Binder 1
Binder 1
psychology I.
Sensation and perception
CogPsy modules
• I. Perception
• II. Memory and Attention
• III. Higher order cognitive processes: thinking & reasoning
Requirements
• Written exam will include:
• Literature
• E. B. Goldstein: Sensation and Perception (8th edition!) (bookfi.net)
• M. W. Eysenck & M. T. Keane: Cognitive Psychology (6th edition)
(http://gen.lib.rus.ec)
• Only chapter 1
• Powerpoint slides - will be uploaded
Cognition
= getting information out of the environment, to get
to know the world
What does it mean?
• ., •
• •
•
• • •
•
•
•• • •
••
• •
Information vs. humans
• Information in itself is totally meaningless
• Our mind has to react to the information, it creates the meaning
• Human perception is limited only on information that we can decode
~~I I I I I I I I I I I
I I I I I I I I I I
I
ree Nerve
ndlng
Rod Cone
•
Signal Receptor Signaling
Is it easy to decode the information?
Why do we need information?
To detect our environment effectively, so that we can survive in it.
→Recognize danger
→Get food
→Avoiding while obstacles travel from one place to another
→Find a warm place to sleep
→Communicate
→Recognize others
→Distinguish people
…
Information = survival
• Our greatest fears are in connection with lack of information,
insecurity, uncertanity, ambigousity
→ fear from dark
• We can’t predict what will happen
• Seeing bad things coming is better, than seeing nothing…
But:
• To access to information is not enough to adapt to our environment
• We also need a system to process information with → mind
40000
35000
30000
25000
20000
15000
10000
5000
0
0
20000
40000
60000
80000
100000
120000
140000
1944
PUBMED
1946
1948
1950
1952
1954
1956
1958
1960
1962
1964
1966
1968
1970
1972
1974
1976
1978
1980
1982
1984
1986
1988
1990
1992
"brain" OR "neuro" OR "neuroscience"
1994
1996
1998
2000
2002
2004
2006
2008
2010
2012
Number of scientific articles based on
2014
2016
2018
What is cognitive Ψ:
• „It is concerned with the internal processes involved in
making sense of the environment and deciding what action
might be appropriate (next slide)”
• These processes include: attention, perception, learning,
memory, language, problem solving, reasoning and thinking.
• Another definition: aiming to understand human cognition by
observing the behavior of people performing various cognitive
tasks.
The system of cognitive processes
Thinking
Imagination
Learning
Memory
Attention
Perception
Sensation
The system of cognitive processes
Thinking
Imagination
Learning
Memory
Attention Input of
information
Perception
Sensation
The system of cognitive processes
Thinking
Imagination Interpreting
Learning information,
giving meaning,
Memory
integrating
Attention features
Perception
Sensation
The system of cognitive processes
Thinking
Imagination Select needed
Learning information
Memory
Attention
Perception
Sensation
The system of cognitive processes
Thinking
Store and recall
Imagination information
Learning
Memory
Attention
Perception
Sensation
The system of cognitive processes
Thinking
ImaginationRecognize, classify
Learning
Memory
Attention
Perception
Sensation
The system of cognitive processes
Manipulate,
Thinking modify
information
Imagination
Learning
Memory
Attention
Perception
Sensation
The system of cognitive processes
Problem solving,
inferences
Thinking
Imagination
Learning
Memory
Attention
Perception
Sensation
Sen ation V: . Perception
---- -- ------ -- ---- -- ------- ------- - '@- ~ J-" -------- - - - - - - - - -
Pink
+
+ Smells
Rose
good
Hurts to
touch
+
Sensation Perception
• Organizes the sensory
Neural response to the stimuli from the environment to
information (features) to
register information
objects, meanings
Needs sensory systems (vision, hearing, olfaction, taste
• Consists of psychological
and touch) to detect physical and chemical stimuli
processes, that are responsible
Constant for recognize, organize and give
meaning to the information
• Turns sensory information
(color, taste, sound…) to
experience
• has way more components than
sensations (expectations…)
„Black patches…”
-
- ~-
„Dalmatian!”
-
- ~-
The need to find meaning
• Detection and recognition activates the reward circles in the brain
• We have been evolved to enjoy recognizing things
• We have been evolved to always search for the meaning
Our perception defines
our reality
• Different information = different
reality
• Different mind processes = different
reality
• Reality is NOT objective.
Sum up and take home
• https://youtu.be/S1jn86eUX0E
Questions, thoughts :)
About the field of cognitive psychology
Sensation
Signals
• Chemical signals:
smell, taste
• Physical tastes:
touch, see, hear
What type of information can we work with?
SMELL
ree Nerve
ndlng
Rod Cone
•
Signal Receptor Signaling
The perceptual process
• Receptor processes
• Sensory receptors are cells specialized to respond to environmental
energy, with each sensory system’s receptors specialized to respond
to a specific type of energy.
• Visual receptors respond to light
• Auditory receptors to pressure changes in the air
• Touch receptors to pressure transmitted through the skin
• Smell and taste receptors to chemicals entering the nose and mouth.
• The receptors transform environmental energy into electrical energy
RECEPTORS
Meissner corpuscles
(touch)
Merkel disks
(touch)
Pacinian corpuscles
f
Krause end bulbs
(touch)
(pressure)
Ruffini endings
Dermis (pressure)
e 0
Figure 1.5 Steps 5- 7: Behavioral responses are perception, recognWon, and action . ©Cengagele in 2014
Neural background of
perception
Bernadett Atndt, 2023.02.13.
Sensation
Signals
• Chemical signals:
smell, taste
• Physical tastes:
touch, see, hear
The perceptual process begins: RECEPTORS
• Sensory receptors are cells specialized to respond to environmental
energy, with each sensory system’s receptors specialized to respond
to a specific type of energy
• Visual receptors respond to light
• Auditory receptors to pressure changes in the air
• Touch receptors to pressure transmitted through the skin
• Smell and taste receptors to chemicals entering the nose and mouth.
• The receptors transform environmental energy into electrical energy
RECEPTORS
Meissner corpuscles
(touch)
Merkel disks
(touch)
Pacinian corpuscles
f
Krause end bulbs
(touch)
(pressure)
Ruffini endings
Dermis (pressure)
ree Nerve
ndlng
Rod Cone
•
Signal Receptor Signaling
Transduction
• Receptor processes
1_ ,
Receptor Light is reflected ◄ Environmental
• Example: visual process processes and transformed .....,._ stimulus
e 0
Figure 1.5 Steps 5- 7: Behavioral responses are perception, recognWon, and action . ©Cengagele in 2014
The perceptual process
• Neural processing:
• The tree is represented by electrical signals in thousands of visual
receptors, and these signals enter a interconnected network of
neurons, first in the retina, then out the back of the eye, and then in
the brain
• This complex network of neurons transmits signals from the
receptors, through the retina, to the brain, and then within the brain.
...
__,.,_.
so o en o
cort r/
~
Cortical organisation
Primary motor cortex Primary somatic
sensory cortex
Skeletal
Motor association area
muscle (premotor cortex)
movement Sensory information
from skin,
Sensory musculoskeletal
association system, viscera,
area and taste buds
- - Visual association
area
~ ,...._ Occipital lobe Vision
Prefrontal -r---:""""
association
area
Iris
Anterior chamber
(filled with
aqueous humor) Optic nerve and
retinal blood vessels
Suspensory ligaments
Ciliary body and muscle Medial rectus muscle
Back
of
eyeb II
The main function of the retina
• Reverse retina
• Photoreceptors: rods and cones – they got the names based on the
shape of their outer segments
• The outer segments of the photoreceptors contain visual pigments
that react to light and trigger elecrtical signals
• Fovea: place of sharp vision (when we look directly on an object, its
image falls on the fovea)
• Blind spot: the spot where the optic nerve leaves the eyeball (there
are no photoreceptors)
The blind spot
• The nerve fibers from the whole retina meet here to build up the
optic nerve
• It is hard to detect, because during the cortical processing, our brain
fills up the blind spot’s visual image (using the visual information
coming from the other eye and the surrounding regions)
Retinal layers Components
signals.
1 Pigmen epithelium
Cell layers of the retina: 9 Nerve fiber layer 10 Inner limiting membrane
Axons at urface of
retina passing via
8 Ganglion cell layer
• Horizontal cells – These cells optic nerve, chiasm
and tract to la eral
are connect to the geniculate ody
vision—the transformation of
light energy into electrical - - Nucleus
energy—occurs in the
receptors for vision: the rods - - - Axon - - - 1
Rod Cone
Rods Memk.raF1e sJ1e lves.
Ii fl e cl itl-i rh ~cl~ pl s.i
~ r c ~ I ~ r pl i; me Fl t -:.
• Color sensitives
exist as transmembrane . .- .. , -
proteins , /~ - - --- _ . Inner
. ,,,,' - . I segment
Current understanding is that cones can be divided into: Mitochondria ----------.. •
Cone cell
Bl nd spo Co es
( ods
80.000
60.000
0.000
20.000
00.000
80000
60.000
0000
20,000
e e
0
7D° 0 SD°
Angle (d gree
·-co
+"'
Q) 0.01
0:::
"0
0
.r:.
threshold and
sensitivity (a)
400 500 600
W velength (nm)
700
en axim m co e se s1 iv,
.s
Dar -adap ed
0 20
Time in dark (min)
Adapting to the dark
• Dark adaptation: the sensibility of the receptors changes with the
time spent in light/dark environment
• Light sensitivity increases in 2 steps: there is an increase at about 3-4
minutes after turning off the light, and 7-10 minutes
• Dark-adapted sensibility is about 100.000 times greater
• Keeping an eye in the dark triggers dark adaptation, which causes the
eye to increase its sensitivity in the dark (pirates’ eye patches)
Neural Convergence
• Convergence occurs when a number of Rods
lllll
rrr r r
(b)
Figure 2.32 How the wiring o the rods and cones determines
de ail vision. (al Rod neural circui s. On the left, stimulating two
neighboring rods causes he ganglion cell to fire. On the right.
stimulating two separa ed rods causes he same effec . {b) Cone
neural circui s. On the le . stimula ing wo neighboring cones
causes wo neighbo ing ganglio cells o ire . On the right.
stimulating two separated cones causes two separated ganglion
cells to fire. This firing o two neurons, with a space between them.
indica es that wo spo s of light have been presented to the cones .
Focus
• The lens can change its shape (and thus the intensity of bending the
light) to adjust the eye’s focus
• Accomodation: the ciliary muscles at the front of the eye tighten, the
curvature of the lense increases
• Either near or far objects can be in focus, but not both at the same
time
More about anatomy and deficits
• Myopia:
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=dWqrnsDtmpU
• Hyperopia:
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=a_cTP1eLJIc
• Laser eye surgery:
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=dKANhIU7Sxk
Anatomy, convergence, adaptation
Quick summary
• https://youtu.be/o0DYP-u1rNM
Lateral rectus muscle
Iris
Anterior chamber
(filled with
aqueous humor) Optic nerve and
retinal blood vessels
Suspensory ligaments
Ciliary body and muscle Medial rectus muscle
Fotoreceptors
→ cones in the
fovea
The visual field
Foveal vision
Parafoveal vision
Peripherial vision
LAT6MEZ6
The visual field
A receptive field
rround
Surround
cone
Bipol
C I
0 < nt r ----
OFF- urtound o optic n rv
n lio C I
201 1
Processing from retina to visual cortex
• Optic nerve is made up of many individual nerve fibers traveling
together. These fibers are the axons of the retinal ganglion cells.
• Each fiber responds only when a small area of the retina is illuminated.
The area that causes the neuron to fire is called the nerve fiber’s
receptive field.
• A fiber’s receptive field covers a much greater area than a single rod or
cone receptor.
...
The receptive fields (RF)
• RFs are arranged in a center-surround
organization, in which the area in the
“center” of the receptive field responds
differently to light than the area in the
“surround” of the RFs.
• Excitatory area – presenting a spot of
light increases firing Surround
{a) (b}
• Inhibitory area - presenting a spot of light
decreases firing
• (a) excitatory-center, inhibitory-surround
receptive field.
• (b) inhibitory-center, excitatory-surround
receptive field
Receptive fields
Receptive field ~ -:-::-~-.!..;;;
Light surround
Photoreceptor
cells (cones)
in retina Horizontal ____.-;;
cell
Bipolar -------
cell
(a) On-Center/Off-Surround
Copyriglrt Houglrton Mifflin Company. All riglrts reserved. Receptive Field of a Bipolar Cell
Receptive fields
• The center-surround receptive fields show that neural processing could result in neurons
that respond best to specific patterns of illumination.
• A small spot of light presented to the excitatory center of the RF causes a small increase
in the rate of nerve firing (a)
• Increasing the light’s size so that it covers the entire center of the RF increases the cell’s
response, as shown in (b).
• When the spot of light becomes large enough that it begins to cover the inhibitory area,
as in (c) and (d). ,., ,, ---
,, --
,., ,,,-
' ., ,, ..... .... -- -' -.._
0 \ ,' 0 \ ((Q)'-:
'
,
/ ;
'
• Thus, because of center-surround antagonism,
I - .- \
I
\
'
0 \
I \
II - 0 - I I 1
r
this neuron responds best to a spot of light that \ + + / I
\ . J \ I \
\. .... - - ,, / ' . ,, I /
--- .,.,,. ✓
'
' ........... _ ,,.,. ,,. /
I -...
........ .__ .,., . .,. ' '- ,.. ,,.
is the size of the excitatory center of the RF.
...
.....
r
I
11
I I
What happens after leaving the retina?
The optic nerve and optic tract
Visllal area
of the thalamus
Retina
Visual
cortex
Optic nerve pathway
..
---
~, • ---
Optic nerve damage: consequences
video: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=cG5ZuK0_qtc
Left E e RightE e Left E e Right E e
00
Optic ero : Ri ht n opsia (=blind)
Opti Chiasm
Optic Trncl
0
Optic chiasm: Bitempo.rnl. hemianot:>sia
Lateral
geniculate
nucleus
Visual cortex (LGN)
V2
Inferior temporal
cortex (ITC)
What type of stimulus is the visual cortex sensitive for?
I I I I
1- - 1+ + 1- - I
1- I 1- I
I - 1+ I - 1
I
1-
-
- ++ -
I
I
I
+ 1
-
I
I
1111111111 11 I
1- -
I -
I -
1- _
1+
I
I+
I
1- _ 1
+ 1 -
I
I
- 1
+ 1_ - I 0
l 0
f t
on
t
off
I I I - 1
Recep ive 1eld-
s1mple cortical cell
(a) (b) (c)
.
I
8.
I
:,
0 • https://youtu.be/IOHayh06LJ4
Q.
E
Vertical
Orientation (degree )
(d)
Figure 3.23 (a) The receptive field o a simple co ical cell. (b) This cell responds bes o a vertical bar o ligh
ha covers he e c1 a ory area o the recep ive field . (c} The response decreases as he ba is ii ed so that i
also covers he mhibi ory area . (d) Onenta io uning curve o a simple co ,cal cell for a neuron ha responds
best o a vertical bar (onen a ion = 0).
What does the brain do with visual
information?
• The main functions of visual perception are to…
• Detect - the brain recognizes the object
• Discriminate - diffenerciates its parts
• Localize – determines where it is
• Identify - determines what it is
…objects in our environment.
• Different brain regions are responsible for the different functions
What and where pathways
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=nOdXzVL5YKo
Dorsal proce ing: tream: .. where?"
Stimulus ocaliza · o
Lay rs VS
VI t d
V motor r • pon
We
Motion D pth
/Vb
Wa.
Shape/
Vi ual
, hap ,obj c re ogni ion
IL ll1
Color V ITC
Color
timuJus recognition
I Shape
Ventral proce ing am: "wha ?
VI
■
■
V2
Parvocellula:r
Retina LGN
cs, Orlcmmian Color
o~l.kmof
~ Bi 110t12il,u;-
l':J Dlaparl1y
C'cmc:r ' iii
Rwmund.'
0) Shll[le - I
iii
ill
.........•,' __ _
PluDaN ' !I
!ii
iii
I
., !ii
--------------------------~•
Dorsal pathway („Where?”) Ventral pathway („What?”)
„Between two balls of ice cream i’m not able to tell which is vanilla and
which is chocolate.”
Hit by a car, brain tumor, motorcycle accident, inherited
• Our sense of smell enables us to detect low concentrations of
some odorants.
• the detection threshold: the lowest concentration at which
an odorant can be detected.
1. Detecting
odors • rats : 8 to 50 times more sensitive to odors than humans
• dogs: from 300 to 10,000 times more sensitive - depending
on the odorant
• Why? humans have far fewer receptors than dogs
(10 million vs 1 billion)
2. Identifying odors
• When a person can just detect the presence of
an odor, the person not neccessarily can sense
the quality of the odor—whether it is “floral”
or “pepperminty”.
• The concentration at which quality can be
recognized is called the recognition threshold
• Humans can discriminate between 100,000
different odors
2. Identifying odors
• When we have trouble identifying odors, it sometimes results not
from a deficiency in our olfactory system, but from an inability to
retrieve the odor’s name from our memory….
The puzzles of odors
• How does the olfactory system
know what molecules are
entering the nose?
• The mucosa is located high in the
nasal cavity that contains the
receptors for olfaction
• Odorant molecules are carried
into the nose in an air stream,
which brings these molecules
into contact with the mucosa
Olfactory receptor neurons (ORN)
Olfactory
tract
_j
• Vision: visual pigments - sensitive to light
lfactory tract
~ ~- - - Cribrifonn plate
of ethmoid bone • Olfaction: olfactory receptors – sensitive
lfactory bulb
Auditory associati
area
Taste !Gustatory
l ortex Auditory cortex
Temporal lobe
Smell JO Ifactory
~ ortex
Higher-order olfactory processing
• Three main principles:
• (1) many molecules are creating a single
perception like “coffee” or “bacon,” (> 100 Fu I I111111---
different molecules)
• (2) the ability to separate odors from one
amz n n r ......
I I
another in the environment (coffee and
bacon in the same time)
• (3) the effect of past experience and
learning on odor perception (positive
PL■■-1--
experiences – positive rating of the odor)
• These indicate that odor perception must
involve more than just a hardwired
“readout” of the pattern of ORN firing!
• More research is needed…
Henning’s smell prism
Flowery Fruity
Putrid
Spicy Resinous
Burnt
The gustatory system
• Function: choose which foods to eat and
which to avoid
→ Get nutrition rich food, avoid harmful, non-
edible things, detect immature fruit…
→ Evolution: preference for useful things
The taste system
• Function: choose which foods to eat and
which to avoid
• sweetness is often associated with
compounds that have nutritive or caloric
value (important for life). It also trigger
anticipatory metabolic responses that
prepares the gastrointestinal system for
processing these substances
• Bitter compounds have the opposite
effect—they trigger automatic rejection
responses to help the organism avoid
harmful substances
The taste system
• Function: choose which foods to eat and
which to avoid
• sweetness is often associated with
compounds that have nutritive or caloric
value (important for life). It also trigger
anticipatory metabolic responses that
prepares the gastrointestinal system for
processing these substances
• Bitter compounds have the opposite
effect—they trigger automatic rejection
responses to help the organism avoid
harmful substances
• When people are deprived of sodium (salt)
through sweating, they will often seek out
foods that taste salty in order to refuel the
salt their body needs
Basic taste qualities and preferences
• Basic taste sensations:
salty, sour, sweet, bitter and
umami (meaty, savory,
associated with the flavor-
enhancing properties of MSG,
monosodium glutamate)
• Universal – in every culture
• People can describe most of
their taste experiences on the
basis of these sensations
Sour
Sweet
Bitter
Structure of the taste system
The process of tasting begins with the tongue
Papillae of Tongue (receptors are stimulated by taste molecules)
The surface of the tongue contains ridges and
valleys caused by papillae (4 types):
• (1) filiform papillae (found over the entire surface
of the tongue)
Palatine tonsil
• (2) fungiform papillae - shaped like mushrooms
(found at the tip and sides of the tongue)
Lingual tonsil
Circumvallat
• (3) foliate papillae (found along the back of the
papillae tongue on the sides)
• (4) circumvilliate papillae - shaped like flat
mounds (found at the back of the tongue)
Fungiform Filiform papillae
papillae ~ ...,_..;..
Surface of the tounge
Structure of the
taste system Taste buds fl
Circumvallate papilla
Transitional cell
Structure of the taste system
• Each taste bud contains 50–100 taste cells, which have tips
(chemicals contact receptor sites located on the tips)
Epllhellui at tongue
Epiglottis
Circumvallate papilla
Basal
cell
ustatory hairs _/
(mlcrovllll) emerging
from taste pore
Nerves from tongue
• Muscles:
Vagus nerve [X]
→ muscle movements in the
mouth and throat,
including speech
Hypoglossal nerve [XII]
• Taste:
Superficial petronasal neve [IX]
Trigeminus nerve [V]
Facial nerve [VII]
Cortical processing
• The fibers from the tongue, mouth,
and throat make connections in the
brain stem (nucleus of the solitary
tract)
• From there signals travel to the
thalamus
• Then to two areas in the frontal lobe—
rw VII
the insula and frontal operculum
IN ,v
• https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=w
GXoYippog8
The perception of flavor
• Most people look forward to
eating not because it is necessary
for survival but because of the
pleasure it brings
• What these people refer to as
“taste” when describing their
experience of food is usually a
combination of
• taste and smell (+vision?)
• This combination is called
flavor
• We are usually dealing with
not just one or two different
flavors but many
The perception of flavor
• Most of the basic research on flavor
has focused on showing how taste
and smell interact and on factors
that influence our perception of
flavor
• Your experiences?
Ageusea
• Ageusia is the loss of taste functions
Flavour
Try it out!
• “Tasting” With and Without the Nose
• Notice both the quality and the intensity of the taste as you are
drinking coffee, orange juice and wine with and without opened
nostril.
• First, close your nostrils, and notice whether you perceive a flavor.
Then drink the beverage normally with nostrils open, and notice the
flavor.
• Do you feel the difference?
The physiology of flavor perception
• The mouth and the Orthonasal Retro nasal
nose are connected
olfactory olfactory
and both are bulb
bulb
important to sense odor odor
olfactory perception
falvour olfactory perception
receptors ~ receptors
• A number of cortical
areas that serve
both taste and
olfaction are • ••••••••
•
probably involved in
the perception of the
flavor of food 'out-there' 'in-the-mouth'
The physiology of flavor perception
• The OFC receives inputs from the primary
cortical areas for taste and olfaction, as
well as from the primary somatosensory
cortex and from the inferotemporal cortex
in the visual „what” pathway
• The OFC contains many bimodal neurons,
those that respond to more than one
sense
• For example, some bimodal neurons
respond to both taste and smell, and
other neurons respond to taste and vision
→ Multisensory integration
The physiology of flavor perception
12
l-2
.....
Thanks for your attention!
• Sum-up:
• https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=mFm3yA1nslE
• Super tasters:
• https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=W7Pzhvypg9A
Thanks for your attention!
• Sum-up:
• https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=mFm3yA1nslE
• Super tasters:
• https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=W7Pzhvypg9A
The cutaneous senses,
proprioception, kinesthesis
debate
• Cutaneous senses: touch and pain perceptions, usually caused by the
stimulation of the skin
• Proprioception: the ability to sense the position of the body and limbs
• Kinesthesis: the ability to sense the movement of the body and limbs
The importance of touch
Losing of touch?
• When asked which sense they would choose to lose, if they had to
lose either vision, hearing, or touch, some people pick touch
• This is understandable given the high value we place on seeing and
hearing, but making a decision to lose the sense of touch would be a
serious mistake
Losing of touch?
• Because although people who are blind or deaf
can get along quite well, people with a rare
condition that results in losing the ability to feel
sensations though the skin often suffer constant
bruises, burns, and broken bones in the absence
of the warnings provided by touch and pain
\''
....
• Besides, many everyday actions would become extremely hard (eg.
Typing, eating, dressing up, sexual activity)
• Experiments with temporarily anesthetized hands —> People tend to
apply much more force than necessary
Video
• Congenital insensitivity to pain (CIP)
• https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=1vLsZ_dXFAg
The skin
• The heaviest organ in human body
• Warning function (against burning, aching,
dangerous stimuli)
• Prevents body fluids from escaping
• Protects us by keeping bacteria, chemical
agents, and dirt from penetrating our bodies
• Provides us with information about the
various stimuli that contact it (sun’s rays heat
our skin, pinprick is painful, touch of our
beloved)
The skin Epidermis
epidermis
free
nelVe endi gs
(pa·n hea Meissner
cold) corpuscles
(touch)
Merkel
disks Pac nia
(touch) corpuscles
(pressure)
Krause
end bulbs
(touch) Ruffini
endings
(pressure)
---=-a...___. root hair plexus (touch)
Mechanoreceptors close to the surface
1. Merkel receptor
• close to the surface of the
skin, near the epidermis.
• the Merkel receptor fires
continuously, as long as the
stimulus is on
nnnnnrnun
2. Meissner corpuscle
____r-i_
• close to the surface of the
skin, near the epidermis.
• the Meissner corpuscle
fires only when the
stimulus is first applied and •
when it is removed.
Mechanoreceptors deeper in the skin
3. Ruffini cylinder
• located deeper in the skin
• the Ruffini cylinder responds
continuously
• it is associated with
perceiving stretching of the
skin
4. Pacinian corpuscle
. ,, II 11 ..
_r-----i_
• the Pacinian corpuscle FIRIIIID
Willlwww v--111,.,,
responds when the stimulus
is applied and removed.
• it is associated with sensing
rapid vibrations and fine
texture
Coutaneous receptive field
• The area of skin which, when stimulated, influences the firing of the
neuron
Pathways from skin to cortex
• Nerve fibers from receptors in the
peripheral nerves that enter the
spinal cord through the dorsal root
• The nerve fibers then go up along
the medial lemniscal and the
spinothalamic pathway
• Lemniscal pathway: carries signals
related to sensing the positions of
the limbs (proprioception) and
perceiving touch
• Spinothalamic pathway transmits 11,1, ......____
signals related to temperature and
pain
Pathways from skin to cortex
• Fibers from both pathways cross
over to the other side of the brain
(signals from the left side of the
body reach the thalamus in the
right hemisphere)
• Most of these fibers synapse in the
ventrolateral nucleus in the
thalamus (thalamus is involved in
vision and hearing as well)
• From the thalamus, signals travel to Mr11-.,■ 1ilclla--
the somatosensory receiving area
(S1) in the parietal lobe of the cortex
and possibly to the secondary
somatosensory cortex (S2)
Somatosensory cortex
• The somatosensory cortex is
organized into maps that
correspond to locations on
the body
• This body map is called the
homunculus (“little man”).
• The homunculus shows that
some areas on the skin are
represented by large area of
the brain (thumb, lips,
tongue) others by smaler
(back, forearm)
• There are a number of
homonculi mapped in S1 and
(b)
S2
Homunculus
• https://www.youtube. a,
0':/ /4e.., G
«('
s 'P
s,and j w
Lateral Medial
I m n 0 p q r s t u V
•• •• ••• •·• •• •• •· ·• •• •• ••
•• • •• •· •• ••••
•
• •
• • •
•
•· ••
• •·
• • • •· •· •• ••
W X y z
•••• •• ••• ••
•
• •• ••
• ·•
••
Measuring tactile acuity
• The classic method is the two-point threshold
• the minimum separation between two points on the skin that
when stimulated is perceived as two points
• The two-point threshold is measured by gently touching the
skin with two points, and having the person indicate whether
he or she feels one point or two
Cortical mechanisms for tactile acuity
There is a strong correlation between
• A) the density of receptors in the skin
• B) the representation of the body in
the brain and the
• C) acuity at different locations on the
body
Two-point thresholds for humans:
Two-point threshold on the hand
• There is a high density of Merkel receptors
in the fingertips, because the fingertips are 2n1m
the parts of the body that are most
sensitive to details
36 to 75 mm
1.1 mm 3 to 8 mm
2. Perceiving vibration
• When your phone starts ringing in your
hands you can sense these vibrations
with your fingers and hands
• The mechanoreceptor that is responsible
for this is the Pacinian corpuscle (PC)
• The PC consists of layers (like an onion),
with fluid between each layer.
• It transmits only rapidly applied pressure
(like vibration), but does not transmit
continuous pressure Ill Ill
3. Perceiving texture
• We can sense textures ranging from coarse (teeth of a
comb) to fine (the surface of a paper sheet)
• Two important cues:
• Spatial cues - result in feeling different shapes,
sizes, and distributions of these surface elements
Works also without moving
• Temporal cues - provide information in the form of
vibrations that occur as a result of the movement over
the surface —> fine details (try it out)
➢When it is allowed to move the fingers across the
surface, we are able to detect the difference between
the fine textures, but if it is not allowed, it is impossible
4. Perceiving objects
• Identifying objects by intent (active touch) by haptic exploration
• Haptic perception is an extremely complex process because as we will see, the
sensory, motor, and cognitive systems must all work together.
• Researches have shown that people can accurately identify most common objects
within 1 or 2 seconds
Lateral Motion Unsupported Holding
(Texture) (Weight)
,..,._.,,~
. 1- ---
-
...
---:r-- _,
t
'~ /- , /
strategies / \\(
--~
\ -\ '---,·;•'
-;~:
Enclosure
Pressure (Global Shape)
(Hardness) (Volume)
iC~~ -
Static Contad
(Temperature)
To identify objects three distinct systems are used:
Detecting cutaneous sensations
such as temperature, texture,
softness, vibration and the
Sensory movements and positions of the
system fingers and hands
Thinking about the information
provided by the sensory and
motor systems
Compare to previous
experiences, if there is
a match Moving the fingers
→ recognized object Motory and hands
Using different haptic
system strategies to get more
information about the
shape and the texture
Physiology of object perception
• As we move from mechanoreceptors toward the brain,
we see that neurons become more specialized
• This is similar to what occurs in the visual system
• Neurons in the ventral posterior nucleus (in the
thalamus) have center-surround receptive fields that
.....,
·IIURllllld
are similar to the center-surround receptive fields in
the lateral geniculate nucleus (the visual area of the
thalamus)
Monkey arm:
Physiology of object perception
• In the cortex there are neurons that respond to specific orientations and
neurons that respond to movement across the skin in a specified direction
• There are also neurons in the monkey’s somatosensory cortex that respond
when the monkey grasps a specific object
I I
· Ill JIIII
. . .
· ----- f illllllll 11 H
11 111
C•J
r (bJ
Pain
Pain
• Pain functions to warn us of potentially damaging situations
A and helps us avoid or deal with cuts, burns, and broken bones.
• People born without the ability to feel pain might be unaware
A of broken bones, infections, or internal injuries—situations
that could easily be life-threatening
• Definition: “Pain is an unpleasant sensory and emotional
A experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage”
Pain
• Three types:
• Nociceptive pain - is caused by
activation of receptors called nociceptors
• There are a number of different kinds of nociceptors,
which respond to different stimuli—heat, chemical,
severe pressure, cold
• Inflammatory pain - is caused by damage to tissues and
inflammations to joints or by tumor cells
• Neuropathic pain - is caused by lesions or other damage
to the nervous system
Models of pain
• According to the „direct
pathway model”, pain
occurs when nociceptor
receptors in the skin are
stimulated
Node ptors
Skin
• But we already know that pain can be
affected by other factors as well
(e.g. by a person’s mental state
– mood, anxiety)
• Pain can occur when there is no
stimulation of the skin
• Phantom limb - people who have had a
limb amputated continue to experience
the limb after the surgery (somethimes
they feel pain)
• Pain can be affected by a person’s
attention
• The perception of pain can increase if
perception is focused on it, or
decreased if it is ignored
Video
• Mirror therapy – phantom limb
• https://www.youtube.com/watch
?v=hrqi1B9Xbt0
Proprioception
• The ability to sense stimuli arising within the body
regarding
• position,
• motion, and
• equilibrium
•
Muscle splnclea trigger
I raflat to maintain
" ·, "" w "
•. . .. *. . ... •. .... •. .
'
.....
_ .4 ...... ..
-•♦ . ,• • _ ♦ ♦- . . . . . . . . . . . .
•• •· . ••
...... * ♦ *•"' · · · • · - ,- ... ,-
.......... -
.
........ _ .. , ........ - ............. ... *
. +.
-··
·•.-·•·
....
··~·-•····- ·· ....
...
.•
•
-•• ......
♦ +
.
......... .
■
•
-♦
• ...
■
..........
• ....
• • • ... ..
.............
• ••
- • -*- • ..._.... -··
•-·
*
... ••
•
••
■
....
• ■ •
-•
•
·*•'
i
1. ,
. . .. .... . . . ... .
11 • .& • A• A
"'•• ... -,
. . .+ . . • •
-·A ·•-•··· ■•
I: A
... ....
-,· . ,... •
■_ .. I •
*•
• ,
••
•
•
+
,•••••
-
••
* •
♦
I
•
•
-•·■ -
.... •
1#-
-
.. ...
• • • I•
•
* •
• ■
..... ··-·
I • •
••
♦
...
A
•• I
e
■
A.
Functions of Color
• Important signaling function
– Identify and classify things
– Helps facilitate perceptual organization
• Tell one object from another
• Pick out objects within scenes
• Tanaka & Presnell (1999)
• We interact with color all the time
– We pick favourites (blue is the most preferred)
– Associate it with emotions
• Red = embarrassment
• Green = envy
• Purple = rage
Mr I.
• Became color-blind at 65
• Car accindent, concussion
• He became more depressed vs. Partially color-blined are not
disturbed by their decreased color perception
What is color?
• Light = electromagnetic radiation
• It has a quantity — luminance
• And a quality — color
Newton:
• Separated white light with a prism
TR ~TI ,E
01' TH -
LI G
Visible light
White
light
Prism
Visible light
400 500 600 700
Ultra- Broadcast AC
Gamma violet Infrared
X-rays Radar bands circuits
rays rays rays
1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17
10 -5
10 -3
10 -1
10 10 10 10 10 10 10 10 10
Wavelength in nanometers (billionths of a meter)
• Pure/uniqe colors: blue, green, yellow, red
Refraction
• Achromatic colors occur
when light is reflected
equally across the
spectrum
• Transparent things ->
selective transmission:
only some wavelength
pass through them
• Selective reflection:
Chromatic colors occur when
some wavelenghts are
reflected more than others
)
90
.....
I 40
I
Wllvelengll1 (llnl)
• Mixing paints vs. Mixing lights
– Mixing lights: additive color mixture
– Mixing paints: substractive color mixture
S M L S M L S M L
Medium +
long wave lengths
~
Q,
1200
cb - - - -
!E 1000
a),
Mesopic
~
0 800
- - -
c::
'E 600
::)
400
200
0 L..........,~-
Cone photoreceptor
spectral sensitivities:
1
(I)
/
rn- 00100
I
I
(1) I
0 I
-
:c
,m
0.
I,,.., g I
,0
(I.I
.c
,m I
'
I ~
\
(1) I
-c::
\
.2= I
,m I ' 12
(1) I \~
.; \ ,( P
\
\
0
''
4 00 500 600 700
Wavelength (nm)
ml• '
Mlutation or los,s of photopigment proteins, 1
Protanomaly Protanopii. a
•L shiftedl towardls M
Triichromat Protanope
S M IL SM
j]
S M IL SM
5f>
• Monochromatism
– Rare form of color-blindness (10 out of a million)
– Hereditary
– No functioning cones
– See everything in shades of lightness
– Sensitive to bright lights -> often wear dark glasses as protection
• Dichromatism
– Experience some colors
– 3 major form: protanopia, deuteranopia, tritanopia
– protanopia & deuteranopia are inherited through a gene located on
the X chromosome (sex-linked)
Trichromat IDichromat
(protanope: no L-cone)
• Protanopia
– Affects 1% males, 0,02% females
– Perceives short-wavelength light (blue); as the wavelength
is increased, the blue becomes less saturated, and finally
grey (=neutral point)
• Deuteranopia
– Affects 1% males, 0,01% females
– Perceives blue at short wavelength, sees yellow at long
wavelength
• Tritanopia
– Rare, affects 0,002% males, 0,001% females
– Perceives blue at short wavelength, red at long wavelength
(a) (b) (c ) (d )
Protanope
400 t 700
492
(a)
Deutera11110,pe
700
498
(b)
Trltanope
400 t· 700
570
(c)
Trikt,omat Dikromat l\lonokromat
111 1 1
Sensitivity
• Human: about 400-700 nm
• Birds, fish, insects: UVA
• Phintella vittata (spider): UVB too!
(a) (b)
The Relation Between Color and Form
• The visual system determines an object’s form, and then color
fills in the form
• ↕
• Close connection between processing of form and color
• Color may play a role in determining form
Contract-effects
Color constancy
• We perceive the colors of objects as being relatively constant
even under different illumination
– Chromatic adaptation: prolonged exposure to chrimatic colors
– Surroundings
– Memory color: the effect on perception of prior knowledge of the
typical colors of objects
Lightness constancy
• The amount of light reaching the eye from an
object depends on:
– Illumination – the total amount of light that is
striking the object’s surface
– Object’s reflectance – the proportion of this light
that the object reflects into our eyes
• Lightness constancy: the perception of
lightness is determined by the object’s
reflectance (not the intensity of illumination!)
– Remains the same no matter what the illumination
F nw:::irn 1-1 ArtP1<:nn
Look at blue Look at yellow
squares squares
• Content © 2008 R.Beau Lotto
• http://www.lottolab.org/articles/illusionsoflight.asp
• Content © 2008 R.Beau Lotto
• http://www.lottolab.org/articles/illusionsoflight.asp
• Content © 2008 R.Beau Lotto
• http://www.lottolab.org/articles/illusionsoflight.asp
• Content © 2008 R.Beau Lotto
• http://www.lottolab.org/articles/illusionsoflight.asp
• https://www.illusionsindex.org/i/grey-strawberries
„The dress”
Depth perception
Three sources of space information
1. Body
• Body surface localization
– Identifying the own body as spatial stimulus
• Proprioception
– Position of body parts compared to each other
• Kinesthesis
– Percieving the body in movement
2. EGOCENTRIC space
Percieving the space around us – the referential center is the own body
3. ALLOCENTRIC space
Percieving the space around us – in an independent spatial system
g
Goodale & Milner (1992, TINS)
Movement, localization
egocentric
Ventral:
Object-recognition
- ~
Pnmary .
allocentric visual
Ii..
Cortex 1
lnfeJtote ·- poral
Cortex
„The inner GPS"
If the rat was in a particular part of the labirynth, the same neuron group of it’s
hyppocampus has been active
Being in another part of the labirynth activated other type of neurons – „space cells”
for mapping our environment
The different type of visual stimuli used for
depth perception
= spatial cues
Oculomotor cues
Relying on muscles responsible for eye-movements
Oculomotor Oculomotor nerve ( II}
1, Convergence
IJT----.!L Oc lomotor
2, Accommodation ( II) nerve
division
Clliaty
gangron
POSl'ERl'0R
Accomodation
• Increasing and decreasing
the lens’ refractive power
• Ciliary muscles
• Try it with your finger! Clo e "foe
·······• i••·
------
.. ■
.. ■ ■ ■ I ■ I ■
ACCOMMODATION
• Definition: Accommodation is the mechanism by which the
Unaccommodated I Accommodated
lens ____,.___ lens
RELAXED ; ACCOMMODATED 4
Convergence
Convergence
(a) (b)
1""11..._.,,....._ _ Panu:,~n!s
Convergence F ll:$ trJfr At·~a
'
\
\ J
I
\
\
\ J
I '
\
\
\ ear ting'e
I '
\
\
\
'
,ear finge
.Dafrd
.Tulp
~
Horopter
• Imaginary line, where there is
no disparity
Monocular static cues
Occlusion
Occlusion
• The object that at least paritally
covers the other is closer than the
one that is covered (a} Eye and scene
h'I
' ~
I
\?II ·ii
•I '
Perspective
convergence
Texture
gradient
Texture
gradient
• Perceived density
Atmospheric
perspective
Atmospheric
perspective
• Sharpness of the objects
• Light blue tint
Shadows
Shadows
false shadows…
Monocular
non-static cues
Motion parallax
Motion-produced
cues
• Motion parallax
• Furhter objects seem to
„move” slower
• Deletion and accretion
• Occlusion due to
movement
►
Move
•
Fixation point
Your movement
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=QWMFpxkGO_s
Adelbert Ames, Jr. (August 19,
1880-1955) was an American
scientist who made contributions
to physics, physiology,
ophthalmology, psychology, and
philosophy. He pioneered the
study of physiological optics at
Dartmouth College, serving as a
research professor, then as
director of research in the
Dartmouth Eye Institute.
He conducted important research into aspects of binocular
vision, including cyclophoria and aniseikonia. Ames is
perhaps best known for constructing some illusions of visual
perception: the Ames room, the Ames window, and the
Ames chair.
Kinetic depth – Ames window
Ware, C., Chapter 8 of Information Visualization: Perception for Design. 2000, San Fancisco: Morgan Kaufmann.
The Ames-room
• https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=hCV2Ba5wrcs
• Az Ames szoba valós és
észlelt alaprajza
I
'' ''
I
\ ' l
\
'' , of pe rso n B
'' '' \
' \
' \ l
'' ' l
I
I
\ I
\ \
\ \
I
''I ,
\
'
Ap pa ren t
sha pe of' roo m
Impossible objects
Aniseikonia is an ocular condition where there is a
significant difference in the perceived size of images. It can
occur as an overall difference between the two eyes.
Gr. "an" = "not" , + "is(o)" = "equal," + "eikōn" = "image"
Strabismus (axis of the 2 eyes not paralel) too much difference
between the retinal images
Diplopia – the 2 retinal images are not merged
Taking action
Cognitive Psychology I.
Ecological validity
Gradient of flow
Self-produced information
DEMONSTRATION
Stand up. Raise one foot from the ground and stay balanced on the other. Then close your
eyes and notice what happens.
Keeping balance
Vestibular canals – inner ears + receptors in the joints and muscles
+ VISION provides a frame of reference that helps the muscles making adjustments
Conflicting informations?
Lee & Aronson (1974)
13- to 16-month-old toddlers
„swinging room”
26% swayed
23% staggered
33% fell down
(a) Roofl'l s Floor re ns s·a iooary
Adults?
The vortex tunnel
(Museum of Illusions,
Budapest)
Topographical agnosia (inability to recognize landmarks) – loss of brain tissue in the parahippocampal gyrus
Individual differences in wayfinding
(a)
Cognitive map
Edward Tolman
Experiments with rats
Latent learning
The rat did not learn a sequence of moves, but created a cognitive map
of the spatial layout of the maze
It was able to use this map to locate the food
C C C
,----------
A A
( ) plor maz (c) Turi" I for food
Cognitive map
John O’Keefe
Recorded the activity of individual neurons in the The Nobel Prize in
hippocampus Physiology or Medicine
+ noted the rat’s location 2014
Individual neurons fired when the rat was in a specific
place within the box, different cells preferred different
locations
PLACE CELLS = only fire when an animal is in a certain
place in the environment
May-Britt and Edvard Moser Photo: David Bis op, Photo: Wikimedia Photo: Wikimedia
Code distance and direction information Prize share: 1/ 2 Prize share: 1/4 Prize share: 1/4
s
l!
a,
C
"C
u:
(a) (b) {c)
Mirror neurons
0.7
,- Control film: Context Control film : Action Intention film
06 -
,....
__... Intention: Drinking
0.5
._, -
GI
en 0 -
i
s:.(,) ,...
'i! 0.3
c; -
SIi -
Cleaning up
(/)
,_
02
0
- I
....
0
Ac on Or nklng Cl nlng Cleaning up
Perceiving motion
Functions of motion perception
Camuflage
Color, shape, patterning are
similar to those found in the
sorrounding
Movement organises all the
elements into a single figure
that is separated from the
background
Motion attracts attention
Attention is automatically drawn
to salient object
Motion is salient
Understanding events
https://www.youtube.com/watc
h?v=VTNmLt7QX8E
Heider & Simmel (1944)
2 and 1/2 –minute
„house” + three „characters”
Subjects created stories to explain the
objects actions
Life without motion perception
Case study
the execution of daily activities is imapired
E.g. pouring a cup of tea
Following dialogues
Crossing the street
Photo receptors
Akinetopsl
Edf\h ♦
n, Ell Facts.oro
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=QV59aAtznSg
Studying motion perception
•••
• •
Real motion = actual motion of an object
Illusory motion = perception of motion without Phi Phenomenon
Larsen et al (2006)
Control condition (simultaneous)
Real motion display (back and forth movement)
Apparent motion display (flashed one after another)
(a) Contro
The movement area of the brain
Human subjects
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)
Temporarily distrupts the normal functioning of
the neurons
If a particular behavior is distrupted, the distrupted
area of the brain is involved in that behavior
Applying TMS to the MT cortex
Difficulty determining the direction in which the
dots was moving
Temporary akinetopsia
Microstimulation
Percep Ion
.•-
(a) ( (
Motion of point-Light Walkers
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=
rEVB6kW9p6k
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=
KT89CQ2nRpo
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=
1F5ICP9SYLU
-- . ,- -
.
,
~
~ . ~,~,,' -~ <
· -1 •
•• •
-
- - - ;J
-.:- -~ •
•
-. -
~
~ ,•,,
.. .,,,,. --
:lit~
-. ,-.
,
.... ' ~
.
.... -.
,,
,
_,,....
-~
~
• - _, ~
.
.
--
_,,....
-~
~
• -
..
_, ~
.
Structuralism
Wilhelm Wundt
Sensation vs perception
Sensations
Response to stimulation
Analogues to the atoms of chemistry Structuralists:
Perceptions Number of sensations add up to create
More complex conscious experiences such as our our perception of the face
awerness of objects
Combine sensations to create complex
perceptions
Structuralism
Wilhelm Wundt
Sensation vs perception •: I
Sensations
Response to stimulation Structuralists:
Number of sensations add up to create
Analogues to the atoms of chemistry
our perception of the face
Perceptions
More complex conscious experiences such as our
awerness of objects
Combine sensations to create complex
perceptions
Gestalt psychologists
Gestalt
Wertheimer (a) One light flashes
Toy stroboscope
Creating an illusion of movement by rapidly alternating
two slightly different pictures
This cannot be explained by the structuralist point of (b) Darkness
view
Apparent movement
Illusory movement
Can’t be explained by sensations (c) The second light
Perceptual system creates the perception of flashes
movement where there actually is none
„The whole is different than the sum of its parts”
Illusory contour
No physical edges are present (d) Flash-dark-flash
It cannot be explained by sensation since there aren’t
any sensations along the contours
Georges Seurat
Pointillism
Pointillism is a technique of painting in which small,
distinct dots of color are applied in patterns to form an
image.
Principles of perceptual organization
Good continuation How continuation affects us?
•••••
• • • ••
• • • ••
• • • ••
• • • ••
• • • ••
How do you perceive this?
Principles of perceptual organization
Similarity
• • • ••
Similar things appear to be grouped
together • • • ••
Grouping can also occur because of
similarity of shape, size, or orientation • • • ••
• • • ••
• • • ••
• • • ••
How do you perceive this?
-■ ♦ ■ -
-■ --♦
Principles of perceptual organization
Proximity (nearness)
•••••
Things that are near each other
appear to be grouped together • • • ••
• • • ••
• • • ••
• • • ••
• • • ••
How do you perceive this?
Principles of perceptual organization
Proximity (nearness)
•• • ••
Things that are near each other
appear to be grouped together •• • ••
•• • ••
•• • ••
•• • ••
•• • ••
How do you perceive this?
Principles of perceptual organization
Proximity (nearness)
• •• ••
Things that are near each other
appear to be grouped together • •• ••
• •• ••
• •• ••
- -- - - • •• ••
- -- -- -- - ♦
-
• •• ••
- -- -- How do you perceive this?
Principles of perceptual organization
Common fate
Things that are moving in the same direction appear
to be grouped together
E.g., flock of hundred of birds (1 group/unit) -> some
of them start flying in another direction (2
groups/unit)
Works with dissimilar objects too
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=LZMaTtPHBMk
\
~~,
'\~,~
\' '~
),.;!~~
,~
~
...,
••••••••••
• • • •• • • • ••
• • • •• • • • ••
• • • •• • • • ••
• • • •• • • • ••
• • • •• • • • ••
•••••••••• • • • •• • • • ••
• • • •• • • • ••
• • • •• • • • ••
• • • •• • • • ••
• • • •• • • • ••
Perceptual segregation
Figure-ground segregation
Edgar Rubin, 1915
Figures are
More thing like and more memorable
Seen as being in front of the ground
If the vase is seen as a figure it appears to be in
front the white background
Border separating the figure from the ground
appears to belong to the figure
Image based factors that determine which
area is the figure
Areas lower in the fiels of view are more The typical scene that we percieve regularly
likely to be perceived as figure (Ehrenstein, 1930;
Koffka, 1935)
Vecera et al., 2002
Upper-lower preference but no left-right
preference
Figures are more likely to be
perceived on the convex side of
borders (bulge outward) (Kanizsa &
Gerbino, 1976)
Palmer, 1975 B
blob
(a) (b)
(c) (d)
Neural background
FFA – fusiform face area
PPA – parahippocampal place area
EBA – extrastriate body area
Binocular rivalry
Tong et al., 1998
Colored glasses (house or face)
Binocular rivalry occured
Images remained the same on the retina
The perception of the images alternated back and forth every few seconds
fMRI – brain activity changed depending on what the person was experiencing
PPA – Parahippocampal place area
Epstein and Kanwisher (1998)
„place area”
It responded to pictures of buildings, furnished and empty rooms
Parahippocampal cortex (PHC)
Identifies the location without any statements regarding its function
Spatial layout hypothesis
Responds to the surface geometry/geometric layout of a scene
Scenes ccause larger responses than buildings
But buildings are partial scenes
PHC responds to to qualities that are relevant to navigation through a scene or locating
a place (Epstein, 2008)
Mullay & Maguire, 2011 – (sense of three dimensional space)
Space defining (large oak bed)
Space ambigous objects (small white fan heater)
Visual agnosia
Impairment of visual object recognition with otherwise intact vision
Two types of visual agnosia
Apperceptive agnosia
Individuals’ knowledge of the objects is intact but they are unable to organize/integrate
the different pieces of information into a coherent whole/object.
Associative agnosia
Characterised by fairly normal perceptual processing but an impaired ability to derive
the meaning of objects
Prospagnosia
Mental Imagery
The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience
▪ When we think of
▪ ….the best way to load a luggage in the trunk of a car
▪ …the fastest route to go from point A to point B
▪ …easiest way to assemble a bookshelf
we visualize actions/use mental images instead of performing them
▪ Mental images are representations that are similar to perceptions but they
do not require an external stimulation to be created
▪ Representations preserve the perceptible properties of the stimuli they
represent (Kosslyn et al., 2006)
▪ The stimulus itself is not present physically
▪ Relies on top-down processing
Early studies on mental imagery
"
(1978)
13
Zenon Pylyshyn
▪ Analog Representation
How to resolve the imagery debate?
▪ Neuroimaging data
▪ If visual imagery relies on representations and cognitive processes
similar to those involved in during visual perception, then visual mental
imagery should rely on the same brain areas that support visual
perception (Kosslyn, 1994)
▪ fMRI or PET -> prove correlation
▪ TMS or lesions -> information about causation
▪ Neural substrates of visual imagery are similar to those of visual
perception
▪ Visual cortex is widely activated during imagery
▪ Positive correlation between visual cortex activity and vividness of
imagery (Amedi et al., 2005)
Neural background of visual mental
imagery (Pearson, 2019)
▪ Voluntary mental imagery is based on combinations of
information retrieved from stored memory
1
Arithmetic
False memory
avigation
Moral decisions
Sports
Creativity
Motor contro l
▪ 9. The overall appearance of the shop from the opposite side of the road
▪ 10. A window display including colours, shapes and details Of individual items for
sale
▪ 11. You are near the entrance. The colour, shape and details of the door.
▪ 12. You enter the shop and go to the counter. The counter Assistant serves you.
Money changes hands.
▪ Finally think of a country scene which involves trees, mountains
and a lake. Consider the picture that comes before your mind’s
eye.
▪ Different strategy?
20 'O
:::r
A>
a
A>
responding >,
<.>
C:
~
▪ More than poor metacognition of visual imagery
Cl>
~ 0 0
g' 30 C:
... ,:s
20 0
▪ Dorsal pathway 10
YES NO
• Physical definition • Perceptual definition
• The falling tree causes pressure • If no one is in the forest there will
changes be no experience
• Sound • Sound perception
• „the sound had a frequency of • „the piercing sound of the trumpet
1000 Hz” filled the room”
The sound 1gh Perception:
Hig Pitch
▪ Most sensitive at frequencies between 2000-4000Hz Figure 11.3 Three d1 erent frequencies o a pure tone. Higher
frequencies are assoc ted w1 h the perception of high r pitches
(speech!)
▪ https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=XLfQpv2ZRPU
▪ Human frequency range: 20Hz to 20000Hz
▪ Changes with age
▪ https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=VxcbppCX6Rk
How Old Are You r Ears? {Hearing Test)
- ~~~
Pitch
https://virtualpiano.net/
frequency separated by a
multiple of two
- E.g., A1 = 27.5Hz, A2 = 55Hz,
A3 = 110Hz t
Low pitch
C2 – 65.4 Hz
High pitch
C7 –
t
- Similar perceptual experiences approx. 2000 Hz
Amusia
▪ „tone deafness”
▪ Inability to determine the direction of the pitch change
▪ Impaired identification of pitch direction but not basic pitch
discrimination
▪ Right temporal lobe
▪ Decreased volume of the superior temporal arcuate fasciculus in the
right hemisphere
▪ Connects the temporal and frontal lobe
R I iv Arnpli u nd D
fo E ironmental Sounds
SO D ITlJO LSIDB
10,000,000 0
P rcep 10n:
Louder
rce 10n:
Quieter Louder Low So
Time
't J
Figure 11.4 Three d. r nt amphrudes of a pu e one Lar r
amph ud 1s associated w, the percep on of greater loudness.
Timbre --
m
'V
20
Guitar
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=VRAXK4QKJ1Q I
C o
&.
•
a:• 0 200
C
20
sounds different
&_ 10
c!
20
C
&. 0
i
o 200 0,000
Frequ ncy (Hz)
Hearing loss
membrane
Cells of
.......
1::
,
..
,....
•!.~
degeneration ~::
,:! Malle us lncus
high frequencies
\
' .'.,
,,. ,,,
. ': .'. ,',''_.:'-·--~◄"/
Organ of Corti
Basilar Scala
Sound waves Auditory canal Tympanic Round membrane tympani
membrane window
Noise-Induced hearing loss
▪ Loud noises cause degeneration of the hair cells
▪ United States Occupational Safety and Health
Agency
▪ Workers cannot be exposed to sound levels greater
then 85 dBs for an 8-hour work shift
▪ Leisure noises
▪ Listening to music (headphones), riding motorcycles,
playing musical instruments (in a band), concerts,
sport events
▪ Depends on the intensity and the length of the
exposure
▪ Ear protection/turn down the volume
Hidden hearing loss (Plack et al., 2014)
▪ Normal performance on a standard hearing test
▪ Measuring tresholds for hearing tones across the frequency
spectrum
▪ Quiet room
▪ Indicate when you hear a tone
▪ Difficulties in a noisy environment
Speech
▪ Basic unit of speech = PHONEME
▪ = shortest segment of speech that, if changed, would change
the meaning of a word
▪ E.g., BIT/PIT/BAT
▪ Refers not to letters but to speech sounds that determine the meaning
of what people say
Multimodal perception
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=2k8fHR9jKVM
Multimodal perception
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=2k8fHR9jKVM
8
different senses
▪ McGurk effect (McGurk & MacDonald, 1976)
▪ Audiovisual speech perception
▪ In a noisy environment we use information
provided by the speaker’s lip movements
Figure 13.9 Th McGu k ect. wo e 1s saym /ba-oa/.
u her lip ov m n s co respand a/ a ta/, so h liste r r po s
arin /fa-fa/.
The role of meaning
▪ Easier to perceive phonemes that appear in a meaningful
context (Rubin et al., 1976)
▪ Leg, bat, sin (580ms)
▪ Jum, baf, teg (631ms)
▪ Respond by pressing a key as rapidly as they can when you hear a
sound that begin with ‚b’
▪ Phonemic restoration effect
▪ https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=pF1IfcfHKD4
▪ https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=fLPQZgYMJ7Q
▪ „The state governors met with their respective legislatures convening in the
capital city”
▪ Bottom-up (nature of the acoustic signal)
▪ Top-down (context that produces expectations in the listener)
Speech segmentation
▪ Foreign language ~ „unbroken string”
▪ Perception of words is not based only on the energy stimulating
the receptors
▪ Knowledge of the meaning of words
▪ Transitional probabilities
...... t&te Sound ...,for . .
\Wfdl·······•'•..... -Nob
that lt II c11Jiajt 1D tel fRlffl 11118
........... Mid ...... . .
Oll•llagia Spa■• .... ......
of u.. Sandin.
s p BG T A IO N
▪Read the following words: „Anna Mary
Candy Lights Since Imp Pulp Lay Things”
lll
▪ „The apple was eaten by the girl”
▪ „The boy was pushed by the girl”
▪ Difficulty with the second sentence –
wether the girl pushed the boy or the boy
▪ Problems in processing the structure pushed the girl?
of sentences
▪ Cognitive control deficit during the use of
language
Wernicke’s aphasia
▪ Werncike’s area, temporal lobe
▪ The produced speech is grammatically correct and fluent but
incoherent
▪ Produce meaningless speech +
unable to understand
11111 t:'DIII wtb,oul lmOII'. sentences and writing
IIWtll!ClaadllOW
▪ More widespread difficulties in
myamLUlll-
understanding compared to
DOW
p. )
Broca
▪ Broca – hardships with sentences where the
meaning depends on the context
▪ „word deafness”
TO TE T - hort rion
am : - - - - - - - - - - - - , - - - - - - - ; - - - - - . - : - . - - - - - - - - -
rlirth: _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ ,ar~ ncl months.
Schooling: _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ Oatc: _ _ _ _ __
E1t1mia n _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ __
Part I ( LI tokens)
I • Touch a ircl .
2- Touch a. quare .
. Touch a yel l , 1 en
4 - Touch ,, r.:d C)nc .
. Touch u black one.
o- I ouch a gre n one.
7-Touch a whit· ond .
l'art 2 (Only I• rg t ken )
. Touch the yellow quarc.
9. Touch the blzck circle.
I 1he n 1rcle.
I 1he "hiI quarc.
Part 3 ( II
I • • le.
□□
I quarc.
I uarc re n.
I le.
Part 4(0n ly
lo
1
e .-.xi :rnd 1he gre,•11
qwre ::ind the black
uar • and the gl'Ct'fl
re.
•
0
I
Part - ( 11 lokens)
rck and red ci re le.
What is the human imagination? What is this amazing individuals can experience vivid colour without colour
ability, which most of us have, that allows us to travel information stimulating the retina2. In post-traumatic
through space and time, testing out different virtual stress disorder (PTSD), individuals experience flash
worlds, objects, foods, fears and pleasures? When we backs or vivid, intrusive memories of trauma expe
think about the sensory characteristics of something rienced as involuntary imagery 3. Such conscious,
like an apple or a sunset, most of us have a conscious involuntary sensory experiences, without a direct cor
visual experience of those things. We literally become responding sensory input, have elsewhere been dubbed
conscious of some version of the apple or sunset, albeit ‘phantom perceptions’2. One proposed overarching
a degraded, fuzzy or weak experience. framework is that internal experiences like imagery
Mental imagery has played a central role in discus can be divided into two types of imagery-like experi
sions of mental function for thousands of years, first by ences, where one is voluntary and the other involuntary.
philosophers, then by psychologists, and now by neuro These two categories are analogous to the subtypes of
scientists. Almost any behaviour or cognitive process attention — endogenous and exogenous4 — and, as in
that might gain from sensory simulation tends to utilize the early days of research into attention, current work
mental images. is just beginning to shed light on the commonalities
However, to a minority of individuals, the idea that and differences between these two forms of phantom
people can have a voluntary conscious sensory experi perception2.
ence in their mind’s eye comes as a complete surprise; Importantly, mental imagery covers all five senses;
these individuals lack the ability to voluntarily form however, visual mental imagery research has tended to
mental images: aphantasia1. On the other side of the dominate, as is also the case with perception research.
spectrum, strong imagery plays a core role in many Focusing research on one faculty can make the endeav
anxiety disorders, depression, schizophrenia and our more tractable and studying vision has several
Parkinson disease, and is increasingly being harnessed advantages over studying the other senses. Humans
as a uniquely powerful tool for psychological treatment. are visual creatures; this is clear when we look at the
Mapping out imagery’s seemingly contradictory contri proportion of cortical tissue assigned to processing
butions to human cognition, whereby imagery can be visual information as compared to the other senses.
both advantageous and clinically disruptive, or even Additionally, visual perception is vivid and full of
possibly unnecessary (aphantasia), offers exciting novel detailed information, often making visual stimuli
insights into the human mind. informative tools, as is the case with many illusions that
School of Psychology, When people talk about the mind’s eye, they typi induce forms of involuntary phantom vision. However,
The University of New South
Wales, Sydney, Australia.
cally refer to the voluntary experience of creating a it is worth noting the limitations this focus on vision
conscious sensory experience at will. However, there are has on our overall understanding of mental imagery
e-mail: jpearson@
unsw.edu.au many examples of involuntary sensory experiences that and it is perhaps too often assumed that findings
https://doi.org/10.1038/ are equally decoupled from direct sensory input. For from visual imagery will naturally generalize to the
s41583-019-0202-9 example, in synaesthesia and in many visual illusions, other senses.
Reverse directionality
Neural mechanisms of visual imagery Due largely to the imagery debate (Box 1), the major
The reverse direction of neural Mental imagery involves activity across a large neural ity of neural imaging work on mental imagery has
information flow, for example, network spanning frontal areas right back to primary focused on early visual areas and the overlap or similar
from the top-down, as opposed sensory areas. The neuroscience of imagery can be ities between visual imagery and visual perception. As
to the bottom-up.
divided into work looking at the mechanisms involved such, not much brain imaging work has investigated this
in triggering the imagery process, mechanisms of gener systemic model of voluntary imagery in operation as a
ation or manipulation, as well as mechanisms underlying whole, though evidence from various different studies
the strength and vividness of visual imagery content and does appear to support it.
its overlap with sensory perception. However, imagining
an apple, doing a mental rotation task or imagining per Frontal areas. Forming or manipulating a mental image
forming a sporting activity are three very different tasks does appear to involve activity in the frontal areas7–10.
that too often get referred to simply as imagery. Much However, the activity in frontal areas seems largely inde
work has been done investigating visual imagery in pendent of the precise content of the imagery9–11. This
particular, and the role of the primary visual cortex suggests that frontal areas play more of a general organ
(V1), for reasons relating to the imagery debate (Box 1). izational or executive role in coordinating spatial and
Further, more recently, the large individual differences sensory areas, rather than holding imagery representa
in imagery strength and vividness have been harnessed tions or content per se. The frontal cortex shows up as
to obtain information about the neural mechanisms of an active area in many functional magnetic resonance
imagery. Here, a brief overview is given of the evidence imaging (fMRI) studies7,9–12. However, despite some
for the involvement of different neural areas and how evidence of reverse directionality through the processing
imagery is processed across the network of these areas. hierarchy5,6, it remains unclear how the different areas
throughout the brain cooperate dynamically to achieve
Imagery creation: vision in reverse the goal of an imagery experience. Part of the difficulty
It is hypothesized that voluntary mental imagery is based in parsing out the isolated role of frontal areas is due
on combinations of information retrieved from stored to the diversity of types of imagery tasks performed in
memory. Accordingly, a very simple yet intuitive model fMRI experiments. For example, imagining an apple,
of functional voluntary imagery has been proposed as doing a mental rotation task or imagining a physical
depicted in Fig. 1: a reverse visual hierarchy5,6. In addi activity are three very different tasks that logically should
tion to the empirical evidence, this model also makes involve different brain areas, yet all get referred to under
intuitive sense as it is difficult to imagine content you the umbrella term ‘imagery’13,14.
have never been exposed to and therefore have no mem
ories of. Typically, in imagery generation, we are com Hippocampus. Some evidence suggests that the hippo
bining different content that our senses have previously campus might be required to form complex or spatially
been exposed to and is stored in our memory. distributed images. When individuals form such images,
a blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response has
been documented in the hippocampus15,16. Likewise,
Box 1 | Imagery debate
individuals with hippocampal damage show spatially
Starting in the 1970s and running until the 2000s, a debate over the nature of mental related impairments in constructing imagined experi
representation dominated mental imagery research21,134. The debate hinged on the ences and their narrative descriptions lack richness and
question of what formats the brain could represent information in. One side of the spatial coherence17. When single neurons are measured
debate argued that information about visual objects was stored in a symbolic, and even directly in humans, a small percentage of hippocampal
language-like, format135, whereas the other side argued that such information can be
cells do respond to imagery18; however, other work has
stored in a number of different ways, including depictive formats. Accordingly,
the debate largely focused on the two representational options: propositional and
failed to show a role for the hippocampus in imagery cre
depictive. A propositional format would be something similar to a descriptive format ation19,20. In sum, although probably involved in spatial
used in a language. Depictive representations are often referred to as pictorial for and memory elements of imagery, the exact role of the
shorthand, as depictive representations require a functional space like a two-dimensional hippocampus in imagery remains unclear.
XY coordinate plane or picture (see ref.21 for examples).
Throughout the 1970s and 1980s, various behavioural paradigms produced evidence Visual cortex. Questions about the role of the early visual
that imagery could be represented in a depictive format, only for the other side of the cortex, in particular V1, in mental imagery have domi
debate to point out alternative interpretations of the data136,137. When neuroimaging nated brain imaging work on mental imagery for the last
became available in the 1990s, the debate focused on showing a blood oxygenation 25 years. From the early 1990s to late 2000s, over 20 studies
level-dependent or positron emission tomography response to imagery in the primary
investigated the role of area V1 in imagery3. Most of
visual cortex, following the logic that the primary visual cortex represents visual
information depictively.
these studies were motivated by what is referred to as the
In 2015, Pearson and Kosslyn made the strong claim that the so-called imagery debate imagery debate, now generally considered to be over21
was over21 and the abundance of evidence that imagery can be depictive, while still (Box 1). However, those studies were not without con
involving semantic information, is now overwhelming. Evidence spans from behavioural troversy; although many did not find significant BOLD
evidence that imagery is represented locally in retinotopic visual and orientation space responses above baseline in the early visual cortex10,22–27,
to functional magnetic resonance imaging voxel-wise Gabor wavelet models trained on many others did13,28–38. In hindsight, these discrepancies
depictive visual features (perceptual retinotopic location, spatial frequency and can be explained by inter-study differences in imagery
orientation); see ref.21 for a full discussion. Few public debates have lasted so many tasks, imagery content (for example, a single visual fea
decades; this unique dialogue adds to the rich and often controversial history of ture versus a complex image), as well as by individual
imagery research.
differences in imagery strength or vividness3,35,39.
Voxel-wise models of
With the advent of multivariate decoding or pre Default mode network and imagery
perception dictive data analysis techniques in fMRI, there is now The default mode network (DMN) is a networked group
Magnetic resonance imaging clear evidence that the content of mental imagery can of brain areas that regularly show up during periods of
and functional magnetic be decoded from early visual areas, including V1 and V2 non-task rest, it is largely defined by the functional con
resonance imaging decoding
(refs38,40–42). It is interesting that this appears to be the nectivity or temporal relationships between activity in
methods that are constrained
by or based on individual voxel case despite relatively low BOLD amplitude responses. spatially remote areas such as the posterior cingulate
responses to perception, which The decoding algorithms used by these approaches can cortex, dorsal medial prefrontal cortex and hippocam
are then used to decode be trained on visual perception, visual working mem pus43. However, the DMN also shows similarities with
imagery.
ory content and, of course, imagery, and in all cases can networks identified as being active during cognitively
Spatial transformations
accurately decode the content of mental imagery40,42. demanding tasks44. Interestingly, the networks known
Transformations in a spatial Further, when these algorithms are trained on perceptual to be involved in reexperiencing the past or creating
domain. images and constrained by voxel-wise models of perception possible future experiences overlap with the DMN45–48
(the decoding model is based on perceptual information and has become known for its association with such
Qualia
such as contrast and spatial frequency), imagery can still processes. One of the few studies to look at conscious
The conscious sense or feeling
of something, different from be accurately decoded, suggesting imagery in the early manipulation, construction and destruction of mental
detection. visual cortex is composed of the same visual features as images showed that multiple areas, including the dorso
afferent sensory perception42. lateral prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal cortex, pos
Taken together, the abovementioned brain imaging terior precuneus and occipital cortex act together as a
work involving the visual cortex suggests there are pat network to construct and deconstruct abstract mental
terns of activity common to perception and imagery that images, that is, images that are not representations from
emerge as early as V1, but become increasingly similar specific episodic memories7. In sum, evidence suggests
with ascension up the visual processing hierarchy3. This that the DMN is involved in imagery generation or con
visual hierarchy trend fits well into the overall model of struction; however, because most of these studies have
imagery dynamics (Fig. 1). If voluntary imagery is a top- investigated complex scenes from episodic memory or
down, triggered instance of sensory–memory recall that future scenarios, it is hard to separate the relationships
uses the visual cortex (and depending on imagery content, between network elements and temporal and memory
hippocampal and/or parietal areas), then it naturally fol components from the isolated act of forming the pure
lows that higher-level visual areas (for example, visual area sensory elements of a mental image. Although consider
3 and fusiform face area) that are physically and/or synap ing that many individuals are likely engaging in imagery
tically closer to the trigger source (frontal and medial tem during a resting state scan, when they are typically asked
poral lobe), would have stronger or more perception-like to relax and think about whatever they like, it should not
representations than more distant areas like V1 (Fig. 2). be surprising that the DMN relates to imagery. Future
work should compare the DMN with measures of pure
visual imagery of abstract features or objects and probe
mind-wandering imagery content during these resting
3 scans to divulge the role of the DMN in imagery.
&
imagery signal ding sensory cortices. For example, involuntary motion
Imagery imagery will involve the middle temporal area, whereas
(High-level brain areas) involuntary colour imagery will involve V1 and V4.
More overlap What currently remains unknown, is the involvement,
between if any, of higher-level, non-sensory areas like the frontal
imagery and and parietal cortices (Fig. 3). Although it seems logical
perception
areas that these areas would not be involved in generating
involuntary imagery, we lack good datasets to show this.
Strong representations
Neural mechanisms of imagery strength
At least since the 1800s, people have wondered why
imagery differs so much from person to person. Now
Less overlap that the imagery debate is resolved21, the mechanisms
between of imagery, how an image is formed and why some
imagery and
perception are stronger than others (both inter-individually and
areas intra-individually) remains one of the primary research
Perception questions.
Strong representations
(Low-level brain areas) Recent work has begun to investigate the relation
Bottom-up ship between V1 surface size and imagery character
perception
v
istics59. V1 surface size, like imagery, can vary between
individuals substantially60. Research using objective
Fig. 2 | Graphical depiction showing the two streams — bottom-up perception and perceptual measures of imagery (the binocular rivalry
top-down voluntary imagery. Voluntary imagery and perception have a greater overlap technique) (Box 2) has shown that the surface size of
in high-level areas (dark blue) than in lower-level areas (light blue). This graphical depiction
both V1 and V2, but not V3, negatively predicts the
may well not hold for involuntary imagery.
sensory strength of mental imagery59. In other words,
those with stronger imagery had smaller primary visual
actually present51–53. Nevertheless, despite these data sug cortices. Further, the binocular rivalry technique allows
gesting that the animal might have a conscious imagery- imagery precision to be tested across a number of visual
like experience of the absent stimulus, we simply do features. In this study, the precision of visual imagery
not know. was measured by the spread, or generalization, of
Related work with humans has demonstrated that imagery across both spatial orientation and retinotopic
colour memory of colour-specific fruit (for example, an visual space. Both these measures of imagery precision
orange is typically orange; a banana is typically yellow) showed a positive predictive relationship with V1 sur
can be decoded from the BOLD response in V1 when face size59, much like perception does61. Interestingly,
humans view grey-scale images54. These brain-imaging those with the strongest imagery and smallest V1s did
studies are supported by behavioural research suggest not have the most precise imagery, suggesting that, as
ing we have a colour experience when viewing such imagery becomes stronger, it also becomes less precise.
grey-scale colour-specific images55 (Fig. 3). Other work This relationship between more vivid and stronger
has demonstrated that the perceptual ‘filling in’ of low- imagery and a smaller visual cortex is also an emerg
contrast moving texture patterns leads to activity in V1 ing trend in the clinical literature. Human brain his
and secondary V2 (ref.56) and, likewise, colour filling or tology shows that the size of V1 and its total number
‘neon colour spreading’ is associated with greater activity of neurons are reduced in schizophrenia compared
in V1 for the corresponding regions of the visual field to normal healthy control brains by around 25%62.
where the illusion is experienced57. This is compared to the overall brain size reduction
Prior expectations about an upcoming visual stim in patients with schizophrenia of only around 2%.
ulus can have a strong effect on subsequent sensory Individuals with schizophrenia show enhanced self-
perception; indeed, fMRI work has demonstrated that reported vividness of mental imagery63 and individuals
a sensory template of the upcoming stimulus is for with schizotypal personality disorder show stronger sen
med in V1 even when the perceptual stimulus is never sory imagery in the objective binocular rivalry imagery
presented58. Interestingly, it remains unknown how paradigm compared to controls64. Likewise, in PTSD,
voluntary or involuntary such a representation is; are patients who report more vivid voluntary self-initiated
individuals taking up a strategy of using voluntary imagery65 also show signs of reduced visual cortex
imagery to aid the upcoming perceptual task? Or are size66,67. Furthermore, stimulant-dependent individuals
these representations purely reflexive and involuntary? who demonstrate strong involuntary imagery that plays
Schizotypal personality Are these representations conscious? New work suggests an inductive role in substance dependencies68 also show
disorder that images can form prior to conscious voluntary effort, a reduction in grey matter around the visual cortex69.
A mental disorder suggesting the existance of non-conscious involuntary It is worth noting, however, that the link between
characterized by social anxiety, 41
thought disorder, paranoid
images . Fig. 3 shows a proposed overview of some of imagery strength and V1 size is correlational, and so,
ideation, derealization and the types of imagery and their possible overlap in the despite this relationship showing up across both clinical
transient psychosis. visual cortex. and non-clinical populations, it does not clearly specify a
has been taken as a measure of the sensory strength or imagery93, and takes researchers one step closer to
visual energy of imagery76 (see Box 2 on measurement being able to recreate the sensory contents of the mind.
techniques). Just like visual perception, visual imagery However, despite the evidence suggesting commonali
seems to be largely local in retinotopic and orientation ties between weak perception and imagery, the two are
space42,59,79,83,90,91, lending further support to the similarities clearly very different conscious experiences and future
between weak visual perception and visual imagery. work should aim to map out why non-clinical imagery
A criticism of some imagery research was the lack of is not experienced with the full conscious experience of
methodology to differentiate visual imagery and visual afferent perception.
attention. For example, if an individual imagines a par
ticular colour while they are performing a perceptual Imagery in normal cognitive function
task involving colour, they will be differentially attending Recent years have produced many publications linking
to the colour they imagine. Hence, any effects of imagery mental imagery to a range of cognitive processes from
and attention will be difficult to separate. Importantly, episodic and visual working memory, spatial navigation,
in some of this research, the effect of prior imagery and reading comprehension to creativity and moral decision-
prior visual attention can be dissociated in a number of making (Fig. 5). If mental imagery is a sensory simula
ways such as through the timing of these effects and their tion of events, objects or scenarios in isolation from the
susceptibility to sensory disruption83; this provides fur afferent sense organ-induced versions of these things,
ther evidence that these sensory-like effects are driven by then how and when do we use imagery in everyday life?
imagery and not attention. In addition, these perceptual
priming effects are linked to the reported imagery vivid Memory and imagery
ness on a trial-by-trial basis74, suggesting the effects are Despite the fact that the fields of mental imagery, visual
coupled to the imagery generation process. working and autobiographical episodic memory have
Brain imaging work also provides evidence that developed independently, evidence suggests that many
imagery can be thought of as a form of top-down weak people utilize imagery to perform most forms of visual
perception. Multiple studies have now used BOLD acti memory tasks94–97. Indeed, in the episodic memory liter
vation patterns during afferent visual perception to later ature, thinking about the future is referred to as imagin
decode the contents of mental images42,91,92, suggesting ing the future. Depending on who you are and probably
an overlap in neural representation. Further, deep learn how strong your imagery is, the role of imagery across
ing algorithms trained on a limited set of perceptual fea different types of memory might seem obvious and self-
tures can be used to decode untrained features in mental evident. However, the fields of imagery and working
memory have diverged and until recently have remained
largely separate98.
Box 2 | New measurement techniques In regard to visual working memory, when people are
Like dreaming or hallucinations, research into mental imagery has been constrained by asked how they complete such tasks, they typically report
a lack of reliable objective measurement methods. From the early use of questionnaires using two primary strategies. One strategy is to pick
by Galton in the 1800s110, self-report questionnaires have been the gold standard used out salient features in a test array that the person is
to measure imagery. However, recent years have seen a flurry of new and more objective required to remember and encode them in a propositio
measurement techniques. nal or phonological form, which is then compared to
Behavioural techniques the test99. The other strategy commonly described is the
In 2008, we published the first work showing how you could use a visual illusion, called creation of detailed mental images during the retention
binocular rivalry, to measure imagery. This technique involves randomly cuing an interval, which are then compared to the subsequent
individual to imagine a coloured object before a brief rivalry presentation. The content test arrays94,95,99,100. These descriptions are synonymous
of the imagery primes dominance in the rivalry illusion in a manner that reflects the with descriptions of mental imagery and it is likely that
imagery vividness both on a trial-by-trial and individual differences basis74,83. Importantly, these individuals use mental imagery as a mnemonic
this technique works even when a cognitively demanding task is added between the tool to retain visual information. Both behavioural and
imagery period and the rivalry presentation. This technique can be made even more brain imaging work now suggests that sensory imagery
objective, in terms of removing direct subjective reports, by measuring rivalry
representations are used by people who have imagery to
dominance with an embedded probe task138. In this case, rivalry dominance is assessed
using an embedded probe task, where the probe is easier to detect when it is complete some forms of visual working memory
embedded in the dominant rivalry pattern, and then the influence of imagery on rivalry tasks40,94,95,101. Additionally, both imagery and visual
can be determined by the level of probe detection. Using such a performance-based working memory can change perception83,102, and they
dependent variable instead of rivalry dominance reports means that imagery can be show similar capacity functions103,104. However, research
measured without any subjective reports. In addition, such methods can be used to also suggests that not all people use visual imagery
measure imagery of different visual features such as pure colour or motion imagery, or to solve visual working memory tasks, with irrele
even complex object imagery, using the one dependent measure90,133,138, or even vant visual information impairing only some partic
compare imagery and synaesthesia using the one common measure79. ipant’s performance on visual working memory tasks.
Imaging techniques Specifically, only participants with good imagery appear
Recent functional magnetic resonance imaging research has clearly shown that to be affected by irrelevant visual information94,95,105, sug
imagery content can be objectively investigated using brain imaging40,42,139. What gesting that only they use visual imagery and low-level
someone imagines can be decoded or predicted based only on the brain’s response to visual regions to perform these tasks. The idea that only
the afferent perceptual versions of that same content. In fact, imagery content can now good imagers use visual imagery, and hence the visual
be decoded with perceptual training data from different object classes93, and other
cortex, to perform visual working memory tasks may
methods like electroencephalography and pupil dynamics can also be used.
help explain much of the current controversy in both the
Low
Noise level in visual cortex
High Why does it matter that imagery is utilized across
Low such a large range of cognitive processes? We have
Weakest imagery known since Francis Galton’s seminal paper on mental
imagery in 1880 that the spectrum of imagery vivid
ness spans from aphantasia to hyperphantasia (highly
vivid, almost realistic imagery)110. Accordingly, if many
cognitive processes use imagery, but imagery can vary
so much across the population, then it follows that the
Top-down signal strength
In addition to featuring alongside or as a symptom of small proportion of otherwise healthy people report
the disorder, visual imagery increasingly plays a role in having no visual experience at all when they attempt
evidence-based psychological treatment. For example, to imagine something. In other words, their minds are
cognitive behavioural therapy often includes ‘imaginal completely blind — no matter how hard they try, they
exposure’, which involves the patient repeatedly imag do not have a conscious visual experience of mental
ining a feared object or context until their anxiety level imagery; this phenomenon has only recently been given
subsides123. Imagery rescripting has been somewhat the name congenital aphantasia1. Initially, it was unclear
successful in treating PTSD, social and snake phobias, if aphantasia was more of an issue of imagery reporting,
and a variety of other psychological disorders124. It typi in that people with aphantasia might still have func
cally involves the practice of rescripting the imagery in a tional imagery, but not be aware of it or have a differ
sequence to a more positive or adaptive outcome. ent criterion for reporting it. However, recent research
As the evidence grows that visual imagery can trigger goes beyond subjective self-reports of aphantasia and
a larger emotional response than propositional or sym has shown that, along with floor scores on imagery
bolic thoughts125, the understanding of imagery both as a questionnaires, people with aphantasia score signifi
symptom and as a strategic therapeutic treatment grows. cantly lower on the sensory binocular rivalry measure
Clinical research should aim to leverage data and theory of visual imagery50 (Box 2); in other words, the sensory
from fundamental brain research on imagery as soon as strength of their imagery is also lacking, suggesting that
possible, as there are many promising avenues for uti aphantasia is more than poor metacognition of visual
lizing or manipulating imagery as a novel therapeutic imagery.
intervention. Interestingly, although people with aphantasia scored
lower than controls on all imagery vividness or content
Extreme differences in visual imagery measures, they actually scored slightly higher than con
Aphantasia. Since Galton’s seminal paper in the 1800s110, trols on spatial imagery questionnaires50. This suggests
the idea of a category of people whose mind is com that, although the ‘what’ or content of imagery typically
pletely blind was largely forgotten until recently1,126. associated with the ventral stream of visual processing
Initially based just on self-report, it seemed that a is lost, the ‘where’, or spatial properties, associated with
dorsal areas might still be intact. It also suggests that
aphantasia could dissociate along the classic ‘what and
where’ processing pathways in the brain127. Accordingly,
Prospective Eye witness
areas of the brain that process spatial properties, includ
memory memory ing the hippocampus, may not be the underlying cause
of aphantasia. Other work provides evidence that peo
Arithmetic ple with aphantasia can perform easy and medium, but
False memory not hard visual working memory tasks128. Likewise,
a case report of two people with aphantasia showed
Navigation that they scored low on ratings of ‘reliving’ their epi
sodic memories, although they still reported believing
Episodic Mental Visual working
memory imagery memory Moral decisions these memories106. Many individuals with aphantasia
describe visual imagery during dreams1, suggesting that
Sports
involuntary forms of imagery might still be intact.
Although the popular media has shown a fascination
Creativity with aphantasia, it is still early days in terms of scientific
Motor control research. Perhaps the most poignant question surrounds
Reading
the idea that aphantasia could either be just one end of a
Mind wandering
comprehension spectrum of imagery abilities or it could be in a category
all of its own, different to people with very weak imagery.
Further research is needed to shed light on these two
Fig. 5 | Graphical depiction of the cognitive processes related to mental imagery. possibilities.
The cognitive processes shown in the dark blue zone — visual working and episodic The fact that individuals with aphantasia can still per
memory — are there because reasonable evidence exists that, in people with mental form many daily tasks that non-aphantasics use men
imagery (excluding people with aphantasia), they are largely reliant on mental imagery tal imagery for, for example, visual working memory,
for normal functioning and there is a ‘good’ amount of evidence to support this suggests they use a different strategy and brain mech
relationship94,95. Evidence has shown that those with strong imagery will utilize it as a anisms to do so. Presently, it is unknown what mem
mnemonic tool to perform visual working memory tasks94,95. This evidence is beyond ory aids they use, perhaps semantic encoding or some
correlational and has used sensory perturbation methodologies (see section on non-representational geometry or symbols. It is worth
memory)94,95. Likewise, episodic memory research has specifically looked at the rich thinking about the possible implications of this, if one
sensory nature of remembered past experiences as well as the vividness of projecting
was to simply go by performance on a compound task
oneself into possible future episodic experiences96,109. Across a range of different
methodologies, effect sizes and degree of evidence, the cognitive processes shown in like visual working memory, it would be very difficult to
the lighter blue zone have been hypothesized to involve mental imagery, although the differentiate a person with aphantasia and a person with
degree of evidence is less than that for the dark blue zone. The outer lighter blue zone normal mental imagery. However, it is safe to say that
shows moral decisions140, eye witness memory141, false memories142, arithmetic, the underlying neural mechanism used to perform the
navigation143, sports144, reading comprehension145, mind wandering146 and creativity147. task by these two individuals would be quite different.
Such a scenario underlines the importance of testing as imagery research gains popularity, researchers will
imagery ability when investigating cognitive processes reignite eidetic and hyperphantasic imagery research.
like visual working or episodic memory. Further, this
suggests that using imagery as a cognitive tool is not The future of imagery research
fundamental to performing other cognitive functions Now that the imagery debate is over21, and we have mul
and has interesting implications for the way we should tiple new objective methods to investigate imagery, what
theorize about imagery as a cognitive tool and its possible should imagery researchers focus on?
role in evolution. Visual perception is a constructive affair; it involves
both feedforward and feedback signals working closely
Hyperphantasia or eidetic imagery. Hyperphantasia, and interactively together. Hence, isolating the effects of
or what has previously been called eidetic imagery, is bottom-up feedforward or feedback signals during per
at the other end of the imagery spectrum — consisting ception is very difficult. Voluntary imagery is perhaps
of strong and vivid imagery. Early in the 20th century, the only pure form of sensory representation that can
the topic of eidetic imagery attracted much attention, be solely due to feedback signals and can occur in com
commonly described as a highly detailed, almost photo- plete sensory isolation (although not always). Hence, it
realistic image experienced directly after seeing the represents a unique window to study the dynamics of
object, but also days or weeks later129–132. Eidetic imagery, feedback signals in the brain and understand the con
like aphantasia, is only observed in a small percentage structive nature of visual perception — an important
of the population, mainly in children, with frequency step of reductionism to understand the mechanisms
estimates from 0–11% in children129. Unlike afterim behind sensory perception. As perception is interactive,
ages, eidetic images remain still while the eye moves, and hence cannot be understood without parsing out the
are experienced in positive colours, projected out into mechanisms of feedback, it can be argued that imagery
space (much like a form of synaesthesia) and individuals is the key to understanding normal visual perception. It
use present, not past tense language, when referring to will be interesting to see how imagery research can be
the image. harnessed to understand visual perception.
Eidetic imagery has been assessed in a number of Several pertinent questions regarding imagery
ways, from verbal descriptions to more objective meth remain, including the issue of why imagery rarely feels
ods such as showing individuals random-dot stereo as strong and vivid as afferent sensory perception; the
grams, pictures of hundreds of randomly placed dots, causes underlying aphantasia and the large range of
with those in the centre shifted to give the perception individual differences in the experience of imagery;
of depth when the two images are shown one to each whether there exists a training protocol that might
eye simultaneously; however, for eidetic imagers the improve imagery strength; whether imagery indeed
depth patch could be seen clearly, even when the two induces more activity in visual cortical areas, as opposed
images were shown up to 24 hours apart, by combin to being just modulatory; and whether voluntary or
ing the first or eidetic image with the second perceptual involuntary imagery could ever exist unconsciously133.
image130. Because modern day psychophysics, fMRI As many of the new methods to investigate imagery are
and transcranial magnetic stimulation methods have only newly available, it is exciting to think over all the
not been used to investigate eidetic imagery, we do not future imagery research waiting to be performed.
have a lot of data to suggest a possible mechanism or
why it seems more prevalent in children. Hopefully, Published online 5 August 2019
1. Zeman, A., Dewar, M. & Della Sala, S. Lives without mechanisms for visual working memory. Curr. Opin. 17. Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Vann, S. D. & Maguire, E. A.
imagery — congenital aphantasia. Cortex 73, Neurobiol. 15, 175–182 (2005). Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine
378–380 (2015). 9. Yomogida, Y. Mental visual synthesis is originated in new experiences. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104,
This article documents and coins the term the fronto-temporal network of the left hemisphere. 1726–1731 (2007).
aphantasia, described as the complete lack Cereb. Cortex 14, 1376–1383 (2004). 18. Kreiman, G., Koch, C. & Fried, I. Imagery neurons
of visual imagery ability. 10. Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L. G. & Haxby, J. V. Distributed in the human brain. Nature 408, 357–361 (2000).
2. Pearson, J. & Westbrook, F. Phantom perception: neural systems for the generation of visual images. 19. Maguire, E. A., Vargha-Khadem, F. & Hassabis, D.
voluntary and involuntary non-retinal vision. Trends Neuron 28, 979–990 (2000). Imagining fictitious and future experiences: evidence
Cogn. Sci. 19, 278–284 (2015). 11. Goebel, R., Khorram-Sefat, D., Muckli, L., Hacker, H. from developmental amnesia. Neuropsychologia 48,
This opinion paper proposes a unifying framework & Singer, W. The constructive nature of vision: direct 3187–3192 (2010).
for both voluntary and involuntary imagery. evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging 20. Kim, S. et al. Sparing of spatial mental imagery in
3. Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A. & Kosslyn, S. M. studies of apparent motion and motion imagery. patients with hippocampal lesions. Learn. Mem. 20,
Mental imagery: functional mechanisms and clinical Eur. J. Neurosci. 10, 1563–1573 (1998). 657–663 (2013).
applications. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 590–602 12. Mellet, E. et al. Functional anatomy of spatial mental 21. Pearson, J. & Kosslyn, S. M. The heterogeneity of
(2015). imagery generated from verbal instructions. mental representation: ending the imagery debate.
4. Egeth, H. E. & Yantis, S. Visual attention: control, J. Neurosci. 16, 6504–6512 (1996). Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 10089–10092 (2015).
representation, and time course. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 13. O’Craven, K. M. & Kanwisher, N. Mental imagery This paper proposes an end to the ‘imagery debate’
48, 269–297 (1997). of faces and places activates corresponding stimulus- based on the discussed evidence that imagery can
5. Dijkstra, N., Zeidman, P., Ondobaka, S., Gerven, M. A. J. specific brain regions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, be represented in the brain in a depictive manner.
& Friston, K. Distinct top-down and bottom-up brain 1013–1023 (2000). 22. D’Esposito, M. et al. A functional MRI study of mental
connectivity during visual perception and imagery. 14. Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G. & Thompson, W. L. Neural image generation. Neuropsychologia 35, 725–730
Sci. Rep. 7, 5677 (2017). foundations of imagery. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, (1997).
6. Dentico, D. et al. Reversal of cortical information flow 635–642 (2001). 23. Knauff, M., Kassubek, J., Mulack, T. & Greenlee, M. W.
during visual imagery as compared to visual 15. Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D. & Maguire, E. A. Using Cortical activation evoked by visual mental imagery
perception. Neuroimage 100, 237–243 (2014). imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic as measured by fMRI. Neuroreport 11, 3957–3962
7. Schlegel, A. et al. Network structure and dynamics of memory. J. Neurosci. 27, 14365–14374 (2007). (2000).
the mental workspace. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 16. Bird, C. M., Capponi, C., King, J. A., Doeller, C. F. 24. Trojano, L. et al. Matching two imagined clocks:
16277–16282 (2013). & Burgess, N. Establishing the boundaries: the the functional anatomy of spatial analysis in the
8. Ranganath, C. & D’Esposito, M. Directing the mind’s hippocampal contribution to imagining scenes. absence of visual stimulation. Cereb. Cortex 10,
eye: prefrontal, inferior and medial temporal J. Neurosci. 30, 11688–11695 (2010). 473–481 (2000).
25. Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E. & Buckner, R. L. 47. Hassabis, D. & Maguire, E. A. Deconstructing episodic 72. Winkler, A. M. et al. Cortical thickness or grey matter
Memory’s echo: vivid remembering reactivates memory with construction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, volume? The importance of selecting the phenotype
sensory-specific cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 299–306 (2007). for imaging genetics studies. Neuroimage 53,
11125 (2000). 48. Gerlach, K. D., Spreng, R. N., Gilmore, A. W. & 1135–1146 (2010).
26. Formisano, E. et al. Tracking the mind’s image in Schacter, D. L. Solving future problems: default 73. Bakken, T. E. et al. Association of common genetic
the brain I: time-resolved fMRI during visuospatial network and executive activity associated with variants in GPCPD1 with scaling of visual cortical
mental imagery. Neuron 35, 185–194 (2002). goal-directed mental simulations. Neuroimage 55, surface area in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA
27. Sack, A. T. et al. Tracking the mind’s image in 1816–1824 (2011). 109, 3985–3990 (2012).
the brain II: transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals 49. Levine, D. N., Warach, J. & Farah, M. Two visual 74. Pearson, J., Rademaker, R. L. & Tong, F. Evaluating
parietal asymmetry in visuospatial imagery. Neuron systems in mental imagery. Neurology 35, 1010 the mind’s eye: the metacognition of visual imagery.
35, 195–204 (2002). (1985). Psychol. Sci. 22, 1535–1542 (2011).
28. Le Bihan, D. et al. Activation of human primary visual 50. Keogh, R. & Pearson, J. The blind mind: no sensory 75. Rademaker, R. L. & Pearson, J. Training visual
cortex during visual recall: a magnetic resonance visual imagery in aphantasia. Cortex 105, 53–60 imagery: improvements of metacognition, but not
imaging study. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 90, (2017). imagery strength. Front. Psychol. 3, 224 (2012).
11802–11805 (1993). 51. Sakai, K. & Miyashita, Y. Neural organization for the 76. Pearson, J. New directions in mental-imagery research:
29. Sabbah, P. et al. Functional magnetic resonance long-term memory of paired associates. Nature 354, the binocular-rivalry technique and decoding fMRI
imaging at 1.5T during sensorimotor and cognitive 152–155 (1991). patterns. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 178–183 (2014).
task. Eur. Neurol. 35, 131–136 (1995). 52. Messinger, A., Squire, L. R., Zola, S. M. & Albright, T. D. 77. Keogh, R., Bergmann, J. & Pearson, J. Cortical
30. Chen, W. et al. Human primary visual cortex and Neuronal representations of stimulus associations excitability controls the strength of mental imagery.
lateral geniculate nucleus activation during visual develop in the temporal lobe during learning. Preprint at bioRxiv https://www.biorxiv.org/content/
imagery. Neuroreport 9, 3669–3674 (1998). Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 12239–12244 (2001). 10.1101/093690v1 (2016).
31. Ishai, A. Visual imagery of famous faces: effects of 53. Schlack, A. & Albright, T. D. Remembering visual 78. Terhune, D. B., Tai, S., Cowey, A., Popescu, T. &
memory and attention revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage motion: neural correlates of associative plasticity Kadosh, R. C. Enhanced cortical excitability in
17, 1729–1741 (2002). and motion recall in cortical area MT. Neuron 53, grapheme-color synesthesia and its modulation. Curr.
32. Ganis, G., Thompson, W. L. & Kosslyn, S. M. Brain 881–890 (2007). Biol. 21, 2006–2009 (2011).
areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual 54. Bannert, M. M. & Bartels, A. Decoding the yellow of 79. Chiou, R., Rich, A. N., Rogers, S. & Pearson, J.
perception: an fMRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 20, a gray banana. Curr. Biol. 23, 2268–2272 (2013). Exploring the functional nature of synaesthetic colour:
226–241 (2004). 55. Hansen, T., Olkkonen, M., Walter, S. & Gegenfurtner, dissociations from colour perception and imagery.
33. Klein, I., Paradis, A. L., Poline, J. B., Kossly, S. M. K. R. Memory modulates color appearance. Cognition 177, 107–121 (2018).
& Le Bihan, D. Transient activity in the human calcarine Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1367–1368 (2006). 80. Arieli, A., Sterkin, A., Grinvald, A. & Aertsen, A.
cortex during visual-mental imagery: an event-related 56. Meng, M., Remus, D. A. & Tong, F. Filling-in of visual Dynamics of ongoing activity: explanation of the large
fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12 (Suppl. 2), 15–23 phantoms in the human brain. Nat. Neurosci. 8, variability in evoked cortical responses. Science 273,
(2000). 1248–1254 (2005). 1868–1871 (1996).
34. Lambert, S., Sampaio, E., Scheiber, C. & Mauss, Y. 57. Sasaki, Y. & Watanabe, T. The primary visual cortex 81. Wassell, J., Rogers, S. L., Felmingam, K. L., Bryant, R. A.
Neural substrates of animal mental imagery: calcarine fills in color. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, & Pearson, J. Biological psychology. Biol. Psychol.
sulcus and dorsal pathway involvement — an fMRI 18251–18256 (2004). 107, 61–68 (2015).
study. Brain Res. 924, 176–183 (2002). 58. Kok, P., Failing, M. F. & de Lange, F. P. Prior 82. Kraehenmann, R. et al. LSD increases primary process
35. Cui, X., Jeter, C. B., Yang, D., Montague, P. R. expectations evoke stimulus templates in the primary thinking via serotonin 2A receptor activation. Front.
& Eagleman, D. M. Vividness of mental imagery: visual cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 1546–1554 Pharmacol. 8, 418–419 (2017).
individual variability can be measured objectively. (2014). 83. Pearson, J., Clifford, C. W. G. & Tong, F. The functional
Vision Res. 47, 474–478 (2007). 59. Bergmann, J., Genc, E., Kohler, A., Singer, W. impact of mental imagery on conscious perception.
36. Amedi, A., Malach, R. & Pascual-Leone, A. Negative & Pearson, J. Smaller primary visual cortex is Curr. Biol. 18, 982–986 (2008).
BOLD differentiates visual imagery and perception. associated with stronger, but less precise mental This study shows that the content of visual imagery
Neuron 48, 859–872 (2005). imagery. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3838–3850 (2016). can bias or prime subsequent binocular rivalry; this
37. Reddy, L., Tsuchiya, N. & Serre, T. Reading the mind’s This study shows that stronger but less precise paper was the basis for using binocular rivalry as
eye: decoding category information during mental imagery is associated with a smaller primary and a measurement tool for imagery.
imagery. Neuroimage 50, 818–825 (2010). secondary visual cortex. 84. Ishai, A. & Sagi, D. Common mechanisms of visual
38. Dijkstra, N., Bosch, S. E. & van Gerven, M. A. J. 60. Stensaas, S. S., Eddington, D. K. & Dobelle, W. H. The imagery and perception. Science 268, 1772–1774
Vividness of visual imagery depends on the neural topography and variability of the primary visual cortex (1995).
overlap with perception in visual areas. J. Neurosci. in man. J. Neurosurg. 40, 747–755 (1974). 85. Tartaglia, E. M., Bamert, L., Mast, F. W. & Herzog, M. H.
37, 1367–1373 (2017). 61. Song, C., Schwarzkopf, D. S. & Rees, G. Variability Human perceptual learning by mental imagery.
39. Kosslyn, S. M. & Thompson, W. L. When is early in visual cortex size reflects tradeoff between local Curr. Biol. 19, 2081–2085 (2009).
visual cortex activated during visual mental imagery? orientation sensitivity and global orientation This study shows that training with a purely imaged
Psychol. Bull. 129, 723–746 (2003). modulation. Nat. Commun. 4, 1–10 (2013). visual stimulus transfers to improve performance in
40. Albers, A. M., Kok, P., Toni, I., Dijkerman, H. C. 62. Dorph-Petersen, K.-A., Pierri, J. N., Wu, Q., perceptual tasks.
& de Lange, F. P. Shared representations for working Sampson, A. R. & Lewis, D. A. Primary visual cortex 86. Lewis, D. E., O’Reilly, M. J. & Khuu, S. K. Conditioning
memory and mental imagery in early visual cortex. volume and total neuron number are reduced in the mind’s eye associative learning with voluntary
Curr. Biol. 23, 1427–1431 (2013). schizophrenia. J. Comp. Neurol. 501, 290–301 mental imagery. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 1, 390–400 (2013).
This paper shows that both imagery and visual (2007). 87. Laeng, B. & Sulutvedt, U. The eye pupil adjusts to
working memory can be decoded in the brain 63. Sack, A. T., van de Ven, V. G., Etschenberg, S., Schatz, D. imaginary light. Psychol. Sci. 25, 188–197 (2014).
based on training on either, showing evidence & Linden, D. E. J. Enhanced vividness of mental 88. Brascamp, J. W., Knapen, T. H. J., Kanai, R., van Ee, R.
of a common brain representation. imagery as a trait marker of schizophrenia? Schizophr. & van den Berg, A. V. Flash suppression and flash
41. Koenig-Robert, R. & Pearson, J. Decoding the Bull. 31, 97–104 (2005). facilitation in binocular rivalry. J. Vis. 7, 12 (2007).
contents and strength of imagery before volitional 64. Maróthi, R. & Kéri, S. Enhanced mental imagery and 89. Tanaka, Y. & Sagi, D. A perceptual memory for low-
engagement. Sci. Rep. 9, 3504 (2019). intact perceptual organization in schizotypal contrast visual signals. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95,
This paper shows that the content and vividness personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 259, 433–438 12729–12733 (1998).
of a mental image can be decoded in the brain up (2018). 90. Chang, S., Lewis, D. E. & Pearson, J. The functional
to 11 seconds before an individual decides which 65. Morina, N., Leibold, E. & Ehring, T. Vividness effects of color perception and color imagery. J. Vis.
pattern to imagine. of general mental imagery is associated with the 13, 4 (2013).
42. Naselaris, T., Olman, C. A., Stansbury, D. E., Ugurbil, K. occurrence of intrusive memories. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. 91. Slotnick, S. D., Thompson, W. L. & Kosslyn, S. M.
& Gallant, J. L. A voxel-wise encoding model for early Psychiatry 44, 221–226 (2013). Visual mental imagery induces retinotopically
visual areas decodes mental images of remembered 66. Chao, L. L., Lenoci, M. & Neylan, T. C. Effects of post- organized activation of early visual areas. Cereb. Cortex
scenes. Neuroimage 105, 215–228 (2015). traumatic stress disorder on occipital lobe function 15, 1570–1583 (2005).
This study shows that mental imagery content can and structure. Neuroreport 23, 412–419 (2012). 92. Thirion, B. et al. Inverse retinotopy: inferring the visual
be decoded in the early visual cortex when the 67. Tavanti, M. et al. Evidence of diffuse damage in content of images from brain activation patterns.
decoding model is based on depictive perceptual frontal and occipital cortex in the brain of patients Neuroimage 33, 1104–1116 (2006).
features. with post-traumatic stress disorder. Neurol. Sci. 33, 93. Horikawa, T. & Kamitani, Y. Generic decoding of seen
43. Fox, M. D. et al. The human brain is intrinsically 59–68 (2011). and imagined objects using hierarchical visual
organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional 68. Kavanagh, D. J., Andrade, J. & May, J. Imaginary features. Nat. Commun. 8, 15037 (2017).
networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 9673–9678 relish and exquisite torture: the elaborated intrusion 94. Keogh, R. & Pearson, J. Mental imagery and visual
(2005). theory of desire. Psychol. Rev. 112, 446–467 working memory. PLOS ONE 6, e29221 (2011).
44. Smith, S. M. et al. Correspondence of the brain’s (2005). 95. Keogh, R. & Pearson, J. The sensory strength of
functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. 69. Ersche, K. D. et al. Abnormal brain structure voluntary visual imagery predicts visual working
Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 13040–13045 (2009). implicated in stimulant drug addiction. Science 335, memory capacity. J. Vis. 14, 7 (2014).
45. Østby, Y. et al. Mental time travel and default-mode 601–604 (2012). 96. Aydin, C. The differential contributions of visual
network functional connectivity in the developing 70. Song, C., Schwarzkopf, D. S., Kanai, R. & Rees, G. imagery constructs on autobiographical thinking.
brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 16800–16804 Reciprocal anatomical relationship between primary Memory 26, 189–200 (2017).
(2012). sensory and prefrontal cortices in the human brain. 97. Schacter, D. L. et al. The future of memory:
46. Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., J. Neurosci. 31, 9472–9480 (2011). remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron 76,
Poulin, R. & Buckner, R. L. Functional-anatomic 71. Panizzon, M. S. et al. Distinct genetic influences 677–694 (2012).
fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron on cortical surface area and cortical thickness. 98. Tong, F. Imagery and visual working memory: one and
65, 550–562 (2010). Cereb. Cortex 19, 2728–2735 (2009). the same? Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 489–490 (2013).
99. Berger, G. H. & Gaunitz, S. C. Self-rated imagery and 116. Holmes, E. A. et al. Mood stability versus mood 135. Kosslyn, S. M. Mental images and the brain.
encoding strategies in visual memory. Br. J. Psychol. instability in bipolar disorder: a possible role for Cogn. Neuropsychol. 22, 333–347 (2005).
70, 21–24 (1979). emotional mental imagery. Behav. Res. Ther. 49, 136. Pylyshyn, Z. W. What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s
100. Harrison, S. A. & Tong, F. Decoding reveals the 707–713 (2011). brain: a critique of mental imagery. Psychol. Bull. 80,
contents of visual working memory in early visual 117. Tiggemann, M. & Kemps, E. The phenomenology of 1–24 (1973).
areas. Nature 458, 632–635 (2009). food cravings: the role of mental imagery. Appetite 45, 137. Pylyshyn, Z. Return of the mental image: are there
101. Borst, G., Ganis, G., Thompson, W. L. & Kosslyn, S. M. 305–313 (2005). really pictures in the brain? Trends Cogn. Sci. 7,
Representations in mental imagery and working 118. Connor, J. P. et al. Addictive behaviors. Addict. Behav. 113–118 (2003).
memory: evidence from different types of visual 39, 721–724 (2014). This review provides an updated summary of the
masks. Mem. Cognit. 40, 204–217 (2011). 119. May, J., Andrade, J., Panabokke, N. & Kavanagh, D. imagery debate.
102. Kang, M.-S., Hong, S. W., Blake, R. & Woodman, G. F. Visuospatial tasks suppress craving for cigarettes. 138. Chang, S. & Pearson, J. The functional effects of prior
Visual working memory contaminates perception. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 476–485 (2010). motion imagery and motion perception. Cortex 105,
Psychon Bull. Rev. 18, 860–869 (2011). 120. Michael, T., Ehlers, A., Halligan, S. L. & Clark, D. M. 83–96 (2017).
103. Keogh, R. & Pearson, J. The perceptual and Unwanted memories of assault: what intrusion 139. Stokes, M., Thompson, R., Cusack, R. & Duncan, J.
phenomenal capacity of mental imagery. Cognition characteristics are associated with PTSD? Behav. Res. Top-down activation of shape-specific population codes
162, 124–132 (2017). Ther. 43, 613–628 (2005). in visual cortex during mental imagery. J. Neurosci.
This study shows a new method to measure the 121. Holmes, E. A., James, E. L., Kilford, E. J. & Deeprose, C. 29, 1565–1572 (2009).
capacity function of visual imagery and shows that Key steps in developing a cognitive vaccine against 140. Amit, E. & Greene, J. D. You see, the ends don’t justify
it is quite limited. traumatic flashbacks: visuospatial tetris versus verbal the means: visual imagery and moral judgment.
104. Luck, S. J. & Vogel, E. K. Visual working memory pub quiz. PLOS ONE 5, e13706 (2010). Psychol. Sci. 23, 861–868 (2012).
capacity: from psychophysics and neurobiology to 122. Shine, J. M. et al. Imagine that: elevated sensory 141. Dobson, M. & Markham, R. Imagery ability and
individual differences. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 391–400 strength of mental imagery in individuals with source monitoring: implications for eyewitness
(2013). Parkinson’s disease and visual hallucinations. Proc. R. memory. Br. J. Psychol. 84, 111–118 (1993).
105. Pearson, J. & Keogh, R. Redefining visual working Soc. B 282, 20142047 (2014). 142. Gonsalves, B. et al. Neural evidence that vivid
memory: a cognitive-strategy, brain-region approach. 123. Foa, E. B., Steketee, G., Turner, R. M. & Fischer, S. C. imagining can lead to false remembering. Psychol. Sci.
Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 28, 266–273 (2019). Effects of imaginal exposure to feared disasters in 15, 655–660 (2004).
106. Greenberg, D. L. & Knowlton, B. J. The role of visual obsessive-compulsive checkers. Behav. Res. Ther. 18, 143. Bird, C. M., Bisby, J. A. & Burgess, N. The
imagery in autobiographical memory. Mem. Cognit. 449–455 (1980). hippocampus and spatial constraints on mental
42, 922–934 (2014). 124. Hunt, M. & Fenton, M. Imagery rescripting versus imagery. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 142 (2012).
107. Sheldon, S., Amaral, R. & Levine, B. Individual in vivo exposure in the treatment of snake fear. 144. Jones, L. & Stuth, G. The uses of mental imagery
differences in visual imagery determine how event J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 38, 329–344 (2007). in athletics: an overview. Appl. Prev. Psychol. 6,
information is remembered. Memory 25, 360–369 125. Holmes, E. A. & Mathews, A. Mental imagery and 101–115 (1997).
(2017). emotion: a special relationship? Emotion 5, 489–497 145. Dils, A. T. & Boroditsky, L. Visual motion aftereffect
108. D’Argembeau, A. & Van der Linden, M. Individual (2005). from understanding motion language. Proc. Natl Acad.
differences in the phenomenology of mental time 126. Zeman, A. Z. J. et al. Loss of imagery phenomenology Sci. USA 107, 16396–16400 (2010).
travel: the effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion with intact visuo-spatial task performance: a case of 146. Christian, B. M., Miles, L. K., Parkinson, C. &
regulation strategies. Conscious Cogn. 15, 342–350 ‘blind imagination’. Neuropsychologia 48, 145–155 Macrae, C. N. Visual perspective and the characteristics
(2006). (2010). of mind wandering. Front. Psychol. 4, 699 (2013).
109. Vannucci, M., Pelagatti, C., Chiorri, C. & Mazzoni, G. 127. Ungerleider, L. G. & Haxby, J. V. ‘What’ and ‘where’ in 147. Palmiero, M., Cardi, V. & Belardinelli, M. O. The role
Visual object imagery and autobiographical memory: the human brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 4, 157–165 of vividness of visual mental imagery on different
object Imagers are better at remembering their (1994). dimensions of creativity. Creat. Res. J. 23, 372–375
personal past. Memory 24, 455–470 (2015). 128. Jacobs, C., Schwarzkopf, D. S. & Silvanto, J. Visual (2011).
110. Galton, F. Statistics of mental imagery. Mind 5, working memory performance in aphantasia. Cortex
301–318 (1880). 105, 61–73 (2017). Acknowledgements
This paper was the first formal empirical paper 129. Gray, C. R. & Gummerman, K. The enigmatic eidetic The author thanks R. Keogh, R. Koenig-Robert and A. Dawes
investing imagery vividness, including the first image: a critical examination of methods, data, and for helpful feedback and discussion on this paper. This paper,
report of what is now called aphantasia. theories. Psychol. Bull. 82, 383–407 (1975). and some of the work discussed in it, was supported by
111. Holmes, E. A. & Mathews, A. Mental imagery in 130. Stromeyer, C. F. & Psotka, J. The detailed texture Australian National Health and Medical Research Council
emotion and emotional disorders. Clin. Psychol. Rev. of eidetic images. Nature 225, 346–349 (1970). grants APP1024800, APP1046198 and APP1085404,
30, 349–362 (2010). 131. Haber, R. N. Twenty years of haunting eidetic imagery: a Career Development Fellowship APP1049596 and an
112. Hackmann, A., Bennett-Levy, J. & Holmes, E. A. where’s the ghost? Behav. Brain Sci. 2, 616–617 Australian Research Council discovery project grant
Oxford Guide to Imagery in Cognitive Therapy (Oxford (1979). DP140101560.
Univ. Press, 2011). 132. Allport, G. W. Eidetic imagery. Br. J. Psychol. 15,
113. Blackwell, S. E. et al. Positive imagery-based cognitive 99–120 (1924). Competing interests
bias modification as a web-based treatment tool for 133. Kwok, E. L., Leys, G., Koenig-Robert, R. & Pearson, J. The author declares no competing interests.
depressed adults: a randomized controlled trial. Clin. Measuring thought-control failure: sensory
Psychol. Sci. 3, 91–111 (2015). mechanisms and individual differences. Psychol. Sci. Peer review information
114. Crane, C., Shah, D., Barnhofer, T. & Holmes, E. A. 57, 811–821 (2019). Nature Reviews Neuroscience thanks D. Kavanagh, J. Hohwy
Suicidal imagery in a previously depressed community This study shows that, even when people think they and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution
sample. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 19, 57–69 (2011). have successfully suppressed a mental image, it is to the peer review of this work.
115. Hales, S. A., Deeprose, C., Goodwin, G. M. still actually there and biases subsequent perception
& Holmes, E. A. Cognitions in bipolar affective disorder (a possible candidate for unconscious imagery). Publisher’s note
and unipolar depression: imagining suicide. Bipolar 134. Kosslyn, S. M. Image and Mind (Harvard Univ. Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional
Disord. 13, 651–661 (2011). Press, 1980). claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.