Martin Fisher (Auth.), George Stricker, Martin Fisher (Eds.) - Self-Disclosure in The Therapeutic Relationship-Springer US (1990)
Martin Fisher (Auth.), George Stricker, Martin Fisher (Eds.) - Self-Disclosure in The Therapeutic Relationship-Springer US (1990)
Therapeutic Relationship
Self-Disclosure in the
Therapeutic Relationship
Edited by
George Stricker and Martin Fisher
Derner Institute of Advanced Psychological Studies
Adelphi University
Garden City, New Yark
v
vi Contributors
Judith C. Simon, 329 South San Antonio Road, Los Altos, California
94022
vii
viii Preface
GEORGE STRICKER
MARTIN FISHER
Contents
PART I. INTRODUCTION
Chapter 1
The Shared Experience and Self-Disclosure. . . . . . . . . . . .. . . .. . . 3
Martin Fisher
Chapter 2
Self-Disclosure in Religious Spiritual Direction: Antecedents
and Parallels to Self-Disclosure in Psychotherapy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17
Bede J. Healey
Historical Perspectives of Self-Disclosure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17
Conditions for Self-Disclosure. . . .. . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . 19
Self-Disclosure in Religion and Psychotherapy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21
How Spiritual Direction Differs from Psychotherapy. . . . . . . . . . . . 22
Self-Disclosure in Spiritual Direction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
The Desert Fathers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24
Self-Disclosure in Practice. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25
Conclusion. . . . .. . . . . .. ... . . . . . .. . . .. .. . . . . .. . . . . . . .. . . . . .. . 26
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
ix
x Contents
Chapter 3
Self-Disclosure and Classical Psychoanalysis. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
Robert C. Lane and James W Hull
The Problem of Self-Revelation ............................... 31
Self-Disclosure and the Early Analysts. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33
Contemporary Views on Self-Disclosure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37
Special Events . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41
Discussion ................................................. 42
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44
Chapter 4
Show and Tell: Reflections on the Analyst's Self-Disclosure 47
Sa bert Basescu
Chapter 5
Self-Disclosure in Rational-Emotive Therapy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61
Windy Dryden
The Basic Principles of Rational-Emotive Therapy... . ..... . ... . . 61
The RET Approach to Client Self-Disclosure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62
Therapist Self-Disclosure in RET. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65
Overcoming Obstacles to Disclosure in Clients and Therapists. . 71
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74
Chapter 6
Self-Disclosure in Psychotherapy and the
Psychology of the Self. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
Lawrence Josephs
A Developmental Analog for the Process of Self-Disclosure in
Psychotherapy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
Implications for Psychotherapy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
Conclusion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88
Contents xi
Chapter 7
The Role of Implicit Communication in Therapist
Self-Disclosure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
Jonathan M. Jackson
Traditional Views of Self-Disclosure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
Special Events . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96
Progressive Views of Self-Disclosure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97
Characteristics of Neutrality-Seeking Therapists. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101
Chapter 8
Transference, Countertransference, and Therapeutic Efficacy in
Relation to Self-Disclosure by the Analyst. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103
Esther Menaker
Chapter 9
Self-Disclosure and the Nonwhite Ethnic Minority Patient 117
Adelbert H. Jenkins
The Racial Context of American Society . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118
Engaging the Minority Client in Psychotherapy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 119
Managing the Early Sessions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120
''Agency'' in the Minority Client. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121
Language and Psychotherapy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 123
The Therapist's Skill: "Linguistic Competence". .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124
Sociolinguistic Issues in Psychotherapy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124
Self-Disclosure to Oneself. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 126
Some Dynamics of Language Difference in Therapy. . . . . . . . . . . . 127
Therapist Self-Disclosure .................................... 129
The "Contextual Unit" . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130
Conclusion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 131
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 131
xii Contents
Chapter 10
Feminist Therapy Perspectives on Self-Disclosure. . . . . . . . . . . . . 135
Laura S. Brown and Lenore E. A. Walker
Introduction. . . . . . .. . . . .. . . . . . . . ... . . . . . . . . .. . . . . .. . .. . . . . . . 135
A Brief Review of Self-Disclosure in Feminist Therapy Theory. . . 136
Self-Disclosure in Feminist Therapy Theory.. . . .. . . . . . . .. . . . . . . 139
The Nature of Self-Disclosure in Feminist Therapy. . . . . .. . . . . . . . 141
Problems in the Use of Self-Disclosure in Feminist Therapy. . . . . 144
Current Applications of Self-Disclosure in Feminist Therapy. . . . 148
Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151
Chapter 11
Self-Disclosure and Psychotherapy with Children and
Adolescents. .. . . . .. . . . . . .. . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . .. . . 157
Nicholas Papouchis
Introduction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 157
The Concept of Self-Disclosure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 157
Self-Disclosure in Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy with Adults . . . 159
Self-Disclosure with Children and Adolescents:
A Developmental Perspective. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 162
The Therapist's Self-Disclosure with Adolescents. . . . . . . . .. . . . . . 167
The Therapeutic Alliance and Self-Disclosure.. . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . 169
The Adolescent Therapist as an Object of Identification . . . . . . . . . 170
Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 172
References ........ :. . . ... .. . . . . .. . . . . . . . . ... . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . 172
Chapter 12
Self-Disclosure in Psychotherapy: Working with Older Adults 175
Lisa R. Greenberg
Introduction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 175
Role of Self-Disclosure for Older People .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 176
Psychotherapy and the Elderly . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 180
Self-Disclosure by Therapist. . . .. . . . . . . . . .. . . .. . . . . . . .. . . .. . . . 183
Self-Disclosure by Older Patients . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 185
Contents xiii
Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 188
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 188
Chapter 13
Self-Disclosure in Group Psychotherapy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 191
Sophia Vinogradov and Irvin D. Yalom
Chapter 14
Criteria for Therapist Self-Disclosure 207
Judith C. Simon
Introduction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 207
The Study. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 208
Findings .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 209
Criteria .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 212
Modeling. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 213
Discussion ........................................ . . . . . . . .. 220
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 224
Chapter 15
Self-Disclosure in Holocaust Survivors: Effects on the Next
Generation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 227
Arlene Cahn Gordon
Introduction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 227
Self-Disclosure in Stages of Survival. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 228
Self-Disclosure: A Developmental Framework. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 232
Referential Activity: A Measure of Self-Disclosure. . . . . . . . . . . . .. 233
The Study of Self-Disclosure in Survivors and Their Children. .. 234
Unique and Universal Implications. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 237
Therapeutic Implications. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 242
xiv Contents
Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 243
References.... . . . . . . .. . .. ... . . . . . . . .... . . . . . . . ... . . . . .. . . . .. 243
Chapter 16
From Secrecy to Self-Disclosure: Healing the Scars of Incest. .. 247
Mary Gail Frawley
Chapter 17
Issues in the Disclosure of Perinatal Death ...... . . . . . . . . . . . . . 261
Douglas]. Peddicord
The Problem of Perinatal Death. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 261
Unique Characteristics of Perinatal Death ....... ; . . . . . . .. . . .. .. 262
The Impact on Parents.............. .................... ..... 264
Barriers to Disclosure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 267
Self-Disclosure and Its Effects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 271
References. . . .... .. . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . .. . ... . .. . . .. . . . . . . .. 272
Chapter 18
Self-Disclosure and Psychotherapy. .. . . ... . . . . .. . ... . . . . . . . .. 277
George Stricker
Index...................................................... 291
I
Introduction
1
The Shared Experience
and Self-Disclosure
Martin Fisher
3
4 Martin Fisher
patient puts up a struggle in the interest of his illness against the person who
is helping him. How improbable such an assertion must sound! Yet it is true;
and when its improbability is pointed out to us, we need only reply that it is
not without analogies. A man who has gone to the dentist because of an
unbearable toothache will nevertheless try to hold the dentist back when he
approaches the sick tooth with a pair of forceps. (pp. 286-287)
In this description Freud has outlined the profound essence of what
sets psychoanalytic inquiry apart from other psychotherapies that are
not described as psychodynamic in nature.
In a chapter that I wrote for a previous volume (1982), I point out
what seemed to me a critical distinction between the Freudian view of
resistance and an existential exploration referred to by Bugental (1965).
As significant a place as Freud accords the resistance, existential-
analytic theory regards this discovery as even more fundamental. The
resistance shown by the patient in the therapeutic session-the ways in
which he/she avoids awareness, displaced responsibility, maintains his/
her alienation, and so on-is the very pattern through which the patient
maintains hislher inauthentic relation to life, is the very source of his
nonbeing (p. 88).
In the above model, Freud implies that the patient's resistance is to the
process of psychoanalysis and/or the psychoanalyst and results in the pa-
tient's unconscious avoidance of these external forces. Freud emphasizes this
idea when he writes: I cannot recommend my colleagues emphatically
enough to take as a model in psychoanalytic treatment the surgeon who puts
aside his own feelings, including that of human sympathy .... The justifica-
tion for his coldness in feeling in the analyst is that it is the condition which
brings the greatest advantage to both persons involved. (191211964)
Freud (1949/1964) makes a clear-cut distinction between analyst and
patient, with the analyst invariably being the more "grown-up" of the
two. He makes this explicit when he states: "Too many neurotics have
remained so infantile that in analysis too they can only be treated as
children."
Another view of resistance, more consistent with my own, is ex-
pressed by the existential psychoanalyst Bugental (1965). In his book
The Search for Authenticity, Bugental contends that
... the resistance is the shield the patient erects to forestall the feared con-
frontation with the reality of his being in the world. (Thus, the therapist who
thinks of the resistance as a warding off of his own efforts misses the point
and confuses the patient.) The therapist's task is to help the patient redis-
cover the conflict within himself that gave rise to the resistance and other
defenses and constrictive maneuvers. (p. 43)
In truth then, the patient resists not the therapist and/or therapy but
the dread of discovering himself. As Bugental continues,
Resistance is the name that we give to the general defensive wall the patient
The Shared Experience and Self-Disclosure 7
puts between himself and the threats that he finds linked to being authentic.
Resistance is (simply) anti-authenticity. (p. 43)
We have moved, then, away from the psychoanalyst's couch into the
wider and real-life experience of the world.
The existential model of resistance implies threat. Simply stated, it
represents the threat of nonbeing. The resistance, then, is humanity's
constant effort to avoid the pain of feeling, thinking, or reexperiencing
nonbeing. There seems little doubt that the prototype for this potential
nonbeing lies in the early traumatic feelings of potential nonbeing that
each separation repeatedly arouses in the infant.
In viewing Freud's model, he implies that the patient's resistance is
to the process of psychoanalysis and/or the psychoanalyst and results
in the patient's unconscious (and sometimes conscious) avoidance of
these external forces. In reviewing Bugental's and the existential posi-
tion the resistance can be seen to be a fear and avoidance of the patient's
own self-induced (conscious and unconscious) meeting the real (au-
thentic) self; the fear of discovery, the fear of being seen as, or feeling in
some way, damaged. The patient then is avoiding coming to terms with
his/her own existence in the belief that he/she is somehow bad and/or
damaged and rejectable.
At this time it should be noted that resistance is not confined to the
analytic consulting room; neither is the transference phenomenon
which is also in the service of the resistance. These two phenomena
(transference and resistance) are now readily recognized to be ubiqui-
tous and are present in the individual in therapy as well as in those who
never see the inside of a therapy consulting room.
A further extension of the existential view is that humans seek to
avoid anxiety-the anxiety of freedom. Freedom represents a world of
increased contingencies. An increase in contingencies represents more
possibility of failure, rejection, and loss of self-esteem. More pointedly,
however, this same acceptance of freedom brings the reality of ultimate
nonbeing (death) painfully into awareness.
Think, if you will, what is implied in the existential view of authen-
ticity; authenticity representing the antithesis of resistance. Bugental
suggests that the authentic person is, at first, broadly aware of himself,
his interpersonal relationships, and all dimensions of his real world.
Secondly, the authentic person accepts the fact that life represents
choices, that he goes forth to meet those choices, and that decisions are
the very stuff of life. And third, the authentic person assumes full re-
sponsibility for approaching these contingencies, making decisions,
and accepting full responsibility for his acts.
It is agreed that transference is unconscious and distorted (by defi-
nition) and arises out of early childhood experiences.
8 Martin Fisher
that his discoveries and revelations will not result in his own rejection (at
least) and his psychological death (at worst).
What emerges in the diminished capacity for basic trust appears to
be the loss or diminution of intimate relatedness. Distrust encourages
"secrets" (banished to the preconscious or unconscious); intimacy
(shared experience) encourages openness.
The literature that points to the effect on the infant of mothering
(parenting) is rich and voluminous. A. Balint (1965) points out that
... maternal love is the almost perfect counterpart to the love for the
mother .... Thus, just as the mother is to the child, so is the child to the
mother-an object of gratification. And just as the child does not recognize
the separate identity of the mother, so the mother looks upon her child as part
of herself whose interests are identical with her own. The relation between
mother and child is built upon the interdependence of the reciprocal instinc-
tual aims. What Ferenczi said about the relation of man and woman in coitus
holds true for his mother-infant relation. He meant that in coitus there can be
no question of egoism (love interest of the self) or altruism (love interest of
the other); there is only mutuality, i.e., what is good for one is right for the
other also. In consequence of the natural interdependence of the reciprocal
instinctual aims there is no need to be concerned about the partner's well-
being. (p. 101)
References
Balint, A. (1965). Primary love and psychoanalytic technique (M. Balint ed.) New York:
Liveright.
Balint, M. (1965). Primary love and psychoanalytic technique (M. Balint ed.) New York:
Liveright.
The Shared Experience and Self-Disclosure 15
Bugental, J.ET. (1965). The search for authenticity. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Eisenbud, R.J. (1977). Personal communication.
Erikson, E. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton & Co.
Fisher, M. (1977). The potential for authentic relatedness in group psychoanalysis. Group
Process, 7 (2), 141-150.
Fisher, M. (1983). Intimacy. (M. Fisher & G. Stricker Eds.) New York: Plenum.
Frank, K. (1977). The human dimension in psychoanalytic practice. New York: Grune &
Stratton.
Freud, S. (1966). The complete introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. New York: w.w. Nor-
ton. (Originally published, 1917).
Goldman, H.A. (1988). Paradise destroyed: The crime of being born. Contemporary Psycho-
analysis, 24, (3), 420-450.
Gilland, B.E., James, R.K., & Bowman, J.T. (1989). Theories and strategies in counseling and
psychotherapy (2nd ed.) Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.
Peterson, R., & Moran, G. (1988). Attachment theory. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, (6),
611-636.
Racker, H. (1968). Transference and countertransference. New York: International Universities
Press.
Schafer, R. (1973). The idea of resistance. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 54, 259.
Wolstein, B. (1971). Human psyche in psychoanalysis. Chicago: Ill: c.c. Thomas.
Yalom, I. (1980). Existential psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books.
2
Self-Disclosure in Religious
Spiritual Direction
ANTECEDENTS AND PARALLELS TO
SELF-DISCLOSURE IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
Bede J. Healey
17
18 Bede J. Healey
between the director and the directee, but the director is primarily a
facilitator of the primary relationship, that of God and the directee.
May (1982) points out that there have been periods when "psycho-
logical phenomena were seen in only spiritual terms," and then another,
more recent period where "spirituality was often seen in psychological
terms" (p. 4). He differentiates the two in terms of content and intent.
Regarding content, psychotherapy focuses on the emotional and mental
processes. Spiritual direction focuses on prayer, the relationship with
God, and religious experiences. The intent of psychotherapy, according
to May, is to encourage more efficient living, and to achieve a sense of
self-mastery both over oneself and one's life circumstances. Spiritual
direction's intent is to foster a sense of freedom from attachments and a
surrender to God and God's will. An attachment can be anything in a
person's life that clouds his or her vision and sense of God's will. As a
person becomes less concerned with nonessentials, that person will be
able to attend to what is essential, a deeper understanding of what that
person believes to be God's will for him or her.
None of the other attempts at differentiation seem as useful and as
clear as May's distinction of content and intent. The benefit of this ap-
proach toward distinguishing psychotherapy from spiritual direction is
that one can consider the tools as not belonging to either, but being
common to both. This is consonant with Leech's notion of the simi-
larities between the two approaches mentioned previously.
Certainly Merton, even though he does not use the word, places
great emphasis on the importance of self-disclosure. This is nothing
24 Bede J. Healey
One can see the similarity here between this statement and the
belief in psychoanalytic theory that it is often the offhand comments or
seemingly innocuous thoughts that can have the most meaning.
Merton (1968), in summarizing the value of understanding the De-
sert tradition for present-day spiritual direction, states:
The Master does not merely lecture or instruct. He has to know and analyze
the inmost thoughts of the disciple. The most important part of direction is
the openness with which the disciple manifests to the spiritual Father not
only all his acts, but all his thoughts. (p.17)
To make his point Merton then quotes St. Anthony: "The monk
must make known to the elders every step he takes and every drop of
water he drinks in his cell, to see if he is not doing wrong" (p. 17).
The totality of self-disclosure is thus emphasized.
Self-Disclosure in Practice
Conclusion
Self-disclosure is integral to the religious practice of spiritual direc-
tion, and is a common element in both direction and therapy. While
religion can claim historical precedence, psychotherapy has done much
recently to increase our understanding of the role and uses of self-
disclosure. My reading of the literature shows a willingness on the part
of religion to use (too willingly? see Kemp, 1985) psychotherapy's ad-
vances and apply them to the practice of spiritual direction. May (1982)
feels that a too-ready application of psychological principles closes one
off from the rich spiritual heritage of the Western spiritual tradition.
Psychotherapy, on the other hand, has not been as willing to carefully
consider the role of self-disclosure in spiritual direction as developed in
religious circles. Again, for hundreds of years, the religious tradition
provided for both an individual's psychological and spiritual needs. A
careful look at the accumulated wisdom of the centuries could prove
helpful to the practice of psychotherapy.
McNamara, for example, believes the Christian tradition is funda-
mentally oriented toward individuation, and provides an outline for the
process, which eventually leads to wholeness of both the individual and
society. Perhaps overstating the point, McNamara (1975) writes:
The early Christian diagnosis of man makes Freud's comparable attempt
seem like a very weak cup of tea indeed. The discoveries of analytical psy-
chology do little else than repeat, in modern phraseology, and with detailed
empirical evidence, the principle injunctions of the Christian way. (pp. 404-405)
It has only been recently that there has been a revival of interest in
the rich traditions of the religious and spiritual past. Perhaps, as the
ideas and sources become more readily available, further exploration of
the psychological principles developed over the centuries by the various
spiritual traditions will take place.
References
Acharya, E (1982). The guru: The spiritual father in the Hindu tradition. In J.R. Sommer-
feldt (Ed.), Abba: Guides to wholeness and holiness East and West. Kalamazoo, Mich:
Cistercian Publications.
Self-Disclosure in Religious Spiritual Direction 27
Barry, w.A., & Connolly, W.J. (1982). The practice of spiritual direction. New York: Seabury
Press.
Callahan, A. (1988). Traditions of spiritual guidance: Thomas Merton as spiritual guide.
The Way, 28, 164-175.
Connolly, W.J. (1975). Contemporary spiritual direction: Scope and principles, an intro-
ductoryessay. Studies in the Spirituality of Jesuits, 7, 95-124.
Doran, R.M. (1979). Jungian psychology and Christian spirituality: 1. Review for Religious,
38, 497-510.
Dyckman, K.M., & Carroll, L.P. (1981). Inviting the mystic, supporting the prophet. New York:
Paulist Press.
Edwards, T. (1980). Spiritual friend. New York: Paulist Press.
Fry, T. (Ed) (1981). RB 1980: The Rule of Saint Benedict. Collegeville, Minn: Liturgical Press.
Geromel, E. (1977). Depth psychotherapy and spiritual direction. Review for Religious, 36,
753-763.
Green, A. (1988). Rethinking theology: Language, experience, and reality. Reconstruction-
ist, 54, (Sept.), 8-13,30.
Kemp, H.Y. (1985). Psychotherapy as a religious process: A historical heritage. In E.M.
Stern (Ed.), Psychotherapy and the religiously committed patient. New York: Haworth
Press.
Leech, K. (1977). Soul friend: The practice of Christian spirituality. San Francisco: Harper &
Row.
Lonsdale, D. (1985). Bookshelf. The Way Supplement, 54, 94-103.
Lou£, A. (1982). Spiritual fatherhood in the literature of the desert. In J.R. Sommerfeldt
(Ed.), Abba: Guides to wholeness and holiness East and West. Kalamazoo, Mich: Cistercian
Publications.
May, G. (1982). Care of mind, care of spirit. San Francisco: Harper & Row.
McCarty, S. (1976). On entering spiritual direction. Review for Religious, 35, 854-857.
McDargh, J. (1984). The life of the self in Christian spirituality and contemporary psycho-
analysis. Horizons, 11, 344-60.
McNamara, W. (1975). Psychology and the Christian mystical tradition. In C. Tart, (Ed.),
Transpersonal psychologies. New York: Harper & Row.
Merton, T. (1968). The spiritual father in desert tradition. Cistercian Studies, 3, 3-23.
Merton, T. (1960). Spiritual direction and meditation. Collegeville, Minn: Liturgical Press.
Nemeck, EK., & Coombs, M.T. (1985). The way of spiritual direction. Wilmington, Del:
Michael Glazier.
Schneiders, S.M. (1984). Horizons on spiritual direction. Horizons, 11, 100-112.
Sullivan, EP. (1983). Lyric psalms: Half a psalter. Washington DC: Pastoral Press.
Walsh, w.J. (1976). Reality therapy and spiritual direction. Review for Religious, 35, 372-385.
Ward, B. (1975). The sayings of the desert fathers. Kalamazoo, Mich: Cistercian Publications.
Ward, B. (1984). Spiritual direction in the desert fathers. The Way, 24, 61-69.
Ware, K. (1974). The spiritual father in Orthodox Christianity. Cross Currents, 24, 296--320.
Wilson, W. (1982). Spiritual sonship: A report on experience. In J.R. Sommerfeldt (Ed.),
Abba: Guides to wholeness and holiness East and West. Kalamazoo, Mich: Cistercian
Publications.
Yalom,1. (1975). The theory and practice of group psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books.
II
Theoretical Perspectives
3
Self-Disclosure and
Classical Psychoanalysis
Robert C. Lane and James W. Hull
31
32 Robert C. Lane and James W. Hull
sex of the patient, and the stage of therapy? Other more technical ques-
tions, such as those regarding the nature, intensity, and timing of self-
revelation, also have been raised.
Currently there exits a strong polarization among therapists of dif-
ferent schools regarding these issues. Dissatisfaction with classical psy-
choanalysis has included the criticism that its practitioners, most of
whom eschew self-revelation, can be aloof, nonresponsive, and seem-
ingly unavailable. The various therapies of the human potential move-
ment (Murphy, 1958) have been one outgrowth of such criticism. Many
of these approaches advocate "therapist's transparency," a willingness
to share immediate impressions as well as past experience with the
patient. This is said to make the therapist more humane, to bind thera-
pist and patient together in an exchange of intimacies. The therapist
provides a model for personal growth, facilitating spontaneous, genu-
ine, and even "creative" being (Culbert, 1961; Trilling, 1975). Examples of
such approaches include humanistic/existential therapy, client-centered
therapy, sensitivity training and the encounter movement, experiential
therapy, gestalt therapy, bioenergetics, and some varieties of cognitive-
affective therapy.
Family therapists also have argued for the usefulness of self-
disclosure. Whitaker related success in family therapy mainly to the
therapist's self-involvement in the process, including the sharing of
unique personal reactions, confusion, and uncertainties (Whitaker &
Malone, 1953; Napier & Whitaker, 1978; Whitaker & Keith, 1981). He
argued that self-disclosure might be the only strategy that could rescue
a family treatment from impasse, but cautioned that it could produce
harmful effects for therapist and family alike. Others who have made
similar points include Garfield (1987) and Watzlawick (1978).
While these voices have echoed across the current professional
scene, many psychoanalysts have continued to advocate the neutrality
and anonymity of the "blank screen," feeling that self-revelation may
contaminate the transference and interfere with its resolution. Expres-
sions of the analyst's personal opinions, attitudes, and feelings still are
seen by many as a sign of countertransference difficulties. Texts on
psychoanalytic technique commonly espouse this position (e.g., Feni-
chel, 1945; Fine, 1982; Glover, 1955; Greenson, 1967; Langs, 1982; Men-
ninger, 1958; Strean, 1982). The psychoanalytic community has not been
unanimous in its rejection of self-revelation, however, and in recent
years some analysts have begun to explore how judicious self-disclosure
may further the work of analysis. This has led to a rediscovery of the
early writings of Ferenczi.
In the present chapter we discuss the problem of therapist self-
disclosure from the perspective of classical psychoanalysis, tracing the
Self-Disclosure and Classical Psychoanalysis 33
historical debate and current controversy around the benefits and costs
of such intervention. We will not take up the question of patient self-
disclosure, and also will limit ourselves to comments about indivi-
dual psychotherapy. Many new approaches advocating therapist self-
revelation have evolved from the group therapy field and in this sense
our comments may not be directly applicable to those modalities where
therapist self-disclosure is most common. Nevertheless, we feel that
psychoanalytic thinking about self-disclosure in the analytic relation-
ship highlights central issues and problems that can be applied usefully
to other therapeutic modalities.
doctor that is separate from the work of analysis. Gay (1988) provides
many examples of Freud's self-revelations and "non-neutral" behavior
with patients. For example, his analysis of Eitington, which was one of
the first training analyses, was conducted during leisurely strolls
around Vienna. At times Freud gave gifts to patients, and he was known
to remit fees when a patient fell on hard times. His decision to analyze
his daughter Anna directly contradicted earlier technical recommenda-
tions regarding neutrality and the need to function as a blank screen. He
assisted Max Graf in the analysis of his own young son (Little Hans),
and later when Jung began to analyze his wife Freud encouraged him,
thinking that he might meet with some success. The Rat Man was pro-
vided with a meal when he arrived for a session hungry, and other
solicitous behavior was directed toward Wortis (1954), Doolittle (1956),
Blanton (1971), and Kardiner (1977).
Freud's self-disclosure with patients finds its complement in his
writings for other professionals. In The Interpretation of Dreams (1900) he
reported and analyzed some of his own most revealing dreams, often
reporting highly embarrassing childhood memories which were uncov-
ered by his associates. For example, the discussion of the dream of
Count Thun includes memories of an incident when he was seven or
eight and urinated on the floor of his parents' bedroom, prompting his
father to predict he never would amount to anything. Gay (1988) also
describes how self-revelation and "wild analysis" were a regular part of
the culture and collegial interaction of the early analytic community.
While on the ship to America, Freud and Jones passed the time by
analyzing each other's dreams. Offhand interpretations of colleagues'
personalities, dreams, and slips of the tongue were common, with diag-
nosis of paranoia and homosexuality freely given. "They all practiced in
their circle the kind of wild analysis they decried in outsiders as tactless,
unscientific and counterproductive .... Freud played this game with the
rest" (Gay, 1988, p. 235).
Freud's abandonment of the seduction theory led him to emphasize
intrapsychic conflicts and minimize the role of the actual environment in
the causation of neurosis. By contrast, Ferenczi placed increasing em-
phasis on the role of real trauma in pathogenesis (Lum, 1988a,b). In his
clinical diary he maintained that psychoanalysis placed too great an
emphasis on the role of fantasy, and whenever analysis went deeply
enough a traumatic-hysterical basis of neurosis could be uncovered. He
drew a parallel between the child traumatized by the hypocrisy of
adults, the mental patient traumatized by the hypocrisy of society, and
the analysand whose trauma is revived by the technical rigidity of the
analyst. The analyst's hypocrisy consists of the denial of counter-
transference feelings, and it is this that reactivates the earliest traumas
of the patient.
Self-Disclosure and Classical Psychoanalysis 35
patients, a woman who had been in analysis for two years and whose
treatment was stalemated. Ferenczi first found this patient disagreeable
and in overcompensation had yielded to all her wishes. She came to
believe that he was in love with her and that she had found the ideal
lover. Frightened by this, he retreated, interpreting the negative emo-
tions she shouldbe feeling toward him. She responded with the same
interpretations directed toward him, which he felt were not unjustified.
He decided to express his feelings freely to her, and noted that she began
to make progress again in her sessions. Carrying the experiment fur-
ther, he scheduled double sessions or alternating sessions, one for her
and one for him.
Gradually Ferenczi became aware of the problems of mutual anal-
ysis, including the patient's wish to deflect attention from herself, the
impossibility of letting oneself be analyzed by every patient, the need to
respect patients' sensibilities, and the problem posed by the discretion
owed to other patients. He specified the limitations of mutual analysis:
it should be practiced strictly according to the needs of the patient, and
no further than necessary for the patient. Eventually he considered
letting himself be analyzed only after the patient's analysis was com-
pleted. When his patient began to develop delusional ideas about their
collaboration, suggesting they prolong it indefinitely and that without it
Ferenczi would lose his therapeutic skill, he terminated the experiment.
In reflecting on this experience he concluded that mutual analysis was
only a technique "of last resort," when the training analysis had been
incomplete.
Lum (1988a,b) documents Freud's strong disapproval of Ferenczi's
technical innovations. Freud felt that Ferenczi was obsessed, first with
the need to cure by extreme deprivation (active technique) and later with
the need to cure by love (relaxation technique). He chastised Ferenczi
particularly about rumors that he permitted kissing. The analytic com-
munityat the time reacted with amazement and extreme criticism. More
recent evaluations have been less condemning. Rachman (1988) points
out that in addition to challenging the classical position that the analyst
should be a "mirror," Ferenczi was the first dissident analyst to offer an
alternative to the Oedipus complex. Ferenczi was one of the first psycho-
analysts to call attention to the crucial importance of the earliest mother-
child relationship. In identifying early affect deprivation as one cause of
infant death, he predated Spitz' (1946) work on marasmus. Finally, in his
emphasis on the analyst as a human partner in the therapeutic ex-
change, with a focus on transference-countertransference dilemmas, he
paved the way for later interpersonal and self psychological pioneers
such as Sullivan, Fromm, and Kohut.
Self-Disclosure and Classical Psychoanalysis 37
Special Events
Some events during psychotherapy by their very nature lead to
therapist self-disclosure. Examples are serious illness in the analyst,
pregnancy and birth, and marriage. Most of the literature to date has
dealt with illnesses in the analyst. Rosner (1986) identifies this as one
situation where self-disclosure may be necessary for the patient's emo-
tional health. To regard serious illness as no different than separations
due to vacation or minor illness represents denial on the part of the
analyst. Not revealing information gives the patient's fantasies free rein,
there is no opportunity to deal with distortions, and the failure to reveal
information, especially when it can provide relief, and runs the risk of
introducing real issues of exclusion, abandonment, and rejection. On
the other hand, divulging information prevents the emergence of trans-
ference distortions and may greatly complicate the patient's working
through of hostility toward the analyst. Crucial technical questions in-
clude: Who should inform the patient that sessions are to be cancelled?
How much factual information should be provided? Should different
patients be provided with different amounts of information, based on
their personality and the stage of their analysis?
With regard to the patient's reactions to the analyst's illness, Dew-
ald (1982) identified the problem as "the need adequately to explore the
full gamut of the patient's responses, affects and associations to the
illness, and to do this in the face of countertransference temptations
either defensively to promote premature closure and evasion of more
threatening affects, or to use the experience for exhibitionistic, mas-
ochistic, narcissistic or other neurotic satisfaction" (p. 361). He argued
that too much information may inhibit the patient's fantasies and reac-
tions, thereby interfering with the analysis, while too little information
may overburden the patient's adaptive capacity. He attempted to vary
his approach to the needs of each patient and the phase of therapy: some
factual material was offered to those in the beginning phase so that they
would not drop out, while the least factual information was provided to
those in the middle phase. He felt that patients in the terminal phase
"didn't need information."
Abend (1982) maintained that how much factual information is of-
fered should depend on how well the patient can maintain analytic
productivity "in the absence of factual information." He feels that most
analysts who have written on this topic have arrived at a solution close
to Dewald's, but cautions analysts to be aware of their own unconscious
needs that may be served by the transmission of factual information
about illness, even when it seems technically correct to provide such
information and the subsequent analytic work appears unimpeded or
even enhanced by the disclosure.
42 Robert C. Lane and James W. Hull
Discussion
Psychoanalysts have concerned themselves with the problem of
self-revelation since the earliest days of Freud. Self-disclosure was a
regular part of Freud's demeanor in the consulting room, and in their
Self-Disclosure and Classical Psychoanalysis 43
References
Abend, S.M. (1982). Serious illness in the analyst: Countertransference considerations.
Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 30, 365-379.
Benedek, E. (1973). Fourth world of the pregnant therapist. Journal American Medical Wom-
en's Association, 28, 365.
Blanton, S. (1971). Diary of my analysis with Sigmund Freud. New York: Hawthorne Books.
Breger, L. (1984). Discussion of D. Ehrenberg, Psychoanalytic engagement II: Affective
considerations. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 20, 583-589.
Culbert, S.A. (1961). The therapeutic process of self-disclosure. In J. T. Hunt & T.M.
Tomlinson (Eds.), New directions in client-centered therapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Dewald, P.A. (1982). Serious illness in the analyst: Transference, countertransference, and
reality responses. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 30, 347-363.
Doolittle, H. (1956). Tribute to Freud with unpublished letters to the author. New York: Pan-
theon Press.
Dupont, J. (Ed.). (1988). The clinical diary of Sandor Ferenczi. M. Balint & N. Jackson, trans.
Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Ehrenberg, D. (1984). Psychoanalytic engagement II: Affective considerations. Contempor-
ary Psychoanalysis, 20, 560-583.
Fenichel, 0. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis. New York: W W Norton.
Ferenczi, S. (1933). Confusion of tongues between adults and the child. International Jour-
nal of Psychoanalysis, 30, 225-230.
Fine, R. (1982). The healing of the mind. New York: The Free Press.
Freud, A. (1954). The widening scope of indications for psychoanalysis: Discussion. Jour-
nal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 2, 607-620.
Freud, S. (1900). The interpretation of dreams. Standard edition, Vol. 4. London: Hawthorne
Press.
Freud, S. (1912). Recommendations to physicians practicing psychoanalysis. Standard edi-
tion, Vol. 12. London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1919). Lines of advance in psychoanalytic therapy. Standard edition, Vol. 17.
London: Hogarth Press.
Garfield, R. (1987). On self-disclosure: The vulnerable therapist. Contemporary Family
Therapy, 9, 58-78.
Gay, P. (1988). Freud: A life for our time. New York: WW Norton.
Gitelson, M. (1952). The emotional positions of the analyst in the psychoanalytic situation.
International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 33, 1.
Glover, E. (1955). The technique of psychoanalysis. New York: International Universities
Press.
Greene, M. (1982). Discussion of H.S. Strean, Extra-analytiC contacts: Theoretical and
clinical considerations. In H.S. Strean (Ed.), Controversy in psychotherapy. Metuchen,
NJ: Scarecrow Press.
Self-Disclosure and Classical Psychoanalysis 45
Greenson, R. (1967). The technique and practice of psychoanalysis. New York: International
Universities Press.
Jourard, S. (1971). Self-disclosure: An experimental analysis of the transparent self. New York:
Wiley-Interscience.
Kardiner, A. (1977). My analysis with Freud. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Katz, J.B. (1978). A psychoanalyst's anonymity: Fiddler behind the couch. Bulletin of the
Menninger Clinic, 42, 520-524.
Khan, M. (1963). The concept of cumulative trauma. In The privacy of the self. London:
Hogarth Press, 1974.
Khan, M. (1986). Outrageousness, compliance and authenticity. Contemporary Psycho-
analysis, 22, 629-652.
Langs, R. (1976). The therapeutic relationship and deviations in technique. International
Journal of Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 4, 106-141.
Langs, R. (1982). Psychotherapy: A Basic Text. New York: Jason Aronson.
Lax, R.E (1969). Some considerations about transference and counter-transference mani-
festations evoked by the analyst's pregnancy. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 50,
363.
Ludmer, R.1. (1988). Creativity and the self. (Discussion of S. Ferenczi, Confusion of tongues
between adults and the child.) Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 24, 234-239.
Lum, W.B. (1988a). Sandor Ferenczi (1873-1933)- The father of the empathic-interpersonal
approach. Part I: Introduction and early analytic years. Journal of the American Academy
of Psychoanalysis, 16, 131-153.
Lum, W. B. (1988b). Sandor Ferenczi (1873-1933) - The father of the empathic-interpersonal
approach. Part II: Evolving technique, final contributions and legacy. Journal American
Academy of Psychoanalysis, 16, 317-347.
Menninger, K. (1958). Theory of psychoanalytic technique. New York: Basic Books.
Murphy, G. (1958). Human potentialities. New York: Basic Books.
Napier, A. Y., & Whitaker, C. (1978). The family crucible. New York: Harper & Row.
Paluszny, M., & Poznanski, E. (1971). Reactions of patients during pregnancy of the psy-
chotherapist. Child Psychiatry Human Development, 1, 266.
Rachman, A. W. (1988). The rule of empathy: Sandor Ferenczi's pioneering contributions to
the empathic method in psychoanalysis. Journal of the American Academy of Psycho-
analysis, 16, 1-27.
Rosner, S. (1986). The seriously ill or dying analyst and the limits of neutrality. Psychoanaly-
tic Psychology, 3, 357-371.
Spence, D. (1984). Discussion of D.B. Ehrenberg, Psychoanalytic engagement, II: Affec-
tive considerations. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 20, 589-595.
Spitz, R.A., & Wolf, K.M. (1946). Anaclitic depression: An inquiry into the genesis of
psychiatric conditions in early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 2, 313-342.
Strean, H.S. (1981). Extra-analytic contacts: Theoretical and clinical considerations. Psycho-
analytic Quarterly, 50, 238-259.
Strean, H.S. (1982). Controversy in psychotherapy. Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow Press.
Tarnower, W. (1966). Extra-analytic contacts between the psychoanalyst and the patient.
Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 35, 399-413.
Thompson, C. (1964). Sandor Ferenczi, 1873-1933. In M. Green (Ed.), The collected papers of
Clara Thompson. New York: Basic Books.
Trilling, 1. (1975). Sincerity and authenticity. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Watzlawick, P. (1978). The language of change. New York: Basic Books.
Weiner, M.E (1969). In defense of the therapist. Psychosomatics, 10, 156.
Weiner, M.E (1972). Self-exposure by the therapist as a therapeutic technique. American
Journal of Psychotherapy, 26, 42.
Weiss, S.S. (1975). The effect on the transference of "special events" occurring during
psychoanalYSis. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 56, 69-75.
46 Robert C. Lane and James W. Hull
Whitaker, c., & Keith, D. (1981). Symbolic-experiential family therapy. In A. Gurman & D.
Kniskern (Eds.), Handbook of family therapy. New York: Brunner/Maze!'
Whitaker, c., & Malone, T. (1953). The roots of psychotherapy. New York: Blakiston.
Wortis, J. (1954). Fragments of an analysis with Freud. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Zaslow, S. (1988). Comments on "Confusion of Tongues." [Review of S. Ferenczi, Confusion
of tongues between adults and the child.) Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 24, 211-225.
4
Show and Tell
REFLECTIONS ON THE ANALYST'S SELF-DISCLOSURE
Sabert Basescu
The classical analytic stance has been that of the blank screen, with the
analyst maintaining anonymity. This article discusses the conceptual
bases for this position and how these conceptual issues are undergoing
modification. These changes impact upon the role of analytic anonymity
and consequently upon the nature of the analyst'S self-disclosure. The
article focuses on the writer's personal experience with self-disclosure in
the context of therapeutic work.
Sabert Basescu • Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York
10003, and the Westchester Center for the Study of Psychoanalysis and Psychotherapy, 516
Hommocks Road, Larchmont, New York 10538. Versions of this paper were presented at
The Manhattan Institute for Psychoanalysis on November 20, 1987, and at the NYU
Postdoctoral Program for Psychoanalysis on February 5,1988.
47
48 Sabert Basescu
relationship between the two. He goes on " ... the analyst who main-
tains a posture of aloofness-that is, the analyst who has confused the
behavior of anonymity with the goal of neutrality-offers the patient no
context within which to appreciate the nature of his transference" (p.
85).
But there is even a further question as to the desirability of the
neutral stance itself-especially when it doesn't reflect the true state of
the analyst's feelings-and how often is it that we analysts feel truly
neutral? Wachtel (1987) suggests, "For some patients, the stance of
neutrality can contribute to their tendency to invalidate their own per-
ceptions and even to doubt their own sanity" (p. 66). The increasing
recognition and acknowledgment that the analytic relationship is a fully
human encounter between two, more alike than different, fallible hu-
man beings, imply that neutrality may be primarily a technical fiction,
more honored in the breach, rather than an analytic attribute.
However, what indisputably is an analytic attribute is the striving
to analyze and understand deviations from neutrality, which mayor
may not be countertransferential, and to deal with them openly in the
context of the therapeutic relationship. Ehrenberg (1982) observes,
"What defines an analytic relationship is that our impact, whether the
result of deliberate interventions or the result of inadvertent aspects of
our participation, must be explicitly clarified ... The hallmark of an ana-
lytic relationship is that it is essential that there be no covert manipula-
tion and that the patient be cognizant of whatever critical transactions
have occurred, as well as their impact" (p. 540). It is by virtue of this
openness that the essential attribute of psychoanalytic neutrality is
maintained, namely, that control of the patient's life remains in the pa-
tient's hands, not in the analyst's.
Anonymity of the analyst is considered essential to another central
dynamic of traditional psychoanalysis, the formation of the trans-
ference neurosis. The analyst scrupulously refrains from any self-
disclosure in order to avoid providing substitute gratifications that the
pqtient may get from the transference. This condition of abstinence fos-
ters the process of regression that lies at the core of the transference
neurosis. It can certainly result in an intensification of affect, but a
question has been raised about the nature of the regression. The theory
holds that the regression is temporal-to an earlier time in the person's
history; but critics claim that the regression is structural-to a lower
level of personality integration. Structural regression may be quite dan-
gerous-particularly to people whose personality integration is shaky
to start with. Aside from the danger, however, there is much uncertainty
about the relationship between transference neurosis and change. It
may well be that people get better in spite of a transference neurosis,
rather than because of one.
Show and Tell: Reflections on the Analyst's Self-Disclosure 51
time, I was sorely tempted to disclaim any responsibility for the paint-
ing that I didn't like either.) Somebody was pleased that I didn't wear a
tie. Somebody else assumed I was going to a bar mitzvah when I did
wear one. (It was actually a funeral.) My books have been criticized. My
plants have been taken to mean that I'm good at making people grow.
My cough meant I was getting a cold. My eyes showed I was tired. My
car proved I didn't know much about cars, and the loud voice on the
other end of the phone indicated I was a henpecked husband. Not all of
such conclusions are accurate, but some are, and some are more accurate
than I initially gave them credit for being.
There are further, perhaps more profound, ways in which analysts
reveal themselves inadvertently. The questions asked and not asked, the
content focused upon, the connections made, the fleeting and not-so-
fleeting emotions invariably displayed, the facts remembered and the
facts forgotten-all convey information about the analyst's interests,
values, theories, anxieties, and emotions. Singer (1968) claims that an-
alysts are, at times, reluctant to make correct interpretations. "The more
to the point and the more penetrating the interpretation, the more ob-
vious it will be that the therapist is talking and understanding from the
depth of his own psychological life .... It takes one to know one, and in
his correct interpretation the therapist reveals that he is one" (p. 369).
Patients come to know their analysts through the shared experience
of the therapeutic relationship. The quality of that subjective knowing is
not simply dependent upon knowing about objective facts of the an-
alyst's personal life. Singer maintains that patients' readiness to know
their analysts-that is, to make use of the experience that is always
available to them-is a measure of their psychological health (1977).
So much for showing, which takes place whether we wish it or not.
Now about telling, which is deliberate, intentional, and chosen, al-
though it may also be experienced as driven, compelled, or manipu-
lated. What analysts tell their patients about themselves runs the full
range of personal facts, opinions, feelings, reactions, associations,
memories, experiences, fantasies, and dreams. Very few analysts tell all,
but some "wild" analysts do. It's a safe bet, however, that all analysts tell
something. The motivations, circumstances, and rationales vary tre-
mendously, as do the therapeutic consequences.
For example, Masud Kahn (1986) informed his patients of his
brother's death. He writes, " ... Analysts rarely speak about events in
their personal life that affect their work mutatively. The death of my
brother had changed my whole outlook on life, and I knew my patients
would sense it; so I told them as much. It is not a question of trans-
ference or countertransference, but actually real, lived life that makes
our fatedness or destiny, and about which we are often somewhat de-
Show and Tell: Reflections on the Analyst's Self-Disclosure 53
vious, both with ourselves and others" (p. 644). Similarly, Singer (1971)
reports that when his wife became seriously ill, requiring him fre-
quently to cancel appointments, he told his patients the reason.
One analytic candidate I was supervising learned suddenly that she
had a cancerous growth and had to have a mastectomy. In discussing
how best to inform her patients, she decided she would tell them in
general terms that she would be out for some weeks for surgery but she
did not feel comfortable being more specific than that. However, when
she returned to work after the surgery and was confronted by her pa-
tients' fantasies-especially the fantasies that were correct or near cor-
rect-she was unable to contain her own distress and burst into tears.
At that point she told them the fuller story.
Some years ago I had a back condition that left me unable to sit,
although I could stand or lie down comfortably enough. I chose to
continue working and informed the people I was seeing that I would
have to use the couch myself for about a month. I could discern no
disruptive impact on the work and have since talked with a number of
other analysts who have had similar experiences with similar results.
In none of these situations or a host of others like them have I heard
of destructive consequences for the analysis. One might anticipate that
the analyst's misfortune or infirmity would mobilize sadistic, vengeful,
or hostile impulses or, conversely, inhibit them. People do react in highly
individual ways; but a common theme seems to be the desire to be
helpful. The asymmetrical structure of the analytic relationship, with
the patient expected to be needy and the analyst helpful, can induce a
humiliating sense of uselessness in the patient, without the opportunity
for reality-oriented and constructive relatedness (see Singer, 1971). A
woman dreamed that when she came for her session, she found me on
the couch with a fever and she put a cool, damp washcloth on my fore-
head. We both understood the dream to mean that she wanted to be
helpful to me as I was to her.
One moral to be drawn from these experiences is that when the
analyst's life impacts upon his or her work, it is in the service of clarify-
ing the patient's attempts to make sense out of the relationship to acknow-
ledge the facts of life. However, there are dangers in such disclosures,
having less to do with the patient's ability to handle the facts and more to
do with the analyst's motives in disclosing them. If the analyst is exploit-
ing the patient by eliciting sympathy, warding off criticism or anger, or
manipulating a feeling of intimacy, then the disclosures are likely to be
destructively double-binding.
Other occasions on which I have found it useful to tell of events in
my life have to do with my feeling that some messages are better con-
veyed through recounting experiences than by saying what you mean.
54 Sabert Basescu
That is, in order to convey that I understand something that has been
told me, I might briefly tell of a similar experience that I've had. Or
when working with someone who feels so separate and different from
the rest of the human race, I might say something about myself that
bridges their feeling of distance.
One woman said, "I had a bad weekend. Other people are stable.
I'm so up and down. I hide my rockiness." I said, "Don't we all." She:
"You too?" I: "Does that surprise you?" She: "Well, I guess not. You're
human too." I understood that to mean she also felt human, at least for
the moment. On another occasion she expressed her anguish that some-
thing was so wrong with her because her therapy took so long. I said I
was in analysis for eight years, and that was only the first time. She said
she felt better knowing that I didn't feel she was taking too long and
wasn't fed up with her. I am aware that she may have meant something
was wrong with me because her therapy took so long, and that's an issue
we have also dealt with.
There are those inevitable times when the analyst'S personal issues
intrude upon the work and disrupt the patient's understanding of what's
happening. The most likely outcome is for the patient to assume the
blame and experience self-contempt. At such times it is essential that
the analyst acknowledge the intrusion. Ehrenberg (1984) puts it: "No
matter how entangled analysts find themselves they must be able to re-
establish the analytic integrity of the relationship. The process of so
doing actually becomes the medium of the analytic work, and may
involve making the analyst'S reactions explicit to engage the patient in a
collaborative way" (p. 563).
Prior to my taking a brief out-of-town trip, a man who knew where I
was going wished me a good time and began to tell me of interesting
things to do there. I somewhat brusquely replied that I'd been there
before. In the session following my return he said he was angry and had
been upset for hours after the previous session, feeling rejected by me,
as if something were wrong with him. Although these were characteris-
tic reactions of his, I suggested that there might be other possible expla-
nations. I went on to say that I did cut him off because I was not going to
have that kind of vacation and felt somewhat deprived about it. He said it
helped to know that and I said that he was always ready to see some-
thing wrong with himself. Sometimes there were things wrong with
others.
Michels (1983) asserts, "The primary data of psychoanalysis are
neither what happens in childhood nor what happens in adult life, and
not even the cause-effect relationship between them; the primary data
are what the patient says in the analyst'S presence, how the analyst
responds, and how the patient can make constructive use of the experi-
Show and Tell: Reflections on the Analyst's Self-Disclosure 55
ential and dialectical process" (p. 61). This points to what I think is the
predominant arena of analysts' self-disclosure, namely, what the an-
alyst says about his or her reactions to what transpires in the relation-
ship between the two people. It is predominant in importance, in
relevance to the therapeutic work, and in frequency of occurrence. It is
also probably the least controversial area of analysts' self-disclosure.
The patient-therapist relationship is the most immediate and expe-
rientially cogent arena in which to explore the patterns of interpersonal
relatedness and the role that each person plays in actively creating and
maintaining these patterns. What is unique about psychoanalysis, as
opposed to other therapies, is the focus on this relationship, the nature
and structure of which colors the way in which all other experiences are
dealt with in the analysis. That is, it acts like a filter system through
which other experiences are seen and processed. The exploration of this
filter system itself is the primary work of psychoanalysis.
The direct person-to-person encounter between patient and analyst
"creates a history together, experienced as relevant engagement, and
characterized by wholeness and complexity" (Held-Weiss, 1986, p. 3).
The truthfulness with which the participants can explore and acknowl-
edge what it is that's going on between them empowers the relationship
to be an agent for change. Lichtenberg (1986) describes it as " ... the
combination of the unique interaction of a relationship with tensions at
its junction and the joint search for meaning that constitute an analysis,
or rather that constitute the curative experience that a successful analy-
sis is" (p. 73).
The mutuality of relevant self-revelation works against the mysti-
fication of experience in the relationship and allows for the development
of intimacy and trust. In that context unattended-to or anxiety-laden
aspects of relatedness can be acknowledged and clarified, and resis-
tance overcome. Wolf (1983) suggests that, "Since in essence, resistance
is nothing but fear of being traumatically injured again, the decisive
event of its analysis is the moment when the analysand has gained
courage from these self-revelations of the analyst to know that the an-
alyst does not need to feed on the patient to achieve cohesion and har-
mony" (p. 500).
As I have written elsewhere (Basescu, 1987), I at times feel
prompted to express something I'm thinking or feeling in a session; but
I almost always respond to a patient's questioning me about it. If I am
asked, I generally answer. For example, a man, himself a therapist,
previously told me that he looks to see if I am glad to see him. This time,
as he scrutinizes me when I come to the waiting room, something
strikes me funny, and I can't stop myself from broadly smiling as he
enters the office. He asks me why I'm smiling and I tell him it's because
56 Sabert Basescu
he's scrutinizing me. He says, "That's no answer. I realize you don't have
to answer me." I say, "Your looking at me had a whimsical quality that
struck me funny." He says, "That's an answer. Thanks. That helps me
know something about myself." When I asked him what it helped him
know he told me that often people don't take him seriously and that
perhaps he doesn't present himself as serious.
A woman, who is familiar with psychotherapeutic literature but not
in the profession herself asked me if I would give her a reprint or refer-
ence for anything I've written. I told her I didn't care what she read but I
didn't want to give her what she asked for. She asked why not, saying
that she could get it on her own and this would simply save her some
trouble. That made sense to me but I still didn't feel right about doing it,
although I wasn't clear why. She pressed for a reason and I said, "Well,
let's see if I can be clear about it. It's because I don't want to participate in
or reinforce something that I don't know the meaning of. My job is to
analyze the meaning, not to collude with it." She said, "I understand
that," and then told me she wanted something of mine to have while I
was away on vacation. That then was what we dealt with.
I generally find that the process of answering questions about what
is going on with me at the moment helps clarify for both of us what's
happening. It often has the additional consequence of enabling further
exploration of the patient's experience. It conveys a respectful attitude
toward the other and enhances the spirit of collaboration.
Another class of personal reactions that I tend readily to express are
those that are discrepant with what seems to be going on. I've said
things like, "You're smiling while you tell me this, but I feel sad. I
wonder why," or, "I feel like I'm listening to a lecture," or "I have the
feeling of being buttered up." I think it is fairly common for analysts to
rely on their own reactive emotional sensibilities for clues to under-
standing the less obvious aspects of what is being enacted. Disclosing
these feeling reactions invites an experiential exploration of more than
meets the eye. It also conveys to the other person the kind of impact he or
she is having.
Vulnerable people tend to defend themselves against humiliation in
ways that often bring about the very hurts they are trying to avoid. The
therapeutic relationship affords a unique opportunity to experience
these defensive patterns and their consequences in a context that allows
for learning, not simply blind repetition. I saw a woman who was in
therapy with me, a therapist herself, at a professional meeting. From her
look and manner I gathered she would be more comfortable if I kept my
distance, which I did. In the session following she told me how hurt she
was that I was so unfriendly, as if I were letting her know that she should
make no mistake about the boundaries of our relationship. I told her how
Show and Tell: Reflections on the Analyst's Self-Disclosure 57
I had felt warned away by her appearance and thought I was complying
with her wishes. This led to our discussing other aspects of the same
pattern. I observed that she never expressed interest in any aspect of my
life, such as where I went on vacation or if I enjoyed it. She explained that
she was fearful of my seeing her as intrusive and slapping her down for
it. Her stance was that of rigid avoidance to forestall rejection. Her
impact was that of indifference.
In thinking about the way I say things to people in therapy sessions,
I realize that sometimes I know in advance what I'm going to say and
sometimes I don't. I may formulate something or mull it over momen-
tarily before I say it, or it may just come out. A woman said to me
critically that she never knew what to expect from me. I said, "That
makes two of us," and that just came out. Two weeks later she said my
remark stuck with her and she realized that unless you're rigid you don't
always know what you're going to say.
This bears upon the issue of the therapist's spontaneity. I think that
when I'm working effectively I am functioning spontaneously-and
that does not refer to whether or not I mull things over before I say them.
It refers to functioning freely in the mode appropriate to being the
analyst in a psychoanalytic relationship. One's way of being is influ-
enced by the nature of the relationship, as in a marriage or friendship, or
in a classroom or social gathering. The different structures elicit differ-
ent modes of being and different behaviors. The differences are a func-
tion of varying meanings, purposes, and intentions. The manifestations
of spontaneity vary as well.
While there clearly may be detrimental consequences to the an-
alyst's self-disclosures under the best of circumstances, I think most
problems are not caused by the analyst'S true spontaneity but by the lack
of it. That is, inappropriate self-disclosures are those compulsively
driven by the analyst's personal needs or they are responses to the
patient's intimidating manipulations. If the analyst is operating out of
countertransferential reactions, such as needing to impress the patient,
or being defensive, seductive, hostile, controlling, fearful, or placating,
then personal revelations are likely to be intrusive, diverting, burden-
some, inhibiting, or otherwise countertherapeutic. Saying whatever
comes to mind may be the mark of thoughtless impulsivity. Being fully
present, available, and freely responsive to the ongoing interpersonal
interactions are, to my mind, the hallmarks of spontaneity.
The fact that the distinctions are often difficult to make has led
many analysts to warn against self-disclosures. Gill (1983), for example,
is concerned that the analyst's subjective experience may be defensive
and that revealing it may result in shutting off further inquiry into the
patient's experience. He is wary about changing the patient's analysis
58 Sabert Basescu
References
Basescu, S. (1977). Anxieties of the analyst: An autobiographical account. In K.A. Frank
(Ed.), The human dimension in psychoanalytic practice. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Basescu, S. (1987). Behind the "seens": The inner experience of at least one psychoanalyst.
Psychoanalytic Psychology, 4, 3.
Coltart, N. (1986). 'Slouching toward Bethlehem ... ' or thinking the unthinkable in psy-
choanalysis. In G. Kohon, (Ed.), The British school of psychoanalysis, the independent
tradition. New Haven & London: Yale University Press.
Show and Tell: Reflections on the Analyst's Self-Disclosure 59
In this chapter I will first outline briefly the basic principles of rational-
emotive therapy; then consider how rational-emotive therapists view
client and therapist self-disclosure; and, finally, deal with common ob-
stacles to such self-disclosure and how these may be overcome.
61
62 Windy Dryden
events and that help people adjust to those events and then to move on
with their lives. Such emotions are distinguished from inappropriate
negative emotions which, according to RET theory, stem from dogmatic
irrational beliefs. Such emotions are anxiety, depression, anger, and
guilt-emotions that are seen as being unconstructive responses to neg-
ative life events and that inhibit people from adjusting to those events
and from getting on with their lives.
While helping the client to distinguish his inappropriate from his
appropriate negative emotions at C, the therapist will often help the
client to distinguish between irrational or rational beliefs at B. Assum-
ing that the client has a disturbed inappropriate negative emotion at C,
the RET therapist will proceed to encourage the client to disclose what
was the triggering activating event at A in the ABC framework. Here the
RET therapist will encourage her client to disclose a specific example of
the activating event; this is because the client, in real life, reacted to a
specific activating event and his irrational belief was in fact triggered by
such a specific event. The therapist will discourage her client from de-
scribing activating events in vague or overly discursive terms, since such
disclosures will not aid the identification of specific irrational beliefs at
B, later in the assessment process.
At this phase of assessment a particularly powerful intervention
tool is the use of "inference chaining" (Moore, 1983). In RET theory,
A stands not only for the event but also for the client's interpretations
or inferences about the event, and it often occurs that clients make
themselves disturbed not so much about the actual event but about the
inferences that they make about the event. In inference chaining the
therapist seeks to help the client to identify and disclose the most rele-
vant inference in the chain (Le., the one that served as a trigger for the
irrational belief).
A brief example of inference chaining follows:
Therapist: So at point C you felt anxious. Now what were you anx-
ious about? (Here the therapist encourages the client to disclose what it
was about A that he was disturbed about.)
Client: I was anxious because I thought that the woman might
reject me.
Therapist: And what would be anxiety-provoking in your mind if she
did reject you?
Client: All my friends would see this and they would laugh at me.
Therapist: And what would be anxiety-provoking about that in
your mind?
Client: Well, if my friends would laugh at me, I would gain the
reputation of being a wimp.
Therapist: And if people generally thought of you as a wimp ... ?
64 Windy Dryden
that there are neither good humans nor bad humans and that no human
being is more valuable or more worthy than any other. All people are
equal in humanity although they may be unequal in terms of their
different traits, behaviors, thoughts, feelings, etc. Thus the RET thera-
pist does not see herself as being more valuable than her client, although
she will tend to consider that she is more knowledgeable about under-
standing emotional problems.
Given that the effective RET therapist would accept herself for her
errors and flaws and for past and present emotional disturbances, she
will, as often as is therapeutically advisable, show her client how she
upset herself about experiences similar to those with which her client is
concerned and how she used RET to overcome such emotional distur-
bances. Note that in choosing this mode of self-disclosure, the RET
therapist is providing her client with a coping model of overcoming
emotional disturbance and not with a mastery model. The therapist who
employs a mastery model approach to self-disclosure will stress that she
has never experienced a problem similar to the client's because she
thinks rationally about similar events with which the client is strug-
gling. This approach is unproductive because it unduly emphasizes the
inequality between therapist and client and deemphasizes their shared
humanity.
A coping model of self-disclosure, on the other hand, where the
therapist outlines that she too has experienced or is currently experienc-
ing similar emotional disturbances but is able to get over these by using
RET methods and techniques, indicates to the client that both therapist
and client share the same experiences, although one is more adept at
present in overcoming them than the other. Such a coping model em-
phasizes the shared humanity of the therapist and client while not bela-
boring the inequality that exists between therapist and client in the
therapeutic enterprise. The therapist who utilizes the coping model of
self-disclosure can furthermore outline the process of solving emotional
problems for the client, and as such self-disclosure can often be a power-
ful therapeutic tool.
Let me use a personal example to illustrate this. I used to have a
very bad stammer and was not only ashamed about this but also anxious
about speaking in public because I was scared that if I revealed my
stammering in public, other people would laugh at me and I would
severely condemn myself if this occurred. I often disclose this fact to
clients who not only experience similar problems concerning stammer-
ing but who also have problems that are exacerbated by their anxiety
that these problems may be revealed publicly. My self-disclosure is often
along. these lines:
"You know, as we find out r.lOre about your problem I am myself
Self-Disclosure in RET 67
Cautions
therapists. For example, there are clients who idealize their therapists
and for whom disclosure of therapist fallibility may come as a very
painful blow, with the result that such clients may make themselves
(albeit needlessly) depressed and anxious about this. Other clients may
distort the content and the purpose of therapist self-disclosure to dis-
credit the therapist in his or her community. It is difficult for RET thera-
pists to predict which clients will have such negative responses to
therapist self-disclosure; but it is possible to gain such information, and
certain signs in the clients' psychopathology may provide clues for ther-
apists in this respect.
For example, if a client has a history of extreme anger and vengeful-
ness when their view of a person is threatened, this is perhaps a clue
that the therapist may not wish to disclose personal information to the
client that may threaten the client's view of the therapist. However, it
should be noted that unless there are signs to the contrary, RET thera-
pists do tend to use self-disclosure whenever therapeutically advisable
and will often take risks (although not foolhardy risks) in this regard.
Of course RET therapists can never guarantee that their self-disclosures
will have positive effects. Therapists who demand certainty that their
clients will react favorably to their disclosures will probably never self-
disclose. Given the fact that RET therapists are not afraid to take calcu-
lated risks in therapy, they will take the chance of gaining therapeutic
leverage by disclosing their problems and how they overcame them after
disputing any demands they may have about this being acceptable to
the client.
Now that I have outlined the value and risks of therapist self-dis-
closure in RET, I would like to emphasize how important it is for thera-
pists to elicit feedback from their clients concerning the impact of their
self-disclosures (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979). Thus at the end of a
session during which I have disclosed to a client some of my problems
and how I have overcome them, I generally ask the client what impact
this had on him, how he felt about my disclosure, what he learned from
my disclosure, and whether or not he preferred me to disclose this
information. If the therapist establishes her own system of feedback
with the client, then it is possible to gauge the likely future benefit of
therapist self-disclosure with that client. When a client indicates that
therapist self-disclosure is not helpful, then that is perhaps a good
guide for the therapist not to disclose her problems and the way she
overcame them in future to that client.
her clients' behavior. In addition to showing the client that he does not
equal his behavior, it is also important that the client be shown that,
although he is acting in a way that the therapist finds negative, he does
not have to continue to act that way, and that the therapist has faith and
confidence that he can improve his behavior. I have personally found the
combination of both disclosing negative reactions about certain aspects
of my clients' behavior in the context of teaching the RET position on (1)
the difference between the person and his acts, and (2) the possibility of
change and providing encouragement, to be especially therapeutic.
Clients
In my experience there are three main reasons clients do not dis-
close their experiences and problems to their therapists in RET: (1)
shame, (2) need for the therapist's approval, and (3) reluctance to partici-
pate in the therapeutic process.
When clients are ashamed about disclosing their experiences to
their therapists they tend to believe that they must not have these prob-
lems in the first place and that they are in some way unworthy individ-
uals for having them. Thus, rather than disclose both the nature of their
original problem to themselves and their therapist, they unwittingly
give themselves a second problem (Le., shame about the original prob-
lem) that accounts for their disclosure avoidance. If this is the case, it is
important for the RET therapist to deal with the client's shame about the
problem before encouraging him to disclose the nature of this problem.
If the client does admit to feeling shame, one way of doing this is to label
the client's problem "X" and help the client to see that "X" is really the
activating event (A in the ABC framework) and the feeling at C is shame.
The client can then be helped to identify the irrational beliefs at B that
underpin his experience of shame. If this assessment proves to be cor-
rect and the client acknowledges that this is the case, then the RET
therapist can deal with the irrational beliefs about the undisclosed prob-
lem without having to know what the problem is. Then, having been
72 Windy Dryden
helped to overcome his shame about the problem, the client may find it
easier to disclose the problem to his therapist.
The second reason a client avoids disclosure of his problems is that
he fears that if he discloses the problem to the therapist she would
somehow disapprove of him and he would condemn himself because he
thinks he needs the therapist's approval. First, it is important to ascer-
tain that this is the reason the client does not disclose the true nature of
his problems and, if so, this is again dealt with using the ABC frame-
work, with A = the prospect of losing the therapist's approval, B = "I
need the therapist's approval and I am less worthy if I don't have it," and
C = anxiety. Then the therapist can help the client to dispute and
change the irrational beliefs that underpin the anxiety that stops him
from disclosing the true nature of his problems. Once again, if this
proves successful, the client is likely to disclose his problems after he
has overcome his need for the therapist's approval.
The final reason for the client's unwillingness to disclose his prob-
lems to his therapist is a general reluctance to being a client in the first
place. This often occurs when the client is told that if he does not partici-
pate in the therapeutic process, he will incur a penalty, for example,
from the courts, or from his partner who may leave the relationship. My
approach to this situation is to show the client that there is no reason he
has to disclose his problems to me or to participate fully in the therapeu-
tic process and that he has every right not to do so. However, I also point
out to the client that since he is choosing to attend, even under duress,
he might as well work on a problem as defined by him rather than work
on a problem that has been defined for him by a third party. Once the
client sees that I am on his side and not on the side of the referring agent,
he is more likely to see me as an ally and begin to use the therapeutic
process for his own benefit rather than for the benefit of others.
Therapists
RET therapists are usually quite willing to disclose not only their
problems and how they have overcome them but also their personal
reactions to their clients; although, as I have suggested, such disclosure
needs to be made in the context of sound clinical decision making.
However, some RET therapists are reluctant to employ self-disclosure in
this way and, in my experience in supervising such therapists, there are
three main reasons that this occurs: (1) a need to be seen as thoroughly
competent by the client; (2) a need for the client's approval; and (3) the
belief that therapists should not have psychological problems.
When RET therapists believe that they have to be competent under
all conditions, they tend to avoid the appropriate use of therapist self-
Self-Disclosure in RET 73
disclosure because they predict that if they disclose that they have had
emotional problems, even though they have overcome them, their clients
will judge them as being incompetent; and if this was the case, they
would condemn themselves for this. While supervising therapists who
have this belief, I have used a double-barreled approach to this problem.
First, I encourage the therapist concerned to assume that her worst fear
is realized, i.e., that if she discloses herself to her clients they will indeed
see her as being incompetent as a therapist. Having encouraged her to
imagine that her worst fear has come true, I then help her to see that it is
her beliefs (B) about this situation (A) that leads to her reluctance to self-
disclosure at C. I then help her to identify and to challenge the irrational
belief that underpins this experience, namely: "I must be seen as a
competent therapist, otherwise I'm unworthy." When this has been
done, the therapist is in a better position to reassess logically the likeli-
hood that her clients will actually see her as incompetent. Here I use
both my own example of past self-disclosure to clients (as outlined
earlier in this chapter) and the fact that Albert Ellis (the founder of RET)
also employs self-disclosure to show that competent RET therapists do
self-disclose and that there seems to be little evidence that our clients
generally see us as less competent (although some may have done so in
the past).
The second reason RET therapists are reluctant to self-disclose is
their need for their clients' approval. This is related to the need to be
seen as competent; but here the therapist is more concerned with the
approval of her clients than with their judgments of her competence.
Again I suggest a double-barreled approach to this issue - helping the
therapist first to identify and challenge her irrational beliefs before help-
ing her to reevaluate the likelihood that her clients will disapprove of her
if she does disclose to them some of her personal experiences.
Finally-and this is perhaps more true of novice therapists than
more experienced ones-I find that some RET therapists have a dire
need to see themselves as thoroughly mentally healthy. This is, in fact, a
paradox, since if they demand that they must have perfect mental health,
they are in fact disturbed because of the very existence of such a de-
mand. This need to be problem-free stems partly from a misunder-
standing of what it means to be a therapist. Being a therapist does not
mean that one must be free of all psychological problems; rather that one
has such problems, but can use RET techniques to overcome them. The
approach to helping a therapist overcome this rigidity is quite similar to
what has already been outlined in this chapter. I first encourage her to
assume that her unrealistic expectation is true, namely that good thera-
pists do not have any emotional problems. Then I help her to see that
there is no reason why she must be a good therapist by this criterion
74 Windy Dryden
and, if she is not, she can still accept herself and continue to overcome
her problems. I then help her to reevaluate her unrealistic expectation
and encourage her to see that being a good therapist does not mean
being problem-free but means, in part, that one can apply what one is
teaching others to oneself and one's own life situation. In using RET to
help RET therapists in these ways, it is my experience that they become
less anxious about disclosing themselves to their clients and do so ap-
propriately and therapeutically during the process of RET.
References
Beck, A. T., Rush, A.J., Shaw, B.E, & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New
York: Guilford.
Bordin, E.S. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working
alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 16, 252-260.
Dies, RR (1973). Group therapist self-disclosure: An evaluation by clients. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 20, 344-348.
Dryden, W. (1987). Counselling individuals: The rational-emotive approach. London: Taylor &
Francis.
Ellis, A. (1972). Psychotherapy and the value of a human being. In W. Davis (Ed.), Value and
valuation: Aetiological studies in honor of Robert A. Hartman. Knoxville: University of
Tennessee Press.
Ellis, A., & Dryden, W. (1987). The practice of rational-emotive therapy. New York: Springer.
Moore, RH. (1983). Inference as "PC in RET. British Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 1 (2),
17-23.
6
Self-Disclosure in Psychotherapy and
the Psychology of the Self
Lawrence Josephs
75
76 Lawrence Josephs
right. Yet these senses of self do not function with complete autonomy
but are dependent upon the mother's facilitating responsiveness: Her
enthusiastic yet non-domineering encouragement bolsters self-agency;
her consistency and reliability supports self-coherence; her affective at-
tunement and responsiveness evokes self-affectivity; and her continuity
over time supports self-history. The relationship with mother becomes
internalized in the form of what Stern (1985) calls "Representations of
Interactions that have been Generalized (RIGS)" (p. 97). When a RIG is
activated in the absence of the mother, the infant encounters an "evoked
companion" (p. 111). The analog in psychotherapy is that, over time, the
patient develops representations of experiences with the therapist as a
self-regulating other who in absence is reconstituted as an evoked com-
panion. Serving as a growth-promoting self-regulating other requires
more than a correct technique of management: a highly personal empa-
thic immersion in the experience of the other. Such an empathic immer-
sion invariably expresses and reveals some private essence of the self of
the self-regulatory other. Bollas (1987) suggested that each mother pos-
sesses her own particular idiom of mothering, an aesthetic of being that
becomes a feature of the infant's self so that "we learn the grammar of
our being before we grasp the rules of our language" (p. 36). Analo-
gously, the therapist's self-regulatory function is mediated through his
unique somato-affective presence; an individualistic sensibility that in-
tuitively guides his aesthetic of handling and being with the patient.
Thus the personal idiom of parent/therapist handling becomes trans-
lated into the internalized self-care system of the child/patient.
Between the seventh and ninth month of life, the infant develops a
subjective sense of self in which it is realized that "inner subjective
experiences, the 'subject matter' of the mind, are potentially shareable
with someone else" (Stern, 1985, p. 24). The infant at this point can sense
that others distinct from oneself possess a mind of their own in which
they are capable of sharing a similar experience with the infant. This is
the level of true intersubjectivity in which physical intimacy in terms of
a sensitive somato-affective presence give rise to psychic intimacy in
which issues of openness to disclosure versus privacy and inscrutability
become relevant. At this level the infant is not only eager to share its
own subjective experience but can begin to become curious as to the
nature of the private subjective experience of another. Intersubjectivity
at this stage is still predominantly preverbal-communication transpir-
ing through the medium of empathic affect attunement. The implication
for psychotherapy is that self-disclosure is very much mediated through
the quality and intensity of spontaneous nonverbal affective responsive-
ness. Since spontaneous affective responsiveness is to a significant de-
gree modulated by volitional control and therefore subject to a theory of
80 Lawrence Josephs
the problem as residing solely within Susan rather than in the relation-
ship that developed as a process of mutual influence. It was to this
implied aggrandizement of myself at Susan's expense that she took
offense.
This vignette illustrates the double-edged nature of therapist verbal
self-disclosure. If we assume that therapists rarely consciously dissimu-
late in their verbal self-disclosure, it can also be surmised that they
rarely reveal more than a partial truth, and that what is left unsaid may
be as important as what is explicitly stated. Regardless of what the
therapist does or doesn't say about himself, the patient in registering the
therapist's somato-affective presence is developing some representation
of the therapist's experiential self to which the patient will compare any
verbal self-disclosure the therapist makes. When Susan asked me if I
was bored, it would have been transparently non-genuine if I denied all
negative affect, as though treatment was proceeding in some interesting
manner (i.e., a normopathic evasion). To have said "yes, I'm bored"
would have been wounding, since, after all, Susan was fishing for some
reassurance that she wasn't a totally superficial and boring person. In
responding, "I feel frustrated," I was attempting to acknowledge that
aspect of treatment which was an arduous task for both parties involved,
yet imply that however difficult a challenge, it was not yet a hopeless
situation. Nevertheless, the patient as interpreter of the therapist's expe-
rience (Hoffman, 1983) surmised that I was not only frustrated but bored
and blaming as well.
It would seem that the patient may be the best spokesperson for the
therapist's experiential self. After all, who can better speak for the
unique idiom of the therapist's way of being as conveyed in his somato-
affective presence than the patient, who has been the subject of the
therapist's best efforts to establish empathic rapport? Of course, the
patient is not always so articulate in describing his experience of the
therapist as a person, colored as it is by his or her own fears of psychic
intimacy. Much of the patient's response to the therapist's impact as a
person is expressed nonverbally, outside of the patient's awareness.
Thus, an important aspect of therapy is helping the patient become
aware of and articulate unconscious perceptions of the therapist. In recent
years this focus has been vigorously advocated by Langs (1976) and Gill
(1982). Curiously, although both analysts acknowledge the importance
of recognizing the plausibility of the patient's unconscious perception of
the therapist, rather than view it as primarily a product of transference
distortion, their attitude towards therapist verbal self-disclosure as a
means of validating the patient's perception is rather conservative.
A danger of therapist verbal self-disclosure is that it may foreclose a
thorough exploration of the patient's own perceptions of the therapist.
Self-Disclosure in Psychotherapy 83
When Susan asked if the therapist was bored, the therapist could have
inquired as to what about the therapist gave her the impression that he
might have been bored with her and what it would mean to her if the
therapist was indeed bored with her. Such a response could have helped
the patient get in better touch with her own intuition and develop
greater trust in her own perceptions. Foreclosing such exploration
through verbal self-disclosure could readily be taken by the patient to
imply that the therapist's verbal self-disclosure is to be honored as re-
ceived wisdom that must be accepted at face value. In so doing, the
therapist assumes an authority to define the objective nature of the
interpersonal reality that implicitly devalues the patient's perception of
the therapist in favor of the therapist's own self-perception. In the cur-
rent example, whether I said I was frustrated, bored, challenged, stimu-
lated, and so on would not alter one bit the validity of Susan's perception
of me as bored and blaming, no matter how discrepant with my own
self-perception.
Despite the potential pitfalls of therapist verbal self-disclosure,
there are dangers in failing to verbally disclose the self. For example,
Susan was quite sensitive to the distinction between the phrasings-
"You feel I'm criticizing you" and "When I criticized you, you felt at-
tacked." Although both statements convey a measure of empathy for her
feeling criticized, the first statement could readily be construed as im-
plying "You only imagine I'm criticizing you but in reality I'm not,"
whereas the second statement implies consensual validation, an ac-
knowledgment that the therapist can entertain the view that he had
indeed been critical, albeit unconsciously. For the therapist simply to
explore the patient's perceptions of the therapist, while withholding
comment as to whether those perceptions are congruent or incongruent
with the therapist's own self-perceptions, is to treat the patient as
though the patient were living in a world of illusion rather then engaged
in a real relationship with a flesh-and-blood person who communicates
his feelings and attitudes in innumerable nonverbal ways. For Susan, it
was not enough for the therapist to empathize with her feeling blamed,
for she required some validation of her sense of reality-that the thera-
pist had actually been critical and that she had not fabricated her feel-
ings out of some hypersensitivity to feedback. To withhold comment
lends a sense of unreality to such a situation. Her question, '1\re you
bored too?" is a form of reality-testing, so that to fail to provide some
self-disclosing response is to thwart her efforts at reality-testing.
Kohut (1984) suggested that at points of empathic failure and mis-
understanding, therapist verbal self-disclosure may be useful:
In a properly conducted analysis, the analyst takes note of the analysand's
retreat, searches for any mistakes he might have made, nondefensively
84 Lawrence Josephs
acknowledges them after he has recognized them (often with the help of the
analysand). (p. 67)
Susan, one arduous aspect of therapy was learning how to tolerate the
psychic intimacy of opening herself up to my perceptions of her, slowly
learning that in taking in some of my ego-alien views of her and making
them her own she need not lose or sacrifice her enduring sense of self-
sameness and self-continuity, and could perhaps enrich her sense of self
in the process.
Conclusion
Self-disclosure in psychotherapy is never received wisdom whose
meaning is transparent. Self-disclosure is always an intersubjective pro-
cess, and the self that the discloser transmits is rarely identical with the
self the listener receives, registers, and reflects back to the discloser.
This discrepancy is inevitable because empathy, our only tool for grasp-
ing the self of the other, is an imperfect instrument that can aim at
approximation but can never achieve absolute identity. Self-disclosure is
also an intrapsychic process in which the self is disclosed as an object to
oneself, in the vernacular, "getting to know oneself." The self that is
disclosed as an object to oneself is also always a construction rather than
a received wisdom that miraculously arises from the oracular pro-
nouncement of the unconscious mind. In conclusion, it should be re-
membered that each act of self-disclosure in psychotherapy is, as well,
an act of self-creation.
References
Atwood, G.G., & Stolorow, R.D. (1984). Structures of subjectivity: Explorations in psycJwanaly-
tic phenomenology. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bollas, C. (1987). The shadow of the object: Psychoanalysis of the unthought known. New York:
Columbia University Press.
Gill, M. (1982). Analysis of transference: Vol. I. New York: International Universities Press.
Hoffman, 1. (1983). The patient as interpreter of the analyst's experience. Contemporary
Psychoanalysis, 19, 389-422.
Josephs, L. (1988). A comparison of archaeological and empathic modes of listening.
Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 24, 282-300.
Josephs, L. (1989). The world of the concrete: A comparative approach. Contemporary
Psychoanalysis, 25, 477-500.
Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self: A systematic approach to the psychoanalytic treatment of
narcissistic personality disorders. New York: International Universities Press.
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York: International Universities Press.
Kohut, H. (1984). How does analysis cure? Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Langs, R. (1976). The bipersonal field. New York: Jason Aronson.
McDougall, J. (1985). Theaters of the mind: Illusion and truth on the psychoanalytic stage. New
York: Basic Books.
Self-Disclosure in Psychotherapy 89
Stern, D.N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and
developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.
Winnicott, D.w. (1968). The maturational process and the facilitating environment. New York:
International Universities Press.
III
Therapeutic Issues
7
The Role of Implicit Communication in
Therapist Self-Disclosure
Jonathan M. Jackson
93
94 Jonathan M. Jackson
for the patient to identify with, in the service of resolving his conflicts
with self and others.
Compared with Loewald's advocacy of the therapist's neutrality,
Stone (1961) relents somewhat, and allows for an attitude of benign
friendliness toward the patient. The aim of this attitude, however, is
consonant with Loewald's. That is, Stone wants to encourage an unfold-
ing of the transference (motivated from persistent and unmodified
wishes toward people in the past) that for some patients would be inhib-
ited by too much silence, self-reserve, and nonresponsiveness on the
part of the therapist. Benign friendliness is used to promote the patient's
transference, but not to interact with it. An example of neutrality mixed
with benign friendliness is seen in the advice of a colleague, who shied
away from directly addressing patients by name. She argued it could
imbue the transference with a warm (using first names) or cool (using
last names) tone and, in so doing, interfere with revival and subsequent
understanding of a patient's sense of warmth or coldness with his or her
early objects. She illustrated this by citing an instance of a patient leav-
ing a scarf behind in her waiting room at the end of a session. The
therapist ran after the patient and caught her attention visually just
before she passed through the elevator door, never once needing to call
out the patient's name. This technique was supported by another illus-
trating how one may telephone a patient without addressing him or her
by name either.
Kohut (1977) too is concerned with neutrality. He advocates a posi-
tion of empathic involvement wherein the therapist functions as a self-
object for the patient, facilitating a transference based on what the
patient's developmental level requires. The therapist's personal reactions
to the patient are seen as countertransferential to the extent that they
veer from the patient's needs for empathic resonance and prematurely
impose on the patient an awareness of the therapist as a separate person.
Responding too freely out of one's personal reactions (a break in empa-
thy vis-a.-vis the patient) is traumatic because the patient is not yet able
to function as a separate, differentiated self and so does not have the
reciprocal capacity to treat the therapist as a separate self. Neutrality in
this view is a matter of shielding the child-patient from the disruptive
realities of the parent-therapist. In this sense, the therapist fosters a
regressive relationship that is conceived by the therapist to be the
needed restorative one, as opposed to the actual one.
Prior to Kohut, Winnicott (1965) developed the notion of holding the
patient, a therapeutic function that keeps impingements from the outside
environment to a minimum. Impingements refer to a class of events, in-
cluding at times revelations of the therapist's personal reality, that dis-
rupt the patient's continuity of experience and thus create dissociative
96 Jonathan M. Jackson
Special Events
According to the writers mentioned who represent a wide array of
theoretical schools, self-disclosure is viewed as a shift away from ordi-
nary therapeutic endeavors, to be utilized sparingly or not at all. When
it cannot be avoided, self-disclosure is often seen as evidence of uncon-
trolled countertransference which interferes with the patient's ability to
use optimally the therapeutic situation.
Therapist Self-Disclosure 97
In Hoffman's view, and he cites the work of Gill (1982), Racker (1968),
and Levenson (1981) as supportive of his central notions, the patient is .
engaged in interpretive activity just as the therapist is. Hoffman posits
two ideas that form the basis for this conclusion. According to one idea,
the patient senses that the therapist's behavior is at best only partially
indicative of the therapist's entire experience. Thus, the patient is in an
ambiguous situation regarding the entirety of who the therapist is, and
Therapist Self-Disclosure 99
References
Fenster,S., Phillips,S., & Rapaport. (1986). The therapist's pregnancy: Intrusion in the analytic
space. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Ferenczi, S. (1988). Confusion of tongues between adults and the child: The language of
tenderness and of passion. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 24, 196-206. (Originally pub-
lished 1933.)
Fromm, E. (1951). The forgotten language. New York: Grove Press.
Gill, M.M. (1982). Analysis of transference I: Theory and technique. New York: International
Universities Press.
Greenberg, J. (1986). Theoretical models and the analyst's neutrality. Contemporary Psycho-
analysis, 22, 87-106.
Hirsch, I. (1981). Authoritarian aspects of the therapeutic relationship. Review of Existential
Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 105-133.
Hoffman, I.Z. (1983). The patient as interpreter of the analyst's experience. Contemporary
Psychoanalysis, 19, 389-422.
Jourard, S.M. (1971). The transparent self. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Katz, J. (1978). A psychoanalyst's anonymity: Fiddler behind the couch. Bulletin of the
Menninger Clinic 42, 520-524. New York: International Universities Press.
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York: International Universities Press.
Langs, R. (1975). The therapeutic relationship and deviations in technique. International
Journal of Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 4, 106-141.
Levenson, E. (1981). Facts or fantasies: The nature of psychoanalytic data. Contemporary
Psychoanalysis, 17, 486-500.
Loewald, H. (1960). On the therapeutic action of psychoanalysis. International Journal of
Psychoanalysis 41, 16-33.
Racker, H. (1968). Transference and countertransference. New York: International Universities
Press.
Singer, E. (1971). The patient aids the analyst: Some clinical and theoretical observations.
In B. Landis and E.S. Tauber (Eds.), In the name of life-Essays in honor of Erich Fromm,
New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Stone, L. (1961). The psychoanalytic situation. New York: International Universities Press.
Strupp, H. (1969). Psychoanalytic psychotherapy and research. In L.D. Eron and R. Cal-
lahan (Eds.). The relation of theory to practice in psychotherapy. Chicago: Aldine
Publishing.
Tarnower, W. (1966). Extra-analytic contacts between the psychoanalyst and the patient.
The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 35, 399-413.
102 Jonathan M. Jackson
Weiss, E. (1975). The effect on the transference of special events occurring during psycho-
analysis. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 56, 69-75.
Winnicott, D.W. (1965). Maturational processing and the facilitating environment. New York:
International Universities Press.
8
Transference, Countertransference, and
Therapeutic Efficacy in Relation to
Self-Disclosure by the Analyst
Esther Menaker
Very little has been written in the psychoanalytic literature about self-
disclosure-that is, self-disclosure by the analyst. In fact there is no
such category in the Grinstein Index of Psychoanalytic Writings. This is
not surprising since the very essence of the philosophy of classical psy-
choanalytic technique rests on the premise that the" cure" for the patient
depends on the creation of a "transference neurosis" within the context
of the psychoanalytic situation; and this in turn depends on the neu-
trality of the analyst. Neutrality in this case means a minimum of self-
revelation: no disclosure of opinions, values, or advice; no sharing of
personal experiences of biographical facts. The analyst is to remain a
nonperson in the name of fostering the development of the trans-
ference-that projection onto the person of the analyst of each individ-
ual's specific way of loving-or hating-which is the legacy of
constitution and early childhood experience (Freud, 1953). In the face of
a person's search for psychological help the therapist, according to clas-
sical psychoanalytic theory, is to remain neutral, that is, not interact with
the patient in any way other than to foster the uncovering of uncon-
scious impulses and to communicate such insights to the patient. This
procedure is founded on the premise that help or cure for the patient is
103
104 Esther Menaker
undertaking? I think not, for the simple reason that transference reac-
tions occur constantly in all our life situations, as I have already re-
marked, and as Freud himself stated. We carry with us the baggage of
our past which influences all our perceptions. It used to be called, in
academic psychological circles, our apperceptive background. The im-
print of past experience influences our perception of present-day events,
including the perception of people and their interaction with us. Fur-
thermore, the self-disclosure of the analyst is not-nor should it be-a
contrived technique calculated to further the therapeutic process. The
analyst unavoidably reveals him or herself in the course of interacting
with the patient through appearance, voice tone, way of expressing
herself, gait and body stance, to mention only a few of the cues that we
all pick up in the course of any human interaction. But self-disclosure of
the analyst in the analytic situation, as I understand it, is more than
that. It is a spontaneous empathic response to the patient's communica-
tion by sharing an analogous feeling or experience in the life of the
analyst. The purpose of the disclosure is to underscore for the patient
the fact that the analyst has understood the import of the communica-
tion and that he or she affirms the patient's reality. Sometimes, of course,
such an interaction goes awry. The analyst may not have understood, or
not have understood fully. But no matter; for the very willingness to
share experiences on the part of the analyst creates an atmosphere con-
genial to dialogue; and in the course of the subsequent interchange
greater understanding and trust is inevitably achieved and a closer bond
between the two participating individuals is created.
The fostering of an understanding bond between patient and thera-
pist in the name of arriving at a mutually acceptable understanding of the
patient's reality is, of course, the opposite of the Freudian approach,
which seeks to uncover a reality unknown to the patient-the repressed
and therefore unconscious, unacceptable instinctual impulses which
are seen as the cause of neurosis or of neurotic symptoms. A major
vehicle for this archaeological task is the analysis of the transference:
that projection of the patient's inner emotional life, complete with distor-
tions, upon the relatively nonparticipating personality of the analyst.
Thus a "truth" is arrived at that supposedly reflects hitherto unknown
feelings, wishes, and impulses. Once such previously unconscious im-
pulses are brought into awareness, they exist in the domain of the ego
and can be volitionally accepted or rejected, acted upon or not. The
possibility of choice ~as become part of the patient's psychological life.
The premise that underlies this approach to therapy is that the curative
factor in the analytic situation is the uncovering of the unconscious
(largely through free association, the analysis of dreams and fantasies,
and the analysis of the transference), making it known to the patient and
108 Esther Menaker
helping him or her to work through and assimilate the newly acquired
knowledge. The "real" or "actual" relationship of the patient to the an-
alyst is rarely of any consequence, since the analyst is viewed as an
outside observer, not as a participant in a relationship. This is the model
in broad strokes of the classical Freudian philosophy of therapy. It is
based primarily on a theory of conflict in which human development
takes place primarily as a struggle between drives that seek pleasure
(the release of tension) and superego constraints that represent the de-
mands of society.
A different view of development leads to a very different theory of
therapy-one in which the active role of the analyst plays a crucial part,
and in which the element of self-disclosure may contribute to the out-
come. It is in the approach of self psychology as it was begun by Heinz
Kohut that the patient's relationship to and interaction with the analyst
is decisive for change and/or cure. The analyst's way of observing as a
self psychologist differs from that of the Freudian analyst. For Kohut
advocates what he terms an introspective, empathic stance. In following
this directive the analyst is an active participant in the interchange with
the patient, observing the patient not as an object to be comprehended
from a distance outside oneself, but as someone whose emotions one
can take in even to the extent of momentarily losing oneself and merging
with the patient. It is through such empathy that the self-psychologist
hopes to make good those deficits in the patient's development that are
responsible for the maladaptations and unhappiness that brought him
or her to seek psychotherapeutic treatment. Self psychology rarely
speaks of neurosis or of conflict, but rather of arrests or deficits in an
individual's development due to familial experiences that failed to pro-
vide adequate nourishment for the child's developing self. A major as-
pect of the analyst's task is to provide that nourishment.
The rudiments of self-structure are given from the beginning of life
in the very nature of a child's constitution-in the way of responding to
the environment, the sensitivity to stimuli, and the reactivity of the
nervous system, for example. There are great individual differences in
the basic psychobiologic nature of individuals, for each person is
unique. Yet this uniqueness is further augmented by the specific nature
of the familial experience in the course of which the structure of the self
is laid down through processes of internalization. The child takes into
him- or herself the parents' very way of being: first the external traits-
the way of speaking, of walking, of gesturing; then the more internal-
the way of thinking, of relating to others, of feeling about oneself. Thus
the self of the child is structured.
Of course, the child is not a duplicate of his/her parents, for the
identifications have been added to the initial predispositions and a new
Transference, Countertransference, and Therapeutic Efficacy 109
upon a reproduction of the Mona Lisa. The picture which had just been
given to my mother by a neighbor was standing against a wall waiting to
be hung. I had been frightened by her strange enigmatic smile and ran
screaming down the hall to ask my mother: "Who is that Indian down
the ha1l?" Even as an adult and even after having seen the original, I
have never succeeded in feeling comfortable with da Vinci's supposed
mas terpiece .
Such small vignettes out of my own life, particularly out of my
childhood that revealed my own emotions gradually helped Karl to
experience his own feelings in a less muted form. My disclosures also
enabled him to perceive and experience me as a real person with
strengths and weaknesses, with tastes and values that sometimes coin-
cided with his own, but were sometimes quite different. What is thera-
peutically important is that my revealed authenticity helped him to
delineate and define his own.
But what of the transference, that supposedly therapeutic vehicle
through the analysis of which memories are to be recovered, repres-
sions lifted, insight gained? The transference both in life and in the
therapeutic situation, since it is an individual's way of relating to others,
occurs inevitably. Yet the classic analytic situation, by virtue of the an-
alyst's lack of participation, intensifies and distorts the projection of the
patient's emotions onto the person of the analyst. I am reminded in this
connection of a film that grew out of Brazelton's infant research which I
had the good fortune to have seen in 1977.
The researchers were studying mother-infant interaction. One
frame showed an infant of about nine months sitting in his high chair,
expectantly awaiting the arrival of his mother. When she entered the
room, smiling, cooing, and expressing her pleasure in being with him,
the little boy smiled, made gleeful sounds and body movements which
he could scarcely contain for sheer joy. The same child on another occa-
sion in which the setup was the same was confronted by his mother, not
with a smiling face but with a "dead" face in which no emotion was
expressed. The disappointed and frustrated child tried in every way-
by cooing and smiling himself, and by physically reaching out toward
his mother-to elicit a response from her. It was all to no avail. She
remained stony-faced. Finally, in frustration, the child began to cry, and
what began as a cry that expressed need, longing, disappointment, and
anxiety at being thus abandoned, turned into a cry of rage.
In the psychoanalytic situation, a similar scenario is recreated: the
childhood situation of the patient in which "The inevitable hierarchy of
the parent as protector but inevitably as final authority on the one hand
and dependent child on the other is repeated in the authoritative posi-
tion of the analyst and the inevitably submissive position of the patient"
112 Esther Menaker
References
Brazelton, T. B. (1977). Demonstration at NPAP's celebration of the twenty-fifth anniversary
of their acquisition of the Psyclwanalytic Review.
Freud, S. (1953). Dynamics of the transference. Collected Papers Vol. II, London: Hogarth
Press.
Menaker, E. (1988). Pitfalls of the transference. Paper delivered at the Adelphi University
Conference, How people change: Inside and outside therapy. October 1, 1988. (in press.)
Menaker, E. (1989). Appointment in Vienna. New York: St. Martin's Press.
9
Self-Disclosure and the Nonwhite
Ethnic Minority Patient
Adelbert H. Jenkins
Adelbert H. Jenkins • Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New
York 10003.
117
118 Adelbert H. Jenkins
If any client or patient (in this chapter I will use these two terms
interchangeably to describe the person seeking psychotherapy) is to be
helped (s)he must be enabled to talk freely and disclose or reveal aspects
of the self that may be seldom discussed or even thought about, but
which represent important limitations to that person's functioning.
With the minority client, even where the therapist gives evidence of
being well-meaning, (s)he may very well be seen as representing a social
structure that has been oppressive and judgmental and that seeks con-
tinually to work to its own advantage at the expense of the minority
individual (Bulhan, 1985). Thus, the American racial context puts an
added burden on the already difficult processes involved in self-
disclosure. Nevertheless, it is this writer's view that psychotherapy in
cross-ethnic situations can be useful to nonwhite minority clients and
the problems that impede such work can be overcome. With these
thoughts in mind I will focus the discussion on two issues: (1) engaging
the minority client in therapy; and (2) addressing language factors in the
cross-ethnic psychotherapy situation.
Cultural Paranoia
Those writing about work with black clients have frequently spoken
about the reticent and rather suspicious manner with which these cli-
ents relate to mental health workers (Block, 1981). Ridley (1984) writing
about the "nondisclosing black client" points out the appropriateness for
black people of a kind of "cultural paranoia" (Grier & Cobbs, 1968), a
wariness regarding the behavior and motivations of white people. He
suggests that for the therapist working with a black client, the dynamics
of self-disclosure involve recognizing the difference for a given client
between "functional paranoia," based on maladaptive personal reac-
tions not related to race, and cultural paranoia. A "healthy cultural
paranoiac" -low on functional paranoia and high on cultural para-
noia-is more likely to be disclosive to a black therapist and nondisclo-
sive to a white therapist, in this view. A "functional paranoiac" -low on
cultural paranoia and high on nonracially based paranoid reactions-
would be nondisclosing to either a black or a white therapist. Ridley
goes on to suggest different strategies for addressing the client within
this conception.
The Gibbs perspective is consistent with the idea that the minority
client, though troubled, and in some sense "demoralized" (Frank, 1973,
p. 314) as (s)he comes to therapy, nevertheless attempts to take an active
and purposeful role in the therapy situation. To some degree, these are
aspects of the exercise of "psychological agency" that minority people
122 Adelbert H. Jenkins
(p. 17). Language processes are at the heart of the therapeutic enterprise,
of course, and almost by definition are fundamental to issues related to
self-disclosure.
Self-Disclosure to Oneself
It is not only that subtly pejorative attitudes on the therapist's part
stifle revelation to another person; an important intrapersonal process is
shut off as well. Self-disclosure involves not only communication be-
tween persons, it involves the individual's disclosing hidden or unar-
ticulated aspects of experience to himself/herself. Citing a clinical
example, Basch (1988) reports having commented to a patient who de-
scribed himself as not being "much of a talker," that the problem wasn't
so much his inability to talk to other people. The primary difficulty was
"that you can't talk things over with yourself inside your head. Without
words, it's hard to think things through, to consider different possi-
bilities" (p. 230). The opportunity to develop new labels and new con-
structions of past and current experience allows the person a changed
way of "coming at" the world.
Self-Disclosure and the Nonwhite Ethnic Minority Patient 127
that they speak" (p. 335). These patients do not necessarily come into
therapy for identity conflicts related to language, but as therapy pro-
gresses, problems come to light that sometimes reveal a number of
concerns regarding ethnicity and acculturation.
Language Switching
In some instances, with bilingual patients, the use of "language
switching" is advocated, that is, explicitly encouraging the client to
Self-Disclosure and the Nonwhite Ethnic Minority Patient 129
Therapist Self-Disclosure
Much of the discussion in this chapter has pointed implicitly to the
complementary issue regarding self-disclosure, namely self-revelation
by the therapist. Again, much could be said about this topic, but we will
have to confine ourselves to a few points here. Humanistic therapists
write of the need for "genuineness" in the therapeutic encounter, which
could include disclosure by the therapist of personal feelings or history
130 Adelbert H. Jenkins
Conclusion
I have proposed here that enhancing self-disclosure in therapy with
the nonwhite ethnic minority client requires first that the therapist be
fully conscious of the ongoing racial context in which (s)he and the
patient live. It requires the therapist's recognition that (s)he is in some
way affected by this atmosphere and brings that to the treatment
situation.
I have also proposed that we must add to the usual, too-simple view
of the minority patient as only a victim, facing the double jeopardy of
social oppression and personal psychopathology. The nonwhite ethnic
minority client, like all people, is an agent seeking to further a sense of
competence in life. Minority patients in the interests of survival bring
the same carefully evaluative attitudes to therapy that they have used
historically to adapt to institutions in this society. The effective thera-
pist must be open from the beginning to the special scrutiny of hislher
egalitarianism. (S)he must also be prepared to develop a special sensi-
tivity to the dynamics of language use in the therapy situation. I believe
that there is no question that dynamic psychotherapy relevant to the
patient's personal needs and ethnocultural heritage can be empowering
for minority clients. As Lerner (1974) notes:
Generic psychotherapy is fully compatible with social change because it is an
attempt to restore personal power-self-understanding, self-control, self-
direction, and self-esteem - through the development of an honest, empathic
egalitarian relationship with another human being. (p. 53)
I believe also that such a process can occur in cross-ethnic therapy
situations if a skilled therapist is honest and open with himself/herself
and prepared to learn.
References
Acosta, F.X., Yamamoto, J., & Evans, L.A. (1982). Effective psychotherapy for low-income and
minority patients. New York: Plenum.
132 Adelbert H. Jenkins
Banks, G., Berenson, B.G., & Carkhuff, R.R. (1967). The effects of counselor race and
training upon the counseling process with Negro clients in initial interviews. Journal
of Clinical Psychology, 23, 70-72.
Basch, M.E (1980). Doing psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books.
Basch, M.E (1988). Understanding psychotherapy: The science behind the art. New York: Basic
Books.
Block, C.B. (1981). Black Americans and the cross-cultural counseling and psychotherapy
experience. In A.J. Marsella & P.B. Pedersen (Eds.), Cross-cultural counseling and psy-
chotherapy (pp. 177-194). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.
Bulhan, H.A. (1985). Frantz Fanon and the psychology of oppression. New York: Plenum.
Carkhuff, R.R., & Pierce, R. (1967). Differential effects of therapist race and social class
upon patient depth of self-exploration in the initial clinical interview. Journal of Con-
sulting Psychology, 31, 632-634.
Cheek, D.K. (1976). Assertive black .. . puzzled white. San Luis Obispo, CA: Impact.
Comas-Diaz, L. (1988). Cross-cultural mental health treatment. In L. Comas-Diaz &
E.E.H. Griffiths (Eds.), Clinical guidelines in cross-cultural mental health (pp. 335-361).
New York: Wiley.
Comas-Diaz, L., & Griffiths, E.E.H. (Eds.). (1988). Clinical guidelines in cross-cultural mental
health. New York: Wiley.
Dudley, R., & Rawlins, M. (Eds.). (1985). Special issue: Psychotherapy with ethnic minor-
ities. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 22, No.2 (Supp.).
Edelson, M. (1971). The idea of a mental illness. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Edelson, M. (1975). Language and interpretation in psychoanalysis. New Haven: Yale Univer-
sity Press.
Fanon, E (1967). Black skin, white masks. New York: Grove.
Frank, J.D. (1973). Persuasion and healing: A comparative study of psychotherapy (2nd ed.).
Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Gardner, LaM. H. (1971). The therapeutic relationship under varying conditions of race.
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 8, 78-87.
Geller, J. D. (1988). Racial bias in the evaluation of patients for psychotherapy. In L. Comas-
Diaz & E.E.H. Griffiths (Eds.), Clinical guidelines in cross-cultural mental health (pp. 112-
134). New York: Wiley.
Gibbs, J.T. (1985). Treatment relationships with black clients: Interpersonal vs. instrumen-
tal strategies. In Advances in clinical social work practice. Silver Spring, MD: National
Assoc. of Social Workers, Inc.
Gibbs, J.T. (Ed.). (1988). Young, black, and male in America. Dover, MA: Auburn House.
Gill, M.M. (1983). The interpersonal paradigm and the degree of the therapist's involve-
ment. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 19, 200-237.
Grier, W.H., & Cobbs, P.M. (1968). Black rage. New York: Basic Books.
Griffith, M.S., & Jones, E.E. (1979). Race and psychotherapy: Changing perspectives. In
J.H. Masserman (Ed.), Current psychiatric therapies, Vol. 18 (pp. 225-235). New York:
Grune & Stratton.
Gumperz, J.L & Hernandez-Chavez, E. (1972). Bilingualism, bidialectalism, and class-
room interaction. In C.B. Cazden, v.P. John & D. Hymes (Eds.). Functions of language
in the classroom (pp. 84-108). New York: Teachers College Press.
Jenkins, A.H. (1982). The psychology of the Afro-American: A humanistic approach. Elmsford,
NY: Pergamon.
Jenkins, A.H. (1985). Attending to self-activity in the Afro-American client. Psychotherapy:
Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 22, (2 S), 335-341.
Jenkins, A.H. (1988). Contributions of a rigorous humanism to psychoanalytic theory. Paper
presented at American Psychological Assn. meeting, Atlanta, Ga.
Jenkins, A.H. (1989). Psychological agency: A crucial concept for minorities. Theoretical and
Philosophical Psychology, 9, 4-11.
Self-Disclosure and the Nonwhite Ethnic Minority Patient 133
Jones, E.E. (1978). Effects of race on psychotherapy process and outcome: An exploratory
investigation. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 15, 226-236.
Jones, E.E. (1985). Psychotherapy and counseling with black clients. In P. Pedersen (Ed.),
Handbook of Cross-cultural counseling and therapy. Westport, CT.: Greenwood.
Jordan, W. (1968). White over black. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press.
Kernan, C.M. (1971). Language behavior in a black urban community. Monographs of the
Language-Behavior Research Laboratory, University of California, Berkeley, No.2.
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York: International Universities Press.
Korchin, S.J. (1976). Modern clinical psychology. New York: Basic Books.
Kove!, J. (1984). White racism: A psychohistory (2nd ed.). New York: Columbia University
Press.
Lerner, B. (1972). Therapy in the ghetto: Political impotence and personal disintegration. Bal-
timore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Lerner, B. (1974). Is psychotherapy relevant to the needs of the poor? In D.A. Evans & W.L.
Claiborn (Eds.), Mental health issues and the urban poor (pp. 49-54). Elmsford, N.Y.:
Pergamon.
Levenson, E.A. (1983). The ambiguity of change: An inquiry into the nature of psychoanalytic
reality. New York: Basic Books.
Luborsky, L. (1984). Principles of psychoanalytic psychotherapy: A manual for supportive-
expressive treatment. New York: Basic Books.
Marcos, L.R. (1976). Bilinguals in psychotherapy: Language as an emotional barrier. Amer-
ican Journal of Psychotherapy, 30, 552-560.
Marcos, L.R., & Urcuyo, L. (1979). Dynamic psychotherapy with the bilingual patient.
American Journal of Psychotherapy, 33, 331-338.
McGoldrick, M., Pearce, J.K., & Giordano, J. (Eds.). (1982). Ethnicity and family therapy.
New York: Guilford.
Paster, VS. (1985). Adapting psychotherapy for the depressed, unacculturated, acting-out,
black male adolescent. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 22, (2 S),
408-417.
Pedersen, P. (Ed.). (1985). Handbook of cross-cultural counseling and therapy. Westport, CT:
Greenwood.
Plasky, P., & Lorion, R.P. (1984). Demographic parameters of self-disclosure to psycho-
therapists and others. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 21, 483-490.
Ridley, C.R. (1984). Clinical treatment of the nondisclosing black client: A therapeutic
paradox. American Psychologist, 39, 1234-1244.
Rogers, C.R. (1957). The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality
change. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21, 95-103.
Ross, S. (1983). Variables associated with dropping out of therapy. (Doctoral dissertation,
New York University, 1983). Dissertation Abstracts International, 44, 616B.
Rozensky, R.H., & Gomez, M. Y. (1983). Language switching in psychotherapy with bilin-
guals: Two problems, two models, and case examples. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research
and Practice, 20, 152-160.
Russell, D.M. (1988). Language and psychotherapy: The influence of nonstandard English
in clinical practice. In L. Comas-Diaz & E.E.H. Griffiths (Eds.), Clinical guidelines in
cross-cultural mental health (pp. 33-68). New York: Wiley.
Rychlak, I.E (1979). Discovering free will and personal responsibility. New York: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Rychlak, J.E (1988). The psychology of rigorous humanism (2nd ed.). New York: New York
University Press.
Schafer, R. (1978). Language and insight: The Sigmund Freud Memorial Lectures 1975-1976,
University College London. New Haven, Conn. and London: Yale University Press.
Schwaber, E.A. (1983). A particular perspective on psychoanalytic listening. Psychoanaly-
tic Study of the Child, 38, 519-546.
134 Adelbert H. Jenkins
Smith, M., & Dejoie-Smith, M. (1984). Behavior therapy for nonwhite, non-YAVIS clients:
Myth or panacea? Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 21, 524-529.
Smith, w.o., Burlew, A.K., Mosley, M.H., & Whitney, W.M. (1978). Minority issues in
mental health. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Strupp, H.H., & Binder, J.L. (1984). Psychotherapy in a new key: A guide to time-limited
dynamic psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books.
Sue, S. (1977). Community mental health services to minority groups: Some optimism,
some pessimism. American Psychologist, 32, 616-624.
Sue, D., & Sue, S. (1987). Cultural factors in the clinical assessment of Asian Americans.
Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 55, 479-487.
Sue, S., & Zane, N. (1987). The role of culture and cultural techniques in psychotherapy: A
critique and reformulation. American Psychologist, 42, 37-45.
Thomas, A., & Sillen, S. (1972). Racism and psychiatry. New York: Brunner/Maze!'
Trudgill, P. (1974). Sociolinguistics: An introduction. Harmondsworth, England: Penguin
Books.
White, RW. (1963). Ego and reality in psychoanalytic theory. Psychological Issues, 3, (11).
Williams, F. (1970). Language, attitude, and social change. In F. Williams (Ed.), Language
and poverty (pp. 380-399). Chicago: Markham.
10
Feminist Therapy Perspectives
on Self-Disclosure
Laura S. Brown and Lenore E. A. Walker
Introduction
Unlike many other theories of psychotherapy, feminist therapy theory
has from its inception promulgated the idea that self-disclosure by the
therapist, may be a valuable part of the therapy process (Greenspan,
1986). This concept appears in some of the earliest literature addressing
the nature of the client-therapist relationship in feminist therapy (Man-
der & Rush, 1974; Lerman, 1976; Rawlings & Carter, 1977), and has
continued to form a core of the feminist therapy mandate to empower
clients, as well as heal their wounds. Guidelines for the use of self-
disclosure can be found in the Ethical Code of the Feminist Therapy
Institute (FTI), which specifically refers to ethical ways in which self-
disclosure can and should be used by a therapist (Feminist Therapy
Ethical Code, 1987), and discussion of the implementation of this code
and its norms is ongoing (Lerman & Porter, 1990). This proactive em-
brace of self-disclosure is unique among theories of psychotherapy, and
reflects certain core tenets of feminist therapy theory regarding the
relationship of client and therapist and the role of the therapist in the
healing process.
In this chapter, we will review those aspects of feminist therapy
theory that tend to support the use of self-disclosure in therapy, discuss
the development of that theory, including some of the early difficulties
Laura S. Brown • 4527 First Avenue NE, Seattle, Washington 98105. Lenore E. A.
Walker. Walker and Associates, 50 South Steele Street, Suite 850, Denver, Colorado 80209.
135
136 Laura S. Brown and Lenore E. A. Walker
are dependent and whom you love (e.g., incestuous fathers, battering
partners) implies careful attention to the arrangement of power and
dependency in the psychotherapeutic relationship. Feminist therapists
have attempted to develop strategies for reducing rather than reinforcing
the power imbalance. Therapist self-disclosure has been such a strategy
in a relationship of inherently unequal power such as psychotherapy.
The corrective experience, as conceptualized from a feminist thera-
py framework, has included the therapist's ability to aid her client in
expanding herlhis vision of the alternatives available so that she/he can
make informed choices rather than nonconsciously acquiescing to the
norms of sexist stereotypes (Bern & Bern, 1976). Change is construed as
occurring in all domains, e.g., cognitive, affective, and behavioral, and
a variety of techniques have been integrated into feminist therapy prac-
tice to provide the opportunity for such changes. Bibliotherapy, body
work, dream analysis, political analysis, skill enhancement, cognitive
restructuring and reframing, desensitization, assertion training, gestalt
awareness techniques, and various approaches to affective catharsis
have all found a place in the work of feminist therapists (Dutton-
Douglas & Walker, 1989; Rosewater & Walker, 1985).
In picking and choosing different techniques from among already
extant therapies, first generation feminist therapy developed as a hy-
brid. However, each of these approaches, often developing initially in
ignorance of others, has maintained the convergence on those modal
principles of feminist therapy described, and resemble one another
closely enough to be all included under the rubric of feminist therapy.
While some groups of feminist therapists have concentrated on main-
streaming its perspective into traditional approaches to psychotherapy,
(cf. the work ofthe Stone Center group of authors) others have aimed at
creating a system separate and unique from the mainstream (Brown,
1988a). Second and third generation feminist therapy includes both
these perspectives (Dutton-Douglas & Walker, 1988).
work, and that the real self of the therapist would always be integrated
into any symbolic representations that the client might develop.
If a particular feminist therapist is well-known and her opinions
represented by the media when she is called upon as an expert in her
field, she also self-discloses; at times, this can lead to distortions of the
therapist's actual beliefs that must then be clarified with clients. Addi-
tionally, such self-disclosure means that the therapist may be known,
occasionally inaccurately, to potential clients; without the therapist's
making overt self-disclosures, clients enter treatment with knowledge of
who the therapist is (both authors of this chapter, for example, have had
new clients inform us that we were chosen after reading one of our
published works). Often, particular issues provoke controversy within
feminist communities (e.g., pornography, surrogate motherhood).
When a feminist therapist is known to espouse one among many di-
verse positions on such an issue as the result of her own active participa-
tion in the feminist political discourse, she also self-discloses, whether
she chooses to do so actively in a therapy session or not. Such political
activity is considered ethically imperative for feminist therapists (Femi-
nist Therapy Ethical Code, 1987); consequently, no matter how low the
level of exposure that a particular feminist therapist might choose to
express her political activism, it will lead to some degree of self-
disclosure by implication.
Finally, the use of self-disclosure in feminist therapy was fueled by
reaction against the overly distant, pseudo-anonymous style of ortho-
dox psychoanalysis, which was the highest-status and often primary
therapy modality being practiced during the period in which feminist
therapy began to develop. Feminist therapists have continued over time
to be critical of an overly distant style on the part of androcentrically
trained therapists (Jordan, Surrey, & Kaplan, 1983; Greenspan, 1986).
In fact, some recent authors (Lerman & Porter, 1990) have suggested
that being overly distant may be as damaging and thus ethically prob-
lematic in therapy as is an overt violation of a boundary under the guise
of "closeness." Many early feminist therapy writings contain first-
person accounts of frustration and struggles against the wall of a silent
male analyst (Chesler, 1972; Report of the Task Force, 1975; Mander &
Rush, 1974; Greenspan, 1983). Since criticism of other aspects of psycho-
analytic theory and practice is also a common aspect of first generation
feminist therapy writings, it should not be surprising to find that one
remedy suggested for transforming therapy to a more human exchange
was the encouragement of personal openness and genuineness on the
part of the therapist. In this regard, early feminist therapy thinking was
also strongly influenced by the work of Rogers (1951, 1961) and Perls
(1971) regarding the value of the use of self by the therapist. The
144 Laura S. Brown and Lenore E. A. Walker
and makes her more human, reducing the "us vs. them" quality of the
interaction, and underscores the impact of the therapist's personhood
on the interaction rather than her formal training. Although the poten-
tial danger here is that the therapist's personality becomes more impor-
tant than her skills, thus increasing the risk of a cult developing around
a particular therapist (Brown, 1990b), in the best of all worlds it allows
the client to experience therapy as an interchange between two humans
rather than between "expert" and "patient." Self-disclosure can decrease
the isolation of the client, allowing her to see how her problems have
been encountered even by her therapist. In theory, self-disclosure is one
response that a feminist therapist can make to the client's needs for
sisterhood and solidarity.
In practice, however, self-disclosure can become distorted into a
strategy for the therapist to reduce her own isolation. Hill (1990) has
pointed out that therapists (feminist as well as otherwise) often engage
in low-level violations of their clients' boundaries by self-disclosing at
inappropriate times in order to meet their own needs for emotional
intimacy, contact, nurturance, and validation. Those feminist therapists
who themselves feel marginal (Coffman, 1988) may have an even more
difficult challenge in selecting appropriate times for self-disclosure with
the client's needs as sole or primary.
As Hill points out, feminist therapists may be particularly vulner-
able in this regard because of our commitment to appear as genuine
human beings rather than as mystified therapy goddesses. If we are
steadfast in our commitment, we may lose some of the usual intangible
rewards of the work of therapy, e.g., feeling oneself glowingly respons-
ible for the client's progress or savoring the role of expert and guide. On
occasion, the tension generated by balancing genuineness with respect
for the client's boundaries and a focus on the client's wants and needs
may be the catalyst for inappropriate self-disclosures, statements that
serve primarily to soothe the therapist rather than enlighten or em-
power the client. One client, speaking of another feminist therapist with
whom she had chosen to terminate treatment reported that she had
decided to stop because she was tired of hearing about how the therapist
had dealt with their many shared issues. "I wanted the time and space
to talk about my experiences, and didn't want to hear about what she
had done. After a while it stopped being helpful and started feeling
selfish."
In this and other similar cases the therapist may be substituting
self-disclosure and the use of personal experience for reliance on appro-
priate training and consultation, access to information, or therapy exper-
tise. It is thus noteworthy that the Feminist Therapy Ethics Code
requires attention to consultation, supervision, ongoing training, and
148 Laura S. Brown and Lenore E. A. Walker
assistance for several months during each year. During the period of
time in which her condition was quite audibly developing and being
diagnosed, her clients felt free to ask the questions they needed in order
to feel secure in their therapist's availability, and certain that the thera-
pist was pursuing appropriate medical intervention. Yet they knew that
the therapist would not intrude that information into the relationship,
but rather would make it available upon request.
While these examples may seem highly unusual, in fact it is our
experience that most therapists are likely to have events occur in their
nontherapy lives that visibly leak into their practice. Rather than pro-
moting a game of hide-and-seek between client and therapist, the femi-
nist therapy model gives clear guidelines about the appropriateness of
self-disclosure, as well as giving the therapist the message that she/he
will need to plan in advance with how to disclose when such inevitable
events as birth, death, illness, or personal transformation of some kind
occur.
Since many of the clients who seek feminist therapy are victims/
survivors of violent or otherwise dysfunctional families in which prob-
lems were never discussed in an open manner, and in which secrets
were kept, even though people could sense that something serious was
wrong, the therapists' willingness and comfort in providing information
about their own situations serves as an opportunity to process old pain
about secrets and the denial of one's perceptions. The timing of dealing
with issues such as these in the life of the therapist needs to be carefully
evaluated with regard to each client. The therapist does not disclose ad
libitum, but rather to those clients who inquire or who appear to need to
know, and only the information that is appropriate for the particular
client-therapist dyad at that point in time. In some cases when the client
has a history of needing explicit support to ask for clarification, having
already discussed with the therapist at the inception of treatment that
asking is acceptable and will be responded to in a genuine and positive
manner can be an essential element in the client's ability to ask, and the
therapist may refresh this permission by clarifying that this may be one
of the times when the client has questions they wish to raise.
Such a reliance on the client's initiation of the request for more
information rather than simply providing it to her is one of the major
transitions in the use of self-disclosure that we have seen occur over the
two decades of feminist therapy practice, a change that brings the prac-
tice even closer to the norm of empowering the client. Now, rather than
the therapist deciding what part of her experiences the client should
hear, the client is given encouragement and permission to ask for infor-
mation as she/he perceives the need for it, but also the same support to
not receive it. This makes the act of self-disclosure less likely to be an
Feminist Therapy Perspectives 151
Conclusion
Women have, in the past two decades, reclaimed and recreated their
ancient role and work as healers through the development of feminist
therapy. Being a feminist therapist requires special training in feminist
techniques and philosophy and a grounding in the new feminist schol-
arship on the psychology of women. It also leads to a connection to a
larger spiritual movement which emphasizes the interrelatedness be-
tween women's mental and physical health. Our growing awareness of
the extent to which women, people of color, other oppressed groups,
and the very planet itself have been violated by patriarchal power and
attempts to impose control makes this feminist political perspective
crucial if we are to develop the long-term strength and strategies to heal
these wounds and return power to those who have been violated
(Spretnak, 1982; Greenspan, 1988). Our belief, consonant with that of
most feminist therapists, is that the basic tenets of feminist therapy are,
at their core, those which are necessary for good therapy, in which there
is both closeness and empathy, yet respect for boundaries and human
diversity. The use of self-disclosure has been a primary technique uti-
lized by feminist therapists in building a feminist, egalitarian, and gen-
uinely healing approach to psychotherapy.
References
Ballou, M., & Gabalac, N. (1985). A feminist position on mental health. Springfield, IL:
Charles C. Thomas.
Bern, S. & Bern, D. (1976). Case study of a non-conscious ideology: Training the woman to
know her place: In S. Cox (Ed.), Female psychology: The emerging self. Chicago: SRA.
152 Laura S. Brown and Lenore E. A. Walker
Berman, J.S. (1985). Ethical feminist perspectives on dual relationships with clients. In
L.B. Rosewater & L.E.A. Walker (Eds.), Handbook of feminist therapy: Womens issues in
psychotherapy. New York: Springer.
Bleier, R. (1984). Science and gender. New York: Pergamon.
Broverman, I.K., Broverman, D., Clarkson, R, Rosencrantz, P., & Vogel, S. (1970). Sex-role
stereotypes and clinical judgments of mental health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 34, 1-7.
Brown, L.S., & Liss-Levinson, N. (1981). Feminist therapy. In R. Corsini (Ed.) Handbook of
innovative psychotherapies. New York: Wiley.
Brown, L.S. (1984a). Finding new language: Getting beyond analytic verbal shorthand in
feminist therapy. Women and Therapy, 3, 73-80.
Brown, L.S. (1984b). The lesbian-feminist therapist and her community. Psychotherapy in
private practice, 2, 9-16.
Brown, L.S. (1986). Diagnosis and the zeitgeist: The politics of masochism in the DSM-III-
R. In R. Garfinkel, (Chair), The politics of diagnosis: Feminist psychology and the DSM-IIJ-
R. Symposium presented at the 94th Annual Convention of the American Psychologi-
cal Association, Washington, o.c., Aug.
Brown, L.S. (1987a). Learning to think about ethics: A guide for the perplexed lesbian
feminist therapist. Keynote address presented at a conference, Boundary dilemmas in
the client-therapist relationship: A working conference for lesbian therapists. Los Angeles,
CA, Jan.
Brown, L. S. (1987b). Beyond "Thou shalt not": Developing conceptual framework for ethi-
cal decision-making. In L. Garnets, (Chair), Ethical and boundary dilemmas for lesbian
and gay psychotherapists, Symposium presented to the 95th Annual Convention of the
American Psychological Association, New York, Aug.
Brown, L.S. (1988a). The meaning of a multi-cultural perspective for theory-building in feminist
therapy. Presented at the Seventh Advanced Feminist Therapy Institute, Seattle, WA,
May.
Brown, L.S. (1988b). Feminist therapy perspectives on psychodiagnosis: Beyond DSM and ICD.
Keynote address presented at the First International Congress on Mental Health Care
for Women, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Dec.
Brown, L.S. (1990a). Structuring the business of feminist therapy responsibility. In H.
Lerman & N. Porter (Eds.), Ethics in psychotherapy: Feminist perspectives. New York:
Springer.
Brown, L.S. (1990b). Confronting ethically problematic behaviors in feminist therapy col-
leagues. In H. Lerman & N. Porter (Eds.) Ethics in psychotherapy: Feminist perspectives
New York: Springer.
Caplan, P.J. (1984). The myth of women's masochism. American Psychologist, 39, 130-139.
Cammaert, L., & Larsen, C. (1989). Feminist frameworks of psychotherapy. In M.A.
Dutton-Douglas & L.E.A. Walker (Eds.), Feminist psychotherapies: Integrations of thera-
peutic and feminist systems. Norwood NJ: Ablex Publishing.
Cardea, C. (1985). The lesbian revolution and the 50-minute hour: A working-class look at
therapy and the movement. Lesbian Ethics, 1, 46-68.
Chesler, P. (1972). Women and madness. New York: Doubleday.
Coffman, S. (1988). Developing a feminist model for clinical consultation: Combining diversity
and commonality. Presented at the Seventh Advanced Feminist Therapy Institute,
Seattle, WA, May.
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, III, revised. (1987). American Psychiatric
Association, Washington, D.C.
Dutton-Douglas, M.A., & Walker, L.E.A. (1989). Feminist psychotherapies: Integrations of
therapeutic and feminist systems. Norwood NJ: Ablex Publishing.
Dykewomon, E. (Ed.) (1988). Surviving psychiatric assault. Sinister Wisdom, 36.
Feminist Therapy Perspectives 153
Feminist therapy ethical code. (1987). Feminist Therapy Institute, Denver CO: Author.
Gilbert, L.A. (1980). Feminist therapy. In A. Brodsky & R. Hare-Mustin (Eds.), Women and
psychotherapy: An assessment of research and practice. New York: Guilford.
Gore, S., & Walker, L.E.A. (1986). International women's mental health agenda: Modifications
from the UN End of the Decade Meeting in Nairobi, Kenya. Symposium presented at the
National Conference of the Association for Women in Psychology, Oakland, CA, Mar.
Greene, B. (1986). When the therapist is white and the patient is Black: Considerations for
psychotherapy in the feminist heterosexual and lesbian communities. Women and
Therapy, 5, 41-46.
Greenspan, M. (1983). A new approach to women and therapy. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Greenspan, M. (1986). Should therapists be personal? Self-disclosure and therapeutic
difference in feminist therapy. Women and Therapy, 5, 5-17.
Greenspan, M. (1988). Creating a political spiritual model for feminist therapy. Keynote ad-
dress presented at the First International Congress on Mental Health Care for Women,
Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Dec.
Hill, M. (1990). On creating a theory of feminist therapy. In L.S. Brown & M.P.P. Root
(Eds.). Diversity and complexity in feminist therapy. New York: Haworth Press.
Hoagland, S. L. (1988). Lesbian ethics: Toward new values. Palo Alto, CA: Institute of Lesbian
Studies.
If sex enters into the psychotherapy relationship. (1987). Committee on Women in Psychology.
Washington, D.c.: American Psychological Association.
Jordan, rv., Surrey, J.L., & Kaplan, A.G. (1983). Women and empathy: Implications for psycho-
logical development and psychotherapy. Wellesley, Mass: Stone Center for Developmental
Studies and Services.
Kanuha, V. (1990). The importance of an integrated analysis of oppression in feminist
therapy. In H. Lerman & N. Porter (Eds.), Ethics in psychotherapy: Feminist perspectives.
New York: Springer.
Kaplan, M. (1983). A woman's view of the DSM-III. American Psychologist, 38,786-792.
Kravetz, D. (1976). Consciousness-raising groups and group psychotherapy: Alternative
mental health resources for women. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, 13,66-71.
Lerman, H. (1976). What happens in feminist therapy. In S. Cox (Ed.) Female psychology:
The emerging self. Chicago: SRA.
Lerman, H., & Porter, N. (Eds.). (1990). Ethics in psychotherapy: Feminist perspectives. New
York: Springer.
Mander, A.V., & Rush, A.K. (1974). Feminism as therapy. San Francisco: Random Housel
Bookworks.
Maracek, L & Kravetz, D. (1977). Women and mental health: A review of feminist change
efforts. Psychiatry, 40, 323-329.
Perls, F.S. (1971). Gestalt therapy verbatim. New York: Bantam.
Pheterson, G. (1988). From the therapeutic to the political stage: Psychological disciplines in
feminist thought and action. Keynote address presented at the First International Con-
gress on Mental Health Care for Women, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Dec.
Rawlings, E., & Carter, D. (Eds.) (1977). Psychotherapy for women: Treatment toward equality.
Springfield Ill: Charles C. Thomas.
Raymond, J. (1986). A passion for friends: Toward a philosophy of female affection. Boston:
Beacon Press.
Reiker, P. P., & Carmen, E. D. (Eds.). (1984). The gender gap in psychotherapy: Social realities and
psychological processes. New York: Plenum.
Report of the task force on sex bias and sex-role stereotyping in psychotherapy. (1975).
American Psychological Association Task Force. American Psychologist, 30, 1169-1175.
Rogers, c.R. (1951). Client-centered therapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Rogers, c.R. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
154 Laura S. Brown and Lenore E. A. Walker
Rosencrantz, P.S., Delorey, c., & Broverman, I.K. (1985). One half a generation later: sex-role
stereotypes revisited. Paper presented at the 93rd Annual Convention of the American
Psychological Association, Los Angeles, CA, Aug.
Rosewater, L.B. (1989). Feminist therapies with women. In M.A. Dutton-Douglas &
L.E.A. Walker (Eds.), Feminist psychotherapies: Integrations of therapeutic and feminist
systems. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Rosewater, L.B. & Walker, L.E.A. (Eds.) (1985). Handbook of feminist therapy: Women's issues
in psychotherapy. New York: Springer.
Rothblum, E.o., Solomon, 1., & Albee, G. (1986). A SOciopolitical perspective of DSM-III.
In T. Millon & G. Klerman (Eds.), Contemporary directions in psychopathology: Toward the
DSM-IV. New York: Guilford.
Spretnak, C. (Ed.). (1982). The politics of women's spirituality. New York: Anchor/Doubleday.
Steiner, c., Wycoff, H., Goldstine, D., Lariviere, P., Schwebel, R., & Marcus, J. (1975).
Readings in radical psychiatry. New York: Grove Press.
Sturdivant, S. (1980). Therapy with women: A feminist philosophy of treatment. New York:
Springer.
Walker, L.E.A. (Ed.) (1984a). Women and mental health policy. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Walker, L.E.A. (1984b). Battered women, psychology and public policy. American Psycholo-
gist, 29, 1178.
Walker, L.E.A. (1986). Diagnosis and politics: Abuse disorders. In R. Garfinkel (Chair) The
politics of diagnosis: Feminist therapy and the DSM-lII-R. Symposium presented at the
94th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Washington,
o.c., Aug.
Walker, L.E.A. (1988). Diagnostic systems (DSM-III) and violence against women. Keynote
address presented at the First International Congress on Mental Health Care for
Women, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Dec.
Weisstein, N. (1970). Kinder, kuche, kirche as scientific law: Psychology constructs the
female. In R. Morgan (Ed.), Sisterhood is powerful. New York: Vintage.
Women and psychotherapy: A consumer handbook. (Rev. ed.). (1985). Committee on Women in
Psychology. Washington, 0. C.
Wycoff, H. (Ed.). (1976). Love, therapy and politics: Issues in radical therapy- The first year.
New York: Grove Press.
Wycoff, H. (1977). Solving women's problems through awareness, action, and contact. New York:
Grove Press.
IV
Therapeutic Modalities
n
Self-Disclosure and Psychotherapy
with Children and Adolescents
Nicholas Papouchis
Introduction
The focus of this chapter is a discussion of the concept of self-disclosure
as it applies to therapeutic work with adolescents and children. This
begins with a brief discussion of the relevance of self-disclosure in psy-
chotherapeutic work with adults, in order to demonstrate the impor-
tance of using a developmental perspective in understanding this
phenomenon in work with a younger population. It will also briefly
review the positions taken by different analytic schools of thought re-
garding the therapist's self-disclosure in work with adult patients.
In the discussion that follows, the terms analyst and psychothera-
pist will be used interchangeably, although the author acknowledges
that there are technical differences between the two.
157
158 Nicholas Papouchis
to his patients, and like a mirror, should show them nothing but what is
shown to him." This facilitated the projection of the patient's inner life
onto the person of the therapist (analyst), and contributed to the devel-
opment of the transference neurosis. The resolution of the resulting
psychic conflict was achieved through the process of the analyst's inter-
pretations, which led to insight and "working through." However, some
respected psychoanalytic practitioners (Stone, 1961; Greenson, 1969)
have indicated that there are situations that call for information about
the therapist as a person. Similarly, Schafer (1983), in describing the
classical analyst's empathic stance, noted that "there is always room in
the analytic work for courtesy, cordiality, gentleness, sincere empathic
participation and comment, and other such personal, though not so-
cially intimate, modes of relationship." While Schafer was describing the
analyst's departures from a position of neutrality, his remarks are
equally applicable to the question of self-disclosure.
In spite of these attempts to articulate the classical analyst's empa-
thic response to the patient, the emphasis on interpretation as the major
agent of therapeutic change has led most psychoanalysts working with
adults to pay insufficient attention to the relational issues that contrib-
ute to therapeutic change. In the discussion that follows I will refer to
these relational aspects of the therapeutic relationship as the inter-
personal-object relationship between therapist and patient. This term is
intended to capture both the style of relating and the inner psychologi-
cal makeup of both participants. I believe that this term encompasses
more than what Greenson and Wexler (1969) meant by the term "real
relationship." .
In the last 20 years, clinicians working with adult patients within
the classical tradition have begun to emphasize the reality aspects of the
therapeutic relationship. Greenson and Wexler's (1969) description of the
importance of the "real relationship" in therapy underscored the contri-
bution of the therapist's personality to the therapeutic alliance. They
noted that the rules of abstinence can be adhered to all too literally. They
suggested that the analyst feel free to offer an expression of compassion
or concern, or even an admission of technical error, when appropriate.
In the language of the present discussion, they encouraged "inter-
personal" self-disclosure on the part of the classical analyst. Similarly,
psychoanalytic clinicians working with more severely disturbed pa-
tients have emphasized the importance of modifying the therapist's
neutral stance in working with more disturbed patients whose lack of
psychic organization left them unable to tolerate the therapist's dis-
tance. (Arieti, 1974; Meissner, 1986; Pine, 1985; Winnicott, 1965). Their
recommendations underscored the importance of the interpersonal-
object relationship between therapist and patient as the foundation that
Psychotherapy with Children and Adolescents 161
Here the developmental tasks and needs of our younger patients are an
important determinant in understanding how the phenomenon of self-
disclosure manifests itself in both patient and therapist. For example, it
is not always possible, nor even desirable, for our younger patients to be
self-disclosing in the way that our adult patients are. Latency-age chil-
dren express themselves symbolically through activity as well as
through language. Thus, the child therapist, working with his patient in
a combination of verbal and play therapy, looks for the resolution of
psychic conflicts in the child's play, and in descriptions of his behavior
from teachers and parents, rather than solely through the child's verbal
productions. Premature verbal disclosure of psychic conflict, even when
possible, may tax the child's developing ego resources and impede
rather than facilitate future ego development.
The child therapist must be aware of the developmental tasks that
the latency age child faces. Each of these tasks requires the child's in-
creasing ego mastery of the issues he is faced with. Therapeutic efforts
are directed toward the removal of the obstacles that interfere with
mastery of these tasks, and facilitating the child's development through
periods of disequilibrium as these tasks are mastered (Pine, 1985). The
child therapist, aware of the ways in which his child patient's premature
self-disclosures may represent a threat to a developing sense of self, and
the child's ego resources, modifies the degree to which the child's self-
discovery proceeds.
Let us articulate some of the important tasks of the latency period
before proceeding further:
1. Increasing independence from parents, including opportunities
for other objects of authority with whom to identify.
2. The development of peer relations, including the mastery of is-
sues of competition and cooperation in work and play.
3. Increased ego functioning manifested in greater defensive con-
trol over instinctual life, adaptation to reality, and the capacity
for intellectual mastery of school-based learning.
To illustrate the importance of this knowledge in working with chil-
dren, let us specify the age of a hypothetical patient as six years old. The
child at this age, early in latency, is concerned with repression of pre-
oedipal issues, and is struggling with the mastery of his inner life (Born-
stein, 1951). Premature self-disclosure of sexual or aggressive material
thus represents a threat to the child's developing psychic equilibrium.
Further, the child, at this age, may be using language which is still too
intimately tied to emotional experience. Words may be experienced as
equivalent to the actions they describe. From this perspective, the thera-
pist's insistence on continued exploration of dynamic issues represents a
distinct threat to the child, and is to be carefully avoided.
164 Nicholas Papouchis
Anna Freud has emphasized how the child's developing object rela-
tionships demand that the child therapist or analyst must be anything
but impersonal or shadowy (1976). The latency-age child, like the adoles-
cent, still has his original objects present. The parents exist, not only in
fantasy, as they do with adult patients, but continue to determine the
child's gratifications and disappointments in his everyday life. Thus, the
analyst enters the therapeutic relationship as a new object and the devel-
oping interpersonal-object relationship that ensues contributes signifi-
cantly to the child's development.
In contrast to Melanie Klein's position regarding the interpretation
of transference, Anna Freud (1976) argues that the behavior of the an-
alyst of children is not solely intended to produce a transference that can
be interpreted. Instead, she notes that the person of the analyst is of
great interest to the child, whom he (the child) endows with a variety of
attractive and interesting qualities. Further, the analyst's educational
efforts, and setting of limits informs the child what the analyst sanc-
tions or disapproves of. Thus, as with the adolescent therapist, the child
therapist reveals himself, and his belief system in his efforts at educa-
tion or limit setting. These behaviors are invariably self-disclosing.
Other child therapists (Ross, 1964; Ginott, 1964) have also pointed
out that the child's questions must be responded to differently than an
adult's. Children's questions are often centered on trying to find their
place in the world, and who belongs where. The therapist who deflects
their questions by interpreting the unconscious reasons for their asking,
demonstrates a lack of interest in these concerns, and may interfere with
the child's ability to master reality. Although it is useful to give the child
an opportunity to explore their fantasies about the way they "would like
things to be," the therapist should also respond to questions in a man-
ner that is simple and direct (Ross, 1964).
Similarly, the therapist who intersperses his interpretative inquiries
about the meaning of a child's curiosity about the therapist with some
brief and general answers to the child's questions, addresses both the
unconscious and reality-oriented meaning of the child's concerns (Gin-
ott, 1964). As the treatment relationship develops, the therapist may also
be able to respond directly to the child's inquiries about his mood. To the
extent that the therapist comfortably responds to direct questions about
countertransferential responses that are evident in the analyst's behav-
ior, he enhances the child's faith in the therapist's reliability, and helps
the child to realize that some temporary problem in the relationship
does not interfere with the core of the interpersonal-object relationship
between them (Colm, 1964). These self-disclosures also support the
child's developing ability to experience feelings of ambivalence toward
Psychotherapy with Children and Adolescents 165
the significant people in his life. It should also be emphasized here, that
those feelings which the analyst experiences as the result of unresolved
inner conflicts are not part of the self-disclosure.
Let us turn next to the issues of self-disclosure in therapeutic work
with adolescents. Issues of self-disclosure with this population become
more complex because of the complexity of the developmental tasks
which the adolescent faces.
The early adolescent patient, unlike the latency age child, has devel-
oped the capacity to represent psychic issues symbolically in a verbal
manner. Yet, he may be equally unprepared to be verbally self-disclosing.
The emergence of drive activity at puberty, the dramatic physical
changes that ensue, and the confused, and at times embarrassed sense
of inner states of feeling often make the early adolescent acutely sensi-
tive to what is private and what is public. Adolescents are also acutely
sensitive to issues which threaten their desire for autonomy, and which
reawaken dependent wishes for support from parents. It is often not
until mid-to-Iate adolescence when ego functioning and the capacity for
formal operational thought (Flavell, 1963) have developed sufficiently,
that adolescents feel some sense of mastery over their inner life. At this
time in development, interpersonal skills have developed to the point
where heterosexual relationships have begun. The adolescent has be-
gun to develop some confidence in his sexual identity and more intimate
verbal self-disclosure may become possible without representing a
threat to their sense of self.
Even at this point in the adolescent's development, he is rarely able
to approximate the sustained self-examination that one sees with adult
patients. The therapist who naively follows an adolescent's open self-
revealing discussion in a therapy session, without being aware of the
threat that this material presents to the patient, is often surprised when
his adolescent patient misses the next session. Or, if the material has
been too threatening, the patient decides to terminate therapy. This can
often happen without the adolescent appearing to be aware of how
threatening the experience of self-disclosure was. From this develop-
mental perspective, it is important for the therapist to know that verbal
self-disclosure on the part of his younger patient may not always be a
sign of therapeutic progress, but rather may represent a danger that
threatens to disrupt a developing therapeutic alliance.
John, an articulate 16-year-old adolescent, had spent the previous session
describing with considerable clarity how he felt his father was competing
with him, and favored his younger brother. He began the next session by
166 Nicholas Papouchis
saying that he had thought about discontinuing therapy, but he wasn't sure
why. When his therapist pointed out to him that he had talked a great deal
about angry feelings toward his father in the previous session, he responded
by saying that he had felt guilty about complaining so angrily, but had not
connected these guilty feelings with his thoughts of stopping therapy. The
therapist then slowly began to help John face and accept the anger which he
avoided in a number of other interpersonal situations.
The therapist often helps the adolescent patient by indicating that
there are some things "that may be too upsetting to talk about right
now." Or that the ability to understand the nature of what they are
feeling may proceed at a slower pace. This is not to say that the adoles-
cent therapist should help the patient avoid self-disclosing. Instead, the
therapist monitors the impact of all self-disclosures on the patient's atti-
tudes toward therapy.
Before proceeding to a discussion of the therapist's self-disclosure
with adolescents, it will be helpful to review briefly some of the major
developmental tasks of adolescence. Blos (1979) has described the period
of adolescence as "the second separation-individuation process."
Among the major developmental tasks of this developmental period are:
1. Intrapsychic separation from the family of origin.
2. Mastery of the biological event of puberty.with the necessary
changes in ego functioning, and the initial mastery and accep-
tance of sexuality.
3. The intimately related tasks of the development of a sense of
identity and an ego-ideal.
The therapist working intensively with adolescents will invariably be
forced to help his adolescent patient deal with all these issues. More
often than not, they are going on simultaneously, and the therapist has
to choose which of them to work on at that moment. This usually de-
pends upon which of the issues is highest on the adolescent's list of
psychic priorities. The therapist often finds himself helping his patient
deal with feeling overwhelmed, while respectfully encouraging his pa-
tient's developing ability to cope with the concrete problem at hand.
Similarly, the therapist may deliberately not interpret what he knows to
be a deeply conflictual issue, in favor of supporting the adolescent's
resourceful autonomous coping with that issue. This is not to say that
the adolescent therapist avoids the interpretation of conflictual material.
Rather, that his primary focus is more likely to be on the interpretation of
defenses against affects which interfere with ego development (Meeks,
1986).
Sam, a depressed 17-year-old boy, was constantly fighting with his parents.
Many of his disputes were with his mother to whom he had a decidedly
erotic attachment. His fights were thus designed to distance him from an
Psychotherapy with Children and Adolescents 167
make him a more attractive attachment figure. The person of the adoles-
cent therapist is also available for the adolescent to discuss ideas with,
disagree and argue with, and to accept support from, since that support
need not threaten the adolescents' fragile sense of autonomy.
Adatto (1966) has used the term "special friend" to describe the
nature of the relationship between adolescent and therapist described
above. Clinicians have often described the relationship as avuncular, to
characterize the important interpersonal-object relationship that de-
velops between them. Let me also emphasize that the therapist does not
offer himself as a replacement for the lost relationship with the parents.
To the contrary, the therapist should actively interpret his adolescent
patient's efforts to place him in a parental role (Meeks, 1986). Instead,
the therapist, depending upon the age of the adolescent, is a trusted
adult who stands somewhere between the adolescent's struggle to hold
onto wishes to remain a child, and his drive to become an autonomous
individual on the way to adulthood.
To do this the therapist must present himself as an authentic, caring
presence with whom both aspects of the struggle for autonomy may be
discussed without unduly threatening the adolescent's self-esteem.
When the therapist offers evasive responses to direct questions, refuses
to help solve real problems in the life of the adolescent, or continues to
insist on focusing only on the unconscious meaning of the adolescent's
statements, the adolescent's resistance stiffens and the development of a
therapeutic alliance is impeded. Instead, the therapist who demon-
strates his support for the adolescent's interest in expanding his ego's
mastery of both his inner life and reality, and his efforts to develop a
sense of identity, can become a trusted ally. From this perspective,
"interpersonal" self-disclosing statements from the therapist help to
demonstrate to the adolescent that he or she is to be trusted.
We are taught that with adult patients all questions about the thera-
pist are grist for the mill. In contrast, with adolescents and children, one
is more likely to respond directly to questions that do not seem to be
related to dynamic issues. Thus, the therapist who answers an early
adolescent's question as to whether they (the therapist) had hobbies as a
teenager, is more likely to be trusted than the therapist who feels com-
pelled to "analyze" all such questions. Similarly, the therapist who di-
rectly and comfortably answers an adolescent patient's question about
his life, and then inquires about the nature of the adolescent's curiosity
in this area, is more likely to facilitate true self-examination in his ado-
lescent patient, than the therapist who carefully works at showing the
patient that his questions have dynamic significance.
Sheila, a 17-year-old black adolescent, had recently been discharged from the
hospital, and was living again in a foster home. Several months later, during
Psychotherapy with Children and Adolescents 169
one of her outpatient visits, as she talked with an air of casualness about her
relationship with her parents, she noticed that her therapist had tears in his
eyes. After summoning her courage, she asked the therapist why he seemed
to be crying. He replied simply that her description of her life had made him
sad. She then began to weep openly, and reported later that it was one of the
few times she had been able to cry about the fact that her mother had been in
a mental hospital most of her life, and that she rarely saw her alcoholic father.
From that time on, she called her therapist the "feeling doctor."
a love relationship he was having. His parents had told him that he should
forget about the relationship with this younger girl. However, he was unable
to do so and continued to pine for her. His therapist, instead of analyzing his
inability to do so, said simply. "In my day, the pain you are feeling over this
relationship was thought of as one of the painful introductions to adult-
hood." Jim relaxed noticeably and in subsequent sessions was able to begin
to talk about how difficult it had always been for him to end relationships
with people, no matter how problematic they were.
Summary
References
Adatto, c.P. (1966). On the metamorphosis from adolescence into adulthood. Journal of the
American Psychoanalytic Association, 14, 485-509.
Anthony, E.]. (1969). The reactions of adults to adolescents and their behavior. In Gerald
Caplan & Serge Lebovici (Eds.), Adolescence, psychological perspectives, New York: Basic
Books.
Arieti, S. (1974). Interpretation of schizophrenia. New York: Basic Books.
Psychotherapy with Children and Adolescents 173
Nilsson, D.E., Strassberg, D.S., & Bannon, J. (1979). Perception of counselor self-
disclosure: An analogue study. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 26(5):399-404.
Papouchis, N. (1982). Intimacy and the psychotherapy of adolescents. In Martin Fisher &
George Stricker (Eds.), Intimacy. New York: Plenum.
Pine, E (1985). Developmental theory and clinical theory. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Renik, 0. (1986). Countertransference in theory and practice. Journal of the American Psycho-
analytic Association, 34(3):699-708.
Ross, A.O. (1964). Techniques of therapy. In Mary R. Haworth, Ed., Child psychotherapy:
Practice and theory. New York: Basic Books.
Schafer, R. (1983). The analytic attitude. New York: Basic Books.
Springmann, R.R. (1986). Countertransference: Clarifications in supervision. Contempor-
ary Psychoanalysis, 22, 256-277.
Stone, 1. (1961). The psychoanalytic situation. New York: International Universities Press.
Sullivan, H.S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: w.w. Norton.
Truax, C.B., Carkhuff, R.R. (1965). Client and therapist transparency in the psycho-
therapeutic encounter. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 12, 3-8.
Weiner, I. (1970). Psychological disturbance in adolescence. New York: Wiley-Interscience.
Winnicott, D. W. (1965). The maturational processes and the facilitating environment. New York:
International Universities Press.
12
Self-Disclosure in Psychotherapy
WORKING WITH OLDER ADULTS
Lisa R. Greenberg
Introduction
A factory worker in her mid-60s recently came to me for treatment. She
showed many symptoms of agitated depression and described herself as
"always" having been extremely "nervous." She told me that it had
always been her strategy to try to "erase" her feelings. Ten minutes
into her first session, she hesitated in response to a question I had
asked about her daughter, and then said, "Well, there was an inci-
dent ... maybe subconsciously ... no, let's go on." When I encouraged
her to tell me what she was thinking, she reported an event that had
occurred 20 years earlier, during which she had failed to protect her
daughter as she thought proper. She was able to begin to see that her
attempts to push aside her guilt had not worked for her, and to consider
various ways in which her feelings about the incident had affected both
her relationship with her daughter and her own sense of herself. She left
the session somewhat teary but greatly relieved and committed to con-
tinuing therapy.
Like many other distressed older people, she had paid such a high
price for so long for trying to minimize her own experience that the
merest taste of a relationship in which her feelings were valued was
enough to lead her to reveal highly personal material in a helpful, posi-
tive way. Is this woman unusual? Do older people as a group self-
disclose easily and effectively, or hesitantly and with no clear results?
175
176 Lisa R. Greenberg
in the feeling that "I do what I feel like doing" after a lifetime of caring
for others, many others expressed the flip side of this and spoke elo-
quently of feelings of loneliness and isolation. Many widows spoke with
sadness of not having a spouse, or any relationship with a man, while
others specifically mentioned the pain of not feeling close to anybody.
Several felt that developing new friendships was harder in their old age
than it had been at other points in their lives. All these concerns seemed
to be expressions of deeply felt desires for intimate, confiding relation-
ships. While the lack of these relationships had not led any of these
women to become depressed or in any other way symptomatic, the
frequency and eloquence with which this issue was mentioned by wom-
en who were otherwise reluctant to acknowledge painful feelings is
indeed a strong statement, spoken by older people themselves, of the
felt need for confiding relationships in older peoples' lives.
Indications of the importance of relationships in which self-
disclosure plays an important part come from other sources as welt
including studies on the importance of reminiscence for the elderly.
Reminiscing, defined by McMahon and Rhudick (1961) as the "act or
habit of thinking about or relating past experiences, especially those
considered most personally significant" (p. 292), can be either an inter-
nal or an interpersonal process, but, when interpersonat seems likely
to be an act of self-disclosure. McMahon and Rhudick's finding, that
reminiscing serves many positive functions for the elderly, including
maintaining self-esteem, bolstering a sense of identity, and working
through loss, again argues for the value of self-disclosure for the elderly.
Lewis and Butler (1974) formalized the process of reminiscing in the
elderly with their "life-review therapy." They believe that, particularly
for people in their 60s, a process of "life-review" occurs, in which the
older person looks back over his or her life, reviews and resolves con-
flicts and comes to terms with both past and present. They believe that
life review can take many forms, but noted that it is greatly enhanced by
the presence of listeners, in which case it becomes a process of self-
disclosure, which the authors clearly believe has value.
As suggested, however, the notion that confiding relationships are
of value for the elderly is not universally held. Jerrome's (1981) stU9.y of
friendship and aging among middle-class British women is an example.
Her research was anthropological in nature, and consisted of depth
interviews and participant observation. Her 66 subjects ranged in age
from their late 50s to mid-80s. Most relevant to this work is her conclu-
sion that the main need in friendships among the elderly women she
studied seemed to be for companionship in a pleasurable activity. She
stated specifically that neither the exchange of help nor the exchange of
confidences seemed important in these friendships. The relationships
Working with Older Adults 179
she described appeared to be close and sustaining ones for the people
involved, and included much time spent together in a wide range of
mutually enjoyable activities, including shopping, gardening, card
playing, and, in particular, eating and drinking. Self-disclosure, how-
ever, simply did not appear to be an important component, at least as
observed and reported by Jerrome.
Andersson's (1985) work provides some support for Jerrome's con-
clusions. Working in Stockholm, he designed and studied an interven-
tion program for a group of elderly women who described themselves as
lonely. The intervention consisted of neighborhood groups with be-
tween three and five members who met four times, the first and last time
with a home-help assistant present. Each group meeting was a discus-
sion of a particular topic. Interviews held six months after the meetings
showed that participating subjects had less feelings of loneliness and
meaninglessness, more social contacts, higher self-esteem, greater ability
to trust, and lower blood pressure than before the program. There was,
however, no change regarding the availability of a close friend or confidant.
In considering his results, Andersson cited Jerrome's breakdown of
friendship into three levels of intimacy, the first being shared activity,
the second, reciprocal help, and the third, an exchange of confidences.
Referring to the work of Jerrome and others, he hypothesized that under
the normal circumstances of daily life, the optimal level of intimacy
appears to be the exchange of reciprocal help. The next level of intimacy,
the exchange of confidences, or self-disclosure, he proposed as promot-
ing adaptation only in times of social loss. Andersson, however, did not
consider that such losses do come along particularly frequently in the
lives of elderly people, and therefore, the presence of a confiding rela-
tionship could be viewed as protective for all older people. It should also
be noted that the findings of both Jerrome and Andersson seem to be in
some contrast with my own; the women who described to me feelings of
loneliness seemed clearly to be seeking, not companions with whom to
share particular activities or exchange help, but true friends with whom
to share themselves.
Emery and Lesher's (1982) paper on the treatment of depression in
the elderly is also germane. This work was identified as part of a series of
depression-treatment outcome studies at the University of Southern
California's Andrus Gerontology Centers, but appears to be anecdotal in
nature. This work supports the idea that self-disclosure is not univer-
sally of value to the eldefly, but may be helpful under certain conditions.
These authors came at the problem from another angle, claiming that the
value of self-disclosure for a particular individual depends not on that
person's circumstances at a given time, but on personality. Specifically,
they felt that the elderly can be divided into two personality modes,
180 Lisa R. Greenberg
dependent and autonomous, and that these modes influence the causes,
symptomatology, and appropriate treatment for a particular individual's
depression. They saw the dependent elderly as introspective and eager
to discuss feelings. For this group, despite a tendency to overdisclose,
self-disclosure in psychotherapy was generally helpful, in part because
it contributed to the formation of a closer bond with the therapist. On
the other hand, according to these authors, encouraging self-disclosure
in the treatment of typically underdisclosing, autonomous elderly de-
pressed patients can cause problems. Their concern seems to be that
self-disclosing will lead these fiercely independent people to conclude
that it is only the therapist who is capable of solving their problems and
that they themselves are helpless.
In summary, there is considerable evidence that relationships in
which self-disclosure plays a role are as beneficial for the elderly as for
the rest of us. Except for a small subgroup identified by Emery and
Lesher, no one has found any negative effects of self-disclosure, despite
the wide range in age, mental health status, and nationalities of the
elderly populations studied, as well as the widely differing research
methodologies used. Even those least enthusiastic about the value of
self-disclosure generally agree that while it may not be of interest or
value to a particular subgroup of the older population, such as Jerrome's
healthy, community-based female British sample, it probably has value
to other groups. When self-disclosure has been found to be of value, the
value has often been considerable. For instance, Lowenthal and Haven
argue for the importance of a confidant in protecting against social
losses, and Murphy's data show that a confiding relationship helps to
protect an elderly person from depression.
usefulness of therapy for anyone over age 45. Other factors, both psy-
chological and societal, are clearly involved as well. Tallmer (1982) cited
the glorification of independence in this age group, and the correspond-
ing notion of help-seeking as immature. Today's elderly are also not part
of a birth cohort in which seeing a therapist is easily understood by
peers or family. Pfeiffer (1976) also pointed to the importance of the
misconception that, in the elderly, a degree of depression, emotional
upset, and forgetfulness are to be expecte~, and therefore do not war-
rant treatment. These assumptions would clearly not be made if the
same symptoms were found in a younger person. I can recall a psychi-
atrist arguing that a hospitalized man was suffering, not from a major
depression critically in need of treatment, but from what he labeled "a
masked old-age thing," for which treatment of any type would be
useless.
Societal images of the elderly as entrenched in their ways and un-
able or unwilling to change also contribute to decreasing the likelihood
that an older person will be referred for therapy. Once a referral is made
and treatment is begun, societal pressures also seem to affect the length
and nature of the treatment offered, with a bias toward briefer and less
insight-oriented therapy for older patients. Lakin, Oppenheimer, and
Bremer (1982), for example, speaking specifically of group treatment,
advocated "an acceptant, encouraging, supportive and nonconfronta-
tional mode" (p. 452), including the "mild amplification of feelings," and
strong encouragement of sharing and comparing feelings and experi-
ences. They believe that most elderly people prefer to discuss concrete
issues, and they specifically discourage group leaders from focusing on
intra-individual or interpersonal struggles within the group. It is note-
worthy that, while the desire to focus on concrete issues would be
viewed as a resistance by most therapists working with younger people,
when seen in work with the elderly, it is accepted as a virtually immuta-
ble quality.
Other writers, including some who view themselves as advocates of
psychotherapy for the elderly, also promote supportive rather than in-
sight-oriented treatment strategies. Pfeiffer (1976), for example, dis-
cussed the need for "significant modifications in technique" in working
with older people. Among other changes, he endorses increased activity
in the treatment by the therapist, including "symbolic giving," and
"specific or limited goals" for the work. He views old age as associated
with loss, and saw the therapeutic relationship with an older patient as a
symbolic replacement for these losses. He believes in the value of a long-
term commitment to the patient but emphasized that this does not re-
quire frequent or prolonged contacts between patient and clinician, as-
sumed by Pfeiffer to be a physician. He mentioned, for example,
182 Lisa R. Greenberg
terms of loss. Instead, he cites those who view old age as a time of
transformation, which involves additions as well as losses, and his view
of therapy includes a belief in the value of helping an older patient to
explore and make use of what has been added. Cohen also cautioned
that, in focusing on the specifics of psychotherapy with the elderly,
there is a risk of neglecting much of what is known about psychotherapy
in general, and is equally applicable to work with all patients, regardless
of age. Related to this is Wylie and Wylie's (1987) contention, in one of
the few case studies to be found of an analysis of an older patient, that
the reluctance of an analyst to begin an analysis with an old patient is a
countertransference resistance.
Self-Disclosure by Therapist
Having briefly summarized the writings of therapists who advocate
supportive treatment for the elderly, and then those whose orientation
is more analytic, it is interesting to observe that the issue of the thera-
pist's self-disclosure arose only in the work of those, i.e., Pfeiffer and
Goodstein, who advocate more supportive treatments. Furthermore, as
alluded to earlier, both Pfeiffer and Goodstein seem to use self-
disclosure to foster and maintain an empathic connection between
patient and therapist, but not to deepen the therapeutic process and
promote insight. The therapist's role is not to assist the patient in deve-
loping attitudes and skills necessary to build a better life, but simply to
satisfy symbolically the patient's needs. This approach seems patroniz-
ing and is based on certain highly questionable assumptions.
Perhaps the primary assumption here is that "the elderly" can
usefully be viewed as a homogeneous population. It is no doubt true
that there are many elderly patients who, because of some combination
of physical disability, psychiatric disturbance, and cognitive impair-
ment, are truly unlikely to be able to form new relationships and to find
many sources of gratification in their lives. For these people, assuming
the therapist can determine who they are, using Pfeiffer's model of the
therapist as a permanent symbolic substitute for lost relationships is
probably valuable. Some support for this treatment strategy can be
found in Murphy's finding, earlier cited, that even relatively infrequent
contacts with a confidant protects against depression.
The danger, however, lies in believing that this treatment approach
is appropriate for all older patients. For most old people, the role of
therapist should be, as it is for patients of all ages, to foster growth and
development, and to help the patient to be able eventually to gratify his
or her needs outside the therapeutic relationship. Support for these
184 Lisa R. Greenberg
goals as realistic can be found from several sources. Murphy (1985), for
example, found her elderly subjects who ranged in age from 65 to 89, to
have a "remarkable capacity" to form new close relationships after their
depressive episodes had passed. Jerrome, too, found that most of her
older subjects were able to increase their circle of friends to the degree
they desired. If, in fact, most older people are able to form new positive,
intimate relationships, at least when they are not depressed, this argues
that the role of a therapist should be to aid older people in the resolution
of their difficulties, and not to supply them directly with intimate rela-
tionships. Perhaps it is most significant that clinical researchers have
not found a relationship between patient's age and therapy outcome
(Blum & Tallmer, 1977), indicating that major therapeutic modifications
may not be necessary for successful psychotherapy with the elderly.
This leads us to the question of whether self-disclosure by the thera-
pist has a role in insight-oriented treatment with the elderly. It seems to
me that, as with patients of other ages, self-disclosure by a therapist
working with an elderly patient mayor may not be helpful, mayor may
not be countertransferential, and mayor may not advance the process.
The factor that determines whether a particular self-disclosure by the
therapist is helpful seems to me to be largely independent of the pa-
tient's age.
Several incidents come to mind. One was of unintended by unavoid-
able self-disclosure, which had positive consequences. Some years ago,
I broke my foot and arrived at work in an inpatient psychogeriatric
setting with a cast up to my knee. To my surprise, a very depressed and
withdrawn man, after laughing (somewhat sadistically, it seemed to
me) at my feeble attempts to walk, was able to consider my ability to
continue to work and apparent good spirits. Previously, I had been seen
as young, healthy, and problem-free, and therefore, as unable to com-
prehend his pain and inability to function. My cast, however, impelled
him to ask some version of the question, "If you have that and are still
OK, why aren't I managing despite my problems?" He began to shift
from accepting his paralyzing depressions as inevitable to questioning
his own investment in not functioning.
Similarly, when I became engaged to marry, one of my patients in
both group and individual therapy was a psychotic woman in her early
70s, who had always been single. When I told her of my plans to marry,
she reacted with a bizarre tirade comprising attacks both on me for my
presumed abandonment of her and, even more vehemently, on herself
for having failed ever to marry. With the help of me and my coleader
and, perhaps most particularly of her elderly fellow group members,
however, she was able to work through some of these feelings. She came
away with at least a limited sense that there were many possible ways of
Working with Older Adults 185
living, and that it was by no means essential to have been married to feel
that one's life has been of value.
This anecdote also points to a relation between the value of self-
disclosure by the therapist and the age of the patient. Self-disclosure by
a young therapist, often of something important in his or her present
life, can at times serve to promote the patient's process of life review, as
described by Lewis and Butler and previously discussed. Any mention
at all of such events as marriage or pregnancy, for example, often trig-
gers a flood of memories of the patient's own experiences of these major
life events, and thereby provide an opportunity to work on any unre-
solved issues.
There is, however, also a caveat regarding a young therapist's self-
disclosures to an older patient. Therapists at times attempt to use self-
disclosure to communicate to patients that they understand and can
empathize with the patient's experience, when, in fact, their own life
may have been so different that their capacity for understanding and
empathy is not as great as they would like. Today's elderly have had life
experiences that differ dramatically from those of their therapists, par-
ticularly if their therapists are much younger. Often, therapy is occur-
ring between one person who has directly experienced immigration, the
Great Depression, and one, if not two, world wars. Setting aside issues
that are purely personaC these differences alone are important. Rather
than forcing an unreal connection by expressing too much empathy too
quickly, or sharing a life experience that is only mildly similar to that of
the patient, it is often more helpful for a young therapist to listen with
attention and respect, and to be open to being educated. My sense is
that comments that are intended by the therapist to build a connection
with the patient by pointing to similarities between them are often
instead felt as disappointing intrusions by the patients in that they re-
mind the patient that there is, after alC a wide gap between them.
This leaves open the question of what self-disclosing statements by
the therapist do and do not tend to be helpful. Perhaps most helpful are
those remarks that the therapist freely makes about his or her own life,
with no expectation of any particular response, while those that almost
urge the patient to experience a connection that mayor may not be real
can be instead distancing and hurtful.
Summary
This chapter has considered the issue of self-disclosure and the
elderly from several perspectives. Most of the literature on the value of
self-disclosing relationships for the elderly concludes that for the elderly,
as for people of other ages, confiding relationships are of value, particu-
larly in maintaining morale during periods of loss. This is a powerful
argument in favor of psychotherapy, a particularly intense and valuable
self-disclosing relationship, for older people. It is thus particularly un-
fortunate that old people tend to be underrepresented as therapy pa-
tients, and that the therapy that is done with this population tends often
to be more directive and supportive than is the case with younger pa-
tients. While most investigators only consider the role of the therapist'S
self-disclosure in therapy with the elderly in discussions of supportive
treatments, self-disclosure by the therapist can facilitate an insight-
oriented treatment as well. For example, the therapist's self-disclosure
can provide an opportunity for the patients to rework significant events
in their own lives.
Barriers to self-disclosure by elderly patients are often observed.
These include issues arising from the ways in which these patients were
socialized, and others based on transference-countertransference dy-
namics. These barriers are minimal for many patients but, even for
those for whom they are considerable, they are not obstacles that make
therapy impossible, but therapeutic challenges that must be worked
through as part of the valuable and gratifying work of treating older
people in psychotherapy.
References
Andersson, 1. (1985). Intervention against loneliness in a group of elderly women: An
impact evaluation. Social Science and Medicine, 20 (4), 355-364.
Blum, J., & Tallmer, M. (1977). The therapist vis-a.-vis the older patient. Psychotherapy:
Theory, Research and Practice, 14 (4), 361-367.
Cohen, G.D. (1981). Perspectives on psychotherapy with the elderly. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 138 (3), 347-350.
Emery, G., & Lesher, E. (1982). Treatment of depression in older adults: Personality consid-
erations. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 19 (4), 500-505.
Goodstein, R.K. (1982). Individual psychotherapy and the elderly. Psychotherapy: Theory,
Research and Practice, 19 (4), 412--418.
Greenberg, 1.R. (1982). Subjective experiences of elderly women as a function of employ-
ment history and experience of the past. Doctoral dissertation, Adelphi University.
Dissertation Abstracts International, 43 (03-B), 870-1054. (University Microfilms No.
AAD82-1900l).
Hatfield, E. (1982). Passionate love, compassionate love, and intimacy. In M. Fisher and G.
Stricker (Eds.), Intimacy. New York: Plenum.
Working with Older Adults 189
Jerrome, D. (1981). The significance of friendship for women in later life. Aging Society, 1,
175-197.
Lakin, M., Oppenheimer, B., & Bremer, J. (1982). A note on old and young in helping
groups. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 19 (4), 444-452.
Lewis, M.l., & Butler, R.N. (1974). Life-review therapy. Geriatrics, 29, 165-173.
Lowenthal, M.E, & Haven, C. (1968). Interaction and adaptation: Intimacy as a critical
variable. In B. Neugarten (Ed.), Middle age and aging. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
McMahon, A. w., & Rhudick, P.J. (1964). Reminiscing: Adaptational significance in the
aged. Archives of General Psychiatry, 10, 292-298.
Murphy E. (1982). Social origins of depression in old age. British Journal of Psychiatry, 141,
135-142.
Murphy, E. (1985). The impact of depression in old age on dose social relationships.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 142 (3), 323-327.
Pfeiffer, E. (1976). Psychotherapy with elderly patients. In L. Bellak & T. Karasu (Eds.),
Geriatric Psychiatry. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Tallmer, M. (1982). Intimacy issues and the older patient. In M. Fisher & G. Stricker (Eds.),
Intimacy. New York: Plenum.
Wylie, H.W., & Wylie, M.L. (1987). The older analysand: Countertransference issues in
psychoanalysis. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 68 (3), 343-352.
13
Self-Disclosure in Group
Psychotherapy
Sophia Vinogradov and Irvin D. Yalom
191
192 Sophia Vinogradov and Irvin D. Yalom
Too little self-disclosure can prevail when the entire group is blocked
and unable to share any personal material. There are many causes for
this situation: distrust of the group leader, fear that the group may not be
196 Sophia Vinogradov and Irvin D. Yalom
out early in the course of group therapy (Yalom, 1966, 1985). Although
patients should be encouraged to take risks in the group, to reveal them-
selves, and to obtain feedback and reinforcement for their behavior, they
may -if they reveal too much too soon - exceed their own tolerance and
that of the rest of the group. The member who has self-disclosed too
early may feel so much shame in the group that the interpersonal re-
wards seem insignificant or unsatisfactory. Furthermore, such individ-
uals may threaten other members who are willing to be accepting and
supportive, but who are not yet prepared to reciprocate with an equiva-
lent degree of self-disclosure. Consequently, they refuse to "join" with
the discloser, who may be placed in such a vulnerable and isolated
position that he or she often chooses to flee.
Evelyn was a faithful but largely silent member of a newly formed support
group for women engineering students. As the group began to become more
interactive, other members started turning to Evelyn and attempting to draw
her out. She steadfastly resisted these attempts, frustrating the group and
perplexing the leaders.
At the fourth meeting, during a rather general discussion on the subject
of intimacy, Evelyn suddenly revealed that she was having an extramarital
affair with her advisor, and that this was turning into an exploitative and
abusive relationship. The other group members, although sympathetic, were
stunned by this dramatic and unexpected revelation from a usually silent
and resistant individual.
During ensuing sessions, members found they were having a great deal
of difficulty offering Evelyn helpful feedback. In addition, all further at-
tempts at eliciting more material from her were useless; Evelyn "clammed
up" again and refused to share anything more with the group. Three weeks
later, she dropped out of the group without any forewarning.
more difficult to maintain their fictitious beliefs about the group leader.
By gradually revealing more of him or herself, by reacting to the patient
as a real person in the here and now, the therapist helps members to
confirm (or disconfirm) attitudes and abilities attributed to him or her
on the basis of unconscious fantasies. Second, the judicious use of thera-
pist transparency models interpersonal sharing and honesty for the rest
of the group. In this manner the therapist can reinforce norms of self-
expression and self-exploration, while modeling responsibility and
restraint.
A group leader noticed that, once again, Barbara, an unusually shy member,
had not spoken at all during the session. She had sat quietly with a pained
expression on her face, and now, towards the middle of the meeting, she was
glancing frequently at her watch. "You know; Barbara," the leader said. "I've
noticed you looking at your watch as if you're wondering when the meeting
will be over. I sense some pain in you and I'd like to bring you into the group
today-one of my roles is to help all members participate. Yet I feel caught in
a dilemma; I feel paternal towards you, like I want to rescue you, but I often
do too much of that and it feels like I'd just be infantilizing you. Also, there's
a part of me that doesn't want the obligation of being your rescuer."
therapist write a brief synopsis after each session and mail it to group
members, who can read the summary before the next session. Thera-
pists can use this format not only to comment on events in the group, but
to explain why they made the interventions they did, or how they
reached certain conclusions.
Another technique allows group members to watch post-group ses-
sions where the therapist, co-therapist, and any observers describe in
detail their interpretations of the group and their reasons for arriving at
those conclusions. Some rehash sessions even allow for a brief feedback
period from group members (see Yalom, 1983).
Concluding Remarks
Self-disclosure in group psychotherapy is an absolutely integral
part of the therapeutic process, and carries risks and gains not associ-
ated with self-disclosure in individual therapy. It occurs when members
reveal personal material about their outside lives and, even more impor-
tantly, when they explore their feelings and reactions to each other in the
here and now of the group session. Appropriate and adaptive self-dis-
closure in the group is a complex function of timing, content, and reci-
procity. Therapists playa more active and self-revealing role in this
process than they do in individual psychotherapy, as they model respon-
sible personal and professional transparency in the group.
References
Bloch, S., & Reibstein, J. (1980). Perceptions by patients and therapists of therapeutic
factors in group psychotherapy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 137:274-278.
Bloch, S., Crouch, E., & Reibstein, J. (1981). Therapeutic factors in group psychotherapy.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 38:519-526.
Cohen, M., & Dies, R. (1976). Content considerations in group therapist self-disclosure.
International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 23:71-88.
Johnson, D., & Ridener, 1. (1974). Self-disclosure, participation, and perceived cohesive-
ness in small group interaction. Psychological Reports, 35:361-363.
Jourard, S.M., & Lasakow, P. (1950). Some factors in self-disclosure. Journal of Abnormal
Social Psychology, 56:91-98.
Lieberman, M., Yalom, Y.D., & Miles, M. (1973). Encounter groups: First facts. New York:
Basic Books.
Peres, H. (1947). An investigation of non-directive group therapy. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 11:159-172.
Query, W. (1964). Self-disclosure as a variable in group psychotherapy. International Journal
of Group Psychotherapy, 14:107-115.
Truax, c., & Carkhuff, R. (1965). Client and therapist transparency in the psychotherapeu-
tic encounter. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 12:3-9.
204 Sophia Vinogradov and Irvin D. Yalom
Worthy, M., Gary, A., & Kahn, G. (1969). Self-disclosure as an exchange process. Journal of
Personality Social Psychology, 13:59-63.
Yalom, 1.0. (1966). A study of group therapy dropouts. Archives of General Psychiatry,
14:393-414.
Yalom, 1.0. (1967). Prediction of improvement in group therapy: An exploratory study.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 17:159-168.
Yalom, 1.0. (1985). The theory and practice of group psychotherapy. (3rd ed.). New York: Basic
Books.
v
Extratherapeutic Manifestations
14
Criteria for Therapist Self-Disclosure
Judith C. Simon
Introduction
The psychotherapeutic relationship is unique to interpersonal relation-
ships. The role definition includes agreement that one person, the pa-
tient, will openly discuss his or her personal life, while the therapist will
function in a manner that will further the patient's psychotherapeutic
gains. The hours spent together generate a special closeness and inten-
sity, discussing primarily the patient's emotional life.
Inherent in any ongoing intimate relationship is each person's learning
about the other. Implicit to the goal of a psychotherapeutic relationship,
however, is a one-way intimacy in which the patient is the primary self-
discloser. Discussion of the therapist's personal life is not necessarily
part of the relationship. However, the therapist cannot avoid imparting
some personal information; for example, the way the office is decorated,
personal dress, the management of appointments, and nonverbal body
language all give the patient clues about the therapist.
Since therapists' personal revelations are frequently a component
of psychotherapy, questions arise regarding when, why, and what is
disclosed.
When Freud first discussed transference and, later, counter-
transference, he acknowledged the impact of the therapist'S personality
and responses on the psychotherapeutic work (1959). Therapeutic neu-
trality was the goal. Therapist self-disclosure came to be viewed as the
antithesis of the detached observer. It is important to distinguish be-
tween neutrality as a therapeutic stance and therapist self-disclosure as
Judith C. Simon • 329 South San Antonio Road, Los Altos, California 94022.
207
208 Judith C. Simon
The Study'
I explored criteria for intentional verbal self-disclosure by experi-
enced therapists who practice long-term psychotherapy. "Intentional
self-disclosure" was defined as verbal behavior through which thera-
pists consciously and purposefully communicate ordinarily private in-
formation about themselves to their patients.
Eight experienced therapists were interviewed. "Experienced" was
defined as having 10 or more years of private clinical work. These eight
were selected by ranking 27 returned questionnaires on a scale from
high to low disclosing and choosing those four who fell at each of these
Criteria for Therapist Self-Disclosure 209
Findings
Three themes, theoretical orientation, the psychotherapy relation-
ship, and therapist self-awareness, emerged from the interviews and
provide the context for the discussion of the criteria for therapist self-
disclosure.
Theoretical Orientation
answer their questions too quickly, it's more valuable to help them to explore.
"Deprivation" is a bad word, but that's the one Freud used. It conveys harsh-
ness. I don't mean that.
Therapist Self-Awareness
Criteria
All eight subjects self-disclosed at times and utilized the same crite-
ria for self-disclosure. The differences emerged in the therapists' indi-
Criteria for Therapist Self-Disclosure 213
Modeling
Modeling emerges as the most common criteria for therapist self-
disclosure. Therapists reported that they often served as models of adult
behavior by demonstrating problem-solving approaches, coping skills,
self-acceptance, and assertiveness. All of the subjects said that they
used self-disclosure to model more frequently with adolescent patients
than with adults because of the adolescent's need for help with the
developmental task of becoming more autonomous. Identification with
the therapist was viewed as helping patients face life and was encour-
aged by the therapist's appropriate self-disclosures.
Both the high and low disclosers felt that handling an error made
with a patient presents therapists with an important opportunity to
model the ability to err and the capacity to apologize.
The high disclosers felt modeling was an essential component of
psychotherapy and that by showing their patients their own honesty
and openness they were modeling relationships in general. One subject
wanted to model someone who was transparent and who could demon-
strate a way to live without fear or secrets, a stated psychotherapy goal
214 Judith C. Simon
of his. They believed that good therapists are models and in so being,
communicate to their patients, "You can do these things, too."
These therapists freely disclosed personal information to illustrate
behavior in specific situations in addition to modeling openness in gen-
eral as essential to a functioning relationship.
Personal information was readily offered, especially to adolescent
patients, and these therapists viewed this as re-parenting and/or educa-
tion about the adult world. Personal experiences of their teen years
which paralleled their patients' were freely disclosed. Adolescents'
questions about the therapist's experiences in the adult world were also
responded to fully. Examples cited were career evolution, earning ca-
pacity, and marital problems.
I explored use of therapist self-disclosure when there are stressful
circumstances in the therapist's life. All high disclosers felt that it was
very important to fully disclose information about these kinds of crises,
primarily to model a way of coping. "Teach them how to deal with it,
including what you did that helped and what you did that didn't help."
In the same vein, two of these subjects recommended revelation of infor-
mation about any personal crisis or significant occurrence. "I might
even reveal that I fight with my wife. People don't often know that it's
okay to get angry at someone close."
Fewer examples of modeling were presented by the low disclosers,
although modeling was also their primary criteria for self-revelation.
While they rarely self-disclose in order to model with neurotic adult
patients, modeling with adolescent patients was an appropriate devia-
tion from their usual nondisclosing stance. For example, "Some of these
kids haven't had decent adults to look up to. If I can provide that, I'm
not going to withhold because I'm a Freudian." They freely told stories
about their own adolescence to offer themselves for identification.
One therapist said that he sometimes shared a personal experience
that showed his bad judgment to model that "even the idealized thera-
pist is not a perfect person."
All of these subjects have shared their own reactions to difficult
personal situations in order to indicate that their patients' intense feelings
were appropriate. Examples included their emotions in responding to
the death of a parent and the frustrations and fatigue of early parenting.
Three felt that they would model with more disturbed patients to
show them ways of coping with specific situations. The emphasis was
on specific. These therapists were inclined to share emotions with this
population to demonstrate that emotional responses were, indeed,
sometimes appropriate. A common example was "That would make me
angry too." They did not present themselves as overall objects of identi-
fication. Encouraging imitation in social situations, for example, dining
Criteria for Therapist Self-Disclosure 215
out, and interactions with co-workers, were also uses of therapist self-
disclosure with the purpose of modeling.
Two of these therapists felt that universalizing was a successful tool
to reduce some insecurities, and that they included themselves as
models in this respect. "Sometimes we're just at the mercy of the powers
that be" was a common phrase.
Validating Reality
you're going to have to violate the contract, the frame, involuntarily, and
that's different from a patient wanting to have information." A patient
failing to ask about an obvious injury to the therapist could be viewed as
an avoidance of reality. These subjects felt that they would reveal some
information in this situation to push the patient to confront reality.
Therapist Satisfaction
While all the subjects reported satisfaction from their work, the fulfill-
ment derived from practicing psychotherapy was different for the thera-
pists in the two groups.
The high disclosers were very clear about the gratification they
obtained from their patients. They chose orientations that support thera-
pist openness, and it is the mutuality of openness in the psycho-
therapeutic relationship that provides much of their professional
satisfaction. They acknowledged their own enjoyment of their friendly
relationships with patients and their belief that therapist and patient
both grow in the relationship. Therefore, their conceptualization of crite-
ria for therapist self-disclosure was casual and relatively unstructured.
Two of these therapists said that they would pursue discussions
with patients about most topics of interest to the therapist. For example,
they agreed that they would elicit information about a patient's vacation
if the destination was of personal interest.
The low disclosers also reported satisfaction from the psycho-
therapy relationship. However, therapist satisfaction was not a criterion
for self-disclosure. For them, it was the psychotherapy process and rela-
tionship, including intellectual stimulation, that provided fulfillment.
Discussion
The original research question, "What are the factors in therapist
self-disclosure?" can be answered concisely. The main factor is therapist
theoretical orientation. At the same time, it is clear that this concise
answer, while accurate, is inadequate. Numerous other factors impact
therapist disclosure. Theoretical orientation, the psychotherapeutic
frame, the psychotherapeutic relationship, the therapist's personality,
and therapist self-awareness are all themes associated with therapist
self-disclosure.
The four high-disclosing therapists defined their orientations simi-
larly: they agreed on loose boundaries between themselves and their
patients; they opposed therapeutic neutrality; they espoused equality;
and they were active participants in the interchange between them-
selves and their patients. The four low-disclosing therapists also defined
their orientations consistently: they agreed about defined boundaries;
they valued neutrality; and they believed in being fairly inactive in
interactions with their patients to encourage transference.
Attitudes about transference both exemplified and were crucial to
the differences between the two subject groups. Therapists who believe
that transference work is the crux of the psychotherapy process self-
Criteria for Therapist Self-Disclosure 221
colorful personalities who were "out front" with their likes and personal
philosophies. The interviews were entertaining, lively, and lengthy. In
contrast, the low disclosers' interviews were briefer and more focused.
They were more formal and restrained (neutral?) in their manner. These
interviews were less lively and more intellectually provocative.
Offices were decorated in a manner consistent with orientations.
For example, one high discloser proudly displayed family photographs.
Another had numerous art objects reflecting an ethnic interest. These
therapists had more desire to share themselves and did so physically
as well as verbally. The low disclosers' offices were austere by compari-
son. Colors were more muted and the displayed art less reflective of
personal taste.
One can question which office style is more appropriate and/or
supportive of psychotherapeutic work. Clinicians and patients must feel
comfortable and supported in the therapist's office. The therapist's at-
tention to the physical environment is important, whether it be a clear
reflection of the therapist as a person or an aspect to support the thera-
pist's professional stance. Sensitivity to one's patient population was
considered. The therapists' personal dress also reflects the therapist as a
person and, in this study, their attire was consistent with their orienta-
tion and office decor.
Therapist self-awareness was assessed by the questioning of per-
sonal psychotherapy experience. There appeared to be a relationship
between valuing self-awareness and working with transference. When
working within a neutral frame and encouraging patients' transference
projections, knowing oneself is important. The high disclosures did not
regard self-awareness as being related to their professional functioning.
The fact that all the subjects utilized similar criteria for therapist
self-disclosure and that all the subjects did self-disclose leads to the
conclusion that self-disclosure is a fact in most psychotherapy. The sim-
ilarity in the purposes of therapist self-disclosure expressed by all the
subjects blurs some of the striking differences between the two groups.
The high disclosers did not think through their processes in making
disclosures because they valued overall openness. They were not con-
cerned about being too revealing since they considered self-revelation
an integral component of the therapy process as well as their personal
styles. For the low disclosers, intentional self-disclosure demanded
thought. It was this discriminative thinking process in contrast to readi-
ness to self-disclose that differentiated the two subject groups.
Several further issues emerged from this study. Diagnosis as a facet
of therapist self-disclosure was explored. All the subjects gave less at-
tention to it than to the other criteria, which leads to the tentative con-
clusion that diagnosis is a minor criterion.
Criteria for Therapist Self-Disclosure 223
References
Abend, S.M. (1982). Serious illness in the analyst: Countertransference considerations.
Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 30 (2), 365-379.
Alger, I. (1973). Freedom in analytic therapy. Current Psychiatric Therapy, 9, 73-78.
Dewald, P.A. (1982). Serious illness in the analyst: Transference, countertransference, and
reality responses. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 30 (2), 347-363.
Flaherty, J.A. (1984). Self-disclosure in therapy: Marriage of the therapist. International
Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 34 (2), 442-452.
Freud, S. (1959). Recommendations to physicians practising psychoanalysis. In E. Jones
(Ed.), Collected papers, Vol. II. New York: Basic Books.
Goldberg, R (1984). Personal observations of a therapist with a life-threatening illness.
International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 34 (2), 389-396.
Greenson, R.R. (1978). The real relationship between the therapist and the psychoanalyst.
In R.R. Greenson (Ed.), Explorations in Psychoanalysis, New York: International Uni-
versities Press.
Palombo, J. (1987). Spontaneous self-disclosure in psychotherapy. Clinical Social Work Jour-
nal, 15 (2), 107-120.
Reich, A. (1951). On countertransference. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 32, 25-31.
Rogers, C.B. (1961). The concept of a fully functioning person. In C.B. Rogers (Ed.), On
becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Rosie, J.S. (1980). The therapist's self-disclosure in individual psychotherapy: Research
and psychoanalytic theory. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 25, 469-472.
Criteria for Therapist Self-Disclosure 225
Simonson, N.R., & Bahr, S. (1974). Self-disclosure by the professional and paraprofession-
al therapist. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 359-363.
Truax, C.B., & Mitchell, K.M. (1971). Research in certain interpersonal skills in relation to
process and outcome. In A.E. Bergin & S.L. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy
and behavior change: An empirical analysis. New York: John Wiley.
Weiner, M. (1972). Self-exposure by the therapist as a therapeutic technique. American
Journal of Psychotherapy, 31, 42-51.
Weiner, M. (1974). Personal Openness with Patients: Help or hindrance. Texas Medicine, 76,
60-65.
Weiner, M. (1983). Therapist disclosure. Baltimore: University Park Press.
Yalom, 1.0. (1980). Existential psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books.
15
Self-Disclosure in Holocaust Survivors
EFFECTS ON THE NEXT GENERATION
Introduction
The Holocaust was woven into the fabric of life.
It was always there. The natural tapestry of my life. It was my first memory.
There was never a time in my life where there wasn't an Auschwitz.
It was very much a part of my life. Growing up I wondered what it was like
not to be a child of a survivor.
It was always a way of splitting time. Before the war and after the war.
Arlene Cahn Gordon. 15 Dogwood Drive, West Orange, New Jersey 07052.
227
228 Arlene Cahn Gordon
the symbol" from external sources. Danieli (1982) states that survivors
encountered negative reactions and attitudes thatled to a "conspiracy of
silence" about the Holocaust. The survivors' experiences were too horri-
fying for most people to listen to. Even those who consciously wanted
to listen, avoided asking questions, which they rationalized by a belief
that they didn't want to add to the survivors' pain. Danieli discusses the
consequences of this conspiracy of silence for survivor families. Feeling
betrayed by the inability of their relatives to share their sense of loss,
grief, and rage, survivors felt even more isolated. Some survivors had
sustained the horrors of the war in order to bear witness: the imposed
silence was particularly painful for them. The importance of self-
disclosure, of intimate sharing and acknowledgment of the past, was
evident in many survivors' reactions to this barrier. Some families
sought out and clung to fellow survivors, establishing groups, organiza-
tions, or communities based on common experiences or residence before
the war. Some survivors withdrew into their current families and used
their children as their constant, captive audiences.
Many authors have suggested that survivors' experiences interfered
with their ability for "good enough" parenting (Barocas & Barocas,
1980). Survivors have been described as emotionally unavailable, over-
protective, and using their children for their own conscious and uncon-
scious needs (Barocas & Barocas, 1973; Freyberg, 1980; Phillips, 1978;
Sigal & Rakoff, 1971). Rakoff, Sigal, and Epstein (1967) discuss how
children are expected to give meaning to their parents' lives and to
restore the lost objects, goals, and ideals of their parents. The children
are expected to become extensions of their parents, as well as symbols of
all that their parents lacked in their own lives. Many of the problems
described in children of survivors have been related to their parents'
inability to allow their children to assume individual identities.
Impairment of symbolizing processes in survivors may be related to
their parenting difficulties. Freyberg (1980) states that mothers were
frequently bewildered with their role, having lost access to their intro-
jected prototype of a good mother. Sonnenberg (1972) comments that
the ego functions necessary for parenting are lost to some extent in
every survivor. I suggest that these parenting difficulties are related to
impairments in parents' overall ability to symbolize. Those survivors
who did not maintain internalized good objects, i.e., whose capacity to
symbolize was impaired, most likely had more difficulty as parents
themselves.
The purpose of this brief outline of the role of the self-disclosure at
subsequent stages of survival is twofold. First, it is to suggest the critical'
role of acknowledgment of experience in maintaining a sense of related-
ness with oneself and one's world. Second, and directly following from
232 Arlene Cahn Gordon
discharge. The more depressed an individual, the less able to link expe-
rience to word. Secondly, Jaffe (1985) related mothers' referential activity
to their speech interaction with their children, which was related to the
children's separation tolerance. These studies are significant because
they relate the ability to acknowledge experience, referential compe-
tence, to two of the major difficulties discussed in relation to both sur-
vivors and children of survivors, namely depression and separation
individuation problems.
Given the results of the referential activity studies described above,
consider the following: (1) Depression evident in survivors is related to
their difficulties communicating their war experiences. (2) In children of
survivors, depression may be related to their inability to "name" their
own experience and that of their parents. (3) The prevalence of diffi-
culties with separation in children of survivors may also be associated
with difficulties in their capacity to form symbols. While depression and
separation have been studied extensively with this population, the me-
diating variable, symbol formation, has not been addressed.
In this excerpt the language is clear and rich in descriptive detail. Con-
crete examples of specific actions and feelings bring the event to life. In
contrast is a sample from a low-referential activity monologue:
Urn, let's see. Giving birth. (Chuckles.) That was a real exciting thing. That
was a traumatic experience. Urn, never expecting to go natural I went natu-
ral. That really is like number one in life experiences. Urn, you know I had a
wonderful doctor and my husband was very supportive. Urn, everything
really went smoothly even though I wanted to get uh, epidural needle and
everything, urn I was able to do it without anything. And it's been great, you
know.
Self-Disclosure in Holocaust Survivors 237
My mother was able to talk freely about life after the war, but she did not
want what happened to her during the war to intrude. She never verbalized
it. It was too painful. She still denies her background has had an effect on her.
It's become pivotal to me. Not that I went seeking it. It started to intrude in
my consciousness.
This woman knew how to regulate what she heard even though she felt
it was too much at times. The important factor is that the information
was accessible, if she wanted it.
In the following quote, the central thesis of this study is clearly
articulated by a child of survivors herself:
She doesn't understand why the war affects me. She was in the camps. I said
'Mom, I've heard about the war since I was a baby.' She thinks it's separate.
She doesn't understand how much little words accumulate and are your life
experience.
connections to her past, her child in some sense "inherits" such diffuse
fears. Thus, while connections to the past pervade all current reality, the
ability to label these connections determines the extent to which the
past is contained or perpetuated.
While this process of discrimination and labeling is necessary in
every parent-child unit, I suggest that survivors have an additional
symbolic step to perform. They must in a sense relearn what is safe in
the external world and symbolize the distinction effectively, in order to
communicate to their child a sense of the world as safe. Other individ-
uals do this, to a large extent, once only as they are growing up. While
particular events may be emotionally loaded and signify aspects of their
own past that were not considered safe, for survivors it was their entire
universe.
A possible implication of this finding is that the more able an indi-
vidual is to acknowledge her experience, the less likely it is to be acted
out or to cause adjustment difficulties such as depression or separation-
individuation problems. The testing out of this inference would be a
logical next step for research.
What are the more universal implications of the relationships found
in this study? I'd like to return to the significant relationship found in all
participants between referential activity based on the second mono-
logue and number of facts known about mothers' difficult experiences.
It may not be surprising that the more specific facts participants knew
about their mothers' terrible experiences the more specific and clear they
could describe the event. This finding might be explained simply as a
function of memory; the more facts children heard, the more facts they
can tell. The relationship between knowledge of facts and the expres-
siveness and evocativeness of their description is more difficult to ex-
plain in such direct terms. Based on what is known about referential
activity, what appears to be measured in this case is what these women
have internalized and symbolized of their mothers' experiences in verbal
and nonverbal representations of their own.
Consider the following excerpt from a child of survivors' monologue
about her mothers' experiences symbolized in a dream which her
mother had told her:
One day, one night I should say, she had a dream. And she dreamt that she
was back in Tarnigov; which was a very happy place for her. You know, she
loved that town. And she dreamt that, um, she walked into the center of the
town, and she saw her father. And she ran to him and she said, "Papa, Papa,
aren't you happy to see me?" And he said, "Yes, yes an-butIwanna tell you
that the, you know, the linens are here and the silver is there, and this, these
valuables I put here for you." And then he started walking away. And she
said, "Where are you going? Aren't you-I just got home. Aren't you happy
to see me?" And he said, "Yes, but you must listen very carefully." And he
242 Arlene Cahn Gordon
just went on and on and on. And then he went into a house and started
walking up the stairs. And she said, "Where are you going?" And she ran,
tried to go up the stairs behind him. And he said, "No, no, no, you can't come
with me now." And he went up the stairs and disappeared. And she said that
she woke up screaming. That she was so frightened by this dream that she
couldn't even tell anybody about it. ..
This woman with high referential activity describes her mother's
dream with a sense of immediacy because, in a sense, the experience is
"alive" within her. It may not even be an accurate summary of what she
was told. It is now her memory of her mother's experiences in multi-
layered symbolic form.
Of importance for this discussion is that referential-activity scores
from these monologues about parent experiences were correlated with
specific facts known in all participants. While the correlation was greater
for children of survivors, the important finding with this monologue
was that there was a significant relationship between referential activity
and mothers' communication factors in both groups. By this relation-
ship, the study recognizes a more general phenomenon, that is so clear
in the survivor population-and that is that parental acknowledgment
of one's own experience is related to one's children's symbolic ability.
Confirmation of the generalizability of this study will come through
further research. This study has implications for children of survivors of
other large-scale, man-made disasters as well as more general human
interaction. Lifton (1968) has compared personality traits in children of
Hiroshima survivors to children of Holocaust survivors. The transmis-
sion of symbolic impairment may also be evident in such devastating
disasters. In addition, this research may have implications for children
whose parents have experienced physical or psychological trauma, ill-
ness, or loss of various types. The development of groups of children of
alcoholics and children of the chronically ill, like children of survivor
groups, suggest the general importance, for one's own health, of label-
ing parental trauma. Eventually, with more sensitive instruments, it
may be possible to identify the symbolic transmission processes of more
universal life-crisis issues on children's symbolic activity. This study did
not tap the process of acknowledgment of specific traumas and the con-
sequences of a failure to do so. While I suggest that this process is in
many ways unique in children of Holocaust survivors, there is also
overlap with other personal traumas and cultural disasters, and this
needs to be studied.
Therapeutic Implications
What are the implications of these findings for the treatment of
Holocaust survivors and their children? Children of survivors need to
Self-Disclosure in Holocaust Survivors 243
Conclusion
Self-disclosure occurs in a social context; it is fundamental to the
experience of relatedness with others and ultimately with oneself. This
study began with a theoretically based assumption, that one's ability to
acknowledge experience is acquired in a relationship with one's parents.
It concludes, based on empirical data, that a child's ability to acknowl-
edge experience is affected by the parents' ability to do so. I am thus
suggesting a powerful intergenerational connection between self-dis-
closure and relatedness. This connection may account for a continuous
ripple effect of traumatic experiences on subsequent generations. It is
also the connection that allows for the eventual containment of the ef-
fects of traumatic events. Self-disclosure within a relationship, be it with
parent, child, friend, or therapist, allows one to extend and enhance the
fabric of life woven by previous generations.
References
Barocas, H., & Barocas, C. (1973). Manifestations of concentration camp effects on the
second generation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 820-821.
Barocas, H., & Barocas, C. (1980). Separation-individuation conflicts in children of Holo-
caust survivors. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 11(1), 6-14.
Bettelheim, B. (1960). The informed heart. New York: Avon Books.
244 Arlene Cahn Gordon
Bruner, J. (1983). Child's talk: Learning to use language. New York: w.w. Norton.
Bucci, W. (1982). The vocalization of painful affect. Journal of Communication Disorders, 15,
415-440.
Bucci, W. (1984). Linking words and things: Basic process and individual variation. Cogni-
tion, 17, 137-153.
Bucci, w., & Freedman, N. (1978). Language and hand: The dimension of referential
competence. Journal of Personality, 46, 594-622.
Bucci, W., & Freedman, N. (1981). The language of depression. Bulletin of the Menninger
Clinic, 45(4), 334-358.
Cahn, A. (1987). The capacity to acknowledge experience in Holocaust survivors and their chil-
dren. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Adelphi University.
Chodoff, P. (1963). Late effects on the concentration camp syndrome. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 8, 323-333.
Danieli, Y. (1982). Families of survivors of the Nazi Holocaust: Some short- and long-term
effects. Stress and Anxiety, 8, 405-42l.
Eagle, M.N. (1984). Object-seeking and object relations. In Recent developments in psycho-
analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Eitinger, L. (1961). Pathology of the concentration camp syndrome. Archives of General
Psychiatry,S, 371-379.
Erikson, E. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
Frankl, Y.E. (1959). Man's search for meaning. Boston: Beacon Press.
Freud, S. (1951). Mourning and Melancholia. In J. Strachey (Ed. and tran.), The standard
edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud. London: Hogarth Press.
(Originally published, 1917.)
Freyberg, J. (1980). Difficulties in separation-individuation as experienced by offspring of
Nazi Holocaust survivors. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 50(1).
Hjelle, L., & Ziegler, D. (1976). Personality theories: Basic assumptions, research and applica-
tions. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Jaffe, L.E. (1985). Maternal symbolic and psycholinguistic behaviors and the child's evolving
capacity to tolerate separation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Adelphi University.
Kaye, K. (1982). The mental and social life of babies: How parents create persons. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Kestenberg, J. (1972). Psychoanalytic contributions to the problem of children of survivors
from Nazi persecutions. The Israel Annals of Psychiatric and Related Disciplines, 10,
311-325.
Klein, H. (1971). Families of Holocaust survivors in the kibbutz. Psychological Studies,
67-92.
Krystal, H. (1968). Massive psychic trauma. New York: International Universities Press.
Lifton, R.J. (1968). Observations on Hiroshima survivors. In H. Krystal (Ed.), Massive
psychic trauma. New York: International Universities Press.
Niederland, W.G. (1968). An interpretation of the psychological stresses and defenses in
concentration camp life and the late after-effects. In H. Krystal (Ed.), Massive psychic
trauma. New York: International Universities Press.
Phillips, R.E. (1978). Impact of Nazi Holocaust on children of survivors. American Journal of
Psychotherapy, 32(3), 370-378.
Pilcz, M. (1979). Understanding the survivor family. In L. Steinitz & D. Szonyi, Living after
the Holocaust: Reflections by children of survivors in America. New York: Bloch Publishing
Company.
Prince, R. (1985). Knowing the holocaust. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 5(1), 51-6l.
Rakoff, Y., Sigal, J., Epstein, N. (1967). Children and families of concentration camp sur-
vivors. Canada's Mental Health, 14, 24-26.
Rapaport, D. (1958). The theory of ego autonomy: A generalization. Bulletin of the Men-
ninger Clinic, 22, 13-35.
Self-Disclosure in Holocaust Survivors 245
Sigal, J" & Rakoff, V. (1971). Concentration camp survival: A pilot study of effects on the
second generation. Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal, 16, 393-397.
Sonnenberg, S. (1972). Workshop report: Children of survivors. Journal of the American
Psychoanalytic Association, 22, 200-203.
Vygotsky, 1.5. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge: M.LT. Press.
16
From Secrecy to Self-Disclosure
HEALING THE SCARS OF INCEST
Mary Gail Frawley. Pomona Clinic, Robert L. Yeager Health Complex, Pomona, New
York 10970. The author thanks Michelle Collins, Michael O'Toole, and Ann Kuehner for
their review of this manuscript and for their valuable editorial suggestions.
247
248 Mary Gail Frawley
Despite the turbulence, and even tragedy, that may follow a sur-
vivor's initial disclosure of her incest, the opportunity to reveal the
secret and to discuss her abusive experiences with another more usually
signals the beginning of recovery from the incest trauma for many for-
mer victims (Brunngraber, 1986; Courtois, 1988; Herman, 1981; West-
erlund, 1983).
Since, by definition, women who have not disclosed their incest
secret are unavailable to researchers, it is difficult empirically to validate
the importance of disclosure. In the author's study (Frawley, 1988), how-
ever, a significant correlation was found between the age at which a
woman first disclosed her incest and several measures of adult sexual
functioning, with disclosure later in life related to more frequent or
more severe sexual dysfunction. Significance was achieved at least at the
p<.05Ievel. These results are consistent with Brunngraber's (1986) find-
ings, generated in a study of 21 adult incest survivors, that disclosing
their incest experiences to others was considered by the women to be
positively related to overall adjustment to their past victimization. Sim-
ilarly, Courtois (1980) found that all 30 adult survivors of childhood
sexual abuse that she interviewed deemed participation in her research
helpful. They specifically cited the importance of the catharsis involved
in discussing their experiences with a nonjudgmental other. These find-
ings indeed suggest that an incest survivor's self-disclosure is crucial to
reopening intra- and interpersonal channels to intimate relatedness.
Beyond findings like these, it is in clinical work with incest survivors
that the central role of disclosure in revitalizing a capacity for intimacy is
most evident.
Various books and articles discuss individual and group treatment
strategies for wmk with incest survivors (Blake-White & Kline, 1985;
Courtois, 1988; Faria & Belohavek, 1984; Forward & Buck, 1978; Gordy,
1983; Herman, 1981; McGuire & Wagner, 1978). While, to the author's
knowledge, there have been no systematically conducted outcome
studies on the retrospective treatment of father-daughter incest, a num-
ber of writers concur that many of the long-term negative sequelae of
incest are amenable to treatment through psychotherapy.
In the author's own clinical experience, several incest survivors have
demonstrated marked improvement in their capacity for intimate re-
latedness subsequent to disclosing their incest histories. Angelina, for
instance, is a 40-year-old woman who had not disclosed her incest secret
until beginning psychotherapy for anxiety and depression following the
end of her 16-year marriage. During that marriage, Angelina experi-
enced chronic vaginismus-involuntary contraction of the vaginal mus-
cles that prevents penetration. It was also clear that the lack of sexual
intimacy between Angelina and her husband paralleled an emotional
Self-Disclosure among Incest Survivors 255
tive episodes, combined with other aspects of her history and symp-
toms, strongly suggested that this woman was a father-daughter incest
survivor who had repressed the memories of her abuse.
After about six months of therapy during which the author would
occasionally question Betsey about possible childhood sexual experi-
ences, Betsey had a dissociative experience at home with her boyfriend
during which she reenacted her father's pushing her to her knees to
perform oral sex on him. During this episode, she revealed to her boy-
friend that her father had threatened to cut her arms off if she ever told
about the abuse. The self-mutilation was clearly an enactment of the
threatened punishment and was evoked as her memories of the abuse
neared consciousness through the therapy.
Since Betsey reenacted her abuse, she has gradually recovered more
memories and can now tolerate remembering without dissociating.
There has been no self-mutilation since that time, and Betsey now says,
"I wouldn't even want to cut myself. It never hurt before but I know it
would hurt me to do it now." While she still has much work to do in
therapy, Betsey has begun to develop a more compassionate attitude
toward herself and she is beginning to repair the relationships with her
children. Within the therapy, Betsey's eye contact has markedly in-
creased and there is an emerging interest in the therapist as a person
rather than as someone who is simply fulfilling a professional role.
Betsey was once unable to walk down the hall with the author to her
office because it made her "too nervous" to be that close to and some-
what out of role with the therapist. Recently, however, she loped down
the hall alongside the author chatting animatedly about a newspaper
article she had read that day. It is quite apparent that, in Betsey's case,
regaining the memories of her childhood incest and discussing them
with another human being has improved this woman's capacity for inti-
mately knowing and relating to herself and others.
Many clinicians who treat incest survivors recommend group ther-
apy as a primary or important ancillary treatment modality (Courtois,
1988; Gordy, 1983; Herman, 1981; Westerlund, 1983). Group therapy af-
fords the former incest victim the opportunity to expand her disclosure
network; to reality-test negative self-concepts with women who share
similar traumatic histories; and to develop within the holding environ-
ment of the group more intimate relatedness with self, other, and self-in-
relation-wi th -other.
A survivor group run by the author confirms the recommendation
of others in the field; the group experience seems to stimulate increased
trust in self and other, in part through expanded self-disclosure in an
environment of safety and common ground. In this group of women
ranging in age from 20 to 50, it has been particularly poignant to ob-
Self-Disclosure among Incest Survivors 257
References
Adams, c., & Fay, J. No more secrets. San Luis Obispo: Impact Publishers.
Allen, c.v. (1980). Daddy's girl. New York: Berkley Books.
Armstrong, 1. (1978). Kiss daddy goodnight. New York: Pocket Books.
Bagley, c., & Ramsey, R. (1986). Sexual abuse in childhood: psychosocial outcomes and
implications for social work practice. Social Work Practice in Sexual Problems, 33-47.
Banmen, J. (1982). The incidence, treatment and counseling of incest. International Journal
for the Advancement of Counseling,S, 201-206.
Bass, E., & Thornton, 1. (1983). I never told anyone. New York: Harper & Row.
Becker, J., & Skinner, 1. (1983). Assessment and treatment of rape-related sexual dysfunc-
tions. The Clinical Psychologist, 36, 102-105.
Becker, J., Skinner, L., Abel, G., & Treacy, E. (1982). Incidence and type of sexual dysfunc-
tion in rape and incest victims. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 8, 65-74.
Blake-White, J., & Kline, C. (1985). Treating the dissociative process in adult victims of
childhood incest. Social Casework, 66, 394-402.
Briere, J., & Runtz, M. (1986). Suicidal thoughts and behaviors in former sexual abuse
victims. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 18, 413-423.
Briere, J., & Zaidi, 1. (1988). Sexual abuse histories and sequelae in psychiatric emergency room
258 Mary Gail Frawley
patients. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Soci-
ety, Atlanta, GA, Aug.
Brunngraber, L.s. (1986). Father-daughter incest: Immediate and long-term effects of sex-
ual abuse. Advances in Nursing Science, 8, 15-35.
Butler, S. (1978). Conspiracy of silence: The trauma of incest. New York: Bantam Books.
Cohen, T. (1983). The incestuous family revisited. Social Casework, 154-161.
Courtois, C.A. (1980). Studying and counseling women with past incest experience. Vic-
timology, 5, 322-334.
Courtois, C.A. (1988). Healing the incest wound. New York: w.w. Norton.
Ellenson, G.S. (1986). Disturbances of perception in adult female incest survivors. Social
Casework, 67, 149-159.
Epstein, L. (1979). The therapeutic use of countertransference data with borderline pa-
tients. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 13, 248-275.
Erikson, E.H. (1980). Identity and the life cycle. New York: w.w. Norton.
Fairbairn, W.R.D. (1941). A revised psychopathology of the psychoses and psycho-
neuroses. Psychoanalytic studies in the personality. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Fairbairn, W.R.D. (1943). The repression and the return of bad objects. Psychoanalytic
studies of the personality. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1984.
Faria, G., & Belohavek, N. (1984). Treating female adult survivors of childhood incest.
Social Casework, 65, 465-471.
Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child sexual abuse. New York: The Free Press.
Forward, S., & Buck, C. (1978). Betrayal of innocence. New York: Plenum Press.
Frawley, M.G. (1988). The sexual lives of adult survivors of father-daughter incest. Disser-
tation Abstracts International, 49. (University Microfilms No. 88-06,457.)
Fromuth, M.E. (1986). The relationship of childhood sexual abuse with later psychological
and sexual adjustment in a sample of college women. Child Abuse and Neglect, 10, 5-15.
Gelinas, D.J. (1983). The persisting negative effects of incest. Psychiatry, 46, 312-332.
Gold, E. (1986). Long-term effects of sexual victimization in childhood: an attributional
approach. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 471-475.
Gordy, p.L. (1983). Group work that support adult victims of childhood incest. Social
Casework, 300-307.
Gorney, J.F. (1979). The negative therapeutic interaction. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 15,
288-337.
Herman, J. (1981). Father-daughter incest. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hill, E. (1985). The family secret. New York: Dell.
Jehu, D., Gazan, M., & Klassen, C. (1985). Common therapeutic targets among women
who were sexually abused in childhood. Social Work and Human Sexuality, 3, 25-45.
Kaufman, I., Peck, A., & Tagiuri, C. (1954). The family constellation and overt incestuous
relations between father and daughter. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 24,
266-277.
Kempe, R.S., & Kempe, H. (1984). The common secret. New York: W.H. Freeman.
Kernberg, O. (1984). Severe personality disorders. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Landis, c., Landis, A.T., Bolles, M.M., Metzger, H., Pitts, M.W., D'Esopo, D.A., Moloy,
H.C., Kleegman, S.L & Dickinson, RL. (1970). Sex in development. College Park, MD:
McGrath Publishing Company.
McGuire, L.S., & Wagner, N.N. (1978). Sexual dysfunction in women who were molested
as children: One response pattern and suggestions for treatment. Journal of Sex and
Marital Therapy, 4, 11-15.
Meiselman, K.c. (1980). Personality characteristics of incest history psychotherapy pa-
tients: a research note. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 9,195-197.
O'Hare, J., & Taylor, K. (1983). The reality of incest. Women changing therapy. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Haworth Press.
Self-Disclosure among Incest Survivors 259
In late 1978 a son was born to my wife and me. Thirteen weeks prema-
ture and weighing just under two pounds, he died, after struggling
courageously for 11 days, on Christmas Eve. The events of his birth, life,
and death changed things-my sense of myself, my understanding of
the world, my feelings about life-to an extent that is difficult to convey
even now. And at the time very few people seemed willing to see, to
acknowledge, to understand that.
For me, part of the process of working-through involved the dictum
that one becomes expert in what one must. Nearly two and a half years
of research then, which included in-depth interviews with 35 parents
(20 mothers, 15 fathers) who had suffered a stillbirth or infant death
within the preceding 4-10 weeks, produced a dissertation (Peddicord,
1982) and a clinical familiarity with an experience that forces individuals
to make contact with what Stricker and Fisher in their Preface call "the
dark, fearful, and often untouched areas within ...."
261
262 Douglas J. Peddicord
uncommon experience. Despite the fact that more deaths occur in the
first few days of life than in any subsequent period of childhood, such
an event seems nearly unthinkable, an affront to the spectacular ad-
vances of medical technology and treatment in this century. But, in
reality, depending upon the definition of perinatal mortality utilized,
(ranging from a generally accepted time frame between the 20th week of
gestation and the 28th day of life to much broader limits, such as from
conception to one year after birth), these "rare" tragedies actually occur
from 70,000 to 250,000 times each year in the United States alone.
Though a great many mothers and fathers are forced to confront one
of the truly nightmarish anxieties of parenthood, until quite recently
researchers and clinicians largely shied away from the topic of perinatal
death. Thus, as late as 1970, Kennell, Slyter, and Klaus, while noting
detailed descriptions of mourning responses of adults to the loss of a
spouse, parent, friend, or terminally ill child, could state that theirs was
the first report in English on the reactions of parents to the loss of a
newborn. The taboo nature of the subject and dearth of research al-
lowed for caregiver assumptions marked by avoidance and denial:
Not too many years ago it was common practice for the physician of a mother
who just had a stillbirth to deny her physical contact with the dead baby's
body, prescribe tranquilizers to minimize her grief, and recommend forget-
ting about the sad event as soon as possible, perhaps by quickly attempting
another pregnancy. (Leon, 1987, p. 186)
Rando (1986) asserts that the loss of a child is unlike any other, that
parental grief is especially severe, often complicated and long lasting,
and marked by major symptom fluctuations over time. Lippman and
Carlson (1977) state: "Throughout the literature is the pervasive theme of
loss, of feeling devastated, and of the need to find the means to meet and
overcome these traumatic experiences" (p. 171). Investigators agree that
mothers (and fathers, in general) mourn and that this is usually mani-
fested in an acute grief reaction, as delineated initially by Lindemann
(1944). On occasion, marked pathological reactions such as severe de-
pression, reactive psychosis, and significant somatic difficulties have
been noted (Cullberg, 1971). Feelings of guilt and lowered self-esteem in
the mothers of deceased infants have been cited frequently (Benfield,
Leib, & Reuter, 1976; Peppers & Knapp, 1980; Miles & Demi, 1986).
What upsets me most is the unfulfilled potential of that child ... like that it
wasn't a real person. It's almost like if we could not have children and always
wanted one-it's that same sort of unfulfilled potential that is the saddest
thing to me, provokes the most in me. And at the same time the little being
that was and now isn't, isn't and it's ... there is nothing to remember really.
Like with a funeral people can get together and talk about the good times
they had (with the person) but for us there really weren't any good times to
be talked about. (Peddicord, 1982, p. 89)
nize that this unnatural event could happen to them and their own children.
Bereaved parents represent the worst fears of these other parents and they
become the victims of social ostracism and unrealistic expectations as other
parents attempt to ward off the terror generated within them ... It is common
for bereaved parents to experience feelings of abandonment, helplessness,
and frustration as reactions to their experiences with other parents. They
often complain that they feel like "social lepers." (p. 38)
Mothers
Simply put, mothers mourn more than do fathers in reaction to a
perinatal death; they report more sadness, more crying, more sleep
difficulties, more preoccupation, more anger, more guilt. Such findings
seem unsurprising in light of the truly intimate (literally symbiotic)
Perinatal Death 265
The newly bereaved mother may be reluctant to reveal typical reactions such
as transient hallucinatory experiences of a baby crying or the powerful urge
to steal another mother's infant for fear that she would be considered insane.
She may consciously suppress her tendency to cry in response to any re-
minders of babies fearing that if she is not able to control her grief, it will
overwhelm her and never end. (Leon, 1987, p. 187)
Fathers
with genuine life and death, it can be hard to continue treating work or
career as the most important thing in one's life or, simply, that "half of
the details that keep you so busy are really incidental" (Peddicord, 1982,
p. 90). They question their values, their purpose. And some fathers
(especially those without other surviving children) feel disoriented-
having rearranged their lives, even changed jobs, in preparation for
parenthood, only to be faced with needing to reorder priorities, goals,
etc., yet again.
Their behavior limited by a cultural role that discourages expression
of feelings, and reluctant to confront (as mothers do) the vulnerability
and powerlessness inherent to their experience, fathers not infrequently
couple avoidance with acting-out. The diversion offered by working too
much or drinking too much or having an affair takes away the sting of a
shameful knowledge-that he too has "failed" in the attempt to create a
new life, has failed at the primary paternal function of protecting his
child.
Barriers to Disclosure
Parents confronted with a perinatal death live out a dreaded fear
come true, are denied a happy expectation the rest of the world takes for
granted. Yet at a time when they most need support, most need "narcis-
sistic supplies," mothers and fathers find their grief discounted, their
pain minimized.
In the death of an infant a primal separation anxiety is enacted-
fear of loss/abandonment becomes loss/abandonment. Fisher (1982)
points out that resistance (whether to therapy per se or to the sharing of
experience or to intimacy) can be understood as "humanity's constant
effort to avoid the pain of feeling, thinking, or reexperiencing nonbeing"
(p. 117). The literal nonbeing of the infant is nightmarish, and the resis-
tance-of parents to the acceptance of the event forced by the reality
principle, and of others to the possibility of establishing an identifica-
tion, of genuinely sharing the parents' experience-is the central barrier
to self-disclosure and authentic relatedness.
In the Couple
For parents who have lost infants, expect high levels of disagreement and
misunderstanding ... much higher levels of grief in mothers than in fathers,
greater eagerness in the father to return to normal (especially in sexual
relations), and consequent high stress on the marriage and consideration of
divorce, especially by mothers. (p. 426)
Caregivers
by the 4%-8% of cases that do not end happily, the cases that constitute
their "failures." Most often men, and trained to be in control always, like
fathers they have difficulty tolerating, let alone expressing, feelings of
vulnerability and powerlessness in the face of tragedy, and like fathers
they tend to use avoidance and rationalization. Nearly half the mothers
interviewed by the author said that their doctors responded inade-
quately at the point in time when the pregnancy went from normal and
uneventful to potentially catastrophic, for example, with premature la-
bor-they felt placated, not taken seriously, treated as hysterical.
Mothers often felt "dumped" or "abandoned" then when transfer to the
care of attending physicians at a high-risk, regional neonatal center was
initiated-and frequently the doctor who had been following the preg-
nancy did not make further contact with his patient.
In trying to "protect" parents from the reality of their experience,
caregivers at times make stunningly incorrect assumptions. For in-
stance, a nurse commented to an unmarried, teenage mother that her
loss, a stillbirth, must have been in some way a relief (presumably be-
cause her circumstances for raising a child would have been so difficult);
the young woman experienced the interaction as denigrating and cruel,
because she was counting on the child to give her "purpose ... I was
finally going to do something ... 1 had something to put my life into"
(Peddicord, 1982, p. 107). Another assumption is that a stillbirth is some-
how less real, less painful, than the loss of a live-born infant, but the
mother (and, to a lesser extent, father) has in fact lived with the fetus for
a number of months and what she (he) deals with is not simple removal
of a "benign tumor." One mother said, "After so much attention in the
delivery room the night before, no one came in to talk or help me clean
up or anything ... but I had had a baby!" (Peddicord, 1982, p. 100).
Sometimes caregivers assume a model of grieving that proceeds by
neat "stages." Not uncommonly parents are characterized then as
mourning incorrectly; not talking, not sharing their feelings enough, for
example, or remaining "stuck" in anger and guilt (emotions that care-
givers find especially uncomfortable, unreasonable). But the experience
of parents cannot be grasped so easily; a willingness to understand the
meaning of this death/loss to this particular person, to appreciate this
individual's character structure and defenses, to allow and feel a pain
that threatens to annihilate the self, is required. Grief is not so much a
problem to be solved as a process in which caregivers can be genuine
participants. Self-disclosure and intimacy are two-way streets and for
most caregivers the resistance is too great-they neither disclose (to the
other or to themselves) their own internal contents nor do they truly
apprehend the experience of the mothers and fathers with whom they
come in contact.
Perinatal Death 271
present identification and a hope for the future, i.e., "if the therapist
could be repaired perhaps I can be also." A genuine person, the self-
disclosing therapist can (as in all therapies) be hated too, but the
patient is less likely to worry that herlhis own disclosures will induce
abandonment.
With the common unwillingness of friends, family, and even care-
givers to tolerate the primitive anxiety engendered by the death of an
infant, to acknowledge the parents' loss in a genuine way, or even to
allow mourning to occur, often the greatest pain of a perinatal death is in
the aloneness it creates; internal devastation and emptiness are matched
by external isolation. It is that barrier to intimacy which self-disclosure
by a caregiver or therapist can breech, which self-disclosure by a parent
can overcome.
References
Benfield, D. G., Leib, S.A., & Reuter, J. (1976). Grief response of parents after referral of the
critically ill newborn to a regional center. New England Journal of Medicine, 294,
975-978.
Benfield, D.G., Leib, S.A., & Vollman, J.H. (1978). Grief response of parents to neonatal
death and parent participation in deciding care. Pediatrics, 62, 171-177.
Cullberg, J. (1971). Mental reactions of women to perinatal death. In N. Moriss (Ed.),
Psychosomatic Medicine in Obstetrics and Gynecology, Third International Congress. Lon-
don: Karger-Basel.
Fish, W.C (1986). Differences of grief intensity in bereaved parents. In T.A. Rando (Ed.),
Parental loss of a child. Champaign: Research Press.
Fisher, M.N. (1982). The shared experience: A theory of psychoanalytic psychotherapy. In
M.N. Fisher & G. Stricker (Eds.), Intimacy. New York: Plenum.
Freud, S. (1917/1958). Mourning and melancholia. Standard Edition of the Complete Psycho-
logical Works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 14). London: Hogarth Press.
Furman, E. (1976). Caring for the parents of an infant who dies: Comment. In M.H. Klaus
& J.H. Kennell (Eds.), Maternal-infant bonding. St. Louis: Cv. Mosby.
Gardner, R.A. (1969). The guilt reaction of parents of children with severe physical dis-
ease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 26, 636-644.
Kennell, J.H. Slyter, H., & Klaus, M.H. (1970). The mourning response of parents to the
death of a newborn infant. New England Journal of Medicine, 283, 344-349.
Leon, I.G. (1987). Short-term psychotherapy for perinatal loss. Psychotherapy, 24, 186-195.
Lindemann, E. (1944). Symptomatology and management of acute grief. American Journal
of Psychiatry, 101, 141-148.
Lippman, CA., & Carlson, K. (1977). A model liaison program for the obstetrics staff. In
CE. Hollingsworth & R.o. Pasnau (Eds.), The family in mourning: A guide for health
professionals. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Mazor, M.D. (1979). Barren couples. Psychology Today, May; 101-112.
Miles, M.S., & Demi, A.S. (1986). Guilt in bereaved parents. In T.A. Rando (Ed.), Parental
loss of a child. Champaign: Research Press.
Parkes, CM. (1965). Bereavement and mental illness. British Journal of Medicine and Psychol-
ogy, 38, 13-26.
Peddicord, D.J. (1982). Perinatal death: some aspects of parental reactions (Doctoral disser-
tation, Adelphi University, 1982). Dissertation Abstracts International, 43, 1263B.
Perinatal Death 273
Peppers, L.G., & Knapp, R.J. (1980). Motherhood and mourning: Perinatal death. New York:
Praeger.
Rando, T.A. (1986). The unique issues and impact of the death of the child. In T.A. Rando
(Ed.), Parental loss of a child. Champaign: Research Press.
Schiff, H.S. (1977). The bereaved parent. New York: Crown.
VI
Conclusion
18
Self-Disclosure and Psychotherapy
George Stricker
277
278 George Stricker
freed to live a more authentic and fulfilled life. The role of the therapist is
less simple. Disclosure is part of a relationship between equals, it can
serve to model the comfortable expression of uncomfortable experi-
ences, and it also creates a climate in which self-revelation is acceptable
and valued. The therapist must recognize, however, that it is the growth
of the patient that is paramount, so that the disclosure is confined to
experiences occurring in, or relevant to, the treatment.
As long as the growth of the patient is paramount, there is some
question whether a relationship that is inherently asymmetric really is
between equals. More likely, two individuals meet who are equal in
their humanity and in their adult status, unequal in the particular rela-
tionship in which they find themselves, and, by disclosing themselves
to each other, they close the gap between themselves in the treatment.
Perhaps it might even be said that as equality increases, the need for
treatment decreases, and treatment ends well when the two participants
are equal in the room, as they are outside it. This statement also sug-
gests that the limits of treatment may be set by the limitations of the
therapist, for the patient cannot be taken any further than the therapist
has traveled.
Healey takes a historical perspective and views self-disclosure as a
part of the Judeo-Christian religious experience. Indeed, as this experi-
ence was, and for many people still is, an essential healing experience,
the parallel to psychotherapy is compelling. The key to the religious
experience lies in the disclosure of the individual to God and of God to
the individual. The priest/rabbi serves as the mediator between God and
the individual, much as a therapist plays an essential role in the dis-
closure of the individual to the self. The role of relationship and trust is
central to self-disclosure in the religious experience, as it is in the thera-
peutic experience, and the role of ritual in both experiences cannot be
overlooked. The intent of the two experiences may be different, but the
process bears striking similarity, as does the central role of the self.
Healey does suggest that one essential difference is that psycho-
therapy is dyadic (patient and therapist) but religious experience is tri-
adic (individual, spiritual director, God). One may take a sacrilegious
view and suggest that psychotherapy is also triadic, and that our princi-
ple goal is to bring the patient in closer touch with the God within. This
internal God may be variously conceptualized as the superego, the par-
ental introjects, or the authentic self. In any case, the disclosure of the
individual to this forbidden internal component, and the acceptance of
the dread secrets by that component, carry the seeds of forgiveness,
acceptance, and growth. The religious person, through disclosure of
sinful or heretical thoughts and feelings, and the acceptance of these,
will experience himself as achieving greater communion with his God.
Self-Disclosure and Psychotherapy 279
stand all aspects of the relationship and to leave all decisions about the
patient's life in the hands of the patient. With this conception of neu-
trality, it is possible to self-disclose and to remain neutral.
Basescu uses the interesting phrase "show and tell" in his title. It
points out that some of the self-disclosure is unavoidably shown, such
as the therapist's clothing, voice quality, office appointments, etc.; but
other aspects of self-disclosure are consciously told. The therapist must
be aware of both, and strive for understanding of the motivation behind
and impact of both. Just as we ask the patient to strive to understand, it
is critical for the therapist to understand the motivation behind self-
disclosure. Self-disclosure is not inherently good or bad; it can be well-
timed, well understood, geared to the needs of the patient, and highly
constructive; it also can be random, defensive, countertransferential,
geared to the needs of the therapist, and deleterious to the course of the
treatment.
Josephs approaches self-disclosure from the standpoint of self psy-
chology, a particularly relevant theoretical position given the emphasis
that has been placed on disclosing to the self. The distinction that is
drawn between the verbal self and the experiential self is a crucial one,
and one of the primary functions of self-disclosure is to narrow the gap
between these two selves, so that the sometimes false, presented self
becomes consistent with the authentic, experienced self.
Josephs also emphasizes the essential intersubjectivity of any self-
disclosure. In order for self-disclosure to occur, there must be a person
who self-discloses and a person who receives the self-disclosure. When
the discloser is the patient and the receiver is the therapist, the therapist
serves a self-regulatory function, aiding the patient in developing a
clearer sense of the meaning and import of the self-disclosure. When the
discloser is the therapist and the receiver is the patient, the patient may
also serve a self-regulatory function for the therapist but, more impor-
tantly, that same information is used to further develop a sense of the
experiential self. Two important conclusions follow from this formula-
tion. First, a recognition of intersubjectivity also involves a necessary
opting for reality over fantasy, as more value is placed on the quality of
the real relationship than on an illumination of the construction of a
fantasied relationship. Secondly, the intersubjectivity can be triadic,
with the patient, the therapist, and the patient's experienced but not
verbalized self all participating. Of course, the therapist'S experienced
self is also a participant; but that is not the focus of the therapeutic
encounter. In this model, the patient will verbalize a self-disclosure,
receive information from the response of the therapist, and modify the
conception of the experienced self as a result. Similarly, the patient may
receive a self-disclosure from the therapist, attempt to integrate it, and,
Self-Disclosure and Psychotherapy 281
tive role model in a more egalitarian setting. With both children and
adolescents, the limitations of a developing ego are respected and, while
respecting the presence and importance of the parents, the need for an
empathic, reality-bound adult is understood.
There is a logical extension of this sensitive, developmentally based
approach to the use of self-disclosure. It is clear that children and ado-
lescents usually are caught up in developmental tasks that make the
additional burden of self-disclosure sometimes too overwhelming to be
therapeutically valuable. It is also clear that, either despite or because of
the presence of parents, they often require a clear, empathic adult to be
available as a role model and an object of attachment. It is not as clear
why the chronological passage of time automatically erases these con-
siderations. For many adults, self-disclosure may also represent a bur-
den that must be tempered by the careful therapist, and the need for an
empathic role model or attachment figure has not disappeared with the
passing years. It seems likely that the titration of self-disclosure should
be geared to developmental needs, regardless of the chronological age of
the patient.
At the other end of the developmental spectrum, special considera-
tions are also indicated in working with older adults, the topic of Green-
berg's chapter. This chapter is built on a strong research foundation,
with much evidence cited that indicates the value of a confidant in the
adjustment and happiness of an elderly person. Intimate relationships
seem to serve as a buffer against demoralization and depression, and
self-disclosure is an important contributor to intimacy. In psycho-
therapy, some writers view an empathic, supportive relationship, but-
tressed by the therapist'S use of self-disclosure, as an end in itself,
providing the elderly patient with gratification that is absent elsewhere
in his life. Although this may be of value to some, there are many elderly
patients who can benefit from standard psychotherapy, with self-
exploration and growth seen as realistic and legitimate goals. For these
patients, the considerations governing the use of self-disclosure are
much the same as they would be for other adult patients, with judg-
ments about the needs of the patient and the capacity of the therapist
determining the introduction, appropriateness, and timing of any self-
disclosure. As with any patient, revelations communicating affective
attunement are likely to be helpful, and those displaying unresponsive-
ness or misattunement are not. On the other hand, the patients may not
be as free to self-disclose as a younger person might, stemming in part
from the norms to which this age cohort was socialized, as well as from
more dynamic concerns that might be subject to the same interpreta-
tions and interventions as would occur with younger patients.
Consideration must also be given to modalities other than individ-
286 George Stricker
291
292 Index