Pearson Marr Archetype Indicator
Pearson Marr Archetype Indicator
net/publication/216101940
CITATIONS READS
15 20,600
1 author:
Robert McPeek
Center for Applications of Psychological Type
11 PUBLICATIONS 119 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Robert McPeek on 04 June 2014.
“
Many predictions about psychological type and archetype were
confirmed in this study, which combined data for the Pearson-Marr
Archetype Indicator® and self-reported type preferences and stress
levels.
”
The Pearson-Marr Archetype Indicator and
Psychological Type
Robert W. McPeek
Center for Applications of Psychological Type
®
Table 1. Type Distribution for PMAI Respondents and SRTT Comparisons With U.S. National
Representative Sample.
Robert W. McPeek
demographics of the study’s sample and Pearson and As the F-ratios for the Hotelling’s Trace values for all
Marr’s (2002) archetypal descriptions. For example, four MANOVAs were significant (all p < .001), these
Pearson and Marr characterized Sages as interested in tests were followed by a univariate (ANOVA) analysis
scholarship and learning, and the most outstanding comparing means on each archetype for the four MBTI
characteristic of the current samples is arguably its dichotomies. These univariate F-values are the results
highly educated (over 50% with graduate degrees) reported in the following sections. The stress hypotheses
demographic. Pearson and Marr also described Seekers were tested by dividing subjects’ scores on each arche-
as interested in self-improvement, Magicians as “highly type as close to the median as possible (whole number
self-aware” (p. 29) individuals interested in how “the scoring prevented an exact 50–50 break), and conduct-
mind and the spirit work” (p. 29), and Creators as ing two-tailed t-tests with above/below median as the
interested in new inventions and methodologies. These independent variable and stress scores (1–5) as the
descriptions are consistent with the characteristics of dependent variable.
individuals interested in self-assessment. The biases of Of the 37 total hypotheses, 28 were confirmed at a
this study’s self-selected sample may thus be interpreted probability level of less than .05 (26 of the 28 at .01 or
as anecdotal support for the accuracy of Pearson and lower), with another 2 hypotheses approaching con-
Marr’s archetype descriptions. ventional statistical significance levels (p < .073 and
Archetypes, Type Preferences, and Stress p < .081). These are summarized below in TABLE 3.
Summary Results. There were 37 predicted relation- (SEE PAGE 59.) All 12 main hypotheses (one for each
ships between archetype scores, type preferences, and archetype) were strongly confirmed, 11 with a proba-
stress, as outlined in the Introduction. The type prefer- bility level of .001 or lower and one with p < .004. In
ence hypotheses were first tested using four multivariate summary, these results provide strong support for the
analyses of variance (MANOVA), with each preference predictions derived from Pearson and Marr’s (2002,
dichotomy (E–I, S–N, T–F, J–P) as an independent vari- 2003) archetypal descriptions.
able and the 12 archetype scores as dependent variables.
Note: Bold = main hypothesis confirmed; Background shading = unpredicted finding; italics = opposite to predicted result. *p < .05; ** p < .01, *** p < .001.
higher on three (Orphan, Destroyer, and Seeker) as well group actually scoring lower than other ages, contrary to
as stress. Gender clearly exerts a strong influence on PMAI predictions. Warrior scores were marginally significantly
scores, although the differences were largely inconsistent higher in the predicted direction, but given the power
with predictions from Pearson’s archetype theory. of analysis with a large subject pool and the number of
Age Hypotheses. Lack of data (only three subjects tests performed, this is probably meaningless.
younger than 20 years of age) prevented testing 2 of
the 12 age-specific hypotheses derived from Pearson’s DISCUSSION
theory. To test the remaining 10 hypotheses (2 of each The primary result of this research is a confirmation of
for 5 age groupings), scores were grouped as either predictable relationships between type and the arche-
inside or outside the targeted age range, and archetype type scales and descriptions of the PMAI. Overall, 21 of
scores were compared for these two groupings. The 28 (75%) predicted relationships between archetype
results are shown in TABLE 6. (SEE PAGE 63.) and type were confirmed, including all 12 of the primary
The only result that met the customary significance predications, one for each PMAI scale. Three more of
criterion of p < .05 was for Ruler, with the target age the 28 relationships approached statistical significant
Archetype Target Age Size of Target Age All Other F value P Result
Range Target Group Ages df = 1,
Group Mean Means 1,375
(p < .10). In addition, seven of nine predications apparent when considered from a type perspective than
regarding self-reported stress levels and archetype from Pearson and Marr’s (2002) archetypal description.
scores were confirmed. Myers et al. (1998) described the Extraverted attitude as
These results accomplished the main goal of this incorporating “a desire to act on the environment”
research. The results of the PMAI are meaningfully and “an action-oriented . . . way of meeting life” (p. 26).
related to type preferences and stress, provide evidence Although Pearson and Marr place more emphasis on a
for the convergent validity of the instrument, and Warrior’s discipline and determination, their description
augment the validity evidence summarized in Pearson is sprinkled with action-oriented phrases (e.g., Warriors
and Marr (2003). will “face the most fierce antagonist” and “enjoy spear-
Unconfirmed Hypotheses and Unexpected heading a crusade”) (p. 15).
Results. The few unconfirmed hypotheses and the The lack of a significant relationship between
occasional unpredicted relationships that emerged from Warrior scores and stress may also be related to activity
the data analysis may help us better understand and level. The research on learned helplessness (e.g., Peterson,
refine Pearson and Marr’s (2002) archetype descriptions. Maier, & Seligman, 1993) suggests that inescapable
For example, Orphan scores were higher for Introverts stress leads to a lethargic, low activity state. Chronic stress
than for Extraverts. Although unpredicted, this finding might thus lead to lower Warrior scores, working in
is not surprising, as the label “Orphan” implies social opposition to the original hypothesis that High Warriors
isolation. Also, the careful approach to life of the Orphan would experience higher stress, for a net cancellation.
archetype described by Pearson and Marr is consistent This, as well as the relationship between Warrior scores
with the research of Harker, Reynierse, and Komisin and J–P type preferences, may also come to light with
(1998), in which friends and relations described further study.
Introverts as “hesitant” and “cautious.” Surprisingly, Caregiver scores were not higher for
Likewise, the unpredicted increased preference for Extraverts than for Introverts, although the direction of
Extraversion over Introversion for Warrior is more the difference was in the predicted direction. Possibly,
C O N TA C T
Robert W. McPeek
Center for Applications of
Psychological Type
2815 NW 13th Street, Suite 401
Gainesville, FL 32609
352.375.0160
[email protected]
This Journal is being made available through the collaborative efforts of Dr. Tom Carskadon, Editor of the Journal of Psychological Type, and
the Center for Applications of Psychological Type, Inc., CAPT, worldwide publisher. Dr. B. Michael Thorne serves as Executive Editor of the
Journal of Psychological Type.
Journal of Psychological Type is a trademark or registered trademark of Thomas G. Carskadon in the United States and other countries.
CAPT is a not-for-profit organization dedicated to the meaningful application and ethical use of psychological type as measured through the
Myers-Briggs Type Indicator instrument.
Myers-Briggs Type Indicator, Myers-Briggs, and MBTI are trademarks or registered trademarks of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Trust in
the United States and other countries.
Center for Applications of Psychological Type, Inc. and CAPT are trademarks or registered trademarks of the Center for Applications of
Psychological Type in the United States and other countries.
ISSN 0895-8750.