(Sutton, 1996) - Stage Theories of Health Behaviour 240-277
(Sutton, 1996) - Stage Theories of Health Behaviour 240-277
STEPHEN SUTTON
6
STAGE THEORIES OF HEALTH
BEHAVIOUR
1 General background
That a whole chapter is dedicated to stage theories in this edition of the book
is indicative of the enormous amount of interest and research activity that
such theories have generated in recent years. This chapter discusses three
stage theories: the transtheoretical model (TTM; Prochaska and DiClemente
1983; Prochaska et al. 1992, 2002; Prochaska and Velicer 1997), the pre-
caution adoption process model (PAPM; Weinstein and Sandman 1992,
2002a, 2002b), and the health action process approach (HAPA; Schwarzer
1992, 1999, 2001, 2004; Schwarzer and Fuchs 1995a, 1995b), although we
argue that the last is not a genuine stage theory but a continuum theory like
the theory of planned behaviour (TPB; Ajzen 1991, 2002; Conner and
Sparks, Chapter 5 in this volume). Other stage theories that should be
mentioned, but are not discussed in this chapter, are the health behaviour
goal model (Gebhardt 1997; Maes and Gebhardt 2000), the Rubicon model,
or model of action phases (Heckhausen 1991; Gollwitzer 1996), a four-stage
model that forms the theoretical background to the work on implementation
intentions (see Sheeran et al., Chapter 7 in this volume), the AIDS risk
reduction model (Catania et al. 1990), and theories of delay in seeking
health care (Safer et al. 1979; Andersen et al. 1995).
We start by presenting a hypothetical three-stage theory to explain the
assumptions of stage theories and how they differ from continuum theories,
and we then discuss the TTM, the PAPM and the HAPA in turn. The
decision to discuss three theories necessitated some modications to the
recommended chapter format. In particular, because the PAPM and the
HAPA are relatively new and have limited evidence bases compared with
the TTM, sections on developments and application of the model are not
included for these two theories.
224 Stephen Sutton
simple three-stage theory outlined above has a more complex structure than
most of the theories discussed in this book. A corollary of this more com-
plex structure is that stage theories are also more difcult to test than other
kinds of theories. More specically, stage theories should be contrasted
with continuum theories. A classic example of a continuum theory is the
theory of reasoned action (TRA; Ajzen and Fishbein 1980; Conner and
Sparks, Chapter 5 in this volume). According to the TRA, the likelihood of
performing the target behaviour is a linear function of the strength of
intention to do so, which is treated as a continuous variable. A person may
move to action from any point on the intention continuum, though their
probability of doing so is assumed to be higher the further along the con-
tinuum they are. Two pseudostages, I and II, could be created by arbitrarily
dividing the intention continuum into two segments. People in these two
pseudostages would be expected to differ on variables that are assumed to
inuence intention strength (i.e. attitude and/or subjective norm). However,
this would not be a genuine stage theory for the following reasons:
1 the ‘stages’ have been arbitrarily created by dividing a continuum;
2 there is no assumption that people in Pseudostage I (low intention) have
to move into Pseudostage II (high intention) before they can move to
action;
3 everyone in the target population is assumed to have an intention with
respect to performing the target behaviour; similarly, everyone is
assumed to have an attitude and a subjective norm with respect to the
target behaviour;
4 the factors that inuence movement along the continuum (i.e. attitude
and/or subjective norm), and that therefore increase the likelihood of
action, are assumed to be the same at every point on the continuum and
to have the same effect sizes at every point on the continuum; and
5 the same intervention (i.e. one designed to increase attitude and/or
subjective norm) would be used regardless of the recipient’s position on
the continuum.
Table 6.1 The TTM constructs, adapted from Prochaska et al. (2002)
Construct Description
Stages of change
Precontemplation Has no intention to take action within the next six
months
Contemplation Intends to take action within the next six months
Preparation Intends to take action within the next 30 days and has
taken some behavioural steps in this direction
Action Has changed overt behaviour for less than six months
Maintenance Has changed overt behaviour for more than six months
Decisional balance
Pros The benets of changing
Cons The costs of changing
Self-efcacy
Condence Condence that one can engage in the healthy
behaviour across different challenging situations
Temptation Temptation to engage in the unhealthy behaviour across
different challenging situations
Processes of change
Experiential processes
Consciousness raising Finding and learning new facts, ideas, and tips that
support the healthy behaviour change
Dramatic relief Experiencing the negative emotions (fear, anxiety,
worry) that go along with unhealthy behavioural risks
Self-reevaluation Realizing that the behaviour change is an important
part of one’s identity as a person
Environmental Realizing the negative impact of the unhealthy
reevaluation behaviour or the positive impact of the healthy
behaviour on one’s proximal social and physical
environment
Self-liberation Making a rm commitment to change
Behavioural processes
Helping relationships Seeking and using social support for the healthy
behaviour change
Counterconditioning Substituting healthier alternative behaviours and
cognitions for the unhealthy behaviour
Reinforcement Increasing the rewards for the positive behaviour
management change and decreasing the rewards of the unhealthy
behaviour
Stimulus control Removing reminders or cues to engage in the unhealthy
behaviour and adding cues and reminders to engage in
the healthy behaviour
Social liberation Realizing that the social norms are changing in the
direction of supporting the healthy behaviour change
Stage Theories of Health Behaviour 227
Behaviour Authors
processes. For example, consciousness raising was used most in the con-
templation or preparation stages in 80 per cent of studies on smoking and
psychotherapy but was used most in the action or maintenance stages in 88
per cent of studies on substance abuse, exercise and diet change; and
reinforcement management was used most in the action or maintenance
stage in nearly all studies of exercise, smoking and psychotherapy but was
used most during contemplation or preparation in two-thirds of the studies
on substance abuse and diet change.
The mainly linear patterns found by Rosen (2000), particularly for
behavioural processes, do not provide strong support for a stage model. If
differences between process use between adjacent stages are interpreted as
causal effects of process use on stage transition, Rosen’s ndings suggest
that interventions should encourage the use of behavioural processes
throughout the process of change from pre-contemplation through action.
As noted, the ndings for experiential processes were more variable.
One problem with Rosen’s (2000) analysis is that he combined studies
that used different staging methods. Given the differences between the
different methods, it would be preferable to combine only studies of a
particular behaviour that used the same staging method.
Table 6.3 Mean sample-weighted corrected effect sizes (d+) for differences
between adjacent stages from the meta-analysis by Marshall and Biddle (2001)
Variable k PC vs C C vs PR PR vs A A vs M
Effect sizes for comparisons of adjacent stages are shown in Table 6.3. All
the effect sizes for self-efcacy were positive and signicant; the effect size
differed for different comparisons, though this was not tested formally. The
effect sizes for the pros of changing were all positive and signicant except
for contemplation to preparation. The cons of changing showed signicant
decreases across successive stages. Effect sizes for the processes of change
were based on fewer studies (k = 5) than for the other variables. For each of
the behavioural processes, the largest effect was for the transition from pre-
contemplation to contemplation and the smallest effect was for the tran-
sition from action to maintenance; the difference between action and
maintenance was non-signicant for four out of ve processes. For the
experiential processes, the largest effect again occurred between pre-con-
templation and contemplation. Differences between action and main-
tenance were non-signicant for four processes and signicantly negative
(i.e. showed a decrease) for the fth (self-re-evaluation).
Do these results support a stage model? For pros and experiential pro-
cesses, there is clear evidence for a discontinuity pattern. There is a steep
increase between pre-contemplation and contemplation, little or no
increase between contemplation and preparation, and an increase between
preparation and action. For both behavioural and experiential processes,
there is further evidence of discontinuity in that preparation to action is
associated with an increase whereas action to maintenance is not.
Marshall and Biddle (2001) interpret their ndings as mainly supportive
of the TTM predictions. However, our interpretation of the ndings in
terms of discontinuity patterns leads to somewhat different conclusions. If
we assume that a difference in process use between two adjacent stages
reects a causal effect of process use on the likelihood of making the
transition, then Marshall and Biddle’s ndings suggest, for example, that
pre-contemplators who use behavioural processes relatively frequently
(compared with others in that stage) are more likely to move to the con-
templation stage but that people in the action stage who use behavioural
processes relatively frequently (compared with others in that stage) are not
more likely to move to the maintenance stage (with the possible exception
of counter-conditioning). It seems unlikely that the TTM would make these
predictions. Similarly, the ndings suggest that contemplators who make
more frequent use of experiential processes are not more likely than others
in the same stage to move to the preparation stage.
This highlights an important difference in the way in which the Rhode
Island group interprets cross-sectional data on stage differences and the
interpretation suggested by Weinstein et al. (1998c) and Sutton (2000b).
Consider Figure 6.3, which shows a hypothetical pattern of means across
stages; assume that this represents the ndings for behavioural processes.
The Rhode Island group would interpret the relatively frequent use of
behavioural processes among people in the action and maintenance stages
as indicating that use of these processes is particularly important at these
stages and therefore needs to be encouraged. The alternative interpretation
focuses on the differences between adjacent stages rather than the absolute
232 Stephen Sutton
Figure 6.3 Hypothetical pattern of means across the ve TTM stages. [PC = pre-
contemplation; C = contemplation; PR = preparation; A = action; M =
maintenance.]
identical in the two studies, the mean T-scores and standard deviations will
in general differ between the studies. Primary studies of the TTM should
therefore always report stage means and standard deviations based on the
raw scores as well as, or instead of, means and standard deviations based on
T-scores. Meta-analyses that combine data across studies in the way that
Rosen (2000) did should use the means based on the raw scores. The
technique used by Marshall and Biddle (2001), namely to compute stan-
dardized effect sizes for each pair of adjacent stages, is not affected by this
problem.1
who are ready to quit smoking. At one month, 30 participants had pro-
gressed to contemplation, one participant had progressed to preparation,
and ve participants had progressed to action. Contrary to the hypothesis, a
greater percentage of participants in the stage-mismatched condition (54
per cent) progressed than in the stage-matched (30 per cent) or assessment-
only (35 per cent) conditions; however, this difference was not signicant.
Signicantly more smokers in the stage-mismatched condition tried to quit
smoking than in the stage-matched condition.
Quinlan and McCaul (2000) suggest that a mismatched intervention may
have different effects depending on whether it is matched to a later stage in
the sequence (as in their own study) or to an earlier stage. For example,
although it may not be detrimental for smokers in the pre-contemplation
stage to receive an intervention designed for those in the preparation stage, it
may be counterproductive to give preparers an intervention designed for pre-
contemplators. The Dijkstra et al. (1998a) study provided very weak support
for this hypothesis. Nevertheless, it may be worth testing in future studies.
In the most recent study, Blissmer and McAuley (2002) studied physical
activity. 288 university staff were randomly assigned to four conditions,
including: (a) stage-matched materials (personalized, stage-appropriate
covering letter plus stage-matched manuals) delivered via campus mail on a
monthly basis; and (b) stage-mismatched materials delivered in the same
way. After 16 weeks, 40.4 per cent of the matched group had progressed
one or more stages compared with 31.8 per cent of the mismatched group.
This difference was in the predicted direction. The authors did not report a
signicance test, but secondary analysis showed that it did not approach
signicance at the 0.05 level: 2(2) = 0.91, p = 0.634. A limitation of the
study, which the authors acknowledge, is that 57 per cent of participants
were in the action or maintenance stage at baseline, and the short follow-up
period would have prevented those who had recently entered the action
stage from progressing to maintenance.
Considered together, these three experimental studies of matched and
mismatched interventions found little or no evidence for the stage model
predictions. Intervention studies that have compared TTM-based stage-
matched interventions with generic, non-matched interventions or no-
intervention control conditions are considered in Section 2.6.
2.3 Developments
This section outlines several variants of the TTM. First, a group of
researchers in the Netherlands has developed a version of the TTM and
applied it in a number of studies of smoking cessation (e.g. Dijkstra et al.
1996, 1997, 1998a, 2003; De Vries and Mudde 1998). The stage denitions
in the Dutch version of the model differ from the most widely used TTM
denitions in that the pre-action stages are dened purely in terms of
intention: preparation is dened as planning to quit in the next month and
contemplation as planning to quit in the next six months but not in the next
month. In some studies, the group has subdivided the pre-contemplation
Stage Theories of Health Behaviour 237
not possible to show that the TPB variables were more strongly associated
with stage or were better predictors of stage transitions than the TTM
variables or whether they contributed additional predictive power. There
are some similarities between the two sets of variables. For example,
behavioural beliefs in the TPB are similar in some respects to the pros and
cons in the TTM. However, they differ from pros and cons in that they are
based theoretically on the expectancy-value principle and they distinguish
between expectancy (belief strength) and value (outcome evaluation).
Stages of change
Are you currently a smoker?
* Yes, I currently smoke
(For smokers only) In the last year, how many times have you quit smoking for at
least 24 hours?
(For smokers only) Are you seriously thinking of quitting smoking?
* Yes, within the next 30 days (PREPARATION STAGE if they have one 24-hour
quit attempt in the past year – refer to previous question . . . if no quit attempt
then CONTEMPLATION STAGE)
* Yes, within the next 6 months (CONTEMPLATION STAGE)
Scoring:
Experiential processes
Consciousness raising 4, 11
Environmental re-evaluation 6, 12
Self-re-evaluation 8, 15
Social liberation 3, 14
Dramatic relief 7, 18
Behavioral processes
Helping relationships 10, 16
Self-liberation 2, 13
Counterconditioning 1, 17
Reinforcement management 5, 20
Stimulus control 9, 19
Scoring:
Positive affect/social situation 1, 4, 7
Negative affect situations 3, 6, 9
Habitual/craving situations 2, 5, 8
Scoring:
PROS 1, 3, 5
CONS 2, 4, 6
Note: It states on the website that ‘All measures are copyright Cancer Prevention Research
Center, 1991. Dr James O. Prochaska, Director of the CPRC, is pleased to extend his
permission for you to use the Transtheoretical Model-based measures available on this website
for research purposes only, provided that the appropriate citation is referenced.’
Table 6.5 Staging algorithm for exercise, from Marcus and Simkin (1993)
Items
1 I currently do not exercise
2 I intend to exercise in the next 6 months
3 I currently exercise regularly
4 I have exercised regularly for the past 6 months
Scoring
Pre-contemplation: Item 1 = true and Item 2 = false.
Contemplation: Item 1 = true and Item 2 = true.
Preparation: Item 1 = false and Item 3 = false.
Action: Item 3 = true and Item 4 = false.
Maintenance: Item 3 = true and Item 4 = true.
stages’ (Velicer et al. 1990: 273). However, the assumption that the two
measures are interchangeable has been challenged by Segan et al. (2004a).
Compared with the long forms, the short-form measures are more sui-
table for use in studies that use telephone interviewing and in intervention
studies involving repeated assessment. Using the short forms, all the con-
structs in the TTM can be measured with a total of 35 items. However, it is
likely that the reliability of the short-form measures is lower than that of
the long forms, and content validity may also be compromised because a
construct may not be adequately represented by two or three items. For
example, the short-form decisional balance scale does not include items
about the health consequences of smoking, the nancial costs, or the belief
that smoking helps keep weight down.
In the measures of the TTM independent variables, scale scores are
created by computing the sum or the mean of the item scores. The short-
form processes of change measure consists of 10 two-item scales assessing
recent frequency of use; aggregate scores can also be created for the
experiential and behavioural processes respectively. Borland et al. (2000)
improved the wording of several of the items and also discussed some
remaining problems. The short-form temptation measure has three sub-
scales: positive affect or social situations; negative affect situations; and
habitual/craving situations. An aggregate temptation score can also be
computed. Finally, the short-form decisional balance measure comprises
two subscales representing the pros and cons of smoking, respectively. The
usual practice is to standardize these separately and then compare them (see
Section 2.2).
The Rhode Island group’s website lists measures of the TTM independent
variables for exercise and some other health-related behaviours, though the
full set of measures is not given for all the behaviours listed. Note that, for
many behaviours, the condence measure may be more appropriate than
the temptation measure.
2.5.2 Method
Participants were 325 cigarette smokers who called the Quitline telephone
counselling and information service in Victoria, Australia, and were quit at
either the three-month follow-up and/or the six-month follow-up. The
mean age was 39 years (range 17 to 78) and 55 per cent were women; mean
cigarette consumption at baseline was 21 cigarettes a day.
Participants were recruited into the study after their reason for calling the
Quitline had been dealt with. Of the callers asked to participate in the study,
77 per cent did so. They completed a telephone interview (time 1) and were
posted the same questionnaire at three months (time 2) and six months
(time 3), and a shorter version at 12 months (time 4). Response rates to the
follow-ups were 76 per cent, 74 per cent and 68 per cent respectively.
The sample was part of a larger study that had several intervention
groups. Segan et al. (2004a) argue that the interventions should not be
regarded as an interfering factor because they are presumed to have their
effect by inuencing the TTM independent variables, not by producing
change in fundamentally different ways.
The predictor variables consisted of the short-form measures of the
processes of change, the pros and cons of smoking, and temptation to
smoke (see Section 2.4).4 Two of the experiential change processes, dra-
matic relief and social liberation, were not measured. Minor modications
were made to the wording of some of the change process items (see Borland
et al. 2000). At follow-up, smoking status was assessed by the question ‘Are
you currently a cigarette smoker?’ (Yes/No), and length of abstinence by the
question ‘How long ago did you quit?’, answered in days, weeks or months.
In the analysis, Segan et al. (2004a) compared ex-smokers who had quit
for less than one month and ex-smokers who had quit for more than one
month. Although the TTM does not make this distinction, the authors
present both theoretical and empirical reasons for using this time point. For
example, relapse rates are likely to differ signicantly between these groups.
In terms of the TTM stages, those who were quit at time 2 were in the
action stage; those who were still abstinent at time 3 were either still in
the action stage or had moved to maintenance. Ex-smokers who had quit for
less than one month at time 3 were in the action stage; those who were still
off smoking at time 4 had moved to the maintenance stage. Ex-smokers who
had quit for more than one month at time 3 were in action or maintenance;
those who were still abstinent at time 4 had moved to the maintenance stage.
Predictors of relapse were examined by conducting a series of logistic
regression analyses (one for each predictor measure), controlling in each
case for intervention condition and length of abstinence.
Stage Theories of Health Behaviour 245
2.5.3 Results
Relapse between time 2 and time 3 Thirty per cent of the 247 participants
who were quit at time 2 relapsed by time 3. Relapse at time 3 was predicted
by higher aggregate temptations (p = 0.001), higher positive/social temp-
tations (p < 0.001), higher habit/addictive temptations (p = 0.03), higher
negative/affective temptations (p = 0.004), higher aggregate behavioural
change processes (p = 0.01), higher reinforcement management (p = 0.02)
and higher helping relationships (p = 0.03). There were signicant inter-
actions for the aggregate behavioural processes and reinforcement man-
agement (p = 0.03 and 0.02 respectively). For those who had quit for less
than one month higher levels of behavioural process use and reinforcement
management predicted relapse, whereas for those who had quit for more
than one month the levels of these variables were similar for both relapsers
and quitters.
Relapse between time 3 and time 4 Thirty-ve per cent of the 204 par-
ticipants who were quit at time 3 relapsed by time 4. Relapse was predicted
by higher aggregate temptations (p = 0.005), higher positive/social temp-
tations (p = 0.001) and higher habit/addictive temptations (p = 0.007).
2.5.4 Discussion
The authors note a number of potential limitations of the study. First, there
may be predictors of relapse that are specic to very recent ex-smokers in
the acute withdrawal phase (i.e. quit for less than a week), but it was not
possible to examine this because of the relatively small sample size in this
group. Second, the sample consisted of smokers who had sought help by
phoning a quitline; it is possible that the predictors of relapse would be
different in smokers who try to quit without help. Third, the short-form
measures may not adequately assess the TTM constructs (see Section 2.4).
Use of behavioural processes predicted relapse between time 2 and time 3
(though not between time 3 and time 4). However, the ndings were
contrary to the TTM predictions, with more frequent use of behavioural
processes predicting relapse between time 2 and time 3. The authors suggest
that higher use of behavioural processes may indicate greater difculties
with staying quit and hence greater likelihood of relapse. The only TTM
variables that predicted relapse in both time periods were higher levels of
temptations to smoke, a nding that is consistent with Marlatt and Gor-
don’s (1985) relapse prevention model.
There was some evidence that predictors of relapse differed between
those who had quit for less than one month and those who had quit for
more than one month. Based on these ndings and other evidence, Segan et
al. (2004a) suggest that by dividing ex-smokers into those who have quit
for less than six months (actors) and those who have quit for more than six
months (maintainers), the TTM provides an overly simplistic account of the
post-cessation phase.
This research group has recently developed a new model that species
seven ‘perspectives’ (stages) in the process of smoking cessation, including
246 Stephen Sutton
four post-cessation stages (Borland 2000; Borland et al. 2004; Segan et al.
2004a). Based on a detailed and insightful analysis of the task of quitting
smoking, the model is a promising alternative to the TTM and is potentially
applicable to other health behaviours.
Figure 6.4 The PAPM compared with the TTM, from Weinstein and Sandman
(2002a). [Only the acting/action and maintenance stages can be regarded as
equivalent across the two models.]
The PAPM differs from the TTM in a number of ways (Figure 6.4). It has
more stages: seven instead of ve. Unlike the TTM, there is a stage (decided
not to act) that is a side-path from the main sequence (although a person
who reaches this stage may of course return to Stage 3 at some point and
Stage Theories of Health Behaviour 249
continue moving towards action). The decided to act stage is similar to the
preparation stage in the TTM (at least when preparation is dened purely in
terms of intentions or plans and not in terms of past behaviour). At rst
glance, deciding about acting appears to be similar to contemplation in the
TTM. However, being undecided about doing something may not be the
same as seriously thinking about doing something in the next six months.
Weinstein and Sandman (1992) suggest that the contemplation stage may
include both individuals who are undecided about action and those who
have already decided to act. Note that, unlike the TTM, none of the pre-
action stages in the PAPM refers to specic time periods, which means that
they are less arbitrary and perhaps more likely to represent genuine stages.
Finally, the PAPM in effect splits the TTM pre-contemplation stage into
three stages (1, 2 and 4), which seem to represent important distinctions; in
particular, it seems important to distinguish between (a) having never
thought about adopting a precaution and (b) having thought about it and
decided not to act.
1 2 3 4 5 6
1 P P
2 P P
3 P P P
4 P P
5 P P P
6 P
Table 6.6 shows a transition matrix for the six-stage version of the PAPM
(without the maintenance stage). Allowable transitions are indicated by
ticks. The diagonal consists entirely of ticks, meaning that a person can stay
in any of the stages indenitely. For example, one person may remain
blissfully unaware of the health threat while another person may be con-
stantly trying to decide what to do. Transitions above the diagonal repre-
sent forward movements. The ticks in this part of the matrix indicate the
transitions illustrated in Figure 6.4, for example moving from being una-
ware about the issue to being aware but unengaged. Transitions in the
upper diagonal that do not have a tick represent forward skips. Such skips
may sometimes occur. For example, a person may make a decision to do
something on the spur of the moment without having thought about it.
Thus, they may move directly from Stage 2 to Stage 5, skipping Stage 3. It is
possible to interpret this example in terms of the person moving rapidly
through the intervening stage rather than skipping it completely. Con-
ceptually, it is neater to proscribe skips and to assume that change follows
the sequence postulated in Figure 6.4. In practice, it is difcult or impossible
to distinguish between the two interpretations.
250 Stephen Sutton
Table 6.7 Issues likely to determine progress between stages, from Weinstein and
Sandman (2002b)
behaviours. Although the factors listed in Table 6.7 seem plausible, and there
is a lot of indirect supporting evidence, there is as yet little direct evidence
from the few studies of the PAPM that have been conducted to date.
Behaviour Authors
small. (Weinstein and Sandman note that the higher rate of testing by
undecided participants in Study II may be a consequence of the rather
aggressive intervention that occurred after their stage of testing had been
assessed.) Although not tested formally, this is evidence not simply for what
the Rhode Island group calls a stage effect (Prochaska et al. 2004) but for
the predicted discontinuity pattern. No studies of the PAPM have examined
other stage transitions over time or have investigated predictors of stage
transitions in longitudinal studies.
Table 6.9 Stages of testing adoption and subsequent test orders (per cent ordering
a test), from Weinstein and Sandman (1992)
Table 6.10 Percentage of participants who progressed one or more stages toward
testing, from Weinstein et al. (1998a)
Condition
Table 6.11 PAPM stage classication algorithm, from Weinstein and Sandman
(2002b)
Note: The words in curly brackets could be replaced with other precautions to develop a
staging algorithm for these precautions.
Behaviour Authors
(2001) term) that needs further specication before the theory can be fully
operationalized and tested. Empirical applications of the model have only
recently started to try to represent the volition phase by including measures
of planning and phase-specic self-efcacy as well as behaviour (e.g. Snie-
hotta et al. in press-b).