial ratings,
their medical and personal characteristics has been were found to subsequently rate the chosen object
ruled out by research demonstrating that choosing as more desirable and the unchosen object as less
predicts treatment efficacy even when the presuma- desirable. However, participants who were assigned
bly effective treatments are actually placebos (e.g., to receive a particular appliance or who were asked
Brown, Fowler, Rasinski, Rose, & Geers, 2013; to choose between appliances that they initially
Geers & Rose, 2011; Rose, Geers, Fowler, & Rasinski, viewed as highly discrepant in desirability showed
2013; Rose, Geers, Rasinski, & Fowler, 2012). little change in their attitudes toward the appliances
Rather, evidence suggests these treatment choice after receiving the one they were assigned or had
PATALL 139
choose what particular items or information they e xperiences of control are inherently rewarding and
learn, but how they learn (e.g., Metcalfe, 2009). For motivating at a neural level (e.g., Leotti & Delgado,
example, although extensive evidence points to the 2011, 2014; Murayama et al., 2016), having been
limitations of massed study and the benefits of adaptively selected for evolutionary survival (Leotti
spacing study over time (e.g., Cepeda, Pashler, Vul, et al., 2010).
Wixted, & Rohrer, 2006), Son (2010) found that According to this biological perspective, humans
spacing study enhanced learning among both un- have developed a biological system that rewards
dergraduate students and elementary school students choosing due to the adaptive benefits of organisms
only when it had been the students’ own choice being able to select behaviors that lead to desirable
and not when spacing study had been forced on outcomes and avoid undesirable outcomes. In line
the student. with this, when faced with an initial choice between
Finally, choosing may influence memory and options of similar expected value, animals and humans
learning not only through enhanced perceived con- alike demonstrate a preference for options that lead
trol or prioritized self-relevant processing, but also to an additional choice over options that do not
because it may motivate particular ways of thinking. (e.g., Bown, Read, & Summers, 2003; Suzuki, 1999).
Specifically, choosing may activate analytic cognition Support for this biological perspective also comes
rather than holistic cognition in which people focus from evidence that from an early age, both people
on differences rather than similarities or connections and animals react with a great deal of stress over the
between objects (Oyserman, Sorensen, Reber, & restriction of choice in their behavior and even have
Chen, 2009). This happens because choosing itself inhibited physiological and behavioral stress responses
requires a decision maker to discriminate between when they have some choice, or at least perceive
options (e.g., Bettman, Luce, & Payne, 1998; Russo control, in a stressful situation (e.g., Clubb & Mason,
& Leclerc, 1994) and makes the concept of inde- 2003; Mineka & Henderson, 1985; Thompson, 1981).
pendence salient, a construal known to be linked with The neuroscientific evidence also supports the
greater analytic thinking (e.g., Varnum, Grossman, idea that the brain is hardwired to react positively to
Kitayama, & Nisbett, 2010). As such, recent experi- choosing. Research suggests that brain areas, such as
ments have suggested that making a choice, thinking the bilateral ventral striatum, ventromedial prefrontal
about choices, or viewing others make choices triggers cortex, and the midbrain, known to be associated
an analytic mindset in which people attend primarily with reward and the subjective experience of value,
to focal objects rather than background changes and have been found to be activated when participants
readily categorize objects (e.g., Savani, Stephens, & receive or anticipate choice (e.g., Leotti & Delgado,
Markus, 2017). Taken together, cognitive research on 2011; Leotti et al., 2010), with more options being
choosing has suggested that choice motivates partic- linked with greater activity in these rewarding areas
ular ways of construing or processing information of the brain (e.g., Fujiwara et al., 2013; Aoki et al.,
that often lead to benefits for memory and learning. 2014). For example, Tricomi and colleagues (Tricomi,
However, it remains possible, though unexplored, Delgado, & Fiez, 2004) found that the striatum was
that choice–cognition links may have undesirable activated to a greater extent with rewards that
consequences as well (e.g., Savani et al., 2017). followed choices than the same rewards without
choices. In even more direct evidence, Leotti and
Biological and Neuroscience Perspectives Delgado (2011) demonstrated through functional
Given the extensive research documenting the links magnetic resonance imaging that participants expe-
between choosing and motivation, cognition, per- rienced increased activation of the bilateral ventral
formance, and well-being from psychological and striatum and the midbrain in response to being
behavioral perspectives, it is not surprising that presented with a cue that indicated they would
scholars have argued that there are biological roots receive a choice rather than be forced to select a
at the heart of the link between choice and motiva- particular key in a computer task, even though all
tion (e.g., Leotti et al., 2010) and neuroscientific keys had the same expected reward value.
evidence to suggest that choice has motivational In line with research suggesting that choices and
underpinnings (see Leotti et al., 2010; Murayama, perceptions of control mitigate stress responses in
Izuma, Aoki, & Matsumoto, 2016, for reviews). stressful situations, neuroscientific evidence also
What research from this perspective suggests most suggests that choosing may buffer against aversive
basically is that choosing and corresponding experiences, such as negative feedback. Murayama
PATALL 141
effortful and draw on a limited resource for not incompatible with the personal benefits. Markus
self-control, analogous to a source of energy or and Kitayama (2003) labeled this implicit under-
strength, that can be depleted. Since all acts of standing that choices are plentiful and “actions are
volition or self-regulation draw on the same re- ‘freely’ chosen” and “contingent on one’s own pref-
source, any act of choice will have a detrimental erences, intentions, [and] motives” (p. 7) pervasive
effect on subsequent acts that continue to require among Westerners the disjoint model of agency.
self-regulation. Consequently, engaging in a choice Savani and colleagues (Savani & Rattan, 2012;
can result in a state of fatigue or self-regulatory re- Savani, Stephens, & Markus, 2011) argued that this
source depletion in which the individual experiences understanding of choice may lead Westerners to
a decrement in the capacity to initiate activity, believe that most (or all) actions and outcomes can
make choices, or further self-regulate. A number be construed in terms of one’s choices and, thus,
of studies, as well as a meta-analysis, have demon- “regardless of social contextual circumstances, indi-
strated the depleting effect of choice (e.g., Hagger, viduals are responsible for their own actions and life
Wood, Stiff, & Chatzisarantis, 2010). For example, outcomes, and . . . they have the right to control
Baumeister and colleagues (Baumeister et al., 1998) their own lives free from the constraints of other
found that participants who were given a choice of people and of society” (Savani et al., 2011, p. 795).
which side to take in a debate persisted for less time Indeed, evidence has highlighted the complexity
and made fewer attempts at solving subsequent puz- of choice effects on socially relevant outcomes. For
zles compared to participants who were not asked example, in line with a psychosocial perspective,
to make a debating choice. In other studies, par- Patall and Leach (2015) found in one study that
ticipants who made choices among household prod- choice may help to solve the motivational dilemmas
ucts, among college courses, or about the content that result in cheating (e.g., Ruedy, Moore, Gino, &
of courses demonstrated a reduced ability to exert Schweitzer, 2013) by promoting an individual’s sense
self-control on a subsequent task, such as drinking of competence, control, and interest. Specifically,
a bad-tasting beverage or persisting and performing the opportunity to make task choices mitigated un-
well on a challenging math task (Vohs et al., 2008). dergraduate students’ tendency to misreport their
That said, it is important to note that there is an score on an experimental task. However, the benefits
important methodological difference between the of choices were not entirely desirable and follow-up
studies exploring motivation from self-regulatory studies revealed that choice may also prime a disjoint
depletion and self-determination perspectives. model of agency that emphasizes that people have
Namely, the effect of making choices within the an unfettered right to control their own outcomes.
context of the self-regulatory depletion framework Specifically, a follow-up study (Patall & Leach, 2015)
has always been tested by examining subsequent suggested that perceptions of having choice oppor-
resources, persistence, or motivation on a task separate tunities in the classroom predicted perceptions of
from and largely unrelated to the choice-making having greater opportunity to cheat, which in turn
activity. Within the self-determination theory per- predicted more favorable attitudes toward cheating
spective, the effects of choice are typically examined and greater anticipated cheating behavior. This un-
for the same tasks for which choices were originally desirable relationship between perceptions of choice
made. Given the extensive evidence for both per- and anticipated cheating through perceptions of op-
spectives, it seems reasonable to suggest that choice portunity occurred simultaneously with benefits of
making may provide motivational benefits for those perceiving choice for beliefs about competence and
activities and objects related to the choices previously control that mitigated anticipated cheating.
made and simultaneously incur undesirable motiva- Likewise, Savani and colleagues (Savani et al.,
tional and self-regulatory costs for activities and 2011), and Savani and Rattan (2012) found in a
objects unrelated to the choice made. series of investigations that choice predicts unde-
A dark side of choice is revealed in sociocultural sirable social and interpersonal consequences among
perspectives as well. Perceiving choices has several Westerners, including decreased support for com-
positive psychological, cognitive, and behavioral munally beneficial policies, increased victim blaming,
consequences, particularly for individuals within and decreased empathy for disadvantaged people
American society, because it allows individuals to (e.g., Savani et al., 2011). For example, participants
feel that they direct and control their own actions who were asked to watch a video of a college student
and outcomes. However, those same experiences can engaging in everyday activities and monitor the
also have interpersonal and societal costs that are choices he or she made (versus monitor each time
PATALL 143
in control (e.g., Langer, 1975; Leotti et al., 2010; would reconcile discrepancies, Moller et al. (2006)
E. A. Skinner, 1996). Moreover, the imperfect nature found that when an unrestricted autonomous form
of people’s perceptions of the world (e.g., Kahneman, of choice was provided, it had a beneficial effect in
Slovic, & Tversky, 1982; Nisbett & Ross, 1980) terms of persistence and performance outcomes,
means that having and making choices does not whereas depletion resulted when a controlled choice
guarantee that an individual will perceive the oppor- was provided. A meta-analysis my colleagues and
tunity to choose or experience a sense of autonomy I conducted reconfirmed this pattern (Patall et al.,
and control (e.g., Katz & Assor, 2007). By the same 2008). We found that the average effect of choice
token, choosing may sometimes live only in one’s on intrinsic motivation was smaller in studies in
mind. For example, Americans readily perceive all which participants were subtly pressured to choose
their actions as choices (e.g., Savani, Markus, Naidu, a particular option or were provided options grossly
Kumar, & Berlia, 2010). Sometimes people perceive dissimilar in attractiveness (to increase the likeli-
making a choice that evidence suggests they did not hood that all participants would choose a particular
make (e.g., Wegner, 2002) or benefit from choices target option) compared to studies in which this
that had no real consequences (e.g., Langer, 1975; subtle pressuring or strategic pairing of options was
Taforodi, Milne, & Smith, 1999). With these dis- not used.
tinctions in mind, it becomes easier to understand Along similar lines, Choi and Fishbach (2011)
why choice opportunities and choosing can have found across four studies that instrumental choice,
such diverse effects. in which the goal of choosing is to accomplish some
separable end (i.e., to consume a product), under-
Conditions That Build Up and Build mined mental resources and resulted in less interest
On Autonomy and Interest in a chosen product relative to not choosing. In
Choosing derives many of its benefits from facili- contrast, experiential choice, in which the goal of
tating a sense of autonomy (e.g., Ryan & Deci, the choosing was merely to express ones’ preferences
2000). But, when choice is disconnected from the and interests, increased mental resources and inter-
experience of autonomy or even conflicts with est in the product. In other words, when choosers
the experience of autonomy, it may lose its power become extrinsically motivated to choose exclusively
to motivate adaptive outcomes (e.g., Katz & Assor, as a means to bring about some separable desirable
2007). Some mixed findings may emerge because outcome, choosing may have fewer psychological
not all choices (or perceptions of choices) facilitate benefits and the decisions people make can be un-
autonomy or build on people’s interests. dermined. But, when choosing itself is an autono-
Moller and colleagues (Moller, Deci, & Ryan, mous experience of expressing one’s preferences and
2006) highlighted the importance of considering interests without the pressure that comes with being
how the choices being given support feelings of overly focused on outcomes, choosing builds psy-
autonomy in an attempt to integrate contradictory chological resources and the outcomes of choosing
predictions from a self-determination theory perspec- are valued.
tive compared to a self-regulatory strength depletion The importance of designing choices that build
framework. Specifically, Moller et al. (2006) suggested up autonomy goes beyond avoiding the overt pres-
that contradictory findings result from the lack of sure or control that may characterize some forms
differentiation between choices that either promoted of choice. For choosing to have psychological and
participants’ sense of autonomy or provided them performance benefits, it must allow an individual
with a controlled form of choice. Moller et al. to express his or her true preferences (e.g., Katz &
(2006) suggested that most often in studies of ego Assor, 2007; Flowerday & Shell, 2015; Tafarodi,
depletion, a controlled form of choice is implemented Mehranvar, Panton, & Milne, 2002) and self-
in which participants are led to pick a particular regulate his or her behavior (e.g., Reeve et al.,
option. That is, while participants are told they have 2003). Reeve et al. (2003) suggested as much based
a choice among options, they are subtly pressured to on a review of existing literature and their own
pick a particular option. In contrast, studies coming series of studies in which they found that when
out of the self-determination perspective generally choice was designed in such a way that it allowed
provide an unrestricted choice with no indication people to freely regulate their ongoing behavior, it
provided as to which option should be chosen. effectively enhanced intrinsic motivation. However,
In support of their hypothesis that differentiating when the choice only allowed participants to select
between autonomous and controlled forms of choice among task options provided by an experimenter,
PATALL 145
is undermined (e.g., Katz & Assor, 2007; Iyengar, among options that are very similar, nonlinear in
Huberman, & Jiang, 2004; Iyengar & Lepper, 2000). their trade-offs, or unattractive may influence the
When people are prepared to make choices that effort needed to make a decision and, in turn, the
bring about the outcomes they desire, choice is benefits of choosing (e.g., Botti & Iyengar, 2004;
empowering. But, when people lack prerequisite Higgins, Trope, & Kwon, 1999; J. Wang, Novemsky,
knowledge or skills for the task, choosing may have Dhar, & Baumeister, 2010). Choosers make subop-
fewer benefits. For example, more efficacious indi- timal choices and delay making choices when
viduals desire more options, feel more satisfied with confronted with equally attractive or highly risky
their decisions when they had more options, and, in options (Luce, 1998; Mischel & Ebbesen, 1970;
turn, perform better; the opposite is true of less effi- Shafir, Simonson, & Tversky, 1993; Shafir & Tversky,
cacious individuals (e.g., Chua & Iyengar, 2005; 1992; Yates & Mischel, 1979). Higgins et al. (1999)
Reed, Mikels, & Löckenhoff, 2012; Scheibehenne, found that children demonstrated less intrinsic mo-
Greifeneder, & Todd, 2010). M.-T. Wang and Eccles tivation when given two equally preferred activities
(2013) found that middle school students’ percep- to choose from compared to when just one preferred
tions of being provided with choices at school during activity was offered. Likewise, when required to
academic tasks positively predicted engagement for choose among unwanted options, the effort needed
high achievers but negatively predicted engagement to make a decision may be greater (Higgins, 1998;
for low achievers. In a series of three studies with Janis & Mann, 1977; Lewin, 1951), diminishing the
college students, Patall and colleagues (Patall, benefits of choosing. In one study (Botti & Iyengar,
Sylvester, & Han, 2014) found that the provision of 2004), participants were asked to either choose or
choice enhanced perceived competence and intrinsic not choose among either appealing or unappealing
motivation and, in turn, willingness to engage in the yogurt flavors. Although all subjects preferred to
game again and improved task performance when choose for themselves, choosers reported greater sat-
initial perceptions of competence were high; however, isfaction and ate more yogurt than nonchoosers only
motivation diminished with choice when perceived when the yogurt options were appealing. When the
competence was low. This pattern held whether ini- yogurt options were unappealing, choosers were less
tial perceived competence was simply measured, satisfied and ate less yogurt than nonchoosers.
manipulated by altering the difficulty of the task, or The number of options provided or the total
manipulated with competence feedback. number of choices an individual makes within a
Given that there can regularly be misalignments limited time frame may also increase the effort
between choice opportunities and the chooser’s skills required to choose and influence the effects of pro-
or knowledge, it makes sense that people would viding choice. While a self-determination model
sometimes relinquish choice to trusted others (e.g., might predict that too few options or choices may
Bao & Lam, 2008; van Petegem, Beyers, Vansteenkiste, not be powerful enough to bolster the individual’s
& Soenens, 2012) or need choices to be accompanied sense of autonomy, a decision-making or self-
by clear expectations, guidelines, or scaffolding regulatory perspective suggests that many options
to benefit (e.g., Vansteenkiste et al., 2012; Thiede, or choices requires exertion of more effort and
Anderson, & Therriault, 2003). energy. As the cognitive “workload” of deciding be-
tween options and making choices increases with the
The Role of Level of Effort number of options and discrete choices available,
The extent to which a choice is effortful may also play choice may come to be experienced as overwhelming
a role in its effects. Inevitably, all choosing, even rather than motivating and decision-making may
simple choices, may require some effort expenditure become impaired as a consequence (Botti & Iyengar,
that has costs as well as benefits (e.g., Chernev, 2003; 2006; Greenleaf & Lehmann, 1995; Huffman &
Hagger et al., 2010; Luce, Bettman, & Payne, 1997; Kahn, 1998; Malhotra, 1982; Payne et al., 1993;
Simonson, 1992). The greater the extent to which Shugan, 1980).
making a choice is an effortful process, the less likely In fact, research has supported the proposition
it will be to afford motivational benefits and the more that more choice does not necessarily lead to greater
likely it will be that choosing is accompanied by motivation. The meta-analysis I performed with my
regret or is avoided altogether (e.g., Grant & Schwartz, colleagues (Patall et al., 2008) suggested that there
2011; Chernev, Böckenholt, & Goodman, 2015). were an optimal number of choices to be made, with
Some choices are structured such that they 2 and 4 choices producing the greatest effect on in-
are harder to make. For example, making choices trinsic motivation compared to when participants
PATALL 147
In fact, evidence suggests that individuals from students, motivation and performance were most
more interdependent cultures construe fewer ac- enhanced when making a personal choice. In another
tions of themselves and others as choices, are more example, Bao and Lam (2008) found, in a series of
likely to construe their actions as choices when they studies, that Chinese students’ motivation and per-
involved a response to another person, are less likely formance on academic tasks were also enhanced
to choose based on their own personal preferences, when mothers or teachers made choices for them,
and prefer fewer options when there is a choice to but only if they felt close to their mother or teacher.
make (Henderson, 2013; Savani, Markus, & Conner, When they did not feel close, making a personal
2008; Savani et al., 2010). Moreover, previous choice enhanced their motivation and performance
research suggests that one’s cultural background
most strongly. However, students’ level of autono-
may influence the extent to which choosing results in mous motivation positively related to self-reported
dissonance, motivates action, supports well-being, behavioral engagement regardless of how close stu-
or has unintended social or interpersonal conse- dents felt to their teacher. Together, these findings
quences (e.g., Brown et al., 2015; Heine & Lehman, suggest a point made previously, namely, that
1997; Iyengar & Lepper, 1999; Kitayama, Snibbe, choosing is not synonymous with autonomy. To the
Markus, & Suzuki, 2008; Savani et al., 2011). For extent that having another person make a choice for
example, Heine and Lehman (1997) found that after an individual is perceived to be an autonomous ex-
choosing among similarly desirable CDs, Canadians perience, deferring choice to others may be equally
demonstrated the classic dissonance finding, express- or more motivating than making a personal choice.
ing increased liking for chosen CDs and decreased Even aside from the influence of broad cultures
liking for unchosen CDs. However, Japanese across nations, when the microculture or context
participants demonstrated no such spreading of
prescribes a cooperative group orientation, personal
alternatives. Similarly, Kitayama et al. (2008) showed choice may have less value. Hagger and colleagues
that the standard dissonance paradigm did not (Hagger, Rentzelas, & Chatzisarantis, 2014) found
produce dissonance reduction among Japanese
that after inducing a individualistic group norm,
participants. However, when asked to estimate the British undergraduate students in the personal choice
preference of the average college student before condition exhibited greater intrinsic motivation than
making choices, Japanese participants demonstrated students who had aspects of the task selected for
a spreading of alternatives, whereas American them. However, when the group norm prescribed
participants demonstrated a significant spread of collectivism, students assigned a task by an in-group
alternatives regardless of whether they were primed member were more intrinsically motivated than
to think of others. In other words, for European participants who made a personal choice or had a
American participants, choosing produces dissonance task prescribed to them by an out-group member.
to the extent that one is worried about his or her own Along these lines, social class, like nation, provides
preferences and competence. However, for Japanese a set of experiences that influences the meaning and
participants, choosing produces dissonance to the value of choice (e.g., Stephens, Fryberg, & Markus,
extent that one is worried about objections and 2011). For middle-class individuals (i.e., those with a
possible rejection by others. college degree or higher), high-quality choices are
The effect of choice on intrinsic motivation, plentiful and agency for these individuals emphasizes
learning, well-being, and performance outcomes also using choice to express preferences and control the
varies depending on the cultural context (e.g., Bao environment (Snibbe & Markus, 2005). In contrast,
& Lam, 2008; Brown et al., 2015; Iyengar & Lepper, among working-class individuals (i.e., those without
1999; Katz & Assor, 2003). For example, Iyengar a college degree), high-quality choice opportunities
and Lepper (1999) found that intrinsic motivation may be fewer and notions of agency emphasize in-
was enhanced for students of all cultural backgrounds tegrity, honesty, and the expression of self-control.
when a personal choice was made compared to For these individuals, personal choice may be less
when an unfamiliar person (i.e., the experimenter) important than fitting in with others (Snibbe &
made a choice for the individual. However, among Markus, 2005; Stephens et al., 2007). In line with
Asian American students, intrinsic motivation, this reasoning, research suggests that the choices of
performance, learning, and other related outcomes individuals from middle-class backgrounds tend to
were enhanced most when trusted authority figures reflect attempts to differentiate themselves from
or peers made choices for them compared to when a others and engender more motivation when choos-
personal choice was made. For Caucasian American ing facilitates this goal (Snibbe & Markus, 2005;
PATALL 149
benefits of choosing. A nuanced understanding of Botti, S., & Iyengar, S. S. (2004). The psychological pleasure and
both the promise of choice and its limits is impera- pain of choosing: When people prefer choosing at the cost of
subsequent outcome satisfaction. Journal of Personality and
tive. Given the many potential benefits and limits of Social Psychology, 87, 312–326.
choice and its practical implications across a variety Botti, S., & Iyengar, S. S. (2006). The dark side of choice: When
of interpersonal settings such as the classroom, choice impairs social welfare. Journal of Public Policy and
workplace, clinic, or courtroom, it is important that Marketing, 25, 24–38.
future research continue to provide a firm founda- Brehm, J. (1956). Post-decision changes in desirability of
alternatives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52,
tion on which to base conclusions about the role of 384–389.
choice in human functioning. Brehm, J. W. (1962). A dissonance analysis of attitude-discrepant
behavior. New York, NY: Gaines Dog Research Center.
Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R. (1959). Re-evaluation of
References choice alternatives as a function of their number and
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned qualitative similarity. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology,
behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Eds.), Action-control: 58, 373–378.
From cognition to behavior (pp. 11–39). Heidelberg, Germany: Brown, J. A., Fowler, S. L., Rasinski, H. M., Rose, J. P., & Geers,
Springer. A. L. (2013). Choice as a moderator of placebo expectation
Amabile, T. M. (1979). Effects of external evaluation on artistic effects: Additional support from two experiments. Basic and
creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, Applied Social Psychology, 35, 436–444.
221–233. Brown, J. A., Oikawa, M., Rose, J. P., Haught, H. M., Oikawa,
Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: A H., & Geers, A. L. (2015). Choosing across cultures: The
componential conceptualization. Journal of Personality and effect of choice complexity on treatment outcomes. Journal
Social Psychology, 45, 357–376. of Behavioral Decision Making, 28, 515–528.
Amabile, T. M., & Gitomer, J. (1984). Children’s artistic Bown, N. J., Read, D., & Summers, B. (2003). The lure of
creativity: Effect of choice in task materials. Personality and choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 16, 297–308.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 209–215. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2002). Control processes and
Amabile, T. M., Hennessey, B. A., & Grossman, B. S. (1986). self-organization as complementary principles underlying
Social influence on creativity: The effects of contract-for behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 304–315.
rewards. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 14–23. Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D.
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review
motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261–271. and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380.
Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the Chaiken, S., & Baldwin, M. W. (1981). Affective–cognitive
classroom: Students’ learning strategies and motivation consistency and the effect of salient of behavioral information
processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260–267. on the self-perception of attitudes. Journal of Personality and
Aoki, R., Matsumoto, M., Yomogida, Y., Izuma, K., Murayama, Social Psychology, 41, 1–12.
K., Sugiura, A., . . . Adolphs, R. (2014). Social equality in the Chernev, A. (2003). When more is less and less is more: The role
number of choice options is represented in the ventromedial of ideal point availability and assortment in choice. Journal of
prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 6413–6421. Consumer Research, 30, 170–183.
Aronson, E. (1999). Dissonance, hypocrisy, and the self-concept. Chernev, A., Böckenholt, U., & Goodman, J. (2015). Choice
In E. Harmon-Jones & J. Mills (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: overload: A conceptual review and meta-analysis. Journal of
Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 103–126). Consumer Psychology, 25, 333–358.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Choi, J., & Fishbach, A. (2011). Choice as an end versus a means.
Assor, A., Kaplan, H., & Roth, G. (2002). Choice is good, but Journal of Marketing Research, 48, 544–554.
relevance is excellent: Autonomy-enhancing and suppressing Chua, R. Y. J., & Iyengar, S. (2005). The effects of choice, goal, and
teacher behaviors predicting students’ engagement in school creative self-efficacy on divergent thinking and creative outcomes.
work. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 261–278. Paper presented at the Academy of Management Annual
Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. Meeting, Honolulu, HI.
American Psychologist, 44, 1175–1184. Cloutier, J., & Macrae, N. (2008). The feeling of choosing:
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, Self-involvement and the cognitive status of things past.
NY: Freeman. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 125–135.
Bao, X., & Lam, S. (2008). Who makes the choice? Rethinking Clubb, R., & Mason, G. (2003). Animal welfare: Captivity
the role of autonomy and relatedness in Chinese children’s effects on wide-ranging carnivores. Nature, 425, 473–474.
motivation. Child Development, 79, 269–283. Collins, B. E., Ashmore, R. D., Hornbeck, F. W., & Whitney,
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. R. E. (1970). Studies in forced compliance: XIII and XV in
(1998). Ego-depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? search of dissonance producing forced compliance paradigm.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265. Representative Research in Social Psychology, 1, 11–23.
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Cooper, J., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). A new look at dissonance
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
New York, NY: Academic Press. social psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 229–266). New York, NY:
Bettman, J. R., Luce, M. F., & Payne, J. W. (1998). Constructive Academic Press.
consumer choice processes. Journal of Consumer Research, 25, Cordova, D., & Lepper, M. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and the
187–217. process of learning: Beneficial effects of contextualization,
PATALL 151
Henry, R. A. (1994). The effects of choice and incentives on the Kitayama, S., Snibbe, A. C., Markus, H. R., & Suzuki, T. (2008).
overestimation of future performance. Organizational Behavior Is there any “free” choice? Self and dissonance in two cultures.
and Human Decision Processes, 57, 210–225. Psychological Science, 15, 527–533.
Henry, R. A., & Sniezek, J. A. (1993). Situational factors affecting Klein, S. B., & Loftus, J. (1988). The nature of self-referent
judgments of future performance. Organizational Behavior encoding: The contributions of elaborative and organizational
and Human Decision Processes, 54, 104–132. processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 5–11.
Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of Kornell, N., & Metcalfe, J. (2006). Study efficacy and the region
interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111–127. of proximal learning framework. Journal of Experimental
Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 609–622.
American Psychologist, 55, 1217–1230. Krapp, A. (2005). Basic needs and the development of interest and
Higgins, E. T. (1998). From expectancies to worldviews: Regulatory intrinsic motivational orientations. Learning and Instruction,
focus in socialization and cognition. In J. M. Darley & 15, 381–395.
J. Cooper (Eds.), Attribution and social interaction: The legacy Langer, E. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality
of Edward E. Jones (pp. 243–309). Washington, DC: and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.
American Psychological Association. Langer, E. J., & Rodin, J. (1976). The effects of choice and
Higgins, E. T., Trope, Y., & Kwon, J. (1999). Augmentation and enhanced personal responsibility for the aged: A field
undermining from combining activities: The role of choice experiment in an institutional setting. Journal of Personality
in activity engagement theory. Journal of Experimental Social and Social Psychology, 34, 191–198.
Psychology, 35, 285–307. Leotti, L. A., & Delgado, M. R. (2011). The inherent reward of
Huffman, C., & Kahn, B. E. (1998). Variety for sale: Mass choice. Psychological Science, 22, 1310–1318.
customization or mass confusion? Journal of Retailing, 74, Leotti, L. A., & Delgado, M. R. (2014). The value of exercising
491–513. control over monetary gains and losses. Psychological Science,
Humphreys, G. W., & Sui, J. (2015). The salient self: Social 25, 596–604.
saliency effects based on self-bias. Journal of Cognitive Leotti, L. A., Iyengar, S. S., & Ochsner, K. N. (2010). Born to
Psychology, 27, 129–140. choose: The origins and value of the need for control. Trends
Iyengar, S. (2010). The art of choosing. New York, NY: Grand in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 457–463.
Central. Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science; selected theoretical
Iyengar, S. S., Elwork, R. F., & Schwartz, R. (2006). Doing papers (D. Cartwright Ed.). New York, NY: Harper & Row.
better but feeling worse: Looking for the “best” job Lewin, K. (1952). Group decision and social change.
undermines satisfaction. Psychological Science, 17, 143–150. In G. E. Swanson, T. M. Newcomb, & E. L. Hartley
Iyengar, S. S., Huberman, G., & Jiang, W. (2004). How much (Eds.), Readings in social psychology (pp. 459–473). New York,
choice is too much? Contributions to 401(k) retirement NY: Holt.
plans. In O. S. Mitchell & S. P. Utkus (Eds.), Pension design Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Patall, E. A., & Messersmith, E. E.
and structure: New lessons from behavioral finance (pp. 83–95). (2013). Antecedents and consequences of situational interest.
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 591–614.
Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.2012.02080.x
choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Lisman, J. E., & Grace, A. A. (2005). The hippocampal–VTA
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 349–366. loop: Controlling the entry of information into long-term
Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is memory. Neuron, 46, 703–713.
demotivating: Can one desire too much of a good thing? Luce, M. (1998). Choosing to avoid: Coping with negatively
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 995–1006. emotion-laden consumer decisions. The Journal of Consumer
Iyengar, S. S., Wells, R., & Schwartz, B. (2006). Doing better Research, 24, 409–433.
but feeling worse: Looking for the “best” job undermines Luce, M. F., Bettman, J. R., & Payne, J. W. (1997). Choice
satisfaction. Psychological Science, 17, 143–150. processing in emotionally difficult decisions. Journal of
Jacobs, J., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Parents, task values, and real life Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
achievement-related choices. In C. Sansone & J. M. 23, 384–405.
Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The Lyubomirsky, S., & Ross, L. (1999). Changes in attractiveness of
search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 405–439). elected, rejected, and precluded alternatives: A comparison of
San Diego, CA: Academic Press. happy and unhappy individuals. Journal of Personality and
Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision making, a psychological Social Psychology, 76, 988–1007.
analysis of conflict, choice and commitment. New York, NY: Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1991). Enhancing student
Free Press. motivation: A schoolwide approach. Educational Psychologist,
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under 26, 399–427.
uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Malhotra, N. K. (1982). Information load and consumer decision
University Press. making. Journal of Consumer Research, 8, 419–430.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self:
analysis of decisions under risk. Econometrika, 47, 263−291. Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation.
Katz, I., & Assor, A. (2003). Is autonomy important for non- Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.
Western students? Examining autonomy as a universal human Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Models of agency:
propensity. Paper presented at the 84th Annual Meeting of Sociocultural diversity in the construction of action. In
the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, IL. V. Murphy-Burman & J. Berman (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium
Katz, I., & Assor, A. (2007). When choice motivates and when it on Motivation: Vol. 49. Cross cultural differences in perspectives
does not. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 429–442. on self (pp. 1–57). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
PATALL 153
Reeve, J., Nix, G., & Hamm, D. (2003). Testing models of the Schwartz, B. (2004, April). The tyranny of choice. Scientific
experience of self-determination in intrinsic motivation and American, 71–75.
the conundrum of choice. Journal of Educational Psychology, Schwartz, B., Ward, A. H., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S.,
95, 375–392. White, K., & Lehman, D. (2002). Maximizing versus
Rodin, J., Rennert, K., & Solomon, S. K. (1980). Intrinsic satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice. Journal of Personality
motivation for control: Fact or fiction. In A. Baum & and Social Psychology, 83, 1178–1197.
J. E. Singer (Eds.), Advances in environmental psychology: Vol 2. Shafir, E., Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1993). Reason-based
Applications of personal control (pp. 131–148). Hillsdale, NJ: choice. Cognition, 49, 11–36.
Erlbaum. Shafir, E., & Tversky, A. 1992. Thinking through uncertainty:
Rogers, T., Kuiper, N., & Kirker, W. (1977). Self-reference and Nonconsequential reasoning and choice. Cognitive Psychology,
the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality 24, 449–474.
and Social Psychology, 35, 677–688. Shepard, J., & O’Grady, A. (2017). What kinds of alternative
Rohlfs Domínguez, P., Gámiz, F., Gil, M., Moreno, H., Márquez possibilities are required of the folk concept(s) of choice?
Zamora, R., Gallo, M., & de Brugada, I. (2013). Providing Consciousness and Cognition, 48, 138–148.
choice increases children’s vegetable intake. Food Quality and Shugan, S. M. (1980). The cost of thinking. Journal of Consumer
Preference, 30, 108–113. Research, 7, 99–111.
Rokke, P. D., & Lall, R. (1992). The role of choice in enhancing Shultz, T. R., Lévéille, E., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Free-choice
tolerance to acute pain. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, and cognitive dissonance revisited. Personality and Social
53–65. Psychology Bulletin, 25, 40–48.
Rose, J. P., Geers, A. L., Fowler, S. L., & Rasinski, H. M. (2013). Simonson, I. (1992). The influence of anticipating regret and
Choice-making, expectations, and treatment positivity: How responsibility on purchase decisions. Journal of Consumer
and when choosing shapes aversive experiences. Journal of Research, 19, 105. doi:10.1086/209290
Behavioral Decision Making, 27, 1–10. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York,
Rose, J. P., Geers, A. L., Rasinski, H., & Fowler, S. L. (2012). NY: Macmillan.
Choice and placebo expectation effects in the context of pain Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of
analgesia. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 35, 462–470. Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 549–570.
Ruedy, N. E., Moore, C., Gino, F., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2013). Smetana, J. G., Campione-Barr, N., & Daddis, C. (2004). Longi
The cheater’s high: The unexpected affective benefits of tudinal development of family decision making: Defining
unethical behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, healthy behavioral autonomy for middle-class African
105, 531–548. American adolescents. Child Development, 75, 1418–1434.
Russo, J. E., & Leclerc, F. (1994). An eye-fixation analysis of Snibbe, A., & Markus, H. R. (2005). You can’t always get what
choice processes for consumer nondurables. Journal of you want: Educational attainment, agency, and choice.
Consumer Research, 21, 274–290. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 703–720.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and Son, L. K. (2010). Metacognitive control and the spacing effect.
the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. Cognition, 36, 255–262.
Savani, K., Markus, H. R., & Conner, A. L. (2008). Let your Stephens, N. M., Fryberg, S. A., & Markus, H. R. (2011). When
preference be your guide? Preferences and choices are more choice does not equal freedom. Social Psychological and
tightly linked for North Americans than for Indians. Journal Personality Science, 2, 33–41.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 861–876. Stephens, N. M., & Levine, C. S. (2011). Opting out or denying
Savani, K., Markus, H. R., Naidu, N. V. R., Kumar, S., & Berlia, discrimination? How the framework of free choice in
N. (2010). What counts as a choice? U.S. Americans are American society influences perceptions of gender inequality.
more likely than Indians to construe actions as choices. Psychological Science, 22, 1231–1236.
Psychological Science, 21, 391–398. Stephens, N., Markus, H. R., & Townsend, S. S. M. (2007).
Savani, K., & Rattan, A. (2012). A choice mind-set increases the Choice as an act of meaning: The case of social class. Journal
acceptance and maintenance of wealth inequality. Psycho of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 814–830.
logical Science, 23, 796–804. Suzuki, S. (1999). Selection of force- and free-choice by monkeys.
Savani, K., Stephens, N. M., & Markus, H. R. (2011). The Perceptual and Motor Skills, 88, 242–250.
unanticipated interpersonal and societal consequences of Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference
choice: Victim blaming and reduced support for the public effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
good. Psychological Science, 22, 795–802. 121, 371–394.
Savani, K., Stephens, N. M., & Markus, H. R. (2017). Choice as Tafarodi, R. W., Mehranvar, S., Panton, R. L., & Milne, A. B.
an engine of analytic thought. Journal of Experimental (2002). Putting oneself in the task: Choice, personalization,
Psychology: General, 146, 1234–1246. and confidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
Scheibehenne, B., Greifeneder, R., & Todd, P. M. (2010). 648–658.
Can there ever be too many options? A meta-analytic Tafarodi, R. W., Milne, A. B., & Smith, A. J. (1999). The
review of choice overload. Journal of Consumer Research, confidence of choice: Evidence for an augmentation effect on
37, 409–425. self-perceived performance. Personality and Social Psychology
Schraw, G., Flowerday, T., & Lehman, S. (2001). Promoting Bulletin, 25, 1405–1416.
situational interest in the classroom. Educational Psychology Takahashi, M. (1991). The role of choice in memory as a function
Review, 13, 211–224. of age: Support for a metamemory interpretation of the self-
Schwartz, B. (2000). Self-determination: The tyranny of freedom. choice effect. Psychologia: An International Journal of
American Psychologist, 55, 79–88. Psychology in the Orient, 34, 254–258.
PATALL 155
CH A PT ER
Paul J. Silvia
Abstract
Curiosity is an old, intriguing, and vexing construct in the psychology of motivation. This chapter
reviews the major strands of thought on curiosity and motivation. These strands include: (a) curiosity
as a motive to reduce negative states, such as uncertainty, novelty, arousal, drive, or information gaps;
(b) curiosity as a source of intrinsic motivation that fosters learning and exploring for their own sakes;
and (c) curiosity as a stable motivational difference between people that leads to differences in knowledge,
goals, achievement, and experience. The scope of psychological thought on curiosity defies a simple
integration, but it offers inspiration for researchers curious about why people learn and explore in the
absence of obvious external rewards.
Keywords: curiosity, interest, exploration, intrinsic motivation, novelty, openness to experience,
personality
The joys of history are many. Some are guilty joys, is called for. Instead, we will aim to cultivate a
like snickering at the weird use of semicolons and sense of perspective—some of psychology’s prob-
em dashes in old scholarly books, but some are lems should be appreciated rather than solved—and
more noble, like recognizing that most of the major a sense of interest in the diverse, wide-ranging, and
problems in psychology were identified prior to the occasionally quirky body of thought on curiosity.
invention of air conditioning, paperback books, or This chapter extracts and explores the three major
hierarchical linear modeling. Curiosity is an old strands of thought on curiosity’s motivational nature.
concept in the study of human motivation, and The first strand proposes that curiosity is a kind of
like many of psychology’s venerable problems, the deficit motivation: It motivates people to fill gaps
problem of curiosity seems tractable enough to be in knowledge, reduce unpleasant uncertainty, and
intriguing but too complicated to ever solve. The minimize aversive states of drive. The second strand
history of psychology thus offers modern researchers proposes that curiosity is a kind of intrinsic moti-
many interesting takes on curiosity: Most of the vation: It motivates people to explore and learn for
major schools of thought in motivation science their own sakes. The third strand proposes individual
have had something to say about what curiosity is, differences in curiosity: Variation in curiosity trans-
how it works, and what it does, if anything. lates into big differences in behavior and life
In this chapter, we will consider some of the major outcomes, although the models disagree about the
themes that have emerged in the history of thought motivational nature of between-person variation.
on curiosity and motivation. Our goal is not to de-
velop a unified model of curiosity by reinterpreting Curiosity Is Motivation to Reduce
past work in terms of a modern theory. History pro- Novelty and Uncertainty
vides many such models, and the fact that there are so Our first major strand of thought on curiosity’s
many suggests that some history-induced modesty motivational nature proposes that curiosity and
157
exploratory behavior represent attempts to reduce learning. For example, the opportunity to view a
novelty and uncertainty. In such models, gaining novel maze compartment or complex display or to
knowledge and exploring the world are instrumental manipulate puzzles can reinforce other behaviors
acts, not ultimate goals in themselves. Instead, learn- (e.g., Butler, 1953; Harlow & McClearn, 1954;
ing and exploring allow people to reduce something Myers & Miller, 1954).
unpleasant, such as feelings of uncertainty, ignorance, Eventually, it became clear that the classical drive
information deprivation, arousal, or drive. Curiosity reduction model simply could not accommodate the
is thus akin to scratching a mental itch or filling a many demonstrations of curiosity and exploration.
mental hole. Motivation psychologists thus searched for exten-
Motivation psychology’s grandest drive-reduction sions and modifications of Hull’s approach. The ear-
model comes from Clark Hull’s body of work, which liest models simply proposed a new drive, such as a
had a tremendous influence in its time. Hull cer- curiosity drive, an exploration drive, or a boredom
tainly was not the first psychologist to propose a drive (see Fowler, 1965). These new drives did not
hydraulic approach to motivation, but his model of stick, but they were an ironic development—the
drive and reinforcement was probably the first major behavior theorists had often mocked instinct theo-
scientific theory of motivation to inspire extensive ries of motivation for proposing new instincts to
empirical research. We cannot cover the complexity deal with challenging findings.
and development of Hull’s model here—for the Berlyne (1960) proposed a particularly clever
details, curious and motivated readers can consult revision of drive theory in his first major model of
Hull’s (1943, 1952) own books or reviews by his curiosity. He suggested that organisms do indeed
contemporaries (Atkinson, 1964; Bolles, 1967). prefer a low level of arousal, but arousal is nonlinearly
In broad strokes, Hull formalized several ideas related to the novelty, complexity, uncertainty, and
that had been proposed during the early writings on conflict of stimuli in the environment. He suggested
psychological drives. First, organisms experience a a U-shaped relationship between actual arousal and
state of drive as a result of internal deficits or strong novelty: Arousal is high when stimuli are very low
stimulation from the environment. Second, reducing and very high in novelty. Understimulation and
the state of drive is rewarding, so behaviors that result overstimulation thus both increase arousal, which
in drive reduction are reinforced and hence more then motivates arousal reduction. Understimulation
likely to happen during later periods of drive. This promoted diversive exploration, in which bored
model thus accommodated both the intensity and organisms search for anything that would boost
the direction aspects of motivation, and it integrated arousal potential; overstimulation promoted specific
problems of motivation with problems of learning. exploration, in which activated organisms examine
But from the outset, curiosity and exploratory behav- the arousal-producing stimulus to reduce its novelty
ior seemed hard to explain. Seeking stimulation and and arousingness. The nonlinear function relating
approaching unfamiliar stimuli increase drive; within arousal and arousal potential was clever, but it did
Hull’s model, it is not obvious why an organism at not catch on (Berlyne, 1967).
rest would cease resting and start exploring. Another modification of Hull’s approach was
Unfortunately, the accumulation of facts inter- the optimal arousal approach, a family of models
fered with Hull’s elegant theory, a common occupa- that proposed that the ideal level of arousal was
tional hazard in science. One peculiar phenomenon moderate instead of low (Fiske & Maddi, 1961;
that predated Hull’s work was spontaneous alterna- Hebb, 1955; Hunt, 1965; Leuba, 1955; Zuckerman,
tion. Rats in T mazes will often alternate between 1969). As a result, many classic Hullian findings
arms of the maze, presumably for no real reason could be retained—organisms will act to reduce
apart from the variety afforded by the arm less drive when arousal exceeds the optimal level—
traveled (Montgomery, 1951, 1952). Harlow showed while accommodating curiosity. This is a neat idea,
similar findings with primates. When given puzzles but optimal arousal models never inspired much
to work with, the primates often worked on them research despite the broad interest in them in their
vigorously in the absence of food rewards; if any- time. As Hull’s model ebbed and alternative models
thing, food rewards and prior food deprivation of motivation emerged, researchers moved away
seemed to interfere with learning these complex tasks from the notion that reinforcement depends on
(Harlow, 1953). Most fatal, however, was research arousal (Atkinson, 1964). Once this notion was
that showed that drive-increasing stimuli—such as dropped, optimal arousal models became a com-
novelty and intense stimulation—could reinforce promise for a conflict that no longer existed.
SILVIA 159
his many later landmark works. Dewey contended
that educators typically used one of two methods to
motivate students, neither of which worked well. In
Positive
the effort method, instructors use self-discipline and Primary Reward
System
willpower as the main mechanisms of learning—if
the material is tedious, students must simply slog
through it. In the interest method, instructors use
diverting, novel, and flashy elements to attract
Negative
Primary Aversion
students’ attention to basically boring material— System
“when things have to be made interesting, it is
because interest itself is wanting,” Dewey (1913, p. 11)
quipped.
As an alternative, Dewey proposed connecting
material to students’ own interests. Children already Figure 9.1 Berlyne’s (1971) model of reward and aversion
had important interests, and new activities could systems.
become interesting in their own right if they ad-
vanced or linked to an existing interest. Dewey system, generates positive affect as stimuli become
argued that interest was a developmental process more novel, complex, and uncertain. The positive
rather than a momentary state—it motivated people affect motivates and rewards engagement with new
to immerse themselves in the activity, and it made things, so this model qualifies as a for-its-own-sake
the process valuable and self-relevant. Dewey’s con- model of curiosity and motivation. A second system,
ception of interested action probably represents psy- the primary aversion system, generates negative
chology’s first fleshed-out treatment of interest and affect as stimuli become more novel, complex, and
intrinsic motivation: Interested actions are internal- uncertain. The aversion system, however, has a tem-
ized and autonomous, and over time they cultivate poral offset—it takes higher levels of stimulation for
competence and successful development. it to kick in—so the combined effect of the two sys-
In the modern era, the notion of curiosity as a tems is to create positive approach and engagement
source of intrinsic, for-its-own-sake motivation and then eventual aversive withdrawal as stimuli in-
emerged in many areas of psychology. Within social crease in intensity.
and personality psychology, the rise of models of Berlyne’s (1971) reward–aversion model is im-
intrinsic motivation in the 1970s (Deci, 1975)— portant for historical reasons. Berlyne developed
particularly the line of work that developed into the model as part of a shift into the psychology of
self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985, aesthetics (e.g., Berlyne, 1972, 1974), and his re-
2000)—is probably the best known example of a search team profoundly affected how the small but
model of curiosity as a source of intrinsic motiva- valiant community of aesthetics researchers thought
tion. Deci and Ryan (2000, p. 233) argued that about aesthetic preference and experience (Silvia,
“intrinsically motivated behaviors are those that are 2012). The model itself, however, never stuck, perhaps
freely engaged out of interest without the necessity because behavior theory was obviously waning in
of separable consequences,” and research in this the 1970s and emerging approaches to curiosity and
area commonly measures engagement, exploration, to the neuroscience of reward seemed more fresh
and interest (Deci, 1992, 1998; Krapp, 2002). This and exciting.
area of work is reviewed in another chapter (Ryan, Within emotion psychology, research on curios-
Ryan, Di Domenico, and Deci, this volume), so here ity and interest got its start with the work of Silvan
we will only express awe at the enormous amount of Tomkins (1962), who gave much of early emotion
research that self-determination theory has inspired. science its start. Tomkins argued that emotions were
Within behavior theory, Berlyne (1971, 1973) the organism’s primary motivational system. First,
proposed a new model of curiosity that shifted from at the level of action dynamics, emotions made
the position that reinforcement and exploration some events significant and thus gave them priority.
depended on arousal reduction. As an alternative, Tomkins proposed a central assembly that organ-
he proposed that several brain systems interacted to ized the components of behavior into effective
promote exploration or avoidance. Figure 9.1 de- action. People cannot do many behaviors at once,
picts how two of these systems relate to positive and so they need a mechanism that elevates some tasks
negative affect. The first system, the primary reward and situations over others. Tomkins’s theory thus
SILVIA 161
Overall, the intrinsic motivation approach to exercise in those in whom it is innately strong.
curiosity is more vibrant than the drive reduction In men of the latter type it may become the main
approach—it is the backdrop for most contemporary source of intellectual energy and effort; to its impulse
research. The three domains we have covered— we certainly owe most of the disinterested labors of
social and personality psychology, behavior theory, the highest types of intellect.
and emotion psychology—capture the bulk of (p. 50)
modern research. Social and personality psychol-
This passage is doubly intriguing: It proposes
ogy contains much of the experimental research on
individual differences in curiosity, a topic that has
situational and dispositional influences on curiosity
since been widely studied, and it suggests that these
and interest; behavior theory has evolved into mature
differences become exaggerated across the life span,
psychobiological models of novelty-seeking and
a topic that has not.
reward, such as Panksepp’s (1998) SEEKING system;
Modern individual differences research began a
and emotion psychology continues to explore what
long time after McDougall’s work, and models of
makes things interesting, how interest operates across
trait-like curiosity represent both themes we have
domains, and how interest develops.
considered so far. It is hard to organize the hurly-
burly world of self-report curiosity scales—there
Some People Are More Curious are many scales and many constructs. Some models
Than Others have their roots in Berlyne’s models of curiosity.
Our third strand of thought concerns individual
Researchers have developed scales to measure indi-
differences in curiosity. William McDougall (1960),
vidual differences in epistemic, sensory, specific, and
the infamous and maligned instinct theorist, was one
diversive forms of curiosity (e.g., Collins, Litman,
of the first psychologists to speculate about stable
& Spielberger, 2004; Litman & Spielberger, 2003;
between-person variability in curiosity. In modern
Piotrowski, Litman, & Valkenburg, 2014), and
terms, his instincts are modular and automated mo-
Spielberger and Starr (1994) proposed that trait
tivational systems that are evoked by stimuli and
curiosity corresponds to Berlyne’s (1971) primary
opportunities in the environment. Instinctive action
reward system and that trait anxiety corresponds to
has a cognitive aspect (an apprehension of an object),
the primary aversion system. People high in trait
a behavioral tendency (a motivational urge to act),
curiosity can thus tolerate more uncertainty and
and an affective aspect (an emotional feeling).
novelty before withdrawing. Other studies outside
McDougall (1960) proposed an instinct of curi-
the Berlyne tradition have suggested social (Renner,
osity, which was the instinct associated with
2006) and intrapersonal (Litman, Robinson, &
exploratory behavior—the impulse “to approach
Demetre, 2017) forms of curiosity.
and examine more closely the object that excites it”
For the most part, recent research has focused on
(p. 49)—and with the emotional state of wonder. He
global curiosity as a source of intrinsic motivation.
argued that “the native excitant of the instinct would
Kashdan (2009), for example, approaches individ-
seem to be any object similar to, yet perceptibly differ-
ual differences in curiosity from the perspective of
ent from, familiar objects habitually noticed” (p. 47),
positive psychology. In Kashdan’s work, curiosity is
while recognizing that higher levels of novelty and
a tendency to notice, seek, value, and embrace
uncertainty evoked fear. It is tempting to poke fun
novelty, uncertainty, and challenge. The Curiosity
at poor McDougall and his many instincts, but for
and Exploration Inventory, a brief self-report scale,
many decades the notion of a curiosity instinct was
was developed to capture this model of curiosity
the prevailing model of the motivational basis of curi-
(Kashdan et al., 2009). The model is explicitly mo-
osity, and it had a big influence (e.g., Cameron, 1922).
tivational, in that curious people respond to nov-
McDougall (1960) thought that the curiosity
elty with an appetitive, approach-oriented response.
instinct was less important to humans, so he believed
As a consequence of exploring new and challenging
that it was more variable as a result:
things across the life span, curious people are ex-
This instinct, being one whose exercise is not of prime pected to have a wide range of positive outcomes.
importance to the individual, exhibits great individual For example, curious people typically have better
differences as regards its innate strength; and these academic achievement (Kashdan & Yuen, 2007),
differences are apt to be increased during the course of higher subjective well-being (Gallagher & Lopez,
life, the impulse growing weaker for lack of use in 2007), and a stronger sense that their daily lives and
those in whom it is innately weak, stronger through goals are meaningful (Kashdan & Steger, 2007).
SILVIA 163
something instead of nothing? Why do people persist personal goals—remains obscure. In everyday
with seemingly capricious activities in the absence curiosity, people are probably exploring activities
of obvious external rewards and in the presence of and domains that they have engaged with before
more important goals? Why do apparently trivial and know a lot about (Durik et al., 2017;
things capture attention and action? The wellsprings Prenzel, 1992)—their hobbies and interests are
of action proposed by motivation theories are usually important to understanding how curiosity
abstract, but the kinds of actions motivated by cu- influences their behavior. So far, most of the work
riosity are usually mundane. Nevertheless, curiosity on hobbies and interests has been done in
is an important mechanism in the development of educational research, which emphasizes a distinction
knowledge and competence, so it is reassuring that between situational interest and individual interest
something is coming out of the hours of people (Renninger and Su, this volume; Schiefele, 2009).
exploring their worlds. Future work should examine how interests relate to
situational states and enduring traits (e.g., Durik &
Future Directions Harackiewicz, 2007) and how idiosyncratic interests
1. How can the different strands of thought on develop (Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Silvia, 2006).
curiosity be connected? The urge to weave beats
strong in the hearts of motivation psychologists. References
Within each strand, there could be interesting Arnold, F. (1910). Attention and interest: A study in psychology and
education. New York, NY: Macmillan.
bridges. In the intrinsic motivation strand, there
Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. New York,
could be fertile connections between self- NY: Van Nostrand.
determination theory and emotion psychology, two Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. (1970). The dynamics of action.
areas that have not had much to do with each New York, NY: Wiley.
other. In the drive-reduction strand, researchers Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal
emotion expressions. Journal of Personality and Social
could explore the value of a single broad model—
Psychology, 70, 614–636.
something akin to Kruglanski’s (2004) lay Bar-Anan, Y., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2009). The feeling
epistemics model or Litman’s (2005) I-D model— of uncertainty intensifies affective reactions. Emotion, 9,
for unifying curious and incurious motives for 123–127.
seeking knowledge. Between each strand, Berlyne, D. E. (1957). Uncertainty and conflict: A point of
contact between information-theory and behavior-theory
researchers could consider bridges between
concepts. Psychological Review, 64, 329–339.
intrinsic motivation and drive reduction. Such Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York,
bridges would shift what the field means by NY: McGraw–Hill.
curiosity, but they could illuminate the broader Berlyne, D. E. (1965). Structure and direction in thinking.
workings of cognitive motives. New York, NY: Wiley.
Berlyne, D. E. (1967). Arousal and reinforcement. Nebraska
2. How do curious traits influence curious
Symposium on Motivation, 15, 1–110.
states? Many models of individual differences Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York,
invoke motivational mechanisms, largely along NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.
the lines of drive reduction, intrinsic motivation, Berlyne, D. E. (1972). Ends and means of experimental
or both. Despite this conceptual overlap, there is aesthetics. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 26, 303–325.
Berlyne, D. E. (1973). The vicissitudes of aplopathematic and
not much integration between the state and the
thelematoscopic pneumatology (or the hydrography of
trait approaches to curiosity—with many hedonism). In D. E. Berlyne & K. B. Madsen (Eds.), Pleasure,
interesting exceptions, naturally—perhaps reward, preference (pp. 1–33). New York, NY: Academic Press.
because self-report assessment and cross-sectional Berlyne, D. E. (Ed.) (1974). Studies in the new experimental
designs dominate individual differences research. aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic
appreciation. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
Future work on curious people ought to tackle
Bolles, R. C. (1967). Theory of motivation. New York, NY:
the paradigms used in state research, particularly Harper & Row.
the measures of experience, cognition, and Butler, R. A. (1953). Discrimination learning by rhesus monkeys
behavior used to illuminate knowledge-seeking to visual-exploration motivation. Journal of Comparative and
and exploration. Physiological Psychology, 46, 95–98.
Cameron, E. H. (1922). Psychology and the school. New York, NY:
3. What does the midrange level of curious
Century.
motivation look like? Most work on curiosity has Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2007). Personality and
explored situational aspects or stable individual music: Can traits explain how people use music in everyday
differences. The midrange—the level of idiosyncratic life? British Journal of Psychology, 98, 175–185.
SILVIA 165
In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self- “exploration.” Journal of Comparative and Physiological
determination research (pp. 405–427). Rochester, NY: Psychology, 47, 428–436.
University of Rochester Press. Oleynick, V. C., DeYoung, C. G., Hyde, E., Kaufman, S. B.,
Kruglanski, A. W. (2004). The psychology of closed-mindedness. Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2017). Openness/intellect: The
New York, NY: Psychology Press. core of the creative personality. In G. J. Feist, R. Reiter-
Kruglanski, A. W., & Fishman, S. (2009). The need for cognitive Palmon, & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of
closure. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook creativity and personality research (pp. 9–27). New York, NY:
of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 343–353). Cambridge University Press.
New York, NY: Guilford Press. Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human
Langsdorf, P., Izard, C. E., Rayias, M., & Hembree, E. A. (1983). and animal emotions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Interest expression, visual fixation, and heart rate changes in Piotrowski, J. T., Litman, J. A., & Valkenburg, P. (2014).
2- to 8-month old infants. Developmental Psychology, 19, Measuring epistemic curiosity in young children. Infant and
375–386. Child Development, 23, 542–553.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Prenzel, M. (1992). The selective persistence of interest. In
Oxford University Press. K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp. (Eds.), The role of
Leuba, C. (1955). Toward some integration of learning theories: interest in learning and development (pp. 71–98). Hillsdale,
The concept of optimal stimulation. Psychological Reports, 1, NJ: Erlbaum.
27–33. Rawlings, D. (2003). Personality correlates of liking for
Litman, J. A. (2005). Curiosity and the pleasures of learning: “unpleasant” paintings and photographs. Personality and
Wanting and liking new information. Cognition and Emotion, Individual Differences, 23, 395–410.
19, 793–814. Rawlings, D., Twomey, F., Burns, E., & Morris, S. (2002).
Litman, J. A., Hutchins, T. L., & Russon, R. K. (2005). Personality, creativity, and aesthetic preference: Comparing
Epistemic curiosity, feeling-of-knowing, and exploratory psychoticism, sensation seeking, schizotypy, and Openness
behavior. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 559–582. to Experience. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 16, 153–178.
Litman, J. A., & Jimerson, T. L. (2004). The measurement of Reeve, J. (1993). The face of interest. Motivation and Emotion, 17,
curiosity as a feeling of deprivation. Journal of Personality 353–375.
Assessment, 82, 147–157. Renner, B. (2006). Curiosity about people: The development of
Litman, J. A., Robinson, O. C., & Demetre, J. D. (2017). a social curiosity measure in adults. Journal of Personality
Intrapersonal curiosity: Inquisitiveness about the inner self. Assessment, 87, 305–316.
Self and Identity, 16, 231–250. Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2003). The do re mi’s of
Litman, J. A., & Silvia, P. J. (2006). The latent structure of trait everyday life: The structure and personality correlates of
curiosity: Evidence for interest and deprivation curiosity music preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
dimensions. Journal of Personality Assessment, 86, 318–328. 84, 1236–1256.
Litman, J. A., & Spielberger, C. D. (2003). Measuring epistemic Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2001). Imagery and text: A dual coding
curiosity and its diversive and specific components. Journal of theory of reading and writing. Mahwah, NJ.: Erlbaum.
Personality Assessment, 80, 75–86. Sansone, C., & Thoman, D. B. (2005). Interest as the missing
Loewenstein, G. (1994). The psychology of curiosity: A review motivator in self-regulation. European Psychologist, 10, 175–186.
and reinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 75–98. Sauter, D. A., & Scott, S. K. (2007). More than one kind of
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and Openness happiness: Can we recognize vocal expressions of different
to Experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, positive states? Motivation and Emotion, 31, 192–199.
1258–1265. Schiefele, U. (2009). Situational and individual interest. In
McCrae, R. R. (1994). Openness to Experience: Expanding K. R. Wenzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation
the boundaries of Factor V. European Journal of Personality, 8, at school (pp. 197–222). New York, NY: Routledge.
251–272. Silvia, P. J. (2005). What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal
McCrae, R. R. (2007). Aesthetic chills as a universal marker of structure of interest. Emotion, 5, 89–102.
Openness to Experience. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 5–11. Silvia, P. J. (2006). Exploring the psychology of interest. New York,
McCrae, R. R., & Sutin, A. R. (2009). Openness to Experience. NY: Oxford University Press.
In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual Silvia, P. J. (2007). Knowledge-based assessment of expertise in
differences in social behavior (pp. 257–273). New York, NY: the arts: Exploring aesthetic fluency. Psychology of Aesthetics,
Guilford Press. Creativity, and the Arts, 1, 247–249.
McDougall, W. (1960). An introduction to social psychology. Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest—The curious emotion. Current
London, UK: Methuen. (Original work published 1908) Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 57–60.
Montgomery, K. C. (1951). The relationship between Silvia, P. J. (2010). Confusion and interest: The role of knowledge
exploratory behavior and spontaneous alternation in the emotions in aesthetic experience. Psychology of Aesthetics,
white rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 75–80.
44, 582–589. Silvia, P. J. (2012). Human emotions and aesthetic experience:
Montgomery, K. C. (1952). A test of two explanations An overview of empirical aesthetics. In A. P. Shimamura &
of spontaneous alternation. Journal of Comparative and S. E. Palmer (Eds.), Aesthetic science: Connecting minds,
Physiological Psychology, 45, 287–293. brains, and experience (pp. 250–275). New York, NY: Oxford
Murray, J. C. (1904). An introduction to psychology based on the University Press.
author’s handbook of psychology. Boston, MA: Little, Brown. Silvia, P. J. (2017). Curiosity. In P. A. O’Keefe & Judith
Myers, A. K., & Miller, N. E. (1954). Failure to find a learned Harackiewicz (Eds.), The science of interest (pp. 97–107).
drive based on hunger; evidence for learning motivated by Zurich: Springer.
SILVIA 167
CH A PT ER
10 Flow
The Experience of Intrinsic Motivation
Abstract
Flow is an optimal psychological state characterized by the enjoyment of deep absorption in what
one is doing. This psychological state is autotelic (i.e., rewarding in itself ); experiencing flow
intrinsically motivates individuals to engage in activities that are conducive to it. Research on the
flow experience has shed light on the phenomenology of intrinsic motivation since Csikszentmihalyi
(1975) first introduced the flow concept. This chapter (a) describes the dimensions and conditions
of the flow experience, (b) reviews research on its psychological covariates, (c) highlights conceptual
and operational differences among four flow-related constructs, (d) discusses theory and research
on the temporal dynamics of flow experience, and (e) summarizes research on the neurophysiology
of the flow state.
Psychologists have had an abiding interest in what in what they are doing. The flow state—“the holistic
motivates people to act when moved neither by sensation that people feel when they act with total
biological needs such as hunger nor by external involvement” (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975, p. 4)—has
rewards or punishments (Ryan & Deci, 2000;
been described as optimal experience because of
Sansone & Harackiewicz, 2000). Intrinsic motiva- the positive conjunction of cognition, affect, and
tion, the impetus to act that lies within the activity motivation that characterizes it (Csikszentmihalyi
itself, typically is studied in tandem with, and & Csikszentmihalyi, 1988; Delle Fave, Massimini,
counterpoint to, extrinsic motivation. Some efforts & Bassi, 2011) and as autotelic experience because
to understand it have focused on drivers within the the goal of action lies within the activity itself
person: curiosity, interest, an instinct to play, or a need (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975).
to feel competent. As a way of approaching intrinsic In this chapter, we focus on the experience (vs.
motivation, the flow model has been distinguished antecedents, outcomes, or applications) of flow. We
by its focus instead on phenomenology—what the summarize theory and research about the dimensions,
person is experiencing while engaged in an activity conditions, and covariates of flow experience and
for its own sake. Understanding intrinsic motivation discuss conceptual and operational distinctions
phenomenologically focuses attention on the ever- among several flow-related constructs. Then, we
shifting quality of present experience; understanding turn to what we see as two notable directions for
the quality of experience requires understanding theory and research on the experience of flow. The
the dynamic interaction between person and envi- first concerns the temporal dynamics of flow expe-
ronment from which this experience arises. Research riences, including the phenomenon of emergent
begun in the 1970s suggests that activity is intrinsically motivation. The second concerns the neuroscience
motivating when people enjoy focused immersion and physiology of the flow state.
169
Dimensions of the Flow State flow state is characterized by a heightened sense of
Drawing inferences from extensive interviews with control.
musicians, rock climbers, chess players, and others,
Csikszentmihalyi (1975, 1990) has summarized the Heightened Sense of Control
following dimensions of the flow state: (a) merging People in the flow state experience a sense of control
of action and awareness, (b) complete concentration, over their actions and the immediate environment.
(c) heightened sense of control, (d) loss of self- This sense of control includes feeling that one will
consciousness, (e) distortion of temporal perception, know how to respond to changes in the environment
and (f) autotelic experience. These characteristics are as well as embracing the challenges that come into the
crucial to the development of flow theory and attentional field (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975). While it
measurement of flow (e.g., Engeser & Rheinberg, is impossible to feel control over every aspect of life,
2008; Jackson & Eklund, 2002). humans strive to fulfill a desire to achieve behavior–
event contingencies (Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995).
Merging of Action and Awareness In part because people in the flow state experience a
Often in life, people are doing the activity at hand heightened sense of control over their actions and
without attending to it. Common, routine activities the environment, afterward they may look back and
such as commuting and household chores are par- say that the activity was worth doing for its own
ticularly susceptible to the detachment of action and sake—autotelic (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975).
awareness (Hektner, Schmidt, & Csikszentmihalyi,
2007). In contrast, people often report flow activities Autotelic Experience
are absorbing because their attention is devoted to the Autotelic experience refers to experience that is
activity at hand and nothing else––their concentra- rewarding in itself. No external rewards are needed
tion is complete. to motivate the activity at hand. Note that the
existence of an external reward does not disqualify
Complete Concentration on Task at Hand an activity from being autotelic. As a widely cited
Attention is a limited cognitive resource, and multiple example, a surgeon interviewed by Csikszentmihalyi
sources are competing for attention at every moment. (1975) reported that his immersion, concentration,
People in the flow state process only stimuli that are and enjoyment during a surgery were more moti-
relevant to the task at hand, filtering irrelevant stimuli vating than the prestige and salary the job brought.
out (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975). Concentration is so In fact, the surgeon described his experience in one
intense that it absorbs the person’s full attention. In operation as so enjoyable and absorbing that he was
other words, attention is not divided and spent on unaware of the time and even of debris falling from
distracting activities—or on worrying about how the ceiling of the operating room when part of it
one is being evaluated by others. collapsed during the operation (Csikszentmihalyi,
1975, p .133). Although it may be uncommon to
Loss of Self-Consciousness remain unaware if the ceiling collapses, people in
People experiencing flow report that they are so the flow state often are unaware of the passage of
concentrated they lose awareness of the self and are time—temporal perception is distorted.
freed from self-evaluations made by themselves and
others. An implication is that people may not expe- Distortion of Temporal Perception
rience happiness while in the flow state because the The proverbs “time flies when you are having fun”
pleasant experience requires attention to their own and “happiness takes no account of time” are per-
inner states, which complete concentration on the haps good descriptions of this dimension of the flow
task at hand makes impossible. Instead, happiness state—distortion of temporal perception. When
and a sense of accomplishment and contentment people are completely absorbed and concentrated in
are only experienced in retrospect (Csikszentmihalyi, an enjoyable activity, they are more susceptible to
1975, 1997). Csikszentmihalyi (1990) also postu- losing track of time. Some theorists describe this
lated that people preoccupied with the self, such as aspect of the flow state as potentially harmful, par-
those high in narcissism, are less likely to experience ticularly if the involvement in the activity impacts
flow because their attention is constantly divided the quality of other aspects of life negatively (see
between themselves and the task. He also noted that Schüler, 2012, for a discussion of negative conse-
the loss of self-consciousness does not imply loss of quences associated with different dimensions of
control of a person’s body and action. Rather, the flow experience). It is noteworthy that time does
Challenges
Arousal
Anxiety Flow
Anxiety
Anxiety Flow
Challenges
Challenges
average
Fl
Boredom/
Apathy
Boredom Relaxation
Apathy Relaxation
Boredom
Skills Skills
The Experience
The Original Model The Quadrant Model Fluctuation Model
Figure 10.1. The relationship between challenge–skill levels and experiential outcomes as depicted in three models.
The original model and experience fluctuation model are adapted by permission of Oxford University Press from pp. 94 and 95 of “The Concept of
Flow,” Jeanne Nakamura and Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi, Handbook of Positive Psychology (2002; C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez, Eds.). The quadrant model is
adapted from Figure 15.4, p. 251 of Optimal Experience (1988; M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. Csikszentmihalyi, Eds., Cambridge University Press).
Merging of
Loss of self-
action and
consciousness
awareness
Challenge-skill
balance
Complete Distortion of
concentration Immediate Clear time perception
feedback goal
Heightened
Autotelic
sense of
experience
control
Figure 10.2 The relationships among conditions and dimensions of flow experience as depicted in the condition–experience model.
Abstract
This chapter reviews the literature on congruence between implicit (unconscious) and explicit (conscious)
motives. The conventional wisdom that implicit and explicit motives are statistically independent is shown
to be incorrect. Meta-analyses of past studies indicate that, on average, implicit and explicit motives
are weakly positively correlated rather than uncorrelated. The correlation becomes stronger when
methodological shortcomings of past research, such as unreliability of measurement, are overcome.
Nevertheless, the relation remains modest enough that the discrepancy between implicit and explicit
motives carries important information about personality congruence. The relation between implicit and
explicit motives has been found to vary systematically as a function of substantive moderator variables,
such as self-determination, self-monitoring, and body awareness. Motive congruence is predicted distally
by satisfaction of basic needs during childhood and proximally by stress among individuals who have
difficulty regulating affect. Motive congruence predicts important outcomes, including volitional strength,
flow, well-being, healthy eating, and relationship stability. The chapter closes with a discussion of future
research directions, such as the distinction between congruence and integration constructs.
Keywords: motive congruence, incongruence, implicit motives, explicit motives, moderators,
self-determination, self-monitoring, well-being, personality, motivation
187
c onstructs. Picture Story Exercises and questionnaires personality coherence and has been posited to have
are thought to assess different kinds of motive
important implications for well-being (McClelland,
constructs that are conceptually and statistically
Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989; Weinberger &
independent. Questionnaires assess explicit motives— McClelland, 1990).
verbally encoded values that are consciously accessible. The substantive interpretation of the lack of
Picture Story Exercises assess implicit motives— correlation between PSE and questionnaire meas-
spontaneously expressed concerns or preferences ures provides an unflattering portrait of the human
that are not verbally encoded or directly accessible condition—it suggests that the conscious sense of
to consciousness. self is fundamentally divorced from underlying
An implication of this substantive interpretation unconscious motivations. However, an alternative
of statistical independence is that individuals differ possibility is that PSEs and questionnaires are
markedly in motive congruence—the extent to uncorrelated because of limitations of one or both
which an individual’s levels of implicit and explicit assessment methods. Thus, it may be the assessment
motives are aligned. For the sake of illustration, we methodology, rather than the human psyche, that
have depicted a correlation of r = .00 between im- is in disarray. Of course, these two possibilities are
plicit and explicit need for achievement (nAch) in not mutually exclusive. Our objective in this chapter
Figure 11.1, based on hypothetical data. Each data is to review the literatures on the correlation between
point represents the implicit nAch and explicit implicit and explicit motives and on substantive and
nAch scores for an individual. Some individuals— methodological factors that moderate this r elation.
those whose scores fall near the line y = x—have We also review the literatures on the antecedents
similar levels of implicit and explicit achievement and consequences of motive congruence. In the
motives; that is, the extent to which they value following, we begin by providing a more d etailed
achievement is comparable to the level of concern historical overview of traditional and contemporary
with achievement that they reveal spontaneously perspectives on the relation between PSE and ques-
through the stories they tell. These individuals tionnaire motive measures.
display a high level of motive congruence. Other
individuals—those whose scores fall far from the The Relation Between Picture
line y = x—have dissimilar levels of implicit and ex- Story Exercises and Questionnaire
plicit achievement motives; the extent to which they Measures of Motives
value achievement is much higher or lower than the In their classic book The Achievement Motive,
level of concern with achievement that they reveal McClelland et al. (1953) described a study in which
through their stories. These individuals display a their PSE measure of nAch was found to be uncor-
low level of motive congruence (or, equivalently, a related with a three-item questionnaire concerning
high level of incongruence). Motive congruence in effort to achieve. Although significant positive cor-
core life domains, such as achievement, affiliation, relations emerged in some subsequent studies (e.g.,
and power, is regarded as a fundamental indicator of deCharms, Morrison, Reitman, & McClelland,
1955), many studies failed to document a significant
relation between PSE and questionnaire measures
of nAch. Findings were similar for PSE and ques-
tionnaire measures of need for affiliation (nAff) and
Explicit nAch
Incongruent Congruent 1
McClelland and others who have argued that implicit and
explicit motives are statistically independent have sometimes
Figure 11.1 Hypothetical data in which the correlation between qualified this claim by stating that implicit and explicit motives are
implicit and explicit need for achievement (nAch) is r = .00. “generally,” “essentially,” or “largely” independent. We acknowledge
Individuals who are more congruent have data points that fall this fact but emphasize McClelland’s focus on independence
closer to the line y = x. for the following reasons: (a) McClelland appeared to use these
(D)
Motive
Questionnaire is an
PSE Que invalid method
(B)
Motive
(A)
Implicit Explicit
(C) (F)
Implicit and explicit motive
Implicit Explicit Implicit Explicit
constructs are independent
PSE Que
(G)
Implicit Explicit
To various degrees, each of
the above may contribute
PSE Que to lack of correlation
Figure 11.2 Models that have been used to explain the lack of correlation between implicit and explicit motives (third column);
models depicting the tacit assumptions that underlie the explanatory models (second column); and a general model that is relatively
free of assumptions (first column).
PSE = Picture Story Exercise; Que = questionnaire.
Explicit nAch
Explicit nAch
Implicit nAch Implicit nAch Implicit nAch
r = .74 + r = .00 r = .35
Figure 11.3 Hypothetical data showing how a relatively weak correlation may be the net result of combining two subgroups, one in
which the correlation is strong and one in which the correlation equals zero.
Abstract
This chapter focuses on interest as a cognitive and affective motivational variable that develops and can
be supported to develop. It provides an explanation of Hidi and Renninger’s (2006; Renninger & Hidi,
2016) four-phase model of interest development and its relation to other approaches to interest,
including interest conceptualized as an emotion, experience, task features, value, or vocational interest,
and considers issues pertaining to the identification and measurement of interest as a variable that
develops. Following this, the chapter reviews research that tracks interest over time as well as studies
that focus on earlier and/or later phases of interest, with particular attention to (a) the triggering of
interest in both earlier and later phases of interest, (b) maintaining interest once it has been triggered,
(c) fluctuations in interest, and (d) shifts between phases in the development of interest. Two studies
of interest development are reviewed in depth and their complementarities are described to illustrate
how consideration of study complementarity can provide validation and insight about interest
development. Finally, a Punnett square is used to demonstrate how it can enable the identification of
relations among a learner’s phase of interest, the achievement demands of the learning environment,
and metacognitive awareness, in addition to suggesting next steps for the study of interest development.
Keywords: achievement, affect, interest, interest development, knowledge, metacognition
Interest and Its Development, Revisited evelops and can be supported to develop. The
d
Interest is powerful. Its presence has been repeatedly chapter centers on aspects of development that are
demonstrated to benefit learning—it promotes not yet well understood—the conditions that sup-
attention, goal setting, and strategy use, and it is also port the development and deepening of interest
malleable.1 Moreover, interest may be supported to and their implications for both theory and practice.
develop at any age, whether a person is in or out of We begin with an analysis of an excerpt from Helen
school. In this chapter, we review research that Keller’s autobiography, The Story of My Life (Keller,
contributes to understanding interest as a cognitive 1903). Helen’s case, as it is presented in her autobi-
and affective motivational variable that both ography, can illustrate critical aspects in the devel-
opment of interest, starting with the initial triggering
1
This chapter is an updated and revised version of our 2012
of interest through to the point where she asks
chapter, written for the first edition of the Oxford Handbook of questions, reflects on them, and voluntarily and
Motivation. We draw on and extend points discussed in independently follows through to seek answers and
Renninger and Hidi’s (2016) volume, The Power of Interest feedback.
for Motivation and Engagement. Whereas that volume provides Helen Keller was the first blind person to receive
detail about interest, its development, and implications for
research and practice, here we focus on issues and open
a bachelor’s degree. She became a world-famous
questions central to further clarifying present understanding of activist, wrote books about her experience and b eliefs,
interest development. and is now widely considered one of the most
205
inspirational people of the 20th century. Through made a connection between the finger spelling and
the support of her tutor, Anne Sullivan, Helen the water and then engaged the challenge of revisit-
“discovered” language, communication, and society. ing the prior lessons that had been so frustrating to
Helen’s interest development is paraphrased briefly her. She did not simply decide to be interested in
below: communication. Rather, it seems that she needed
to encounter the connection to communicate, and
Rendered both deaf and blind at a young age, Helen it was the connection that triggered her eventual
stumbled around like a feral animal for many years. interest in communication more generally.
The adults around her were unable to reach or tame Second, Helen was not aware that she was devel-
her, pitying her and letting her do anything she oping an interest as her tutor worked with her. The
wanted. When Anne Sullivan, a young and potential triggers of finger spelling did not “take”
financially strapped tutor, was hired to help Helen, until the incident with the water. Even at that point,
she found a bright but horribly spoiled 7-year-old it is not clear that she would have described finger
girl who was unable to see the implications of her spelling, or communication more generally, as
own behavior and its effect on other people. Anne something in which she was personally invested and
did not approve of the way that Helen grabbed food that would hold her interest. However, her knowl-
from various people’s dinner plates and broke things edge and valuing of the possibility of communicat-
during temper tantrums. Anne disciplined, and ing using finger spelling were increasing.
Helen fought back both physically and with pranks. Third, Helen’s interest developed in a context
Anne recognized that Helen was bright and decided where her strengths and needs were accounted for
to teach her how to finger spell, thinking that this and she was not being graded or assessed: Anne
might help her to communicate with others. Anne worked with her so that she would understand and
would put an object in one of Helen’s hands, and in be able to think and explore. Once her interest
the other quickly spell the name for the object. Even began to develop, Helen was extremely successful by
though Helen could imitate well, she did not any number of measures.
understand what Anne was trying to teach her. Her Fourth, Helen’s interest continued to develop
patience ran out quickly, and the lessons would end because, once she made the connection between
in tears and yelling. finger spelling and communicating, she then wanted
Everything changed one day when Anne pumped to seek information. This led her to continue to
water into Helen’s hands and spelled “water.” The stretch her own understanding.
event appeared to trigger Helen to make a Fifth, once she began seeking information, Helen
connection between the fluttering movement in her began to self-regulate and to explore and seize oppor-
hand and the cold liquid spilling over her skin. tunities to learn—opportunities that were ostensibly
Suddenly, Helen realized what Anne had been trying present before, but that she may not have been able
to show her as she had doggedly spelled word after to recognize without external support.
word into her hand all those weeks. From then on, It appears that it was not until Helen made a con-
Anne could hardly keep up with Helen, who dragged nection between finger spelling and communication
her around demanding a word for everything she that she began to pose her own questions, seek an-
encountered, everything that had been there before. swers, and reflect—a point when her interest in com-
munication was clearly developing. As Helen’s case
Anne’s efforts to help Helen make connections reveals, the development of interest has phases that
between signs and what they represent could be de- precede what to the outside observer might be readily
scribed as potential triggers for interest. We do not identified as interest. In fact, her interest continues to
know why or how the trigger of the water served as develop beyond the phase that is detailed here.
a catalyst. In fact, Helen thought at first that it was
some kind of game. It seems likely that many factors Defining Interest and Interest
contributed to her revelation. Development
We know, however, that a few elements of Helen’s Interest refers both to the psychological state of learn-
story are particularly important to the description ers during their engagement with particular content
and understanding of interest development. First, (e.g., communication, mathematics, basketball)
the development of her interest involved seemingly and to their motivation to continue to reengage
repetitive and ineffective external support before she that content over time. First, we summarize the
Definition • Psychological state resulting from • Psychological state that involves • Psychological state and the beginning • Psychological state and a relatively
short-term changes in cognitive focused attention to a particular of relatively enduring predisposition enduring predisposition to reengage
and affective processing associated class of content that reoccurs to seek reengagement with a particular a particular class of content over time
with a particular class of content and/or persists over time class of content over time
Learner • Attends to content, if only • Reengages content that previously • Is likely to independently reengage • Independently reengages content
characteristics fleetingly triggered attention content • Has both stored knowledge and value
• May or may not be reflectively • Is developing knowledge of content • Has both stored knowledge and • Is reflective about the content
aware of the experience • Is developing a sense of the stored value • Is likely to recognize others’
• May need support to engage from content’s value • Is reflective about the content contributions to the discipline
others and through instructional • Is likely to be able to be supported • Is focused on his or her own • Self-regulates easily to reframe
design by others to find connections to questions questions and seek answers
• May experience either positive content based on existing skills, • Has positive feelings • Has positive feelings
or negative feelings knowledge, and/or prior experience • May not persevere when confronted • Can persevere through frustration
• May not persevere when with • Is likely to have positive feelings with difficulty and challenge to meet goals
confronted with difficulty • May not persevere when with • May not want feedback from others • Appreciates and may actively seek
• May simply want to be told confronted with difficulty feedback
what to do • May want to be told what to do
From The Power of Interest for Motivation and Engagement by K. A. Renninger & S. E. Hidi, 2016, Table 1.2, p. 13. Copyright by Taylor and Francis, reprinted with permission.
and the tools that they have created (e.g., books, tasks, feel they understand what is expected of them and
software, exhibits; Renninger & Hidi, 2016, 2019). have a teacher who is responsive and provides sup-
Empirical support for the four-phase model has port for autonomy (see related discussions in Frenzel,
been provided by studies conducted across a wide Goetz, Pekrun, & Watt, 2010; Renninger, Kensey,
range of domains and contexts, with learners who Stevens, & Lehman, 2015; Tsai, Kunter, Lüdtke,
vary in age (e.g., Harackiewicz, Durik, Barron, Trautwein, & Ryan, 2008). The stage fit of the person
Linnenbrink, & Tauer, 2008; Lipstein & Renninger, to the environment has been described as supporting
2007; Michaelis & Nathan, 2015; Wang & Adesope, feelings about the worth (the value, task interest,
2016). Furthermore, developments in affective neu- utility, cost) of continued engagement (e.g., Wigfield,
roscience indicate that the triggering of interest is Eccles, Schiefele, Roeser, & Davis-Kean, 2006).
associated with activation of the reward circuitry in Whether a person is able to make an independent
the brain (e.g., Gottlieb, Oudeyer, Lopes, & Baranes, decision to reengage has also been found to affect the
2013; Panksepp, 1998; see discussion in Renninger & relation between the affective and cognitive compo-
Hidi, 2016). In other words, all persons are h ardwired nents of interest, a relation that affects the experience
to develop interest, and interest in any content may of interest (Ainley, 2007; Sansone & Thoman, 2005a,
be supported to develop. 2005b) as well as the likelihood that interest will
However, the research also indicates that whether develop and deepen (Renninger, 2000).
interest, once triggered, is supported to develop Although learners at all ages with varying expe-
depends on whether the task leads learners to find riences may develop new interests at any time, age
meaning in it, enabling them to make connection also affects how and whether interest is likely to
to their knowledge and value for the content. Some develop. Undergraduates, for example, may be able
examples include the meaning Helen found in to self-generate ways in which to sustain interest in
finger signing, meaning that students may be view of a task that they find boring by finding some
encouraged to identify for themselves in a course reason that the task could be beneficial to them
they are taking (Hulleman, Durik, Schweigert, & (e.g., Sansone et al., 1992). This capacity is related
Harackiewicz, 2008; Hulleman & Harackiewicz, to their awareness of how they are thinking and
2009), or meaning that emerges when learners are learning in the situation (a boring task that needs to
allowed or take charge of shaping class activities be completed, the need to pass a course that they are
(Cobb & Hodge, 2004; Meyer & Turner, 2002). taking), their metacognitive awareness, and their
The match between the strengths and needs of the ability to generate strategies to address it. Conversely,
learner and available support, described by Eccles younger children find ways to continue to engage
and Midgley (1989) as the stage fit of the environ- only if tasks are already of interest, although they
ment (see also Bronfenbrenner & Ceci’s, 1994, also may be more open than older learners to trying
discussion of the bioecological model), is critical to to learn new topics or participate in new activities
the development of interest. When support from (Renninger, Sansone, & Smith, 2004). At about 8
the learning environment is lacking (or perceived to to 10 years of age, they begin comparing their own
be lacking), interest can fall off, go dormant, or dis- capacities to those of others and then need a differ-
appear altogether (Bergin, 1999). Renninger (2000), ent form of support to persevere on tasks they have
for example, described the case of a talented chess not yet tried or tasks they are aware others already
player who ceased playing chess because there was do at a much more advanced level then they do
no one to challenge him. Renninger and Lipstein (Renninger, 2009).
(2006; see also Renninger & Hidi, 2019) also
reported that interest may decline when students Conceptualizations of Interest Not
do not perceive opportunities to connect to the Specifically Focused on Development
work they are doing and/or feel that their ideas are Understanding how interest can be supported to
respected and heard. Their findings are consistent develop is of particular concern to those who support
with those of Kunter, Baumert, and Köller (2007), others to learn, whether in or outside the school
who found that within the same classroom there context. However, the conceptualization of interest
were students whose interest would develop and as a cognitive and affective motivational variable that
students whose interest would decrease. Kunter develops is only one of the ways in which interest is
et al. observed that the development of interest is defined and studied (see extended discussion in
likely to be more related to students’ personal expe- Renninger & Hidi, 2011). Krapp (2002, 2007), for
rience of the classroom—for example, whether they example, describes interest development as a process
Figure 12.1 Punnett square of the possible relations among learner phase of interest, metacognitive abilities, and achievement
demands of the learning environment.
13 Achievement Goals
Abstract
This chapter describes the achievement goal construct’s origins and highlights noteworthy
developments in the literature. Specifically it describes how the original dichotomous model of
achievement goals developed into the modern trichotomous, 2 × 2, and 3 × 2 models of achievement
goals. This historical overview is then used to provide the context for the key theoretical and
conceptual issues surrounding the current achievement goal approach and a discussion of how these
issues have an impact on empirical studies on achievement goals, especially focusing on the
measurement of achievement goals. Finally, the text highlights several emerging lines of research in the
literature. This includes achievement goal complexes, expansion of the competence-based model of
achievement goals (e.g., potential-based achievement goals), and possible methodological improvement
in assessing achievement goals.
Keywords: achievement motivation, achievement goals, approach–avoidance, competence, theory of
intelligence
229
to accurately understand the achievement goal Elliott, 1983; Nicholls, 1984): It distinguishes b etween
construct and the nature of current theoretical and two different classes of achievement goals (i.e.,
empirical issues. After discussing these key issues, dichotomous model ). For example, Dweck proposed
we then turn to several emerging and promising lines the distinction between learning goals and performance
of research (e.g., goal complexes) for future work on goals. Nicholls put forth the distinction between task
achievement goals.1 involvement and ego involvement. Others proposed
similar distinctions (e.g., competitive vs. individual-
Historical Overview istic goal structures discussed by Ames), but we will
The achievement goal construct is the product of focus on Dweck’s and Nicholl’s d istinctions because
several different lines of research that were conducted they particularly influenced the development of the
by different researchers influencing each other. In achievement goal construct.
other words, unlike typical theories in psychology,
the achievement goal construct does not have a Dweck’s Conceptualization
single theoretical origin but emerged as the mixture Dweck conceived the achievement goal construct
of different lines of thought. Indeed, these different through her earlier work that examined children’s
lines of research initially did not even use the term behavior and verbal reports in response to task feed-
achievement goals. In this sense, it may not be back (Diener & Dweck, 1978, 1980; Dweck, 1975;
appropriate to call the framework of achievement Dweck & Reppucci, 1973). These studies found
goals achievement goal theory; rather, it is best termed that children can be classified into two different
theories of achievement goals or the achievement goal types according to how they respond to the experi-
approach (Elliot, 2005). ence of failure feedback: Helpless children, who
exhibit a pattern of maladaptive responses such as
Dichotomous Model decrements in expectancies, persistence and perfor-
The original contributors of the achievement goal mance, increased negative affect, and the attribution
construct include Carole Ames, Carol Dweck, of failure to the lack of ability, and mastery children,
Marty Maehr, and John Nicholls; they worked on who exhibit an adaptive and persistent pattern of
independent research programs at the University of responses such as enhanced expectancies, persistence,
Illinois. In the fall of 1977, they started regular sem- and performance, increased positive affect, and the
inars on achievement motivation at the Institute for attribution of failure to the lack of effort. Dweck
Child Behavior and Development in the Children’s was interested in identifying the underlying root of
Research Center (Elliot, 2005; Roberts, 2001). these distinct response patterns because the causal
These meetings produced a series of papers (Maehr attribution and achievement motive approaches,
& Nicholls, 1980; Nicholls & Dweck, 1979), and it which were the predominant traditions at the time,
is these papers that provided the initial, founda- seemed unable to explain such individual differences.
tional ideas of the construct of achievement goals. For example, causal attribution theory indicates
It should be noted that research on achievement that children would experience decreased feelings of
motivation in the 1970s was heavily influenced competence (expectancy) and self-esteem when they
by the causal attribution tradition (Weiner, 1985; attribute their failure to an internal and stable cause
Weiner, Heckhausen, & Meyer, 1972; Weiner & (i.e., ability), but there was not much work that
Kukla, 1970) and the achievement motive tradition examined what factors contributed to this maladap-
(Atkinson & Raynor, 1978; McClelland, Atkinson, tive attributional pattern. In contrast, achievement
Clark, & Lowell, 1976). At that moment, the motive research focused on individual differences in
researchers noticed some weaknesses and limitations achievement settings but overemphasized (relatively
of these predominant theoretical frameworks, and stable) dispositions and tended to overlook the role
their theoretical developments on achievement goals of the cognitions and beliefs that people have in
construct can be viewed as an attempt, in part, to achievement situations (Dweck & Wortman, 1982).
address these limitations (Murayama, 2003). It is in that context that Dweck focused on the
Early work on the achievement goal construct role of goals to explain the differential response pat-
has a common feature (e.g., Ames, 1981; Dweck & terns of children (i.e., helpless pattern and mastery
pattern) in response to a failure experience (Dweck,
1
This chapter draws on basic structure and content from the
1986, 1999; Dweck & Leggett, 1988). Dweck pro-
first edition (Murayama, Elliot, & Friedman, 2012), but includes posed that two different types of goals, leaning goals
revised and new material, as well as original material. and performance goals, are the critical factors that
14 Goal Attainment
Abstract
This chapter begins with a description of how the goal concept emerged in the history of the
psychology of motivation to better understand the important role it plays in current research on
motivation. The chapter then turns to the self-regulation of goal pursuit. The effects and underlying
processes of two different self-regulation strategies will be discussed in detail: mental contrasting and
forming implementation intentions. The chapter concludes with a report of the results of recent
intervention studies that combine the self-regulation strategies of mental contrasting and forming
implementation intentions to help people enhance goal attainment in the health, academic, and
interpersonal domains.
Keywords: goals, goal contents and framing, mental contrasting, fantasies, obstacles, implementation
intentions, self-regulation strategies, self-control, willpower, behavior change interventions
It is Friday afternoon. On Monday, you must give an Based on learning theory advanced by early
important presentation. Even though you are highly animal psychologists (Hull, 1943; Spence, 1956),
motivated to do an excellent job (i.e., desirability the strength of the tendency to make a response was
and feasibility are high), you did not find the time at first considered a function of an organism’s skills
to prepare during the week. So you set yourself a (or habit strength), its needs, and the incentive
goal to use the weekend to develop an impressive value of the desired outcome. For example, how fast
presentation. But how do you attain this goal? an animal runs toward a box containing food
depends on its habit strength, its need for food
Goals Versus Motivation (expressed in hunger), and the quality and quantity
The term motivation is commonly used to explain of the food. With the advance of the cognitive revo-
why a person in a given situation selects one lution in psychology, these determinants of motiva-
response over another or makes a given response tion, as well as the concept of motivation itself,
with great energization or frequency. Imagine a became more elaborated. Tolman (1932) postulated
person looking for someone else in a crowd. She various mental processes that “intermediate in the
gets excited when she finds that person, and then she causal equation between environmental stimuli
runs toward her. Each of these responses involves and . . . overt behavior” (p. 2). These intermediate
motivation, which can manifest itself cognitively processes entailed concepts of purpose (ends
(e.g., looking), affectively (e.g., excitement), and and means) as well as expectations (e.g., means
behaviorally (e.g., running). To the question of expectations, end expectations, and means–end
what drives motivation, the history of the psychology expectations). A few years later, Festinger (1942)
of motivation has offered ever more sophisticated and Atkinson (1957) drew on that work in their
answers. research on what motivates humans to select and
247
perform tasks of varying difficulty. They suggested will choose either a very easy or a very difficult task,
that people weigh the incentive value of the d esired which is an effective strategy to avoid shame (because
outcome with the expectancy that it would actu- very easy tasks are likely to be solved and failure on
ally occur. too-difficult tasks can easily be explained by external
Social cognitive learning theorists (e.g., Bandura, factors). Finally, researchers have differentiated
1977) went a step further, factoring in whether one among types of incentives as well (Heckhausen,
feels confident to successfully perform the neces- 1977). For instance, in the realm of achievement,
sary behavior required to arrive at a desired out- anticipation of positive self-evaluations (e.g., “I will
come (efficacy or control beliefs). These theorists do really well!”), positive evaluations by others
also alluded to further relevant expectancies, such (e.g., praise by the teacher or parents), higher order
as whether the situation by itself would produce positive consequences (e.g., successful professional
the desired outcome (Heckhausen, 1977; Mischel, career), and consequences that go beyond achieve-
1973), whether performing a given behavior would ment (e.g., having a good time with coworkers) can
indeed lead to the desired outcome (Bandura, 1977), all motivate people to do well on given tasks.
whether achieving the desired outcome would be Given this differentiation in thinking about the
instrumental to accruing further positive conse- determinants of motivation (i.e., needs, incentives,
quences down the road (Vroom, 1964), whether the and expectancies), one may wonder whether the
desired outcome could be attained (Oettingen, concept of goals is at all needed. In our opinion,
1996), and whether the future in general would be the concept of goals helps the explication of the read-
bright (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; iness to make a certain response. Ajzen and Fishbein
Scheier & Carver, 1987). (1969) suggested that this readiness should be assessed
Adding these various expectancy-related variables in terms of a person’s intention to make the response.
helped to explicate in more detail the can aspect (or Mischel (1973) went a step further and argued that
feasibility aspect) of the motivation to make a cer- such intentions can be conceived as self-imposed or
tain response: Can the desired outcome be brought assigned goals that imply standards that the person
about? But the cognitive revolution also helped intends to meet (with respect to quality and quantity
to explicate the want aspect (or desirability aspect) criteria). Doing so allows asking new questions, such
of the motivation to make a certain response: Do as how people arrive at goal attainment.
I really want the desired outcome? This desirability Extensive research has shown (summary by
issue was originally captured by Hull (1943) and Gollwitzer & Oettingen, 2012) that goal content and
Spence (1956) as the concept of need and the goal framing affect the likelihood of goal attainment.
concept of incentive. With respect to need, the With respect to goal content, the perceived desira-
cognitively
inspired psychology of motivation bility and feasibility of the goal matter. Perceived
ventured into the concept of motives (for a summary, desirability is high when the goal is in line with the
see McClelland, 1985a), defined as the class of person’s needs (e.g., needs for autonomy, compe-
incentives that a person finds attractive (e.g., achieve- tence, and social integration; Ryan, Sheldon, Kasser,
ment, power, affiliation, intimacy). McClelland & Deci, 1996), wishes or fantasies (Oettingen &
(1985b) discovered that depending on whether this Mayer, 2002), possible selves (Oyserman, Bybee, &
preference for certain classes of incentives was Terry, 2006), higher order goals (e.g., identity
measured implicitly (as assessed by the Thematic goals; Gollwitzer & Kirchhof, 1998), and attitudes
Apperception Test) versus explicitly (as assessed by (i.e., the expected value of achieving the goal at
attitude questionnaires), it predicts the execution hand; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980). But perceived feasi-
of different types of motive-related responses: bility also matters (Bandura, 1997). When people feel
actions people spontaneously engage in versus
that they can perform the responses that produce the
decisions people make after thoughtful deliberation. desired goal, they are said to have strong self-efficacy
Researchers also found that whether an incen- beliefs (Bandura, 1997; or control beliefs as referred
tive is hoped for versus feared matters. For instance, to by Ajzen, 1991), which promotes high goal com-
a person with a strong achievement motive, long- mitment and successful goal pursuit.
ing for the pride associated with success, will choose Relevant structural features of one’s goals refer to
a task of medium difficulty to pursue; this level of many different aspects. First, goal striving is said to
difficulty provides the most information about one’s depend on whether the aspiration or standard that
achievement potential. However, a person who abhors is specified in the goal is challenging and specific
the shame associated with failure (Atkinson, 1958) (Locke & Latham, 2002, 2006). Framing of the
Abstract
Human behavior is directed at goals. Although goal pursuit is traditionally regarded as an endeavor that
requires conscious awareness, experimental evidence in psychology suggests that human goal pursuit
can originate and unfold in the unconscious. Accordingly, goal-directed behavior could be motivated
outside conscious awareness in the current situation or environment. This chapter reviews past and
current research examining the evidence for such unconscious motivation of goal-directed behavior.
The review is organized around two themes. The first theme deals with research that analyzes goal
pursuit as automated behaviors, thereby addressing the operational function of repetition for motivated
processes in directing and controlling behavior in the absence of conscious awareness. The second
theme concerns the quest of understanding the unconscious sources of human goal pursuit and
includes a discussion of recent work on reward cueing, aimed at addressing the question of how reward
signals in the environment can motivate behavior outside awareness.
Keywords: rewards, unconscious processes, motivation, regulation, priming
Any meaningful behavior humans engage in is goal In the current chapter, we aim to clarify how
directed. Humans do not behave randomly; they goal pursuit might emerge largely outside aware-
behave to realize specific states or outcomes they ness. We will depart from the literature on habits
find desirable. From taking a walk in the park to and ideomotor theory, arguing that stimuli in the
buying groceries or making coffee, they pursue out- environment can activate outcome representations
comes, which requires keeping an eye on the prize, that, in turn, can trigger the associated actions that
choosing the right courses of action, and monitor- have produced these outcomes in the past. However,
ing their progress. Although conscious processes we assume that for goal pursuit to be supported by
play an important role all the way from planning to the effort it requires, rewarding properties of the
the execution of behavior, this role may decrease as outcome play a crucial role. We will review recent
individuals plan and execute particular goal-directed work that investigates how such motivational
actions repeatedly in the same context. Most people properties can motivate behavior without much
make their morning coffee absentmindedly, ponder- conscious intervention. We believe that a more
ing what the day is going to bring, instead of engaging thorough understanding of how goal pursuit may
in careful deliberation or planning the process of operate under the radar of conscious awareness is
coffee making. Consciousness, then, may drop out beneficial for understanding and intervening in
of the equation for such well-rehearsed goal-directed human behavior because the majority of the goals
behaviors. Although such examples are numerous, it we pursue day to day are repetitive in nature, in
is less clear how goal pursuit is possible without terms of how we aim to attain them as well as the
much assistance of consciousness. context in which we do so.
269
Goal Pursuit Without Awareness: Some to build on evolutionarily old systems (midbrain
Preliminary Thoughts and Findings structures) that appear to develop in a preverbal stage
Modern theories of goals consider goal pursuit mainly and are guided by what is pleasant and aversive during
a conscious affair: In the event of a challenge or socialization experiences. Self-attributed motives, that
opportunity, we compare potential courses of actions is, motives that a person can express and report on
to determine which one to pursue to produce the de- explicitly, are thought to depend on evolutionarily
sired outcome, mainly based on the expected value of more recent systems (cortical structures) that develop
the outcome that motivates the pursuit (Deci & Ryan, later in childhood and are sensitive to language via
1985; Keeney & Raiffa, 1976). We then deliberate and verbal commands from others, self-instructions, and
select the means that will produce the outcome and explicit knowledge about norms and values. As a
monitor the progress toward the goal as we engage in result of the early, nonverbal way in which they are
them (Gollwitzer, 1990). However, several lines of acquired, implicit motives tend to develop independ-
research suggest that d ifferent aspects of goal pursuit ently from conscious awareness and hence are difficult
may operate fairly automatically, without much to articulate. Self-attributed motives, in contrast, are
thought and under the radar of conscious awareness. suggested to rely on consciousness and are therefore
First, there is a rich literature on judgment and readily accessible to verbal reports. Accordingly, im-
decision-making suggesting that affect plays a key plicit motives must be assessed indirectly, for example,
role in the formation of attitudes, expected value, and with projective instruments such as the Thematic
choice, often bypassing deliberative processes. Such Apperception Test (see Schultheiss & Brunstein,
an affect-driven influence on human behavior has 2001). Self-attributed motives can be measured
been demonstrated in several research programs. For directly with instruments that rely on the capacity for
instance, people can implicitly form attitudes on the introspection, such as self-report questionnaires.
basis of simple evaluative conditioning p rocedures Research on implicit motives largely focuses
in which neutral stimuli (CS) are linked to affective on three main social needs, namely achievement
stimuli (UCS), sometimes without being aware of (i.e., the desire to prosper and gain success), power
the conditioning process (de Houwer, Thomas, & (i.e., the desire to influence and control others), and
Baeyens, 2001; Hofmann, de Houwer, Perugini, affiliation (i.e., the desire for friendly social interac-
Baeyens, & Crombez, 2010). Furthermore, studies tions). Once these motives are established, they orient,
employing decision-making tasks suggest that judg- select, and energize behavior (McClelland, 1985).
ment and choice are nearly impossible without emo- Indeed, several studies reveal that people’s b ehavior
tional processes. Human subjects have been shown to can be reliably predicted by achievement, affiliation,
approach decision options and avoid others based on and power motives that are measured by the Thematic
their bodily sensations and feelings that accompanied Apperception Test or alternative projection measures
the decision-making process, an effect that does not (for an overview of different measures of implicit
seem to rely on consciousness and has been dubbed motives, see Schultheiss & Pang, 2007) and this
the somatic marker hypothesis (Damasio, 1994). predictive value does not n ecessarily correspond with
A nice illustration of this hypothesis pertains to the the predictive value of explicitly generated motives.
act of flipping a coin to decide what to do (e.g., when Finally, others have argued that such implicit
one needs a new car and one can choose between a motives can be acquired as a situational goal state.
Skoda or a Volvo). It turns out that a certain positive Situational goal states are shaped by direct experience
or negative sensation can become manifest when tails and other types of learning to act in a goal-directed
or heads determines one decision of a specific course way (e.g., I want to earn money) in a specific context
of action, indicating a preference was already present. (e.g., when I enter the office), such that after some
Second, rooted in Murray’s (1938) concept of repetition the context is able to trigger the pursuit
needs and its role in personality, a substantial of the goal at hand (Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai,
research program on social motivation suggests Barndollar, & Trötschel, 2001). Such priming e ffects
that specific patterns of preferences and decision- (effects in which the mere exposure to information
making can be driven by implicit motives. Implicit renders knowledge, such as a psychological concept,
motives are defined as motivational dispositions that ready for later use) are proposed to build on knowl-
operate outside of people’s conscious awareness and edge structures including the context, the goal itself,
are aimed at the attainment of specific classes of incen- and actions as well as opportunities that may aid
tives (McClelland, Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989; goal pursuit (Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2000, 2003;
Schultheiss et al., 2008). These motives are presumed Bargh & Gollwitzer, 1994; Kruglanski et al., 2002).
Action–Outcome Learning
Figure 15.1 Action–(desired) outcome representations However, actions can also be triggered through out-
(ADORE) framework for understanding priming effects on come representations. Such action–outcome learning
goal pursuit based on Custers and Aarts (2010). capitalizes on the human capacity of learning to
(s → o´ → r → o).
of an outcome that is associated with the response of the motor programs that produce them. Such
semantic motor resonance (Zwaan & Taylor, 2006)
Important support for such a perspective comes has also been demonstrated in functional magnetic
from the empirical observation that humans repre- resonance imaging studies where subjects were
sent their actions in terms of their observable effects exposed to action words. It was found that merely
or outcomes and establish associations between the reading words related to finger, mouth, or foot move-
outcomes and the motor programs that produce the ments produced activation in the same areas in the
outcome (s–o; Hommel et al., 2001; Jeannerod, 1997; premotor cortex that were activated when perform-
Prinz, 1997; Vallacher & Wegner, 1987). As a conse- ing the actual corresponding actions (Pulvermüller,
quence, action can follow from an ideomotor 2005). This demonstrates that reading words related
principle (James, 1890): Merely thinking about or to action–outcomes triggers the corresponding motor
activating a representation of a certain outcome (o´) programs, which provides a mechanism by which
moves and programs the human body in the service more complicated semantic information related to
Initial Low Reward is not consciously Rudimentary: VS and its Facilitation of performance
experienced immediate outputs
Full High Reward is consciously Rudimentary: VS and its Facilitation of performance;
experienced immediate outputs; higher strategic decision-making
level: MPFC, ACC, DLPFC and reflections on rewards
Note. VS = ventral striatum; MPFC = medial prefrontal cortex; ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; DLPFC = dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex. Based on Bijleveld, Custers, and Aarts (2012a).
Abstract
Personal goals guide behavior toward a desired outcome, motivate behavior over time and across
situations, provide direction and meaning, and contribute to the acquisition of skills and subjective
well-being. The adaptiveness of goals, however, might vary with dimensions such as their orientation
toward the achievement of gains, maintenance of functioning, or the avoidance of losses. We argue that
goal orientation is most adaptive when it corresponds to the availability of resources and the ubiquity
of losses. In line with this argument, younger adults show a predominant orientation toward gains,
whereas goal orientation shifts toward maintenance and avoidance of loss across adulthood. This shift
in goal orientation seems adaptive both regarding subjective well-being and engagement in goal pursuit.
A second goal dimension that has been largely overlooked in the literature is the cognitive representation
of goal pursuit primarily in terms of its means (i.e., process focus) or its ends (i.e., outcome focus).
This chapter also investigates the antecedents and consequences of goal focus. In particular, it highlights
the importance of factors related to chronological age (i.e., the availability of resources, future time
perspective, goal orientation, motivational phase) for the preference for and adaptiveness of adopting
an outcome or process focus.
Keywords: adult development, goal orientation, goal focus, means, ends, resources, time perspective
285
The Importance of Goals for Adult and (b) whether a person focuses on the outcome
Development of goal pursuit (short-term and long-term conse-
Laypeople as well as motivation researchers seem to quences) or on the process of goal pursuit (means of
agree that setting and pursuing goals has positive goal attainment) (e.g., Freund & Hennecke, 2015;
consequences. As Einstein reportedly said, “If you Sansone & Thoman, 2005; Zimmerman & Kitsantas,
want to live a happy life, tie it to a goal, not to 1997).
people or objects.” In fact, psychological research The importance of personal goals for adult
suggests that goals give life meaning and direction development has been acknowledged by different
and contribute to happiness and subjective well-being action-theoretical approaches (e.g., Brandtstädter &
(e.g., Emmons, 1996; Klinger, 1977; Little, 1989). Renner, 1990; Freund & Baltes, 2000; J. Heckhausen
Goals can be defined as cognitive representations of & Schulz, 1995). In particular, the model of selec-
personally desired (or dreaded) states to be approached tion, optimization, and compensation (SOC model,
(or avoided), such as becoming a physician (or Baltes & Baltes, 1990) has stressed the importance
not becoming unemployed) through action. More of setting, pursuing, and maintaining personal goals
specifically, they encompass means of goal pursuit for successful development.
and desired outcomes of it (e.g., Kruglanski, 1996).
Because goals are composed of means and ends, Successful Development Through
goals might channel and organize information in Personal Goals
terms of means and ends (e.g., Woike, Lavezzary, & One of the central propositions of life-span psy-
Barsky, 2001): Activating a goal implies that the chology is the multidirectionality of development.
associated means and ends (as well as their emotional That is, development—in any phase of the life span—
correlates such as enjoyment or fear) are also comprises not only trajectories of growth but also
activated. Consequently, the activation of goals
trajectories of decline (Baltes, 1987). Successful
enhances the likelihood of engaging in goal-relevant development has often been defined as the maximi-
behaviors (i.e., means) (e.g., Bargh & Gollwitzer, zation of gains and the simultaneous minimization
1994). Goals, then, direct attention, affect informa- of losses (see Freund, Nikitin, & Riediger, 2012, for
tion processing, and motivate behavior. Thereby, goals a review of definitions and models of successful devel-
organize behavior over time and across situations, opment). According to the SOC model (Baltes &
and provide a sense of direction and purpose in life Baltes, 1990), an optimal ratio of gains to losses
(Freund, 2007). Moreover, goal pursuit enhances can be achieved by the orchestrated use of three
performance (e.g., Austin & Vancouver, 1996; processes of developmental regulation, namely selec-
Emmons, 1989, 1996; Freund, 2007). Therefore, tion, optimization, and compensation. As elaborated
the goal concept seems particularly well suited for in more detail elsewhere (e.g., Freund, 2006; Freund
understanding how people develop successfully & Baltes, 2000), the action-theoretical specification
over time. of the SOC model posits that developing and com-
Importantly, as Ryan and colleagues put it, not mitting to a hierarchy of personal goals (i.e., elective
all goals are created equal (Ryan, Sheldon, Kasser, & selection) and engaging in goal-directed actions and
Deci, 1996). Goals differ in their content, con- means (i.e., optimization) are essential for achieving
creteness, difficulty, time frame, and orientation higher levels of functioning (i.e., maximizing gains).
toward gains and losses (e.g., Austin & Vancouver, To maintain a given level of functioning in the
1996; Freund & Ebner, 2005; Little, 1989; Locke & face of inevitable losses in resources people encoun-
Latham, 2002; Wiese & Freund, 2005). Such goal ter throughout their lives, people have to compensate
dimensions influence the adaptiveness of goals. for their losses (e.g., by substituting goal-relevant
Various goal dimensions have been distinguished, means that are no longer available). When the costs
such as approach–avoidance (e.g., Elliott & Friedman, for optimization or compensation outweigh the
2007), promotion–prevention (e.g., Higgins, 1997), expected gains, according to the SOC model it is
intrinsic–extrinsic (e.g., Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, more adaptive to reconstruct one’s goal hierarchy
1999), and mastery–performance (e.g., Dweck & by focusing on the most important goals, developing
Leggett, 1988). This chapter centers around two new goals, or adapting goal standards (i.e., loss-based
goal dimensions that we believe to change system- selection). Thus, the SOC model conceptualizes
atically across adulthood: (a) the orientation of processes promoting gains (elective selection, opti-
personal goals toward gains, maintenance, or the mization) and also processes to counteract losses
prevention of losses (e.g., Freund & Ebner, 2005) (compensation, loss-based selection).
Figure 16.1 Integrating goal focus into H. Heckhausen’s model of action phases.
lists in mind for integrating the various goal-relevant this phase. Moreover, in a number of studies,
means, weighted by subjective likelihood of Gollwitzer and his colleagues (for an overview, see
attaining each step (Gigerenzer, Todd, & the ABC Gollwitzer, 1996) showed repeatedly and consistently
Research Group, 1999). Even if all the necessary that clear and strong implementation intentions
information were available, such an approach would contribute to goal achievement. Implementation
overburden cognitive capacities and might not even intentions specify goal-related means and actions,
lead to better decisions. Therefore, focusing on the situations in which to apply those means, and the
outcome and the value attached to the consequences right timing of acting on a given goal. Moreover,
of a potential goal before making a decision seems implementation intentions have important cognitive
more likely and more adaptive than taking detailed effects (i.e., implemental mindset): They focus
stock of the necessary means attached to the differ- attention on goal-relevant information and ward
ent outcomes into account. In fact, people focus off distractions (including questioning the value of
more on the values of the outcome than on strate- the selected goal), heighten the accessibility of situa-
gies of goal pursuit during the predecisional phase tional cues allowing goal-related actions (thereby
(H. Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, 1987). enhancing the likelihood of seizing the right moment
If a goal is not self-selected, the predecisional and opportunity), and lead to being particularly
phase is not relevant and people move directly to optimistic about achieving the goal. All these
the preactional phase, which describes the phase after characteristics of planning enhance the likelihood
having committed to a goal and before actually of initiating and completing intended goal-related
engaging in goal-relevant actions. In the preactional actions or applying goal-related means (Gollwitzer &
phase, people plan the implementation of intentions Brandstätter, 1997). Taken together, the literature
as to how, when, and where to start goal-relevant suggests that during the preactional phase, people
actions and means. If the means of goal pursuit are focus on the actual process of goal pursuit rather
well established and highly routinized, it is likely that than the outcome. When, during this phase, par-
people will proceed to implementing goal-relevant ticipants are instructed to nevertheless focus on the
actions (Bargh & Gollwitzer, 1994). If, however, the positive outcomes of goal attainment rather than
means are not yet known and routinized, the focus the process of goal pursuit, people appear to make
is likely on finding out the best way to pursue the overly optimistic plans. As shown by Fishbach and
goal (Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1997, 1999). In line Choi (2012), outcome-focused gym users predicted
with this, H. Heckhausen and Gollwitzer (1987) they would work out longer than they ended up
demonstrated that the preactional phase is associated working out, whereas process-focused gym users
with elaboration of plans and strategies of how to showed no such difference. This suggests that,
implement goal pursuit. Findings on the implemen- whereas thinking of outcomes may be motivating
tational mindset are highly compatible with the when not having begun to pursue the goal, they may
assumption of a predominant process focus during not help when immersed in goal pursuit.
Abstract
This chapter discusses two self-evaluation motives, self-enhancement (to pursue, maintain, or augment
the positivity of self-views—more so than objective standards would warrant) and self-protection (to
avoid, repair, or minimize the negativity of self-views—even at the expense of truthful feedback). Under
the self-centrality breeds self-enhancement principle (i.e., self-enhancement and self-protection will be
particularly influential in personally important domains), the chapter elaborates on five pillars of the
two motives: self-serving bias, better-than-average effect, selective self-memory, socially desirable
responding, and overclaiming. The chapter also considers other reasons for why self-enhancement
and self-protection are motivated (e.g., fluctuations in motive strength as a function of self-threat and
self-affirmation) and rules out nonmotivational explanations (e.g., expectancies, egocentrism, focalism).
Self-enhancement and self-protection are worthy of a place in the pantheon of human motivation.
Keywords: self-enhancement, self-protection, self-serving bias, better-than-average-effect, selective
self-memory, overclaiming, socially desirable responding
307
three enduring psychological functions. First, through The Self-Serving Bias
the construction of self-favoring narratives, they help The self-serving bias (SSB) is aptly described by one
to maintain an optimal state of positive emotions— of Murphy’s laws: “If more than one person is re-
a function we (Alicke & Sedikides, 2019) have sponsible for a miscalculation, none will be at fault.”
labeled psychological immunity. Second, they In psychological terms, people attribute their failures
contribute to the advancement and defense of
(e.g., subpar task performance) to external factors
psychological interests or goals, be it concrete (e.g., such as task difficulty, harsh evaluators, or bad luck;
skills and abilities like intelligence, athleticism, and however, they attribute their successes (e.g., effective
musicality) or abstract (e.g., popularity, social status, task performance) to internal factors such as disci-
or security; Alicke & Sedikides, 2009; O’Mara & pline, effort, or ability (B. Weiner, 1972). Crucially,
Gaertner, 2017). Last, they foster psychological the SSB is underlain by the self-centrality breeds
and physical health (Alicke & Sedikides, 2011; self-enhancement principle. People eagerly take re-
Sedikides, Gaertner, & Cai, 2015). It is for those sponsibility for successes that have serious implica-
reasons that an argument has been made for the tions for the kind of person they are (e.g., competent,
evolutionary significance of the two motives, intelligent, trustworthy) and promptly displace on
implicating them in species survival and repro- others or situations responsibility for failures that
duction (Sedikides & Skowronski, 2000; Sedikides, have grave consequences for them (i.e., depict them
Skowronski, & Dunbar, 2006). as incompetent, stupid, or untrustworthy).
The SSB is robust and pervasive. It is observed
The Five Pillars of Self-Enhancement among, children, adolescents, and adults (Mezulis,
and Self-Protection Motivation Abramson, Hyde, & Hanklin, 2004) and among
Over 100 years ago, William James (1907) offered a organizational employees (Corr & Gray, 1996),
remarkable insight (p. 31): “I, who for the time have athletes (de Michele, Gansneder, & Solomon, 1998),
staked my all on being a psychologist, am mortified college students (Zuckerman, 1979), drivers (Stewart,
if others know much more psychology than I. But 2005), and individuals working on interdependent
I am contended to wallow in the grossest ignorance (i.e., dyad-based) task outcomes (Sedikides, Campbell,
of Greek.” In contemporary vernacular, people value Reeder, & Elliot, 1998). It is also observed in both
and accentuate those domains of knowledge, activity, Western and East Asian cultures (Sedikides, Gaertner,
or personality that are tethered to their self-esteem. & Cai, 2015; Mezulis et al., 2004).
Other luminaries, such as Rosenberg (1965) and There are other reasons attesting to the motiva-
Rokeach (1973), expressed a similar idea, which tional nature of the SSB besides the self-centrality
has achieved the status of a psychological principle breeds self-enhancement principle. The SSB is de-
that underlies many theories of the self, including fensively magnified as level of self-threat rises: The
terror management theory (Pyszczynski, Greenberg, more self-threat people experience, the stronger they
Solomon, Arndt, & Schimel, 2004), the contingen- will display the SSB (Campbell & Sedikides, 1999).
cies of self-worth model (Crocker & Wolfe, 2001), Also, it is nondefensively attenuated following self-
and the self-concept enhancing tactician model affirmation: After writing about their important (vs.
(Sedikides & Strube, 1997). This principle, dubbed by unimportant) values, people are far less likely to
Gebauer, Wagner, Sedikides, and Neberich (2013) manifest the SSB, if at all (Sherman & Kim, 2005).
self-centrality breeds self-enhancement, implies that the Two nonmotivational explanations for the SSB
self-enhancement and self-protection motives will have been proposed (Sedikides & Alicke, 2012).
be particularly influential in personally important One refers to differential expectancies for success
(i.e., central) domains. Put otherwise, signatures of and failure: Based on their personal histories, people
self-enhancement and self-protection will reflect simply anticipate more successes than failures.
strivings to perceive oneself as a “good person” or a However, the SSB is observed even when success and
“good member of the culture,” namely, to boost or failure expectancies are controlled for (Sedikides
guard one’s self-esteem. et al., 1998; see also Campbell & Sedikides, 1999;
We use the self-centrality breeds self-enhancement Weary, 1979). The other explanation refers to
principle as a unifying theme in discussing the impression management, that is, strategic attempts to
proposed five key signatures (i.e., pillars) of the two present oneself favorably to others. Impression man-
motives. These pillars are the self-serving bias, the agement may reflect the self-enhancement motive
better-than-average-effect, selective self-memory, so- in itself (Sedikides, Hoorens, & Dufner, 2015).
cially desirable responding, and overclaiming. Regardless, the SSB occurs even when controlling
Abstract
This chapter explores motivational questions that arise when the psychology of girls and women is
viewed through the lens of objectification theory, which highlights the centrality of appearance
concerns, or body projects, for girls and women in the early 21st century. The chapter examines three
theoretical perspectives for what motivates sexually objectifying views and treatment of women and
women’s own participation in self-objectifying body projects, which have garnered compelling evidence:
a power-related motivation, an existential motivation, and a system-justifying motivation. It then
investigates the cultivation, through developmental processes and immersion in objectifying
environments, of self-objectification at a trait level, as well as the more direct situational experiences
that induce states of self-objectification. And, finally, it reviews evidence that self-objectification,
though motivating in itself, carries significant consequences for girls’ and women’s health, well-being,
and full participation in the world.
Keywords: sexual objectification, self-objectification, body, gender, motivation
321
gazed at and evaluated based on physical appearance, in terms of how (she) looks or appears to the senses;
an effect termed self-objectification (Fredrickson & and silencing reflects the treatment of sexually
Roberts, 1997). objectified people as lacking the capacity to speak.
In this chapter, we explore the motivational Indeed, each of Nussbaum’s (1999) and Langton’s
questions that arise when we view the psychology of (2009) components can be seen in cultural repre-
girls and women through the lens of objectification sentations as well as interpersonal treatment of women
theory. First, we ask a why question: Why are wom- and girls. In psychology, Fredrickson and Roberts
en’s bodies viewed and treated as sexually objectified (1997) argued that sexual objectification occurs
things? In other words, what are the psychic and along a continuum, extending beyond p ornography
structural motivators of the sexual objectification of to the wider cultural context, which normalizes the
women and women’s own self-objectification? Next, commodification of women’s bodies just about
we examine a how question: How are girls and women everywhere, and that this cultural context induces
compelled to take on their own body projects? How girls and women to adopt a third-person perspective
does the culture of objectification translate into and on their own bodies (i.e., to self-objectify).
take hold of girls’ and women’s own trait-level self- But why? Why are women’s bodies sexually
concepts? How do situational or contextual features objectified? What motivates this cultural, interper-
of girls’ and women’s environments motivate states sonal, and even intrapsychic treatment? Most lay
of self-objectification? Finally, we argue that, despite thinking on this question gets reduced to “boys and
its motivating elements, the body project that so men can’t help it,” and this way of thinking has its
many girls and women are engaged in carries a host roots in an evolutionary perspective, which argues
of cognitive, emotional, behavioral and even health that the sexual objectification of women’s bodies is
consequences. part and parcel of the naturally selected mating
strategy of human males (Buss, 2007). However, the
Theoretical Frameworks on Sexual argument that men’s visual inspections of women’s
and Self-Objectification bodies, the emphasis placed on women’s bodies as
Objectification has been the topic of Western philo- the most important feature of them, and even that
sophical treatment for some time. Kant (1996) women’s own attention to and enhancement of
argued that treating human beings “only as a means” their attractiveness are all motivated by a drive for
was morally suspect. Modern-day philosopher Martha heterosexual mating have generally failed to be sup-
Nussbaum (1995) defined objectification as the treat- ported empirically. For example, both kindness and
ing of persons as “things,” separating them from intelligence ranked above physical attractiveness in
their human attributes or characteristics. Nussbaum a cross-cultural study of males’ desired characteris-
(1999) identified seven components of objectification: tics in a potential female sex partner (Buss et al,
instrumentality and ownership involve treating a 1990). As well, a study of the eye movements of
person as a tool or commodity; denial of autonomy male and female participants made while judging
and inertness involve seeing a person as lacking self- the attractiveness of photographs of female bodies
determination and agency; fungibility is characterized found no evidence for fixations on fertility-relevant
by viewing a person as interchangeable with others areas, illustrating that these areas had no impact on
of his or her “type”; violability represents someone attractiveness judgments, as would be the case if
as lacking boundary integrity; and denial of subjec- mating were the motivation for sexual objectifica-
tivity involves believing that a person’s experiences tion (Cornelissen, Hancock, Kiviniemi, George, &
and feelings can be neglected. Tovée, 2009).
Feminists Dworkin (1989) and MacKinnon (1987) Finally, an evolutionary explanation for what
described how pornography functions as sexual motivates the sexual objectification of women fails
objectification, a particular form of the dehuman- in at least two more important ways. First, an evo-
ization of women and a primary cause of gender lutionary view does not adequately account for the
inequality. More recently, philosopher Rae Langton fact that objectification occurs along a continuum
(2009) added three more features to Nussbaum’s from seemingly benign or even benevolent (e.g.,
seven, which she argued are of particular relevance widespread cultural displays of idealized women’s
to feminist understandings of sexual objectification: bodies, men’s interpersonal treatment of girls and
Reduction to body involves treating someone as iden- women with catcalls or ogling) to abjectly dehu-
tified with (her) body or body parts; reduction to manizing (e.g., rape, trafficking, and even sexually
appearance occurs when a person is treated primarily motivated torture or murder). In other words,
Abstract
A significant goal in many countries around the world is promoting children’s motivation so that they
may achieve at their full potential. There is much evidence supporting the idea that parents play a
significant role in either facilitating or undermining children’s motivation. The focus of this chapter is on
how relatedness between children and their parents shapes the development of children’s motivation
and ultimately achievement. Three sets of ideas about how relatedness between children and their
parents contributes to children’s motivation are reviewed. An integration of the three sets of ideas is
provided to highlight key themes as well as suggest innovative directions for future research.
Keywords: achievement, attachment, motivation, parent–child relations, parenting
Feeling related to others is fundamental to human the academic arena has also been of interest (for a
functioning (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Deci & review, see van IJzendoorn, Dijksta, & Bus, 1995).
Ryan, 2000). For most humans, their earliest expe- The central goal of this chapter is to integrate
rience with relatedness takes place in the context of several lines of theory and research in which chil-
their relationships with their parents. Such relation- dren’s relationships with their parents serve as a
ships are unique in that they are often the first in context for the development of their motivation,
children’s lives, with children depending on their with implications for their learning in the academic
parents to provide them with important physical arena (for reviews of other ways in which parents
and psychological resources (Clutton-Brock, 1991; contribute to children’s motivation, see Grolnick,
Thompson et al., 2005). It is thus not surprising Friendly, & Bellas, 2009; Pomerantz & Grolnick,
that even as children enter into relationships with 2017; Pomerantz, Kim, & Cheung, 2012; Pomerantz,
others such as their peers, their relationships with Moorman, & Litwack, 2007). We first briefly d escribe
their parents retain substantial significance through- the major categories of motivation on which inves-
out adolescence, if not into adulthood (Collins & tigators studying children’s academic functioning
Steinberg, 2006; Offer & Offer, 1975). Indeed, generally focus. Second, we highlight the relevant
children’s relationships with their parents have
postulates and supportive research of the two fore-
been identified as key contexts for virtually all most theories linking relatedness between children
aspects of their psychological development (for and their parents to children’s academic functioning.
reviews, see Cassidy & Shaver, 2008; Groh, Fearon, We begin by reviewing Bowlby’s (1969, 1973, 1980)
IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Roisman, attachment theory and its extensions to such func-
2017; Thompson, 2006). Although much of the tioning (e.g., Bretherton, 1985; van IJzendoorn et al.,
attention in this vein has been directed toward the 1995). We then discuss Deci and Ryan’s (1985,
development of children’s functioning in the social 2000) self-determination theory, with an emphasis on
arena, the development of children’s functioning in its application to parents’ socialization of children’s
337
motivation (e.g., Grolnick, Deci, & Ryan, 1997; (i.e., children’s investigation of their environment
Grolnick & Farkas, 2002). Third, we move beyond so that they become knowledgeable about it). Among
the focus of these theories on the quality of related- children who are securely attached, their relation-
ness to another dimension of relatedness—children’s ships with their parents serve two primary functions.
sense of responsibility to their parents. Fourth, we First, their parents act as a secure base from which
integrate the different ideas about the role of chil- children can investigate their world. Second, their
dren’s relatedness to their parents in their academic parents serve as a safe haven, such that children are
functioning, suggesting important questions to be able to return to their parents for comfort as they
answered in the future. face challenges. For securely attached children, their
attachment and exploration systems are balanced,
Major Categories of Children’s Motivation which is not the case for insecurely attached children.
Theory and research concerned with children’s These children experience anxiety over the possibility
academic functioning have generally focused on
of losing their parents’ attention, which interferes
three major categories of motivation (for a review, with their exploration.
see Wigfield et al., 2015). First, is that of how Several studies find that when children are securely
capable children feel in regard to accomplishing attached to their parents in the early years of life,
the academic tasks they encounter. This includes they are better able to explore their environment at
children’s perceptions of competence, expectations this time, often demonstrating enhanced competence
for future performance, feelings of efficacy, and (e.g., Belsky, Garduque, & Hrncir, 1984; Frodi,
sense of control. Second, children’s investment and Bridges, & Grolnick, 1985). For example, in one of
engagement in academic tasks are of import, the first studies on this issue, Matas, Arend, and
including not simply the value children assign to Sroufe (1978) observed that children who were se-
such tasks and the amount of time they spend on curely (versus insecurely) attached to their mothers
them, but also their use of effective learning strate- at 18 months were more effectively engaged in
gies, such as the planning and monitoring of their problem solving 6 months later in that they worked
learning. A third category is the reasons behind chil- longer on problem-solving tasks, with more enthu-
dren’s investment and engagement. One of the most siasm and less frustration. Perhaps because of their
studied set of reasons is children’s autonomous (e.g., enhanced exploration, securely attached children
enjoyment and personal importance) versus con- are more cognitively competent (e.g., their language
trolled (e.g., avoidance of shame and attainment of is more developed) than are insecurely attached
rewards) reasons—often referred to as intrinsic (versus children, over and above their early IQ (e.g.,
extrinsic) motivation. Also receiving substantive O’Connor & McCartney, 2007; van IJzendoorn
attention in this vein is children’s concern with
et al., 1995). Children with an ambivalent insecure
developing (i.e., mastery motivation) rather than attachment to their parents (i.e., children alternate
demonstrating (i.e., performance motivation) their between seeking and resisting their parents)
competence. All three categories of motivation appear to be at greater risk for a lack of exploration
contribute to children’s achievement (for a review, than are children with an avoidant insecure attach-
see Wigfield et al., 2015). Thus, they have all received ment (i.e., children physically and affectively avoid
attention in the theory and research concerned with their parents; e.g., Belsky et al., 1984; Frodi et al.,
the role of parents in the development of children’s 1985). Frodi and colleagues (1985) speculated that
academic functioning. although both types of insecurely a ttached children
feel anxious over their parents’ attention, avoidant
The Attachment Theory Perspective children are more likely to displace their anxiety
The idea that children’s relationships with their by engaging in activities that give the appearance
parents contribute to their motivation is evident of greater exploration compared to their ambivalent
in Bowlby’s (1969, 1973, 1980) and Ainsworth’s counterparts.
(Ainsworth, 1973; Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & A critical question is whether children’s early
Wall, 1978) writings in the context of attachment attachment to their parents contributes to their
theory (see also Bretherton, 1985). Focusing on the academic functioning over the longer term as chil-
first 2 years of life, both Bowlby and Ainsworth dren progress through school. A key mechanism
made the case that the quality of children’s by which it may do so is the internal working
attachment to their primary caregivers, who are models children develop of themselves and
often their parents, shapes children’s exploration others (Jacobsen, Wolfgang, & Hofman, 1994;
20 Motivational Neuroscience
Abstract
The goal of the present chapter is to show the relevance of neuroscience research to human
motivation researchers. The first part of the chapter discusses the current status of the possible
integration of motivational psychology and neuroscience into the new emerging field of motivational
neuroscience. The second part identifies 15 brain structures and 5 neural pathways that underlie most
of the neural basis of human motivational states. The third part examines how recent findings in
neuroscience have advanced the understanding of 14 widely studied motivational concepts, including
those automatically activated by environmental stimuli (hunger, thirst), those learned from the
rewarding properties of environmental stimuli (incentives, rewards, expectancy, value), and those
proactively generated by the individual (agency, volition, self-regulation, goals, curiosity, intrinsic
motivation, psychological needs, and autonomous self-regulation). The chapter concludes with
suggestions for future research.
Keywords: motivation, neuroscience, striatum, reward, dopamine, prefrontal cortex
We organized this chapter into three sections. First, such as hormones and psychophysiological states,
we provide a contemporary update on the status but the endocrine system (hormones) and autonomic
of the newly emerging field of motivational neu- nervous system (psychophysiology) are regulated
roscience. Second, we identify this new field’s by these brain-based processes. So, typically, the
subject matter, which revolves around the brain subject matter and dependent measures emphasized
structures and neural networks that underlie human in motivational neuroscience investigations are the
motivational states. Third, we provide a neurosci- activation of specific brain structures, networks,
entific perspective on a wide range of motivational and neurotransmitters.
states that are highly studied by motivational Motivational neuroscientists are a loosely con-
psychologists. nected group of scholars who focus their attention
on the aforementioned brain-based processes to help
Motivational Neuroscience: Current Status them better understand motivation and motivated
of the Field behavior. Their research is often top-down because it
Motivational neuroscience is the empirical study typically begins with a well-studied motivational
of all the brain-based processes that energize, phenomenon (e.g., self-regulation, intrinsic motiva-
direct, and sustain behavior. Brain-based processes tion) and then proceeds to investigate the phenom-
include brain structures (e.g., striatum), pathways enon from the new, fresh perspective of neuroscience.
of interconnected neural structures (e.g., mesocor- That is, neuroscience is typically used to supplement
tical pathway), and the neurotransmitters that and extend what has previously been discovered
organize these pathways (e.g., dopamine). The field about that motivational phenomenon, and research-
also includes measures of bodily based processes, ers do this by incorporating neuroscientific methods
355
and findings into their programs of research. remain dead-end streets because educators have
An example of this approach can be seen in the simply been unable to utilize or apply the progress
application of neuroscience methods and dependent being made in neuroscience (or vice versa). Attempts
measures to better understand the undermining to construct a few one-way streets have been made.
effect that extrinsic rewards sometimes have on intrin- For instance, educators are hopeful that off-the-
sic motivation (Murayama, Matsumoto, Izuma, & shelf, commercially available headsets (e.g., portable
Matsumoto, 2010). electroencephalography headsets) can be utilized to
Another group of scholars are neuroscientists monitor students’ attention, motivation, and learning
who are interested in incorporating motivational as they are exposed to learning materials, much in
phenomena (e.g., reward, value, self-control) into the same way that movie studios are hopeful of
their programs of research. This research is often utilizing facial recognition software with artificial
bottom-up because it typically begins with a deep intelligence to learn how movie-goers respond
understanding of specific brain mechanisms and emotionally to their film content. But there are no
then proceeds to investigate their motivational two-way streets, and there are no one-way streets
implications. An example of this approach can be that start in the land of educational psychology and
seen in the brain-centric approach to understanding end in the land of neuroscience. Overall, the two
reward-motivated approach behavior (Berridge & fields continue to be largely independent domains
Kringelbach, 2008). of scholarship.
Currently, human motivation study and neuro- Some fields have fared better in building inter-
science are for the most part two different and sepa- disciplinary (two-way) streets with neuroscience,
rate fields. To understand the implications of having including cognitive neuroscience (Gazzaniga, Ivry, &
two independent working groups focused on moti- Mangun, 2014), affective neuroscience (Davidson &
vation study, it is instructive to examine the relation Sutton, 1995; Panksepp, 1998), social neuroscience
between neuroscience and a different field of (Decety & Cacioppo, 2011; Harmon-Jones &
study—namely, educational psychology. This is so Inzlicht, 2016), and neuroeconomics (Loewenstein,
because, 2 decades ago, Richard Mayer (1998) char- Rick, & Cohen, 2008). With regard to motivational
acterized the relationship between neuroscience and neuroscience, there is some, but only a little, overlap
educational psychology through the imagery of between motivational science and neuroscience.
dead-end, one-way, and two-way streets. He charac- Unfortunately, the methods, equipment, data
terized (and lamented) the relation between neuro- analytic techniques, and research methodologies of
science and his field as an intellectual landscape neuroscience often prove too overwhelming for
characterized by dead-end streets in which the two motivational psychologists to integrate into their
fields of study had little in common and each con- programs of research, at least not without collabo-
tributed little to the enrichment of the other. He rations with neuroscientists. Further, the knowledge
also observed (and again lamented) an intellectual base, research methodologists, data analytic tech-
landscape of one-way streets in which neuroscience niques, and data-collecting machinery in neurosci-
research was unidirectionally applied to educational ence are developing at an incredibly rapid pace. This
psychology. For instance, neuroscientists identified state of affairs makes it clear that a motivational
the limits of hippocampal-based short-term memory psychologist cannot be a part-time neuroscientist.
(e.g., cognitive overload), and educational psychol- For now, motivational psychologists necessarily
ogists revised their theories of learning and their partner with full-time neuroscientists to get their
recommendations for the design of instruction work done and their questions answered. So, there
accordingly (e.g., Paas, Tuovinen, Tabbers & van are some meaningful barriers that are keeping
Gerven, 2003). He could find very few one-way motivational psychologists from becoming full-
streets from educational psychology to neuroscience. fledged motivational neuroscientists.
The metaphor Mayer offered to enrich interdis- Similarly, neuroscientists have a difficult time
ciplinary activity was that of a two-way street. In this bringing motivational psychology into their programs
scenario, neuroscientific study influences, informs, of research. What motivational psychologists have
and enriches educational psychology, while educa- to offer are motivation theories (and applied problems
tional psychology study influences, informs, and to solve). Neuroscientists tend to stay intellectually
enriches neuroscience. It has been 20 years since the close to basic motivational processes, such as reward,
publication of that paper, and it seems that these value, and conflict, and resist subjectively experienced
two-way streets never materialized. Most streets motivational phenomena. There is little deductively
Hypothalamus
Prefrontal Substantia
cortex nigra
Insular cortex
(C)
Caudate nucleus
Putamen
Orbitofrontal Insular cortex
cortex
Ventral Globus pallidus
tegmental
area
Ventromedial
Amygdala
prefrontal cortex
Ventral striatum &
Nucleus accumbens
Figure 20.1 Anatomic location of 17 key motivation- and movement-relevant brain structures.
Note: A medial sagittal section (A), a lateral sagittal section (B), and a coronal section (C) of the brain.
SUBCORTICAL REGIONS
HYPOTHALAMUS
Responsive to natural rewards.
AMYGDALA
Responsive to reward characteristics. DOPAMINE SYSTEM
Abstract
What determines effort intensity in instrumental behavior? According to motivation intensity theory,
effort should be proportional to experienced task difficulty as long as success is possible and justified
and low when success is impossible or excessively difficult, given the available benefit. When task
difficulty is unspecified or unknown, effort should be proportional to the importance of success. This
chapter reports an extensive program of research that has operationalized effort intensity as
cardiovascular reactivity during task performance and used multiple manipulations of variables
influencing subjective task difficulty (e.g., performance standards, instrumentality, ability, fatigue, mood,
depressive symptoms, implicit affect, implicit and biological aging) and the amount of justified effort
(e.g., material incentive, instrumentality, needs, personal and social evaluation, mortality salience). In
the second edition of this handbook, this chapter focuses on recent empirical evidence for the
principles of motivation intensity theory and discusses challenges for other theoretical accounts.
Keywords: effort, motivation intensity, cardiovascular response, demand, achievement
373
Richter, Gendolla, & Wright, 2016; Wright & correspond to difficulty—people should invest greater
Kirby, 2001). Before discussing recent research, we effort the more difficult they perceive their task to
will first describe motivation intensity theory and be. But, importantly, effort should drop b ecause of
its origin—the principle of resource conservation. disengagement if success appears to be impossible
Following pioneering work by Gibson (1900), or if the necessary effort is not justified by the bene-
psychologists recognized early that organisms tend fit it should accrue. This is because in both cases
to mobilize just the resources that are necessary for high engagement would violate the basic principle
goal attainment (e.g., Hull, 1943; Tolman, 1932). of resource mobilization. Thus, put in one sentence,
Moreover, drawing on the idea that effort functions to motivation intensity theory predicts that effort rises
cope with obstacles during goal pursuit, it was proportionally to subjective task difficulty as long
postulated that resource mobilization follows a
success appears to be possible and the necessary
“
difficulty law of motivation” (e.g., Ach, 1935; effort is justified (Figure 21.1).
Hillgruber, 1912). Accordingly, effort is mobilized
proportionally to the experienced difficulty of The Role of Benefit
instrumental behavior—the greater the obstacles
An important point about the central proposition
encountered during goal pursuit, the more effort is just discussed is that expected benefit should not
mobilized. Brehm’s motivation intensity theory is determine effort directly. Rather, benefit should de-
the most influential elaboration of these basic prin- termine effort indirectly by setting the upper limit
ciples of resource mobilization. of what is justified and what people are willing to
do. Brehm referred to this upper limit as the level
Motivation Intensity Theory of potential motivation—a concept that has drawn
Motivation intensity theory (Brehm et al., 1983; additional attention recently (e.g., Kruglanski,
Brehm & Self, 1989; see also Brehm, 1975) is Chernikova, Rosenzweig, & Kopetz, 2014). In theory,
grounded in the resource conservation principle. factors related to benefit (e.g., the value of and need
People are expected to mobilize effort (a) only to the for an available incentive) should have no impact
degree that is needed and (b) only when expendi- on effort as long as success is possible and benefit is
ture yields sufficient return (i.e., a benefit) to justify great enough to justify the required effort.
the necessary effort. The required effort is deter- To illustrate, consider a person offered at different
mined by the difficulty of instrumental behavior— times $10, $20, and $30 to lift a weight that he or
the difficulty of activity that must be carried out to she could lift and was willing to lift for $10. The
attain desired outcomes and avoid aversive ones. present view suggests that the person’s effort should
Consequently, effort should vary nonmonotonically correspond to the difficulty of the lift, not the value
with the perceived difficulty of instrumental behav- of the incentive, at the different points. Next, consider
ior. As long as success is viewed as both possible and what would happen under conditions where this
worth the investment that it requires, effort should person was unwilling to lift the weight even for $30.
(A) (B)
(Potential Motivation)
Effort
Effort
(Potential Motivation)
unspecified low moderate high extreme unspecified low moderate high extreme
Task Difficulty Task Difficulty
Figure 21.1 Theoretical predictions of the joint impact of task difficulty and potential motivation on effort intensity.
(A) Predictions for effort mobilization when low effort is justified (i.e., low potential motivation). (B) Predictions for the condition that high effort is justified
(i.e., high potential motivation). (Figure adapted from Gendolla & Wright, 2009, p. 134. Copyright: Oxford University Press, reproduced with permission).
0
Reactivity Scores (in ms)
–2 6
–4 4
–6 2
–8 0
Low Moderate High Impossible Low Moderate High Impossible
Task Difficulty Task Difficulty
Figure 21.2 Preejection period (PEP, A) and systolic blood pressure (SBP, B) reactivity in dependence on task difficulty in the study
by Richter, Friedrich, and Gendolla (2008).
(Copyright: Society for Psychophysiological Research, reproduced with permission).
w
Lo
ility
Ab
igh
yH
ilit
Ab
TASK DIFFICULTY
Figure 21.3 Relation between effort and difficulty for people with low and high ability.
(Adapted from a highly similar figure presented by Wright and Franklin, 2004, p. 190. Copyright: Erlbaum Press.)
–1
–2
–3
–4
Low High
Performance Standard
Figure 21.4. Systolic blood pressure (SBP) reactivity as a function of difficulty for low- and high-fatigue participants.
Based on data presented by Wright, Martin, and Bland (2003).
importance determining the point at which they success as excessively difficult or impossible at all
do so as long as they perceive success as possible. difficulty levels.
Indeed, older people mobilized more resources and
show stronger effort-related responses of the CV Moderation of Affective Influences
system than younger adults when they cognitively on Effort
performed on a similar level (B. T. Smith & Hess, Objective task difficulty also systematically moder-
2015). Moreover, Hess and Ennis (2012) found ates the earlier discussed mood impact on effort
stronger SBP responses in older adults during both mobilization. The effects of positive and negative
a fatigue induction period in which participants were moods in easy and difficult tasks resemble those of
presented a simple or complex counting challenge the just-presented effects of high and low ability and
and an ensuing fatigue influence period in which high and low fatigue. When people in a depressed
participants solved multiplication problems. or happy mood work on a task with fixed task diffi-
However, a qualified exception is seen in an ex- culty, the mood–behavior model (Gendolla, 2000)
periment by Stewart, Wright, and Griffith (2016) posits that they will pragmatically use both types of
that examined CV responses in cognitively healthy information—mood and task difficulty—to calibrate
older adults and patients diagnosed with mild cog- effort. Consequently, as shown in experiments (e.g.,
nitive impairment, the prodromal state of clinical Gendolla & Krüsken, 2001b, 2002b), in objectively
dementia. Mild cognitive impairment is character- easy tasks, effort is higher in a negative mood than
ized by cognitive decline that is greater than that in a positive mood, because subjective demand and
expected with normal aging, but not so great that mobilized resources are higher in a negative mood.
it interferes with daily function. Investigators as- But when a task is objectively difficult, effort is higher
signed the cognitively healthy older adults and in a positive mood than in a negative mood. The
mild cognitive impairment patients three versions reason for this moderation is that subjective demand
of a modified Sternberg memory task designed to for a difficult task is high but still feasible in a positive
range in difficulty from low to high. As expected, mood, whereas it appears to be overchallenging in a
work-related SBP responses rose with difficulty for negative mood. However, when objective task
cognitively healthy older adults. By contrast, the difficulty is extremely high, so that succeeding is
responses were consistently low for mild cognitive obviously impossible, mood cannot provide addi-
impairment patients, possibly because they viewed tional diagnostic information. Here, as a result of
1.5
Anger Primes
1
Fear Primes
0.5
0
–0.5
–1
Reactivity (ms)
–1.5
–2
–2.5
–3
–3.5
–4
–4.5
–5
Easy task Difficult task
Figure 21.5 Cardiac preejection period reactivity during a short-term memory task in the experiment by Chatelain, Silvestrini, and
Gendolla (2016).
(Copyright: Elsevier, reproduced with permission).
Abstract
In humanistic psychology, autonomy, internalization, and the self constitute fundamental concepts
to explain adaptive behavior and well-being. However, the momentary mental processes, their
interactions, and individual differences that constitute a causal fundament for these concepts are yet
underexplored. The authors will analyze these processes against the backdrop of a functional approach,
Personality Systems Interactions Theory, which conceives the self as one out of several neurocognitive
systems and highlights its role for autonomous motivation and self-regulation. We attempt to provide
answers to questions such as the following: Which momentary mental processes and underlying
neurocognitive systems (e.g., large-scale brain networks) facilitate the establishment of stages of
internalization? Can the self become inhibited in a way that even highly internalized goals and values
may not manifest in behavior? Which role does the self play in emotion regulation and decision-making,
and how do these processes in turn facilitate autonomous behavior? The authors believe that the
present functional analysis advances a conciliation between a phenomenologically-oriented, humanities
view and a process-oriented natural science view on human motivation.
Keywords self-determination theory, personality systems interactions theory, predictive and reactive
control systems theory, functional approach, autonomous motivation, intrinsic motivation, self-
regulation, emotion regulation, action orientation, decision-making, cognitive dissonance reduction,
affective consonance production, self-access, self-actualization, self-infiltration, volition
Autonomous motivation, internalization, and the self worldviews might meet. Specifically, we use person-
each refer to high-level concepts in psychological ality systems interactions (PSI) theory (Kuhl, 2000a,
functioning, which are so complex that researchers 2000b, 2001; Quirin & Kuhl, 2009b) to explain a
with backgrounds in the natural sciences may number of processes relevant to motivation and
wonder whether they can properly be defined, personality, such as autonomous motivation and in-
measured, or investigated at all, much less neurosci- ternalization, which are two conceptual pillars of
entifically. On the other hand, systems-dynamical self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 2000;
terms like neuropsychological systems or excitatory Ryan & Deci, 2017). SDT and PSI theory may be
versus inhibitory connections sound so mechanistic called theoretical “siblings” (Ryan, 2018, p. 37) as
or biologistic that researchers with phenomeno- they both constitute organismic theories that high-
logical and humanities backgrounds may wonder light the relevance of autonomy for individuals’
whether they do justice to the complex individuality adaptive self-regulation and self-growth.
of a person. We begin by defining some essentials of SDT
In this chapter, we apply a functional systems and PSI theory. Next, we describe four neurocogni-
approach to demonstrate how the two scientific tive systems postulated by PSI theory, how they can
393
be differentially activated by diverse social contexts values remain a mbivalent and do not belong to a
and affordances and how they may relate to stages of core sense of the individuals’ self, that is, to already
internalization and associated phenomena. Thereafter, integrated self-aspects. On the next level labeled
we examine how self-access, conceived of as the acti- identification, individuals identify with the goal in a
vation of the self as one of the neurocognitive systems, way that feels compatible with the self and is thus
facilitates self-regulation processes relevant to effective evaluated more positively (true preference). On the
goal pursuit and well-being. We focus on three such highest level of internalization, called integration,
self-regulation processes, namely self-determined the goal or value becomes more strongly and broadly
decision-making (self-decision), self-determined emo- connected with self-aspects and thus a core and rel-
tion regulation (self-relaxation), and self-determined atively stable feature of the self that individuals
motivation (self-motivation). One major assumption cannot easily deidentify with unless they extensively
is that self-access, along with these three processes, change their self-schema on the basis of accommo-
can chronically or momentarily be blocked (e.g., by dative processes.
negative affect or stress) in such a way that even Self-determination theory holds that the higher
highly internalized goals and values may not mani- the level of internalization, the stronger the self-
fest in behavior. regulated motivation, that is, the motivation that is
Next, we use predictive and reactive control sys- independent of external rewards and punishments.
tems (PARCS) theory (e.g., Tops, Boksem, Luu, & Moreover, a high level of internalization corre-
Tucker, 2010; Tops, Boksem, Quirin, IJzerman, & sponds with well-being and mental health (Chirkov,
Koole, 2014; Tops, Quirin, Boksem, & Koole, 2017), Ryan, Kim, & Kaplan, 2003; Ryan & Deci, 2000,
a neurobehavioral self-regulation theory largely 2017; Sheldon, Williams, & Joiner, 2003), as well as
compatible with PSI theory, to analyze several im- ethical behavior (Arvanitis, 2017; Krapp, 2013). The
portant neural mechanisms of self-determination. degree of internalization is considered to depend on
Finally, we conclude by summarizing our main ar- the degree to which others are esteemed as autonomy
guments, reflecting the present functional systems supporting, empathetically related, and competent
approach, and highlighting its potential for bridg- and thus function as a positively esteemed model
ing the gap between different areas of psychology, (Deci & Ryan, 2000).
as well between a humanities and a natural (and
neuro-) sciences view on motivation. Personality Systems Interactions Theory
How might the processes that define self-
Self-Determination Theory determination be conceptualized within a cognitive
Self-determination theory constitutes perhaps the (neuro-)science approach that analyzes the human
most prominent, cross-interdisciplinary, and out- psyche on the basis of interacting neuropsychological
lasting theory of human motivation. It was devel- systems? To answer this question, we use the frame-
oped to answer the question of when and how much work of PSI theory, a functional systems approach
individuals are motivated to pursue their goals. that provides a microanalysis of self-regulation pro-
Specifically, an aspect of SDT capitalizes on psy- cesses and their interactions (Kuhl, 2000a, 2000b,
choanalytical ideas (e.g., Schafer, 1968), and SDT 2001). Personality systems interaction theory has
hypothesizes the existence of a continuum of the the potential to link SDT-relevant processes with a
degree to which goals or values are internalized into number of cognitive–affective processes that have
the self. At the lowest end of this continuum, goals been described in other theories and that are rele-
and values are not internalized at all and individuals vant to maintain action control. Personality systems
may show complete amotivation, or their behavior interaction theory is an attempt to integrate a
is oriented toward external rewards (e.g., completing number of self-regulation and motivation models
an academic study program to receive social or within a cognitive science framework. Its basic as-
monetary approval or not to be punished by addi- sumption holds that a number of (neuro-)cognitive
tional study fees). On a next level called introjection, systems interact among each other and with affec-
goals and values are internalized up to a degree to tive processes in a specific way to produce human
which individuals d evelop feelings of pleasure or experience and to enable efficient and self-congruent
punishment (fear) themselves, that is, even in the goal pursuit and behavior. Research related to PSI
absence of significant and evaluating others. This level theory is based on both correlational and experi-
refers to the notion of the superego (Freud, 1962) mental research on cognitive and affective states and
or the ought self (Higgins, 1987), where goals and related individual differences.
Integration
sensorimotor, habits
left dorsal predictive system right dorsal predictive system
Self-Decision (incl. VMPFC)
Self-Relaxation
Regression
Vol.
PA NA
Inhib. Volitional Facilitation Self-Inhibition
Demand Threat
Figure 22.1 Full arrows refer to an excitatory influence and dotted arrows refer to an inhibitory influence. PA = positive affect;
NA = negative affect; VMPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
The integrative self provides a holistic overview of goal alternatives and personal preferences. When the integrative self is activated
(self-access), mindful, circumspect decisions can be made (self-decision) that generate an intention that must be kept in intention
memory, the activation of which prevents impulsive enactment of the intention (volitional inhibition). The generation of reward-
related positive affect is considered to resolve volitional inhibition again (volitional facilitation). Self-(determined) motivation, as
released through self-access (including even remote, self-congruent associations about the value/utility of the activity or its
consequences), constitutes an effective and sustainable determinant of positive affect generation. When there are no obstacles
(including the case that action routines are prespecified already), self-goals can immediately be enacted, which is represented by the
efferent pathway from the integrative self to the action system (automatic self-expression). Deviations from predicted goal-directed and
other expectations, that is, novelties, emergencies, and threats, activate the discrepancy system and concomitant negative affect
(provided that deviations exceed an individual’s set point of arousal), which bring the individual from a predictive into a reactive
mode where he or she focuses on the difficulties (thereby initially losing a mindful overview: self-inhibition) and to eventually modify
action plans and intentions by deliberate reflection (arrow from discrepancy system to intention memory). By contrast, intact self-
access facilitates finding potential solutions based on a mindful overview of integrated, prior experiences that puts potential threats
and mistakes in perspective and reduces negative affect (self-relaxation). The long arrow winding around the four cognitive macro
systems represents the continuum of internalization, as postulated by self-determination theory, and indicates which system
prototypically, albeit not exclusively, supports which of the internalization stages. Specifically, discrepancy system, intention memory,
action system, and integrative self (along with their typical concomitant affective states) are considered to support external regulation,
introjection, identification, and integration, respectively. Not least, these systems also show a differential predominance in their
responsivity to social–communicational affordances such as threat, demand, modeling (contagion, imitation, synchronization, etc.),
and regard (autonomy support, understanding, and eventually competence), respectively. See text for detailed information.
of the corresponding implicit parts of the self defines This continuous extension and connecting of auto-
the level of self-congruence of a person. biographical experiences (see extension memory, Kuhl,
The definition of the self as an integrated net- 2000a) provides a major neurocognitive basis of
work of strongly interconnected self-representations self-growth.
inherently implies that this system constitutes a The continuous process of integrating new expe-
major basis of integration at the highest level of in- riences, goals, and values within the existing self-
ternalization reachable. Still, the self can vary with structure, that is, to extensively interconnect them
respect to its degree of integration depending on with a large number of self-aspects, takes time. If
how many important experiences and self-aspects this interconnection is realized to some degree but
have yet been integrated over the life span rather low or instable, we can speak of identified rather
than neglected or repressed (see also Linville, 1987). than integrated goals. As such, the self constitutes a
23 Motivation in Psychotherapy
Abstract
Motivational issues are central to human life. Correspondingly, they are also central to the challenging
endeavor of psychotherapy. Assisting patients to change involves motivational issues at various levels
and at various stages of therapy. Patients might be more or less motivated to begin and to participate
in the different stages of psychotherapy (therapy motivation). Besides these differences in therapy
motivation, an understanding of the broader concepts of motivation in psychotherapy should mandate
that motivational issues be considered in the treatment of all patients, not only those with obvious
deficits in therapy motivation. Motivational issues influence the therapeutic relationship; they should be
considered in tailoring specific interventions and they might be important factors for the onset and
maintenance of psychological disorders. The present chapter presents theoretical and empirical
background information and illustrates therapeutic approaches for dealing with patients’ motivation.
Moreover, it summarizes the implications of basic and clinical research for a motivationally informed
psychotherapy.
Keywords: motivation, goals, psychotherapy, treatment, psychopathology
Motivation is central in life and governs most psy- involves motivational issues at various levels and at
chological processes. According to Heckhausen and various stages of therapy. First, patients might be
Heckhausen (2008, p. 1), more or less motivated to begin psychotherapy and
to participate in the different stages of the psycho-
the psychology of motivation is specifically concerned
therapy process. Often, patients are willing to work
with activities that reflect the pursuit of a particular
very hard during the therapeutic process and to invest
goal and, in this function, form a meaningful unit of
a lot to change their lives and their way of behavior.
behavior. Motivational research seeks to explain these
However, some patients are ambivalent during dif-
units of behavior in terms of their whys and hows.
ferent stages of the therapeutic process. They may be
Consequently, motivational processes also have a ambivalent about whether they should start therapy,
central importance for the change of experience whether they should frame a certain kind of behav-
and behavior, which is the main purpose of psycho- ior as a problematic behavior, or whether they
therapy. Psychotherapy patients seek help for the should take steps to change a problematic behavior.
parts of their lives that they failed to cope with All psychotherapists are confronted with these vari-
themselves. Therapists strive to optimally assist their ations of therapy motivation in their patients, and
patients to change behaviors and experiences to they know that it is useful to skillfully deal with
enable them to live independently after therapy. patients who show reduced therapy motivation.
Because motivational issues are central to human However, a broader perspective on motivation in
life, they are also central to the challenging endeavor psychotherapy indicates that it might be useful to
of psychotherapy. Assisting patients to change consider motivational issues in the treatment of all
417
patients, not only those with obvious deficits in Dweck (2017). Overarching meta-needs have also
therapy motivation: Motivational forces influence been suggested, such as a need for meaning (Heine,
the therapeutic relationship, and it may be wise to Proulx, & Vohs, 2006) or consistency as the most
consider them when trying to build a helpful thera- basic component of psychological functioning
peutic alliance. Moreover, specific interventions might (Grawe, 2004).
be tailored to the motivational background of the Within the concept of motives, a general dis-
patients. For example, the specific situation patients tinction can be made between implicit and explicit
are confronted with in exposure therapy might be motives. Implicit motives are generally seen as endur-
chosen not only because of the nature of their avoid- ing individual motive dispositions, whereas explicit
ance behavior, but also because of the important motives refer to goals that are conscious or con-
personal goals the patient strives for. On the most sciously accessible (Heckhausen & Heckhausen,
fundamental level, some authors have identified 2008). The implicit motivational system consists of
insufficient satisfaction of basic psychological needs as a relatively small number of motives (i.e., achieve-
an important factor in the etiology of various psy- ment, power, and affiliation motives) that are uncon-
chological problems and psychological disorders scious, holistically represented and are more directly
(Dweck, 2017; Grawe, 2004; Ryan, 2005; Ryan, linked to emotional processes. It becomes appar-
Deci, Grolnick, & La Guardia, 2006; Vansteenkiste & ent that the concept of implicit motives as used
Ryan, 2013). Accordingly, from this perspective, the by Heckhausen and Heckhausen (2008) is very
overall goal of psychotherapy should be to increase similar to the concept of needs already described,
the degree of satisfaction of psychological needs to and the concept of explicit motives is very similar
reduce psychopathological conditions. to the concept of high-level goals. Another gen-
Therapists dealing with motivational issues in eral motivational construct are values, which are
psychotherapy could profit from a deeper under- “generally [. . .] defined as preferences for certain
standing of basic motivational principles in human outcomes or modes of conduct” (Locke, 2000,
life. Therefore, the first part of our chapter presents p. 250; see also Rokeach, 1973). Such preferences
theoretical and empirical background information can be shared by an entire community (cultural
as well as selected methods of assessing motivational values) or be individual (personal values). The
constructs that might be relevant for psychotherapy concept of personal values is also very similar to
research and practice. The second part of the chap- high-level goals.
ter reviews important theoretical and empirical lit- A central and well-documented assumption of
erature from clinical psychology research pointing to goal-oriented approaches is that, to a considerable
the relevance of motivational variables in psycho- extent, people’s daily behaviors, thoughts, and emo-
therapy and illustrating therapeutic approaches for tions are linked to the pursuit of personal goals and
dealing with patients’ motivation. In the final part are regulated by feedback regarding goal attainment
of this chapter, we will summarize the implications (Austin & Vancouver, 1996; Carver & Scheier, 1996;
of basic and clinical research for a motivationally Klinger, 1977). Personal goals can be defined as
informed psychotherapy. elaborate cognitive representations of what a person
wants to achieve or avoid in his or her current life
Clinically Relevant Motivational Constructs circumstances and are conscious, symbolically rep-
We will briefly introduce the following motiva- resented, and stored in a language-related manner
tional constructs: psychological needs, motives, (Brunstein, Schultheiss, & Grässmann, 1998). It is
personal goals, values, therapy motivation, and treat- assumed that approach goals are developed to satisfy
ment goals. The concept of psychological needs psychological needs, whereas avoidance goals are
implies that everyone has the same needs, everyone developed to prevent these needs from being hurt
must satisfy them, and, if the individual fails to (Grawe, 2004; see also Elliot, 2008, for a review).
satisfy these needs, aversive outcomes such as People may pursue personal goals as diverse in con-
diminished well-being or psychopathology might tent as, for example, making new friends, improv-
be consequences (Flanagan, 2010). Various lists ing their professional situation, learning how to be
of needs have been proposed, for example, self- more spontaneous, trying to be a better parent, or
enhancement, attachment, pleasure, and orientation/ overcoming fear of rejection (Chulef, Read, &
control by Epstein (1990), relatedness, competence, Walsh, 2001). Grosse Holtforth and Grawe (2000)
and autonomy by Deci and Ryan (1985, 1995), or empirically identified the contents of personal goals
acceptance, competence, and predictability by that therapists considered especially relevant for
Schüler, Brandstätter, Wegner, & Baumann, 2015). goals. In the second step (the nomothetic part),
Several alternatives for assessing implicit motives patients rate these individual goals on various dimen-
have been introduced. For example, Brunstein and sions to allow for interindividual comparisons.
Schmitt (2004) developed an Implicit Association Goals can be rated by the participants themselves or
Test (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) to by independent raters (e.g., categorization of goals
assess achievement motivation by evaluating the by content or according to the approach and
strength of association between achievement-related avoidance quality of the goals). Examples of this
adjectives and the self-concept. Slabbinck and col- idiographic–nomothetic approach are the Measure
leagues constructed a pictorial attitude variant of the to Elicit Positive Future Goals and Plans (Vincent,
Implicit Association Test to assess implicit power Boddana, & MacLeod, 2004), the Personal Projects
(Slabbinck, de Houwer, & van Kenhove, 2011) and Matrix (Little, 1983), the Personal Concerns
affiliation (Slabbinck, de Houwer, & van Kenhove, Inventory (Cox & Klinger, 2002), and its immedi-
2012). However, such assessment tools are too ate antecedents, such as the Concern Dimensions
time-consuming for routine clinical use. Questionnaires (Klinger, Barta, & Maxeiner, 1980)
Explicit motivational constructs can be assessed and the Interview Questionnaire (Klinger, 1987).
efficiently using questionnaire methods. A sample These instruments preceded and/or gave rise to
questionnaire to assess two dimensions of therapy other methods such as the Striving Assessment Scale
motivation is the Autonomous and Controlled (Emmons, 1986) and the Goal Assessment Battery
Motivations for Treatment Questionnaire (Zuroff (Karoly & Ruehlman, 1995). All these approaches
et al., 2007). Based on the self-determination make it possible to assess theory-derived indices
theory proposed by Deci and Ryan (1985), the that seek to achieve a multilevel understanding of
Autonomous and Controlled Motivations for goals (e.g., goal importance, goal achievement,
Treatment Questionnaire measures patients’ auton- goal conflict).
omous motivation (identified and integrated) and Furthermore, various interviews to assess personal
controlled motivation (extrinsic and introjected) for goals have been developed (e.g. Adult Intentional
a treatment. An example for the assessment of and Motivational Systems, AIMS; Wadsworth &
values in the interpersonal realm is the Circumplex Ford, 1983), allowing for an extensive description of
Scales of Interpersonal Values (Locke, 2000). The the various goals and their mutual relationships.
principle of a circumplex structure implies that When standardized goal questionnaires (e.g., Elliot &
variables that measure interpersonal relations are Murayama, 2008; Ford & Nichols, 1991; Grosse
arranged around a circle in a two-dimensional space, Holtforth & Grawe, 2000; Grosse Holtforth &
with the dimensions being agency and communion. Grawe, 2003; Grosse Holtforth, Grawe, & Tamcan,
Personal goals are most often assessed in basic 2004; Reiss & Havercamp, 1998; Ryff, 1989) are
research using a combined idiographic–nomothetic utilized, the participants are presented with goals
approach. The first step (the idiographic part) is to that must be evaluated with respect to dimensions
ask the participants to generate a list of personal such as importance, strain, progress, or realization.
Commitment to
Progress in Emotional
pursue personal
goal striving well-being
goals
Attainability of
personal goals
Figure 23.1 Teleonomic model of subjective well-being (Brunstein & Maier, 2002, p. 163, modified, Reprinted with permission).
goal attainability, is assumed to causally influence supports a person’s goals (Brunstein, 1993; Brunstein,
well-being. If someone pursues his or her goals with Dangelmayer, & Schultheiss, 1996; Ruehlman &
commitment (i.e., he or she identifies with the goals Wolchik, 1988).
and feels motivated to realize them) and if his or her However, a central assumption in the teleonomic
life situation facilitates the attainment of these model is that successful goal striving does not inev-
goals, progress in goal striving is more likely. Goal itably lead to happiness and well-being, because the
progress, in turn, is assumed to contribute to the goals also have to fit the person. Goals may be pur-
person’s emotional well-being. sued for extrinsic or intrinsic reasons, and the vari-
The basic assumptions of the teleonomic model of ous contents of goals also seem to make a difference.
emotional well-being have been supported by a mul- Many studies have shown that the pursuit of goals is
titude of research findings (Brunstein, Schultheiss, & especially associated with a sense of well-being if the
Maier, 1999). For example, through longitudinal goals are well integrated into the person’s self-system
studies on various groups of participants (Brunstein, (self-concordance; Sheldon, 2014), that is, if goals are
1999; Maier & Brunstein, 2001, Wiese & Freund, pursued because one has consciously accepted the
2005), it has been demonstrated that people who values underlying such behavior as personally impor-
(a) are strongly committed to strive for their goals tant and meaningful (identified regulation) or because
and (b) view their life circumstances as favorable for the pursuit of these goals is in itself satisfying and
goal striving achieved a greater degree of progress in rewarding (intrinsic regulation). In contrast, goals
goal attainment and greater increase in emotional are not integrated into the self-system if they are
well-being than people who were less committed to mainly pursued because of external reward (extrinsic
goals for which conditions were more unfavorable. regulation) or internal pressure (e.g. feelings of
In addition, people whose goals are in conflict with guilt or embarrassment, introjected regulation; for
each other or are poorly integrated (Michalak, reviews see Deci & Ryan, 2002; Kelly, Mansell, &
Heidenreich, & Hoyer, 2011) or who have goals that Wood, 2015). Whenever goals correspond with a
are abstract and not clearly formulated (Emmons, person’s personal values, interests, or motives or the
1996) show lower subjective well-being and satisfac- pursuit of the goals is itself satisfying, he or she will—
tion with life (for a meta-analysis, see Gray, Ozer, & even in times when the pursuit of goals is fraught
Rosenthal, 2017). Furthermore, dysregulated goal/ with difficulties or exertion—be more able to activate
action identification seems to relate to various psy- emotional resources and thus persist in the pursuit
chological symptoms and disorders such as depres- (e.g., Sheldon, Prentice, Halusic, & Schüler, 2015).
sion or social anxiety in the sense that patients with In addition, pursuing self-concordant goals is expe-
these disorders represent negative events at a more rienced as more effortless (Werner, Milyavskaya,
abstract and decontextualized level than healthy Foxen-Craft, & Koestner, 2016), and self-regulation
subjects (Watkins, 2011). In addition, more goal is supported by a congruence of motives and goals
progress can be also expected if the social network (Müller & Rothermund, 2018). Michalak, Klappheck,
Figure 23.2 The action phase model (Rubicon model) by Heckhausen, Gollwitzer and Weinert (adapted from Grawe, 2004, p. 50,
Reprinted with permission).
24 Motivation in Education
Abstract
This chapter begins with a discussion of the nature of children’s achievement motivation and how it
develops over the school years, with a focus on the competence-related belief, value, goal, interest, and
intrinsic motivation aspects of motivation that have been emphasized in much recent research on
motivation. Following is a discussion of how different aspects of classroom and school practices
influence motivation and how teacher–student relationships and peer relationships impact students’
motivation. Next is a consideration of how school transitions influence students’ motivation,
describing important differences in the structure and organization of schools at different levels of
schooling. The chapter describes some recent intervention work to boost children’s motivation in
different ways. The final section concerns how recommendations from motivation researchers align
with, or in some cases do not align with, state and national policies for improving student achievement.
The chapter concludes with suggestions for future research, such as the need for more research on
motivation in diverse groups of children and the next steps in motivation intervention research, and
provides further thoughts on motivation research–educational policy links.
Keywords: achievement motivation, development of motivation, motivation and instruction,
teacher–student relationships and motivation, peers and motivation, motivational interventions,
motivation policy
443
Further, opportunities for social interactions around math and reading, even when previous performance
learning have been shown to improve children’s is controlled (see Wigfield et al., 2016, for review).
achievement in reading and other areas (e.g., Guthrie, Bandura’s (1997) construct of self-efficacy also
McRae, & Klauda, 2007; Johnson & Johnson, deals with individuals’ sense of competence; how-
2009). Along with social relationships, it is increas- ever, Bandura defined self-efficacy as a generative
ingly clear that the social contexts and organization capacity where different subskills are organized into
of classrooms and schools also have major influences courses of action. Bandura (1997) reviewed research
on students’ motivation and achievement (Nolen & showing that individuals’ efficacy for different
Ward, 2008; Perry, Turner, & Meyer, 2006; achievement tasks are a major determinant of
Wigfield, Eccles, & Rodriguez, 1998). activity choice, willingness to expend effort, and
In this chapter, we discuss children’s motivation persistence in and out of school (see also Schunk &
in school. We begin with a brief discussion of the DiBenedetto, 2016).
belief, value, and goal constructs prevalent in cur- Dweck and her colleagues (see Dweck, 2017;
rent motivation research and how they develop. We Dweck & Master, 2009, for reviews) discussed a
then discuss the influence of different aspects of construct related to competence beliefs, referred to
classrooms and schools on the development of stu- as individuals’ beliefs about intelligence, or mind
dents’ motivation: tasks provided and classroom sets. They proposed that individuals hold one of two
teaching practices, teacher–student relationships, mindsets: an entity or “fixed” mindset in which indi-
peers and learning in groups, and school transitions. viduals believe that their abilities are fixed at a given
Next, we provide a discussion of some intervention level or an incremental or “growth” mindset in which
work based in the motivation theories and constructs individuals believe that their abilities can increase
reviewed in the first section and how these interven- through effort. These beliefs have important impli-
tions impact children’s motivation. The final section cations for motivation, particularly when academic
of the chapter focuses on whether current national activities or tasks become increasingly difficult. When
educational policies are in line with recommenda- individuals hold growth mindsets they continue to
tions from motivation theorists regarding educa- strive for success even when they are struggling to
tional practices that facilitate students’ motivation. master complex tasks. By contrast, individuals
We close with some suggestions for future research. holding fixed mindsets are more likely to give up
quickly when challenged and believe that showing
The Nature of Student Motivation effort indicates to others that one lacks ability.
Researchers have assessed many different constructs Researchers interested in individuals’ control
they posit to be crucial for students’ academic motiva- beliefs initially made a major distinction between
tion. To organize our discussion of these constructs, internal and external locus of control (e.g., Crandall,
we separate them into two broad groups. One group Katkovsky, & Crandall, 1965; Rotter, 1966. Internal
includes individuals’ sense of their competence control means the individual believes that he or she
and agency to achieve different outcomes. Another controls an outcome; external control means the
group concerns intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, outcome is determined by other things. Researchers
interest, values, and goals. Many of these constructs have confirmed the positive association between
are discussed in greater detail in other chapters in internal locus of control and academic achievement
this handbook. (see Findley & Cooper, 1983). Connell and Wellborn
(1991) integrated control beliefs into a broader
Individuals’ Sense of Competence theoretical framework based on the psychological
and Control needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness
Many researchers interested in motivation focus on derived from self-determination theory (Ryan &
students’ beliefs about their ability and efficacy to Deci, 2016). They linked control beliefs to compe-
perform achievement tasks as crucial motivational tence needs: Children who believe they control their
mediators of achievement behavior (e.g., Bandura, achievement outcomes should feel more competent.
1997; Eccles et al., 1983; Wigfield et al., 2016). When the family, peer, and school contexts support
Ability beliefs are children’s evaluations of their children’s autonomy, develop their competence, and
competence in different areas. Researchers have provide positive relations with others, then chil-
documented that children’s and adolescents’ ability dren’s motivation (which Connell and Wellborn,
beliefs relate to and predict their achievement 1991, conceptualized as engagement) will be positive,
performance in different achievement domains like and they will become fully engaged in different
Martin S. Hagger
Abstract
Given the considerable epidemiological evidence linking regular physical activity with good health and
reduced risk of chronic disease, health psychologists have adopted theories and models of motivation
to understand the antecedents and processes that give rise to health-related physical activity. These
theories are important because they provide the basis for the development and evaluation of
interventions aimed at promoting increased physical activity in a largely sedentary population. This
chapter reviews three of the leading theories that have been applied in physical activity contexts: the
theory of planned behavior, self-determination theory, and achievement goal theory. Advances in
research that have aimed to promote better understanding of the factors that underpin motivation in
physical activity and the relevant processes are also reviewed, including implementation intentions,
the increasing importance of psychological needs, and theoretical integration. In addition, the role of
methodological improvements such as the measurement of implicit motivational processes and the
need for gold standard designs when evaluating physical activity interventions based on these theories
are highlighted. Future research must develop hybrid interventions adopting both motivational and
implemental strategies to change physical activity behavior, research should extend knowledge of the
relative contribution of implicit and explicit motivational processes on physical activity behavior,
and investigations to evaluate physical activity interventions should pay careful attention to design
and evaluation.
Keywords: exercise, planned behavior, intention, autonomous motivation, achievement goals, implicit
processes, intervention design
463
of the antecedents and mechanisms to inform and Protogerou, Johnson, & Hagger, 2018). For example,
design interventions aimed at changing behavior self-efficacy, a very important construct in the field
to promote desirable health outcomes (Littlecott, of social psychology and derived from Bandura’s
Moore, Moore, & Murphy, 2014). In this chapter (1986) influential social cognitive theory, is a key
I will review three dominant social psychological component in numerous theories of motivated
approaches to understanding motivation in physical social behavior such as protection motivation theory
activity and review recent advances in the field that (Rogers, 1975) and the theory of planned behavior
have aimed to enhance understanding and advance (Ajzen, 1991), a specific version of the broader rea
knowledge of how to increase motivation and be soned action approach (Fishbein & Ajzen, 2009),
havior in physical activity. I will first review the and both have been applied to physical activity
research on the motivational theories and the con (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & Biddle, 2002b; Rhodes,
tribution such research has made in identifying the Plotnikoff, & Courneya, 2008). Similarly, the con
key constructs that influence physical activity behav struct of intention, which is a motivational con
ior and which have been most effective in explain struct reflecting the degree of effort and planning
ing variance in physical activity behavior: Ajzen’s an individual is prepared to invest in pursuing a
(1991) theory of planned behavior and the reasoned behavior, is also a key component of numerous
action approach (Fishbein & Ajzen, 2009), Deci theories such as the theory of planned behavior,
and Ryan’s (1985b, 2000) self-determination theory, protection motivation theory, the theories of self-
and Nicholls’s (1989) achievement goal theory. I will regulation and trying (Bagozzi & Kimmel, 1995),
briefly review the research adopting these approaches and the theory of goal-directed behavior (Perugini
and evaluate their importance and level of contribu & Conner, 2000). Again, these theories have been
tion to the literature on motivation and physical adopted to explain behavior in a physical activity
activity. Most important, I will identify the advances, context (Bagozzi & Kimmel, 1995; Perugini &
theoretically, that researchers in the physical activity Conner, 2000). Similarly, these theories have differ
domain have made to each of these theories, such as ent assumptions and perspectives. For example,
the use of implementation intention strategies, the attitudinal theories like the theory of planned
adoption of new perspectives on psychological needs behavior are belief based and focus on behavioral
(e.g., need thwarting), and the introduction of a predictions based on estimates of the future out
2 × 2 achievement goal framework. I will also outline comes of a given behavior and individuals’ evaluation
how theoretical integration may benefit theoretical of those outcomes. In contrast, theories such as
research in physical activity contexts. Finally, I will self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985b,
review recent methodological advances in the psy 2000) adopt an organismic approach, steeped in
chology of physical activity, such as the use of im the humanist tradition, focusing on the contextual
plicit motivation and dual process theories of action, influences on motivated behavior and motivational
the importance of randomized controlled trials, orientations derived from the s atisfaction of innate
intervention mapping, and intervention fidelity to psychological needs. In this chapter I will focus on
ensure that the effective components of motivational three dominant motivational theories applied in
interventions to change physical activity can be iden physical activity contexts: the theory of planned be
tified and replicated precisely. havior, self-determination theory, and achievement
goal theory. I will outline how the adoption of these
Three Key Motivational Theories theories has contributed to the understanding of
in Physical Activity physical activity behavior. I will also review how
The psychology of physical activity is a theory-rich these theories might help move the field forward in
discipline with many motivational theories and terms of developing a more comprehensive theory
models proposed to provide comprehensive and of the antecedents and mechanisms of physical
definitive explanations of health behavior (Hagger, activity behavior and informing interventions and
2010; Hagger, Gucciardi, & Chatzisarantis, 2017). practical solutions to increase motivation to partic
It is, however, important to note that many of these ipate in physical activity and promote engagement
theories have similar components and hypotheses, in physical activity behavior.
such that there is considerable overlap in the defini
tions of constructs and the proposed mechanisms The Theory of Planned Behavior
by which these constructs affect physical activity The theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991) as
behavior (Hagger, 2014; McMillan & Conner, 2007; well as the broader framework, the reasoned action
Hagger 465
shows that control beliefs demonstrate considerable significant even after controlling for previous expe
variance across different populations and behaviors. rience. This indicated that previous decision-making
For example, studies in the physical activity domain processes were accounted for by the variables in the
have identified “age” and “fear of having a heart model, but the most recent decision-making vari
attack” among the control beliefs for older and clin ables remained salient as explanations of variance
ical populations (Godin et al., 1991), but these beliefs in physical activity intentions and behavior. It was
do not feature among the control beliefs of younger concluded that
populations, which focus more on inclement weather
while past behavior had a significant and direct
and lack of time (Hagger et al., 2001). Interestingly,
influence on intention, attitude, perceived behavioral
the comparatively limited research examining rela
control, and self-efficacy, these cognitions are also
tions between the indirect belief-based measures and
necessary for translating past decisions about
the direct measures suggests that multiplicative com
behavioral involvement into action. This is consistent
posites of the belief and value systems do not ac
with the notion that involvement in volitional
count for a high degree of variance in the direct
behaviors such as regular physical activity involves
measures of attitudes, subjective norms, and per
both conscious and automatic influences.
ceived behavioral control (Hagger et al., 2001).
(Hagger et al., 2002a, p. 23)
Suggested solutions are to avoid multiplicative com
posites when evaluating beliefs in tests of the theory This evidence indicates the general recognition
and focus on the intensity (expectancy) measures of the theory of planned behavior as an important
alone, an approach that has been advocated and theoretical approach to the understanding of the
used successfully in studies in physical activity and motivational influences on physical activity behav
other behavioral domains (Ajzen & Fishbein, 2008; ior. The considerable attention paid to the theory in
Bélanger-Gravel, Godin, Bilodeau, Poirier, & the literature is attributable to its effectiveness in ac
Dagenais, 2013; French & Hankins, 2003; Hamilton, counting for variance in physical activity intention
Kirkpatrick, Rebar, White, & Hagger, 2017). and behavior, as well as its relative parsimony and
Formative research adopting the theory of planned role as a flexible framework for the study of psycho
behavior in physical activity contexts has demon social influences and processes that underpin physical
strated that attitudes and perceived behavioral con activity behavior. For example, its role as a flexible
trol consistently and significantly predict intentions framework has been supported by research that
and explain approximately equal proportions of the has shown that the attitude, subjective norm, and
variance in physical activity behavior with a sub perceived behavioral control constructs mediate the
stantially lesser role for subjective norms (Hagger & effect of other distal constructs on intentions and
Chatzisarantis, 2005; Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & behavior such as personality (e.g., Chatzisarantis &
Biddle, 2002a). In addition to individual empirical Hagger, 2008; Conner & Abraham, 2001; Conner,
studies, a meta-analysis of 72 studies applying the Rodgers, & Murray, 2007; Hoyt, Rhodes,
theory of planned behavior in physical activity con Hausenblas, & Giacobbi, 2009; Rhodes & Courneya,
texts supported the trends in the physical activity 2003) and other individual difference variables
data across the literature (Hagger et al., 2002a). (Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2007; Fitch & Ravlin,
Using a meta-analytic path analysis, intention was 2005; Hagger, Anderson, Kyriakaki, & Darkings,
found to be the sole proximal predictor of physical 2007). However, researchers have also indicated that
activity and the effects of attitudes and perceived the theory does not account for all of the variance in
behavioral control on intentions were medium in intention and behavior, nor does it mediate the
magnitude and stronger than the effects of subjective effects of certain “external variables” on intentions
norms. In addition, studies that separated measures and behavior (e.g., Bagozzi & Kimmel, 1995;
of self-efficacy (reflecting personal capacity and Conner & Abraham, 2001; Conner & Sparks, 2015;
confidence estimates) and perceived controllability Rhodes & Courneya, 2003). Paradoxically, this
(reflecting perceived barriers) indicated that self- “weakness” has become the theory’s greatest strength.
efficacy explained additional variance in the predic Ajzen (1991) states that the theory should be viewed
tion of both intentions and behavior. Past behavior as a flexible framework into which other variables
also predicted all the theory constructs and atten can be incorporated, provided they make a mean
uated their effects on intention and behavior. ingful and unique contribution to the p rediction of
Nevertheless, the influences of the social cognitive intentions and there is a theoretical precedence for
constructs on intentions and behavior remained the inclusion of such variables.
Hagger 467
One of the key limitations is that the relationship experience motivation to engage in activities and
between intentions and behavior is far from perfect. behaviors as autonomous or controlling will deter
In fact, it frequently falls considerably short of a mine their persistence with the behavior in the future
large effect size and meta-analytic studies have typi and whether they gain certain adaptive outcomes
cally indicated that the relationship between inten such as satisfaction, enjoyment, and psychological
tions and behavior is relatively modest or even weak well-being. Organismic integration theory (OIT), a
(Hagger, Chan, et al., 2016; Hagger et al., 2002b). subtheory of self-determination theory, seeks to
This suggests a considerable shortfall exists in the provide an explanation for the processes by which
numbers of individuals who convert their positive people assimilate behaviors that are externally regu
intentions into actual behavior (Rhodes & de Bruijn, lated and incorporate them into their repertoire of
2013). For example, Rhodes and de Bruijn’s meta- behaviors that are self-determined and integrated
analysis of research examining relations between in into their personal system. Central to OIT is the
tentions and physical activity participation revealed perceived locus of causality, which represents a grad
that 42% of individuals reported having a positive uated continuum of motivational styles or regula-
intention to exercise but failed to participate in tions. The continuum, known as the perceived locus
physical activity subsequently. Numerous reasons of causality, is characterized by two relatively auton
have been cited for this problem, such as a lack of omous forms of motivation, intrinsic motivation and
correspondence between the measures of intention identified regulation, and two relatively controlling
and behavior, the relative instability of intentions, forms of motivation, external regulation and intro-
and the moderating effect of numerous individual jected regulation (Ryan & Connell, 1989). Important
difference factors such as self-schema. These have for researchers and practitioners in the field of phys
been frequently investigated and research has shown ical activity, individuals who act for autonomous
that the intention–behavior “gap” is strengthened reasons are more likely to persist in the absence
under conditions of high intention stability and of discernable external rewards or contingencies.
among self-schematics (Sheeran & Orbell, 2000b). Therefore, if interventions can promote autonomous
However, the relationship remains relatively modest motives for engaging in physical activity among in
in effect size, which means that people frequently dividuals, it is likely to lead to persistence over time
do not convert their “good” intentions to engage in and cede the health benefits of physical activity to
physical activity into actual behavior. Researchers those individuals. The major theoretical tenets of
have therefore sought to develop strategies that might self-determination theory have been outlined in
assist in moderating the intention–behavior relation detail elsewhere in this volume (see Ryan, Ryan, Di
ship, particularly strategies that enable individuals Domenico, & Deci, 2019, Chapter 6), so the present
to convert their good intentions to engage in physical review will focus on the specific application of self-
activity behavior into actual action. These strategies determination in the domain of health-related
and advances will be reviewed in the “Theoretical physical activity.
Advances” section of this chapter. Research adopting the perceived locus of causality
from OIT has shown that autonomous forms of
Self-Determination Theory regulation are positively related to adaptive behav
Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985b, ioral and psychological outcomes in the domain of
2000) is a prominent motivational theory adopted physical activity. Autonomous motivation is asso
to identify the contextual and interpersonal influ ciated with physical activity participation and
ences on human behavior and has received much adherence over time (Chatzisarantis, Biddle, & Meek,
attention in the physical activity literature (Hagger 1997; Chatzisarantis, Hagger, Biddle, Smith, & Wang,
& Chatzisarantis, 2007, 2008). Self-determination 2003; Fortier & Kowal, 2007; Ng et al., 2012), per
theory is actually a metatheory comprising several ceived competence (Goudas, Biddle, & Fox, 1994),
subtheories that seek to explain human motivation physical activity intentions (Hagger, Chatzisarantis,
and behavior on the basis of individual differences & Harris, 2006b; Phillips, Abraham, & Bond, 2003;
in motivational orientations, contextual influences on Wilson & Rodgers, 2004), and psychological well-
motivation, and interpersonal perceptions. Central being (Gunnell, Crocker, Wilson, Mack, & Zumbo,
to self-determination theory is the distinction be 2013). Furthermore, environmental antecedents such
tween self-determined or autonomous forms of moti as autonomy support (Edmunds, Ntoumanis, &
vation relative to non-self-determined or controlling Duda, 2007) and people’s perceptions that the mo
forms of motivation. The extent to which people tivational context is supportive of their autonomous
Hagger 469
avoidance goals (Barkoukis et al., 2007; Conroy & including physical activity, although significant, is
Elliot, 2004). Future research in the physical activ comparatively weak and is further compromised by
ity domain should be directed toward establishing the inclusion of past behavior (Armitage & Conner,
the links between the achievement goals from the 2001; Hagger et al., 2001). Furthermore, meta-
framework and constructs from self-determination analyses of interventions and experimental manipu
theory. For example, there are recognized congruen lations based on the theory of planned behavior
cies between achievement goal and self-determination aimed at changing intentions have corroborated
theories. However, there is increased need to look at these findings, demonstrating substantially larger
the overlap and distinctions in the context of the effects of interventions on intentions than behavior
2 × 2 framework. This will be investigated in more (Webb & Sheeran, 2006). These data present a
detail in the “Integration of Theories” section of problem for interventions based on this theory be
this chapter. cause it seems that even though people may report
that they have good intentions to engage in physical
Theoretical Advances activity, people do not always behave in accordance
Although the three motivational theories have in with their intentions.
formed exercise psychologists’ understanding of the Solutions to this problem have been presented in
factors that influence physical activity behavior and the form of implemental approaches to behavioral
also provided a useful basis for interventions aimed engagement. Heckhausen and Gollwitzer (1987)
at changing physical activity behavior and health- presented an action-phase model that identifies two
related outcomes, questions remain with respect to complementary processes that lead to action: an in
some of the limitations of the theories and the lack tentional (motivational) phase and an implemental
of information or research in particular areas in the (volitional) phase. The intentional phase encom
physical activity context. I outlined some of these passes the processes that lead to the formation of
limitations and needs for research in the previous intentions to engage in a behavior captured aptly in
sections. For example, the theory of planned behav the theory of planned behavior by the antecedents of
ior is limited in that the link between intentions and intention. However, although intentions to engage
behavior was relatively modest; there is relatively in health-related behaviors may be a prerequisite for
limited information on self-determination theory in behavioral engagement, they are not always sufficient.
the role that psychological need satisfaction plays on The implemental phase outlines the process of how
physical activity behavior; and there is little research the identification of critical cues in the environment
on the conceptual and empirical links between the leads to the enactment of intentions and promotes
ories like achievement goal theory, the theory of strong links between the cue and the planned
planned behavior, and self-determination theory in action. Proponents of the action-phase model have
the physical activity domain. In the next two sec proposed that engaging in strategies that highlight a
tions, I will outline recent developments in the field critical situation or contingency in which the be
of motivation in physical activity that attempt to havior will be initiated will be effective in promot
address these outstanding questions with a view to ing behavioral engagement. Such strategies, known
advancing knowledge and understanding of physical as implementation intentions, require people to pro
activity behavior. pose and write down when and where they will
enact their planned behavior (e.g., “if situation Y
Implementation Intention Approaches occurs, then I will perform response Z !”). Such exer
One of the problems with motivational interven cises promote behavioral engagement by promoting
tions based on theories like the theory of planned increased accessibility of the critical cue in the envi
behavior is that their effects on actual behavior ronment (Aarts, Dijksterhuis, & Midden, 1999)
have been relatively modest (Hardeman et al., 2002; and developing a link in memory between the critical
Sheeran et al., 2016; Steinmetz, Knappstein, Ajzen, situation (Y ) and the planned action (Z ) (Brandstätter,
Schmidt, & Kabst, 2016). The limited success of Lengfelder, & Gollwitzer, 2001). When intentions
such interventions has been attributed to the com are furnished with implementation intentions, be
paratively weak relationship between intentions havioral initiation is therefore more efficient, guided
and behavior observed in formative research on the by automatic processes, and less vulnerable to lapses
theory. For example, meta-analyses have demon in memory or reliant on conscious processing.
strated that the average effect size of the intention– Augmenting intentions with implementation in
behavior relationship for many health behaviors, tentions has shown to be effective in promoting
Hagger 471
model. Psychological need satisfaction therefore acts psychological need satisfaction and, as a consequence,
as a distal factor influencing physical activity behav autonomous motivation to engage in physical activ
ior by promoting autonomous forms of motivation ity. Research has demonstrated a significant relation
toward that specific behavior. Support for this process ship between both perceived and actual autonomy
model has been relatively consistent in the literature support and psychological need satisfaction (Hagger
for both physical activity and other health-related et al., 2006a). It may be that thwarted psychological
behaviors. In fact, studies have demonstrated sig needs in certain contexts are not irreparable, and the
nificant indirect effects of satisfaction of the three provision of autonomy support may be most effec
psychological needs on health-related behavior in a tive in bringing about changes in motivation for
physical activity context (Hagger et al., 2006a). This people with low need satisfaction. However, there is
demonstrates the relatively consistent effects of psy relatively little research adopting this approach in a
chological needs and the process model proposed by physical activity context, and there is a clear need
Deci and Ryan (Deci & Ryan, 1985b, 2000) in their for further inquiry in this direction to confirm these
exposition of the theory. hypotheses.
So what does the future hold for research into
psychological needs? A recent approach has been Integration of Theories
to focus on occasions where psychological needs The Theory of Planned Behavior and
remain unsatisfied or are thwarted (Bartholomew, Self-Determination Theory
Ntoumanis, & Thogersen-Ntoumani, 2009; Gunnell Recently, researchers have sought to integrate psy
et al., 2013; Hein, Koka, & Hagger, 2015). This is chosocial models such as the theory of planned
an important emerging area of research and it could behavior with other motivational theories like self-
be argued that it is as important to examine the determination theory. This is because these ap
effects of occasions when individuals in physical ac proaches are deemed to provide complementary
tivity contexts fail to have their psychological needs explanations of the processes that underlie moti
satisfied and whether that impacts their physical vated behavior (Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2016).
activity participation. If a person perceives the phys This is important with regard to the theory of
ical activity domain not to be a context in which his planned behavior because it provides information
or her needs are likely to be satisfied, this will prob as to the origins of the attitudes, subjective norms,
ably have two effects on the person’s behavior in and perceived behavioral control constructs. Several
that domain. First, it would likely lead to an avoid researchers have integrated these approaches in
ance response and desistance from physical activity mediational models to illustrate the processes that
participation and, second, the individual may seek lead to decisions to engage in social behavior. For
the satisfaction of those needs in other behavioral example, self-determined or autonomous motives
domains. These effects are most likely to occur when from self-determination theory have been shown to
the context fails to support needs. Recent evidence directly predict behavioral intentions (Chatzisarantis,
for this comes from some research conducted on Hagger, Biddle, & Karageorghis, 2002; Hagger
adolescent girls’ unhealthy weight-control behaviors et al., 2002a; Wilson & Rodgers, 2004). However,
(such as skipping meals, taking laxatives, and some researchers have tested a more complete model
vomiting). The research demonstrated that low psy in which different regulatory styles of autonomous
chological need satisfaction was associated with and controlled motivation from self-determination
high levels of these behaviors and this was mediated theory predict intentions via the mediation of atti
by body image concerns (Thøgersen-Ntoumani, tudes and perceived behavioral control. This moti
Ntoumanis, & Nikitaras, 2010). However, percep vational sequence has been supported in a number
tions that significant social agents, such as parents, of studies (Chatzisarantis et al., 2002; Hagger et al.,
supported autonomy were positively related to psy 2002a, 2006b).
chological need satisfaction. Researchers have recently The proposition that self-determination theory
proposed a ‘dual process’ model of self-determination (Deci & Ryan, 1985b, 2000) can augment social
theory, arguing that need support and need thwart cognitive theories such as the theory of planned be
ing are separate processes with independent effects havior has been suggested previously, but it has only
on behavior (Jang, Kim, & Reeve, 2016). recently received empirical support. Numerous au
Taken together, these findings indicate that inter thors have proposed that motivational, organismic
ventions that target autonomy-supportive behaviors theories such as self-determination theory could
of social agents may be a useful means to promote potentially offer explanations for the origins of
Hagger 473
(akin to an identified regulation) and, therefore, for and effort (Chatzisarantis et al., 2002). Since this
more autonomous reasons than intentions based on initial research, the indirect effect of autonomous
subjective norms, which reflected more controlling motives from self-determination theory on inten
aspects of motivation such as external or introjected tions and behavior as stipulated by the proposed
regulations. Together, these results paved the way motivational sequence has been corroborated in
for more comprehensive studies in which the ef several studies (Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2016;
fects of self-determined forms of motivation influ Hagger, Sultan, Hardcastle, & Chatzisarantis, 2015),
enced behavior. including the domain of physical activity (Hagger
Following these pioneering studies, researchers et al., 2009; Pihu, Hein, Koka, & Hagger, 2008;
have been committed to comprehensive tests inte Wallhead, Hagger, & Smith, 2010). Recent meta-
grating the theories adopting hypotheses from both analyses of studies integrating these theories have
component theories to address hypotheses relating provided support for the integrated motivational se
to behavior in numerous contexts. Prominent among quence (Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2009, 2016). The
these studies are those that outline a clear motiva analyses provide useful information for the process
tional sequence in which the generalized motiva by which social contexts influence behavior and rec
tional orientations from self-determination theory ommendations for intervention. For example, we
influence constructs from the theory of planned be have shown that interventions can be designed in
havior in a physical activity context (Chatzisarantis such a way to change perceptions at any stage of the
et al., 2002; Hagger et al., 2002a). In such studies, motivational sequence, targeting either autonomous
the theory of planned behavior acts as a conduit for motives as a distal influence on intentions or atti
the effects of autonomous forms of motivation on tudes and perceived control as a proximal influence.
physical activity behavior. The decision-making con This may lead to hybrid interventions that adopt
structs from the theory of planned behavior reflect techniques from both self-determination theory
the formation of plans to engage in physical activity (Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2009) and the theory of
in the future and represent situational motivational planned behavior (Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2005)
orientations toward physical activity behavior. The to promote increased physical activity participation.
self-determination theory motives serve to indicate
a source of information that influences the decision- 2 × 2 Achievement Goal Perspectives and
making process. For example, autonomous forms of Self-Determination Theory
motivation from self-determination theory are hy Achievement goal theory was developed by research
pothesized to influence attitudes from the theory ers interested in examining the effects of young peo
of planned behavior; an autonomous motivational ple’s perceptions of success and failure on motivation
disposition in a particular domain is likely to be in education contexts (Ames, 1992; Nicholls, 1989).
an impetus to the formation of attitudes oriented An important tenet of the theory is that cues from
toward servicing personally valued goals and mediate the social context, known as the motivational climate,
the effects of autonomous motivation on physical have pervasive effects on motivation and behavior.
activity intentions. Two dimensions have emerged from research exam
Hagger et al. (2002a) found that self-determined ining the effects of motivational climate on motiva
forms of motivation affected intentions to engage in tion in educational settings: a task or mastery-oriented
physical activity behavior, but only via the mediation climate and an ego- or performance-oriented climate.
of attitudes and perceived behavioral control. This A mastery-oriented motivational climate tends to
provided support for the hypothesis that autono promote hard work, effort, cooperation, and personal
mous forms of motivation bias individuals’ decision- development among individuals acting in that cli
making in favor of forming attitudes congruent with mate, whereas a performance-oriented climate tends
their personal goals (attitudes) and perceptions that to engender comparisons with others, competition,
the behavior will lead to competence-related out success based on ability, and reward and punishment
comes (perceived behavioral control). This was cor schedules for success and failure. Research in educa
roborated in a subsequent study that furthered these tion has suggested that a mastery-oriented climate
findings to actual behavior. Autonomous motives tends to engender adaptive motivational patterns and
affected behavior via a motivational sequence begin is linked to increased psychological well-being and
ning with autonomous forms of motivation and persistence in behavior (Ntoumanis & Biddle, 1998).
ending with behavioral engagement mediated by The concepts of motivational climate and in
attitudes, perceived behavioral control, intentions, trinsic motivation from self-determination theory
Hagger 475
variables (Hagger, 2016; Hofmann, Friese, & Wiers, developed through experience, and affect motivation
2008). The second focuses on the importance of and behavior in a variety of contexts. Such orienta
using cutting-edge methodological features when tions may moderate the effects of situational factors
designing interventions based on motivational the that support or thwart intrinsic motivation on behav
ories in physical activity research. I will argue for the ior (Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2011; Hagger, Koch, &
consideration of intervention mapping and report Chatzisarantis, 2015). In addition, it has been sup
ing of intervention protocols such that there is clear posed that these causality orientations may affect
congruence between the target theoretical constructs behavior independent of conscious decision-making
and the intervention components as well as the need (Elliot, McGregor, & Thrash, 2002), in much the
for state-of-the-art techniques to establish the effec same way as individual difference and personality
tiveness of the intervention, including treatment constructs tend to influence behaviors independent
fidelity checks. of intentional processes (Conner & Abraham, 2001;
Rhodes, Courneya, & Jones, 2002). Indeed, recent
Implicit Motivation evidence examining mediational models of motiva
Research in social psychology over the past 10 years tion adopting OIT and basic needs theory have
has begun to shift away from models that focus indicated that generalized constructs such as basic
solely on deliberative, intentional, and explicit in need satisfaction predict exercise behavior directly,
fluences on behavior and sought to develop theories independent of contextual motivational orientations
that account for the nonconscious, impulsive, and and intentions (Hagger et al., 2006b). These pro
implicit influences on human behavior (Bargh & cesses therefore transcend the deliberative route by
Chartrand, 1999; Gibbons, Houlihan, & Gerrard, which these psychological constructs lead to behavior
2009; Hagger, 2016; Hofmann et al., 2008; Kehr, and suggest that people’s global causality orientations
2004; Strack & Deutsch, 2004). Such approaches may affect behavior directly, and the process is
have given rise to so-called dual process models of likely to be one of which the person is unaware and
motivation that recognize that behavior is a function therefore is implicit in nature.
of reflective, deliberative, volitional, and planned Recent research has included implicit motivational
inferences as well as those that are impulsive, auto constructs in the prediction of behavior adopting
matic, nonconscious, and unplanned (Deutsch a self-determination theory approach. Levesque and
et al., 2017; Hagger, 2017; Strack & Deutsch, 2004). Pelletier (2003) adopted priming techniques used
Interest in these automatic and implicit processes in previous studies examining implicit processes
has been mirrored by concomitant advances in meth to activate either autonomous or nonautonomous
ods to measure implicit processes. Research adopting (termed heteronomous) motivational orientations.
implicit processes alongside more traditional self- Using this method, they found that priming auton
report measures of cognition has illustrated that be omous and heteronomous motivation influenced
havior is influenced by both explicit and implicit participants’ perceptions of intrinsic motivation,
social cognitive variables and these effects are rela choice, and competence as well as persistence with
tively independent (Hagger, Trost, Keech, Chan, & subsequent problem-solving tasks consistent with
Hamilton, 2017; Hamilton, Kirkpatrick, Rebar, & explicit, consciously regulated motivational orienta
Hagger, 2017; Perugini, 2005). tions. Similarly, Burton, Lydon, D’Alessandro, and
Given the increasing attention being paid to Koestner (2006) used a lexical decision task to meas
implicit processes, recent research has endeavored to ure implicit autonomous motivation and found that
examine the role of implicit processes in self- this measure predicted psychological well-being
determined motivation and behavior. This is based and academic performance independent of explicit
on theoretical premises that suggest that people have measures of autonomous motivation. Together, these
an implicit bias or propensity to approach behaviors studies suggest that the motivational influences from
in an autonomous or controlling manner. For exam self-determination theory can influence behavior
ple, Deci and Ryan (1985a) proposed causality ori and other outcomes implicitly and these effects are
entations theory, which introduced the notion that independent of explicit motivational orientations.
people have a generalized capacity to be oriented Studies have extended this research in the domain
toward and interpret situations as supportive of their of physical activity and explored the role of implicit
self-determination. Therefore, people exhibit inter motivational orientations from self-determination
individual differences in their generalized causality theory on physical activity enactment and the relevant
orientations, which are global and relatively enduring, processes (Keatley, Clarke, & Hagger, 2012, 2013).
Hagger 477
the individuals administering the intervention as it similar to an instruction manual, giving the precise
is outlined in the intervention protocol. This would details of the intervention so that it can be replicated.
require checks to ensure that those administering Two other important methodological issues must
the intervention were keeping to task and that the be considered when it comes to the design, imple
participants reported carrying out the intervention mentation, and evaluation of theory-based physical
correctly and accurately. This is known as treatment activity interventions. First, it is important that in
fidelity (Bellg et al., 2004) and is widely recognized tervention designers include means to evaluate the
as essential to evaluating behavioral interventions in treatment fidelity of the intervention (Bellg et al.,
physical activity contexts (Quested, Ntoumanis, 2004; Quested et al., 2017). This must come in two
Thøgersen-Ntoumani, Hagger, & Hancox, 2017). forms. First, it is important to evaluate whether the
The aforementioned intervention components intervention has caused change in the specific theo
have been termed the “active ingredients” of retical variable or variables targeted by the interven
interventions, and this has received much recent tion, similar to manipulation checks in experimental
attention in the literature. Various “taxonomies” of research. It is therefore essential that the intervention
health-related behavior change intervention com includes not only the primary outcome variables,
ponents have been developed in the general domain whether that be physical activity behavior or any
of health behaviors (Michie et al., 2013) and, specif target outcome variables related to physical activity,
ically, for physical activity (Michie et al., 2011). The but also measures of the psychological variables re
aim of these taxonomies is to provide a systematic lated to the intervention, both before and after the
description of the components of interventions and implementation of the intervention. Second, it is
provide a common terminology for describing and important that interventionists include means to
operationalizing behavior change interventions. This identify whether the intervention has been carried
is an important step forward in terms of assisting out according to the proscribed protocol. If the in
researchers and intervention designers in being more tervention is delivered by a clinician or a social agent,
explicit in identifying the specific components of an example of a fidelity check might include some
interventions that are proposed to be making the sort of observation of a subgroup of the agents de
change in behavior (Michie, 2008). In addition, livering the intervention and coded independently
complimentary approaches identifying links between for the specific behaviors expected of those carrying
the behavior change components and the motiva out the intervention. It is important that this obser
tional constructs assumed to explain or “mediate” vation is compared to similar observations for the
change in behavior have also been developed (Kok social agents executing the control condition com
et al., 2016). These advances are clearly important ponents of the intervention. This will ensure that the
when it comes to translational research aiming to intervention is carried out precisely and effectively
capitalize on the research identifying antecedents in the manner outlined in the protocol.
and mechanisms from motivational theories applied Finally, I mentioned previously the importance
to physical activity contexts (Hagger, 2010; Moss- of including measures relating to the target theory–
Morris & Yardley, 2008). Furthermore, there is now related variables that the intervention components
a specific protocol for the coding of intervention are purported to target as a means to establish the
components that provides a blueprint for mapping effectiveness of the intervention (Hagger et al.,
the intervention components that are the likely active 2009). However, these components are also likely
ingredients of interventions (Michie & Prestwich, to be the salient mediators of intervention compo
2010). This is a tool not only for those conducting nents and will provide an important test of the
systematic reviews and meta-analyses but also for mechanisms by which the intervention affects be
those designing interventions to consider when it havioral outcomes. As an illustration, two of our
comes to pinpointing the components from forma recent interventions adopting theory-based inter
tive research examining psychological correlates ventions have demonstrated the importance of ex
likely to be the most viable target for intervention. amining the psychological mediators of interven
Interventions aimed at changing physical activity tion components on behavior and motivational
behavior should therefore pay careful attention to outcomes in physical activity (Chatzisarantis &
providing clear details of the constructs that are the Hagger, 2005, 2009). For example, in a school-based
targets of interventions (based on formative research), intervention aimed at increasing physical activity
the intervention components that will be adopted behavior among school pupils, we trained teachers
to give rise to the intervention, and a clear protocol, to present their lessons in an autonomy-supportive
Conclusion
In this chapter I have reviewed three important moti References
vational theories that have provided exercise psychol Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not
ogists and those interested in promoting physical ac to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance
tivity behavior in a largely sedentary population with of mundane behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology,
29, 971–979.
important knowledge of the factors that influence Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational
physical activity and the processes by which these fac Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.
tors affect physical activity: the theory of planned be Ajzen, I., Brown, T. C., & Carvahal, F. (2004). Explaining the
havior, self-determination theory, and achievement discrepancy between intentions and actions: The case of
goal theory. Although these theories have had success hypothetical bias in contingent valuation. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1108–1121.
in explaining variance in physical activity behavior Ajzen, I., & Driver, B. E. (1991). Prediction of leisure participation
and serving as the basis for interventions to change from behavioral, normative, and control beliefs: An application
physical activity, there are limitations and shortcom of the theory of planned behavior. Leisure Sciences, 13,
ings in the theories and in current knowledge of the 185–204.
application of these theories to physical activity. Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, F. (2008). Scaling and testing multiplicative
combinations in the expectancy-value model of attitudes.
These limitations include the link between intentions Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38, 2222–2247.
and behavior and the relations between constructs Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student
in the theories. I have therefore reviewed recent ad motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261–271.
vances that have aimed to address these limitations Andersen, S. M., Chen, S., & Carter, C. (2000). Fundamental
and gaps in the research, such as the adoption of im human needs: Making social cognition relevant. Psychological
Inquiry, 4, 269–275.
plementation intentions and theoretical integration. Arbour, K. P., & Martin Ginis, K. A. (2009). A randomised
In addition, I have also highlighted the importance of controlled trial of the effects of implementation intentions
recent methodological advances in implicit motiva on women’s walking behaviour. Psychology & Health, 24,
tional research and the design of interventions in de 49–65.
veloping future research in physical activity behavior Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. T. (1999). The theory of planned
behavior: Assessment of predictive validity and “perceived
and advancing knowledge and understanding of control.” British Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 35–54.
physical activity behavior. I think the overall message Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2000). Attitudinal ambivalence:
of this chapter, distilling the research on motivation A test of three key hypotheses. Personality and Social
in physical activity, is that high-quality and innova Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1421–1432.
tive research not only is moving motivational theory Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of
planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of
forward but also has genuine application and practi Social Psychology, 40, 471–499.
cal relevance to interventionists and policymakers to Bagozzi, R. P., & Kimmel, S. K. (1995). A comparison of leading
adopt to promote physical activity in populations theories for the prediction of goal directed behaviours. British
and produce healthier lifestyles. Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 437–461.
Hagger 479
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Biddle, S. J. H., & Meek, G. A. (1997).
social-cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. A self-determination theory approach to the study of
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable intentions and the intention–behaviour relationship in
automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479. children’s physical activity. British Journal of Health Psychology,
Barkoukis, V., Ntoumanis, N., & Nikitaras, N. (2007). 2, 343–360.
Comparing dichotomous and trichotomous approaches to Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2005). Effects of a
achievement goal theory: An example using motivational brief intervention based on the theory of planned behavior
regulations as outcome variables. British Journal of Educational on leisure time physical activity participation. Journal of
Psychology, 77, 683–702. Sport and Exercise Psychology, 27, 470–487.
Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., & Thogersen-Ntoumani, Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2007). Mindfulness
C. (2009). A review of controlling motivational strategies and the intention–behavior relationship within the theory of
from a self-determination theory perspective: Implications planned behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
for sports coaches. International Review of Sport and Exercise 33, 663–676.
Psychology, 2, 215–233. Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2008). Influences of
Bauer, U. E., Briss, P. A., Goodman, R. A., & Bowman, B. A. personality traits and continuation intentions on physical
(2014). Prevention of chronic disease in the 21st century: activity participation within the theory of planned behaviour.
Elimination of the leading preventable causes of premature Psychology and Health, 23, 347–367.
death and disability in the USA. The Lancet, 384, 45−52. Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2009). Effects of an
Bauman, A., Bull, F., Chey, T., Craig, C. L., Ainsworth, B. E., intervention based on self-determination theory on self-
Sallis, J. F., . . . IPS Group. (2009). The International Prevalence reported leisure-time physical activity participation.
Study on Physical Activity: Results from 20 countries. Psychology and Health, 24, 29–48.
International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hagger, M. S., Biddle, S. J. H., &
Activity, 6, 21. Karageorghis, C. (2002). The cognitive processes by which
Bélanger-Gravel, A., Godin, G., & Amireault, S. (2013). A meta- perceived locus of causality predicts participation in physical
analytic review of the effect of implementation intentions on activity. Journal of Health Psychology, 7, 685–699.
physical activity. Health Psychology Review, 7, 23–54. Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hagger, M. S., Biddle, S. J. H., Smith,
Bélanger-Gravel, A., Godin, G., Bilodeau, A., Poirier, P., & B., & Wang, C. K. J. (2003). A meta-analysis of perceived
Dagenais, G. R. (2013). Physical activity beliefs among locus of causality in exercise, sport, and physical education
overweight/obese older adults: Results from a theory of contexts. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25, 284–306.
planned behavior elicitation study. International Journal of Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hagger, M. S., & Thøgersen-
Sport Psychology, 44, 145–159. Ntoumani, C. (2008). Effects of implementation intentions
Bellg, A. J., Borrelli, B., Resnick, B., Hecht, J., Minicucci, D. S., and self concordance on health behavior. Journal of Applied
Ory, M., . . . Czajkowski, S. (2004). Special NIH committee Biobehavioral Research, 13, 198–214.
report—Enhancing treatment fidelity in health behavior Conner, M., & Abraham, C. (2001). Conscientiousness and the
change studies: Best practices and recommendations from theory of planned behavior: Toward a more complete model
the NIH behavior change consortium. Health Psychology, 23, of the antecedents of intentions and behavior. Personality and
443–451. Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1547–1561.
Biddle, S. J. H., Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Conner, M. T., McEachan, R., Taylor, N., O’Hara, J., &
Lippke, S. (2007). Theoretical frameworks in exercise Lawton, R. (2015). Role of affective attitudes and anticipated
psychology. In G. Tenenbaum & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), affective reactions in predicting health behaviors. Health
Handbook of sport psychology (3rd ed., pp. 537–559). New Psychology, 34, 642–652.
York, NY: Wiley. Conner, M., Rodgers, W., & Murray, T. (2007). Conscientiousness
Blanchard, C. M., Kupperman, J., Sparling, P., Nehld, E., and the intention–behavior relationship: Predicting exercise
Rhodes, R. E., Courneya, K. S., . . . Rupph, J. C. (2008). behavior. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 29, 518–533.
Ethnicity and the theory of planned behavior in an exercise Conner, M. T., & Sparks, P. (2015). The theory of planned
context: A mediation and moderation perspective. Psychology behavior and reasoned action approach. In M. T. Conner &
of Sport and Exercise, 9, 527–545. P. Norman (Eds.), Predicting and changing health behaviour:
Boyd, M. P., Weinmann, C., & Yin, Z. (2002). The relationship Research and practice with social cognition models (3rd ed.,
of physical self-perceptions and goal orientations to pp. 142–188). Maidenhead, UK: Open University Press.
intrinsic motivation for exercise. Journal of Sport Behavior, Conroy, D. E., & Elliot, A. J. (2004). Fear of failure and
25, 1–18. achievement goals in sport: Addressing the issue of the
Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). chicken and the egg. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 17, 271–285.
Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Cumming, J., & Hall, C. (2004). The relationship between goal
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960. orientation and self-efficacy for exercise. Journal of Applied
Burton, K. D., Lydon, J. E., D’Alessandro, D., & Koestner, R. Social Psychology, 34, 747–763.
(2006). The differential effects of intrinsic and identified Cury, F., Biddle, S. J. H., Famose, J. P., Goudas, M., Sarrazin, P., &
motivation on well-being and performance: Prospective, Durand, M. (1996). Personal and situational factors
experimental, and implicit approaches to self-determination influencing intrinsic interest of adolescent girls in school
theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 750–762. physical education: A structural equation modeling analysis.
Butler, R. (1987). Task-involving and ego-involving properties of Educational Psychologist, 16, 305–315.
evaluation: Effects of different feedback conditions on Darker, C. D., French, D. P., Longdon, S., Morris, K., & Eves,
motivational perceptions, interest, and performance. Journal F. F. (2007). Are beliefs elicited biased by question order? A
of Educational Psychology, 79, 474–482. theory of planned behaviour belief elicitation study about
Hagger 481
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2008). Self- in excess of guideline limits among undergraduate students.
determination theory and the psychology of exercise. British Journal of Health Psychology, 17, 18–43.
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 79–103. Hagger, M. S., Luszczynska, A., de Wit, J., Benyamini, Y.,
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2009). Integrating Burkert, S., Chamberland, P.-E., . . . Gollwitzer, P. M. (2016).
the theory of planned behaviour and self-determination Implementation intention and planning interventions in
theory in health behaviour: A meta-analysis. British Journal health psychology: Recommendations from the Synergy
of Health Psychology, 14, 275–302. expert group for research and practice. Psychology & Health,
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2011). Causality 31, 814–839.
orientations moderate the undermining effect of rewards on Hagger, M. S., Sultan, S., Hardcastle, S. J., & Chatzisarantis,
intrinsic motivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, N. L. D. (2015). Perceived autonomy support and
47, 485–489. autonomous motivation toward mathematics activities in
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2014). Integrating educational and out-of-school contexts is related to mathematics
the theory of planned behaviour and self-determination homework behavior and attainment. Contemporary Educational
theory in health behaviour: A meta-analysis. Psychology & Psychology, 41, 111–123.
Health, 24, 39–39. Hagger, M. S., Trost, N., Keech, J., Chan, D. K. C., &
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2016). The trans- Hamilton, K. (2017). Predicting sugar consumption:
contextual model of autonomous motivation in education: Application of an integrated dual-process, dual-phase model.
Conceptual and empirical issues and meta-analysis. Review of Appetite, 116, 147–156.
Educational Research, 86, 360–407. Hamilton, K., Kirkpatrick, A., Rebar, A., & Hagger, M. S.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2017). Child sun safety: Application of an integrated
(2002a). The influence of autonomous and controlling behavior change model. Health Psychology, 36, 916–926.
motives on physical activity intentions within the theory of Hamilton, K., Kirkpatrick, A., Rebar, A., White, K. M., &
planned behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, Hagger, M. S. (2017). Protecting young children against skin
283–297. cancer: Parental beliefs, roles, and regret. Psycho-Oncology,
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Biddle, S. J. H. 26, 2135–2141.
(2002b). A meta-analytic review of the theories of reasoned Hardcastle, S. J., Fortier, M. S., Blake, N., & Hagger, M. S.
action and planned behavior in physical activity: Predictive (2017). Identifying content-based and relational techniques
validity and the contribution of additional variables. Journal to change behavior in motivational interviewing. Health
of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24, 3–32. Psychology Review, 11, 1–16.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Biddle, S. J. H., & Hardeman, W., Johnston, M., Johnston, D. W., Bonetti, D.,
Orbell, S. (2001). Antecedents of children’s physical activity Wareham, N. J., & Kinmonth, A. L. (2002). Application of
intentions and behaviour: Predictive validity and longitudinal the theory of planned behaviour change interventions: A
effects. Psychology and Health, 16, 391–407. systematic review. Psychology and Health, 17, 123–158.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Harris, J. (2006a). Harris, K. C., Kuramoto, L. K., Schulzer, M., & Retallack, J. E.
From psychological need satisfaction to intentional behavior: (2009). Effect of school-based physical activity interventions
Testing a motivational sequence in two behavioral contexts. on body mass index in children: A meta-analysis. Canadian
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 131–138. Medical Association Journal, 180, 719–726.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Harris, J. (2006b). Head, K. J., & Noar, S. M. (2014). Facilitating progress in health
The process by which relative autonomous motivation affects behaviour theory development and modification: The
intentional behavior: Comparing effects across dieting and reasoned action approach as a case study. Health Psychology
exercise behaviors. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 306–320. Review, 8, 34–52.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hein, V., Pihu, M., Soós, Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents
I., Karsai, I., . . . Leemans, S. (2009). Teacher, peer, and parent and cognitive functioning in motivational and volitional
autonomy support in physical education and leisure-time states of mind. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 101–120.
physical activity: A trans-contextual model of motivation in Hein, V., & Hagger, M. S. (2007). Global self-esteem, goal
four cultures. Psychology and Health, 24, 689–711. achievement orientations and self-determined behavioural
Hagger, M. S., Gucciardi, D. F., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. regulations in physical education setting. Journal of Sports
(2017). On nomological validity and auxiliary assumptions: Sciences, 25, 149–259.
The importance of simultaneously testing effects in social Hein, V., Koka, A., & Hagger, M. S. (2015). Relationships between
cognitive theories applied to health behavior and some perceived teachers’ controlling behaviour, psychological need
guidelines Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1933. thwarting, anger and bullying behaviour in high-school
Hagger, M. S., Hein, V., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2011). students. Journal of Adolescence, 42, 103–114.
Achievement goals, physical self-concept and social physique Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Wiers, R. W. (2008). Impulsive
anxiety in a physical activity context. Journal of Applied Social versus reflective influences on health behavior: A theoretical
Psychology, 41, 1299–1339. framework and empirical review. Health Psychology Review, 2,
Hagger, M. S., Koch, S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2015). The 111–137.
effect of causality orientations and positive competence- Hoyt, A. L., Rhodes, R. E., Hausenblas, H. A., & Giacobbi,
enhancing feedback on intrinsic motivation: A test of additive P. R., Jr. (2009). Integrating five-factor model facet-level
and interactive effects. Personality and Individual Differences, traits with the theory of planned behavior and exercise.
72, 107–111. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 10, 565–572.
Hagger, M. S., Lonsdale, A. J., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. Jang, H., Kim, E. J., & Reeve, J. (2016). Why students become
(2012). A theory-based intervention to reduce alcohol drinking more engaged or more disengaged during the semester:
Hagger 483
and deepening the theory of planned behavior. British Journal planned behavior: A meta-analysis of anticipated affect and
of Social Psychology, 40, 79–98. moral norms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39,
Perugini, M., & Conner, M. (2000). Predicting and understanding 2985–3019.
behavioral volitions: The interplay between goals and Rogers, R. W. (1975). A protection motivation theory of fear
behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 705–731. appeals and attitude change. Journal of Psychology, 91, 93–114.
Phillips, P., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2003). Personality, Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality
cognition, and university students’ examination performance. and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two
European Journal of Personality, 17, 435–448. domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
Pihu, M., Hein, V., Koka, A., & Hagger, M. S. (2008). How 749–761.
students’ perceptions of teacher’s autonomy-supportive Ryan, R. M., Ryan, W. S., Di Domenico, S. I., & Deci, E. L.
behaviours affect physical activity behaviour: An application (2019). The nature and the conditions of human flourishing:
of trans-contextual model. European Journal of Sport Science, Self-determination theory and basic psychological needs. In
8, 193–204. R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of human motivation
Pintrich, P. (2000). An achievement goal theory perspective (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
on issues in motivation terminology, theory, and research. Scholz, U., Schuz, B., Ziegelmann, J. R., Lippke, S., &
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 92–104. Schwarzer, R. (2008). Beyond behavioural intentions:
Prestwich, A., Ayres, K., & Lawton, R. (2008). Crossing two types Planning mediates between intentions and physical activity.
of implementation intentions with a protection motivation British Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 479–494.
intervention for the reduction of saturated fat intake: A Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention–behavior relations: A conceptual
randomized trial. Social Science & Medicine, 67, 1550–1558. and empirical review. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.),
Prestwich, A., Conner, M., Lawton, R., Bailey, W., Litman, J., European review of social psychology (pp. 1–36). London, UK:
& Molyneaux, V. (2005). Individual and collaborative Wiley.
implementation intentions and the promotion of breast self- Sheeran, P., & Conner, M. T. (2017). Improving the translation
examination. Psychology & Health, 20, 743–760. of intentions into health actions: The role of motivational
Prestwich, A., Lawton, R., & Conner, M. (2003). The use of coherence. Health Psychology, 36, 1065-1073.
implementation intentions and the decision balance sheet in Sheeran, P., Maki, A., Montanaro, E., Avishai-Yitshak, A., Bryan,
promoting exercise behaviour. Psychology and Health, 18, A., Klein, W. M. P., . . . Rothman, A. J. (2016). The impact of
707–721. changing attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy on health-related
Prestwich, A., Perugini, M., & Hurling, R. (2009). Can the intentions and behavior: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology,
effects of implementation intentions on exercise be enhanced 35, 1178–1188.
using text messages? Psychology & Health, 24, 677–687. Sheeran, P., Norman, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Evidence that
Protogerou, C., Johnson, B. T., & Hagger, M. S. (2018). An intentions based on attitudes better predict behaviour than
integrated model of condom use in sub-Saharan African intentions based on subjective norms. European Journal of
youth: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 37, 586–602. Social Psychology, 29, 403–406.
Quested, E., Ntoumanis, N., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Hagger, Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Implementation intentions and
M. S., & Hancox, J. (2017). Evaluating quality of repeated behaviour: Augmenting the predictive validity of
implementation in physical activity interventions based on the theory of planned behaviour. European Journal of Social
theories of motivation: Current challenges and future Psychology, 29, 349–369.
directions. International Review of Sport and Exercise Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000a). Self schemas and the theory of
Psychology, 10, 252–269. planned behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30,
Rawsthorne, L. J., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Achievement goals and 533–550.
intrinsic motivation: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000b). Using implementation
Social Psychology Review, 3, 326–344. intentions to increase attendance for cervical cancer screening.
Rhodes, R. E., & Courneya, K. S. (2003). Relationships between Health Psychology, 19, 283–289.
personality, an extended theory of planned behaviour model Sheeran, P., Orbell, S., & Trafimow, D. (1999). Does the temporal
and exercise behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 8, stability of behavioral intentions moderate intention–behavior
19–36. and past behavior–future behavior relations? Personality and
Rhodes, R. E., Courneya, K. S., & Jones, L. W. (2002). Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 721–730.
Personality, the theory of planned behavior, and exercise: A Sheeran, P., Trafimow, D., Finlay, K. A., & Norman, P. (2002).
unique role for extroversion’s activity facet. Journal of Applied Evidence that the type of person affects the strength of the
Social Psychology, 32, 1721–1736. perceived behavioural control–intention relationship. British
Rhodes, R. E., & de Bruijn, G. J. (2013). How big is the physical Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 253–270.
activity intention–behaviour gap? A meta-analysis using the Sheldon, K. M. (2002). The self-concordance model of health
action control framework. British Journal of Health Psychology, goal striving: When personal goals correctly represent the
18, 296–309. person. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-
Rhodes, R. E., Plotnikoff, R., & Courneya, K. (2008). Predicting determination research (pp. 65–86). Rochester, NY: University
the physical activity intention–behavior profiles of adopters of Rochester Press.
and maintainers using three social cognition models. Annals Sheldon, K. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y., & Kasser, T. (2001). What
of Behavioral Medicine, 36, 244–252. is satisfying about satisfying events? Testing 10 candidate
Rivis, A., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Descriptive norms as an psychological needs. Journal of Personality and Social
additional predictor in the theory of planned behaviour: A Psychology, 80, 325–339.
meta-analysis. Current Psychology, 22, 218–233. Sniehotta, F. F., Presseau, J., & Araújo-Soares, V. (2014). Time to
Rivis, A., Sheeran, P., & Armitage, C. J. (2009). Expanding retire the theory of planned behaviour. Health Psychology
the affective and normative components of the theory of Review, 8, 1–7.
Hagger 485
CH A PT ER
Motivational Processes in Youth Sport
26 and Physical Activity
Abstract
Millions of children and adolescents participate in organized sports and physical activities worldwide.
Many in the general public, including parents, coaches, and policymakers, believe that participating in
sport enables opportunities for youth to gain confidence, learn teamwork, and attain life lessons.
However, positive outcomes are not an automatic consequence of participating in sport, illuminated by
reports of negative coaching behaviors, overinvolved parents, and competitive rivalries with teammates
and opponents. The potential for sport to improve youth participants’ perceptions of competence,
relatedness, enjoyment, and self-determined motivation is highly dependent on the quality of
interactions and relationships with important adults and peers. This chapter reviews the literature on
motivational processes in youth sport and physical activity with a specific focus on beliefs and behaviors
by parents, siblings, peers, and coaches that promote and sustain positive psychosocial and behavioral
outcomes for youth participants. Robust findings are translated to evidence-based best practices for
stakeholders in youth sport, and future research directions are offered to delve further into the
conditions that undergird positive motivational outcomes in youth sport and physical activity.
Keywords: children, adolescents, sport, motivation, parents, siblings, peers, coaches
The significance of organized youth sport is etched back nearly 100 years (M. R. Weiss, 2016; Wiggins,
in history (Story, 1995; Wiggins, 2013). Early on, 2013). Interest has delved into the conditions that
sport served as a sanctuary for burgeoning immigrants encourage motivation to participate so that youth
seeking to socialize into American society and as a can accrue potential physical, social, and psycholog-
means to form close friendships, show autonomy ical benefits. In the previous edition of our chapter
from adult authority, and create order amid a chaotic (M. R. Weiss, Amorose, & Kipp, 2012), we compre-
lifestyle associated with industrialization. Later, hensively reviewed how the beliefs and behaviors
20th-century sport became an adult-organized of parents, peers, and coaches affect youths’ self-
enterprise and was viewed as a means to teach skills, perceptions, affective responses, and motivational
instill confidence, and build character, mostly for outcomes in a broad range of physical activity set-
boys. Today, after-school sport and physical activity tings. We update our earlier review by focusing on
programs are ubiquitous among boys and girls ages studies conducted in out-of-school-time sport and
5 to 18 years. Millions of children and youth partake physical activity programs, which enables us to clearly
in traditional team and individual sports as well as convey findings and implications in these contexts.
youth development programs that strive to system- Thus, studies in physical education and unstructured
atically teach life skills (M. R. Weiss, 2016). physical activity are not included. We also exclude
Because of the long-standing stature of sport in most studies of elite child athletes because this popu-
the lives of youth, academic study of the positive lation is unique and minuscule compared to the
and negative consequences of participation dates general population of youth participating in sport.
487
We define motivation as reasons for initiating, social acceptance and approval (be with and make
sustaining, and discontinuing participation in an friends, be recognized by coaches), and (c) to experi-
activity (M. R. Weiss & Amorose, 2008). We examine ence enjoyment (have fun, be challenged). Similarly,
motivation as a cognitive construct in the form of studies on attrition from sport indicated that lack of
motivational orientations—intrinsic, extrinsic, self- improving skills, experiencing negative coaching,
determined, and controlling—and as a behavioral and not having fun paralleled competence, social,
construct, such as choice, avoidance, effort, and and enjoyment reasons for initially participating
persistence. In addition to motivational orienta- (M. R. Weiss & Amorose, 2008). These common
tions and behaviors, we identify related antecedents, motives for participation led to adopting theories
mediators, and outcomes of motivation in youth that highlight perceptions of competence, social rela-
sport. These include perceived competence, auton- tionships, and positive affect as contributors to moti-
omy, relatedness, and enjoyment (Stuntz & Weiss, vational orientations and behaviors (M. R. Weiss &
2010) and similar constructs with different labels Phillips, 2015).
(expectancies of success, perceived control, social Four theories predominate research on motiva-
support, positive affect) depending on the theory. tional processes in youth sport—competence moti-
Our review includes the factors that influence all vation theory (Harter, 1978), self-determination
these motivational constructs. theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985), achievement goal theory
Experiences in sport and physical activity are (Nicholls, 1989), and expectancy-value theory
developmentally significant by providing youth (Eccles et al., 1983). We refer readers to our previous
with opportunities to develop attitudes, behaviors, chapter (M. R. Weiss et al., 2012) for in-depth
and skills that enable them to successfully function explanations of these theories. Here, we focus on
now and in the future. Understanding why youth the common threads of similarity that map onto
initiate and maintain—or conversely cease—partic- consistent findings of participation motives. First,
ipation can provide information on how to opti- although each theory has unique construct labels,
mize physical, social, and psychological benefits all identify perceptions of competence (or perceived
afforded from involvement. Thus, we explore social, ability or expectations of success) as a strong deter-
environmental, and contextual influences on moti- minant of motivational outcomes. Second, each
vational orientations, behaviors, and associated theory highlights influence by significant adults and
constructs in youth sport and physical activity. First, peers (or socializers’ approval, modeling, and rein-
we summarize the early descriptive research on par- forcement; autonomy-supportive and controlling
ticipation motivation, which unveiled reasons for behaviors; or task- and ego-involving motivational
becoming and staying involved and led to selected climate) on psychological constructs (perceived
theories for guiding research, which we also review. competence, autonomy or control, relatedness, goal
Second, we describe consistent findings on sources orientations) and motivational orientations and
and mechanisms of influence on youth motivation— behaviors. Finally, all acknowledge positive affect
family (parents, siblings), peers, and coaches—and (or joy or interest value) as a source, mediator, or
offer future research directions. Third, we translate outcome of motivation. As such, these commonali-
theory and research to applications for promoting ties align with competence, social, and enjoyment
self-determined motivation and participation. We motives for participating in sport.
conclude that positive experiences in youth sport These four theories accentuate the social–environ-
are not automatic but can only be realized when mental (e.g., parental behaviors, friendship quality,
social–environmental conditions meet the develop- coaching style) and intrapersonal factors (e.g., per-
mental interests and needs of youth. ceived competence, task values, perceived autonomy,
goal orientations) that explain motivational orien-
Youth Sport Motivation: From Descriptive tations and behaviors (e.g., effort, participation).
to Theory-Driven Studies Theories diverge slightly in how they construe moti-
Early research on participation motivation was purely vational orientation. Competence motivation entails
descriptive in an effort to uncover major reasons for whether youth are intrinsic or extrinsic in motivational
involvement (M. R. Weiss, 2013). Several studies orientation regarding preference for challenging
revealed robust motives for participating that cut versus easy tasks and independent mastery versus
across age, gender, and sport type: (a) to develop reliance on others for help (Harter, 1981). Achievement
and demonstrate physical competence (learn and goal theorists assess intrinsic and e xtrinsic orienta-
improve skills, do something well), (b) to attain tions to participate in accordance with task and ego
27 Work Motivation
Where do the Different Perspectives Lead Us?
Abstract
Work motivation is a crucial, yet complex resource for employees and organizations. Scholars have
investigated motivation at work through many theoretical lenses that are often examined in isolation
from one another. This chapter seeks to bridge these various perspectives, first by providing a review
of dominant theoretical lenses and second by presenting an integrative framework. The historical
review includes a consideration of reinforcement theory, Maslow’s need hierarchy, valence–
instrumentality–expectancy theory, the theory of planned behavior, goal-setting theory, self-regulation
theories, achievement goal theory, regulatory fit theory, and self-determination theory. Together, these
theories identify key mechanisms through which work motivation directs and regulates behavior, as
well as antecedents and consequences of motivation. The integrative motivation framework distills
insights from the various motivational theories, providing a heuristic to understand what (goal choice:
characteristics and content), how (goal striving: macro- and microprocesses), where, and when
(antecedents: personal and contextual) employees will be motivated to work.
Keywords: employee motivation, rewards, values, needs, goal striving, goal choice, optimal
functioning
What do you pursue at work? Why do you want to psychology over the past 100 years (Kanfer, Frese, &
achieve this goal? And when and where are you Johnson, 2017).
most likely to go for it? These questions have been However, employee motivation has also been
central in guiding the literature on work motivation, shown to be complex (Diefendorff & Chandler,
which aims to understand the processes that affect 2011), and through the years, many different per-
the direction, intensity, and persistence of activities spectives have been developed to understand moti-
at work (Pinder, 2008). Employee motivation not vation at work. Although most theories provide an
only is crucial for employee behavior, but also influ- in-depth analysis of more narrow aspects of employee
ences a variety of other outcomes such as employee motivation (Maslow, 1943; Vroom, 1964), some
well-being and attitudes (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Gagné theories have adopted a broader, more encompass-
& Deci, 2005). Hence, employee motivation has ing approach (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Locke & Latham,
the potential to contribute to organizational success, 1990) (see Kanfer et al., 2017). Efforts to integrate
defined in multiple ways and through multiple path- these theories have reviewed motivation from
ways (Jiang, Lepak, Hu, & Baer, 2012). Given its key perspectives (Grant & Shin, 2012), focused
importance for employees and employers alike, it on particular publication outlets (Kanfer &
may come as no surprise that the study of work Chen, 2016; Kanfer et al., 2017), or emphasized
motivation has been one of the most enduring and key elements in the motivational process across
compelling topics in industrial and organizational theories (Diefendorff & Chandler, 2011).
507
Our first goal is to provide a historical overview behavior does not necessarily stem from hidden
of arguably the key theoretical perspectives that have mental processes. Rather, it can be directed and inten-
dominated the study of (work) motivation: rein- sified through external reinforcement and the moti-
forcement theory (Thorndike, 1911), Maslow’s need vational properties of such reinforcement can be
hierarchy (Maslow, 1943), valence–instrumentality– found by observing behavior. Reinforcement theory,
expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964), the theory of which is grounded in the behaviorist tradition
planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991), goal-setting theory (Thorndike, 1911; Skinner, 1969), assumes that
(Locke & Latham, 1990), and self-regulation theo- people can learn and enact (new) behaviors based
ries (e.g. Carver & Scheier, 1982; Frese & Zapf, 1994; on the consequences of their behavior: Behavior
Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989; Lord, Diefendorff, that is followed by instrumental gains is usually inten-
Schmidt, & Hall, 2010), as well as achievement goal sified, while behavior that is not rewarded or even
theory (Dweck, 1986; Elliot, 1999), regulatory fit punished tends to fade out.
theory (Higgins, 1997), and self-determination theory Studies have shown that the impact of rewards
(Deci & Ryan, 2000). This allows us to understand depends on the type of rewards (e.g. monetary
how the concept of motivation has developed and reward, prize, or verbal feedback, recognition) and
changed over the years. When available, we consider the way the reward is administered (e.g., expected or
each of the perspectives based on the available meta- not, salient during task performance or not, control-
analytic results, instead of focusing on particular ling or informative), as well as the type of outcome
findings from individual empirical studies. under consideration (e.g. performance, interest, cre-
The overview then gives insight into how these ativity), the context of the research (e.g., laboratory
theories have provided the basic principles needed studies versus applied studies in a school, sports, or
to comprehensively understand work motivation in work), and the type of sample (e.g., children, adults)
the early 21st century, that is: (a) To which goals and (see Byron & Khazanchi, 2012; Cameron, Banko, &
activities do they direct their behavior? How do Pierce, 2001; Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999;
they choose between activities? And how do indi- Eisenberger & Cameron, 1996). Remarkably few
viduals reprioritize which goal should be the focus studies have explicitly examined the impact of
of attention and receive valuable, limited resources monetary rewards on employee performance. This
at a given moment? (b) How do individuals regulate limited set of studies shows that extrinsic incentives
their behavior to reach their goals over time and are associated with better employee performance
across changing circumstances? How do individuals (Wiersma, 1992), with this effect being larger for
persist over time, respond to obstacles and setbacks, performance quantity than for performance quality
and decide whether to increase effort, attempt differ- (Cerasoli, Nicklin, & Ford, 2014; Jenkins, Mitra,
ent strategies, or temporarily or permanently abandon Gupta, & Shaw, 1998).
the goal? and (c) Where and when does motivation
come to the fore and have its effects on behavior and Needs Theories
well-being? How can employees and organizations In contrast to reinforcement theory, which empha-
facilitate or hinder the motivational processes? By in- sized the importance of the environment, as well as
tegrating these principles in a heuristics framework in observable indicators of motivation, need theories
the second part, this chapter seeks to build a process focused squarely on internal psychological explana-
perspective on employee motivation that aims to en- tions for motivation. Both the theories on essential
hance our understanding of what, why, where, and biological drives and the simple stimulus–response
when employees are motivated for work. Before pre- paradigm of behaviorism were criticized by human-
senting this integrative motivational framework, we istic psychologists (e.g., Rogers, 1961). They advo-
first present an overview of the theories that have cated that people are not merely subjected to their
dominated the motivation literature. biological drives or reactive to their environment,
but should be considered inherently active and
Overview of the Theoretical Perspectives growth oriented when they—consciously or uncon-
External Stimuli: Reinforcement Theory sciously—strive to satisfy their psychological needs.
Before 1900, motivation was said to be determined According to Maslow (1943), these needs can be or-
by will (Descartes), biological features such as in- ganized into a hierarchy of prepotency: Relative
stincts (Darwin), or uncontrollable drives (Freud) satisfaction of a lower need elevates the relative im-
(see for an overview Reeve, 2014). However, these portance of a need higher in the hierarchy. First,
perspectives were vague, and the idea rose that people must satisfy their survival needs, followed by
Characteristics
Personal Level
Fixed characteristics
Stable traits
Momentary states Well-being
Optimal
Attitudes
functioning
Contextual Level
Social Structural Goal Striving
Job Social support Role conflict Behavior
Team Leadership Inter- Macro Micro
dependence
Organization Climate HR
Extrinsic External regulation
Introjected regulation Current status Desired status
Identified regulation
Integrated regulation Comparison
Intrinsic motivation
Feedback Plans
Mastery Mastery
approach goals avoidance goals
Action
Performance Performance
approach goals avoidance goals
Context
Promotion Prevention
focus focus
goal hierarchy, including needs, goals, values, and a reward or punishment varies greatly and depends
even identities at work. Second, and pertaining to on prior personal experiences. Thus, Maslow high-
the question of how employees regulate their behav- lighted the importance of the content of the outcome
ior and allocate their resources, we contend that em- of behavior. Self-determination theory echoes this in-
ployees invest effort and persist in pursuing their sight by arguing intrinsic goals are of higher quality
goals through regulatory behaviors, being macro (more central to the self ) than extrinsic goals.
(e.g., mastery versus performance orientation) or Additionally, VIE further elaborated that other
micro (e.g., action regulation and resource alloca- characteristics of the goals matter too: Employees
tion) in nature. Third, we focus on where and when not only need to value the outcomes of their behav-
motivation arises, considering both the personal ior (valence; defined higher in the goal hierarchy),
and the situational antecedents of work motivation. but also should believe that they can perform at a
Finally, we describe the so what, or outcomes of work particular level (expectancy; defined lower in the goal
motivation in terms of employee optimal function- hierarchy) and that performance will lead to rewards
ing, including well-being, attitudes, and behavior. (instrumentality; strength of connection between a
current goal and higher level goal). Goal-setting
What: A Hierarchy of Goals theory further emphasizes this focus on goal charac-
Across the motivation literature, it is generally ac- teristics by arguing goals should, most of all, be spe-
cepted that employee motivation is made up of the cific and difficult but still attainable. Self-regulation
direction, intensity, and persistence directed toward approaches stressed the importance of goals eliciting
goal pursuit. However, goals are embedded in a hi- a discrepancy between one’s current and desired
erarchy, with much of the why or value of goal pur- states. Such a discrepancy should be large enough to
suit derived by considering goals and outcomes at be detected but not too large for people to abandon
levels higher in the goal hierarchy and much of the the goal. Self-regulation approaches also argued
how of goal pursuit derived by considering goals at goals can be arranged in hierarchies such that the
lower levels. Both the content and the characteris- achievement of lower order goals is a means by
tics of goals higher in the goal hierarchy matter for which higher order goals are achieved.
deriving value. While behaviorism simply contends Building on this work and the ideas of others, we
people strive to obtain rewards or avoid punishments, agree that the content and characteristics of goals
Maslow (1943) argued that what employees consider or “internal representations of desired end states”
28
Envisioning Progress and Perils
Musings on the Future of Motivation Research in a
Rapidly Evolving World
Abstract
This final chapter reflects on the rich contributions within this Handbook of Human Motivation on
diverse topics including self-regulation, biological mechanisms, awareness, defensiveness and
oppression, and the facilitation of engagement, learning, and behavior change. These processes are
examined within broad theoretical frameworks, as well as in specific domains such as close
relationships, physical activity, work, education, and psychotherapy. Building on these contributions, the
chapter projects forward in time to ask the question of whether future scientists and practitioners will
think these authors from the early 21st century were asking the right questions. The chapter includes
speculation on how developments in technology, research methodologies, big data, and globalization,
among other trends, will reshape the science of motivation not only in its focus and efficacy, but also in
its ethics and applications to the formidable problems likely to be faced by our species on a warming
and crowded planet.
Keywords: human motivation, psychological science, diversity, global trends, interdisciplinary research
The introduction to this handbook highlighted the defensiveness, objectification, and oppression; and
historical turn within the field of motivation, from facilitating interest, choice, learning, and curiosity.
a near-exclusive focus on external sources of control These processes are examined within domains
over behavior, to a concern with the contents of such as close relationships, sport and physical
the “black box”—the psychological experiences, activity, work, education, and psychotherapy. These
processes, and mechanisms through which behavior contributions thus exemplify motivational science
is mediated. Far from being a mistaken route to sci- in the 21st century, in which a deeper understand-
entific knowledge, the study of the black box has ing of both mind and mechanisms is playing a
not only revealed core elements of our human significant role.
nature; it has also provided roadmaps for how to By going inside the black box, we increase our
design interventions to optimize people’s health, appreciation of human commonalities as well as
wellness, and communities. That is, these inner me- variations. Common mechanisms proposed within
diators, both biological and psychological, describe this volume included structural elements in moti-
us as creatures, shed light on why we do what we do, vation, such as the role of feedback within self-
and ultimately provide keys to the organization of regulation, the universal contributions of specific
behavior and behavior change. Chapters within this brain systems to the two defining elements of moti-
second edition of The Oxford Handbook of Human vation, namely, energization and direction, and the
Motivation include research on multiple internal importance of high-quality relationships to resilience
mediators, focusing on topics such as enhancing and wellness. We also see that across varied develop-
self-efficacy, self-regulation, and awareness; decreasing mental and cultural backdrops there are common
527
needs for competence and relatedness and, perhaps Yet as a science of what moves people to action,
more controversial, autonomy. Amid such univer- understanding this interface is of the utmost import
sals, however, research on the inner processes and because new technologies are affecting our lifestyles,
mechanisms of motivation reveals significant varia- attitudes, and behavioral options in myriad ways,
tions, both within and between groups of people. both positively and negatively (Calvo, Vella-Brodrick,
Understanding these moderators of general principles, Desmet, & Ryan, 2016). In addition, our use of
whether they be gender, culture, or more nuanced technologies as researchers opens up new frontiers
genetic and epigenetic differences, is critical to our in both information gathering and ethical challenges.
basic and applied motivational science.
Although the science of motivation has become New Devices, New Behaviors
more complex, it is still far from unified. The prob- Let’s consider technologies already rising within our
lems and perils of motivation can be viewed through horizons. In the near term, people will increasingly
very different lenses. In fact, differences are not just be living within, and relying on, augmented realities;
apparent at theoretical levels—there are different virtual elements will more regularly become part of
metatheories underlying these perspectives, from our perceptual fields. Accompanying this, electronic
the purely mechanistic to the organismic, even implants will very soon allow us to operate devices
within the current volume. The clashes between through thought alone, often allowing effortless
such different perspectives do indeed sometimes fulfillment of intentions. Moreover, we may not
generate heat, but with heat also comes light. In even need to think at all. The likelihood is high that
our view, the very nature of the dialectic of science artificial intelligences will make many of our deci-
requires conflict and consequent attempts at syn- sions for us, from driving our cars to anticipating
theses to resolve those conflicts within more over- our biological and psychological needs. Our social
arching theoretical accounts. relations will increasingly be mediated by social
media, and many of us will traverse a world in
Into the Future which personal information will be accessible to
Having begun this volume with a description of the strangers, even as they first encounter us.
transition from 20th-century thinking to contem- Such changes are just a few of the many we can
porary ideas, in the closing of this handbook we readily imagine, because these changes are already
pause to consider what the future of this field might unfolding. These varied technologies represent exten-
hold. Forecasting is a hazardous function. Consider sions of human capacities and all can be harnessed to
that the greatest scientists of the 20th century could help fulfil our goals, purposes, and needs. Accordingly,
not have been more wrong about what the future they also pose dangers insofar as they can lead to
would hold; Freud, Skinner, Pavlov, Hull, and other unreflective actions, consumerism, alienation from
seminal figures of the past envisioned sciences that others, distortions of priorities, and numerous other
have not come to pass. So, it often seems that even issues of self-regulation and wellness.
those closest to a particular problem may not see the Despite the lack of synchrony between the pace
future accurately. Indeed, in 1901, Wilbur Wright— of motivation research and that of technological
of Wright brothers’ fame—told his brother Orville progress, future researchers will nonetheless look
that a successful flight was 50 years away. Just 2 years back to us, much as we have to those before us in
later, they took to the skies. Nonetheless, we shall the history of motivation science, to see how we
venture into this dangerous territory with some addressed questions concerning our coexistence
speculations, some extrapolating from what we have with emerging technologies. Many of these ques-
learned in this edition and some based only on our tions are straightforwardly motivational in nature:
armchair guesses. So here goes . . . For what purpose will we use these technologies?
How will their use affect us? How can we use moti-
Technology, Human Experience, vational design to enhance the effectiveness and
and Motivation Science phenomenological smoothness of technologies and
Most striking in our vision of the future of the augmentations? How can we regulate the seductions
motivation field is the likely impact of the technol- of new media and technology and better recognize
ogy explosion on societies, individuals, and even and avoid their harms? How can we understand
the methods of our science itself. Psychological sci- motivational ethics in our use of technologies to
ence has only recently been explicitly grappling inform and persuade? Questions such as these high-
with the issues of the human–technology interface. light the need to attend to and understand human
Note: Tables and figures are indicated by an italic ‘t’ and ‘f ’, respectively, following the page number.
533
autonomy 136 Blaga, M. 512 development of 446–47
in basic needs theory 469 Blazo, J. A. 493 and early relatedness 344–45
and choice 137, 144 Bodmann, S. M. 237 and engagement 338
control vs. 149 body image 324 and investment 338
and effort 147 body project 321 and IQ 339
in motivational interviewing 429 Bohn, V. K. 60 other factors in 347
neural mechanisms of 405–7 Boivin, M. 18 and parental beliefs 490
and physical health 138 Bond, R. 21 and relatedness, contextual variations
and self control 125 boredom 171 in 345–47
in self-determination theory 472 Bouquet, C. A. 275 and self-determination theory
student 451–52, 454 Bowbly, John 69, 78, 337 340–42, 344
and student performance 137 Boyd, J. N. 292 and sense of responsibility to
autonomy support 96 brain glucose 121–22 parents 342–43
autonomy-supportive intervention brain structures 357f and socialization 341
program (ASIP) 451–52 Brehm, J. W. 374 variations in 456
autonomy-supportive leadership Bridwell, L. G. 509 See also student motivation
style 498, 500 brief interventions for student motivation Children’s Research Center 230
autonomy-supportive parenting style 491 and national education policy 455 Chirkov, V. 101
autonomy-supportive practices 347 scaling up 452–53 Choi, J. 93
autotelic experience 169 Briggs, C. 423 choice 135–50
autotelic personality 175–76 Brinkmann, K. 381, 383 and analytic mindset 140
Autotelic Personality Questionnaire 175 Bronfenbrenner, U. 494 and autonomy 144
avoidance goals 423, 432 Brownman, A. S. 49 biological and neuroscience perspectives
avoidance tendencies 423–24 Brumberg, Joan 321, 327 in 140–41
avoidant valence 469 Brunstein, J. C. 420 biological/neuroscience views of 140–41
Brustad, R. J. 489, 491, 493 cognitive views of 138–40
B BTAE (better-than-average effect) 309 cultural differences in 147–49
Balanced Inventory of Socially Desireable Burgess, P. W. 255 and depression 141
Responding 312 Burke, B. L. 70 effects of 143–49
Bandura, Albert 4, 12, 248, 444, 511 Burton, K. D. 476 and effort 146
Barber, H. 493 and environmental factors 136
Barreto, P. 384 C as focal task 145
Barsalou, Lawrence 274 Calogero, R. M. 326, 332 future research, directions for 149–50
basal ganglia 359 Canning, E. A. 451 and individual preference 144
basic behavior 419 Capa, R. L. 275 and memory 139
basic needs theory 469, 514 cardiac preejection period reactivity 381f as motivator 136–43
basic trust 400 caretaker-child interactions 49 and social class 148
Bass, B. M. 498 Carnegie Council on Adolescent and social interaction 142–43
Baumann, N. 404, 407 Development 449 choice overload 147
Becker, Ernest 69, 72, 79, 80 Carnevale, P. J. 258 cholinergic system 180
behavior Carstensen, L . L. 292 chronic accessibility 50
basic 419 Carter, C. 473 cingulate cortex 276
and employee motivation 519 Carver, C. S. 68 Circumplex Scales of Interpersonal
ideomotor 273 Casper, F. 421 Values 420
behavioral construct, in motivation 488 Castonguay, L. G. 425 clarification-oriented psychotherapy
behavioral restraint 385–86 casual attribution theory 230 431, 433
behavior change 455 causality orientations theory 476 Clarkson, J. J. 121
behaviorism 3–4, 136, 517 Cesario, Joseph 53 classroom practices 447
behavioristic theory 98 challenge-skill balance 173, 174 classrooms, interventions in 452–53,
being in flow 176 challenge-skill relationship 176 455–57
Belk, R. W. 74 change, models of 427–28 climate, motivational 499, 500
benevolent sexism 324 chatoyant notion of the self 393 coaches
Berking, M. 427 Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. 473 factors influencing behavior of 500
Berlyne, D. E. 160, 160f Chen, S. 473 peer leaders vs. 496
Bernacki, M. L. 454 Cheon, S. H. 452 and youth sport motivation 489,
Bern Inventory of Treatment Goals 420t Chicoine, E. 423 497–500
beta-adrenergetic sympathetic impact 376 child development 488 youth sport motivational practices
better-than-average effect (BTAE) 309 children’s motivation for 500
Bhalla, J. A. 492, 494 and academic functioning 340, 343 coach praise orientation 495
big data 529 and attachment to parents 338–40 Coakley, J. J. 495
Billings, Josh 310 and autonomy-supportive coasting 34
black box model 4 practices 347 cognitive construct, in motivation 488
Blackwell, L. S. 450 and capability 338 cognitive dissonance 138
534 Index
and choice 139 cultural relativism 102 discrepancy-reducing feedback
dissonant motivational state 139 cultural values 101 processes 28
cognitive dissonance reduction. culture discrepancy system 397–98
see affective consonance production and student motivation 456 dispositional flow 175
cognitive mediators 4–5 and youth sport motivation 494 Dispositional Flow Scale–2 175
cognitive modeling 15 curiosity 157–64 distal defenses 71
Cohen, A. L. 255 diverse 159 distributive justice 518
Cohen, J. 423 as for-its-own-sake motivation 159–62 diverse exploration 158
Cohen, P. 423 future research, directions for 164 diversity 531
collective agency 13 instinct of 162 division achievement 15
collective efficacy 22 interest vs. 161 domain-differential view 288
Common Core State Standards 453, 457 personal differences in 162–63 domain-general regulatory focus 49
competence and reduction of novelty/ domain-specific regulatory focus
and achievement goals 234–35 uncertainty 157–59 motivations 50
in basic needs theory 469 specific 159 Domingues, Rholfs 138
defined 234 and wonder 162 dopamine 355
parental beliefs about and self-appraisals Curiosity and Exploration Inventory 162 dopamine pathway. see dopaminergic
of 490 Custers, R. 279 system
in student motivation 444–45 cybernetic approach dopaminergic reward system 399
competence-based model 238–39 defined 27 dopaminergic system 98
competence motivation theory 488, 489 and motivational concepts 29 dorsal striatum 358
competitive anxiety 491 dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 99, 276,
competitive reward structures 448 D 358, 363
competitive youth sports leagues, D’Alessandro, D. 476 Dovido, J. F. 332
friendship in 497 Dang, J 120 Dreher, J. C. 275
concentration 170 data science 529 drive-increasing stimuli 158
Concept Oriented Reading Instruction Davis, N. W. 493 drive theory 136
(CORI) program 452, 454 deactivating emotion 39 DTA (death-thought accessibility
condition-experience model 172–73 death-thought accessibility hypothesis hypothesis) 71, 74
confirmatory factor analyses 51 (DTA) 71, 74 dual process models 476
Connell, J. P. 444 Deci, E. L. 425, 489 Duda, J. L. 490
Conner, M. T. 467 Deci, Edward 90, 92, 94, 286, 337 Dufour, A. 275
contextual-level motivation 473 declarative memory 141 Duval, S. 68, 81
control deKnop, P. 499 Dweck, C. S. 450, 454
in planned behavior theory 465 delay of gratification 257 Dweck, Carol 230, 249, 290, 384, 444
sense of 170 delegation hypothesis 255 Dworkin, A. 322, 323
in student motivation 444–45 Delgado, M. R. 140 Dwyer, L. A. 419, 425
controlling coach behaviors 498 Dembo, T. 232 dynamic casual modeling 407
controlling form of motivation 489 denial of autonomy 322 dysphoria 383
controlling motivation 468 denial of subjectivity 322 dysphoric individuals 383
controlling parenting style 491 DeNisi, A. 512
control theory 511 departmentalization, in secondary E
co-objectification 330 school 449 eagerness 48, 54
cooperative reward structures 448 depression 35 early sport sampling, specialization vs. 494
Cordray, D. S. 452 and choice 141 eating habits 199
core self 400 and emotional reactivity 39 Ebbeck, V. 497
CORI (Concept Oriented Reading mental contrasting 253 Ebner, N. C. 285, 291
Instruction) program 452, 454 and personal goals 423, 424 Eccles, J. S. 445–46, 449, 453, 490, 491
Corker, Katherine 53 and self-integration 400 ecological systems model 494
Cornwell, J. F. M. 58 well-being therapy in treatment of 430 education 443–57
correspondence of content 193 descriptive studies 488 future research, directions for 455–57
cortisol 179 desire to approach success 232–33 motivation-related interventions
Cosmides, Leda 89 desire to avoid failure 232–33 in 450–55
Côté, J. 493 Destin, M. 49 school’s influence on motivation
course content 453–54 Dewey, John 159, 160 in 447–48
Cox, Cathy 78 didactic instruction 15 and school transitions 448–50
Cox, W. M. 424 Di Domenico, T. 91, 99, 368, 406 source of student motivation in 444–47
Crits-Christoph, P. 425 differential expectancies 308, 312 effort 146
cross-domain relationships, differentiated view 231 and autonomy 147
with coaches 498 difficulty law of motivation 374 and choice overload 147
Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly 80, 81, 170, dimensions of well-being 430 and difficulty 379f
171, 173 Dinnerstein, Dorothy 324 effort control 36, 38
cultural psychology 531 discrepancy-enlarging feedback loops 28 effort mobilization 255, 373
Index 535
effort-related cardiovascular response 376 European American children 456 fit, of personal goals 422
egalitarian goal 277 Evans, J. 495 fixed mindset 444
egocentrism 310 Every Student Succeeds Act 453, 457 flexible tenacity 260
ego control 403 exercise and physical activity 463–79 flow 169–82
ego depletion 113–28 future research, directions for 479 constructs related to 173–76
and brain glucose 121–22 integration of theories 472–75 dispositional 173
building resistance to 125–27 measurement and methodological dynamic process in 176
causes of 115–16 advances in 475–79 frequency 175
and challenges to depletion effect motivational theories 464–70 and happiness 170
theory 119–20 theoretical advances in 470–75 and motive congruence 198
and changing the self 116–17 existential anxiety 70 proneness 173, 174
consequences of 116–19 existential protection 324–25 and self-esteem 173
conservation of 123 existential psychotherapy 69 temporal dynamics of 177
and effects of motivation/ expectancy 4, 121 flow condition 173
conservation 122–24 expectancy-value theory flow experience 173, 178
and ego strength 114–15 work motivation in 509–10 conditions of 171–73
and expectancy 121 youth sport motivation in 488–90 and dopamine 180
future research, directions for 127–28 expectations, of coaches 498 emergent motivation and temporal
and interpersonal interactions 115 experience dynamics of 176–79
and memory 118 interpreters of 490–91 future research directions in 179
moderators of 124–25 providers of 490–92 neurophysiology of 179–82
operation of 120–22 experience fluctuation model 171–72, 171f psychological covariates of 173
overcoming 122 experience integration 397 and stress response 179
and passivity 117, 127 experience sampling method (ESM) Flow Proneness Scale 175
physiological markers of 119 174, 178 Flow Questionnaire 174
and rest and replenishment 124 experiential avoidance 430–31 flow scale for human-computer
ego goal orientation 446 experimental system 36 interaction 174
ego involvement 231 explicit motives 418, 420 flow state 170–71, 173
ego-oriented motivation 475 exploratory behavior 158 Flow State Scale-2 174
ego strength 115 extension memory 397. for-its-own-sake motivation 159
and changing the self 116 see also auto-biographical experiences Forman, E. M. 431
and cognition 119 external locus of control 444 Fournier, M. A. 102
and interpersonal interactions 115 external regulation 92, 445, 468 Fraley C. 340, 344
and self control 115 external stimuli 508 Fredericks, J. A. 490
Einstein, Albert 286 extrinsic aspirations. see life goals Fredrickson, Barbara 321, 322, 327
elective selection 286 extrinsic goals 423 Frenzel, A. C. 215–18
electroencephalographic methodology 119 extrinsic motivation 366 Freud, Sigmund 200, 398
elementary school 449 defined 91–92 Freund, A. M. 285, 294
elite child athletes 487, 493 and identified regulation 93 Friedrich, A. 377
Elliot, A. J. 446 and inherent integrative process 92 friendship
Elliot, Andrew 232, 233, 234, 237, 238 and integrated regulation 93 peer group acceptance vs. 494
Elsner, B. 273 and introjection 92 and youth physical activity
embodied motivation 529 and relative autonomy 93 motivation 495–96
emotional drain 331 in self-determination theory 513–14 friendship orientation 495
emotional reactivity 39 extrinsic regulation 422 friendship quality 495–97
employee motivation Fries, A. 428
conceptual framework of 514–15, 515f F Frodi, A. 342
importance of 507 Facchin, S. 324 Fulmer, C. A. 60
See also work motivation facilitators of change 425–27 functional domains 288
empty goal commitments 424 false self 398
endurance 126 family members 489–93 G
engagement 444 fantasy realization 249 Galinsky, A. D. 323
Ennis, G. E. 380 Faucher, E. H. 71 Gallup World Poll 312
entity theory 231 feedback 510 game structures 431
environmental factors feedback loop 28 Gaspard, H. 451
and choice 136 feedback patterns 497–98 Gebauer, J. E. 312
and human experience 136 feedback processes 28–29 Geller, P. A. 431
epinephrine 179 Feixas, G. 424 gender 23
Epstein, Herman 36 female subordination 323 and friendship quality 495
equity theory 518 Ferguson, C. J. 277 and sibling support for youth
ESM (experience sampling method) Festinger, L. 232, 247 sports 493
174, 178 first grade, transition into 448–49 and student motivation 456
ethnicity, and student motivation 456 Fishbein, M. 467 Gendolla, A. 376, 380
536 Index
general psychotherapy (change model) 428 anti-goal 28–29 Harmon-Jones, C. 405
German action theory 511–12 clear 172 Harmon-Jones, E. 77
Gibbons, S. L. 497 egalitarian 277 Hartup, W. W. 495
Gilbert, S. J. 254 emergent 177 Haws, K. L. 49
goal attainment 247–62 goal program 30 Hayes, J. 71
and competitive situations 257 hierarchy of 515–16 Heapy, A. A. 427
derailment 256 learning 230–31, 290 heart rate 376
and goals vs. motivation 247–49 maintenance 291 Heckhausen, H. 295, 399, 417, 418,
and implementation intentions 253–61 managing multiple goals 287 428, 470
interventions related to 261–62 mastery 232 Heckhausen, J. 417, 418
and mental contrasting 249–53 mastery-approach 233 hedonic incentive 386
optimism about 425 mastery-avoidance 233 hegemonic masculinity 323
self-regulation of 249–61 motivation vs. 247–49 Hennecke, M. 294
Goal Attainment Scaling procedure 421 nonfocal 35 Herbert, J. D. 431
goal difficulty 16 performance 230–31, 232 Herzberg, F. 519
goal-directed behavior 254 personal best 235 Hess, T. M. 380
goal-directed response 253 prevention 249 heteronomous motivational
goal focus promotion 249 orientations 476
actional phase 295, 296f, 297 proximal 172, 176 heterosexual maleness 323
and age 291 and self determination theory Hidi, S. 445
and deadlines 298 (SDT) 97 Higgins, Edward T. 47, 49, 54, 58,
and loss avoidance 291 setting 510, 516 512–13
and motivational phase 295–98 in student motivation 445–46 high-maintenance interactions 115
postactional phase 295, 296f subgoal 30 high schools
preactional phase 295, 296, 296f Goals as Action-Desired Outcome student autonomy in 454
predecisional phase 295, 296f Representations (ADORE) 271, transition to 449–50
process and outcome 290t 272f, 279 high-stakes assessments 454
and resource demands 293 goal selection 287 Hillgruber, A. 374
and responses to feedback 298–99 goal-setting theory (GST) Hirt, E. R. 121
and time perspective 292 and employee motivation 519 Hispanic American children 456
goal implementation theory 511 work motivation in 510 Hodge, K. 94
goal orientation goal striving 248 Holt, N. L. 491
change 292–93 goals vs. motivation 247–49 Hom, H. L. 490
maintenance 291 Goldenberg, J. L. 324 homeostasis 29
of parents 490–91 Gollwitzer, P. M. 254, 255, 258, 262, Hommel, B. 273
stability 292–93, 294 428, 470 Horn, J. L. 490
goal orientation and focus 285–300 Goodenow, C. 448 Horn, T. S. 490, 492, 498
and adult development 286–89 Gorman, C. A. 513 Horowitz, L. M. 431
age and 291–92 Gotez, Thomas 215–16 Houser-Marko, Linda 299
change throughout adulthood 288 Goyer, J. P. 455 HR (human resources) 518–19
and change vs. stability 292–95 Graham, S. 456 Huang, Y. 331
and feedback on progress toward Grawe, K. 418–19, 428 Hull, Clark 158
goals 298–99 Greenberg, J. 67, 69, 70, 72 Hulleman, C. S. 237, 451, 452
and motivational phase 295–98 Griffith, H. R. 380 human agency 13
and process vs. outcome 289–99 Grolnick, W. S. 96, 341, 347 human resources (HR) 518–19
and related constructs 289–90 Grosse Holtforth, M. 418–19, 425, 428 Hyde, J. S. 451
and successful development 286–87 Grossman, C. 421 hypomanic personality scale 39
goal pursuit 269–80 group acceptance orientation 495 hypothalamus 361
as automated behaviors 272 group learning 448
environmental reward cues 279 growth mindset 444 I
and environmental stimuli 271 Gruenfeld, D. H. 323 IAPE (implicit-affect-primes-effort) 378
future research, directions for 279–80 GST. See goal-setting theory ICAN intervention 450
and habitual action selection 272–74 Guay, F. 18 idealized influence 499
means vs. outcomes 286 Guenther, C. L. 310 identified regulation 93, 368, 422,
outcome/reward representation gut-feelings 363 445, 468
277–79 Guthrie, J. T. 452 identity relatedness 249
and reward cues 275–77 identity theory 198
stimuli as trigger for 274 H ideomotor behavior 273
unconscious source of 274–75 Hackman, J. R. 518 ideomotor learning study 278
without awareness 270–72 Hagger, M. S. 466, 474 idiosyncratic negative event 251
goal-reason combinations 238 happiness 170 I-D model 163
goals 39 Harackiewicz, J. M. 198, 215, 237, 294, if-then plans 377
achievement (See achievement goals) 446, 451 IJzendoorn, Van. 339
Index 537
imaginary audience 328 interactional justice 518 in youth sports and physical
immediate and unambiguous interdisciplinary research 531–32 activity 489
feedback 172 interest intrinsic reward 366
implementational mindset 296 conceptualizations of 209–11 intrinsic value, in task value 445
implementation intention strategies current research on 212–20 introjected regulation 422, 445, 468
470–71 four-phase model of 207, 208t, 209 introjection 92, 429
implicit-affect-primes-effort (IAPE) 378 future research, directions for 220–21 intuitive behavior system. see action
implicit aging 378 individual 213 system
Implicit Association Test 420, 477 situational 207, 213
implicit–explicit motive congruence student, interventions to improve 450 J
187–201 in student motivation 445–46 Jack, W. 77
antecedents of 197 sustaining 213 Jacobs, J. 456
consequences of 197–99 and wonder 162 James, William 308
future research, directions for 200–201 See also interest development jealousy 493
and independence hypothesis 191–92 interest development 205–21 Jelinek, S. 53
methodological factors in 192–95 and earlier/later phases of interest 213–15 Jia, L. 121
and picture story exercises vs. fluctuations/shifts in 214–15 job design 518
questionnaire measures 188–91 four-phase model of 207, 208t, 209 Joormann, J. 423
and self-determination 196 individual 207–9, 208t Jost, J. T. 326
and stress 197 longitudinal studies on 212–13 Juhl, Jacob 76
substantive variables moderating measurement considerations with justice 518
195–96 211–12
implicit–explicit systems 180–81 situational 208t K
implicit motivation study complementarity with 215–18 Kanfer, R. 511
in exercise and physical activity 476–77 triggers for 212–14 Kanze, D. 53
in psychotherapy 418–20 as value 210 Kappes, A. 252
implicit processes 476 internal frame of reference 102 Kappes, H. B. 253
implicit self 396 internalization 92, 332, 398 Karoly, P. 423
impression management 308 neural mechanisms of 405–7 Kasser, T. 423, 429
incentive-based information 362 in student motivation 445 Kasser, Tim 286
incentive learning 274 see also extrinsic motivation Kazen, M. 404, 407
incremental theory 231 internalized rules 398 Kehr, H. M. 197, 198
independence hypothesis 191 internal locus of control 173, 444 Keller, Helen 205–6, 220
individual interest 207–9, 208t, 213 internally guided decision making 368 Kent, M. 403
individualized consideration 499 Internet-based interventions 433 Kierkegaard, Søren 313
individuated-entity versus aggregate interpersonal interactions 115 Kimiecik, J. C. 490
comparisons 310 interpersonal leadership styles 498, 500 Kimiecik, J. D. 492
Inesi, M. E. 323 interpersonal theory 431 kindergarten, transitions to and
information processing 512 interpreters of experience, parents as from 448–49
inherent integrative process 92 490–91 Kingler, E. 424
inspirational motivation 498 interracial interactions 115 Kipp, L. E. 495, 497, 500
Institute for Child Behavior and intervention design 478 Kirk, T. 258, 262
Development 230 intervention mapping 477–79 Kitsantas, Anastasia 290
instrumentality 322 interventions Klappheck, M. 422–23
insula 360 Internet-based 433 Klappheck, M. A. 425
integrated regulation 93, 445 motivational change in 429 Klauda, S. L. 452
integrative self 395–97 tailored to clients 418, 425 Kluger, A. N. 512
and ego control 403 See also specific interventions Kochansa, G. 347
and implicit self 396 interviews, to assess personal goals 420 Koestner, R. 423, 476
me-self vs. I-self 396 intrapersonal factors, in youth sport Koestner, R. 197
and negative emotion 402–3 motivation 488 Köllner, M. G. 193
and personality development 400, 401 intrinsically motivated behaviors 160 Kosfelder, J. 422–23
and self-congruence 397 intrinsic aspirations. see life goals Koskey, Kristin L. K. 216–17
and self-decision 401 intrinsic goals 423 Krings, F. 324
and self-doubt 402 intrinsic motivation 91, 136, 144, 366–67 Krott, N. R. 251
and self-positivity 400 defined 91 Kuhl, J. 395
and self-relaxation 402–3 and mastery-oriented motivational Kuhn, K. 423
intellectual stimulation 499 climate 475
intention and neural networks 99 L
and physical activity 464 in organismic integration theory 468 Langdon, Rae 322
in planned behavior theory 465–68 in self-determination theory 513–14 Latham, G. P. 375, 387
and self-determination theory 473–74 for students 445–46 leadership, peer 496
intention memory 398–99 students’ age-related decline in 446 leadership behaviors, of coaches 498–99
538 Index
leadership styles, of coaches 498, 500 McClelland, David 187, 188, 189, 192, 248 moratorium diffusion 198
learned motivational states 362–65 McDougall, William 162 morbidity 114
expectancy 363–64 MCII (mental contrasting with Morris, P. A. 494
reward 362–63 implementation intentions) 261–62 mortality concerns 325
value 364–65 McKinley, N. M. 329 mortality salience hypothesis (MS) 70,
learning McRae, A. 452 72–75
enactive 13 meaning 79, 80 motivation
goals 16 Medalia, A. 93 antecedents of 517–18
and goals 230–31 medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) 99, 276 defined 488
in groups 448 Meece, J. L. 446 employee (see employee motivation)
social influences on 443–44 memory 139 modern study of 6
task-involved 446 declarative 141 work (See work motivation)
vicarious 13 extension 397 motivational attunement approach 425–26
learning goals 16, 230–31, 290 intention 398–99 motivational change 428–31
learning theory 247 selective self (see selective self-memory) motivational clarification 428
Lecci, L. 423 mental contrasting 251, 402 motivational climate 474–75, 499, 500
Lee, V. E. 449 and approach/avoidance goals 251 motivational constructs
Lekes, N. 423 and astuteness 252 assessment of 432
Lens, Willy 238 and depression 253 in psychotherapy 418–21
Leotti, L. A. 140 and fantasy realization 249 motivational interventions
Levesque, C. S. 476 and goal pursuit 250 as facilitators of change 425–27
Lewin, Kurt 136, 232, 514, 517 mechanisms of 252 for students 450–55
Liening, S. 197 and reality as obstacle 250 for youth athletes 497
life goals 97, 98 mental contrasting with implementation motivational interviewing 429, 433
Lifton, R. J. 75 intentions (MCII) 261–62 motivational model of alcoholism 424
limbic system 362 mentoring, for youth athletes 497 motivational neuroscience 355–69
Linnenbrick-Garcia, Linda 216–17 mesocortical pathway 361 automatically-activated motivational
Locke, E. A. 375, 387 mesolimbic dopamine system 362 states 362
Locke, K. D. 418 mesolimbic pathway 361 automatically activated motivational
loss-based selection 286 Mestas, M. 240 states 362
low-stakes assessments 455 metacognition 219 autonomous self-regulation 367–68
Ludeke, S. G. 314 metamotivation 60–61 brain structures/pathways, key 357–62
Lydon, J. E. 476 metamotivational beliefs 61 current research on 355–57
metamotivational control 61 future research, directions for 368–69
M metamotivational monitoring 61 learned motivational states 362–65
MacInnes, J. 197 metatheories 528 neural core of 363f
MacKinnon, Catherine 322 Meyer, B. B. 493 person-generated motivational
Maehr, M. L. 451 Michalak, J. 422–25 states 365–67
Maehr, Marty 230 Michigan Omnibus Personality research practices in 355–57
Magee, J. 323 Inventory 311 top down vs. bottom up research
Magee, J. C. 323 mid-brain dopaminergic pathway 407 355–56
maintenance goals 291 middle school motivational phases
Makransky, G. 314 student autonomy in 454 actional 295, 296f, 297
Manderlink, G. 294 transition to and from 449 postactional 295, 296f
Manning, Eli 492 Midgley, C. 451 preactional 295, 296, 296f
Manning, Peyton 492 Midgley, Carol 237 predecisional 295, 296f
Manzey, Christine 216–17 Miele, David 61 motivational processes 15–18
Marien, H. 279 mimicry 273 motivation intensity theory 373–87, 374f
Maslow, A. H. 508–9, 515, 519 mindfulness 103 ability, fatigue and aging 378–80
mastery-approach goals 233, 469 mindset(s) and depressive symptoms 377–78
mastery-avoidance goals 233, 469 interventions focused on improving 450 difficulty/effort, variables
mastery climate 499 in student motivation 444 affecting 377–78
mastery goal orientation 446, 451 Mischel, W 248, 257 effort measurements in 375–76
mastery goals. see learning goals mnemic neglect 311 and effort-related cardiovascular
mastery models 14–15 model of interpersonal motives 431 response 376
mastery-oriented motivational Moitra, E. 431 empirical evidence for 376–77
climate 474, 475 Molden, D. C. 49 importance of success, variables
material incentive 382–83 MoMa intervention program 451 affecting 381–86
in depressed individuals 383 Monti, J. D. 345 material incentive 382–83
and gender-specific effects 385 mood-behavior model 378 objective task difficulty, moderation
and outcome expectancy 383–84 mood disposition 210 effects of 378–81
and social incentive 384 Moradi, Bonnie 331 and unclear difficulty 375
Mayer, Richard 356 Moran, M. M. 496 and unfixed difficulty 375
Index 539
motivation phase (Rubicon model of O parents
action phases) 427 Obama, Barack, and administration 453 as interpreters of experience 490–91
motivation research, future of 527–32 objectification theory as providers of experience 490–92
and cultural psychology 531 and anxiety 324 relationship quality with 496
and methodologies 530 and body image 323 as role models 490, 492
and technology 528–30 cognitive development theories in and youth sport motivation 489–92
motive change 424–25 328–30 youth sport motivational practices
motive congruence co-objectification 330 for 500
antecedents of 197 denial of autonomy 322 passivity 117, 127
and eating habits 199 denial of subjectivity 322 Patalakh, M. 197
integrative model for 190 and dissonance 326 Patterns of Adaptive Learning Scale 237
multiple-moderator approaches 196 and emotional drain 331 Paulhus, D. L. 312–14
and need satisfaction 197 and gender development 329 Pavolvian-to-instrumental transfer 274
and referential competence 197 and instrumentality 322 Payne, S. C. 512
and volitional strength 198 and mortality concerns 325 peer-assisted learning strategies 497
and well-being 198 and ownership 322 peer group acceptance 494–96
and work 199 self-objectification 322, 324 peer leadership 496
See also implicit–explicit motive situational/contextual factors peer modeling 489, 497
congruence in 327 peers
motive-oriented therapeutic relationship situational motivators of 330 and student motivation 448
building 425 socialization theories in 327–28 and youth sport motivation 489, 494–97
motive-oriented treatment plan 432 object-recognition system. see discrepancy PEESE (precision-effect estimate
motor areas 359 system with standard error) 119–20
motor resonance 273 observational learning 14 Pekrun, Reinhard 215–16
Mouratidis, Athanasios 238 obsessive-compulsive disorder 403 Pelletier, L. G. 476
MPFC (medial prefrontal cortex) 99, 276 Oettingen, G. 249, 251, 252, 261, 262, PEP (pre-ejection period) 376
MS (mortality salience hypothesis) 70, 424 PEP (pre-ejection period) reactivity 382
72–75 OIT (organismic integration perceived behavioral control
multidirectionality 286 theory) 468–69 in planned behavior theory 465
Murayama, Kou 98, 141, 237, 240, 356 Oldham, G. R. 518 and self-determination theory 474
Murphy’s Law 308 O’Malley, S. 425 perceived power 465
Murray, Henry 187 openness to experience 163 perceptions of competence
optimal arousal 158 (perceived competence)
N optimal experience 169 and positive reinforcement from
naive treatment concerns 426 optimism, about goal attainment 425 coaches 498
narcissistic personality disorder 431 optimization 286 in youth sport motivation 488
national education policy optional functioning 519 performance-approach goals 446, 469
motivation theory and 453 Orbell, S. 473–74 performance-avoidance goals 446, 469
and researcher recommendations 455, orbitofrontal cortex 358, 361, 362 performance climate 499
457 organismic integration theory performance-contingent
National Research Council 457 (OIT) 468–69 reinforcement 497–98
naturalistic motivational change 428–29 O’Rourke, D. J. 491 performance goal orientation 446
needs 90 other-referenced goal perspective 469 performance goals 16, 230–31, 446–47
need satisfaction 197 outcome expectancy 17, 364 performance orientation 512
needs theories 508–9 outcome focus 289, 290. performance-oriented motivational
negation implementation intention 256 See also goal focus climate 474, 475
negative affect 404 out-group hostility 80 personal best goals 235
negative emotion 402–3 override impulses 123 personal goals
neural mechanisms assessments of 420–21
and personal systems interaction P defined 418–19
theory 405 Pak, H. 249 in psychotherapy 421–24, 432
and self-determination 406–7 parasympathetic deactivation 376 personality disorders 431
and self-referential information 406 parasympathetic nervous system personality interactons theory 197
neuroscience. See motivational neuroscience (PNS) 179, 180 and discrepancy system 395
neutral stimuli 270 PARCS (predictive and reactive control and intuitive behavior control 395
Nicholls, John 230, 231, 446 systems) 394, 405 process of 396f
Niemic, C. P. 96 parental beliefs, in youth sport personality systems interactions
No Child Left Behind 453–55 motivation 490–91 theory 394, 395, 401
Norcross, J. C. 428 parental involvement 345 personal relationships
norepinephrine 179 parental pressure, and youth sport anxiety-buffering effects of 78
Norman, P. 473–74 motivation 491 and existential threats 79
nucleus accumbens 358 parental socialization 489–90 personal systems interaction theory 404
Nussbaum, M. C. 322 parenting style 491–92 person–environment fit 518
540 Index
person-generated motivational states process focus 290. See also goal focus goals in 430
365–67 Prochaska, J. O. 428 and group dynamics 60
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation promotion goals 430 measuring/manipulating of
366–67 prospect theory 123 motivation in 50–51
psychological needs 367 providers of experience, parents as 490–92 and metamotivation 60–61
self-regulation and goals 365–66 provision of choice 143 origins of motivation in 49–50
volition 365 proximal environment 341 prevention motivation in 48–49
PET (precision-effect test) 119–20 proximinal defenses 71 promotion and prevention goals in
PFC (prefrontal cortex) 180–81, 358 Przybylinski, E 259 51–56
phenomenology 69 PSEs. See Picture Story Exercises promotion motivation in 48
physical activity. See exercise and physical psychological change 455 promotion/prevention vs. approach/
activity; youth sport and physical psychological housekeeping 308 avoidance in 51
activity psychological need satisfaction scope of 48
physiological arousal 139 and motivational climate 499 undesired end states 53
Pichora-Fuller, K. M. 382 in self-determination theory 471–72 work motivation in 512–13
Picture Story Exercises (PSEs) 187 psychological science 528, 528–30 reinforcement theory 508
and problems of measurement 189 psychological selection 177 reinforcement value 4
and questionnaire measures 188 psychopathology relatedness, in basic needs theory 469
Pintrich, P. R. 446 development of 421 relatedness of children to parents 337–48
planned behavior, theory of 464–68 and fit of personal goals 423–24 attachment theory on 338–40
and implementation intention psychophysiological states 355 and categories of children’s
strategies 470–71 psychotherapy 417–34 motivation 338–40
integration with self-determination future research, directions for 433–34 contextual variations in 345–47
theory 472–74 motivational constructs in 418–21 importance of early 344–45
Plant, R. W. 425 motivational factors in 424–31 and non-relatedness factors 347
PNS (parasympathetic nervous personal goals in 421–24 self-determination theory on 340–42
system) 179, 180 PTSD (posttraumatic stress disorder) 76 and sense of responsibility 342–44
Pomerantz, E. M. 343–45 Pugh, Kevin J. 216–18 relationship-specific identities 49
portable electroencephalography Punnett square 219f relative autonomy 93
headsets 356 purpose religious orientation 80
positive adaptation 96 and achievement goals 236 Renninger, K. A. 445, 450, 454
positive affect 488 defined 236 reproductive fitness 323
positive reinforcement 497–98 pursuit of goals. See goal pursuit resource allocation 516–17
positive youth development Püschel, O. 423 rest and replenishment 124
framework 489 restraint intensity 386
postactional phase 295 Q retention 14
Postmes, T. 512 quadrant model 171 retrospective self-administered
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) 76 questionnaires questionnaire 175
potential-based achievement goals 239 for assessment of explicit motives 420 reward, and work motivation 508
potential motivation 374, 374f, 383 for assessment of personal goals 420–21 reward-aversion model 160
Powers, T. A. 423 Quinn, D. M. 332 reward cues 275–76
Powers, William 28, 37 Quirin, M. 398, 403, 404 reward prediction error 363
Pratto, F. 332 reward processing 276, 276t, 278, 279
preactional phase 295 R Reznik, I. 49
precision-effect estimate with standard Radtke, T. 402 RFT. See regulatory focus theory
error (PEESE) 119–20 Ramsay and Pang study 193 Richardson, M. 21
precision-effect test (PET) 119–20 randomized controlled trials 477–79 Richter, M. 377
predecisional phase 295 rational system 36 Riediger, M. 294
predictive and reactive control systems raven matrices 251 rigidity/flexibility 260
(PARCS) 394, 405 Rawolle, M. 197 riskiness 54–55
pre-ejection period (PEP) 376 Raynor, J. 287 Roberts, G. C. 495
pre-ejection period (PEP) reactivity 382 reduction to appearance 322 Roberts, T. A. 321, 324, 327
prefrontal cortex 365 reduction to body 322 Roch, R. M. 429
prefrontal cortex (PFC) 180–81, 358 Reeve, J. 451–52, 514 Roeser, R. W. 453
prevention goals 430 referential competence 197 Roisman, G. I. 345
prevention-inducing recall activities 61 regulations 468 role models
Price, M. S. 496, 499, 500 regulatory fit paradigm 49 parents as 490, 492
primary aversion system 162 regulatory focus questionnaire 50 siblings as 493
primary goods 102 regulatory focus strength measure 50 Rösch, A. G. 429
primary reward system 162 regulatory focus theory (RFT) 47–62 Rosenberg, M. 308
principles 30 choice of motivation in 56–59 Rosenzweig, E. Q. 452
priority management 35 desired end states 53 Rosiman, G. 345
procedural justice 518 “fit” in 59–60 Rotter, J. B. 4, 11
Index 541
Routledge, C. 72, 76 self-continuity 294 self-enhancement and self-protection
Rozek, C. S. 451 self control 307–15
Rubicon model of action phases 427–28, and automatization 124 and better-than-average effect 309–10
427f and autonomy 125 five pillars of 308
Russell, Bertrand 315 and brain glucose 121–22 functionality of 307–8
Ryan, R. M. 423, 425, 489 defined 113 and overclaiming 313–14
Ryan, Richard 90, 92, 96, 286 and depletion model 114 and selective self-memory 310–12
Ryff, C. D. 430 and ego strength 115 and self-serving bias 308–9
and endurance 126 and socially desirable responding
S and expectancy 121 312–13
Sachse, R. 431 and morbidity 114 self-esteem
Saguy, T. 331, 332 and smoking 126 and cultural worldviews 77
Sailer, P. 261 self-control strength. see ego strength and flow 173
Sapieja, K. M. 491–92 self-decision 401 and mortality salience hypothesis 74
SBP (systolic blood pressure) 376, 380 self-determination 196 self-evaluation 384
scaling up 452–53 self-determination theory (SDT) 89–104, self-evaluative process 68
Scheier, M. F 68 136, 196, 394, 403 self-infiltration 394, 404
Schiefele, U. 449 2 × 2 achievement goal perspectives self-inhibition 403–5
Schimel, J. 71 and 474–75 self-integration. See integrative self
schizophrenia 255 autonomy-supportive vs. controlling self-internalization 404
Schmitt, C. H. 420 climates in 95–96 self-modeling 15
Schnetter, K. 249 and basic psychological needs 90 self-motivation 403
Schoenrade, P. 80 basic psychological needs in 90–92 self-objectification 322. See also sexual
Scholer, A. 57, 61 and behaviorism 90 and self-objectification
school(s) controlled motivation in 91–92 self-positivity 400
influence of, on motivation 447–48 and cultural values 101 self-positivity bias and self-
transitions between 448–50 economic/political systems in integration 400–405
school reform 457 102–4 self-protection
Schöttke, H. 426 in exercise and physical activity 464, defined 307
Schrager, S. M. 237 468–69, 471–72 and terror management theory 308
Schramm, E. 421 future research, directions in 517 See also self-enhancement and
Schulte, D. 423 and goals 97 self-protection
Schultheiss, O. C. 193, 429 integration with planned behavior self-referenced goal orientation 469
Schultz, W. 362, 363 theory 472–74 self-regulation 15
scientific management 510 internalization and extrinsic defined 510
SDR (socially desirable responding) motivation in 92–93 and goal setting 515
312–13 intrinsic vs. extrinsic aspirations in and habitual responses 258
SDT. See self-determination theory 97–102 and resource allocation 516–17
Sears, P. S. 232 life goals in 96–97 self-regulation theory 510–12
Sedikides, C. 312 motivation and wellness within 90 self-regulatory behavior 27–42
SEEKING system 162 and needs 90 dual process models of 36
selective self-memory 310–12 and need satisfaction 103 and feedback control 28–31
and defensiveness 314 neural mechanisms in 406–7 and hierarchicality 36–37
nonmotivational factors in 314 relationships in 96–97 “how” vs. “what” of 40–41
origins of 311 and self-actualization 90 and impulse vs. control 37–40
and self-boosting 311 and student motivation 444 and priority management 35–36
self-access 400–404 taxonomy of motivation 92f self-regulatory depletion 142, 144
self-affirmation 308 underlying systems in 403 self-regulatory processes 15
self-appraisals of ability 490 and work motivation 513–14 self-relaxation 402–3
self-awareness work motivation in 513–14 self-report questionnaire 236
defined 68 youth sport motivation in 488, 489 self-serving bias (SSB) 308
objective self-awareness theory 68 self-determined motivation 489 self-system therapy (SST) 429–30, 433
self-awareness theory 81, 384 self-doubt 402 self-threat 308, 311
escaping self-awareness 81–82 self-efficacy 4, 18–22, 173 self-threatening feedback 311
and existential anxiety 81 effects of 20–21 self variables, in motivation theory 443
self-boosting 311 and gender 23 sequences 37
self-centrality breeds self-enhancement and performance 512 serious gaming 433
principle 308 and physical activity 464 Sevincer, A. T. 424
self-concepts 397 in planned behavior theory 465, 466 sexism, benevolent 324
self-concordance, of personal goals and self-regulatory processes 21 sex role stereotypes 328
422–23 in social cognitive theory 18–22, 511 sexual and self-objectification 321–32
self-congruence 397 sources of 18–19 culture of 327–30
self-consciousness, loss of 170 in student motivation 444 as existential protection 324–25
542 Index
the motivating but consequential body socially desirable responding (SDR) T
project 331–32 312–13 TARGET approach 447, 451
as power/dominance 323–24 social projection 259 task goal orientation 490–91
situation motivators of 330 social psychological-based task involvement 231
as system justification 325–26 interventions 452–53, 455–57 task-oriented motivation 475
theoretical frameworks for 322–23 social relationships task value 445
sexual objectification and learning 443–44 taxonomies of health-related behavior 478
and anxiety 324 in youth sport motivation 488 teacher–student relationships 447–48
benevolent sexism 324 social support team-building activities 497
and body image 324 from parents 491 team members 518
cognitive development theories in and student motivation 448 technology 528–30
328–30 sociocultural approach to telenomic model of subjective well-being
and female menstruation 325 motivation 447 421–22, 422f
and internalization 332 Soenens, Bart 238 temporal dynamics
and mortality concerns 325 Sperance, A. L. 498, 500 future research directions in 178–79
and power 323 spontaneous alteration 158 research on 178
and self-objectification 324 spontaneous satisfactions 366 study of 177
and sex role stereotypes 328–30 sport commitment model 489 temporal perception 170
socialization theories in 327–28 sports. See youth sport and physical tension state 293
Share, T. 429 activity terror management theory (TMT)
Sheeran, P. 467, 473–74 sport specialization, sampling vs. 494 67–83, 308
Sheldon, K. M. 429, 433 SSB (self-serving bias) 308 and cultural differences 75, 77–78
Sheldon, Kennon 286, 299 SST (self-system therapy) 429–30, 433 current research utilizing 70–72
Short Flow Scale 174 stability goals 294 development of 69–70
sibling rivalries 493 Stangier, U. 424 existential anxiety in 72
siblings 492–93 sternberg memory task 380 and inevitability of death 70
Silvestrini, N. 386 Stevenson, C. L. 493 and mortality salience 385
Simon effect 258, 259 Stewart, C. C. 380 personal dimension in 74–76
Simon task 258 Stewart, Victoria C. 216–17 physical dimension in 72–74
situated cognition 274 stimulus perception 255 and posttraumatic stress disorder
situational interest 207, 208t, 213 Stipek, D. J. 447 (PTSD) 76
Skaalvik, E. 446 strategic vigilance 54 self-awareness in 67–69
Skinner, B. F. 3, 4, 274 Strauman, T. J. 424, 429–30 social dimension in 76–79
Slabbinck, H. 420 striatum 276, 355, 360 spiritual dimension in 79–81
Smith, A. L. 494–96 stroop interference task 115, 119 and theory perspective on human
Smith, J. 449 stroop test 123 motivation 67
Smith, K. 429 Stuart, M. E. 490 transcendence of death in 81–82
Smith, R. E. 497–500 student achievement testing practices 454–55
Smoll, F. L. 497–98 social competence and 448 test-operate-test-exit (TOTE) 27, 30
SNS (sympathetic nervous system) 179 and student–teacher relationship 448 Theeboom, M. 499
social cognitive theory 11–24 student autonomy 451–52, 454 Thematic Appreciation Test 187, 248,
conceptual framework of 12–13 student motivation 270, 419
future research, directions for 22–24 school’s influence on 447–48 theories of reality 77
motivational processes in 15–18 school transitions effect on 448–50 theory of planned behavior (TPB)
and self-efficacy 18–22 source of 444–47 and control theory 511
self-efficacy in 18–22 student–teacher relationships 447–48 and motivation 517
self-regulatory processes in 15 Stuntz, C. P. 495, 498, 500 and work motivation 509–10
symbolic processes in 15 subcortical insula 363 theory of resultant valence 232
vicarious processes in 13–15 subjective norms 465 therapeutic relationship, fostering
and work motivation 511 substance abuse disorders 424 425–26, 432
and youth sport motivation 489 suffering, as therapy motivation 419 therapy expectations 419
social cognitive transference 49 Sullivan, Anne 206 therapy motivation
social comparisons 17–18 superego 405 defined 417
social competence supervisors 518 See also psychotherapy
and peer leadership 496 suppression-oriented implementation 256 Thomas, C. 384
and student motivation 448 Svoboda, R. C. 451 threat immunity 310
social–environmental factors 488 symbolic immortality 75, 79 3 × 2 model 235
social goal orientations 495 symbolic processes 15 time perspective 292
social influence, in youth sport sympathetic nervous system (SNS) 179 TMT. see terror management theory
motivation 489. See also specific system justification theory 326 Tolman, E. C. 247
groups, e.g. parents systems approach, to youth sport Tolman, Edward Chase 4
socialization, parental 489–90 motivation in families 494 Tomkins, Silvan 160–61
social learning theory 11 systolic blood pressure (SBP) 376, 380 Tooby, John 89
Index 543
Tops, M. 398 ventromedial prefrontal cortex 100, 358 in goal-setting theory 510
TOTE (test-operate-test-exit) 27, 30 vicarious processes 13–15 and needs theories 508–9
TPB. See theory of planned behavior; VIE (values–instrumentality–expectancy) in regulatory focus theory 512–13
theory of planned behavior theory 509, 515, 517 in self-determination theory 513–14
transformational leadership volitional decision making 368 in self-regulation theory 510–12
behaviors 498–99 volitional strength 198 work-avoidant goal orientation 446
Transient Hypofrontality Theory 181 Vroom, V. H. 509 work motivation 507–19
transtheoretical model (TTM) 428 in achievement goal theory 512
treatment fidelity 478 W in expectancy-value theory 509–10
treatment goals Wadden, Thomas 252 future research, directions for
formulation of 421, 426–27, 432–33 Wahba, M. A. 509 514–19
in psychotherapy 419, 420t Walkington, C. 454 in goal-setting theory 510
Trzesniewski, K. H. 450 Watson, J. B. 274 and needs theories 508–9
TTM (transtheoretical model) 428 Watson, John 3, 4 in regulatory focus theory 512–13
Turner, J. C. 447 Watt, Helen M. G. 215–16 in self-determination theory 513–14
Twenge, J. M. 101 WBT (well-being therapy) 430, 433 in self-regulation theory 510–12
2 × 2 model 233–35, 469–70 weekend effect 103 Work-Related Flow Scale 174
Weinert, F. E. 428 Wright, R. A. 380
U Weinstein, N. 96 Wright, Wilbur 528
Ullrich-French, S. 494–96 Weiss, M. R. 492–97, 499, 500
unclear difficulty 375 well-being 198 Y
undermining effect 367 dimensions of 430 Yalom, Irvin 74, 76
unfixed difficulty 375 and employee motivation 519 Yamauchi, H 93
Unmüssig, C. 421 telenomic model of subjective Yeager, D. S. 450, 456–57
Urdan, T. 240, 447 421–22, 422f Yeomans, P. D. 431
urge magnitude 386 well-being therapy (WBT) 430, 433 younger siblings 493
utility value 364, 445, 451 Wellborn, J. G. 444 youth sport and physical activity
Wentzel, K. 448 487–502
V Westbrook, Russell 492 best practices for motivation in 501–2
Vallerand, R. J. 473 White, A. 495 coach influence in 497–500
values 17, 418, 420, 445–46 White, R.W. 91 cultural significance of 487
values–instrumentality–expectancy (VIE) White, S. A. 491 parental influence in 489–92
theory 509, 515, 517 Wicklund, R. A. 68, 81 peer influence in 494–97
Van Heil, A. 98 Wigfield, A. 338, 449, 452 sibling influence in 492–93
Vansteenkiste, Maarten 98, 238 Wilko, A. M. 500 studies in 488–89
van Yperen, N. W. 512 Williams, Serena 492
Veling, H. 276 Williams, Venus 492 Z
Ventis, W. L. 80 Wincott, D. W. 398 Zafeiriou, A. 378
ventral striatum 358 wonder 162 Zedelius, Claire 276
ventral tegmental area 360 work 507–19 Zimbardo, P. G. 292
ventrolateral prefrontal cortex 403 in achievement goal theory 512 Zimmerman, Barry 290, 365
ventromedial prefrontal-amygdala in expectancy-value theory 509–10 Znoj, H. 428
pathway 361 future research, directions for 514–19 Zuroff, D. C. 419, 424–25
544 Index