Overregularization in Language Acquisition
Overregularization in Language Acquisition
Author(s): Gary F. Marcus, Steven Pinker, Michael Ullman, Michelle Hollander, T. John
Rosen, Fei Xu and Harald Clahsen
Source: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 57, No. 4,
Overregularization in Language Acquisition (1992), pp. i+iii+v-vi+1-178
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1166115
Accessed: 24-11-2017 06:50 UTC
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms
Society for Research in Child Development, Wiley are collaborating with JSTOR to
digitize, preserve and extend access to Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS OF THE
SOCIETY FOR RESEARCH IN
CHILD DEVELOPMENT
Serial No. 228, Vol. 57, No. 4, 1992
OVERREGU LARIZATION
IN LANGUAGE
ACQUISITION
Gary F. Marcus
Steven Pinker
Michael Ullman
Michelle Hollander
T. John Rosen
Fei Xu
WITH COMMENTARY BY
Harald Clahsen
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS OF THE SOCIETY FOR RESEARCH IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT
Serial No. 228, Vol. 57, No. 4, 1992
CONTENTS
ABSTRACT v
I. INTRODUCTION 1
III. METHOD 24
APPENDIX 145
REFERENCES 157
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 165
COMMENTARY
CONTRIBUTORS 179
STATEMENT OF
EDITORIAL POLICY 181
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
ABSTRACT
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
overregularization by similar-sounding irregulars, but they are not attracted
to overregularization by similar-sounding regulars, suggesting that irregular
patterns are stored in an associative memory with connectionist properties,
but that regulars are not. We propose a simple explanation. Children, like
adults, mark tense using memory (for irregulars) and an affixation rule that
can generate a regular past tense form for any verb. Retrieval of an irregu-
lar blocks the rule, but children's memory traces are not strong enough to
guarantee perfect retrieval. When retrieval fails, the rule is applied, and
overregularization results.
VI
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1. INTRODUCTION
Overregularizations like comed and foots are among the most conspicu
ous grammatical errors in child language, and they have been comme
on for as long as language development has been studied (Bateman,
Brown, 1973; Brown & Bellugi, 1964; Bybee & Slobin, 1982a; Carl
1947; Carroll, 1961; Cazden, 1968; Chamberlain, 1906; Ervin, 1964; Erv
& Miller, 1963; Guillaume, 1927; Kuczaj, 1977a, 1981; Menyuk, 1
Miller & Ervin, 1964; Slobin, 1971, 1978; Smith, 1933; see also Edwards
1970). These errors are made possible by the fact that English has two wa
of creating inflected forms. Most verbs add the suffix -ed to their stems
form the past tense, but about 180 exceptional or "irregular" verbs f
their past tenses in idiosyncratic ways such as a vowel change (come-c
replacement of a final consonant or rhyme (make-made, teach-taught), su
tution of another form (go-went), or no change at all (cut-cut). Overregu
zation errors consist of applying the regular pattern to an irregular
Since children do not hear these forms from their parents, the errors re
the operation of a creative process, presumably corresponding to a m
operation implementing the -ed-suffixation rule posited by grammarians.
Past tense overregularization is just one kind of error in one peripher
aspect of one component of the grammar of one language. Nonethele
has assumed a surprising prominence in cognitive science over the
several decades. Overregularization has been offered as the quintessen
demonstration of the creative essence of human language (Chomsky,
and of the necessity of explaining cognitive processes by rules and repres
tations rather than by rote and reinforcement (e.g., Brown & Bellugi, 196
Lenneberg, 1964; McNeill, 1966; Slobin, 1971; Smith, Langston, & Nisb
in press).
Overregularization has also become famous because of its interest
developmental course, first noted by Ervin and Miller (1963; see also
den, 1968; Miller & Ervin, 1964; Pinker & Prince, 1988). The first over
larization errors seem to appear after a period in which, when child
mark tense on irregular verbs at all, they do so correctly. Overregulariza
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
II. THEORETICAL BACKGROUND
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
broke) and overregularize new ones (e.g., sticked). But it does not predict that
children who first use a given verb properly (e.g., broke) would later err on
that very verb (e.g., breaked), which they do (Cazden, 1968; Ervin & Miller,
1963; the phenomenon is documented in more detail in Chap. IV, Sec.
IIA). Possessing a rule that can be applied to novel verbs and applying a
rule to existing verbs that already have a past tense form and that do not
allow the rule are two different things, and the traditional account does not
distinguish between them.
For similar reasons, the rule-rote distinction sheds little light on the
other arm of the U, the fact that breaked ultimately gives way to exclusive
use of broke. Once children have developed a rule and are overapplying it,
how and why do they curtail its use?
Clearly, understanding the developmental course of overregularization
requires more than distinguishing between rule and rote. We must also
examine how the regular rule interacts with irregular items. Specifically, to
explain differences in the ways children and adults treat irregular verbs, we
must first understand how adults treat irregular verbs. Then we can ask
what children might be doing that adults are not. We must also examine
the information about irregular verbs available from conversation with
adults that the child's language learning process might feed on. With knowl-
edge of what is learned and what information is available to learn it, we can
develop a theory of how the learning works and why it might result in
errors in intermediate stages.
In this chapter, we review the current state of knowledge on these
issues (deferring discussion of the Rumelhart-McClelland model to Chap.
V) and develop a hypothesis that appears adequate to account for both
the development and the cessation of overregularization. The hypothesis is
extremely simple, uniting a standard proposal from formal linguistics (that,
in the human language system, irregular memorized items block the appli-
cation of regular rules) and a standard proposal from cognitive psychology
(that retrieval of items from memory is probabilistic and sensitive to fre-
quency of exposure). According to this hypothesis, children's language sys-
tem, like that of adults, is designed so that retrieval of an irregular form
suppresses overregularization, but retrieval is imperfect, and when it fails,
the regular rule applies as a default, leading to overregularization errors.
The hypothesis will lead us to an equally simple empirical prediction: that,
at all ages, evidence that the child's system is designed to suppress overregu-
larization should be available in the form of low overall rates of overregular-
ization in comparison to utterances containing the correct irregular form.
We will then compare this prediction to competing ones in the literature.
This will set the stage for our investigation of the basic quantitative facts of
overregularization in the following chapters.
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
The problem with overregularizations is not that they have never been
heard before but that the irregular counterpart has been heard. Clearly,
there is a psychological mechanism that causes the experience of hearing
an irregular form to block the subsequent application of a regular process
to that item. Thus, several linguists have defined this phenomenon in terms
of a blocking principle (Aronoff, 1976; Kiparsky, 1982; see also Pinker, 1984,
who called it the unique entry principle): an idiosyncratic form listed in the
mental lexicon as corresponding to a particular grammatical modification
of a word (past tense, in this case) blocks the application of a general rule
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
that would effect the same grammatical modification. Thus, broke, listed as
an idiosyncratic past tense form of break, blocks application of the regular
past tense rule, preempting breaked; geese, listed as a plural of goose, blocks
gooses; better, listed as a comparative of the adjective good, blocks gooder. Of
course, some verbs do have two commonly heard past tense forms, for
example, dived and dove; in these cases, the regular version, which is idiosyn-
cratic precisely because it flouts the blocking principle, would be recorded
from the speech input just as if it were an irregular (Pinker, 1984; Ullman
& Pinker, 1990).
Now that we have a mechanism that causes overregularizations to be
treated as deviant by adults, we can state the developmental problem more
precisely. Children do not apply blocking in the same circumstances that
adults do. The question now is, Why not? One obvious possibility is that
children might have to learn the blocking principle and that overregular-
izations appear before they have done so. In the following section, we show
that this hypothesis is probably wrong and that the source of children's
overregularization errors must be sought elsewhere. Before doing so, how-
ever, we add a few remarks about the status of the blocking principle.
1 The blocking principle, in turn, might be reduced to Kiparsky's (1982) more general
"elsewhere condition": if the conditions licensing one morphological process constitute a
proper subset of the conditions licensing another morphological process, then whenever
the conditions of both processes are met by some item, only the process with the more
restrictive conditions may apply; the other process applies "elsewhere." In this case, an
irregular form like broke applies in the conditions "past tense of the verb break," and the
regular rule applies in the more general condition "past tense of a verb," so the latter is
preempted.
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
forms contrast in meaning." First, blocking rules out only competing mor-
phological variants and is agnostic about synonyms in general (e.g., it doe
not rule out couch and sofa; for recent arguments that languages tolerat
true synonyms, see Gathercole, 1987, 1989; for counterarguments, see
Clark, 1988, 1990).
More important, there are empirical reasons why blocking per se, not
contrast, is needed to explain the fact that irregular verbs cannot be regular-
ized. Neither violations of blocking nor conformity to it can be predicted
from the availability of contrasts in meaning. Members of variant pairs such
as leaped-leapt, dived-dove, and sneaked-snuck do not differ reliably in meaning
(or do so with near-imperceptible subtlety); they seem to be recorded by
the learner in response to the brute fact of hearing them (later we discuss
one reason why they arise to begin with). Conversely, verbs like put, make,
give, take, have, come, go, and throw each have dozens of extremely distinct
meanings, especially in combination with particles such as out, up, off, in,
and away. Nonetheless, without exception, speakers are not tempted to use
the regularized versions of these verbs to express the contrasting meanings;
taked the punishment stoically, gived away the answer, haved a ball, maked out, and
so on are always impossible. This shows that different past tense forms do
not necessarily evoke different meanings and that different meanings do
not systematically call for different past tense forms (for discussion, see
Kim, Pinker, et al., 1991; Pinker & Prince, 1988; and Ullman & Pinker,
1990, 1992).
In addition, blocking just rules out certain kinds of forms in a specific
level of grammatical representation; it does not guarantee that an alterna-
tive way of expressing a given notion exists, and thus it differs from a
communicative principle like contrast that is designed to provide a speaker
with exactly one way to express each notion. In fact, there are special cir-
cumstances where blocking applies but the irregular is ruled out for other
reasons and the speaker is left with an expressibility gap. In particular,
blocking applies at a specific level of the mental representation of word
forms, not to entire words; roughly, it applies at the level of inflected stems.
An entire word may consist of a stem plus a (noninflectional) prefix. For
example, speakers of English intuitively perceive forgo as containing the
stem go. Hence, they cannot say forgoed because went, the listed past tense
form of the stem go, blocks the application of the regular rule to go. How-
ever, speakers' sense of the familiarity or naturalness of an irregular form is
a phenomenon that takes place at the level of the entire word, not the stem.2
10
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
Forgo is a relatively uncommon word, and uses in the past tense are even
rarer, so forwent, while not sounding grammatically impossible like forgoed,
nonetheless sounds stilted and unnatural (Pinker & Prince, 1988; Ullman
& Pinker, 1992). The verb thus has no perfectly usable past tense form.
The regular past is ruled out by blocking (applying at the level of the stem);
the irregular past is tainted by unfamiliarity (applying at the level of the
entire word). This shows that blocking is a mechanism that operates on
specific representations of the speaker's grammatical system.
11
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Osherson, Stob, & Weinstein, 1985; Pinker, 1979). To explain how the adu
grammar is attained, then, one must explain either how children avoid
generating supersets or, if they do, how they expunge their errors.
Overregularization errors in particular pose this problem. A child wh
is producing breaked and broke is speaking a superset of adult English in this
domain, which allows only broke. And overregularization errors in particular
do not reliably occasion negative parental feedback. Kuczaj (1977a, p. 599
noted that, in his investigation, the children (especially his son Abe, wh
was the main subject) were not systematically corrected for overregulariza-
tion errors. The following typical chunk of dialogue, which we have foun
in transcripts of conversations between Kuczaj and Abe (MacWhinney &
Snow, 1985), illustrates his observation:
12
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
Eve it was expansions and partial imitations that occurred more frequently
following her ungrammatical sentences; and for Sarah all five categories of
parental response showed the opposite pattern, occurring more frequently
after well-formed utterances. Unless a child can figure out the kind of parent
he or she has (e.g., a grammatical sentence expander or an ungrammatical
sentence expander), such feedback is useless (see Marcus, 1992). Moreover,
Morgan and Travis showed that, as children get older, even this inconsistent
feedback signal disappears. Since the errors continue after the parent has
stopped supplying potential feedback, unlearning the errors must depend
on some other information source.
S Some such assumption has proved unavoidable to many researchers devising explicit
models of language learning, working in a variety of frameworks. For example, Anderson
(1983) required a constraint similar to blocking when applying his symbol-processing
model of cognition called ACT to the acquisition of morphology (although the principle
was not specific to language acquisition but constrained his production-system architecture
across all the tasks it was given). In their very different connectionist model, Rumelhart
and McClelland (1986) also implemented machinery that carries out a version of blocking.
As we shall see, in their supervised learning paradigm, a special input pathway was de-
signed that presented a single "correct" past tense form representing the parental input,
and an error-correcting learning procedure acted to suppress the tendency of the model
to produce an output that deviated from it. Then a separate mechanism, the whole-string
binding network, which contains explicit representations of correct and overregularized
forms, sets them in competition with one another to select the form that best approximates
the output of the network.
13
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
mance (the adult state) without negative evidence. Blocking would prevent
children from ever generating a superset of English; they would progres
from breaked to broke, never saying both at the same stage.
4 Clark's principle of contrast acts more like blocking per se when it is combined w
her principle of conventionality, which forces the child to use the form that is conventio
in the speech community whenever competing alternatives arise.
14
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
assumes, in which the child actually loses early irregular forms, does not
seem to be accurate given the (admittedly scanty) data available at present.
All the authors that have actually recorded overregularization errors note
that these errors coexist with, rather than replace, the early irregulars (Caz-
den, 1968; Ervin & Miller, 1963; Kuczaj, 1977a, 1981). This is exactly what
all the principles dictating unique forms proscribe. Maratsos (1987) points
to such coexistence, which can last for months or years, as a reason to doubt
the existence of such principles. Children, it seems, do generate a superset
of English (they use break and broke simultaneously), so if blocking is the
explanation of how children eventually abandon this superset, it leaves it a
puzzle why they adopted it to begin with and why they retain it for so long.5
"5 Another possible solution, originally proposed by Kuczaj (1977a, 1981), is that chil-
dren may fail to realize that a given irregular form corresponds to the past tense version
of some stem. Rather, they may treat the irregular past as an independent verb, and
blocking would not apply. Errors would cease when the two verbs were united, presumably
when the child noticed that they were semantically identical except for pastness and (in
most cases) were phonologically similar as well. We discuss this solution in Chap. VII,
Sec. II.
15
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
6 The principal difference between MacWhinney's (1978) and Pinker's (1984) exposi-
tions is that Pinker takes the blocking principle, as it was explicated and justified by
linguists to explain adult knowledge, and simply attributes it to the child, whereas Mac-
Whinney introduced it as a specific new claim about the child's morphological acquisition
system. Note that MacWhinney has since disavowed the claims that regularizations are
produced by a rule and that they are suppressed by rote forms across the board. He
has proposed instead that regularizations are produced by a propertywise generalization
process like that of the Rumelhart-McClelland model and that overregularizations are
suppressed by specific, individually learned inhibitions of each overregularized form by
its irregular counterpart (MacWhinney, 1987, pp. 285, 295; see also MacWhinney & Lein-
bach, 1991; and Chap. VIII, Sec. III).
7 Presumably, the fourth logically possible retrieval pattern can occur as well: the
existence of a stored past tense form linked to a stem is registered, but its content cannot
be recovered, a state like that studied in memory phenomena such as tip of the tongue
(Brown & McNeill, 1966), feeling of knowing (Hart, 1965), and deep dyslexia (Coltheart,
Patterson, & Marshall, 1980). In such cases, we would not see overregularization, because
the activation of the irregular memory entry blocks the rule; nor would we see the correct
irregular, because its content is temporarily unavailable. What would surface is the un-
marked stem.
16
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
but not enough times to be able to recall on demand reliably. A child should
overregularize these verbs, even with a grammatical system identical to
adults'. If children's memory retrieval is noisier than adults', they should
make these errors even more often, holding number of exposures constant.
But regardless of whether there are quantitative differences between chil-
dren and adults, there need be no qualitative differences; blocking of a
regular rule by retrieval of an irregular stored in memory and a memory
retrieval rate of less than 100% are sufficient to account for the phe-
nomenon.
8 Bybee examined 33 surviving verbs from three classes of strong verbs in Old English.
Fifteen have come through in Modern English as irregular verbs; 18 have become regular.
The surviving irregulars have a mean Francis and Kucera (1982) frequency of 515 over
all their inflectional forms, 137 in the past tense; the regularized verbs have a mean
frequency of 21 over all forms, five in the past tense.
17
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
learned that English obligatorily marks tense and that an -ed suffixation rule
is available to do so. However, they can memorize past forms from their
parents and occasionally use them; all past tense forms recorded from chi
dren will thus be correct ones. Overregularizations appear when the regul
rule is acquired. But the previously acquired irregulars do not go anywhere,
nor are they ever incapable of blocking overregularization: they just hav
to be retrieved to be able to do the blocking, and they are retrieved probabi-
listically. The cure for overregularization is living longer, hearing the irreg-
ulars more often, and consolidating them in memory, improving retrievabil-
ity. Crucially, this account serves to demystify overregularization, requiring
no qualitative difference between children and adults during the overregu
larization period.
18
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
B. Competing Predictions
19
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
a regularized version. Thus a child who had been using the correct past
tense of sing-namely, sang-may start using the form singed ... in-
stead. Before the correct irregular form is again learned, some children
have been known to produce forms that combine the irregular and the
regular form. In the case of sing, this would be sanged. Thus children
may proceed through as many as five steps in the acquisition of some
inflections ... No inflection, Adult Form, Overregularization, Transi-
tion, Adult Form. [Reich, 1986, p. 148]
Note that Clark is not claiming that irregular past forms are tossed out or
even that they fade; she says only that irregulars are treated as regular verbs.
Crucially, while they are overregularizing, most children do not successfully
inflect regular verbs with -ed 100% of the time. Brown (1973) reports that
none of the three children he studied were producing regulars more than
90% of the time for six consecutive hours of samples during the time they
began to overregularize, although they achieved this criterion several
months later. We examine these data in detail in Chapter VI; for now
20
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
No clear value for the assumed overregularization rate is given, but "relative
strength of irregular and overregularized forms" implies in context that for
some verbs overregularizations predominate and that for others the correct
form does. There is no reason to expect that overregularizations or correct
irregulars are systematically preferred, and given the large number of differ-
ent biasing factors at work over a large number of verbs and children, we
might expect on statistical grounds that they would roughly even out. We
can therefore read Bowerman as basing her theorizing here on the assump-
tion that the overall overregularization rate does not deviate from 50% by
an amount large enough to attribute to some pervasive single cause.
In a review article on language development, Maratsos (1983, p. 763)
is more explicit about his assumptions about children's overregularization
rate:
21
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
It is not clear from Maratsos's chapter what the figures of "from .20 to .60
of the children's uses" (attributed to an unpublished talk by Kuczaj) refer
to, as we shall see in Chapter IV. For now, it suffices to note that Maratsos
is suggesting that the typical rate of overregularization is not far from 50%
(the literal implication of "alternation"), perhaps 40% (the midpoint of the
range he cites). It is also clear that these are the data he considers as a
refutation of blocking. In his review of Pinker (1984), he writes,
VI. SUMMARY
The mere fact that children develop a regular rule cannot explain wh
they overapply it to irregular verbs. Adults block the application of regular
22
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
rules to idiosyncratic memorized items; we suggest that children do, too, but
their retrieval of idiosyncratic items from memory, especially low-frequency
ones, is probabilistic, and overregularizations occur when it fails. The hy-
pothesis explains how children unlearn their errors in the absence of nega-
tive parental feedback and does not make the dubious prediction that over-
regularizations replace correct irregulars in children's speech. It predicts
that overregularizations should generally be rare in children's speech rela-
tive to correct irregular past tense forms, contrary to most current views
about language development.
23
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Ill. METHOD
I. SUBJECTS
24
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
TABLE 1
CHILDREN STUDIED
Total
Child Age Source Samples Sampling Frequency
Abe ........... 2-6-5-0 Kuczaj (1976) 210 Weekly
Adam .......... 2-3-5-2 Brown (1973) 55 2-3/month
Allison ......... 1-5-2-10 Bloom (1973) 6 Occasionally
April .......... 1-10-2-11 Higginson (1985) 6 Occasionally
Eve ............ 1-6-2-3 Brown (1973) 20 2-3/month
Naomi ......... 1-3-4-9 Sachs (1983) 93 Weekly to monthly
Nat............ 2-8 Bohannon & Marquis (1977) 21 Within 1 month
Nathaniel ....... 2-3-3-9 Snow (unpublished)a 30 Weekly
Peter .......... 1-3-3-1 Bloom (1973) 20 Monthly
Sarah .......... 2-3-5-1 Brown (1973) 139 Weekly
15 children ..... 4-6-5-0 Hall et al. (1984) 30 2 days/child
24 children ..... 2-1-5-2 Gleason (1980) 72 3 samples/child
20 children ..... 1-6-6-2 Warren-Leubecker (1982) 20 1 sample/child
14 children ..... 2-9-6-6 Gathercole (1979) 16 1-4 samples/child
a See MacWhinney and Snow (1985).
25
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
II. PROCEDURE
We tabulated all past tense forms of irregular verbs, correct and ove
regularized, for all the children (for an exhaustive list of irregular verbs in
present-day American English, sorted into subclasses, see the appendix
Pinker & Prince, 1988). Irregular verbs such as dream and dive admitting of
a common regular alternative in adult speech were excluded. Overregular
ations included stem + ed forms like eated and past + ed forms like ated. We
did not search for other possible kinds of error involving the regular suffix
such as sweepened or brecked (for broke); such errors, in any case, are quite
rare. The actual tabulation procedure differed slightly for the differen
children.
For Abe, the data were gathered and tabulated by Kuczaj (1976), the
boy's father. For each month from age 2-5 to age 5-0, Kuczaj recorded the
number of times Abe used each of 70 irregular verbs, the number of times
he produced the present stem of the verb with -ed appended (such as goed
and breaked ["stem + ed" errors]), the number of times he produced doubly
marked pasts in which -ed was added to the irregular past form (such as
wented and broked ["past + ed" errors]), and the number of times he used the
stem form in obligatory past tense contexts (such as Yesterday we go out).
Kuczaj's tables also contain one occurrence of beed, but Kuczaj did not tally
other uses of be, was, or were, presumably because they could also be forms
of the auxiliary be; this would also account for why have-has-had and do-did
are absent. Our analyses of Abe thus omit all three verbs. Kuczaj reported
his data in two ways: in a table listing the number of correct and incorrect
forms, summed over verbs, for each month (his table 18) and in an appendix
listing how many times each individual verb, in each of its possible forms,
was used each month (his app. G). Occasionally, there were discrepancies
between these two tables that we could not resolve from the text of the
thesis, so we relied on tallies of the raw data from Kuczaj's investigatio
provided to us by Michael Maratsos. Data from the months 2-5-2-7 and f
four no-change verbs were absent from these tallies; for them we used t
thesis tables exclusively.
For Adam, Eve, and Sarah, verb usages were tallied on a DEC Microva
II running UNIX. Individual transcript files were combined into a sing
master file for each child. The FREQ program in the CLAN software packag
(MacWhinney, 1990; MacWhinney & Snow, 1990) counts the number o
times every word is used in a particular transcript session, for a particu
speaker, and it was run on the individual transcript files and the combin
files. The combined frequency list for each child was then edited to include
only words that the child may have used as a verb, including words th
occur only infrequently as a verb (e.g., fish, color, ground, milk). For all su
items (i.e., all words that are not exclusively verbs in the child's vocabulary)
26
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
the UNIX utility FGREP, which finds matches of alphanumeric string patterns,
extracted all the transcript lines they occurred in. Each of the resulting lines
was checked by hand and excluded if the matched word turned out not to
be used as a verb. If a word appeared in a single-word utterance, it was
excluded; thus, sleep or put appearing alone were not counted as verbs, but
Adam sleep or put Mommy were included. Verbs repeated in successive senten-
ces such as I falled down. I falled down, Momma were counted separately since
children are capable of saying both a correct and an overregularized version
of a single verb in successive utterances, as in Abe's Daddy comed and said
"hey, what are you doing laying down?" And then a doctor came.
For all instances of Adam, Eve, and Sarah's no-change verbs such as
cut and put, past tense usages were distinguished from present and infinitival
usages by hand; when the transcript did not provide information regarding
the verb's tense, it was inferred from the context. Contractions such as
gimme, gonna, I'm, it's, and doesn't were excluded, as were participles such as
broken or gone and the quasi-modal used to. A very small number of mimicked
utterances at early ages, regular participles, and irregular participles that
are identical to past tense forms may have been included. Intentionally
included were verbs that were not very clearly uttered but were clear
enough for the transcriber to have made a reasonable guess; so were some
slight phonetic variations such as -in for -ing, particularly for Sarah, whose
samples were transcribed more narrowly than the others. Brown (1973)
notes, however, that all three children's speech was carefully transcribed
with regard to the presence or absence of phonetic material corresponding
to inflections.
For Adam, Eve, and Sarah, have, be, and do were included only when
they were used as main verbs (i.e., possessional have, have to unless tran-
scribed as hafta, copula be, and pro-verb do), never when they were used as
auxiliaries (i.e., perfect auxiliary have, progressive and passive auxiliary be,
do used to form questions, negations, or emphatics). This is consistent with
the criteria used by Brown (1973; Brown, Cazden, & de Villiers, 1971),
Bybee and Slobin (1982a), and Slobin (1971).
For the other children, and for the Gleason, Warren-Leubecker, and
Gathercole collections, we used a Sun Microsystems Sparcstation 4 running
under UNIX to tabulate all irregular past tense utterances. Using the FREQ
program, we extracted the number of occurrences for each uniquely irregu-
lar verb listed in the Pinker and Prince (1988) appendix, together with all
forms ending in -ed, with the exception of the no-change verbs, read (which
is orthographically a no-change verb), and all forms of do, be, and have (i.e.,
neither auxiliary nor main verb usages were counted). We then isolated all
the overregularization errors from this list by removing regular verbs and
other part of speech categories. Because we did not check this large collec-
tion by hand, we were unable to exclude overregularized participles such
27
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
as in the window was broked. Since we did exclude correct participle form
they were distinct from the past tense forms (about 60 irregular verbs h
this property; see Pinker & Prince, 1988), this can result in an overestima
of overregularization rates, although it would be small. The word seed
sented particular problems since most of its uses are as a noun rather
as an overregularization of see; these were eliminated by hand. Repeti
were counted separately, as with the Brown children.9
The verb get is complicated. When adults speaking the standard di
use got in a stative possessional sense (as in I've got an ice cream cone), it is
perfect participle of get (meaning "obtain"), accompanied by some for
the auxiliary have. The meaning is possessional because of the semanti
perfect aspect in English: if the state resulting from obtaining something
the past currently holds, you possess it now (Bybee, 1985). However
children do not attend to the auxiliary, it would be natural for the
reconstrue got as a present tense form meaning "possess," and ther
numerous forms like Look, I got an ice cream cone that suggest that they
often use got as a present tense verb. For these usages we would erroneou
credit the child with the correct past form of get. Kuczaj (1976) noted
problem and used the context to distinguish present from past usage
got in Abe's speech. For Adam, Eve, and Sarah, we excluded all forms
were clearly present tense statives. For the other children, our obser
overregularization rates for get are probably underestimates, and sinc
is a frequent verb, the overall overregularization rates across verb to
will be, too. As we shall see when the verb is excluded, however, the degr
of underestimation is small.
28
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
was less than 0.2 percentage points (maximum 1.2 percentage points), with
the machine giving a higher estimate for two of the children and the human
giving a higher estimate for the other two. We also compared the overregu-
larization rates derived by the two methods across verb types. The two sets
of estimates correlated fairly well: r = .98, .77, .91, and .82 for the four
children, respectively, and the overregularization rates averaged over types
were higher when estimated by machine for two of the children, lower
for the other two (never by more than 2 percentage points). Most of the
discrepancy can be attributed to small samples for some verbs, where dis-
agreement over a single sentence can greatly affect the overregularization
rate for that verb (e.g., if a verb was used twice, once correctly and once
incorrectly, its estimated overregularization rate could change from 0% to
100% if one of the sentences is omitted or misclassified). For the children
other than Adam, excluding verbs used fewer than 10 times brings the
correlations between verbs' overregularization rates calculated by hand and
by machine to .98 or greater. Thus, in several analyses in this Monograph,
when it is important to exclude verbs that were produced too few times to
yield reliable estimates of their overregularization rates, we use a criterion
of a minimum of 10 tokens per verb per child.
For several analyses it is necessary to tabulate the frequency of use of
past tense forms in the speech of the adults that habitually talked to the
children. In all cases the same criteria for counting verbs were used for
adults and children. Although we did not distinguish among the different
adults talking to a given child (e.g., the mother, father, or psycholinguists,
who came to be treated like family members), we did keep separate tallies
for the sets of adults talking to each child, so each child has his or her own
adult speech data.
29
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
30
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
fail both to retrieve the irregular and to block the regular and might thus
feed the stem into the regular suffixation process, which ordinarily results
in an overregularization. But the regular suffixation process does not suc-
ceed 100% of the time for regular verbs, so it may occasionally fail here
too, yielding the unchanged stem. Finally, some irregular verbs actually
show no change between stem and past (e.g., hit, cut, put, set), and the child
might analogize the no-change pattern to similar irregular verbs.
During the early stages of language development, where stem forms
are the great majority of forms uttered (Brown, 1973; Cazden, 1968; see
also Chap. VI below), it is reasonable to assume that the absence of intention
to mark is the usual or exclusive cause. Although tense and agreement are
marked obligatorily in languages like English, the child cannot be born
knowing this. Thus, it is not surprising that there might be an initial period
in which children have not yet developed any mechanism for systematic
marking of any specific inflectional feature. Indeed, bare stems constitute
the great majority of young children's verb forms in English, not only in
past tense contexts, but also in progressive and third person singular con-
texts (Brown, 1973). Thus, the simplest hypothesis is that most of these
early stems represent failures to mark tense at all, not attempts that fail for
the three possible reasons listed above.
During the later stages, when the child frequently marks tense on irreg-
ular verbs, it is plausible that some of the stem errors represent attempts to
mark tense that end up unsuccessful. Luckily, we shall see in Chapter VI
that, during the time when overregularization errors occur, stem errors
become rare, so little hinges on whether or not one looks at an index that
includes them. Where relevant, we discuss these possible effects.
31
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Note finally that our study excludes two kinds of errors that are some-
times lumped with overregularization but that are logically distinct from it
In languages with richer inflectional systems than English, children of
inflect a stem with an affix that is incorrect for some feature of the word
such as gender or animacy, or incorrect for some feature of the conte
such as case or definiteness (Pinker, 1984; Slobin, 1973). Pluralizing m
nouns (e.g., waters) or extending the third person singular suffix to oth
persons or numbers (e.g., we walks) would be examples in English. Suc
errors are best characterized as underdifferentiation-an insensitivity to som
10 Brown et al. (1971, p. 2) define obligatory contexts as including "adverbs like yester
day, marginal notations [comments by the transcriber], expansions [by adults], continu
of tense, etc."; attempts by the child to imitate adult sentences containing tensed for
were also counted.
32
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
33
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
IV. THE RATE OF OVERREGULARIZATION
34
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
35
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
TABLE 2
36
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
16
14
12
T 10
_0
-o
%0"8
6-
z
Z
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Overregularization Rate
FIG. 1.-Histogram of overregulari
Hypothetical C
100
" 90
80
_ 80
o
70
u 60
50
40
30
20
10
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Adam
100
2 90
0o 80
S70
u 60
- 50
c 40
30
20
i 10
Eve
100
O 90
S80
70
60
50o
c 40
a 30
20
2 lO
10
0 01.
1-6 2-0
Age
FIG. 4.-Percentage of Eve's irregular past tense forms that are correct (100% minus
the overregularization rate).
38
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Sarah
100
90
o 80
u 70
u 60
i-50
S40
30
? 20
N 10
2-0 2-6 3-0 3-6 4-0 4-6 5-0
Age
FIG. 5.-Percentage of Sarah's irregular past tense forms that are correct (100%
minus the overregularization rate).
Abe
100
? 90
o 80
u 70
60
50
S40
30
20
S10
0 '
2-0 2-6 3-0 3-6 4-0 4-6 5-0
Age
FIG. 6.-Percentage of Abe's irregular past tense forms that are correct (100% minus
the overregularization rate).
39
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Eve, whose samples end at 2-3, we see fairly steady overregularization from
some time in the third year at least into the sixth year. Only for Abe is ther
any hint of a reduction in overregularization rate late in development.
The graphs demonstrate that low rates characterize the entire period
of overregularization. Adam's highest monthly rate of overregularizing i
only 6.8%; Eve's is 23.1% and Sarah's 15.8%. Even Abe, an outlier among
the 25 children, displayed an overregularization rate of only 47.6% in hi
most extreme month (see Fig. 6); this is the highest rate we find among 109
monthly estimates from the four children who were examined longitudi
nally."I
" Note that it would not even be sound to conclude that Abe went through a stage
at which his overregularization rate was 47.6%, because of the statistical phenomenon of
regression to the mean. This particular month was chosen post hoc because of its high
rate. Any monthly estimate reflects a sum of the child's true overregularization rate (a
psychological tendency that could be measured as how frequently the child would overreg-
ularize over a very large number of comparable opportunities at that age) and an error
component (from sampling and other sources of noise independent of the true rate). By
deliberately selecting the month with the maximum observed overregularization rate, we
are simultaneously selecting for samples with high true overregularization rates and for
samples with high noise in the direction of overregularization. The true rate, therefore,
would be expected to be lower than the sampled rate in such months. For similar reasons
it would not be sound to point to Abe's data from 4-8 and conclude that he went through
a stage in which his overregularization rate was zero.
40
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
Chap. III, Sec. IV). The previous section showed that when overregulariz-
ations appear, they neither predominate nor alternate with the correct ir-
regulars but are always a minority. However, one can still test whether a
period of extremely accurate performance precedes the first overregu-
larizations-in other words, whether children at some point in development
get worse (even if they never get very bad).
Our sample includes nine children with extended longitudinal data.
For two of them, Abe and Nathaniel, the first transcript contains an overreg-
ularization. If these transcripts happen to coincide exactly with the boys'
very first uses of irregular past tense forms, the transcripts show that they
did not undergo U-shaped development. But it is more likely that the tran-
scripts began well after irregulars of one or both kinds were being pro-
duced, in which case the transcripts are uninformative. Note that, for the
same reason, any set of transcripts that does not begin early enough in the
child's language development is apt to underestimate the length of any
overregularization-free sequence in which irregular pasts are used.
For the other seven children, one does see correct irregulars in the
transcripts before the appearance of the first overregularization (not sur-
prisingly, since three of them-Adam, Eve, and Sarah-were the ones stud-
ied by Cazden). However, the impression of a developmental change could
be a sampling artifact. Since we now know that children's overall overregu-
larization rate is low, their tendency to overregularize could be unchanged
throughout development, but their early samples might simply be too small
to contain any examples of overregularization. Imagine drawing playing
cards from a deck with replacement, looking for a black king (whose fre-
quency in the deck is approximately equal to children's overregularization
rate). One might have to draw a large number of cards before the first one
appears, even if the deck is complete and properly shuffled.
It is not legitimate to test for a change in rate by comparing overregu-
larization rates before and after the first overregularization because the post
hoc nature of the dividing line will inflate the chances of obtaining a spuri-
ous difference. One stringent test can be conducted as follows. If the child's
overregularization rate is p, then, under the null hypothesis of no change
in this rate over time, the chance that the first irregular verb form in the
sample will be correct is 1 - p. If the likelihood of a child overregularizing
an utterance is unaffected by whether the child overregularized the previ-
ous past tense utterance (an assumption we will examine below), then the
chance that the first two utterances will both be correct is (1 - p)2, the
chance that the first three will be correct is (1 - p)3, and so on. One can
test whether there is an improbable run of consecutive correct irregular
past tenses at the beginning of a child's records by calculating (1 - p)",
where n is the number of irregulars in the transcripts preceding the tran-
script containing the first overregularization. (Correct forms preceding the
41
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
TABLE 3
FIRST
OVERREGULAR-
IZATION CONSECUTIVE
CORRECT IN OVERREGULARIZATION
CHILD Age Sample PRECEDING SAMPLES RATE PROBABILITY
first overregularization
including them would corr
the child had brought the
of that very session.)
Table 3 shows the result
of obtaining a string of co
the early overregularizatio
is very small (.0012 or les
no conclusions about U-sh
regularization, although c
appear substantially befo
overregularization rates.
single sample in a meta-a
bility of observing such lo
the null hypothesis. Follow
individual probabilities t
root of the number of c
score back to probability
their sentences, avoids an
of obtaining the data unde
rate is .000045.
42
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
43
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
The data in Appendix Tables Al-A4 confirm that the overall rate of co
performance does not decline notably with age for any of the children. B
it is not clear that anyone in the psycholinguistics literature has ever clai
that it did, so the point of Stemberger's objection is unclear.
Plunkett and Marchman (1990, 1991) refer to yet another sense of
term "U-shaped development" in arguing for the psychological realit
their connectionist models. In Plunkett and Marchman (1991), the lear
curves all start out at levels of performance far less than 100% and t
increase; the authors call the small wiggles in this overall increasing c
"U-shaped development." Although all the children we examine show
ups and downs in their monthly measures of overregularization rates, the
are many explanations of these blips, of which sampling error is the
plest.13
Finally, in Plunkett and Marchman (1990), any verb that is used once
correctly and then once incorrectly is characterized as undergoing U-shaped
development, which is misleading for a different reason: any stationary
stochastic process (e.g., a string of coin flips) will produce local sequences
with such patterns. Neither the fact that children produce such sequences
nor the ability of their model to do so is surprising.
13 Kruschke (1990, p. 61) makes the point a slightly different way: "The recent work
of Plunkett and Marchman [1991] does not exhibit U-shaped learning, contrary to their
claims. They showed that acquisition fluctuated depending on the particular training
sequence, but they failed to mention that on average their model showed monotonic, not
U-shaped, acquisition."
44
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
ment (the slope of the best-fitting straight line is + 6.0 percentage points
per year), superimposed on seemingly random fluctuations that leave many
late months with higher overregularization rates than early ones. Clearly,
overregularization diminishes extremely gradually. On the basis of a per-
sonal communication from Slobin, Kuczaj (1977a) notes that it is still present
in school-age children of 9 or 10.
Two studies provide us with estimates of the overregularization rate in
older children. Moe, Hopkins, and Rush (1982) report a sample of 10,530
irregular past tense utterances from over 329 first graders. The overregu-
larization rate in their data is 2.8%. Carlton (1947) reports 2,196 past tense
tokens among the speech of 96 fourth graders she recorded. These included
13 overregularizations. If approximately 75% of past tense tokens in speech
are irregular (see Chap. V), the overregularization rate for this group is
0.8%.
Do overregularization errors ever completely disappear? Joseph Stem-
berger has kindly provided us with the full set of past tense overregular-
izations in his corpus of 7,500 adult speech errors. The list includes 25 past
tense overregularizations (18 of the stem, 7 of the irregular past form);
Stemberger (1989) suggests that the rate of adult speech errors might be
about one error per 1,000 sentences. If we assume that all sentences contain
verbs, that about 10% of verbs in casual speech are in the past tense (Adams,
1938; Smith, 1935), and that 75% of adults' verb tokens are irregular
(Slobin, 1971; see Chap. V), we get a very crude estimate of adults' overreg-
ularization rate of .00004-three orders of magnitude lower than pre-
schoolers' and two orders lower than fourth graders'.
So although in one sense both children and adults overregularize, there
is also a dramatic difference in their rates of doing so. Perhaps the differ-
ence is just a consequence of hearing more tokens of each irregular verb as
one lives longer, with more exposures leading to more reliably accessible
memory traces. For example, a negative exponential learning curve with a
time constant of one order of magnitude of improvement in retrieval proba-
bility for every 5 years' worth of irregular past tense tokens could handle
the reported overregularization rates from the preschool years through
adulthood comfortably. In the absence of more plentiful and finer-grained
data, it is premature to claim that there is no qualitative difference between
children and adults, but current evidence does not demand that there be a
difference.
45
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
46
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
that parents use in the past tense less often are more likely to be overregular-
ized by their children.
It is important to note that the correlation between verb overregulariza-
tion rate and verb frequency necessarily interacts with any cutoff designed
to eliminate small samples. The verbs that parents use less often are also
the verbs that their children use less often (see Chap. VII). Therefore,
excluding verbs likely to give rise to sampling errors will also tend to exclude
verbs with higher overregularization rates. Appendix Tables A5-A8 con-
firm that many infrequently used verbs yield high estimated overregulariza-
tion rates. In fact, computing the unweighted mean of the overregulariza-
tion rates of all verb types yields figures of 22% for Adam, 17% for Eve,
18% for Sarah, and 47% for Abe; the mean of the type means for all 25
children is 10.5%, the median 11%. One might wonder whether the rela-
tively high type mean for Abe when it is calculated over all his verb types
casts doubt on the conclusion that his grammatical system suppresses over-
regularization. Such doubts are not warranted. Means calculated over types
do not reflect the functioning of the child's grammatical system because a
verb used once is weighted exactly as strongly as a verb used a thousand
times. Rather, what the typewise mean is capturing is the distribution of
frequencies of the child's irregular verb vocabulary: the more low-frequency
verbs a child uses, the higher the mean. In fact, although the theory invok-
ing blocking and retrieval failure predicts low overregularization rates over-
all, it also predicts high overregularization rates for low-frequency verbs:
100% for verbs that have never been heard or attended to in the past tense
form (like shend for adults), lower but still high rates for verbs that have
been heard occasionally, and so on (see Chap. II). In a sense, a child's
seldom-used verbs are misleading for two reasons: we cannot be confident
about the actual overregularization rate of an irregular verb used once or
twice, and a child cannot be confident that a verb is irregular if he or she
had heard it used only once or twice.
Irregular verbs are, quite generally, overregularized at low rates, but
the verbs themselves can be very different from one another. This suggests
that the overall low overregularization rate reflects some phenomenon that
suppresses overregularization globally across the irregulars (modulo fre-
quency) and is unlikely to be attributable to any general bias of the child to
favor correct irregulars because of some lexical property that many of them
happen to possess. For example, it might be argued that children's gram-
matical systems allowed both overregularizations and correct irregulars but
that they had a response bias in favor of producing irregulars because they
tend to be shorter and simpler than overregularizations and that this is the
cause of the measured overregularization rate being less than 50%. Such a
counterexplanation can easily be ruled out. Among the irregular verbs that
our subjects used, 14 have correct past tense forms that are the same length
47
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Adam
30
25
2 20
w15
E
z
10
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Overregularization Rate
FIG. 7.-Histogram of overregularizat
per verb).
7
Eve
cn
04
E3-
z
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Overregularization Rate
FIG. 8.-Histogram of overregularization
verb).
48
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Sarah
20-
15-
0
0>
5-
0-
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Overregularization Rate
FIG. 9.-Histogram of overregularization rates of Sarah's verbs (10 or more tok
per verb).
7
Abe
6-
5-
4-
E
z
31-
0
0-
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Overregularization Rate
FIG. 10.-Histogram of overregularization rates of Abe's verbs (10 or more tokens
per verb).
49
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
There are other possible interactions among children, verb, and age
that would be noteworthy. One can determine whether any verbs cease to
be overregularized altogether before the end of the period or, alternatively,
whether a child begins to stop overregularizing all verbs at the same time;
the latter finding would suggest that the child learns or develops the
blocking principle only at that point. Another possibility is that various verbs
follow different and largely unsystematic patterns, perhaps because, as we
have suggested, overregularization is a quasi-random performance deficit.
Clearly, low rates of overregularization are not an artifact of a sequence
50
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
Hypothetical Child
90
0 80
70
0
u 60
S50
' 40
0.
S30 I
20
210
0
Young Old
Age
FIG. 11.-Hypothetical developmental sequence for different verbs that would yield
low overregularization rates as an averaging artifact.
51
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
For most of the 70 irregular verbs that Abe used, the curves can best
be described as chaotic and highly variable from verb to verb. They are
most conveniently summarized by pointing out four rough patterns, shown
in Figures 12-15. Some verbs, like eat (Fig. 12), are overregularized in the
earlier transcripts but appear to be completely mastered before the end of
the sampling period. (Other verbs with this pattern include cut, fall, go, make,
think, and throw.) A second class of verbs, such as say (Fig. 13), is rarely
overregularized at any point. (Find, forget, see, and tell are others.) A few,
such as draw (Fig. 14), are overregularized throughout the sample (build is
similar); such verbs were used only rarely and might be overregularized
because they are also rare in parental speech, although sampling error can-
not be ruled out. But many verbs, such as win (Fig. 15), bite, break, blow, buy,
catch, come, feel, get, know, put, and shoot, show no interpretable trend, oscillat-
ing between samples with high and low measured overregularization.
In sum, apart from haphazard variation (possibly due to sampling er-
ror) and overall low or high rates (to be discussed in Chap. VII), the only
meaningful temporal pattern for individual verbs seen in Abe's data is that
a few appear to be mastered in the late 4s, thereafter resisting overregular-
ization completely.
Given that the overregularization rate varies across verbs and, to a lesser
extent, across children and ages, there is no absolute "overregularization
rate"; the estimates will change somewhat with methodological decisions of
which verbs, children, and ages to include. The low estimates in Section I
above included all the samples meeting the criteria described in Chapter
III, without any exclusions based on how the data came out. Still, it is
conceivable that these criteria adventitiously included some samples of ques-
tionable representativeness, relevance, or accuracy. In this section, we will
test the robustness of the low estimated overregularization rate, by eliminat-
ing all samples that could be challenged, for any plausible reason, as poten-
tially deflating the measured overregularization rates. If these worst-case
estimates continue to be low after steps are taken to bias them as high as
possible, one can have more confidence that there is some real process
causing them to be low and that the effect is not an artifact of the adoption
of one selection criterion or another.
First, for some of the children the samples are small, and we may have
caught them in conversations where few irregular verbs were used in the
past tense, or perhaps only a small number of well-mastered ones. Hence,
52
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
we can eliminate all children who contributed less than 100 irregular verb
tokens to the overall sample.
Second, because of possible U-shaped developmental sequences, some
of the youngest children sampled may not have entered the stage at which
overregularization had begun. If so, it would be misleading to count their
samples from the pre-overregularization period (with rates of 0%, by defi-
nition) because the psychological process of interest was not yet occurring.
Moreover, even if overregularization had begun, it may not have reached
its peak. So, following the suggestion of one of the referees of an earlier
draft, one might question the inclusion of Allison, April, Nat, and Peter,
none of whom were recorded any later than 3-1. These children could not
have been excluded a priori because Abe had his worst month of overregu-
larizing at 2-10 and Eve overregularized at 1-8. But let us assume that
these four children were on the verge of overregularizing at high rates and
exclude their data. We will continue to include Eve, even though she falls
within the age range of the children excluded here for being too young;
this decision renders overregularization estimates higher than they would
be if she were excluded. The rationale might be that her language develop-
ment was precocious and hence her true overregularization tendency was
indeed manifested in the available samples.
Third, we might be sampling overregularization-shy children from the
opposite arm of the U, where the errors have already disappeared. The
Hall et al. (1984) children might be suspect for these reasons. (In fact, data
from the children we analyzed in longitudinal detail-Adam, Sarah, and
Abe-do not themselves call for such an exclusion because, while Abe
showed an overall decline in the age range corresponding to the Hall et al.
children, he was still overregularizing at rates from 0% to 22%, and Adam's
and Sarah's overregularization rates increased.)
Turning now to the different samples within longitudinal records, we
can consider the fact that there was a demonstrated overregularization-free
period in the early months of Adam, Peter, and Sarah. So here we will
exclude all correct forms in the transcripts preceding the transcript in which
the first overregularization is found.
Fifth, perhaps not all verbs deserve to be included. Get could be prob-
lematic for children other than Abe, for whom Kuczaj (1976) carefully
coded all forms as the past of get versus the present of got. For Adam, Eve,
and Sarah, recall that we checked contexts and excluded present tense
forms, but some cases counted as past forms could have been present forms.
For the other children the computer counted all instances. We will report
estimates that exclude all instances of got for children other than Abe.
Sixth, one might argue that have, be, and do (included only for Adam,
Eve, and Sarah) are special. Even though we did not include any auxiliaries
in our analyses, these verbs obviously share their morphemes with the auxil-
53
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Abe: Eat
56 13 1 2 3
1001 6 3 2 8 1 5
o 90 1
LM
o 80
0
( 70"
C 60
i 5050
cc 40
0.
0
o 30
L_ 20
10
2 2
Abe: Say
5 15 13 17 12 10 10 5 4 10 9 8 3 11
1001
9 18 21 16 5 13 s
12 62 8
2 7
90
C)
21 8
m 80
0
70
0 60
50
c 40
o 30
"20
No
10
0 .....
2-0 2-6 3-0 3-6 4-0 4-6 5-0
Age
FIG. 13.-Example of a verb that is rarely overregularized at any age
54
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Abe: Draw
100
"i 90
0 . 80
70
0 60
I- 50
40
e 40
0 30
20
10
0
2-0 2-6 3-0 3-6 4-0 4-6 5-0
Age
FIG. 14.-Example of a verb that is overregularized throughout development
Abe: Win
2 2 2 4
100
- 90
o
S80
70
0 60
50
a)
e 40
o 30
20
10
3 1 4
55
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
iaries, and they are irregular not only in the past but in the third perso
singular present. Conceivably, the child might treat nonauxiliary version
of have, be, and do similarly to the auxiliary versions and differently from
other verbs. Actually, when Stromswold (1990) examined overregulariza-
tion rates for auxiliary and nonauxiliary forms of these verbs in a large
sample of ChiLDES children overlapping with the one we use here, she
found that the nonauxiliary versions-that is, the ones that we have been
counting-were overregularized at rates comparable to those of the other
verbs that we report here. Counting correct forms from the children wh
made overregularization errors during the developmental spans in which
they made them (a procedure that yields slightly higher estimates than the
ones we report), Stromswold obtained overregularization estimates of 2.5
for have, 4.3% for be, and 7.5% for do (see also App. Tables A5-A10). But
again we will report the effects of excluding them.
Table 4 summarizes the results of various exclusions on the estimated
"14 Moreover, the shape of the developmental curves for Adam, Eve, and Sarah
virtually unchanged, and the tests of U-shaped development reported in Sec. IIA of C
IV all remain significant.
56
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
TABLE 4
57
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
58
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
10.4% for Sarah) were less than their rates of inflecting regular verbs
(66%-97%) and that this was true for 32 of the 33 individual months
available.
This analysis shows that when children fail to use an irregular past
tense form in a past tense context, they do not treat it identically to a regular
verb; they overregularize the irregular verb far less often than they correctly
suffix a regular verb. In other words, irregular verbs tend to suppress appli-
cation of the regular process even when they are not supplied in the correct
irregular past tense form. Assuming that there is no consistent reason why
children should decide to mark tense on irregular verbs less often than on
regular ones, there are two plausible explanations for this difference. One
is that the child is analogizing the no-change pattern seen in verbs like cut
and set to other irregular verbs. Later, we review abundant evidence that
verbs that end in t or d are particularly susceptible to this interference.
Second, as mentioned in note 7 above, retrieval of an irregular may not be
all or none. On some occasions, the child may not retrieve the content of
an irregular entry (or may not retrieve enough of it to allow the form to be
articulated) but might retrieve the information that a past tense entry exists
(i.e., the child might retrieve only the pointer or tag to the entry). Since it
is the existence of a past tense entry that ordinarily blocks regularization
(rather than the particular phonetic content of any irregular form), such
piecemeal retrieval would be sufficient to prevent an overregularization
without actually supplying the correct irregular form.
59
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
in a published report (Kuczaj, 1977a), and these sources list a much low
range of overregularization rates calculated over uses: .01-.39 for diffe
children and .12-.24 for different subclasses of verbs.'5 Breaking down
rates according to individual children's performance with different s
classes (many obviously with tiny sample sizes) gives a wider range,
the range runs from 0.00 to 1.00, not from .20 to .60. Maratsos (pers
communication, October 1991) has explained to us that he in fact der
these estimates from Kuczaj's unpublished data by averaging overregu
ization rates over verb types for each month, not "uses," as his paper
the range of monthly typewise means ran from .16 to .69, with the bulk
the months falling roughly within the .20-.60 range that he reports.
discussed in Section III above, averages over types should be systemati
higher than the true overregularization rate because they overweight
rare and hence difficult verbs.
"15 These figures, taken from Kuczaj's (1977a) tables, refer to overregularizations as a
proportion of total usages of irregular verbs in past tense contexts, including unmarke
stems. Since most of the children's errors were overregularizations, not stems, the figure
for overregularization rates as a proportion of overtly marked verbs are similar, rangin
from .01 to .40.
60
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
irregular past tense forms and 9 overregularizations and had a mean over-
regularization rate of 8.4%. Of the three children in the fourth group (mean
age 2-7, MLU 4.22), one child produced 15 overregularizations of a single
verb, but the other 32 irregular past tokens were correct; the children's
mean was 13.2%. The mean overregularization rate of the children in the
study was 5.1%.
The ChiLDES archive contains a number of transcript sets that we
excluded for the methodological reasons listed in Chapter III. However, it
is of some interest to check whether they are consistent with the samples that
we did report. Among the transcript sets that are from Standard American
English-speaking children and in CHAT format, there were three sets of
transcripts from individual children and six group transcripts, ranging in
size from 5 to 1,596 irregular past tense tokens (median 54). The mean
overregularization rate of the three individual children was 12.5% (range
5.1%-16.7%). The six group data bases showed overregularization rates
ranging from 0% to 10.8%, with a mean of 3.25%.
The only partial anomaly in the empirical literature consists of the data
reported by Kuczaj (1976, 1977a). Qualitatively, those data are consistent
with our finding of a pervasive bias against overregularizing: neither the
mean overregularization rate for the cross-sectional sample nor that for Abe
comes close to the rates of 50% or higher that had been assumed in the
literature. But there are quantitative differences. As we have seen, Abe is
the outlier from among the 25 English-speaking children we studied in the
ChiLDES data base. Kuczaj's cross-sectional sample (1977a) of 14 children
from 2-6 to 5-6 includes six with overregularization rates comparable to
those reported here (1.1%-8.33%), but for the other eight the rate ranged
from 26.1% to 40.2%, with an average over all the children of 20.9%. Thus,
neither Abe nor most of the 14 children in Kuczaj's cross-sectional sample
appear to come from the distribution of unselected children we analyzed
(most of whom had very similar backgrounds to Kuczaj's subjects). But Abe
and the cross-sectional sample of children appear to come from the same
distribution. What Abe and the sample have in common is that they were
both studied by Kuczaj, as part of the only investigation we discuss that
recorded children's speech expressly for the purpose of studying verbal
inflection. Could there be some systematic factor in Kuczaj's methods that
would push estimated overregularization rates upward?
According to his dissertation, Kuczaj (1976, p. 5) selected his 14 chil-
dren from a larger sample of 23, according to the criteria of "(1) clarity of
speech, (2) willingness to play games (in particular, to imitate model senten-
ces when asked), and (3) willingness of the parents to aid the investigator by
participating as experimenters (that is, by playing games with their child)."
Among the ways that the parents aided Kuczaj were supplying the child
with erroneous feedback, choosing the times to turn the tape recorder on
61
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
and off, and asking the child to talk about specific events that took place in
the past and about specific hypothetical events. (This was another attemp
to elicit past tense forms because in English, as in many languages, the
inflection called "past tense" is also used to express hypothetical events
certain contexts, as in I wish I knew the answer or If I won the lottery.) As Kucz
points out, "The recorded conversations may then not be representativ
samples of normal conversation" (p. 8).
Kuczaj does not mention whether, in recording Abe, he tended to us
the same manipulations that he instructed the parents of his cross-sectional
sample to engage in. The transcripts themselves suggest that he may hav
We tested for a conversational style involving a high degree of past tens
elicitations in Abe's transcripts. Using the CLAN program COMBO, we
searched for occurrences in Abe's parents' speech of three representativ
sequences of words that would tend to call for past tense forms in subs
quent child utterances: what did, what'd, and happened. Abe's parents use
these forms in 1.9% of their utterances, more frequently than 21 of th
other 24 sets of adults in our samples, whose mean was 1.1%.16
It is unclear whether Kuczaj's selection criteria (or the parent's criteri
for when to begin recording) adventitiously correlated with tendencies t
overregularize. However, the instructions to parents to elicit past tens
forms from their children could have elevated overregularization rates, for
reasons we examine in the following section.
There are three reasons to think that efforts to elicit past tense forms
from children might cause them to overregularize more than they would in
their spontaneous speech.
First, if children have any tendency to avoid forms in their spontaneous
speech that they are uncertain of (in particular, the past tense form), then
when the parent, rather than the child, chooses the verb, by asking a leading
question, errors may be more likely. One example might be the followin
dialogue between Abe and his mother (from his 98th sample):
16 Indeed, two of the children whose parents exceeded Abe's in past tense elicitations,
Allison and Nat, were considerably younger, and younger children tend to be prompted
more often in conversation than older children (see Marcus, 1992).
62
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
Father. Abe do you remember what all you did yesterday? See if
you can tell me what all you did yesterday.
Abe. We played the new games we fixed the wagon and we opened
gifts.
Father. What did you do outside?
Abe. Hide. [Played hide and seek.]
Father. You hide? you hid?
Abe. Uhhuh and I count.
Father. You counted?
Abe. Uhhuh.
Mother. Abe was it ... and he counted and came and looked for
US.
Father. What would have happened if they couldn't have found any
water?
"17 Furthermore, providing the stem may have actively suppressed retrieval of the
irregular version. Presenting an adult subject with a subset of a category of remembered
words can impede retrieval of the rest (Slamecka, 1969). We thank Endel Tulving for
pointing this out to us.
63
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
stem form. The effect might underlie exchanges like this one in Abe's 150th
transcript:
64
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
immediate prior occurrence in the parent's prompt. Tests of this effect were
inconclusive: Adam's and Eve's samples contain more overregularizations
of a verb after the parent had just used its stem; Sarah's contained fewer.
In sum, there are reasons to believe that the higher overregularization
rates among the children Kuczaj studied, including Abe, are partly arti-
factual. The data are, as Kuczaj noted, not "representative samples of nor-
mal conversation"; rather, they appear to be part way between naturalistic
speech and experimentally elicited speech. Our analyses of discourse effects
on overregularization, together with our review of data from actual elicita-
tion experiments in the next section, suggest that Kuczaj's methodology
might have led to systematic overestimates of children's spontaneous rate
of overregularization.
Note that we are not suggesting that the entire difference between
Kuczaj's subjects and those in the rest of the literature is artifactual; in
Chapter VI, we will examine the possible effects of high levels of marking
tense on overregularization, a difference between Kuczaj's subjects and the
other children that is of some theoretical interest. Note as well that these
analyses are an attempt to resolve the discrepancy between Kuczaj's data
and the others in the literature and should not be interpreted as a criticism
of Kuczaj's methodology in general. Three of the four hypotheses he was
testing in Kuczaj (1977a) had nothing to do with overall overregularization
rates; rather, they compared the two different kinds of overregularization
errors (eated and ated) and the relative overregularization rates of verbs with
different properties. For these purposes there were good reasons to have
sought samples with large numbers of past tense forms, just as we focus on
Abe in our investigation of the time course of individual verbs in Section
IV above.19 It is only when treating Kuczaj's data as estimates of children's
overall overregularization rates in spontaneous speech that the nonrepre-
sentativeness of the samples must be taken into account.
19 Kuczaj's tests of his fourth hypothesis, however, may have been affected by his
methodology, although of course he was not aware of the effects we now point out. Kuczaj
disagreed with Brown's (1973) claim that irregular past tense marking was acquired before
regular past tense marking, which Brown had based on differences in the relative ages at
which the two kinds of marking were supplied in obligatory past tense contexts more than
90% of the time. Kuczaj noted that the finding might depend on whether overregular-
izations had been counted as incorrect forms or ignored when the percentages were tallied,
which had not been made clear in Brown (1973). (In fact, they were ignored; Brown et
al., 1971.) Kuczaj counted the overregularizations twice: as incorrect irregular past tense
forms and as correct regular past tense forms. The resulting percentages of correct use
in obligatory contexts showed an advantage to regular marking, contrary to Brown's claim.
Thus, Kuczaj's "nonreplication" was partly just due to differences in definitions, but it was
also due to how these differences interact with his special recording circumstances. Be-
cause these circumstances may have led to unusually high overregularization rates, Kuc-
zaj's estimates of irregular marking were thereby depressed and his estimates of regular
marking inflated.
65
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
66
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
67
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
almost always identical to their own choice and thus is not an independent
data set.) Of these 15 comparisons, only one involved a failure to discrimi-
nate irregulars from overregularizations: the middle age group (5-7) in
experiment 2 preferred past overregularizations over irregulars or stem
overregularizations in the forced choice task (although they did not even
produce many such forms in the elicited production task, as noted above).
In other words, the judgment data confirm that children systematically fa-
vor irregular forms as the preferred past tense version of irregular verbs
(see also Lachter & Bever, 1988).
More recently, Cox (1989, p. 204) told children that a puppet "was
learning to talk but was having trouble with some of his words," and the
child was asked to help him say the correct words. Twelve sentences, each
with an overregularized noun or verb, were provided. Children were not
asked to judge the sentences, and there were no correct irregulars among
the experimental stimuli, so we cannot assess children's discrimination abili-
ties from the data. Correction performance was surprisingly poor: none of
the six sentences with verbs was corrected by more than 16% of the children
around the age of 5, and none of the six sentences with nouns was corrected
by more than 28%, except, inexplicably, tooths. Since, as Cox herself notes,
the children who failed to correct an overregularization did not necessarily
use it themselves, a response bias against correcting the puppet too often is
a likelier explanation than an absence of knowledge, especially since she
told the children that the puppet was having trouble with only "some" of
his words but presented no sentences that were actually correct.
Finally, the consistent findings that overregularization rates are low and
that irregulars are preferred to them help explain the otherwise paradoxical
phenomenon that children who have been observed to overregularize will
vehemently correct their parents when they mimic the children's errors
(Bever, 1975; Slobin, 1978; see also Lachter & Bever, 1988). Similarly, Ervin
and Miller (1963) noted that their subjects often corrected their own over-
regularizations; we do not know of any reports of children correcting one
of their irregular past tense utterances to an overregularization.
X. SUMMARY
68
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
69
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
V. THE RELATION OF OVERREGULARIZATION TO CHANGES IN
THE NUMBER AND PROPORTION OF REGULAR VERBS IN
PARENTS' SPEECH AND CHILDREN'S VOCABULARY
70
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
past tense form with the correct version provided by a "teacher," and it
adjusts the strengths of the connections and the thresholds so as to reduce
the difference between the actual state of each output node and the correct
state. By this process of recording and superimposing contingencies be-
tween bits of sounds of stems (e.g., the distinctive features of endings such
as -op or -ing) and bits of sounds of past tense forms (e.g., the features of
-opped and -ang), the model improves its performance over time, and it can
generalize to new forms on the basis of their featural overlap with old ones.
The model contains nothing corresponding to a word or rule and thus
makes no qualitative distinction between regular and irregular mappings;
both are effected by connections between stem sounds and past sounds.
Rumelhart and McClelland's explanation of the sequence of overregu-
larization flowed from the ways in which parallel distributed processing
(PDP) models generalize. In building such a model, there are numerous
ways to bias it toward conservative recording of individual input items,
toward liberal overgeneralization according to frequent patterns, or some
combination. The challenge was to duplicate the child's transition from
conservatism to overgeneralization in a single model. Rumelhart and
McClelland proposed a simple and ingenious hypothesis. Not only are irreg-
ular verbs high in frequency, but the reverse is true as well: the verbs highest
in frequency are irregular. For example, the top 10 verbs in Kucera and
Francis's (1967) frequency list are all irregular. If children acquire verbs
in order of decreasing frequency, they will develop a vocabulary with an
increasing proportion of regular verbs as they begin to run out of the high-
frequency irregulars and encounter more and more regular verbs. In partic-
ular, Rumelhart and McClelland assumed that, at some point in develop-
ment, the child shows "explosive" vocabulary growth, which would result in
a sudden influx of a large number of regular verbs. Because the regular
pattern will be exemplified by many different verbs, the learning procedure
will strengthen many links between stem features and the features defining
the -ed ending. The effects of these newly modified link weights could over-
whelm the existing weights on the links between idiosyncratic features of
irregular stems and the idiosyncratic features of their pasts, resulting in
overregularization. As the irregulars continue to be processed, the discrep-
ancies between the overregularized and teacher-supplied correct forms will
be registered, and the crucial idiosyncratic links will be strengthened over
time, eventually allowing the irregular forms to reappear.
Given these assumptions, Rumelhart and McClelland were able to
model the developmental sequence with one additional assumption: the
vocabulary explosion occurs after the child has just acquired his 10th verb.
Their 10-verb decision results in two training phases. First, the model is
presented with the 10 highest-frequency verbs (excluding do and be, which
can also be auxiliaries), of which only two (20%) happen to be regular,
71
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
10 times apiece. Then the model is presented with that list plus the 41
next-most-frequent verbs, constituting a set in which 80% of the verbs are
now regular, 190 times apiece. In phase 1, the model learned the 10 verb
successfully; when phase 2 begins on the eleventh cycle and it is suddenl
swamped with regulars, the model overregularizes the irregulars. The re
covery process begins immediately, reaching asymptote shortly before th
200th epoch (see Figs. 16 and 17, taken from Rumelhart & McClelland,
1986).
The Rumelhart-McClelland model challenges the traditional account
of overregularization, which depended on separate rote and rule mecha
nisms, in favor of a single mechanism that begins to overregularize because
of an influx of newly acquired regular verbs, a presumed consequence of
vocabulary growth spurt. Let us call this explanation of the cause of overreg-
ularization the vocabulary balance hypothesis; it is also a feature of the more
recent network simulations by Plunkett and Marchman (1990).
Pinker and Prince (1988; see also Prince & Pinker, 1988) examined
Rumelhart and McClelland's assumptions about development. Rumelhart
and McClelland cited Brown (1973) in support of their assumption of a
vocabulary spurt near the onset of overregularization, but Brown did no
discuss vocabulary acquisition at all. According to standard sources (see,
e.g., Ingram, 1989) children's "word spurt" usually occurs at 1-6, about
year too early to account for the onset of overregularization for most chil-
dren, which occurred at a mean age of 2-5 for the seven children examined
in Chapter IV. Pinker and Prince examined Brown's (n.d.) vocabulary lists
for Adam, Eve, and Sarah, which were drawn from five evenly spaced sam-
ples spanning the overregularization sequence, plus a fourth child in the
one-word stage. They found neither an explosive growth in vocabulary near
the onset of overregularization nor, more significant, an increase in the
percentage of the child's vocabulary samples that was regular: the propor
tion regular stayed around 50% before, during, and after the onset. Pinke
and Prince also cited data (partly from Slobin, 1971) suggesting that th
proportion of regular verb tokens among all verb tokens in parental speech
to children is about 20%-30% during overregularization, nowhere near
the 80% proportion that Rumelhart and McClelland used to override th
irregular patterns. They argued that an endogenous transition from rote
to rule is still required to account for the data, as in the traditional account.
Pinker and Prince's (1988) data showing that the proportion of regular
verb types in children's longitudinal samples' stays at around 50% seem
paradoxical at first: if there are only 180 irregular verbs and thousands o
72
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
0
1.0
Q)
Regular
0.9 -
r12
0.8 Irregular
Q)
0.7
0.6
0.5
0 40 80 120 160 200
Trials
FIG. 16.-Performance of the Rumelhart-McClelland model on regular an
verbs as a function of training epochs. The dip in the curve for irregular ver
the 10th epoch corresponds to the onset of overregularization. From Rum
McClelland (1986).
tO
1.0
0.8
Qk
0.6
C.
0.4
0.2
0.0 11 I 1
0 40 80 120 160 200
Trials
FIG. 17.-Tendency of the Rumelhart-McClelland model to overregularize
verbs as a function of training epochs. Overregularization tendency is measu
ratio of the strength of the correct irregular response to the sum of the streng
correct and the overregularized responses. Points below the line correspond to
to overregularize. From Rumelhart and McClelland (1986).
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Although the child would be hearing present and past tenses of all
kinds of verbs throughout development, we assume that he or she is
only able to learn past tenses for verbs already mastered fairly well in
the present tense. This is because the real learning environment does
not, in fact, present the child with present-tense/past-tense pairs.
Rather, it presents the child with past-tense words in sentences oc-
curring in real-world context. The child would therefore have to gener-
ate the appropriate present tense form internally with the aid of the
entire sentence and context, and this, we suppose, requires that the
74
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
child already know the present tense of the word. [Rumelhart &
McClelland, 1987, p. 222]
20 Rumelhart and McClelland refer to the input form as the "present," but the present
tense form would include an irrelevant -s affix for the third person singular; "stem" is
actually what they had in mind.
21 Lachter and Bever (1988) point out that the direction of a cause and effect relation
between possessing regular verbs and overregularization cannot completely be resolved,
even if one were to examine possession of regular verbs per se rather than mastery of the
past tense form of such verbs. The problem, they note, is that once a child has developed
the ability to generate and analyze regular past tense forms productively, he or she no
longer has to memorize such forms from parental speech. As a result, the learning events
and memory space that beforehand would have been dedicated to acquiring regular past
tense forms can now be applied to the learning of brand new verbs, and such verbs should
be acquired at a more rapid rate. We will not pursue this possibility further, although it
would be an important consideration if one were to find the relevant correlation.
75
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
The first method assumes that children produce regular and irregular
verbs in approximately the same proportions that they process regular an
irregular past tense forms in their parents' speech (i.e., it assumes that if a
child uses a verb, he or she knows it, and that children in conversation with
parents will use different verbs in roughly the same proportion as their
parents). Under this assumption, the proportion of verb tokens that are regular
in the child's speech indirectly estimates the proportion of regular learning
episodes.
Second, since the occurrence of a parental token is necessary for a
learning episode to take place, if we assume that children know a constant
proportion of the verb tokens their parents address to them, we can mea-
sure the proportion of verb tokens that are regular in the parent's speech.
In practice, Rumelhart and McClelland ignored token frequency en-
tirely in assembling the training set for their model: every verb was fed in
the same number of times, once per epoch. This assumes that a third mea-
sure, the proportion of regular verb types among all verb types, is the
relevant factor-although it is inconsistent with their psychological interpre-
tation of a learning episode, which would be driven by parental tokens.
Rather, the teaching schedule they actually modeled is more consistent with
some kind of off-line learning, fed by a preprocessor: the child takes a pass
through his entire verb lexicon, feeding each stem-past pair into the pattern
associator once per scan. If we entertain this interpretation of a learning
episode, which corresponds literally to Rumelhart and McClelland's learn-
ing schedule, rather than the token-driven interpretation they discuss, we
can test the vocabulary balance hypothesis by trying to estimate the proportion
of verb types in the child's vocabulary that are regular. Note that it is not valid to
use the percentage of a child's vocabulary that is regular as a surrogate for
the number of on-line learning episodes even if it turns out that the propor-
tion of regular verbs among parental tokens is constant. That is, one cannot
assume that the proportion of regular learning episodes is determined by
the proportion of the child's vocabulary that is regular, because this larger
regular vocabulary could correspond to a larger number of regular types
that the parent is cycling through a constant number of regular tokens
in his or her speech, leaving the proportion of regular learning episodes
constant.
76
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
First, it is clear that both type and token frequency have important
consequences. The Rumelhart-McClelland model overregularized because
it changed its connection strengths with each input pair in a direction that
reduced the discrepancy between computed and input past forms. After
the first epoch, in which the model was suddenly bombarded with regulars,
about 80% of the changes that the model made were designed to make it
more likely to generate regular forms, because 80% of the inputs were new
regulars. Many of the changed connections involved links from phonologi-
cal features that were also shared with irregulars (since most irregulars are
phonologically similar in some way to at least some regulars). Because the
network did not have enough specific feature units to register each verb on
its own set of units, the overlap was high enough that each irregular was
represented by many units whose links had just been adjusted to help pro-
duce the regular ending, and overregularization resulted. This effect would
obviously depend strongly on the number of regular types, because the
wider the range of regular forms that are fed in, the greater the probability
that a given phonological feature of an irregular verb will be shared by
some regular verb and hence develop stronger links to the incorrect regular
pattern. But this effect can also be mitigated by token frequency: if, say,
each irregular had been repeated four times for each regular (reflecting
the real-world higher token frequency of irregulars), the links that joined
features unique to the irregulars to their corresponding irregular past forms
would have been strengthened several times during the epoch to reduce
the errors with such forms, and overregularization would be less likely.
Indeed, one of the noteworthy properties of the Rumelhart-McClelland
model is its distributed phonological representation of words, with no units
dedicated to words per se (see Pinker & Prince, 1988), so there is no physical
basis for a distinction between types and tokens in the model at all. Only
feature-to-feature mappings, whether they be from a single word or a set
of similar words, are represented. The actual behavior of the model will
depend on the number of regular types, the phonological range of the
regular types, their degree of overlap with irregulars, the token frequencies
of both irregulars and regulars, and other factors. In any case, it is clear
that overregularization of an irregular does depend on the ratio of regular
to irregular tokens and hence is relevant to testing the vocabulary balance
hypothesis.
77
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Third, for the analyses in which types are being examined, it may not
be the proportion of verb types that are regular that is the relevant pre-
dictor. The problem is that the competition in pattern associators is not
between the regular pattern and a single irregular pattern shared among
all the irregulars. Rather, irregulars are different from each other, not just
from the regulars. Imagine that at one stage there are six different irregu-
lars, each with a different change (e.g., go-went, come-came, hit-hit, etc.), and
six regulars. At the next stage, there are 12 different irregulars, each with
a different change, and 12 regulars. The proportion of regulars in the
sample remains the same, but the ratio of regulars to any particular irregular
78
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
D. Summary of Tests
We will examine whether there are increases over time in the propor-
tion of verbs that are regular verbs among the child's tokens, the parents'
tokens, and the child's types and, if there are, whether such increases are
related to the child's tendency to overregularize. The relations will be tested
at two levels of temporal detail.
First, we will compare the monthly rate of increase in each vocabulary
factor for the months before the first overregularization with the monthly
rate of increase for the months during which overregularization is taking
place (i.e., the first month containing an overregularization and all the
months after it). This is an objectively specifiable dividing line but requires
some further comments. For Eve, there are a small number of tokens before
the first overregularization, and recall that there is no statistical evidence
that she in fact underwent a transition between stages (see Chap. IV, Sec.
IVA). For Sarah, the dividing line could be questioned. Brown (n.d.; see
Pinker & Prince, 1988) noted that Sarah produced a past tense of hear at
2-10 that was literally pronounced as an overregularization (heared, with a
79
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
schwa instead of the r), but he worried whether it could have been an od
pronunciation of heard. The first completely unambiguous overregulariza
tion does not occur in her transcripts for another 5 months. However, th
hypothetical pattern of distortion that Brown considered is not indepen
dently motivated by Sarah's other mispronunciations: for example, at 3-
she pronounced hurted as hahted, suggesting that if heard was intended but
mispronounced, it would have surfaced as hahd, not hea-ad (Alan Prince
personal communication, January 10, 1989). Furthermore, excluding thi
datum has the effect of weakening the evidence for the vocabulary balan
hypothesis. Therefore, we will not second-guess the transcription, and w
will count heared as Sarah's first overregularization. In any case, the firs
recorded overregularizations of all three children should be taken, not a
the literal moment of arrival of the ability to overregularize, but as a partly
arbitrary dichotomization of the developmental span into a period when
overregularizations are likely to occur and a preceding span when overregu-
larizations are unlikely to occur.
A more precise test comes from correlating the monthly rate of increase
in a vocabulary measure between month t and month t + 1 with the overreg-
ularization rate at time t + 1. This analysis combines the factors that differ-
entiate the pre-overregularization stage from the overregularization stag
and the factors that cause overregularization to be more frequent in on
month than another during the overregularization stage. According to th
Rumelhart-McClelland model, there is no qualitative difference between the
two. Because longitudinal transcripts from a given child are not indepen
dent sampling units, the correlation coefficients are best treated as descrip-
tive statistics; the meaning of tests of significance is not clear. Fortunately,
the issue will seldom arise, as the sign of the correlation coefficients we find
will usually obviate the need for significance testing.
V. PARENTAL TOKENS
The proportion of regular verb tokens among all adult verb tokens
plotted for Adam, Eve, and Sarah in Figures 18, 19, and 20. As mention
in Chapter III, the data come not only from the child's parents but al
from the other adults speaking to the child in the transcripts. For all three
children, a bit more than one-quarter of the parental verb tokens we
regular, and this did not change over the course of development. As me
tioned, the reason the proportion is constant is that most of the high
frequency verbs that are indispensable for casual conversation are irregular,
and they do not move aside to make way for the more numerous but lower-
frequency regulars.
The proportions are similar before and after overregularization begin
80
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
for Adam, 30% before, 29% during; for Eve, 29% before, 25% during; for
Sarah, 25% before, 26% during. More important, the rate of change in
the proportions was not systematically larger during the overregularization
period: for Adam, 1.2 percentage points per month before, - 0.1 during;
for Eve, 0.9 before, - 0.7 during; for Sarah, 0.2 before and during. The
correlations between the rate of monthly change in proportion regular and
the child's overregularization rate are in the wrong direction for Adam
(-.40) and Eve (-.29) and close to zero (.03) for Sarah.
As mentioned, measures based on the child's types are the least psycho-
logically realistic measures to focus on, because they are meaningful only i
rule learning is an off-line pass through the child's vocabulary, which
not correspond to Rumelhart and McClelland's psychological assumptio
But the measures are worth examining both because they represent
form of the vocabulary balance hypothesis that is most likely to be consist
with some developmental trend (since the proportion of regular types mus
increase with development) and because it was in fact a change in regu
types that defined the training sequence given to the Rumelhart-McClellan
model.
81
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Adam
c 100
& 90
a 80
c 70
60
I--
50
5O
040
a)
S20 Adam's Adults
10
eO
Eve
I, 100
o 90-
- 80-
C 70
0 60
1-
50-
40
0j 301 Eve
320
2 Eve's Adults
2 10
eO
0. 0 1-6
0 2-0
Age
FIG. 19.-Percentage of verb tokens that are regular for Eve and the adults conversing
with Eve.
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
Sarah
cc 100
" 90
a 80-
) 70
a)
S60
50
Sarah
S40-
30
r20-
2 10
CL O
200 2-6 3-0 3-6 4.0 4-6 5-0
Age
FIG. 20.-Percentage of verb tokens that are regular for Sarah and the adults convers-
ing with Sarah.
83
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
84
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
years, this is one reason why the vocabulary balance hypothesis could be
false.
However, these sampling considerations could also mean that the decel-
erating growth curve is an artifact. Imagine that the child possesses a con-
stant 500 words throughout development, but manages to produce only 50
during a month's worth of samples. Following the same arithmetic as de-
scribed above, the fact that cumulative vocabulary is a form of sampling
without replacement means that we as investigators will spuriously credit
the child with having "acquired" fewer and fewer new words with each
succeeding month. It is very difficult to tell to what extent the curves in
Figures 21-25 represent a genuine sampling effect in vocabulary learning,
a sampling artifact in measuring vocabulary from production data, or both.
Thus, it is important to supplement these direct estimates with some indirect
measure that is free of this possible bias.
85
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Adam
300
250
S 150
E 100-
z
z
50 50- Cumulative Irregular Verbs
Adam
100
90 Overregularization
80
70
c 60 Cumulative Vocabulary Regular
5 50
"- 40
30
20
10
86
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Eve
300
250
()
,.0
, 200
o
o 150- Cumulative Regular Verbs
E 100
Eve
100
90
80 Overregularization
S70
c 60
O
,) 50 - Cumulative Vocabula
. 40
30-
20
10
87
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Sarah
300
250
')
Cumulative Regular Verbs
4) 200
.0
150
U)
E 100
z
Cumulative Irregular Verbs
50
Sarah
100
90- Overregularization
80
4)
70
30
20
10
0
2-0 2-6 3-0 3-6 4-0 4-6 5-0
Age
FIG. 26.-Proportion of Sarah's cumulative verb vocabulary that is regular and her
overregularization rate (subtracted from 100%).
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
TABLE 5
89
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
90
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
bias but higher variance; there is a complex procedure for selecting the
optimal order for a given estimate. For simplicity's sake, we will uniformly
report estimates of order 4. We have found these generally to be the highest
of the estimates of different order, especially for regular verbs; hence, they
are fairest to the vocabulary balance hypothesis. But in any case we also
found that the growth curves for different order estimates are almost per-
fectly parallel, so the correlations we calculate are not notably affected by
this choice. With these considerations in mind we can cautiously compare
periods of high regular vocabulary growth with periods of low regular vo-
cabulary growth, even if the magnitudes of particular increases and totals
are not to be taken as perfectly accurate estimates.
Figures 27-32 show that the vocabulary estimates obtained from this
method are higher for young children and somewhat lower for older chil-
dren than the cumulative estimates, eliminating the severe deceleration that
was inherently unfavorable to the regular vocabulary balance hypothesis.
They also do not display the unlikely constant 50/50 regular-irregular ratio
that would correspond to interpreting Pinker and Prince's figures as esti-
mates of types.
Overregularization rates are included in the graphs for ease of compar-
ison of their developmental courses. Table 6 shows the rate of growth of
estimated vocabulary per interval during the period preceding the first
overregularization and the period beginning with it; numbers and propor-
tions of regular verbs at the end of these two intervals are shown as well.
The number of irregular types shows a very small increase with time; the
number of regulars shows a larger one. As Table 6 shows, for Adam, the
rate of increase in the proportion of verbs that are regular is larger during
the overregularization stage than before it (1.1 and - 0.2 percentage points,
respectively, per five-sample interval). This, however, is the only comparison
from among all those we have performed that is in a direction consistent
with the vocabulary balance hypothesis. For Adam, the rate of increase in
the proportion of verbs that are regular from one interval to the next shows
no correlation with the overregularization rate at the end of the interval
(r = .004); there was virtually no difference in the number of regular verbs
acquired per interval during the overregularization stage versus before it
(9.7 and 9.1 new regular verbs per interval, respectively); and the sample
correlation coefficient for the relation between the size of the increase be-
tween intervals and the overregularization rate for the second interval is
negative (r = -.08). Sarah acquired regular verbs at a faster rate before
her overregularization period than during it (42.2 and 10.2 additional verbs
per interval, respectively), and these increases correlated negatively with
their ensuing overregularization rates (r = -.26). Similarly, the proportion
of her vocabulary that was regular increased much more rapidly before
than during her overregularization stage (8.3 and 0.4 percentage points per
91
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Adam
300
250
E 200
4e- Q> Estimated Regular Verbs
E 100
50Estimated Irregular Verbs
50
Adam
100
90 - Overregularization
80
S70
60D
60 Estimated Vocabulary Regular
? 50
S40
30
20
10
92
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Eve
300
250
. 200
Estimated Regular Verbs
150
E 100
0-
1-6 2-0
Age
FIG. 29.-Eve's Jackknife-estimated regular and irregular verb vocabulary
Eve
100
90
80~ Overregularization
S70
C1
S 60
S50 SEstimated Vocabulary Regular
S40
30
20
10
1-6 2-0
Age
FIG. 30.-Proportion of Eve's Jackknife-estimated verb vocabulary that is regular and
her overregularization rate (subtracted from 100%).
93
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Sarah
300
o 150
Estimated Irregular Verbs
"E 100
z
50
Sarah
100
90- Overregularization
80
G 70
60-
50 Estimated Vocabulary Regular
L- 40
l30
20
10
0
2-0 2-6 3-0 3-6 4-0 46 5-:0
Age
FIG. 32.-Proportion of Sarah's Jackknife-estimated verb vocabulary that is regular
and her overregularization rate (subtracted from 100%).
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
TABLE 6
Percentage regular.............. 62 a 64
Average rate of increase during
overregularization:
Number of regular verbs ........ 9.7 9.5 7.8
Proportion regular. ............. 1.1 .6 .4
Level at end of transcripts:
Number of regular verbs ........ 213 106 209
Number of irregular verbs ....... 87 78 96
Total ......................... 300 184 305
95
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
children, a set composed of 7,097 types. She reports the 1,003 most fre-
quently used words. The list contains 97 regular verbs and 74 irregular
verbs, or 57% of types regular. These figures are somewhat smaller than
the cumulative and Jackknife proportions that we find for Adam and Sarah
at the end of their transcripts (which range from 69% to 74% regular), but
that is to be expected because both Horns excluded lower-frequency words
which in turn systematically underestimates the number of regular verbs
(When we look at single samples of Adam's and Sarah's speech near the
end of their transcripts, which introduces a similar bias, we ourselves get
lower figures, near 58%.) However, M. D. Horn provided data for th
proportion of verb tokens that are regular, which should be unaffected by
this sampling bias. The token figures are similar to our estimates: 24,581
regular tokens and 80,370 irregular tokens, or 23% regular.
Even better estimates come from Moe et al.'s (1982) figures for first-
grade children (mean age 6-9) because they report all verb tokens, not just
the most frequent ones. Their lists include 418 regular verb types and 108
irregular types, corresponding to 79% of verb types being regular and 33%
of verb tokens. These figures can be compared with the estimates shown in
Tables 5 and 6 for Adam and Sarah at the end of their transcripts: Adam
(5-2), 74% cumulative types regular, 71% Jackknife-estimated types regu-
lar, 33% tokens regular; Sarah (5-1), 73% cumulative types regular, 69%
Jackknife-estimated types regular, 20% tokens regular.
96
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
"23 These figures correspond to the number of verbs actually listed in their append
their text mentions 20 irregular past tense forms and 31 overregularized forms.
97
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
questions about the basis for Marchman and Bates's prediction. First
Plunkett and Marchman repeatedly conclude that the rate of vocabulary
expansion (the factor that we, but not Marchman and Bates, correlated
with overregularizations) is a critical variable in their models' tendency to
overregularize (e.g., Plunkett & Marchman, 1990, pp. 12-13, 18). Indeed,
they note that their model can attain 100% correct performance on any
absolute level of vocabulary size (p. 11). Second, when Plunkett and
Marchman do discuss a putative effect of a critical mass in vocabulary, the
effect in question is "recovery from erroneous performance" (p. 11), exactly
the opposite of the decrement for irregular verbs that Marchman and Bates
are trying to explain. (Later, they discuss generalization to new stems, but
this is still different from overregularization of previously learned ones.)
Third, Plunkett and Marchman's claim for a critical mass effect in their
model (i.e., recovery after vocabulary size reaches 50, constant across differ-
ent rates of vocabulary increase) bears no obvious relation to the simulation
data they present. They cite no numbers, instead inviting the reader to
inspect their figures 1 and 2, but neither figure shows any discontinuity in
performance for irregular verbs at the point corresponding to the 50th verb
acquired. Moreover, their figure 1 shows clear differences in the recovery
profile depending on the rate of vocabulary increase.
The empirical claims.-Marchman and Bates report two relevant empiri-
cal findings. The first is that overregularizations are rarely reported for
children with reported verb vocabularies less than 15-30 words. They take
this as evidence for a critical mass effect, but the reasoning is unsound. If
children's vocabulary increases with age and they begin to overregularize at
a certain point after vocabulary acquisition begins, by logical necessity any
finite data set will yield numbers corresponding to the average and mini-
mum sizes of their vocabulary at the time they begin overregularizing. In
particular, the minimum will simply correspond to the vocabulary size of
the most precocious overregularizer in the sample. This does not constitute
evidence for a critical mass effect, that is, that across variation in other
factors such as rate and composition of vocabulary, a given child will begin
to overregularize when he or she attains a specific vocabulary size.
The second finding is that vocabulary size correlates well with overregu-
larizations, holding linear effects of chronological age constant in a multiple
regression. But this finding, too, is close to being a statistical necessity, not
an empirical discovery. Recall that Marchman and Bates's data on overregu-
larization consist of the number of irregular types that are overregularized
at least once; they have no data on the overregularization rate (the probabil-
ity that the child will use an overregularization as compared to the correct
irregular past). Clearly, a child cannot overregularize a verb that is not in
his or her vocabulary. If the child has a vocabulary of five irregular verbs,
he or she cannot be recorded as having overregularized six verbs. If the
98
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
VIII. SUMMARY
99
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
VI. THE RELATION OF OVERREGULARIZATION TO
THE DEVELOPMENT OF TENSE MARKING OF
REGULAR AND IRREGULAR VERBS
100
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
101
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
can see whether attainment of such levels occurs near the time of the first
overregularization, and whether degree of successful regular marking cor-
relates with overregularization across time and children. The prediction
depends on the assumption that the syntactic requirement of tense marking
has itself been acquired (if children do not know that tense is obligatory,
they could leave regular verbs in past tense contexts unmarked even if they
knew how to mark them), so if children do not reliably mark the tense of
either regular verbs or irregular verbs (which do not depend on a produc-
tive process) in obligatory contexts, this prediction cannot be tested. But if
regular verbs are reliably marked, we have indirect evidence for a produc-
tive regular process; if irregular verbs are reliably marked but regular verbs
are not, we have indirect evidence that the regular process has not yet
developed. (The acquisition of obligatory marking of irregular verbs will be
discussed in a separate section.)
Note that these predictions about levels of irregular and regular mark-
ing appear to contrast with those of the Rumelhart-McClelland model under
the assumption that the model is responsible for early correct performance
with irregulars, with no separate lexical storage. During the early period of
learning, before the influx of regular verbs triggers overregularization, the
model rapidly attained successful performance with both irregular and reg-
ular verbs (80%-85% of correct features generated), with, if anything, a
slight advantage for the regulars (see their fig. 4, reproduced here as Fig.
16). Furthermore, Rumelhart and McClelland point out that their model is
not just doing the equivalent of rote memorization before it overregularizes
but is showing "substantial generalization." Newly presented regular verbs
are inflected with 75% accuracy (chance is 50%) on their very first exposure,
based on the training during the period in which irregulars are being pro-
duced correctly.
102
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
"24 The data are grouped into months to be commensurable with our overregulariza-
tion data. Sarah's recording sessions were half as long but twice as frequent as those of
Adam and Eve; to facilitate comparisons among the three children, Cazden pooled succes-
sive pairs of Sarah's samples. Sometimes these pairs straddled a boundary between months
of chronological age, the units we have been using. When this happened, we divided the
tokens in that pair of samples by two, assigning half to the month preceding the boundary,
half to the month following it.
103
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
TABLE 7
IRREGULAR REGULAR
No. of No. of
Proportion Obligatory Proportion Obligatory
Marked Contexts Marked Contexts
"25 Sarah's correlation with regular marking would be statistically significant if one
were to treat months as independent sampling units; the other correlation coefficients
would not be. Such significance tests, however, are meaningless because monthly transcript
sets are not independent samples from a population, as noted earlier.
104
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
adopted the 90% over 6 hours criterion was because it could be used to
derive an objective rank ordering of the age of acquisition of the mor-
phemes. For our purposes, which do not involve ordering heterogeneous
morphemes, the criterion is in some ways too strict (because extended use
of a morpheme in, say, 75%-85% of obligatory contexts clearly indicates
relevant knowledge) and in others too lax (because a span with a lucky
streak of correct usages could be counted as mastery, only to return to lower
levels because of regression to the mean). Brown notes that the develop-
mental curves show such patterns in several instances.
One convenient benchmark is whether a child marks tense more often
than not in obligatory contexts (i.e., if significantly more than 50% of verb
tokens are marked). The 50% figure is, of course, partly arbitrary, but it
does literally reflect a systematic preference that tense be marked. More-
over, because children initially mark most morphemes at rates far less than
50% (most notably, the regular past tense), it is unlikely to represent the
base rate for the proportion of times a past tense is called for on pragmatic
communicative grounds, and attainment of a level of marking greater than
50% represents movement toward the adult state where tense marking is
truly obligatory.
In Table 7, marking rates that are significantly greater than .5 by a
two-tailed binomial test (computed by the approximation to the normal
distribution) are indicated with an asterisk. All three children, before they
began to overregularize, left regular verbs unmarked more often than they
marked them (Adam and Sarah marked irregular verbs more often than
they left them unmarked). After overregularization had begun, all three
children marked both irregular verbs and regular verbs more often than
they left them unmarked. In sum, for Adam and Sarah, and to a lesser
extent Eve, there is evidence that the onset of overregularization is tempo-
rally associated with attainment of high rates of marking tense on regular
verbs, comparable to rates of tensing irregular verbs.
The data from Adam, Eve, and Sarah help resolve a paradox noted
long ago in the developmental literature. Ervin (1964) remarked that for
some of her subjects there were no regular past tense forms preceding the
first overregularizations. Among the nine children with extended longitudi-
nal transcripts in our sample, we found this to be true for Naomi and April
as well: the first regular verbs marked for past tense appeared a few days
after their first overregularization. The puzzle arises because no specific
rule of morphology can be innate. Therefore the child must acquire any
particular rule on the basis of individual regularly inflected forms memo-
rized from parental speech. All children, then, should be capable of produc-
ing at least some regular past tense forms before their first overregulariza-
tion. The paradox disappears when we see that Adam, Eve, and Sarah used
regular past forms at very low rates before their first overregularization,
105
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
26 These percentages were taken directly from Kuczaj (1976) rather than being calcu-
lated from the transcripts or raw data tables, so in some cases they are based on slightly
different token counts for number of correct irregulars than the ones we have been using
(i.e., the data listed in the second column of App. Table A4).
106
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
and this child also had the lowest overregularization rate except for the
three oldest children. Statistically, the children in the cross-sectional sample
behave similarly to Abe's months; although the simple correlation coeffi-
cient between overregularization and regular marking is low (-.09), the
partial correlation with age held constant is high (r = .47, p < .05).
Summary of developmental patterns.--Several general relations appear to
hold consistently among the children we have examined. Before the first
overregularization, regular verbs in obligatory past tense contexts are left
unmarked more often than they are marked, and they are marked at lower
rates than irregular verbs. In the period beginning with the month of the
first overregularization, there is no consistent difference between the rates
of marking regular and irregular verbs; both are marked more often than
they are left unmarked, often at very high rates. During developmental
spans before overregularization begins to diminish, rates of regular marking
correlate positively with the rate of overregularization.
The pattern is consistent with the hypothesis that the immediate trigger
for overregularization is the acquisition of the process responsible for regu-
lar tense marking, which is independent of early use of correct irregular
forms. Once productive regular marking has been acquired, children can
now inflect the stem productively on any occasion that they try to mark the
past tense of an irregular verb but fail to retrieve its past form. They do so
for the same reason that they can now inflect regular stems at high rates,
despite the initial disadvantage in memorizability that the regulars had
faced.
107
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Adam
100
90 ' Overregularization
80
2 70
&-
S60 h
- 50 - Irregular
o 40
a. 30-
20
10
Adam
100 -
90 i Overregularization
-80
o 60-
"c 50-
0 40
"Q Regular
. 30
20
10
20 26 2m6
2-0 30 36 3-0
40 503-6 4-0 4-6 5-0
Age
FIG. 34.--Proportion of Adam's regular verbs in obligatory past tense contexts that
were correctly marked for tense and his overregularization rate (subtracted from 100%).
108
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Eve
100 Overregularization
90
. 80
2 70 "
60 0 '
50 :
S40
o. 30 0 Irregular
20
10
0-
1-6 2-0
Age
FIG. 35.-Proportion of Eve's irregular verbs in obligatory past tense contexts that
were correctly marked for tense. Overregularizations do not enter into these data but are
shown in the curve at the top (subtracted from 100%).
Eve
Overregularization
100
90
S80
&e 70
o 60
E50
2 40
S30 Regular
20
10
1-6 2-0
Age
FIG. 36.-Proportion of Eve's regular verbs in obligatory past tense contexts that were
correctly marked for tense and her overregularization rate (subtracted from 100%).
109
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Sarah
Overregularization
100-lt
90 as
80 p ..
70O
,60 ".i Irregular
S50
o 40
& 30
20-
10
0
2-0 2-6 3-0 3-6 4-0 4-6 5-0
Age
FIG. 37.-Proportion of Sarah's irregular verbs in obligatory past tense contexts that
were correctly marked for tense. Overregularizations do not enter into these data but are
shown in the curve at the top (subtracted from 100%).
Sarah
100"
90
80 - Overregularization
0
2 70
o 60
S 50 Regular
o 40
4) 30
20
10
Age
FIG. 38.-Proportion of Sarah's regular verbs in obligatory past tense contexts that
were correctly marked for tense and her overregularization rate (subtracted from 100%).
110
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Abe
100 ,*?u. * CIIIII
90 .
S80
2 70
0 60- Overregularization
" 50
40 Irregular
S30
20
10
0
020 2-6 3-0 3-6 4-0 4-6 5-0
Age
FIG. 39.-Proportion of Abe's irregular verbs in obligatory past tense contexts that
were correctly marked for tense. Overregularizations do not enter into these data but are
shown in the curve at the top (subtracted from 100%).
Abe
100----- - ms
Regular
90
S80
. 70
S60 Overregularization
S50
o 40
& 30
20
10
0
020 2-6 3-0 3-6 4-0 4-6 5-0
Age
FIG. 40.-Proportion of Abe's regular verbs in obligatory past tense contexts that
were correctly marked for tense and his overregularization rate (subtracted from 100%).
111
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
"27 Some of the differences among the Brown children may simply reflect how old
they were when Cazden stopped tallying their marking rates and hence the proportion
of early transitional samples that contributed to these means. Note as well that part of th
difference between the Brown children and the Kuczaj children can be attributed to the
different methods that Kuczaj and Cazden used in counting correct past tense forms.
Cazden apparently counted a correct past tense form as "supplied" only if it was in on
of the obligatory contexts for the past tense that she used to identify no-marking errors
other correct pasts were ignored (hence the difference between the number of supplied
irregular forms she tallied and the number of correct irregulars we found, as summarized
in App. Tables A1-A3). Kuczaj appeared to have counted all overt past tense forms as i
their very appearance was prima facie evidence that each one was obligatory in its context
This difference in method is unlikely to be responsible for much of the difference, how-
ever, because, for most of Abe's months and Kuczaj's children, the estimates of tens
marking were 100% or close to it; therefore, subtracting some correct past tense forms
would make little or no difference.
28 Another way of replacing Abe's rate of successful regularization with Adam's wou
be to use their rates of regularizing an irregular verb when the correct irregular past fo
was not produced (i.e., overregularizations as a proportion of overregularizations p
stem forms in past tense contexts; see Chap. IV, Sec. VI) rather than their rates of suffixin
regular verbs in past tense contexts. In that calculation, the discrepancy would be reduc
almost entirely. The comparison would be suspect, however, because the estimate of Ab
rate of regularizing irregular tokens is not independent of his overall overregularizati
rate, the figure we would be adjusting.
112
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
"29 Note that this is a modification of a conclusion stated in an earlier draft of thi
Monograph circulated as a technical report, which was based on the ages at which Brow
90% for 6 hours requirement had been met, before Cazden's full data set was ma
available to us.
113
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
more often than not (Abe's first month). Moreover, although the rate of
irregular marking in obligatory contexts tends to correlate over months with
overregularization rates in all children, the correlations were always lower
than the corresponding ones between overregularization and the rate of
regular marking. Nor is this entirely surprising. Although marking past
tense reliably should increase the number of overregularization errors (and
hence could correlate with the appearance of such errors for sampling rea-
sons if the total number of irregular past forms were low, which it is not
for Adam and Sarah), it does not necessarily increase the rate of overregular-
ization calculated over all irregular verbs marked for past tense. That is
because if a child neglected to mark irregular verbs for tense, we would see
both few overregularizations and few correct irregular past tense forms
(with unmarked stems taking their place), all things being equal.
Nonetheless, we can speculate on two conceivable reasons why past
tense overregularization does tend to occur in the presence of high rates of
irregular past tense marking. One is that the acquisition of the obligatory
tense requirement in English might be an impetus for the acquisition of the
regular suffixation process itself, which is a means for the tense requirement
to be satisfied for all verbs, memorized or not. (We return to this possibility
in Chap. VIII, Sec. IC, where we consider how a regular rule might be
learned.) Second, because tense is stored as part of the lexical entry of
irregular verbs, they might tend to be activated by normal lexical retrieval
processes driven by the intent to communicate certain kinds of semantic
content. In comparison, activation of the regular operation, a grammatical
process, might be invoked more often for grammatical reasons (to satisfy
the tense requirement). That is, broke automatically comes to mind when one
is thinking simultaneously of breaking and of pastness, whereas thinking of
fixing and pastness summons only the word fix, plus a call to the regular
tense-marking operation. If lexical retrieval is more automatic or reliable
than on-line application of regular marking, then once a child's language
system is driven by the grammatical requirement to mark tense, there is a
motive to apply regular marking when retrieval of an irregular fails, and
overregularizations will result. In contrast, without the requirement, tense
might be marked only when the irregular form was lexically activated for
semantic reasons. Whether these speculations can be supported by indepen-
dent data, or whether they are necessary at all, we leave as a question for
future research.
III. SUMMARY
114
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
first appear during a window in which the child goes from leaving most
regular verbs unmarked in past tense contexts to marking them more often
than not, and at rates comparable to those for irregular verbs. An overall
tendency to mark past tense on irregular verbs, combined with imperfect
retrieval of irregular forms from memory and with a regular process that
is capable of applying to any stem, even if closely tied to an irregular past,
sets the stage for overregularization errors.
115
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
VII. FACTORS CAUSING DIFFERENCES
IN OVERREGULARIZATION RATES AMONG VERBS
116
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
verbs (i.e., for each child we converted the overregularization rate for each
verb to a z score, using the mean and standard deviation of the overregulari-
zation rates for different verbs used by that child). The z scores for a given
verb were then averaged across children. These scores, representing the
relative overregularization rate for each verb in the aggregate sample, are
summarized in Appendix Table A9. For the convenience of the reader we
have also linearly rescaled these mean z scores back to proportions that can
be more intuitively interpreted as "average overregularization rates." For
each verb, we multiplied its z score by the mean across children of the
standard deviations that had been used to calculate the z scores (i.e., the
mean of the 25 standard deviations, one for each child, of the overregulari-
zation rates for that child's different verbs), added it to the mean overregu-
larization rate for that verb (including zeros from the children who never
overregularized), and added an additional constant of .02 to raise the least-
overregularized verb from a negative value to zero. Note that these figures
are primarily for ease of comparing relative overregularization rates among
verbs and do not literally correspond to a mean across children.
I. FREQUENCY
Three measures of adult frequency were used: (1) for each verb used
in the past tense by a child, the frequency of the past tense form of tha
verb in the speech of the adults talking to that child; (2) an aggregate
parental frequency measure computed by averaging the 19 sets of adul
frequencies, one set from each child; and (3) the past tense counts from
117
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Francis and Kucera's (1982) corpus of a million words of written text. (We
include correlations with the Francis and Kucera data base because it is the
most commonly used source of frequency information in psycholinguistics;
it should predict children's behavior less well then parental frequency
counts, of course, because it is from written English addressed to adults.)
Frequencies and mean frequencies were converted to logs because the fre-
quencies could range over several orders of magnitude (especially for
Francis-Kucera figures) and because we expected that a frequency differ-
ence of 1 versus 10 would have a greater effect than a frequency difference
of 1,001 versus 1,010. For the purpose of calculating correlations with these
logs, frequencies and mean frequencies of zero were arbitrarily assigned
the value zero.
An important fact about irregular verbs is that their past tense form
although unpredictable in other regards, generally preserve most of
phonological composition of their stems. Go-went and be-was are exceptio
for the other irregulars such as come-came, feel-felt, and bring-brought, th
and stem overlap to an extent that would be uncanny if the pair cons
of two arbitrary words linked only as memorized paired associates. Pi
and Prince (1988) point out that theories of irregular morphology sh
explain this fact, but that the Rumelhart-McClelland model failed to d
118
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
It is possible that there really are no irregular rules, and that the com-
monalities between stem and past (a heterogeneous set of patterns that
30 For Adam, Eve, and Sarah, no-change verbs were included; have, be, and do were
excluded because it is not clear how irregular-rule theories would treat them (all three
have irregular present tense forms, and be has two past forms, was and were, neither
related to the infinitive).
119
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
120
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
effect that it prevents children from mentally linking them over the long
term, accounting for the very existence of overregularization, but does have
a smaller effect over the short term, causing slight delays in mentally unify-
ing past and stem for verbs with dissimilar pasts compared to verbs with
similar pasts. Moreover, if links between stem and past are not immediately
acquired at full strength, stem-past similarity could have an influence on
children's ability to retrieve the past form after some kind of link had been
formed. This could come about if on some occasions the child retrieves the
past only after first mentally activating the stem. Common phonological
material between stem and past might improve the speed or probability
with which a past form is activated, if, for example, both forms are linked
via pointers to a common representation of their phonological constituents,
or if they actually overlap in their underlying phonological representations
at some level. That is, it might be easier to retrieve strung given string than
brought given bring.
If phonological similarity has an effect on overregularization, it would
be the degree of phonological overlap, not the degree of phonological
change, that should be related to overregularization proneness. The degree
of phonological overlap for irregular pairs was quantified as follows. We
began by counting the number of phonemes preserved from stem to past.
For example, forget-forgot has five phonemes preserved, whereas catch-caught
has only one. Counting shared phonemes alone would be highly misleading,
however, because speakers certainly represent words in a format that is
more structured than a simple list of segments. In particular, the
consonant-vowel (CV) skeleton underlying a form is thought to be a distinct
level of representation in phonology (see Kaye, 1990), and this would sa-
liently capture the relatedness of see and saw or throw and threw even if the
sheer number of shared segments was slight-the lack of a consonant at
the end of a verb is itself a feature of similarity. Thus, we also counted the
segments that changed content but preserved their positions in the verb's
CV skeleton (with half the weight of phonemes that preserved both their
content and their skeleton position), so that see and saw were counted as
more similar than say and said. Similarly, consonants that preserved their
content but changed position in the CV skeleton were given a quarter as
much weight as consonants that preserved both content and position, so
that I was given less weight in feel-felt than in steal-stole. (As it turns out,
nothing hinges on this particular way of measuring stem-past similarity;
similar results occur if all phonemes are weighted equally or if shared pho-
nological features, rather than shared phonemes, are counted.)
The prediction is that the amount of preserved phonological structure
should correlate negatively with overregularization rates; that is, the more
similar the stem and past, the less likely the child is to overregularize the
verb. At first it might appear that the prediction has already been tested in
121
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Not only are irregular verb stems similar to their past tense forms, but
they also tend to be similar to other irregular verb stems that have compara-
ble past tense forms. Irregular verbs fall into clusters such as sting-stung,
swing-swung, string-strung, and so on. The minor rules posited by some theo-
122
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
123
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
from both rote lexical storage and rule application, namely, rhyme-drive
analogies. Bybee and Slobin suggested that speakers form schemas for recog-
nizing typical phonological patterns of irregular past tense forms. Children
learn to associate past forms with their stems more easily if they confor
to a past tense schema, and they are more likely to select stored forms that
conform to a schema when producing past tenses.
If belonging to a family of similar irregulars undergoing simila
changes strengthens the memory trace of a given irregular form, it should
be more resistant to overregularization than more isolated irregulars, hold-
ing frequency constant. The prediction that partial regularity blocks over
regularization was first suggested by Slobin (1971) and has been furthe
tested by Bybee and Slobin (1982a) and Kuczaj (1977a, 1978). The mos
robust effect is that verbs that end in t or d are less likely to have -ed added
and are more likely to be uttered in no-change form, than verbs withou
those endings. This is true for both no-change irregulars, leading to im-
proved performance, and other kinds of irregulars, leading to no-chang
errors; both kinds are protected from overregularization (for reviews, se
Pinker & Prince, 1988; and Rumelhart & McClelland, 1986). (We see the
effect in our data as well: of the 11 verbs ending in t or d listed in App
Table A10, 10 of them were overregularized at aggregate rates lower tha
the mean across verbs.) As mentioned, the Rumelhart-McClelland model
duplicated this phenomenon. However, Pinker and Prince (1988) point ou
that the effect is potentially so overdetermined that identifying the psycho-
logically active cause or causes is nearly impossible. The no-change class
large (the largest among the irregular verbs), shows an exceptionless hyper-
similarity (all its verbs end in t or d), involves a single kind of change (none)
shares its verb-final consonant with the regulars, and when regularized r
sults in a phonological pattern (adjacent identical stop consonants) that th
phonology of English tries to avoid. Thus, the existence of an effect o
family strength should be confirmed with other materials.
Bybee and Slobin also showed that children overregularized subclasses
with different kinds of vowel changes at different rates. They attributed th
differences to different degrees of stem-past similarity, as discussed in the
preceding section. But as we have seen, the effects of stem-past similarit
are difficult to demonstrate, if they exist at all. Furthermore, Pinker an
Prince (1988) showed that stem-stem similarity may be the more relevan
factor: the overregularization rates for the different vowel change classe
correlate well with the number of English irregular verbs sharing the vowel
changes that the class members undergo. They suggested that this explain
why the Rumelhart-McClelland model mimicked the ranking of overregu
larization rates for these subclasses, at least in one stage. However, eve
here the existence of a stem family effect was not perfectly clear: Bybe
and Slobin's subclasses, as interpreted by Rumelhart and McClelland, wer
124
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
125
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
126
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
127
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
irregular past tense tokens, and one of them was Abe, who had the richest
data base of overregularizations.
Each of these measures was computed a second time, excluding polysyl-
labic verbs. Pinker and Prince (1988) point out that all irregulars are mono
syllabic except for forms like forget, understand, and overthrow that contain a
prefixed monosyllabic irregular. Therefore, if a verb is nonprefixed and
polysyllabic, it is guaranteed to be regular. If a child became sensitive to
this contingency, he or she could sequester such verbs from the mechanism
giving rise to family strength effects, and our including them in our esti
mates could dilute our estimates of such effects. But the results were virtu-
ally identical in all cases.
V. SUMMARY
128
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
VIII. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
129
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
31 The variation in the phonetic form of the regular affix, whereby d surfaces as t
following unvoiced consonants and id following coronal stops, can be attributed to general
phonological processes operating throughout English, not to the regular process itself (see
Pinker & Prince, 1988).
130
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
greater than current ones would resolve many of the uncertainties in the
current conclusions.
As Pinker (1982, 1984) and Slobin (1982) point out, obligatory gram
matical constraints pose difficult learning problems for the child. The fa
that an inflection is obligatory means that there are no pragmatic cues t
the semantic features that the inflection is encoding; parents must expre
the pastness of a past event, regardless of how relevant it is in the conversa-
tional context. Moreover, once the child has somehow figured out that pa
tense inflection encodes past tense, if he or she mistakenly assumes that
is optional, no parental input short of negative evidence can contradict th
assumption. Pinker and Slobin thus suggest that the child is innately pre
pared to consider obligatory inflectional tense marking as a possible con
straint in the language to be acquired; such a hypothesis is easily discon-
firmed in systems where inflection for tense is in fact optional or no
available. Our seemingly homely explanation for overregularization in
terms of retrieval failure depends on the child having solved these daunting
learnability problems (for an explicit hypothesis as to how the child solve
them, see Pinker, 1984; and for evidence that children solve them quickly
see Stromswold, 1990; the problems are of course finessed in network simu-
lations that are fed correct stem-past pairs in isolation). If the 2-3-year-old
child did not consider tense marking to be obligatory, the failure to retrieve
an irregular past in a past tense context would not automatically lead t
overregularization; the child could simply leave the verb unmarked.
131
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
132
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
poeia, and other forms that cannot be analogized to existing irregulars (see
Kim, Pinker, et al., 1991; Prasada & Pinker, in press). Thus, a regular
pattern is liable to assume majority status if such forms enter a language in
large numbers, as has happened in English since the Middle English period
(Pyles & Algeo, 1982). Third, a regular rule need not be totally insensitive
to properties of the stem it affixes to: multiple rules may exist, each affixing
an allomorph that applies to stems with a different property, although the
conditions would be well defined (e.g., ending in a vowel vs. a consonant)
and applicability to a stem within the class would be all or none, not propor-
tional to the stem's global similarity to previously encountered stems. The
most common hallmark of a regular process in our sense is default status,
the ability to apply freely to any word not already linked to an irregular
form, regardless of whether it is covered by memorization or analogy (e.g.,
We rhumba'd all night). The computational nature of the rule, as concatena-
tion of an affix to a variable standing for the stem, renders it uniquely
capable of playing this default role.
We have said nothing about how any particular regular rule, such as
the English past tense rule, is acquired. Extraction of the suffixation pattern
itself can be achieved by examining the phonological differences between
forms like walk and walked, which can be accomplished by a variety of pat-
tern extraction algorithms (see, e.g., MacWhinney, 1978; Pinker, 1984;
Pinker & Prince, 1988; Rumelhart & McClelland, 1986). A greater challenge
is to show how the child decides whether to internalize a pattern as a regular
rule or as a list of (possibly analogy-supporting) individual items such as
bend-bent. How might this be done? Given the results of Chapter V, children
do not appear to depend on a regular pattern applying to a majority of
tokens or to be influenced by either the relative or the absolute number of
types. Indeed, children begin to overregularize with something on the order
of 100 verbs in their vocabularies, and they mark few or none of them for
past tense before they begin to overregularize the irregulars, raising the
possibility that the child does not need to process large amounts of input
data to seize on the regular pattern. We lack evidence that would allow us
to identify which cues children actually use to acquire a regular rule, but
we can list several logical possibilities, each one a visible consequence of the
regular nature of a rule and hence a potential cue to regularity (for further
discussion, see Pinker & Prince, 1991). We can also check to see if any of
these cues is clearly ruled out by existing evidence on the linguistic input
available to children.
First, Pinker and Prince (1988) suggested that the crucial cue might be
133
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
134
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
& Prince, 1988). For example, ring the city (i.e., "form a ring around") has
the past tense form ringed, not rang, because this sense of to ring has a noun
root, not a verb that could be collapsed with the one underlying ring-rang.
Hearing such forms could, in principle, tell a learner that the -ed affix is
not stored with verb roots and hence must be a regular operation.
Fourth, even a single word with a highly unusual sound pattern, such
as out-Gorbachev'd or rhumba'd, provides information about the generality
and possible default status of a morphological pattern.
Fifth, the syntactic requirement that tense be marked obligatorily may
impel the child's learning mechanism to seek a pattern that can provide the
needed form under a wide range of circumstances. The fact that Adam,
Eve, Sarah, and Abe were marking irregular verbs for tense a majority of
the time near the period in which they were first controlling the regular
suffix lends some credence to this suggestion. Of course obligatoriness itself
cannot distinguish between regular rule and irregular storage plus analogy,
but it might cause the child to promote the most general process to rule
status.
135
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
136
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
eight verbs rhyming with six kinds of irregulars (cry, kick, lick, pick, play,
show, turn, use), one likely deadjectival verb (empty), and an onomatopoeic
form (crack). Thus, the data suggest that each of the possible cues to regular-
ity entertained in this section is available to children; whether they are used,
necessary, or sufficient, however, remains unknown.
As Bybee and Slobin (1982b) point out, it is adults who bear most of
the blame for the permanent regularization of irregulars in a language
They note that one cannot attribute historical change to children's overregu-
larization errors unless children prefer these errors and continue to do so
into adulthood, which, for virtually all irregular verbs in a given generation,
they do not. Given what we know about overregularization in children and
adults (e.g., Ullman & Pinker, 1990, 1992), the following scenario seems
more plausible. A weak irregular memory entry in adults can lead to occa
sional blocking failures, hence regularizations, for the same reason that
137
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
138
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
139
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
140
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
141
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
142
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
negations, inversions, and emphatics, has the irregular forms does and did.
The perfect auxiliary have, like possessional have, maps onto the irregular
forms has and had. Be, whether serving as a copula verb, a progressive
auxiliary, or a passive auxiliary, has the irregular forms am, is, are, was, were,
and been. Moreover, the semantic relations are the same in all cases: the
relation between have a book and had a book is the same as the relation
between have eaten and had eaten; I am tired is to I was tired as I am resting is
to I was resting. Clearly, there are too many of these parallelisms to be
coincidental, and a parsimonious assumption is that the irregular forms of
the main verb and of the auxiliary versions are stored in the same mecha-
nism because, if they were not, at least one divergence among these 10
comparisons would be expected. But their susceptibility to overregulariza-
tion is qualitatively different: in a sample of 40,000 child sentences con-
taining these verbs, Stromswold found that the main verb versions are over-
regularized at rates comparable to those we have found here, whereas the
auxiliary versions of the same verbs were never overregularized. This ex-
treme conservatism was predicted, on the grounds of learnability considera-
tions and other developmental data, by the theory in Pinker (1984), ac-
cording to which the child's language acquisition mechanisms recognize
auxiliary verbs as belonging to a distinguished set of elements because of
certain semantic and phonological properties, and as a result sequester their
lexical representations from regular morphological operations. In other
words, children appear to be capable of treating two verb forms identically
in terms of irregular inflectional patterns but qualitatively differently in
terms of regular inflectional patterns.
Third, Kim, Marcus, et al. (1991) tested children's sensitivity to the
constraint mentioned in Section IC of Chapter VIII that verbs derived from
nouns and adjectives are regular, even if homophonous to an irregular
verb. For example, to high-stick (i.e., "hit with a stick held high") has the past
tense high-sticked, not high-stuck. Recall that the effect is a consequence of
the fact that possessing an irregular past tense form is a property that holds
of verb roots, not verbs in general (unless they have an irregular verb root
as their head); verbs formed from nouns have a noun as their head, not an
irregular verb root; hence, there is no source for an irregular form, and
the default regular rule reliably applies. So in order to predict a verb's past
tense form, its phonological properties do not suffice (nor do its semantic
features; see Kim, Pinker, et al., 1991); its morphological structure must be
input to the relevant mechanisms. The effect has been demonstrated to be
extremely robust in naive adults faced with inflecting novel denominal verbs
(Kim, Pinker, et al., 1991), and Kim, Marcus, et al. (1991) showed it to
hold in 3-8-year-old children as well: children regularize denominal verbs
homophonous with irregulars, such as to fly in the sense of "to cover a piece
of paper with flies," more often than they overregularize the irregular verb
143
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
itself, even when the irregular verb is used with nonstandard meanings and
hence is equally unfamiliar.
These three phenomena suggest that it is necessary to attribute chil-
dren's regularizations (including overregularizations) to a different mecha
nism than their irregulars, on the basis of the qualitatively different inputs
and outputs the two patterns implicate: the production of irregulars but
not overregularizations feeds compound formation; the production of ir-
regulars but not overregularizations is fed by auxiliary verbs; the production
of regularizations of verbs but not their irregular forms is fed by verbs
derived from nouns. We have shown that exactly this kind of distinction
between memory and rule mechanisms provides a simple account for a
huge variety of facts about the rate, onset, and lexical properties of overreg-
ularization.
144
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
APPENDIX
TABLE Al
145
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE A2
TABLE A3
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE A4
Irreg Reg
Corr Overrg Overrg Overrg Mark Mark
Irreg Stem + ed Past + ed Rate Rate Rate
Age (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
2-5 ....... 9 2 2 .31 .31 .76
2-6....... 24 1 0 .04 .58 .63
2-7....... 27 16 2 .40 .69 .87
2-8 ....... 52 12 1 .20 .81 .80
2-9 ....... 102 36 6 .29 1 .98
2-10...... 44 34 6 .48 1 1
2-11 ...... 76 31 11 .36 .96 1
3-0....... 24 16 2 .43 1 1
3-1....... 64 19 11 .32 .99 1
3-2....... 82 8 5 .14 .99 1
3-3....... 91 36 9 .33 .97 1
3-4 ....... 107 38 4 .28 1 1
3-5....... 89 31 1 .26 1 1
3-6....... 74 12 1 .15 1 1
3-7....... 58 4 0 .06 1 1
3-8....... 88 16 2 .17 1 1
3-9....... 80 22 2 .23 1 1
3-10 ...... 69 14 2 .19 1 1
3-11 ...... 75 21 3 .24 1 1
4-0....... 13 2 6 .38 1 1
4-1....... 67 19 7 .28 1 1
4-2....... 42 7 4 .21 1 1
4-3....... 39 14 10 .38 1 1
4-4....... 16 10 0 .38 1 1
4-5....... 29 3 0 .09 1 1
4-6....... 68 3 0 .04 1 1
4-7....... 59 3 1 .06 1 1
4-8....... 38 0 0 0 1 1
4-9....... 26 0 0 0 1 1
4-10...... 62 16 0 .21 1 1
4-11...... 47 6 1 .13 1 1
5-0....... 45 13 0 .22 1 1
147
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE A5
148
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE A6
be ......... 22 0 0 0
light .. .... O 0 0
beat......... 0 0 0 .. . lose........ 3 0 0 0
bend ........ 0 0 0 ... make ....... 7 0 0 0
bite ........ 7 0 0 0
mean........ 0 0 .
bleed ........ 0 0 0 ... meet ....... 0 0 0
blow ........ 0 0 0 put ........ 38 0 0 0
break ....... 15 0 0 0 reada.......
bring........ 2 0 0 0 rend ........ 0 0 0
build ........ 0 0 0 ... ride......... 0 0 0
buy ......... 3 0 0 0 ring ....... 0 0 0 ...
catch ........ 6 0 0 0 rise ........ 0 0 0
choose ...... 0 0 0 ... run........ 0 0 0
come ....... 23 4 0 .15 say ........ 10 0 0 0
cut ........ 0 0 . see ........ 3 1 0 .25
dig ........ O 0 0 send ........ 1 0 0 0
do ......... 7 2 0 .22 shake....... 0 0 0
draw........ 0 0 0 ... shoot....... 0 0 0
drink........ 0 1 0 1 shrink ....... 0 0 0
drive....... 0 0 ... shut ....... 1 0 0 0
eat .......... 0 0 0 sing ....... O 0 0
fall ........ 2 8 0 .80 sit ......... 4 0 0 0
feed ........ O 0 0 sleep........ 0 0 0
feel ......... 0 0 0 slide ....... 0 0 0
fight ....... 0 0 0 spend ...... 0 0 0
find ........ 1 0 0 0 spin ....... 0 0 0
fly.......... 0 0 0 ... spit ........ 0 0 0
forget ...... 18 0 0 0 stand ....... 0 0 0
freeze ...... 0 0 0 steal ....... 0 0 0
get ........ 55 0 0 0 stick ... ..... 1 0 0 0
give ....... 3 0 0 0 strike........ 0 0 0
go ......... 20 5 0 .20 string ...... 0 0 0
grind ...... 0 0 0 sweep ...... 0 0 0
grow ........ 0 0 0 swim ....... 0 0 0
hang........ O 0 0 swing ...... 0 0 0
have........ 20 0 0 0 take......... 0 0
hear ....... 0 0 0 .. teach ....... 0 0 0
hide ....... 0 0 0 ...
tear......... 0 0 1 1
hit ......... 1 0 0 0 tell ........ 2 0 0 0
hold ....... 0 0 think........ 1 0 0 0
hurt ........ 5 0 0 0 throw ....... 0 1 0 1
keep ........ 0 0 0 .. wake........ 0 0
know........ 0 0 0 ... wear........ 0 1 0 1
leave ........ 1 0 0 0 win ........ 0 0 0 ...
lend ....... 0 0 0 wind........ 0 0
let ......... 0 0 ... write ....... 1 0 0 0
lie.......... 0 0 0
149
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE A7
150
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE A8
151
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
04 . : ? " .: . . . . 0 . 0 ": "0000 "
N
z -? .. . .o -? . . .o .o
S 0.0 ...0 .....0 . - ... . 0 . 0 .... .0 0 -
S. . . . . . . . .
. . 0 . .0. . . .. -
. .c..k.b. ..;. . . .. .. . . . . .
0 . . ... . -.
- - Z. C.. 0 . . . ......0
0 0
- --C " -Q 0 Q "- -%Q0 %Q ' " -0 -0 0 0 "
152
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
:O : - :00 : : .0 :
.*..... . . . ....... . .*.*
o . . . . *00
..0 . .0 . . . . . . .
0 0 00~ 00
000 0~ 00....
0 o .......
.-. . . . . . . . . . .
...0 . ....
0 . . .0
o o o o
0. 0 . .. 00 0 . .0...000 .
0 ? .0.
? ? ? ?. .
. . .
...O.
. . ... . ..
. . . ... . .
. . . ?
0 . 0 00. . . . .. ... 0 . .o
* 00 .
* 0 . .00 . . 0I
0 . ; . .
?C~ 91
153
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
O 0 ' ' 0'' o0 ' " 0o
N
0 . . . . .
Y o o oo oo
S. .
S oo
o , .- . .o .oo
. o
. C
...
1 o . .ooo. .
< o o o o o
. . 6
H
(d ?O
Z "
E - - ? ? . ? ' ? ?
13... >
C. C. . . . .. . . .
Z o. ? oo
154
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE A10
155
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE All
ache, act, add, aim, allow, answer, ask, attach, back, bake, balance, bang, bark, bash, believe,
belong, blast, bless, blind, blink, bob, boil, bomb, bop, borrow, bother, bounce, bow, bowl, box,
breathe, brush, bubble, bump, burn, burp, bury, bust, call, camp, care, carry, carve, chain, chan
chase, cheat, check, chew, chirp, choke, chop, clap, claw, clean, climb, clip, close, color, comb, co
cool, copy, cough, count, cover, crack, crash, crawl, criss-cross, cross, crush, cry, curl, dance, dar
dash, decorate, dial, die, dip, dirty, disappear, disturb, dodge, drag, dress, dribble, drill, drip,
drool, drop, drown, dry, dump, dust, empty, end, erase, excuse, exercise, faint, fan, fasten, fetch,
file, fill, finish, fish, fix, fizz, flash, flip, float, fold, follow, fool, frighten, fry, glue, grab, growl,
guess, guide, hammer, hand, handle, happen, hatch, hate, heat, help, holler, hook, hop, hope,
howl, hug, hunt, hurry, hush, invite, iron, itch, jabber, joke, juggle, jump, kick, kid, kill, kiss,
knock, land, last, laugh, leak, lean, learn, lick, lift, like, listen, live, load, lock, look, love, mail,
manage, march, marry, mash, match, matter, measure, melt, meow, mess, mind, miss, mix, mock,
move, murder, name, nap, need, obey, open, organize, own, pack, paint, pardon, park, pass, paste
pat, pay, pee, pee-pee, peek, peel, peep, perch, pick, pin, pinch, place, plant, play, plug, point,
poke, polish, pop, pour, pout, practice, pray, prepare, press, pretend, print, promise, pull, pump,
punch, punish, push, race, rain, raise, rake, reach, reattach, recognize, remember, remind, repair
rest, rinse, rip, rock, roll, rope, row, rub, ruin, rustle, sail, salute, save, scare, scoop, scoot, score,
scratch, scream, screw, scribble, seem, serve, sew, share, sharp, sharpen, shave, shop, shout, shove
shovel, show, shrug, sigh, sip, skate, ski, skip, slap, slip, smack, smart, smash, smell, smile, smoke
smooth, snap, sneeze, sniff, snow, snuggle, soak, sound, spank, spell, spill, splash, spoil, spray,
squash, squeak, squeal, squeeze, squirt, squish, squoosh, stab, stamp, staple, stare, start, starve, sta
steam, steer, step, stir, stop, straighten, stretch, study, suck, surprise, swallow, sway, swish, switch,
talk, tangle, tape, taste, tease, thank, thread, tick, tickle, tie, tighten, tip, touch, trap, trick, trim,
trip, trust, try, tuck, turn, twist, type, unbuckle, unbutton, unchain, unplug, unscrew, untangle,
untie, urinate, use, visit, wag, wait, walk, want, wash, watch, wave, wee-wee, weigh, wet, whirl,
whisper, whistle, wiggle, wink, wipe, wish, wobble, wonder, work, worry, wrap, wreck, yawn, yel
zip, zoom
ask, attach, bake, belong, bless, bow, burn, call, care, carry, change, check, chew, climb, close,
cook, copy, count, crack, crawl, cross, cry, dance, drip, drop, dry, dump, excuse, exercise, fasten,
finish, fish, fix, fold, frighten, growl, hand, happen, help, hop, hug, hurry, jabber, jump, kick, kiss,
knock, laugh, like, listen, live, look, love, march, mess, miss, mix, move, need, open, pack, paint,
park, pay, peek, pick, pinch, play, plug, point, pour, press, pretend, pull, push, rain, reach,
remember, rock, roll, rope, save, screw, shine, shop, show, skip, slip, smoke, snow, spank, spill,
squeal, squeak, squeeze, stay, step, stir, stop, talk, taste, thank, tickle, tie, touch, try, turn, use,
wait, walk, want, wash, watch, whisper, wipe, wonder, work
answer, bang, change, climb, crack, cry, dance, drop, empty, fix, happen, help, jump, kick, lick,
like, look, move, open, pardon, pick, play, show, spill, step, taste, thank, touch, turn, use, wait,
want, watch, wipe
bounce, bump, call, clean, close, comb, count, cover, cross, cry, dance, die, dress, dump, fix,
happen, help, hug, jump, knock, laugh, lift, like, look, love, need, open, organize, paint, park, pat,
peek, pick, pinch, play, poke, pour, pray, press, pull, push, rain, rip, rock, roll, shop, show, smoke,
spank, stay, step, stop, talk, taste, tease, thank, tie, touch, turn, use, wait, walk, want, wash,
watch, wee-wee, work, yawn
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
REFERENCES
Adams, S. (1938). Analysis of verb forms in the speech of young children, and their
relation to the language learning process. Journal of Experimental Education, 7,
141-144.
Anisfeld, M., Barlow, J., & Frail, C. M. (1968). Distinctive features in the pluralization
rules of English speakers. Language and Speech, 11, 31-37.
Anisfeld, M., & Gordon, M. (1968). On the psychophonological structure of English in-
flectional rules. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, 973-979.
Anisfeld, M., & Tucker, G. R. (1967). The English pluralization rules of six-year-old
children. Child Development, 38, 1201-1217.
Aronoff, M. (1976). Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bateman, W. G. (1916). The language status of three children at the same ages. Pedagogical
Seminary, 23, 211-240.
Berko, J. (1958). The child's learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150-177.
Bever, T. G. (1975). Psychologically real grammar emerges because of its role in language
acquisition. In D. Dato (Ed.), Developmental psycholinguistics: Theory and applications
(Georgetown University Round Table on Languages and Linguistics). Washington,
DC: Georgetown University Press.
Bever, T. G. (Ed.). (1982). Regression in mental development: Basic processes and mechanisms.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: The use of single word utterances before syntax. The
Hague: Mouton.
Bohannon, J. N., & Marquis, A. L. (1977). Children's control of adult speech. Child Develop-
ment, 48, 1002-1008.
Bowerman, M. (1982a). Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development.
In E. Wanner & L. R. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Bowerman, M. (1982b). Starting to talk worse: Clues to language acquisition from chil-
dren's late errors. In S. Strauss (Ed.), U-shaped behavioral growth. New York: Academic.
Bowerman, M. (1987). Commentary: Mechanisms of language acquisition. In B. Mac-
Whinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Brown, R. (n.d.). Grammars for Adam, Eve and Sarah at Stages I-V. Unpublished manu-
scripts, Harvard University, Department of Psychology.
Brown, R., & Bellugi, U. (1964). Three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. In
E. Lenneberg (Ed.), New directions in the study of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
157
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Brown, R., Cazden, C., & de Villiers, J. (1971). Directions for scoring fourteen English mor
phemes obligatory in some contexts. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University, Depart-
ment of Psychology.
Brown, R., & Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child
speech. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York
Wiley.
Brown, R., & McNeill, D. (1966). The "tip of the tongue" phenomenon. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 325-337.
Bryant, B., & Anisfeld, M. (1969). Feedback versus no-feedback in testing children's
knowledge of English pluralization rules. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8,
250-255.
Cazden, C. B. (1968). The acquisition of noun and verb inflections. Child Development
433-448.
Cazden, C. B. (1972). Child language and education. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Chamberlain, A. F. (1906). Preterite forms, etc., in the language of English
children. Modern Language Notes, 21, 42-44.
Chao, A. (1987). Estimating the population size for capture-recapture data wi
capturability. Biometrics, 43, 783-791.
Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal behavior. Language, 3, 2
Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harp
Clark, E. V. (1982). The young word maker: A case study of innovation in t
lexicon. In E. Wanner & L. R. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The st
art. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Clark, E. V. (1987). The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acqu
B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl
Clark, E. V. (1988). On the logic of contrast. Journal of Child Language, 15, 317
Clark, E. V. (1990). On the pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language, 1
Coltheart, M., Patterson, K. E., & Marshall, J. C. (Eds.). (1980). Deep dyslexi
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Cox, M. V. (1989). Children's over-regularization of nouns and verbs. Journ
Language, 16, 203-206.
158
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
Derwing, B., & Baker, W. (1979). Recent research on the acquisition of English morphol-
ogy. In P. Fletcher & M. Garman (Eds.), Language acquisition. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (1973). A cross-sectional study of the acquisition
of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2,
267-278.
Gelman, D. (1986, December 15). The mouths of babes. Newsweek, pp. 84-86.
Gleason, J. B. (1980). The acquisition of social speech and politeness formu
H. A. R. Giles (Ed.), Language: Social psychological perspectives. Oxford: Pergam
Gold, E. M. (1967). Language identification in the limit. Information and Cont
447-474.
159
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Halle, M., & Mohanan, K. P. (1985). Segmental phonology of modern English. Linguist
Inquiry, 16, 57-116.
Hart, J. T. (1965). Memory and the feeling-of-knowing experience. Journal of Education
Psychology, 56, 208-216.
Higginson, R. (1985). Fixing-assimilation in language acquisition. Unpublished doctoral di
sertation, Washington State University, Department of Anthropology.
Horn, E. (1925). Appropriate materials for instruction in reading. In G. M. Whipple (Ed.
Twenty-fourth yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education (Pt. 1) (Report of
the National Committee on Reading). Bloomington, IL: Public School Publishing Co
Horn, M. D. (1927). The thousand and three words most frequently used by kindergarte
children. Childhood Education, 3, 118-122.
Ingram, D. (1989). First language acquisition: Method, description, and explanation. New York
Cambridge University Press.
Jacobs, R. A., Jordan, M. I., & Barto, A. G. (1991). Task decomposition through competi-
tion in a modular connectionist architecture: The what and where vision tasks. Cogni
tive Science, 15, 219-250.
Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Inhelder, B. (1974-1975). If you want to get ahead, get a theory
Cognition, 3, 195-212.
Kaye, J. (1990). Phonology: A cognitive view. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kim, J. J., Marcus, G. F., Hollander, M., & Pinker, S. (1991). Children's inflection is
sensitive to morphological structure. Papers and Reports in Child Language, 30, 39-46
Kim, J. J., Pinker, S., Prince, A., & Prasada, S. (1991). Why no mere mortal has ever flow
out to center field. Cognitive Science, 15, 173-218.
Kiparsky, P. (1982). Explanation in phonology. Dordrecht: Foris.
Kruschke, J. K. (1990). ALCOVE: A connectionist model of category learning. Researc
Report No. 19. Bloomington: Cognitive Science Program, Indiana University.
Kucera, H., & Francis, N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American Englis
Providence, RI: Brown University Press.
Kuczaj, S. A. (1976). "-ing," "-s," & "-ed": A study of the acquisition of certain verb inflection
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, Department of Psy
chology.
Kuczaj, S. A. (1977a). The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Journal
of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 589-600.
Kuczaj, S. A. (1977b, March). Old and new forms, old and new meanings: The form-function
hypothesis revisited. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research
in Child Development, New Orleans.
Kuczaj, S. A. (1978). Children's judgments of grammatical and ungrammatical irregular
past tense verbs. Child Development, 49, 319-326.
Kuczaj, S. A. (1981). More on children's initial failure to relate specific acquisitions.Journal
of Child Language, 8, 485-487.
Lachter, J., & Bever, T. G. (1988). The relation between linguistic structure and associative
theories of language learning-a constructive critique of some connectionist learning
models. Cognition, 28, 195-247.
Lenneberg, E. H. (1964). A biological perspective on language. In E. H. Lenneberg (Ed.),
New directions in the study of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lieber, R. (1980). On the organization of the lexicon. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Department of Linguistics and Philosophy.
Lorge, I., & Chall, J. (1963). Estimating the size of vocabularies of children and adults:
An analysis of methodological issues. Journal of Experimental Education, 32, 147-157.
MacKay, D. (1976). On the retrieval and lexical structure of verbs. Journal of Verbal Learn-
ing and Verbal Behavior, 15, 169-182.
160
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
Otis, D. L., Burnham, K. P., White, G. C., & Anderson, D. R. (1978). Statistical inference
from capture data on closed animal populations. Wildlife Monographs, 63, 1-135.
Patel, V. L., & Groen, G. J. (1991). The general and specific nature of medical expertise:
A critical look. In A. Ericsson & J. Smith (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise:
Prospects and limits. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Pinker, S. (1979). Formal models of language learning. Cognition, 7, 217-283.
161
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Prasada, S., & Pinker, S. (in press). Generalizations of regular and irregular m
patterns. Language and Cognitive Processes.
Prasada, S., Pinker, S., & Snyder, W. (1990, November). Some evidence that irr
are retrieved from memory but regular forms are rule-generated. Paper pres
annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans.
Prince, A., & Pinker, S. (1988). Rules and connections in human languag
Neuroscience, 11, 195-202.
Pyles, T., & Algeo, J. (1982). The origins and development of the English langu
New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Reich, P. A. (1986). Language development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Ha
Rosenthal, R. (1984). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Newbury Par
Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E., & Williams, D. (1986). Learning internal repr
by error propagation. In D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland, & the PD
Group, Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of co
1. Foundations. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of En
In J. L. McClelland, D. E. Rumelhart, & the PDP Research Group, Parallel d
processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. 2. Psychological an
models. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1987). Learning the past tenses of En
Implicit rules or parallel distributed processing? In B. MacWhinney (Ed.),
of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about there and then: The emergence of displaced
parent-child discourse. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.), Children's language (Vol. 4)
NJ: Erlbaum.
Sampson, G. (1987, June 12). A turning point in linguistics. Times Literary Su
643.
Scalettar, R., & Zee, A. (1988). Emergence of grandmother memory in feed forward
networks: Learning with noise and forgetfulness. In D. Waltz & J. A. Feldman (Eds.),
Connectionist models and their implications: Readings from cognitive science. Norwood, NJ:
Ablex.
162
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
Slamecka, N. J. (1969). Testing for associative storage in multitrial free recall. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 81, 557-560.
Slobin, D. I. (1971). On the learning of morphological rules: A reply to Palermo and
Eberhart. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar: A theoretical symposium. New
York: Academic.
Ullman, M., & Pinker, S. (1990, October). Why do some verbs not have a single past tense form?
Paper presented at the fifteenth annual Boston University Conference on Language
Development.
Ullman, M., & Pinker, S. (1992). The English past tense system: Native speaker judgments of
regular and irregular forms and the effects of frequency and similarity. Unpublished manu-
script, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Department of Brain and Cognitive
Sciences.
163
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Valian, V. (1991). Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children
Cognition, 40, 21-81.
Warren-Leubecker, A. (1982). Sex differences in speech to children. Unpublished master'
thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology.
Wexler, K., & Culicover, P. (1980). Formal principles of language acquisition. Cambridg
MA: MIT Press.
164
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
165
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
COMMENTARY
Harald Clahsen
The research reported here is supported by a grant from the German Science Foun-
dation (grant C197/5-1). Correspondence should be addressed to Harald Clahsen, Allge-
meine Sprachwissenschaft, Universitat Disseldorf, Universitatsstr. 1, 4000 Diisseldorf 1,
FRG; e-mail: [email protected].
166
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
167
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
verbs that their parents use in the past tense less frequently. Third, Marcus
et al. observed effects of similarity in the distribution of overregularizations
children make fewer overregularization errors for verbs that fall into fami-
lies with more numerous and higher-frequency members (see also Bybee
Slobin, 1982).
Concerning the distribution of errors, it is striking that, of the various
types of potential errors in past tense formation, only one type is productiv
in the data reported by Marcus et al.: overapplications of the regular pas
tense affix -ed to irregular stems (*go-ed, etc.). In contrast, overapplications
of irregular patterns are extremely rare. In the data from Adam, Eve, an
Sarah, for example, there were only four instances in which irregular pa
terns are applied to inappropriate irregular verbs (beat-*bate, beat-*bet, hit-
"*heet, bite-*bat). Overextensions of irregular patterns to regular verbs are
even rarer; Marcus et al. found only one instance of such an irregularization
error, trick-*truck from Adam.'
Fletcher (1991) reported data from a case study of a British child show-
ing that the distribution of past-time forms contrasts in an interesting way
to the one found by Marcus et al. for American children. Fletcher observed
that the British child has two different types of affixes to refer to past
time events, -ed (like the American children) and -en forms, as in *maden
*broughten, *getten, etc. These -en forms appear without auxiliaries and, fre
quently (about 15%-20%), over an extended period of time (roughly from
age 2-5 to age 3-5). Fletcher also noted that there was no identifiable differ-
ence in meaning between the past tense forms and the -en forms. He pro
posed a rather complicated phonological conditioning of the occurrence
of -en. However, the list of verbs to which -en is assigned suggests that
morphological explanation might be more appropriate: -en may be affixed
to irregular verbs but not to regular ones. Thus, forms such as *builden
*cutten, *putten, etc. are documented in the data reported by Fletcher, but
forms such as *walken, *loven, and *saven are not. It will be shown below
that a similar distinction is made by German-speaking children.
Summarizing briefly, I think that there is evidence that both American
and British children distinguish between regular and irregular inflection.
Marcus et al.'s data indicate that the child has two qualitatively different
types of past tense forms: (i) irregular forms that are not productively over-
extended and (ii) the affix -ed, which may occur both with regular and
(inappropriately) with irregular verbs. Fletcher's data suggest that British
children have a separate affix that is restricted to irregular verbs.
168
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
There are five plural affixes in German, -(e)n, -s, -e, -er, and -0, three
of which also allow a variant with an umlaut, for example, der Apfel-die Apfel
169
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
(the apple/apples), die Kuh-die Kiihe (the cow/cows), and der Wald-die Wiilder
(the forest/forests). The use of these plural affixes (except -s) with specif
nouns is highly arbitrary. There exist preferred combinations of nouns and
plural allomorphs that are determined by gender and/or the morphopho
nological characteristics of the noun; however, the list of exceptions is quite
long (cf., among others, K6pcke, 1988; Mugdan, 1977).
The plural -s is special in several ways. On the one hand, -s is extremely
infrequent. It applies to fewer than 30 common nouns and is thus far less
frequent than all the other plural inflections. On the other hand, there ar
various reasons suggesting that -s is the default plural affix in adult German.
First, the use of -s is not restricted by properties of the stem/root to which
it is assigned. Second, the plural -s is used for names (die Miillers, "the
Mullers"), for newly created expressions such as clippings (Sozis [the clipped
form of Sozialisten], "socialists"), for acronyms (GmbHs [the acronym mean-
ing incorporation], "Inc."), and with borrowed and foreign words (Kiosks
"newsstands"). Third, the plural -s is the sole plural marker that does no
occur inside compounds (*Autosberg, "*cars heap"; *Sozistreffen but Sozialis-
tentreffen [clipped and full forms, respectively], "socialists' meeting").
In Clahsen et al. (1992), we proposed an analysis of German noun
plurals in the framework of lexical morphology (Kiparsky, 1982, 1985).
(In this model, differences between regular and irregular inflection are
handled by assigning morphological [and phonological] rules to a set of
ordered levels that operate successively in the derivation of an inflecte
word: irregular inflection takes place at level 1, compounding at level 2
and regular inflection at level 3.) We argued (in contrast to Bybee, 1991
that the German plural system has a regular (default) affix, namely, the
plural -s, which, according to the level-ordering model, is assigned at level 3.
This ensures that the plural affix -s occurs with nouns that do not yet have
a marked lexical entry for plural. Moreover, because the default plural -s
on level 3, it is unavailable to rules of compounding that apply at level 2
The irregular plural allomorphs are on levels 1 or 2 and are therefore
available for compound formation. In this way, the distribution of plural
inside compounds can be explained (for further details, see Clahsen et al.
1992).
Previous studies found that the plural is marked early and that, from
age 3 on, plural markings are used in about 90% of the obligatory
contexts (Park, 1978). It was also found that overregularizations are rar
and that the plural -n is most often used in overregularizations (Schaner
Wolles, 1988; Veit, 1988). This tentatively suggests that German-speaking
170
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
171
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
172
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
In adult and in child German, simple past tense forms are rarely used
in spoken discourse. Instead, events in the past are expressed through what
is traditionally called the present perfect. This consists of a finite auxiliary
(haben, "to have," or sein, "to be") and nonfinite past participle forms-a
situation similar to that found in English. Therefore, the closest equivalent
to English past tense formation is German participle formation.
As can be seen in the following examples, three patterns of participle
inflection are distinguished in traditional grammars: (i) weak inflection,
which involves -t affixation without stem/root changes (2a); (ii) strong inflec-
tion, that is, -n affixation plus stem changes (2b, 2d); and (iii) mixed inflec-
tion, or -t affixation plus stem changes (2c). In a manner similar to that in
English, ablaut and other changes of the verbal root/stem in German occur
in both simple past tense forms and past participles. Only "weak" verbs
never involve root/stem changes (cf. [2a]). Finally, prefixation with ge- is
prosodically determined: ge- occurs in participles when the verbal root/stem
has stress on the first syllable. Because this is most often the case in German
verbs, the ge- prefix is very frequent:
173
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
the rule in (3b) has default status.2 In order to yield the correct distribution,
the order in which the rules in (3) are applied has to be fixed. Otherwise
nothing would rule out participles such as *gegangt in which the default affi
has been (incorrectly) assigned to a strong verb. This order of applicatio
independently follows from the "elsewhere condition" of lexical morphol
ogy (Kiparsky, 1982, 1985). The elsewhere condition is a general constrain
on morphological processes and ensures that property-specific rules have
priority over default rules, which are applied "elsewhere." Thus, (3a) is
applied on all stems/roots with the specific feature [+ PART]; in the re
maining cases, the general rule (3b) applies.
Participle formation of novel or invented verbs confirms the propose
distinction between (3a) and (3b). Consider, for example, the invented ver
faben. There is only one grammatical way of forming a past participle ou
of such a novel verb, the regular gefab-t; stem/root changes (*gefub-t) an
-n affixation (*gefab-en) are ungrammatical. This shows that, when idiosyn-
cratic information on participle roots is not lexically specified as in the case
of novel verbs, the default rule applies.
Crucially, the linguistic differences between -t and -n affixation in Ger-
man past participle formation do not coincide with quantitative differences
in vocabulary statistics. In German, verbs affixed with the participle affix -t
do not outnumber the irregular verbs requiring the participle affix -n.3 This
is in contrast to past tense formation in English, where the default status of
the -ed suffix is confounded with its high frequency.4 With respect to acquisi-
tion, the comparison between the two languages may therefore allow th
qualitative and quantitative differences between regular and irregular sys
tems to be teased apart. Such a comparison will be made in the next section.
2 For the small number of verbs in the so-called mixed class we require an additiona
specification to ensure that, in forming past participles, -t is assigned to the irregular pa
tense root. Otherwise, we would get ungrammatical participles such as *gerennt instead o
gerannt. It is not clear whether this has to be specified in a separate rule (cf. Wunderlich
1991).
S According to Ruoff (1981), 1,000 verb types account for 96% of all verb tokens in
German. The token frequencies are as follows: 47% strong verb tokens, 32% verb tokens o
the mixed class, and 17% weak verb tokens. The type frequencies among these 1,000 ver
are as follows: 502 strong verb types, 50 verb types of the mixed class, and 448 weak ver
types. Thus, in terms of type and token frequencies, verbs requiring the participle affi
-t are similar to verbs requiring the participle affix -n. A second relevant source is Meie
(1964), who counted the 8,000 most frequent word forms in German texts. Among th
1,200 most frequent word forms are 23 strong past participles with the -n affix, 3 partic
ples of the mixed class with the -t affix, and 8 participles of weak verbs with the -t affix
Again, there is no frequency preference for the (regular) -t affix.
4 In Francis & Kucera (1982), there are 26,201 past tense tokens and 2,347 types.
Regular past tense forms make up 59% of the tokens and 91% of the types; irregula
forms are then 41% of the tokens and 9% of the types. Thus, regular forms are clearly
dominant in English, in both type and token frequency.
174
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
175
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
flee-fled), etc. Such errors, however, do not occur in the data. The only
kinds of stem errors we found were regular stems replacing irregular ones,
for example, *gebind instead of gebunden (bounded) and *gewinn instead of
gewonnen (won).
The observed asymmetries in the distribution of errors suggest that
children qualitatively distinguish between regular and irregular inflection.
Specifically, the German data indicate that only default rules such as the -t
affixation rule are overregularized; in contrast, irregularizations, that is,
overextensions of irregular patterns (the affix -n and irregular stems), to
regular verbs are not productive. The same holds for the data on English
past tense formation investigated by Marcus et al. Strikingly, these parallels
between the results obtained from English and those obtained from German
were found despite differences between the two languages in terms of vo-
cabulary frequencies. This shows that the regular-irregular distinction can-
not be reduced to frequency differences.
Conclusion
176
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MARCUS ET AL.
References
Anderson, S. (1985). Phonology in the twentieth century: Theories of rules and theories of represen-
tation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Bybee, J. (1991). Natural morphology: The organization of paradigms and language ac-
quisition. In T. Huebner & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Crosscurrents in second language acquisi-
tion and linguistic theories. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
177
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
MONOGRAPHS
Bybee, J., & Slobin, D. I. (1982). Rules and schemas in the development and use of English
past tense. Language, 58, 265-289.
Clahsen, H. (1991). Child language and developmental dysphasia: Linguistic studies of the acquisi-
tion of German. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Clahsen, H., Rothweiler, M., Woest, A., & Marcus, G. F. (1992). Regular and irregular
inflection in the acquisition of German noun plurals. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Fletcher, P. (1991, September). The significance for the cognition hypothesis of an unusual past
time form in an English child. Paper presented at the Symposium for Rick Cromer,
Cambridge.
Francis, N., & Kucera, H. (1982). Frequency analysis of English usage: Lexicon and grammar.
Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Gordon, P. (1985). Level-ordering in lexical development. Cognition, 21, 73-93.
Halle, M. (1990). An approach to morphology. North-Eastern Linguistic Society, 20, 150-184.
Kiparsky, P. (1982). From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology. In H. V. D. Hulst & N.
Smith (Eds.), The structure of phonological representations (Pt. 1). Dordrecht: Foris.
Kiparsky, P. (1985). Some consequences of lexical phonology. In C. Ewen & J. Anderson
(Eds.), Phonology yearbook (Vol. 2). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
K6pcke, K.-M. (1988). Schemas in German plural formation. Lingua, 74, 303-335.
Meier, H. (1964). Deutsche Sprachstatistik. Hildesheim: Olms.
Mills, A. (1985). The acquisition of German. In D. Slobin (Ed.), The cross-linguistic study of
language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mugdan, J. (1977). Flexionsmorphologie und Psycholinguistik. TiAbingen: Narr.
Park, T.-Z. (1978). Plurals in child speech. Journal of Child Language, 5, 237-250.
Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1992). Regular and irregular morphology and the psychological
status of rules of grammar. In Proceedings of the 17th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley
Linguistics Society. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
Rothweiler, M., & Clahsen, H. (1991, October). Past participles in German child language: A
comparative study of normal and disordered language development. Paper presented at the
Boston University Conference on Language Development.
Ruoff, A. (1981). Hdufigkeitsw6rterbuch gesprochener Sprache. Tiubingen: Niemeyer.
Schaner-Wolles, C. (1988). Plural vs. Komparativerwerb im Deutschen: Von der Diskre-
panz zwischen konzeptueller und morphologischer Entwicklung. In H. Gfinter (Ed.),
Experimentelle Studien zur Flexionsmorphologie. Hamburg: Buske.
Veit, S. (1988). Das Verstandnis von Plural- und Komparativformen bei (entwicklungs)
dysgrammatischen Kindern im Vorschulalter. In G. Kegel et al. (Eds.), Sprechwis-
senschaft und Psycholinguistik. Opladen: Westdeutscher.
Wunderlich, D. (1991). Eine Unterspezifikationsanalyse der deutschen Verbmorphologie.
Unpublished manuscript, University of Diisseldorf.
178
This content downloaded from 103.231.241.108 on Fri, 24 Nov 2017 06:50:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms