Child Sexual Behavior Inventory A Dutch-Speaking Normative Sample
Child Sexual Behavior Inventory A Dutch-Speaking Normative Sample
The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
located on the World Wide Web at:
http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/104/4/885
Eric Schoentjes, MD*; Dirk Deboutte, PhD‡; and William Friedrich, PhD§
ABSTRACT. Objectives. To describe normative sex- development, and an explosion of studies about sex-
ual behavior in Dutch-speaking children; to assess the ual abuse, only a few studies have concentrated on
frequencies of different types of sexual behaviors re- normal child sexual development and experience.2
ported in children by their parents; to analyze the rela- Until recently, knowledge about sexual development
tion of these sexual behaviors to demographic, personal, and behavior of children, mostly based on adult
familial, and general behavioral variables; and to com-
pare the Dutch-speaking sample with American samples.
recollection of sexual feeling or activities during
Method. Nine hundred seventeen children (2–12 childhood years, was often anecdotal or derived
years of age), screened to exclude sexual abuse, were from small or biased samples. Remarkably scarce
rated by their caregiver via parent report consisting of the information based on large-scale surveys or observa-
translated Dutch version of the Child Sexual Behavior tions exists with regard to child sexual development.
Inventory, the Child Behavior Checklist, a life event In the past decade however, the results of a few
checklist, and a questionnaire assessing family nudity large-scale surveys on this subject have been pub-
and parental attitudes regarding sexuality. lished.3–5 A promising instrument used in some of
Results. Frequencies of a wide variety of sexual be- these surveys is the Child Sexual Behavior Inventory
haviors for 2- to 5-, 6- to 9-, and 10- to 12-year-old children
are presented. Sexual behavior was found to be related to
(CSBI).3,4 This parental report measure consists of
the child’s age, maternal education, family nudity, and ;40 questions on sexual behavior and interest. It has
parental attitudes toward sexuality. The positive relation been well-validated in two large-scale surveys of 2-
to general behavioral problems was confirmed. Findings to 12-year-old children.3,4 Since its first publication in
were primarily similar to previously published American Pediatrics in 1991, this method has been increasingly
studies. used in different studies to measure and describe
Conclusion. Our study confirms that sexual behavior sexual behaviors for different categories of children.
in children is varied and related to developmental, per- Its use has also been recommended by different au-
sonal, and familial factors. The relative frequency of the thors for the assessment of sexually-abused chil-
wide variety of sexual behaviors in a Dutch-speaking 6 –10
dren.
normative sample is comparable to American samples.
Pediatrics 1999;104:885– 893; sexual behavior, children, Large-scale studies including direct observation of
family sexuality, Child Sexual Behavior Inventory. children’s sexual behavior, or questioning of the chil-
dren about their sexuality, have not yet been per-
formed to this date. Cultural attitudes and values
ABBREVIATIONS. CSBI, Child Sexual Behavior Inventory; CBCL, about sexuality, and in particular childhood sexual-
Child Behavior Checklist.
ity, certainly hamper this kind of research. However,
retrospective self-report studies, specifically adult
W
ith the ever-increasing focus on the sexual recollections about sexual feelings or activities as a
abuse of children, pediatricians are often child, have been used to document sexual behavior
confronted with questions from parents or in children as normal.
other caregivers about the normality of a child’s sex- Despite controversies about the reliance on paren-
ual behavior or interest in sexual matters. As Finkel- tal (or other caregivers) observations and reports on
hor1 wrote: “We know more about sexual deviance the behavior of their children, they are widely used
than we do about sexual normality. . . (We have) a in clinical and developmental research. Several rea-
vast ignorance of the forces governing the develop- sons support the use of parental report in research
ment and experience of sexual behavior in general.” about children’s development. First, is the expertise
Although in the last 3 or 4 decades there has been a of the parent, who typically is recognized as the most
significant increase in studies of infant and child knowledgeable about their young child’s life. Sec-
ondly, the questionnaire method allows more chil-
dren to be studied than the use of more intrusive
From the *Fund for Scientific Research, Flanders, Belgium; the ‡University methods like observation. Thirdly, the use of paren-
of Antwerp and Antwerp University Center for Child and Adolescent 11
tal questionnaires is relatively economical.
Psychiatry, Antwerp, Belgium; and the §Department of Psychiatry and
Psychology, Mayo Clinic and the Mayo Medical School, Rochester, Minne- Similar to other areas of child development, sexual
sota. behavior is influenced by environmental factors.
Received for publication Oct 13, 1998; accepted Mar 3, 1999. These factors are at the level of the society, and at the
Reprint requests to (E.S.) Antwerp University Center for Child and Ado- level of the family in which the child grows up.
lescent Psychiatry, Lindendreef 1, 2020 Antwerpen, Belgium. E-mail:
[email protected]
Friedrich et al3,4 demonstrated that sexual behavior
PEDIATRICS (ISSN 0031 4005). Copyright © 1999 by the American Acad- in children is related to the child’s family context,
emy of Pediatrics. most specifically, sexual behavior in the family and
ARTICLES 887
Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on March 22, 2007
TABLE 2. Rank Order of Endorsement Frequencies for Shared Items of the CSBI-Versions
No. Item (Abbreviated) CSBI-Antwerp CSBI-13 CSBI-34
(n 5 917) (n 5 880) (n 5 1114)
10 Puts mouth on another child’s/adult’s sex parts 0.2 0.1 0.2
29 Wants to watch TV nudity 0.7 2.7 7.7
15 Ask others to engage in sex acts 1.0 0.4 0.3
17 Puts objects in vagina/rectum 1.6 0.9 0.8
42 Touches animals’ sex parts 1.6 1.3 1.5
7 Masturbates with object 2.0 0.8 3.9
19 Imitates sexual behavior with dolls/stuffed animal 2.9 3.2 1.6
28 Undresses other children 3.1 2.6 1.2
16 Rubs body against people or furniture 4.4 6.7 4.3
14 Makes sexual sounds 4.6 1.4 1.4
9 Imitates the act of sexual intercourse 5.0 1.1 –
40 Overly friendly with men 5.2 7.1 2.8
2 Wants to be opposite sex 5.7 4.9 5.4
30 Puts tongue in mouth when kissing 5.9 2.5 2.3
37 Puts mouth on breast 7.1 2.6 3.0
36 Stands too close 7.2 11.6 21.3
4 Masturbates with hands 7.5 15.3 12.4
33 If girl, overly aggressive, if boy, overly passive 7.9 10.4 –
45 Draws sex parts 7.9 – 2.3
13 Pretends to be opposite sex when playing 8.3 13.0 –
31 Hugs adults not well-known 8.4 7.3 9.5
32 Shows sex parts to children 9.5 8.1 5.3
1 Dresses like opposite sex 11.8 5.8 9.3
20 Shows sex parts to adults 12.9 16.0 9.8
22 Talks about sex acts 12.9 4.7 5.3
27 Talks flirtatiously 13.1 10.6 4.9
8 Touches other people’s sex parts 14.1 6.0 –
12 Uses words that describe sex acts 14.3 8.8 –
21 Tries to look at pictures of nude people 16.3 15.5 6.7
3 Touches sex parts in public 22.1 19.7 14.4
41 Shy about undressing 22.9 38.7 –
34 Very interested in opposite sex 24.3 23.0 17.1
18 Tries to look at people when they are nude 31.7 28.5 21.4
25 Sits with crotch or underwear exposed 40.0 36.4 –
5 Scratches anal and/or crotch area 42.6 52.2 –
46 Plays doctor 42.8 – –
43 Walks around house without clothes 44.5 41.9 –
6 Touches breast 45.9 30.7 27.3
23 Kisses adults not in the family 46.5 36.2 –
35 If boy, plays girl’s toys, if girl, plays boy’s toys 46.5 53.9 –
44 Ask questions about sexuality 48.9 – –
26 Kisses other children not in the family 49.1 33.9 –
24 Undresses self in front of others 52.6 41.2 –
38 Likes to walk in underclothes 54.0 52.9 –
39 Shy with strange men 62.5 64.5 –
11 Touches sex parts at home 63.8 45.8 –
to enable comparison with the present sample. The quencies for the different items. Similar to the anal-
age and sex distributions of these samples differ only ysis of Friedrich et al,4 the children were grouped
slightly (the age and sex distribution of the sample into three age clusters. The rationale for this catego-
can be found in Table 3.). A large majority of items rization can be found in the developmental trend of
have similar endorsement frequencies across the the item mean score as reflected in Fig 1 and as
three samples. This can be illustrated by the fact that discussed above. The groups are: 2- to 5-year-olds, 6-
only 21 out of 70 possible two-group comparisons to 9-year-olds, and 10- to 12-year-olds. The frequen-
differ by .5% and only 6 differ by .10%. cies of sexual behavior in these three age groups are
presented in Table 4. An examination of Table 4
Developmental Course suggests that three categories of sexual behavior can
As suggested by the age effect on the CSBI total be observed when one takes the age of the child into
score, age was considered in the calculation of fre- account. The vast majority of the sexual behaviors as
described by the items tend to decrease in endorse- were too low, age categories were grouped. To eval-
ment frequency at increasing age (eg, “touches sex uate the age effect on the individual endorsement
parts in public,” “touches breasts,” “shows sex parts frequencies of the items, Cochran’s test of linear
to adult,” “tries to look at people when nude,” trend was computed (see Table 4).
“dresses like opposite sex”). Some seem to be more In summary, the vast majority of the items (34 of
frequent in the older-age categories (eg, “talks about 46) reflected a significant age trend (previous prob-
sex acts,” “shy about undressing,” “tries to look at ability established at .01 because of the number of
pictures of nude people”), where others do not seem calculations). Twenty-six of the behaviors were more
to vary between the different age-categories (eg, “im- common in the younger age group and 8 were more
itates sexual behavior with dolls,” “very interested in common in one of both of the older groups.
opposite sex,” “puts mouth on another child/adult
sex parts”). Internal Reliability of the Dutch Translation of the
A x2 analysis was performed to investigate these CSBI
impressions in a more formal manner. Frequency Cronbach’s a coefficient was computed and factor
tables for each item were constructed by putting the analysis was performed to assess the interrelation-
three age-categories in columns and the presence or ship among the 46 items of the Dutch translation of
absence of that behavior in rows (an item score the CSBI. All items were positively correlated with
equalling 0 meant that particular behavior was not the total CSBI score and the Cronbach’s a coefficient
observed by the parents, item scores of 1, 2, or 3 for the 46 items was 0.86. This high a coefficient is an
meant that the behavior had been observed at least argument in favor of the items being added to cal-
once in the past 6 months). When table frequencies culate a total score for the CSBI. Nevertheless, a
ARTICLES 889
Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on March 22, 2007
principal component analysis with varimax rotation However the interrelationships among the constitu-
revealed the presence of 7 stable factors. Loading of ent items in these seven domains, as reflected in their
,0.3 on one factor, and a difference of ,0.1 between individual Cronbach’s a coefficient (Table 5), is rel-
the loading of a single item on 2 different factors, atively modest. Future research is needed to further
were used as exclusionary criteria. This resulted in explore these underlying dimensions with a more
the exclusion of 7 out of 46 items. According to their heterogeneous sample to better define the factors
constituent items the 7 factors, computed by princi- through deleting or rephrasing a number of existing
pal component analysis, could be labeled as: “shame- items or by introducing new ones.
lessness,” “sexual interest,” “boundary problems,”
“gender identity problems,” “sexualized play,” “sex- Relationship of Sexual Behavior to Demographic and
ual intrusiveness,” and “genital manipulation” (see Family Variables
Table 5). Multiple regression analysis was used to examine
The presence of underlying dimensions of behav- the relation of demographic and family variables
ior in the CSBI could not be determined in the first with the CSBI mean total score. The dependent vari-
large-scale sample published by Friedrich et al14in able was defined as the mean total score on the 46
1991. However in a recent publication he suggested items of the translated CSBI. Four demographic vari-
that sexual behavior was not an unidimensional phe- ables were entered as a block; ie, age and gender of
nomenon, and in his most recent sample, six to eight the child, maternal education, and parental profes-
factors could be identified. sional activity.
The factor analysis computed on the Dutch-speak- Only two variables were significantly related to
ing sample seems to support these suggestions. the CSBI mean total score; ie, age (F 5 99.9, R2 5
Percent of total variance explained 8.88% 7.11% 5.90% 5.39% 4.97% 4.99% 6.03%
Cronbach’s a 0.81 0.67 0.57 0.70 0.56 0.49 0.66
* Items numbers 6, 8, 16, 28, 35, 39, and 46 left out.
ARTICLES 891
Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on March 22, 2007
(mean age of the reporting parents, 34.8 years) thus ual behavior (eg, “puts mouth on other child/adult
have a minimum educational status of 18 years. In sex parts,” “puts tongue in mouth when kissing,”
addition, both maternal and paternal reports were “touches animal sex parts,” “puts objects in vagina or
used, because we found that the gender of the re- rectum”).
porter added very little to the variance of the total The findings concerning the developmental trends
score. in the occurrence of different sexual behavior are
The screening question for sexually-abused chil- very similar to those reported by Friedrich et al3,4 in
dren detected 23 children or ;2.5% of the sample. the American normative samples. The individual en-
This proportion is smaller than that found by dorsement frequencies of the different items in the
Friedrich et al4 in his normative samples (;3.5%) and Dutch normative population as a whole, are also in
much smaller than the proportion that could be ex- the same order of magnitude as those found in the
pected based on the epidemiologic data concerning two other large-scale American samples. These find-
the presence of sexually-abused children in the gen- ings seem to suggest that there are no important
eral population. This important discrepancy could differences in the development and characteristics of
partially be explained by the fact that parents of sexual behavior in 2- to 12-year-old Belgian and
sexually-abused children most probably decided not American children.
to participate in the survey and did not return their This relative absence of differences is in marked
questionnaire. Another possible explanation is that contrast to two Dutch studies that used a portion of
whether a child ,12 years of age is sexually abused, the CSBI. Possible explanations for the differences
may be overlooked by caregivers, attributable to the are several. The first is that a very biased sample (1%
fact that a large portion of the disclosures of sexual of the total possible) was used in one study.13 The
abuse take place in preadolescent or adolescent second is that a pediatrician well-known to the par-
years. Epidemiologic data from the Flanders Child ents collected the data in the other.12
Welfare Department report a percentage of 43% of The relationship of sexual behavior and a number
disclosure of sexual abuse past the age of 12 years, of demographic, individual, and familial variables
and only 24.3% before the age of 6 years.16 was also illustrated in our normative sample. The
The decline in overt sexual behavior with age was significant inverse relationship with age was con-
confirmed in our normative sample when the total firmed with regression analysis. Maternal education
score on the CSBI was examined. Caregivers re-
is correlated significantly with sexual behavior, but
ported less overt sexual behavior in 10- to 12-year-
the gender of the child and the parental professional
old children in comparison with 2- to 5-year-old
status did not, which is similar to the findings for the
children. This decline does not seem to be a contin-
American samples. The relation of maternal educa-
uous phenomenon, but seems to occur in phases,
tion to increased reporting of behavior problems in
corresponding to preschool years (2–5 years), middle
children has been noted before, and Friedrich et al3,4
childhood (6 –9 years), and prepuberty (10 –12 years).
Analysis of the endorsement frequencies of the suggests that better educated parents may be in a
individual CSBI items enabled a more detailed de- position to be more observant of their children, thus
scription of the development of sexual behavior in 2- witnessing (sexual) behaviors that less observant or
to 12-year-old children. Although all the listed be- more occupied parents might miss.
haviors appear in the normative sample, there clearly Family nudity and parental attitudes regarding
existed certain behaviors that could be labeled as sexual curiosity of their child or concerning tolerance
more developmentally appropriate than others did. toward masturbation, homosexuality, or extramari-
For preschool children these include: “touches sex tal sex, were also significantly correlated to the re-
parts in public or at home,” “touches or tries to touch porting of sexual behavior of their children and ac-
mother’s breasts,” “undresses self in front of others,” counted for .10% of the explained variance, even
“kisses other children not in the family,” or “walks after removing the effect of age and gender of the
around without clothes or in underclothes.” In 6- to child, maternal education, and parental professional
9-year-old children, a number of these behaviors are status. This could reflect that the sexual behavior of
still endorsed by .20% of the population. Other children is influenced by the family environment in
behaviors tend to be more frequent in this age cate- which they are raised. A more liberal attitude regard-
gory; eg, “asks questions about sexuality” or “shy ing (childhood) sexuality, however, could be re-
about undressing”. Most items reflecting sexual cu- flected in a parent more willing to report sexual
riosity or preoccupation tend to increase with age behavior of their children, without influencing the
and are therefore relatively more frequent in the 10- behavior in itself. It seems reasonable to assume that
to 12-year-old age group. “Tries to look at picture of both mechanisms are responsible for this phenome-
(partially) nude people” and “talks about sex acts” non.
are two examples of behaviors that are infrequent in The relationship of other personal or familial vari-
the lower age group, but appear in .25% of the ables to the sexual behavior were less informative.
children older than 10 years. Separated or divorced parents seem to report higher
A number of behaviors are very unusual and were levels of sexual behavior in their children. Although
rarely endorsed in the three age categories. These the relationship is significant, it does not account for
behaviors tend to have high loadings on the same .1% of the explained variance in our normative
factor in the factor analysis, and are best labeled as sample. Similar observations were made for the
intrusive, aggressive, or more imitative of adult sex- number of children in the family (higher number
ARTICLES 893
Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on March 22, 2007
Child Sexual Behavior Inventory: A Dutch-speaking Normative Sample
Eric Schoentjes, Dirk Deboutte and William Friedrich
Pediatrics 1999;104;885-893
DOI: 10.1542/peds.104.4.885
This information is current as of March 22, 2007
http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/104/4/885#BIBL
Subspecialty Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the
following collection(s):
Neurology & Psychiatry
http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/collection/neurology_and_psychia
try
Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
http://www.pediatrics.org/misc/Permissions.shtml
Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
http://www.pediatrics.org/misc/reprints.shtml