Teti&Gelfand 1991
Teti&Gelfand 1991
REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/1131143?seq=1&cid=pdf-
reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
Society for Research in Child Development and Wiley are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Child Development
Douglas M. Teti
University of Maryland Baltimore County
Donna M. Gelfand
University of Utah
TETI, DOUGLAS M., and GELFAND, DONNA M. Behavioral Competence among Mothers
in the First Year: The Mediational Role of Maternal Self-Efficacy. CHILD DEVELOPME
62, 918-929. This study tests the idea that mothers' self-efficacy beliefs mediate the e
parenting behavior of variables such as depression, perceptions of infant temperame
culty, and social-marital supports. Subjects were 48 clinically depressed and 38 non
mothers observed in interaction with their 3-13-month-old infants (M = 7.35 month
dicted, maternal self-efficacy beliefs related significantly to maternal behavioral com
independent of the effects of other variables. When the effects of self-efficacy were co
parenting competence no longer related significantly to social-marital supports or ma
pression. In addition, maternal self-efficacy correlated significantly with perceptions
difficulty after controlling for family demographic variables. These results suggest that
self-efficacy mediates relations between maternal competence and other psychosocial
and may play a crucial role in determining parenting behavior and infant psychosocia
This research was supported by NIMH grant 41474 awarded to the authors. The authors
thank project director Janiece Pompa and research assistants Cathie Fox, Kim Shimoda, Shari
Harbaugh, Heather Walker, and Leslie Poppe. We also thank Donald P. Hartmann and Penny
Jameson for comments on an earlier draft of this paper, and Stanley Feldstein for his statistical
advice. We are indebted to Salt Lake Valley Mental Health Services, participating practitioners,
and John E. Brockert, Director, Bureau of Vital Records, Utah Department of Health, for assis-
tance in subject recruitment. Finally, special thanks are given to the families who participated in
this study. Reprint requests should be addressed to Douglas M. Teti, Department of Psychology,
University of Maryland Baltimore County, Baltimore, MD 21228.
[Child Development, 1991, 62, 918-929. ? 1991 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
All rights reserved. 0009-3920/91/6205-0003$01.00]
because self-efficacy theory has grown efficacy is the mediating link between
largely out of an experimental paradigm that knowledge and behavior (Bandura, 1982,
has examined changes in self-efficacy as a 1986). Mothers who feel efficacious in the
function of the direct induction of success or parenting role would be expected to be suc-
failure experiences (e.g., Bandura & Wood, cessful in establishing warm and harmoni-
1989), positive and negative moods (e.g., ous relationships with their babies. How-
Kavanagh & Bower, 1985), or performance-ever, mothers who feel inefficacious would
contingent feedback (e.g., Newman & Gold-be expected to have more difficulty. Their
fried, 1987). In these and other instances, handling of their babies may be indecisive,
self-efficacy was examined in regard to insensitive, and awkward because they may
clearly defined laboratory tasks. Here we at- lack the problem-solving skills or the persis-
tempt to examine the role of self-efficacy intence required to establish sensitive interac-
the more ambiguous task of parenting in thetions with their children.
home environment. The present study in-
vestigated the role of maternal self-efficacy Though specific to performance in the
in mediating relations between mothers' be- maternal role, maternal self-efficacy should
havior with infants in the first year of life be sensitive to variations in social-marital
and other factors that might affect the supports, perceptions of infant tempera-
mother-baby interaction, such as mothers' ment, and severity of depression. Self-
social-marital supports, level of depression,efficacy theory suggests that social-marital
and perceptions of infant temperament. supports influence maternal self-efficacy di-
rectly, through social persuasion by inti-
A substantial body of literature has re-
mates and by allowing observation of the
lated quality of mothering to social-marital
parenting behavior of significant support
supports, maternal depression, and percep-
figures. For example, friends and relatives
tions of infant temperament, although the
mechanisms that mediate these relations
may reassure a woman that she is a good
have been unclear. Mothers with low levels
mother, and, by design or accident, they may
demonstrate successful child-care routines
of social support and unhappy marriages
for her. In addition, maternal self-efficacy
have been found to behave less optimally
toward their infants than have mothers with
should be sensitive to mothers' perceptions
of their babies as temperamentally "easy" or
more adequate marital and social supports
"difficult." Goldberg (1977) has speculated
(Cox, Owen, Lewis, & Henderson, 1989;
that maternal feelings of competence and ef-
Crnic, Greenberg, Ragozin, Robinson, &
fectance are likely to be fostered by infants
Basham, 1983; Crockenberg & McCluskey,
who are predictable and manageable, who
1986; Downey & Coyne, 1990; Goldberg &
signal their needs effectively, and who are
Easterbrooks, 1984; Hann, 1989). In addi-
easily soothed. By contrast, low maternal
tion, mothers suffering from depression have
self-efficacy might be expected when moth-
been described as substantially less sensi-
ers must cope with babies who are chroni-
tive and more punitive, negative, and re-
cally fussy, irritable, and difficult to read
jecting toward their infants than have nonde-
(see Cutrona & Troutman, 1986). Finally,
pressed mothers (Cohn, Matias, Tronick,
Bandura (1989) has invoked Bower's
Connell, & Lyons-Ruth, 1986; Field, Healy,
affective-priming theory (Bower, 1983; cited
Goldstein, & Guthertz, 1990; Gelfand &
in Bandura, 1989) to argue that depression
Teti, 1990; Lyons-Ruth, Zoll, Connell, &
predisposes individuals to low self-efficacy
Grunebaum, 1986; Radke-Yarrow, Cum-
and poor performance as a result of the se-
mings, Kuczynski, & Chapman, 1985; Weiss- lective activation of memories of failure ex-
man & Paykel, 1974). Finally, mothers' per-
periences. Thus, depressed women should
ceptions of their infants' temperamental
tend to have lower self-efficacy in the mater-
difficulty have been reported to relate to
nal role than should nondepressed women.
mother-infant interaction, although the na-
ture of this relation has been inconsistent
It is important, however, to emphasize that
maternal self-efficacy and maternal depres-
(Crockenberg, 1986).
sion are separable constructs. Whereas as-
We propose that the influences of mater- sessments of depression such as the Beck
nal depression, social-marital supports, and
Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward,
perceptions of infant temperament on the Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) tap
mother-infant relationship are largely indi- into general depressive symptomatology and
rect and mediated by mothers' feelings of dysphoria, maternal self-efficacy is specific
efficacy in the maternal role. This expecta- to women's perceived performance in the
tion is consistent with the view that self- maternal role. Thus, although maternal self-
efficacy is expected to relate to women's were not included. Most mothers were re-
level of global depression, it is expectedferred
to to the project by their therapists (ex-
have multiple determinants and to have cept three who answered newspaper ads),
clear, straightforward links to maternal be-
and therapists' reported that approximately
havior. 85%-90% of the mothers whom they ap-
In the present study, the mediating roleproached volunteered for the study. Thirty-
eight mothers who were not in therapy of
of maternal self-efficacy was tested using the
any kind were recruited from the same areas
criteria for mediation outlined by Baron and
Kenny (1986). That is, independent vari- of the city as the depressed mothers and
ables should be significantly associated with were located through the State Department
of Vital Statistics birth registry. These moth-
the dependent variable, the mediating vari-
able should show significant relations to ers were sent a description of the project and
both the independent and dependent vari-returned a postcard to the investigators if
they were interested in participating; 15.5%
ables, and statistical control of the mediating
variable should substantially reduce rela-of the mothers who were contacted agreed
tions between the independent and depen-to participate. The nondepressed group had
dent variables. Thus, if maternal self- significantly higher annual family incomes.
efficacy is the final, mediating variable In addition, infant age was significantly
linking parenting behavior (the dependent higher in the nondepressed group, although
variable) to mothers' social-marital supports,the difference in real terms was slight. No
depression, and perceptions of infant tem- other group differences were found for any
perament (the independent variables), then: of the remaining demographic indices.
(a) maternal self-efficacy should relate sig- Procedure
nificantly to perceptions of infant tempera- Two female research assistants who
ment, level of depression, social-marital sup-were unaware of the mothers' psychiatric
ports, and objective assessments of maternalstatus visited mothers in their homes three
behavioral competence; (b) maternal self- times (1.5-2 hours each visit) over a 1-month
efficacy should remain significantly associ- period. Visitors established rapport with
ated with maternal behavioral competence mothers during the first visit by inquiring
after controlling for social-marital supports,about the mothers' recent pregnancy, labor,
level of depression, and perceptions of in- and delivery.
fant temperament; (c) after removing the ef-
fects of maternal self-efficacy, maternal be- Questionnaire measures.-During the
havioral competence should no longer relate three visits, mothers completed a variety of
questionnaires designed to obtain informa-
significantly to social-marital supports, level
of depression, and perceptions of infant tion regarding demographics and maternal
temperament. Thus, maternal self-efficacy psychosocial functioning. Psychosocial mea-
should play a central role in determining sures administered during the first visit in-
maternal competence, mediating any influ- cluded the Beck Depression Inventory
ences of maternal depression, social-marital (BDI; Beck et al., 1961) and the infant fussy-
supports, and infant temperament. difficult subscale from the Infant Character-
istics Questionnaire (Bates, Freeland, &
Method Lounsbury, 1979). The BDI is an exten-
sively used self-report measure of depres-
Subjects sion, and Beck et al. (1981) reported statisti-
Subjects were 86 mothers (including cally48significant relations between BDI
depressed mothers) and their infants whoand clinicians' ratings of depth of de-
scores
were participating in a longitudinal study of in a sample of 226 adult psychiatric
pression
infants of depressed and nondepressed patients. The Infant Characteristics Ques-
mothers (see Table 1 for subject descrip- tionnaire is a brief and behaviorally an-
tions). Mothers were invited to participate choredif measure of perceptions of infant tem-
they had a healthy infant within the first perament
year that has impressive convergence
of life. Ninety-seven percent of the mothers with the Carey Survey of Temperamental
were between 18 and 40 years of age,Characteristics
with (Bates et al., 1979). On the
three mothers between 16 and 17 years of visit, mothers completed a marital
second
age. Infants were 3 to 13 months oldharmony (M = scale adapted from Locke and Wal-
7.35 months). Forty-eight of the mothers lace (1959) and Spanier (1976), the Sense of
were in therapy when recruited and had a
Competence Scale of the Parenting Stress
DSM-III-R diagnosis of depression from Index (PSI; Abidin, 1986), and a Maternal
their therapists. Mothers with bipolar illnessSelf-Efficacy Scale developed for this proj-
TABLE 1
GROUP
Race (%):
White ................................... 100 94.7
Black ...................................... . 0 2.6
H ispanic.......... ............. .................... 0 2.6
Religion (%):
LDS (Mormon)................................. 68.1 55.6
Catholic ........................................ 6.4 13.9
Protestant .................................. .......... 8.5 8.3
None or other ..................................... 17.1 22.2
Educational attainment (%):
Grades 1-8 ...................................... 4.5 5.1
Grades 9-12 ......................................... 16.7 2.6
High school ..................................... 12.1 13.8
Some college/vocational ...................... 47.0 43.6
College graduate .................................... 15.2 33.3
Graduate/professional .......................... 4.5 2.6
Mean family income ($)............................. 17,837 (17,947) 32,577 (15,184)***
Mean hours per week mother employed. 9.5 (15.19) 8.8 (13.9)
Marital status (%):
Single ........................................... 18.8 13.2
Married/living with partner ................ 81.3 86.8
Mean maternal age (years) ........................ 28.52 (5.83) 28.36 (4.64)
Mean infant age (months) ......................... 6.77 (3.12) 7.97 (1.94)*
Sex of infants (%):
Female ........................... ........... 39.6 44.7
Male ............................................. 60.4 55.3
Birth order status of infant ................ 2.40 2.13
Mean hours per week infants in day-care .41 (2.56) 1.31 (7.42)
NOTE.-Numbers in parentheses are standard deviations.
* Significantly different from the depressed group, p < .05.
*** Significantly different from the depressed group, p < .001.
theoretically
ect by the authors. Locke and Wallace (1959) grounded negative relations
and Spanier (1976) each found their between
measure social supports and neuroticism
of marital harmony to converge with scores on the Eysenck Personality Inven-
clini-
cians' judgments of satisfied and tory. malad-
justed marital dyads. Abidin (1986) has re-
ported an alpha reliability of .74 forOur themeasure of maternal self-efficacy is
Sense of Competence Scale, and the scale
intended to be faithful to Bandura's concept
has been found to discriminate predictably of self-efficacy as being highly situation- or
between mothers of hyperactive children domain-specific, in contrast to other, more
and mothers of normal children (Mash & global parental self-efficacy scales, such as
Johnston, 1983) and between physically those by Abidin (1986) and Gibaud-Wallston
abusive and nonabusive mothers (Mash, and Wandersman (1978). Nine of the 10
Johnston, & Kovitz, 1983). On the third visit,4-point maternal self-efficacy items ad-
mothers completed a questionnaire adapta-dressed mothers' feelings of efficacy in rela-
tion of the Interview Schedule for Social In- tion to specific, delimited domains of infant
teraction (Henderson, Byrne, & Duncan- care, such as soothing the baby, understand-
Jones, 1981), a social support measure that ing what the baby wants, getting the baby
assessed availability and adequacy of moth- to understand mother's wishes, maintaining
ers' attachment figures. Henderson et al. joint attention and interaction with the baby,
(1981) have provided evidence for the con- amusing the baby, knowing what the baby
struct validity of these dimensions in finding enjoys, disengaging from the baby, per-
lower scores among individuals who were forming daily routine tasks (e.g., feeding,
new to a geographical area, and by finding changing, and bathing the baby), and getting
TABLE 2
Maternal competence:
Zero-order ............................... .47*** .48*** - .39*** -.30**
Partial ....................................... .44*** .38** -.36** - .20+
Maternal self-efficacy:
Zero-order ....................................48*** - .57*** -.50**
Partial ...........................................44 ** - .54 ** - .39**
Social-marital supports:
Zero-order ...................................- .61 ** - .38***
Partial ........................................- .58*** - 31**
Maternal depression (BDI):
Zero-order ............... ............39***
Partial .......................... ............... .30**
NOTE.-All significance tests are two-tailed.
a Controlling for maternal education, family income, infant
partner).
Sp= .10.
** p < .01.
*** p < .001.
competence and each of the four predictors ried or living with a partner tended to have
were statistically significant and in the ex-higher maternal competence scores (M =
pected direction (positively to maternal 42.16, n = 71) than did mothers without
self-efficacy and social-marital supports and partners (M = 38.89, n = 14), F(1,83) =
negatively to severity of maternal depres- 3.69, p = .058. Maternal competence did not
sion and infant difficulty). As predicted, ma- relate significantly to any other demographic
ternal self-efficacy was significantly and pos- index. Thus, maternal education, family in-
come, infant age, and marital status were
itively related to social-marital supports and
used as covariates in subsequent regression
negatively related to severity of maternal de-
pression and infant difficulty. Table 2 also analyses. With one exception (infant diffi-
shows significant negative correlations be- culty), the correlations among maternal com-
tween social marital supports and maternal petence and all predictor variables remained
depression and infant difficulty and a sig- statistically significant when these covari-
nificant positive association between mater- ates were partialed out (see Table 2).
nal depression and maternally rated infant
Preliminary one-way analyses of vari-
difficulty.
ance were then performed to explore group
In order to determine if any covariates differences on maternal competence and
should be used in subsequent analyses, we each of the four predictors. As expected, the
explored for any simple, zero-order relations depressed mothers as a group had signifi-
between maternal competence and all de- cantly higher BDI scores (M = 23.13, SD =
mographic variables. Correlates of maternal 10.78, range = 3-46) than did the control
competence were maternal education, r = mothers (M = 7.13, SD = 5.17, range =
.32, p = .003, and family income, r = .33, 0-20, F[1,84] = 70.68, p < .0001) and sig-
p = .003. In addition, a positive relation that nificantly lower scores on social-marital sup-
approached significance was found between ports (M = -.91 vs. 1.75, F[1,76] = 33.58,
maternal competence and infant age, r = p < .0001),' maternal self-efficacy (M =
.21, p = .052, and mothers who were mar- 29.63 vs. 33.05, F[1,84] = 12.86, p < .001),
1Analyses were also conducted to examine the possibility that LDS (Mormon) mothers
differed from non-LDS mothers in the amount and range of social-marital supports received. A
one-way analysis of variance revealed no differences in the amount of social-marital supports
reported by LDS and non-LDS mothers. In addition, the variability of social-marital supports
was virtually identical in both groups (LDS mothers: M = .086, SD = 2.57; non-LDS mothers:
M = .393, SD = 2.25, F[1,80] = .55, p = .46).
2 Results were virtually identical when the four demographic covariates were omitted from
analyses.
TABLE 3
Step and
Order of Entry Multiple R R2 Increase df F Change p Beta
1-4:
Maternal education ...................... .31
Family income........................... .40 .16 4,69 3.22 .017 .19
Infant age ...................................... .13
Marital status .............................. .17
5-7:
Maternal depression (BDI) ......... -.34
Infant difficulty .......................... .55 .15 3,66 4.70 .005 -.12
Social-marital supports ................ .24
8:
Maternal self-efficacy ................ .60 .05 1,65 5.23 .026 .31
ficulty scores
be tested, and finally by the two-way interac-or high BDI scores
tive term itself. Only the maternal depres- scores had interm
infant difficulty
ternal was
sion x infant difficulty interaction self-efficacy
sig- scores (Y = 2
nificantly related to maternal 20.63, respectively).
self-efficacy (r2 As these valu
change = .04, F change [1,64] = 5.42,
cate, p =
the interaction was account
.023, beta at entry = -.88). To
the examine this
larger difference in predicted
interaction further, predictedself-efficacy
maternal scores
self-between depressed
efficacy scores were calculated from
mothers with the
highbeta
versus low infant diffi-
culty scores
weights supplied by this regression than between nondepressed
analysis
(following Cohen & Cohen, 1983). Predicted
mothers with high versus low infant diffi-
maternal self-efficacy scores were
culty scores. lowest
among mothers who reported high BDI and
infant difficulty scores (Y = 13.01) and high-
Discussion
est among mothers with low BDI and low
These
infant difficulty scores (Y = 29.38). results support the premise tha
Mothers
who had either low BDI and high infant
maternal dif-
self-efficacy is a central mediat
TABLE 4
Step and
Order of Entry Multiple R R2 Increase df F Change p Beta
1-4:
Maternal education ............. .31
Family income ................. .40 .16 4,69 3.22 .017 .19
Infant age............................. .13
M arital status ....................... .17
5:
Maternal self-efficacy ......... .56 .16 1,68 16.06 <.001 .43
6:
Maternal depression ........ .58 .02 1,67 1.74 .19 -.17
6:
Social-marital supports ....... .60 .04 1,67 3.83 .055 .23
hypothesis that depression predisposes indi- incidental induction of basic problem sol
viduals to low self-efficacy. Especially strik- ing skills.
ing was the finding that maternal depression
had no relation to maternal competence in- The present study has several limita-
dependent of maternal self-efficacy. As with tions that must be addressed. First, it is pos-
infant difficulty, maternal depression related
sible that the relations obtained are in part
to maternal competence only to the extent the result of shared method variance; that
that it potentially affected maternal self- is, our measures of severity of depression,
efficacy. Thus, whereas maternal self- social-marital supports, infant difficulty, and
efficacy may have been influenced by level maternal self-efficacy were obtained directly
of depression, the latter's association withfrom maternal reports. While this concern
maternal behavior appeared to be depen-cannot be minimized, it appears that shared
dent on maternal self-efficacy. The present method variance alone cannot account for
findings thus support the premise that ma- the obtained results. To begin, the maternal
ternal self-efficacy is specific to women'scompetence ratings were made by indepen-
perceived performance in the maternal role dent observers, so not all data took the form
and is a viable construct in its own right. of maternal reports. In addition, maternal
Indeed, maternal self-efficacy was extremely self-efficacy was the most robust predictor of
low among depressed mothers who per- maternal competence. This relation re-
ceived their infants as difficult but substan- mained statistically significant after partial-
tially higher among depressed mothers who ing out the effects of all other variables,
perceived their infants as less difficult. whereas maternal competence was no
These findings suggest that some depressedlonger significantly associated with maternal
mothers may nevertheless develop adequatedepression and social-marital supports when
feelings of efficacy in the maternal role, es-maternal self-efficacy was partialed out. We
pecially if they perceive their babies as hav- would not have expected these differential
ing "easy" temperaments. The present study correlational patterns if the findings ob-
did not determine whether or not easy in-tained were due primarily to shared method
fants of depressed mothers are less at risk variance. A second limitation of the present
than difficult infants. As an easy infant's be- study was that our observations of maternal
havioral repertoire develops, that child may behavior were relatively brief, and despite
become more likely to be perceived as a the fact that corresponding dimensions of
source of stress by a demoralized mother,maternal behavior in the feeding and free-
with negative consequences for the relation- play observation contexts were significantly
ship. Greater focus needs to be given to the intercorrelated, our samples of maternal be-
role of infant temperament in shaping de-havior may not have been truly representa-
pressed and nondepressed mothers' mater-tive. Studies of the psychosocial correlates
nal self-efficacy and mothering skills. of mother-infant interaction have varied
widely with regard to duration of observa-
The significant relation between social-tions, ranging from 3-min sessions of face-
marital supports and maternal self-efficacy to-face interactions (Cohn et al., 1986; Field
supports Bandura's (1982, 1986) premise et al., 1990) to several hours of unstructured
that self-efficacy is sensitive to social persua-activities (Crockenberg & McCluskey, 1986;
sion and modeling influences provided byRadke-Yarrow et al., 1985). The two 10-min
significant others. Social-marital supportsobservational periods used in the present
may also have influenced maternal self- study were viewed as behavioral probes, de-
efficacy indirectly via its relation to maternalsigned to examine affective dimensions of
depression. It is interesting that, althoughmaternal behavior in specific contexts. As a
the significant relation between social- group, our depressed mothers were rated as
marital supports and maternal competenceless competent than nondepressed mothers,
was eliminated when maternal self-efficacya finding that is in line with other studies
was statistically controlled, the relation nev- of depressed mothers that employed either
ertheless approached significance (p = very short (e.g., Cohn et al., 1986) or very
.055). Conceivably, maternal self-efficacylong observational periods (e.g., Radke-
may be only one of several mechanisms Yarrow et al., 1985). Third, our sample was
through which social-marital supports po- heavily skewed with LDS (Mormon) moth-
tentially influence mothers' behavioral com- ers, which might have affected the results.
petence. As Rutter (1987) has discussed, fu- There is no reason to believe, however, that
ture research may wish to examine how psychosocial correlates of mothering among
social-marital supports influence quality ofpredominantly middle-class LDS mothers
mothering directly, perhaps via the direct or should be different from other middle-class
Litt, M. D.
Crockenberg, S. B. (1986). Are temperamental (1988). Cognitive mediators of stressful
dif-
experience: Self-efficacy and perceived con-
ferences in babies associated with predict-
trol.
able differences in care-giving? In J. V. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 12,
Ler-
ner & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Temperament 241-260.
and
social interaction in infants and Locke, H., & Wallace, K. (1959). Short marital-
children:
New directions for child development adjustment
(W. and prediction tests: Their reli-
Damon, editor-in-chief) (pp. 53-73). San ability and validity. Marriage and Family Liv-
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. ing, 21, 251-255.
Crockenberg, S., & McCluskey, K. (1986). Lyons-Ruth, K., Zoll, D., Connell, D., & Grune-
Changes in maternal behavior during the baum, H. U. (1986). The depressed mother
baby's first year of life. Child Development, and her one-year-old infant: Environment, in-
57, 746-753. teraction, attachment, and infant develop-
Cutrona, C. E., & Troutman, B. R. (1986). Social ment. In E. Z. Tronick & T. Field (Eds.), Ma-
support, infant temperament, and parenting ternal depression and infant disturbance:
self-efficacy: A mediational model of postpar- New directions for child development (W.
tum depression. Child Development, 57, Damon, editor-in-chief) (pp. 61-82). San
1507-1518. Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Downey, G., & Coyne, J. C. (1990). Children of Mash, E. J., & Johnson, C. (1983). Parental percep-
depressed parents: An integrative review. tions of child behavior problems, parenting
Psychological Bulletin, 108, 50-76. self-esteem, and mothers' reported stress in
Egeland, B., & Farber, E. A. (1984). Infant-mother younger and older hyperactive and normal
attachment: Factors related to its develop- children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
ment and changes over time. Child Develop- Psychology, 51, 86-99.
ment, 55, 753-771. Mash, E. J., Johnston, C., & Kovitz, K. (1983). A
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., & Guthertz, M. comparison of the mother-child interactions
(1990). Behavior-state matching and syn- of physically abused and non-abused children
chrony in mother-infant interactions of non- during play and task situations. Journal of
depressed versus depressed dyads. Develop- Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 337-346.
mental Psychology, 26, 7-14. Newman, C., & Goldfried, M. R. (1987). Disabus-
Gelfand, D. M., & Teti, D. M. (1990). The effects ing, negative self-efficacy expectations via ex-
of maternal depression on children. Clinical periences, feedback, and discrediting. Cogni-
Psychology Review, 10, 329-353. tive Therapy and Research, 11, 401-417.
Gibaud-Wallston, J., & Wandersman, L. P. (1978, Radke-Yarrow, M., Cummings, E. M., Kuczynski,
September). Development and utility of the L., & Chapman, M. (1985). Patterns of attach-
Parenting Sense of Competence Scale. Paper ment in two- and three-year-olds in normal
presented at the meeting of the American families and families with parental depres-
Psychological Association, Toronto. sion. Child Development, 56, 884-893.
Goldberg, S. (1977). Social competence in in- Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of tempera-
fancy: A model of parent-infant interaction. ment in infancy. Child Development, 52,
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 23, 163-177. 569-578.
Goldberg, W., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (1984). Role Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and pro
of marital quality in toddler development. De- tective mechanisms. American Journal of Or-
velopmental Psychology, 20, 504-514. thopsychiatry, 57, 316-331.
Hann, D. M. (1989). A systems conceptualization Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjust-
of the quality of mother-infant interaction. ment: New scales for assessing the quality of
Infant Behavior and Development, 12, marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Mar-
251-263. riage and the Family, 38, 15-28.
Henderson, S., Byrne, D. G., & Duncan-Jones, Thomas,P. A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and
(1981). Neurosis and the social environment. development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Sydney: Academic Press Australia. Weissman, M. M., & Paykel, E. D. (1974). The
Kavanagh, D. J., & Bower, G. H. (1985). Mood depressed woman: A study of social relation-
and self-efficacy: Impact of joy and sadness ships. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
on perceived capabilities. Cognitive TherapyZoll, D. A., Lyons-Ruth, K., & Connell, D. (1984).
and Research, 9, 507-525. Infants at psychiatric risk: Maternal behav-
Lamb, M. E., & Bornstein, M. H. (1987). Develop- ior, depression, and family history. Paper
ment in infancy. New York: Random House. presented at the 92nd Annual Convention of
Lerner, J. V., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (1986). Tem- the American Psychological Association, To-
perament and social interaction in infants ronto.