(Advances in Mental Health Research) Hazel G. Whitters - A Study into Infant Mental Health_ Drawing together Perspectives of International Research, Theory, and Practical Intervention-Routledge (2022)
(Advances in Mental Health Research) Hazel G. Whitters - A Study into Infant Mental Health_ Drawing together Perspectives of International Research, Theory, and Practical Intervention-Routledge (2022)
This book is a study of infant mental health which blends knowledge and
understanding from three perspectives: international research, theory, and inter
vention. The volume increases awareness of the significance of infant mental
health, adding to the growing body of literature on influences upon lifestyles,
communities, society, and attainment.
The significance of mental health to development has come to the fore in
recent years and research in neuroscience is used to explore, and to understand
the complexities of the human brain. Each infant is exposed to unique influences
before and after birth. Neuroscience, genetics, adverse childhood experiences,
and personalities feature in the chapters as mitigating factors to attainment.
Exemplars create a bridge between research and implementation of recommen
dations, and illustrate the myriad of influences and permutations that can
enhance or hinder development. This book discusses internal influences from an
infant’s biological make-up, alongside the circumstances and relationships within
a family unit, as understanding these key aspects is integral to promotion of each
infant’s life chances. The volume concludes by considering future approaches to
nurturing infant mental health.
Carefully designed to stimulate discussion and professional inquiry, this
volume is an invaluable resource for researchers, academics, and scholars with
an interest in infant mental health.
Schizotypy
New Dimensions
Edited by Oliver Mason and Gordon Claridge
Hazel G. Whitters
Designed cover image: © Getty Images
First published 2023
by Routledge
4 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN
and by Routledge
605 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10158
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 2023 Hazel G. Whitters
The right of Hazel G. Whitters to be identified as author of this work has
been asserted in accordance with sections 77 and 78 of the Copyright,
Designs and Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or
utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now
known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in
any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing
from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or
registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and explanation
without intent to infringe.
British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
Preface viii
Index 181
Preface
Neural development
Research in the field of neuroscience (Dismukes et al., 2019) has greatly
increased understanding of brain development and portrays the composition
of a neuron that has a cell body, axon, and dendrites. Brain growth is com
plex, and dependent upon influences between physiological, emotional, and
genetic characteristics of an infant. An outcome is the creation of links
between neurons. The space between neurons is termed synaptic cleft, and the
process of connection is synaptogenesis.
It is fascinating to read that the branching between dendrites occurs when
the neuron nucleus is full, which means that it has accumulated its maximum
complement of electrical inputs. At this stage electrical inputs will continue to
be received but travel down the axon, cross the synaptic cleft through neuro
transmitters, and branch out to different dendrites. These dendrites lead to
more neurons, and neural connections are established.
Questions emerge from reading about brain processes, for example, does
the neuron nucleus release all the electrical outputs together at the point of
maximum capacity? Perhaps the release occurs in minuscule levels for output
to the axon, in tandem with further input of electrical impulses. Reaching
capacity may be the catalyst for this process to be initiated. I reflect upon my
thoughts on potential patterns or timed sequences in the release, and uptake
of electrical charges, and consider if I can relate this neural process to practice
knowledge. It is important that new learning which contains challenging
issues is retained and can be applied in linking research to daily practice.
The axons are well protected by insulation in the form of myelin. The
myelin allows the electrical inputs to travel very quickly along the axons to
cross the space of the synaptic cleft. Neural circuits which are created
throughout this process are termed synaptic transmission. Over time, pruning
takes place of synapses which are not being used, or less actively used than
others. It seems that the myelination process protects the circuits from being
pruned.
These processes are often represented in training for practice by the use of
cartoon symbols and sequences that demonstrate interdependency of compo
nents. An animation medium can promote understanding of these events in a
straightforward, predictable, and simplistic way. It is also essential that prac
titioners appreciate the intricacies of brain development, and the myriad of
Infant mental health 5
mitigating factors. It is a fascinating topic to study which imposes respect and
worthiness to the role of an early years practitioner. I feel that knowledge of
neural processes instils a sense of wonder, and awe as understanding of the
complexities are gained.
From a practical perspective, a lack of nurturing conditions, or direct
influences from adversities can prevent the neuron nucleus from reaching its
capacity. These barriers are described in practice as hindering or halting
achievement of potential. Subsequent branching and links between dendrites
and further neurons will not take place. This information gives stark realisa
tion of the significance of early intervention work to construction of the
brain’s architecture that affects the baby throughout his lifespan.
Classification of the nervous system by functionality presents a visual pic
ture of the internal layout and workings of the neural space. Mechanisms for
basic life functions are tucked safely in the middle of the brain, surrounded by
the cortex which supports the higher-level functioning of human beings. The
frontal, parietal, occipital, and temporal cortex lobes have essential roles to
play in the infant’s interpretation of the world which is based on sensory
experience and communication with others.
The 27th day after conception is obviously a significant milestone in preg
nancy due to formation of the neural tube which is the foundation for the
brain and spinal cord. It is interesting that one reason for significant brain
growth after birth is to ensure a safe birth passage. The small brain is pro
tected from damage during the birthing process and subsequently grows
rapidly in the early stages of childhood. A baby’s brain increases in weight by
2.5 times from birth to 12 months of age. Neural development in such a short
period is influenced by environmental conditions, internal well-being, and the
infant–primary carer relationship. The first year of life is a sensitive period of
learning for the mother and child thus presenting an optimum opportunity
for early intervention.
old’s vivid reactions to his family circumstances, daily challenges from the
pandemic, his expressions of need, and desire to have his interests met.
Behaviour is language, the original communication media of human beings,
and Jake’s expression of his emotions in the context of adverse childhood
experiences.
Epigenetics
Epigenetics is a familiar concept for researchers, and it indicates links
between environmental influences and activation of genes by changes in the
body before and after birth. Effective planning and implementation of learn
ing opportunities relies on knowledge of each child’s interests and needs in a
context of child-led pedagogy. The findings of Dismukes et al. (2019) indi
cated five important points regarding epigenetics.
These figures portray a broad range and, most importantly, the knowledge
that characteristics are informed by activation of genes and interaction with
environmental influences. The genotype or genetic make-up that affects a
baby’s physical characteristics is innate but greatly influenced pre-birth and
post-birth. Additionally, temperament or phenotype can be affected nega
tively by a parent’s lifestyle and reflected within the infant’s interpretation
and reaction to the world. Common examples of family adversities are par
ental drug or alcohol use, domestic violence, poverty, and isolation in a
community.
In my experience, the physical presentation of an infant who is living in
adversity can change markedly after he has been taken into kinship or foster
care, even for short-term respite. Hair colouring can transform from low-nutri
tional grey to natural healthy pigment, weight and height are gained rapidly, sad
eyes begin to shine with hope and motivation, and demonstration of the positive
aspects of temperament are observable features that indicate good care. It is
important that positive change is recorded in addition to negative as it clearly
highlights the influential aspects of a previous home environment and pinpoints
areas for intervention. Physical and mental well-being can activate an infant’s
motivation to seek out learning as opposed to maintaining his basic bodily
functions within survival mode. This knowledge is gained through observation
and assessment by a practitioner. Many countries use intervention to close the
attainment gap in recognition of the impact of adversities. For example, my work
is based in Glasgow city which is currently responding to the attainment gap in
relation to poverty, and family learning is promoted as an effective strategy to
change (Scottish Government, 2019).
Stress
The hormone cortisol can have positive effects in the short term and negative
for long-term periods if stress is prolonged. The circadian rhythm, as the 24
hour cycle of light and dark, regulates cortisol levels, but lifestyles can cause
changes to these patterns. Many families do not adhere to the traditional
pattern of sleep during darkness and activity during daylight hours. Parents
express that they can spend prolonged sessions on internet activity during the
16 Infant mental health
night and struggle to cope with a waking baby in the early hours of sunrise.
Long-term stress results in lengthy periods in which the body experiences high
levels of cortisol which impacts adversely upon blood sugar, blood pressure,
ability to sleep, and cognitive skill. Knowledge of the destructive properties of
cortisol provides rationale to strategic planning and funding for intervention
(National Scientific Council on the Developing Child, 2006). In addition, I
feel that daily practice is greatly informed through understanding that exces
sive cortisol can stop neural connections occurring and create stress reactions
which are barriers to learning and achievement of potential.
A high level of stress can also result in a stress response system being
underactive. For example, a baby’s lifestyle is profoundly disrupted if parent
ing skills are affected by toxic stress which the parent is unable to reduce. A
baby experiences the negative impact of this stress albeit indirectly from the
source that affects his parents. During these episodes, the baby’s hippo-
campus, which is the area of the limbic system supporting memory and links
to emotion and sensory learning, produces fewer cortisol receptors thus stress
remains at a high level.
I have not heard the concept of learned helplessness applied to practice for
many years, but the associated presentation is well known in a context of
child protection and displayed within observations of infants and primary
carers who have experienced abuse. The young child may appear to be placid
and content to an inexperienced practitioner, or to a parent. The child may
appear preoccupied with his proximal environment, particularly his own
clothing or body. I observe children actively hiding from a perceived threat by
covering their faces with hands or play items, moving under equipment, or
shutting their eyes and dipping their heads downwards towards their chests.
I have frequently observed the presentation of some infants as being non-
responsive and challenging for a practitioner to stimulate with learning
opportunities. Primitive dissociative adaptations, and physical and cognitive
freeze, are accompanied by physiological responses to prepare the body and
mind for dealing with a forthcoming threat. The heart rate slows down, blood
flows away from extremities, endogenous opioids reduce physical pain and
may flood the mind with a sensation of calmness and psychological distancing
from an attack. An infant who operates within a lifestyle of adversities learns
to minimise the potential for stress by limiting human interaction despite the
context of a nurturing nursery.
In the early stages of my career, the children who presented with these
characteristics were termed still children which depicted their body language.
During the 1980s, and 1990s, we observed and recorded this behaviour and
sought to devise practical strategies that supported engagement. As practi
tioners, we did not have access to scientific knowledge or an opportunity to
increase our understanding from research. Throughout a career, it is useful for
practitioners to reflect upon previous practice, and historical memories, in
order to enhance understanding. An increase in comprehension of the cir
cumstances is valuable. Reflection is a key factor in gaining optimum value
Infant mental health 17
from work experiences and raising a practitioner’s ability to link research, and
practice, thus closing the implementation gap.
Social development
Social development of an infant is cultural and contextual. The first rela
tionship and dyad in life before birth is the infant and his mother. By 26
weeks gestation, a foetus can react to the sound of his mother’s voice, and
hearing is known to be one of the first systems to develop.
At birth, babies demonstrate an intense interest in human faces and
attempt to copy gestures. This important finding signifies learning through
socialisation. Normative development indicates that socialisation progresses
18 Infant mental health
from a context of care routines at birth to a baby of 4 months seeking out
stimulation of the wider environment beyond his care needs. Thereafter, an
infant will make use of social referencing by adopting reactions and emotions
of the primary carer as his world is extended.
The framework of reference that the baby uses to engage, to interact, and
to understand the world evolves rapidly throughout the first year. The initial
12 months of life are regarded as one of the most sensitive periods for learn
ing, and the baby should be supported to maintain physical and emotional
health for genetic potential to be fulfilled. Babies learn at different rates, and
the astute practitioner appreciates the importance of repetition and con
solidation of knowledge to support understanding.
Care routines take place many times per day. These nurturing adult–child
interactions provide perfect opportunities for a key worker to observe a baby’s
needs and interests and to support expansion of his inner working model.
This internal framework is based upon experiential learning, and positive or
negative emotions rapidly become associated with care patterns. Secure
attachment is a key concept in services, and early years settings commonly
operate with a key-worker system. This adult–child relationship is not an
exclusive partnership, and an infant can create nurturing relationships with
other staff and students in the age group in which he is based. Some early
years services describe these room-worker relationships as positively extend
ing the rationale of key worker, in a context of secure attachment and learn
ing. During the pandemic, children in nurseries were cared for by a group of
practitioners within a “care bubble” as opposed to designated key workers
due to isolation periods impacting upon the service delivery.
Human beings continue to need secure attachment relationships through
out life to achieve and to support mental and emotional good health.
Attachment is an adaptation to a set of circumstances. Insecure attachment is
exhibited by an inability to self-regulate; therefore, the circumstances must
change to promote agency and a sense of self. Balbernie (2013) explained that
a value of poor should not be linked to attachment as the infant is simply
demonstrating effective survival responses to adversities albeit he is not
securely attached.
It is often the case that concerns about relationships are expressed as a
child “not having secure attachment to an adult.” Each infant has an inherent
predisposition to seek out positive relationships with a primary carer from
birth, but attachment requires a dyad in which an adult responds to a baby’s
emotional and physical needs and provides the optimum conditions for
development. Strengthening of attachment occurs within every moment of
interaction. The conditions nurture and respond to the baby’s overtures for a
supportive relationship.
The relationship with a primary carer is a template that informs future
relational capacity, and it provides the infant with a medium in which he can
learn to regulate emotions, behaviour, and actions. Lack of self-control, and a
limited capacity to regulate, can create conditions of high vulnerability in
Infant mental health 19
childhood and potentially relational and mental-health issues in adulthood.
Four aspects are identified by Balbernie (2013) as stress factors that can
impact upon the infant–mother relationship.
Chapter 2 focuses upon the infant’s relationships and the impact upon well
being and involvement with a learning environment. The chapter describes
the infant’s demonstration of attachment behaviours from birth (Howe, 2005).
Separation protest and proximity-seeking are key behavioural systems that are
expressed in the early years. Discussion is presented of the bio-social homeo
static regulatory system and includes reciprocal adult attachment behaviour
(Fonagy, 1999). Babies are unable to regulate their emotional reactions at
birth; however, the dyadic system develops over time, and it creates an essen
tial baseline of knowledge and understanding for the developing infant
(Fukkink, 2021).
Transgenerational transmission of deprivation is described alongside inter
vention that can support families to change and to develop in a context of
daily living. Three significant aspects of change to the inner working models
of primary carers that support infant mental health are highlighted: beha
vioural, cognitive, and social. Attunement, reciprocity, marked mirroring,
containment, and reflective functioning are described as contributing to the
ability and capacity of an infant to adapt to different environments and rela
tionships. The Circle of Security (Cooper et al., 2016) and the Solihull
Approach (Douglas & Rheeston, 2009) are presented as interventions that
support development of a secure interdependent family unit.
Relationships
Creation of a relationship with another fellow being is an inherent need of
every infant, and a secure attachment relationship is the foundation for a
healthy and fulfilling life. Infants demonstrate attachment behaviours from
birth, for example, seeking close physical contact, holding tightly onto an
adult, and showing preference for familiar human faces and voices with par
ticular emphasis on eye contact with a mother. Separation protest and proxi
mity-seeking are key behavioural systems that are expressed in the early years.
Fonagy (1999) found that reciprocal adult attachment behaviour responds to
and encourages the relationship overtures of an infant. Interactions lead to
the infant experiencing security on a physical and emotional level. Babies are
DOI: 10.4324/9781003358107-2
Relationships, involvement, and well-being 29
unable to regulate their emotional reactions at birth; however, the dyadic
system develops over time, and it creates an essential baseline of knowledge
and understanding for the developing infant. In the early stages, regulation is
based upon the infant’s expectations and role-modelling from the primary
carer’s behaviour. Fonagy (1999) applied the term “bio-social homeostatic
regulatory system” to these processes.
Bonding is presented as the emotional relationship from adult to child, and
attachment is the child’s emotional bond with the parent or a primary care
giver (Association of Infant Mental Health, UK, 2021).
These terms are often used interchangeably by carers. A caregiver can be
closely associated with a proximal environment, for example, a parent within
the home or a key worker in a nursery. During teaching sessions on relation
ship processes to parents, and practitioners, I feel that it is important to
communicate that the infant does not have responsibility for creating a secure
attachment relationship with an adult. Attachment requires a dyad, and the
adult must present appropriate conditions and a responsive relationship in
order for the child’s attachment cues to be recognised and to support his
status of emotional well-being and safety.
The Royal College of Midwives (2020) present common terms relating to
infant mental health: “attunement”, “reciprocity”, “marked mirroring”,
“containment”, “reflective functioning”. These terms can be applied to
describe actions that promote a positive attachment relationship and bonding
between adult and infant.
Bonding
Research indicates a link between maternal oxytocin levels and bonding
between mother and foetus, and mother and infant (Royal College of Mid
wives, 2020). Low levels are associated with higher incidence of postnatal
depression. During a Caesarean section there is no oxytocin released during
the process. There is a delay of 48 hours before oxytocin is detected after this
assisted birth (Levine et al., 2007). However, skin-to-skin contact between a
newborn infant and mother has been found conducive to prompt oxytocin
release at birth (UNICEF, 2021). In many countries, a newborn will be placed
upon his mother’s chest at birth to encourage the attachment processes and a
bond which will last a lifetime.
The parent–child dyad supports the newborn to regulate his emotional
reaction to external influences in the context of responses to objects, people,
environmental conditions, and his changing internal physiological state.
Acquiring and maintaining a status of emotional and physical security forms
the rationale of the human attachment system, and seeking to achieve this
goal is a motivational force upon the infant’s behaviour. Adequate resources
for development are required in the form of a variety of experiences, con
sistent relationships, and time for reflection that supports the infant to absorb
and assimilate knowledge within his inner working model. His reactions,
actions, and behaviour increase in complexity and intent over time. As the
bio-social homeostatic regulatory system evolves, then the infant’s ability and
capacity to demonstrate resilience to adversity increases, and negative influ
ences have less impact upon development.
Attachment is described in theory (Bowlby, 1979) as a bio-behavioural
mechanism which is demonstrated as a response to anxiety and founded upon
Relationships, involvement, and well-being 31
an outcome of survival. Observation of practice indicates that confirmation
and consolidation of the attachment relationship occurs during an infant’s
exploration of his environment. The attachment system is not activated but
maintained and strengthened during this time within adult–child reciprocal
interactions. The dyadic regulation of affect encompasses the primary carer
and infant working in partnership to achieve stability of the child’s emotional
states. Attachment is based upon needs and strategies associated with survi
val, in addition to supporting up-regulation and down-regulation of emotions
encountered during learning.
Howe (2005) presents the child’s organisation of attachment behaviours.
Over time, the child’s inner working model acquires representations of himself
within a proximal environment, his interpretation of other people, and
expectations of a relationship based upon his personal perspective and actions
of others. This base of knowledge leads to the child’s use of attachment
behaviours to increase the responses from primary carers to his need for
availability, and responsivity. His behaviour is communication about his needs
and often indicates how an attachment figure can respond to these needs at a
point of time. For example, an infant who desires physical reassurance will
cry, give eye contact to a primary carer, and raise both arms to indicate that
he wants to be lifted. If the adult lifts the child, then he will move his head
towards her body and hold onto her clothing with both hands. This reinforces
the message to the child that his communication methods are effective, and it
provides the carer with the knowledge and practical skill that responds effec
tively to the particular attachment behaviour of this infant. The carer and
child adapt their behaviour and create a goal-corrected partnership as termed
by Howe (2005).
Fonagy (1999) expressed that activation of the attachment system is
dependent on the child’s interpretation of his world and resultant status of
security, or insecurity. However, a child’s understanding of environmental
influences can also be led by the reaction of his parent or carer. The primary
carer’s reaction is based upon an adult inner working model which in itself
was influenced by childhood experiences, positive and negative. Research
indicates that children who are insecurely attached often have parents with a
similar attachment status, which Fonagy termed the transgenerational trans
mission of deprivation.
Attachment behaviour includes over-regulation, in which the child is pre
occupied with seeking care responses, or under-regulation, in which the child
may avoid potential stress. A disorganised care environment does not support
the child to identify or to use patterns of seeking and gaining secure attach
ment responses. Children who have experienced chaotic and inconsistent car
egiving may perceive adults as a source of fear and reassurance. Anxiety is
experienced by an infant as internal physiological changes in response to the
behaviour of one specific adult, family members within a household, or
strangers in a community (Fonagy, 1999). Behavioural patterns can exist in
the context of chaotic households, but the complexity of identifying, and
32 Relationships, involvement, and well-being
using, these patterns to support mental stability is beyond the developmental
capability of most infants.
Circle of Security
Acquiring the skill of reflective functioning is significant to the development
of secure attachment capacity in parents whose relationships are influenced by
their own childhood adversities. Fonagy (1999) presents potential for change
in a parent’s behaviour if opportunities and interventions can specifically
support the development of this skill. In turn, a parent’s increase in reflective
functioning instigates reflection in the child, and this intersubjective process
contributes to the development of their secure attachment relationship. The
Circle of Security (Cooper et al., 2016) and the Solihull Approach (Douglas
& Rheeston, 2009) are used within my workplace to train practitioners and
parents in the rationale and behaviour associated with reflective functioning
between adult and child. The primary carer, as an attachment figure, provides
two main functions:
1 A safe haven from potential threat. Threat may relate to new and unpre
dictable environments or a young child’s inability to differentiate between
safety and danger. This haven is communicated to the child by the adult’s
body language, availability, and reassurance through role-modelling from
the perspective of an adult’s interpretation of the potential danger. In
addition, attuning to the child’s changing emotions and behaviour as his
knowledge and understanding increases confirms the status of an envir
onment being safe for exploration. A safe haven refers to the attachment
figure and not the environmental setting, although a primary carer can
present a positive interpretation of an environment to the child by her
behaviour and attitude.
2 A secure base results in the attachment system being deactivated as the
child recognises conditions which are conducive to exploration, discovery,
and learning about his proximal and distal world. In practice, the child
does continue to refer to his attachment figure during these forays and to
share his delight in discovery through eye contact and body language.
Infants will often return to the attachment figure every few moments to
gain reassurance. I ask parents to imagine a string of elastic thread con
joining adult and infant. It can be stretched over time, but the connection
remains constant. If an adult responds consistently to a child’s relational
overtures, usually by the use of gesture or verbal interaction, then the
attachment relationship will be consolidated.
Secure and insecure are usually the attachment classifications given to par
ents as a baseline for understanding their role and infant’s behaviours. These
two descriptors represent the child’s emotional well-being in a context of the
parent–child relationship. Secure, ambivalent, avoidant, and chaotic are
Relationships, involvement, and well-being 33
further descriptors that support practitioners to recognise and interpret
these behaviours. A child can have different categories of relationships with
adults and siblings since attachment is a reciprocal phenomenon that
depends on a carer’s responses to the child’s initiations.
Relationship dyad
Difficulties in forming positive relationships do not just affect the individual
but impact upon the relationship dyad, and the behaviour of one partner may
be detrimental to the other. The establishment of secure attachment with a
key worker in a service can provide a change in understanding and expecta
tions of a relationship with a parent for an insecurely attached infant. How
ever, if the parent is unable to change his or her attachment responses and to
provide a safe haven and secure base, then the infant will remain insecurely
attached to his mother or father. Sroufe et al. (2000) linked physical abuse,
lack of emotional care, and empathy to conduct and anxiety issues in a child.
This research suggested that formation of a positive relationship with an adult
who is not a primary carer, for example, a grandparent or a professional, can
mitigate the negative effects of a parent–child relationship.
Doyle (2001) conducted a small significant study twenty years ago that high
lighted the influence of comfort objects in a context of attachment and security.
The participant group was composed of 14 adult survivors of childhood emo
tional abuse. Findings indicated that the parents of some of the participants had
experienced difficulties in demonstrating love but had allowed children to create
positive relationships with others. These children had gained nurture from com
fort toys, pet animals, or personal artefacts. Participants in this study had iden
tified contact with pets as a key source of impactful relationships in childhood.
The aforementioned study cites play-therapy sessions as contributing to an
accumulation of positive experiences for a child, although these sessions do
not directly impact on changing parental behaviour (Doyle, 2001). It is
known that interventions based upon play-therapy principles can have a
major impact upon a child’s resilience to adversity, and his ability and capa
city to minimise the negative effects upon his sense of self can increase. Pre
vious studies have shown that parents may attempt to block opportunities for
a child to form relationships with other significant people, and the research by
Farmer and Owen (1995) indicated that parents may actively sabotage these
relationships. The authors present an example of a child’s relationship with a
play therapist.
Doyle (2001) concluded that social support does not have to be intense or
even long-lasting in order to gain a long-term effect on a child’s security and
positive sense of self. One or more positive relationships can be supplemented
by the use of comfort objects or pets. Due to restrictions imposed by the
current COVID-19 pandemic, to minimise potential for virus transfer, chil
dren cannot bring personal objects into a service from home. However, the
key-worker relationship is formed in the early moments of induction, and this
34 Relationships, involvement, and well-being
strategy has supported creation of a secure attachment between practitioner
and child. Forming patterns of behaviour during transitions are significant
representations of a safe haven that are retained by each child integrating into
a service. Implicit memories are formed that can reconfigure the inner work
ing model and promote a positive blueprint of relationships to the young
child. Transition from home to nursery can be supported by the use of a
nursery toy in place of a home toy as representative of a safe haven. The final
finding from this research indicated that the cumulative effect of multiple
positive experiences is a key aspect to healthy social and emotional develop
ment within the context of adversities.
Separation anxiety
Sroufe et al. (2000) linked challenging relationships to a disorder in the cate
gory of psychopathology. Separation anxiety is one illustration of a relation
ship problem. This condition is one of the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic
which is currently being exhibited in services. Infants are referred for place
ment who have not experienced life outside the home environment due to
lockdown conditions throughout the country. Many infants have adopted the
high level of anxiety and stress that is being experienced and exhibited by
their parents on a daily basis. The continuation of COVID-19 procedures and
regulations has meant that parents are unable to enter a service.
This set of circumstances may appear detrimental to the formation of a
key-worker–infant relationship in a context of separation anxiety. In reality,
many anecdotal reports from services indicate that families can cope with the
home-to-nursery transition more effectively than pre-COVID. Infants and
parents appear to be compartmentalising their relationships to specific con
texts and minimising transition anxiety. Due to COVID conditions, the tran
sition is focused upon two people: the parent and key worker representing a
relational person-to-person transition bridge. Each attachment figure is loca
ted within a specific environment: the parent is outside a service that repre
sents the home environment; and the key worker is inside a service. Previously
a parent would create a person–environmental transition bridge between
home and nursery by entering a service with the child. I feel that key workers
have benefited from these necessary health and safety conditions by focusing
with greater intent, purpose, and understanding on the creation of a secure
attachment relationship with an infant. The practitioner has gained an
increase in self-worth and value of his relationship with key children.
Self-regulation
The ability and capacity to adapt one’s behaviour and actions to different
environments, and relationships with others, are unique to human beings
(Tomasello, 1999). These skills encompass comprehension of other people as
being intentional mental agents which is gained within a specific species and a
Relationships, involvement, and well-being 35
social group culture. In the early stages of infancy, and throughout childhood,
a baby relies on sensitive responding from a primary carer to co-regulate his
emotions. As the months progress over the first year of life, the infant devel
ops purpose in his actions and begins to direct his caregiver. The infant is
influenced at the behavioural and physiological level, and he gains an ele
mentary ability and capacity to make decisions in relation to his felt needs.
The self-regulation processes evolve throughout a lifespan from a foundation
that is created during infancy.
By the end of the first year, an infant has the capacity to use learning skills
in the form of gaze, absorption of information, followed by social referencing,
and imitation. Tomasello (1999) refers to triadic interactions at this stage of
development in which the infant’s focus is extended from self to a primary
carer, and their shared interest in the proximal environment. This research
study found that 12-month-old infants copied intentional actions of a primary
carer which were used for a specific purpose, and the infants ignored acci
dental actions by the adult. Furthermore, findings indicated that infants could
apply the intentional actions to multiple circumstances, which increased their
skill sets and extended cultural learning.
The infant begins to understand his ability to direct an adult’s attention to
his chosen object or area of exploration by the use of direct gesture or subtle
body language. In services, the practitioners will use observations and infor
mation-sharing with parents to acquire intimate knowledge of an infant’s
personality, interests at a point in time, and communication strategies. The
child’s perception of his own mental state depends on ability and capacity to
notice his caregiver’s representation of their shared world and to create a
foundation of knowledge and understanding for personal use. Imaginative or
pretend play between an adult and child provides multiple opportunities for
the developing infant to be guided towards an understanding of reality in his
external world through his internal representation of these experiences. The
parent’s involvement with the child’s internal world, in a context of imagina
tive play, supports development of an understanding of self as an intentional
mental agent. Over time, the child can appreciate that his imaginative world
does not need to replicate his external experiences. Imagination and creativity
lead to the child gaining mastery, control, and increase in self-esteem by
exploring and consolidating his ideas and understanding.
During the teleological period, in the development of self, the child inter
prets behaviour of others within a context of visible outcomes, as opposed to
non-concrete beliefs. The infant may experience anxiety, or satisfaction, with
reference to his internal physiological changes and interpretation of the
external environment. The infant’s body and mind are in a state of arousal.
His experience includes representation of the world by his primary carer,
usually the mother, foster carer, or key worker in the earliest days and
months. The infant’s behaviour is initially a representation of his internal
experiences and reactions. Patterns can rapidly evolve, and persist, particu
larly if the infant is exposed to situations that induce chronic anxiety.
36 Relationships, involvement, and well-being
Mirroring
Mirroring is a key learning medium at this early stage of development, and
infants will copy, and assimilate, a parent’s interpretation and emotional
reaction to circumstances or events. A mother’s interpretative signals will
incorporate her personal comprehension allied with historical life influences
and encompass her parenting goals and attempts to guide her infant. Bowlby
(1979) applied the term of “inner working model”, which is a framework of
reference for the operational skills and executive functioning of human
beings, based upon prior experiences and interpretations of the world. The
inner working model is influenced by a cultural context that reflects family or
community attitudes, in addition to parental values and beliefs that support
decision-making.
Marked mirroring, as described previously, is a term that is applied to this
context. A primary carer may briefly reflect her infant’s emotion then present
a representation that supports containment of his negative emotions. The
adult is momentarily experiencing the infant’s negative emotion and activat
ing his or her own regulation skills to react resiliently. This experiential
learning scenario can occur many times within a daily lifestyle at home or
within services. Fonagy (1999) describes this period as supportive of symbol
formation. The infant is given information to extend his understanding of
internal self and external representation in this intersubjective process with a
primary carer. Comprehension matures over time, and meaning gains com
plexity as multiple experiences are presented to the infant. The caregiver’s
response to the child is assimilated with a representation of his physical,
emotional, and mental state. This knowledge is stored within the infant’s
inner working model and will be refined, consolidated, or reconfigured over
time.
Sensitive responding
Training for practitioners in the early years, and parenting programmes, have
developed rapidly over the past two decades to incorporate knowledge and
understanding of therapeutic support. This approach specifically targets the
infant/child’s emotional state and coping mechanisms (Bratton et al., 2006;
Solihull Approach Parenting Group Research, 2009). The majority of parents
will soothe their child effectively and support his emotional regulation. Fur
ther positive influences will be gained from a key worker within an early years
setting. During these processes, the adult uses containment and supports an
infant to acknowledge his emotions, to reflect, and to gain strategies that
lessen negative impacts. The infant acquires responses that are embedded
within his family culture of values, attitude, and beliefs. The young child will
identify feelings and actions with experiences and eventually associate with
his regulatory responses. The brain accumulates patterns through the creation
of neural networks. These patterns can be regarded as providing a shortcut to
Relationships, involvement, and well-being 37
infants in their use of protective strategies and resilience that minimises the
influence of adversities.
The proximal world of an infant is usually quite narrow, particularly in the
earliest days and months of life. The infant’s response time to access coping
strategies increases as he matures and circumstances and events are experi
enced on multiple occasions. A sensitive caregiver will instinctively contribute
to the child’s intentional mental agency by providing a rationale for his emo
tions and actions, particularly within daily care tasks. An example is gained
from the process of nappy changing. An adult responds to the child’s emo
tional and physical discomfort by presenting a practical solution, thus sup
porting comprehension of intentional mental agency. The child’s teleological
understanding of a wet sensation followed by parent interaction, and ulti
mately a dry sensation, is enriched through the merging of his associative
emotions and communication strategies. As he matures, the infant begins to
use his help-seeking skills proactively in response to his needs. Help-seeking
through targeted communication is a key milestone for babies, and there are
many opportunities within each day and night for an infant to source
responses to his basic physical needs. Parents rapidly gain expertise in inter
preting these signals from their baby.
If parents do not demonstrate sensitive responding to the infant, then his
sense of self can be adversely affected. Exposure to harm generally activates
the attachment system, and infants may seek out physical comfort from
caregivers who are abusers. Chaotic disorganised attachment is created. A rise
in cortisol levels, as a result of this negative parent-to-child interaction, can
result in neuro-developmental abnormalities. The brain’s architecture is affec
ted by negative caregiving patterns over time.
A child who is vigilant due to circumstances of perceived danger may
demonstrate an increased ability to mentalise his parent’s behaviour. This
process occurs within, and contributes to, an insecure attachment relationship
between child and parent. The infant inherently strives to gain comprehension
of his world and to gain a sense of predictability, despite chaotic living con
ditions. An infant may adopt his parent’s negative reactions and assimilate
this image internally as his own sense of self. The process results in the child
experiencing vulnerability and high anxiety even if he is removed from the
source of adversity. His negative representation of himself is stored internally,
and led by implicit memories; therefore, the actual source of adversity does
not need to be present.
Fonagy (1999) also suggested that children who experienced adversities in
early childhood may reject mentalisation of their caregiver’s actions. This strat
egy can provide the child with emotional protection in a context of perceived
harm. Alternatively, a mother may not have the capacity to reflect upon her
infant’s mental state, and instead she dissociates herself from his cries and dis
comfort. This reaction creates a barrier to the child’s understanding of himself
through his mother’s representation. Inability to use reflective capacity can result
in an unstable sense of self in parents and children.
38 Relationships, involvement, and well-being
It is thought that negative memories, in a context of childhood abuse, can
preside over positive experiences and thereafter increase likelihood of mental-
health issues. McCrory et al. (2017) reported that any alteration to threat,
reward, and memory-processing could impact upon socialising ability and
emotional reactions. These effects could be apparent throughout a lifespan as
reaction to new stress is exacerbated.
Transitions
Transitions are particularly significant points of contact. For example, the trau
matised child is entering a service with current experiences of adversity from the
home environment affecting his body and mind. A practitioner can set the scene
for a child to experience a safe and stimulating session by presenting a positive
empathic and nurturing approach. The adult–child relationship supports the
child to interpret the physical, emotional, and social learning environment as
non-threatening. Short-term negative interactions between peers occur within an
early years environment on a daily basis as incremental steps towards social
development. For example, sharing and negotiating the use of toys provides
numerous opportunities for the practitioner to use the secure attachment rela
tionship to promote positive behaviour and to nurture the child’s physiological
and emotional regulation.
Children are able to compartmentalise relationships within different envir
onments, and I have often observed insecurely attached children walking
quickly away from their parents on entering a service building. The child will
focus straight ahead and greet a key worker with positivity. His expectation
during this transition is secure attachment to a practitioner in the service. I
have noticed some children refusing to turn around or to wave goodbye to a
parent despite urgent prompting by the mother or father. Occasionally, a child
puts a hand behind his back to wave but remains focused firmly upon the key
worker and entrance area of the service. It is fascinating to observe these
interactions, and practitioners can gain expertise by seeking out theory and
research that extends their comprehension of transitions.
Upon leaving the service, a child will revert to behaviour that he has learned to
use in the company of his parent. Environmental factors prompt memories and
associated behaviour and actions which the young child has learnt to apply over
time. As the proximal context and carers change then the infant demonstrates
patterns of behaviour that are based upon emotions. An increase in under
standing the complexities of human relationships by a primary carer contributes
to application of supportive strategies in response to each child’s needs.
The infant with ambivalent attachment will try a variety of strategies to
gain the attention he desires from his parent, for example, holding tightly,
hiding his face against a parent, crying for long periods of time, which esca
lates into fear as the parent leaves, and an inability to self-regulate his emo
tions and, consequently, his actions. The timescale of this emotional reaction
may be a few minutes or lengthy periods. The high level of anxiety and the
Relationships, involvement, and well-being 39
direct expression of emotion and needs will subside, and the exhausted child may
revert to “still sitting” and non-interaction with the learning environment. The
infant or young child’s sense of self has been affected negatively. His actions have
failed to achieve his desired outcome. Alternatively, this infant may demonstrate
aggression towards his parent by rejecting her overtures, removing eye contact,
hitting, kicking, and crying angrily. He is expressing negative emotions that are
not being met. The infant has an inherent need for consistency and predictability
of surroundings and relationships.
Over time, the infant’s attachment cues will reduce dramatically if he is not
responded to, and avoidant attachment will be demonstrated. The infant will
stop attempting to develop an attachment relationship, may appear placid,
may remove eye contact from others, may limit his physical explorations, and
may begin to demonstrate introverted behaviour. This infant may appear
more interested in his own body and clothing as opposed to stimulation
within the environment or relationships with others.
Children who have chaotic attachment may experience inconsistent care
and neglect that usually includes direct and indirect abuse. Procedural
memory, as unconscious memory, is the way in which the child develops
expectations of relationships with caregivers which are associated with his
patterns of behaviour. These representations can be changed through formal
intervention and informal responsive support from an adult, sibling, or peer
who nurtures a positive relationship with a child.
The assessment of attachment between parent and child is a challenging area as
demonstration of a relationship by a parent and child is influenced by personalities
and the culture of a family. Ainsworth established a method of measuring attach
ment which continues to be used for children aged 12 months. The “Strange
Situation” has a focus upon behaviours associated with the stage in which a
mother returns to her child after a short absence (Ainsworth et al., 1978). Inter
actions prior to this age can be measured with the Parent–Infant Observation
Scale, the Keys to Interactive Parenting, and the CARE-Index. The Keys to
Interactive Parenting review 12 behaviours including limits and consequences
(Comfort et al., 2011). The CARE-Index places emphasis upon three aspects of
the mother’s behaviour towards her child: sensitivity, control, and unresponsive
ness, in addition to four aspects of the infant: cooperativeness, compulsivity, diffi
culties, and passivity (Crittenden, 1985, Crittenden et al., 1991).
Intervention
Howe (2005) identified four different rationale that encompass intervention
approaches:
These first three principles present definitive tasks to the early years practi
tioner from a strategic perspective and practical skill in building relationships
for a purpose. It is important to be prepared for a meeting with family
members. There have been occasions when I have forgotten to set up the
meeting space, and I recall in Box 2.1 a recent example.
The final three of O’Leary’s principles (O’Leary, 2012) reflect aims for the
practitioner to achieve through body language, attitude, and a mindful pre
sence towards interpretation and responses of others.
showing the pram, bags, bottles, and baby’s clothing to practitioners and
arranges these artefacts deftly and with pre-planned precision. This young
adolescent gives little attention to her baby’s emotional state as he copes
with the influx of new faces in the vicinity of his world and comfort base, the
pram.
His needs at this point in time, and characteristics as a unique and
responsive human being, are unheeded by his primary carer. I notice the
mother lifting the baby out of the pram and placing him across her shoulder.
She automatically faces him away from her view and towards the busy sti
mulation of urban life. This teenage parent straightens her son’s clothing and
realigns the presentation of her own outfit as a reaction to the baby wrig
gling his body in her arms. Eye contact from her baby is not sought by this
mother, verbal interaction, and reassurance, supplemented by face-to-face
nurturing, and mingling her smell to this little infant’s sleep scent is not an
observable action during the encounter. Opportunities for secure attachment
are available, multifold, but remain disconnected.
� Threatening: the parent views the baby from above his body and face,
which blocks the light and creates an intensive looming presence.
� Dissociative: the parent uses an unusually weak voice which is different to
regular communication.
� Deferential or timid: the parent uses a manner of communication that
does not instil confidence in a baby, usually through physical interactions.
� Disrupted: the parent does not repair the relationship following periods
of rupture.
� Affective communication errors: inappropriate and unpredictable reac
tions from the parent to the infant’s emotions.
The research by Davis and Montag (2019) described the use of emotions in
mammals as fulfilling the need for survival, to nurture young, and to encou
rage participation and interaction with a stimulating learning environment.
Early experiences create patterns for the creation of future relationships and a
foundation for self-regulation of emotions and behaviour. Sroufe et al. (2000)
linked effective self-regulation in adulthood with experience gained within an
adult–child dyad during the earliest years.
Barlow and Svenberg (2009) highlighted that a negative impact upon an
infant can be expressed through neglect or intrusion by a mother who is experi
encing depression and low mental health. The infant experiences disruption
periods of inconsistent attention and lack of attention, and there may also be
periods of positive interaction with a primary carer. These babies may develop
insecure attachment in the early stages of life and learn to up-regulate in a bid to
gain attention and down-regulate as a self-protection strategy over time. The
babies acquire behaviours in response to their mothers’ changing moods, abil
ities, and capacity to provide care.
Research has indicated that a parent’s mind-mindedness, as used by Barlow
and Svenberg (2009) in depicting a parent’s sensitive responding to her child’s
needs and emotions, is a more effective predictor of developmental rate than
educational background or socio-economic status. Beebe and Steele (2013)
54 Relationships, involvement, and well-being
identified that the infant’s experiences guide his perception of himself, in
addition to development of strategies to deal with negative emotions, and this
is influenced by interactions with his primary carer. In a context of danger,
the young child may be hypervigilant and more acutely aware of his mother’s
cues in relation to his internal mental state. Fonagy (1999) describes a mother
presenting her child with a particular mental state through actions and beha
viour in response to his needs. This author regarded reflective capacity as a
transgenerational acquisition.
It is also indicated that the infant in adverse circumstances has greater
capacity to interpret the mental state of his primary carer than his own
mental state. A mother may reflect, and consequently exacerbate, the negative
emotions of the infant or ignore his state of stress. The study by Cassidy and
Mohr (2006) indicated a status which was termed as unsolvable fear. The
authors expressed that it appeared impossible for an infant to develop orga
nised attachment responses within a context of a chronic source of fear.
Cassidy (1994) explored the close links between attachment relationships and
the ability and capacity to regulate emotions in infanthood. Infants who have
avoidant attachment, through rejection by a primary carer, will minimise emo
tional impact by actively reducing their relationship cues as a protective strategy.
These actions are described by Cassidy as contributory to maintaining the par
ent’s own state of mind in which the need for attachment is minimised. Alter
natively, infants who have developed ambivalent attachment through
intermittent availability of a primary carer will use a variety of means to capture
attention. These actions represent the infant’s need for an attachment relation
ship and directly influence the parent’s responses to attachment cues.
Underdown (2009) identified touch as a powerful and effective tool in creating
and sustaining a secure attachment relationship between mother and child. This
author indicated that lack of tactile connection adversely affected emotional
development and physical health and growth. The use of infant massage, as a
learned intervention, has increased greatly since McClure founded the Interna
tional Association of Infant Massage in the United States (McClure, 1985). Infant
massage reduces and stabilises cortisol, epinephrine, and norepinephrine, which
relate to the level of stress experienced by a baby or child (Underdown, 2006).
A child with disorganised attachment may experience historical ideas and
emotions with the same intensity as current external events. Parents often express
that their children “are reliving past experiences” in a context of trauma and
child abuse. Practitioners observe children in nurseries immersed in thought and
outwardly exhibiting signs of internal stress, for example, sucking clothing,
twisting hands together, biting lips, or clenching fists. Insecure relationships
during infancy may lead to personality distortions in later stages of development
as described by Fonagy (1999).
An interesting study by Witherington and Crichton (2007) focuses upon the
functionalist approach to emotions. These authors describe the emotion system as
a complex nonlinear system that functions in accordance with interaction of the
components. Development in one area can alter the system, for example, motor
Relationships, involvement, and well-being 55
skill and the accompanying emotions. The authors indicate that behaviour by an
infant is influenced by emotional reaction and used to establish, to maintain, or to
change the relationship with a proximal environment. An example of a new skill is
crawling and venturing beyond the Circle of Security in relation to a primary
carer. This gross motor skill may incur fear or excitement and achievement; thus,
the emotion system is reconfigured based upon an increase in knowledge, under
standing, and potentially new interpretation of the world. The reconfiguration
reflects an impact of emotions as the infant explores, learns, and develops.
A recent publication from the UK Government (2021) describes an
impactful intervention as the Healthy Child Programme in England. It is a
national public-health framework for children and young people. Health
promotion is available from birth to 19 years, or 25 if the young person has
particular health needs. Antenatal, newborn, and infant screening for health
issues are also encompassed within the delivery of this programme. NHS
England provide a postnatal check for new mothers and their babies at 6 to 8
weeks, which includes mental health and well-being of the mother.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of
attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Asmussen, K., Fischer, F., Drayton, E., & McBride, T. (2020). Adverse childhood
experiences: What we know, what we don’t know, and what should happen next.
https://www.eif.org.uk/report/adverse-childhoodexperiences-what-we-know-what-we
dont-know-and-what-should-happen-next.
Association of Infant Mental Health, UK. (2021). The infant mental health compe
tency framework. https://aimh.uk/the-uk-imh-competency-framework.
Barlow, J., & Svenberg, P. O. (2009). Keeping the baby in mind. In J. Barlow & P. O.
Svanberg (Eds.), Keeping the baby in mind: Infant mental health in practice (pp. 1–
16). Hove: Routledge.
Beebe, B., & Steele, M. (2013). How does microanalysis of mother–infant commu
nication inform maternal sensitivity and infant attachment? Attachment & Human
Development, 5(5–6),583–602.
Bomber, L. M. (2007). Inside I am hurting: Practical strategies for supporting children
with attachment difficulties in schools. London: Worth.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. Abingdon and New
York: Routledge.
Boyce, W. T., Levitt, P., Martinez, F. D., McEwen, B. S., & Shonkoff, J. P. (2021).
Levaraging the biology of adversity and resilience to transform pediatric practice.
Pediatrics, 147(2): e20201651.
Bratton, S. C., Landreth, G. L., Kellam, T., & Blackard, S. R. (2006). Child/parent
participation therapy treatment manual. Abingdon and New York: Routledge.
Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences of attachment relationships. Monograph
Social Residential Child Development, 59 (2–3),228–249.
Cassidy, J., & Mohr, J. (2006). Unsolvable fear, trauma, and psychology: Theory,
research and clinical considerations related to disorganised attachment across the
life span. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8(3), 275–298.
56 Relationships, involvement, and well-being
Comfort, M., Gordon, P. R., & Naples, D. (2011). KIPS: An evidence-based tool for
assessing parenting strengths and needs in diverse families. Infants and Young Chil
dren, 24(1), 56–74.
Condon, J. T., & Corkindale, C. (2011). The correlates of antenatal attachment in
pregnant women. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 70(4), 359–372.
Cooper, G., Hoffman, K., & Powell, B. (2016). The Circle of Security. https://www.
circleofsecurityinternational.com/circle-of-security-model/wha
t-is-the-circle-of-security.
Crittenden, P. M. (1985). Maltreated infants: vulnerability and resilience. Journal of
Child Psychology and Development, 26, 85–96.
Crittenden, P. M., Partridge, M. F., & Claussen, A. H. (1991). Family patterns of
relationship in normative and dysfunctional families. Development and Psycho
pathology, 3(4), 491–512.
Cross, D., Fani, N., & Powers, A. (2017). Neurobiological development in the context
of childhood adversity. Clinical Psychological Science Practice, 24, 111–124.
Davis, K. L., & Montag, C. (2019). Selected principles of Pankseppian affective neu
roscience. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12(1025).
Department for Children, Schools and Families. (2010). Breaking the link between
disadvantage and low achievement in the early years: Everyone’s business. Notting
ham: Department for Children, Schools and Families.
Department of Health and Social Care. (2021). The best start for life: A vision for the
1001 critical days. Policy paper. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/the
best-start-for-life-a-vision-for-the-1001-critical-days.
Douglas, H., & Rheeston, M. (2009). The Solihull Approach: An integrative model
across agencies. In J. Barlow & P. O. Svanberg (Eds.), Keeping the baby in mind:
Infant mental health in practice (pp. 29–39). Hove: Routledge.
Doyle, C. (2001). Surviving and coping with emotional abuse in childhood. Clinical
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 6 (3). London: Routledge.
Edelman, L. (2004). A relationship-based approach to early intervention. http//www.
cde.state.co.us/earlychildhoodconnections/Technical.htm.
Farmer, E., & Owen, M. (1995). Child protection practice: Private risks and public
remedies. London: HMSO.
Fonagy, P. (1999). Transgenerational consistencies of attachment: A new theory. Dallas
Society for Psychoanalytic Psychology. http://www.dspp.com/papers/fonagy2.htm.
Fukkink, R. G. (2021). Exploring children’s wellbeing in day-care: How do children
feel all day? European Early Childhood Education Research Journal. https://doi.org/
10.1080/1350293X.2021.2007971.
Geddes, H. (2006). Attachment in the classroom: The links between children’s early
experience, emotional wellbeing, and performance in school. London: Worth.
Heath, H. (2004). Assessing and delivering parent support. In M. Hoghughi & N.
Long (Eds.), Handbook of parenting theory and research for practice (pp. 315–322).
London: Sage.
Howe, D. (2005). Child abuse and neglect: Attachment, development, and intervention.
Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Kondo, M. A., & Hannan, A. J. (2019). Environmental stimulation modulating the
pathophysiology of neurodevelopmental disorders. In L. M. Oberman & P. G.
Enticott (Eds.), Neurotechnology and Brain Stimulation in Pediatric Psychiatric and
Neurodevelopmental Disorders (pp. 31–54). Elsevier Academic Press.
Relationships, involvement, and well-being 57
Laevers, F. (1994). Defining and assessing quality in early childhood education. Leuven:
Leuven University Press.
Levine, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Feldman, R., & Weller, A. (2007). Oxytocin during
pregnancy and early postpartum: individual patterns and maternal-fetal attachment.
Peptides, 28(6), 1162–1169.
Lyons-Ruth, K. (2003). The two-person construction of defenses: Disorganized …
/helpless relational processes. Journal of Infant Child and Adolescent Psychotherapy
2(4), 105–114.
Maslow, A. (2000). Maslow’s hierarchy of needs. Encyclopedia of Personality and
Individual Differences. https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Maslow%E2%80%
99s-Hierarchy-of-Needs-Maslow/7eba323b291188524755a7cc0bda63aaba9c5abd.
McClure, V. (1985). Infant massage: A handbook for loving parents. New York: Bantam
Books.
McCrory, E. J., Gerin, M. I., & Viding, E. (2017). Childhood maltreatment, latent
vulnerability, and the shift to preventative psychiatry: The contribution of func
tional brain imaging. Journal of Child Psychology, 58(4), 338–357.
Miell, D. (1995). The development of self. In P. Barnes (Ed.), Personal, social and
emotional development of children (pp. 190–201). Blackwell: Open University.
O’Leary, C. J. (2012). The practice of person-centred couple and family therapy.
Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Royal College of Midwives (2020). Parental emotional wellbeing and infant develop
ment. https://www.rcm.org.uk/media/4645/parental-emotional-wellbeing-guide.pdf.
Solihull Approach Parenting Group Research. (2009). Solihull approach parenting group
research and NICE guidelines. https://solihullapproachparenting.com/research.
Sroufe, A., Duggal, S., Weinfield, N., & Carlson, E. (2000). Relationships, develop
ment, and psychopathology. In A. J. Sameroff, M. Lewis, & S. M. Miller Handbook
of developmental psychology, 2nd edition (pp. 75–91). New York: Kluwer Academic/
Plenum Publishers.
Svanberg, P. O., & Barlow, J. (2009). Developing infant centred services: The way
forward. In J. Barlow & P. O. Svanberg (Eds.), Keeping the baby in mind: Infant
mental health in practice (pp. 185–198). Hove: Routledge.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The human adaptation for culture. Annual Review Anthro
pology, 28, 509–529.
Trevarthen, C. (2001). Intrinsic motives for companionship in understanding: Their
origin, development, and significance for infant mental health. Infant Mental Health
Journal, 22(1–2),95–131.
Underdown, A. (2006). Young children’s health and well-being. Maidenhead: McGraw
Hill/Open University Press.
Underdown, A. (2009). Keeping the baby in mind. In J. Barlow & P. O. Svanberg (Eds.),
Keeping the baby in mind: Infant mental health in practice (pp. 17–28). Hove: Routledge.
UNICEF. (2021). Baby friendly initiative. https://www.unicef.org.uk/babyfriendly/baby
friendly-resources/implementing-standards-resources/skin-to-skin-contact.
Witherington, D. C., & Crichton, J. A. (2007). Frameworks for understanding emotions
and their development: Functionalist and dynamic systems’ approaches. Emotion, 7(3),
628–637.
3 The developing child
Attainment gap
I consider an infant’s chronological age and his stages of development, which are
led by interaction of gene potential and environmental factors. This knowledge
identifies contributory factors to the expansion of an attainment gap between the
average child and disadvantaged children. Babies and toddlers who attend my
workplace struggle to cope with daily survival, and withdrawal symptoms are
common from birth to 18 months due to parental drug or alcohol use. These
adversities can be experienced directly from mother to foetus or indirectly from
both parents using drugs within the home environment. Asmussen et al. (2020)
recently expressed that further research is required to gain understanding of the
social processes that prolong the impact of adversities, in addition to the emo
tional aspects that increase a child’s resilience. All topics are complimentary to
one another within the context of infant mental health.
DOI: 10.4324/9781003358107-3
The developing child 59
Research indicates that babies can experience internal states in relation to
seeking support, rage, fear, pain, loss, play, and care. These emotions con
tribute to the infant’s or child’s developing sense of self over time. Activation
of a baby’s emotions results in physiological change, for example cortisol
release, and subsequently impacts upon behaviour. An infant’s interpretation
of the world can be led by the primary carer’s reaction, or the key worker’s, in
the circumstances of a child attending services. Young children’s emotional
literacy can easily be supported within daily living. The early stages of self-
regulation can be initiated through sensitive care that supports up-regulation
and promotes a pathway to down-regulation if responses are paced appro
priately to personality and needs. Therapeutic intervention can be used to
identify and to respond to developmental gaps by increasing knowledge and
skills of the child, parent, and practitioner. I have always taught the concepts
of self and agency in partnership, which increases understanding of these
issues to practitioners.
The term “marking”, in relation to mirroring, refers to a parent, or practi
tioner in a service, responding to a child by copying his emotions initially and
subsequently presenting an interpretation of a situation for the child to
assimilate. By using this approach, an adult can nurture a child’s skill of
independent emotional containment. Infants reflect the emotions of a parent
which can be prohibitive of positive emotional development if the parent is
suffering from short- or long-term mental-health issues.
Murad makes a plaintive whimper of fear, his cry is directed randomly out
wards as he is unable to see his mother seated within the nurture corner. The key
worker smiles and quickly gestures to Ebi to approach her son. The induction
forms are deprioritised in preference to the infant’s needs. Ebi kneels beside
Murad. Her large mother-and-baby bag slides forward from her shoulder, and
impatiently she pushes it aside. This mother has one focus only at this moment:
responding to Murad’s emotion. I notice that momentarily Ebi mimicked the
infant’s distressed face, and she acknowledged her son’s angst by expressing
his negative emotion to him verbally. However, a reassuring hand upon his back,
a mother’s broad smile, nodding with positivity, and using motherese to create a
non-threatening atmosphere supported Murad to regain his composure.
In a few moments, this little child leant against his mother, his mouth
uplifted in a smile, and he nodded up and down as his relaxing body
demonstrated liberation from potential threat and fear. I commented to this
mother on the effect of her skills, I described her actions, and I linked to
Murad’s self-esteem and regaining of his composure: marked mirroring. Ebi
shrugged as she looked down shyly, seemingly embarrassed to receive my
recognition. I said her name clearly, and, as this young mother looked up, I
reiterated my message, “Ebi, you are the expert on your child’s needs. I
want to learn from you on how to support Murad to achieve.” This time Ebi
nodded, and I could see that she felt pleased and perhaps empowered.
Similar momentary incidents occur frequently within every early years ser
vice and in a home environment. I often observe marked mirroring from
mother to child upon public transport on my journey home from work. I feel
strongly that practitioners and parents should be given awareness of the value
of their input to a distressed infant. The interaction may appear to be insig
nificant, and a natural response, but it contributes to an essential foundation
for development of a sense of self and regulation of emotions. Senior staff and
key workers should gain confidence in noticing and tracking each other’s
practice and by reaffirming through verbal recognition. Early years practi
tioners should capitalise upon opportunities to give parents positive feedback
on parenting skills, within and out-with interventions.
Trauma impacts upon the comprehension of one’s own mind and subse
quently upon the capacity to appreciate the mind of a baby. Mirroring without
marking can also occur between parent and child. A mother who has unresolved
trauma replicates and emphasises the infant’s own reaction without emotional
containment. The infant is not presented with an understanding of his emotions
in relation to context or supported to develop resilience, or to adopt social
behaviours. Alternatively, the parent’s own emotion which is based upon implicit
memories of historical trauma may be communicated and adopted by the infant.
The former response can prolong the trauma reaction, and the latter contributes
to the baby’s creation of a false sense of self.
The developing child 61
An infant can develop a false sense of self by referring to interpretation of
the world from another person’s perspective and by adopting their emotional
reactions. His representation of the world is a direct copy from his role model,
as opposed to his emotions and behaviour being influenced by experiential
learning. At times, parents may contribute inadvertently to this dependency
by directing the child’s actions and dismissing his cues. Montessori (1964)
promoted the key-worker system in services and elevated the significance of
the role of primary carer to secure attachment figure in the earliest years of
childhood. Siraj-Blatchford et al. (2003) researched the effects from multiple
carers on children’s development in a long-term study called The Effective
Provision of Pre-school Education Project (Department for Education and
Skills, 2004). Findings reported positive effects from the consistent care of a
key worker and professional–parent relationships. This approach to nurturing
children in services continues throughout the world. A responsibility of the
key worker is promotion of each infant’s sense of self and agency.
The use of containment encompasses the delivery of intervention in a ser
vice (Bratton et al., 2006) and supports the skill of reflective functioning. It is
essential that practitioners communicate comprehension of these processes to
each parent in response to individual modes of learning, for example, by
using video feedback to prompt discussion and reflection upon the parent’s
actions and baby’s responses. The aforementioned child–parent relationship
therapy is filmed at each session and used to increase understanding of parent
and practitioner.
Many countries have placed an increase in emphasis on supporting practi
tioners, in a breadth of disciplines, to gain knowledge and practical expertise in
the field of infant mental health. My workplace is informed by the recent Scot
tish framework for practice that promotes understanding from the impact of
trauma upon neurological, biological, psychological, and social development
(Scottish Government & NHS Scotland, 2021). There are five key principles:
A sense of self
By five years of age, most children have developed an elementary sense of the
autobiographical self by experiencing five consecutive stages of learning
(Whitters, 2018).
62 The developing child
1 Physical agency.
2 Social agency.
3 Teleological agency.
4 Intentional agency.
5 Representational agency.
degree motor skill is achievable. Xiang’s memory was certainly active, and
her neural connections were rapidly creating a vivid picture to inform her
inner working model. A smile enveloped Xiang, and her head bobbed up and
down as she demonstrated great motivation to engage with the learning
environment of her nursery playroom and to commence a relationship with
her key worker.
Parenting role
Belsky (1984) identified three domains which affect the parenting role.
The developing child 65
1 The psychological resources of each adult.
2 Personality and particular characteristics of each child.
3 The environmental context of stress and the minimising or maximising
factor of support.
This author expressed that the capacity and ability of the parent is the most
significant factor. It is often the case that concerns about attachment are
expressed as a child not having secure attachment to an adult. Each infant
has an inherent predisposition to seek out positive relationships with a pri
mary carer from birth, but attachment requires a dyad in which an adult
responds to a baby’s emotional and physical needs and provides the optimum
conditions for development. These conditions provide nurture and encourage
the baby’s overtures for a supportive relationship. Insecure attachment is
exhibited by an inability to self-regulate; therefore, the accompanying condi
tions must change in order to promote agency and a sense of self.
Social learning through copying the role model of primary carers can be
described as a key source of children’s behaviour. Parenting programmes
focus upon the promotion of positive behaviour by giving parents under
standing of their negative responses and the impact upon the child’s learning
and development. Positive strategies can be implemented with optimum value
if the parent’s psychological resources are used to increase his or her capacity
and ability to fulfil the parenting role. A parent’s experience is founded upon
his or her own childhood, in addition to parenting skills that accumulate
through caring for several children in a family. A child’s additional support
needs can present a new experience and challenges to the parent’s executive
functioning as she learns to interpret and to respond the child’s understanding
of the world. Practitioners should support parents to use prior knowledge
with confidence in addition to embracing learning opportunities and new
parenting skills.
Environmental enrichment is a term that describes intervention in the form of
stimulation and opportunities for development which are directly targeted to
individual children or families. The ecological systems theory (Bronfenbrenner,
1979) portrays the potential of influences to impact upon the developing child by
presentation in the form of concentric circles. The micro-system, meso-system,
exo-system, and macro-system are composed of influences from different sources
that can impact directly and indirectly upon the child’s ability and capacity to
progress. Although plasticity is regarded as being most relevant to the critical
periods of learning in the earliest years, factors can continue to shape neural
connections throughout a lifespan.
Toxic stress can affect the architecture of the brain, and this finding is sig
nificant to early intervention work and the necessity of supports by multi-
agencies. I find that sources of stress are often described by parents as envir
onmental and circumstantial, for example poor housing, poverty, and neigh
bourhood discrimination. Alleviation of these factors does not necessarily
result in a reduction of toxic stress. Implicit memories which are based upon
66 The developing child
emotions can reproduce the stress reactions although the original source is
minimised. Parents and grandparents will frequently, and emotively, recount
historical instances of sexual abuse to service-providers and exhibit emotions
from a childlike perspective.
It is challenging as a practitioner to respond with the optimum support as
disclosures usually occur in unexpected circumstances. For example, within my
workplace the cloakroom area is a regular discussion venue for parents to exhibit
help-seeking behaviours to professionals (Broadhurst, 2003; Braun et al., 2006).
Since the COVID-19 restrictions to building access have been in place, parents
will frequently discuss issues in the carpark, which is the designated area for
drop-off and pick-up between parent and service-provider (Scottish Govern
ment, 2020). Awareness of potential data confidentiality breaches can impose
pressure on practitioners during such encounters in public spaces; however, par
ents’ overtures should not be rejected. Ingenuity in communication media with
families has been necessary during this uncertain period of the pandemic.
Therapeutic relationship
Hogg and Warne (2010) conducted a study on the responses to mental health
by lay people. This term, as applied by the authors, referred to members of
the public who did not have a professional designated role in the field of
mental health. Participants in the research included a hairdresser, a parish priest,
and a bar-worker. Responses from people in these roles to service-users, in a
general public domain, had previously been described by Hochschild (1983) as
emotional labour. This concept included management of people’s emotions
alongside tasks associated with a daily role or specific responsibilities. Differ
entiation can be made between the concepts of caring for someone, which
encompasses physical, and environmental care, or caring about someone, which
refers to a carer’s responses to a service-user’s needs and emotional reactions
(Hogg & Warne, 2010).
The study by Hogg and Warne (2010) published interesting findings by
highlighting the significant impact from lay people upon the mental health of
a population. Skills of empathy, sensitive responding, and a non-judgemental
attitude can accompany daily encounters between familiar and unfamiliar
people and contribute effectively to good mental health in a community.
Rogers (1990) had categorically stated that specialist knowledge, which is
based upon professional training, is not required in the creation of a ther
apeutic relationship. The research by Rogers identified that relationships with
friends can have therapeutic qualities which may be exhibited momentarily or
arise periodically throughout relational interactions.
The research by Rogers (1990) also highlighted the longevity and con
sistency which could be achieved in a therapeutic relationship between a
professional and client in a context of mental health. Rogers’ six conditions
are used as a framework for the creation and maintenance of therapeutic
relationships and continue to influence guidance and practice today.
The developing child 67
1 Two people in a psychological contact.
2 One person, a client in a state of incongruence.
3 The second person, a therapist in a state of congruence.
4 The therapist experiences unconditional positive regard for the client.
5 The therapist experiences empathic understanding of the client’s frame of
reference and communicates this to the client.
6 This communication is achieved to a minimal degree.
Rogers (1990) expressed that these six conditions require to exist over a period
of time in order to inform constructive personality change in the client. One of
Rogers’ colleagues, Lyon (2014a, 2014b) summarised conditions on the char
acteristics of an effective teacher. Lyon identified one outcome as the trust that
emerged within practice led by the six conditions. Tausch (2014) was another
colleague of Rogers and Lyon, and his research findings indicated that non-gen
uineness created a relationship of mistrust. Demonstration of these conditions in
practice do not simply affect each dyadic relationship but contribute to a positive
atmosphere that permeates through a service. A practitioner who has knowledge,
understanding, and capacity to use these interpersonal skills is extremely valu
able to a service, particularly if his or her responsibilities encompass a leadership
or supervisory role.
Lyon (2014a) and Rogers (1990) agreed that a challenge for the therapist
was supporting the client to set and to achieve a goal of functioning posi
tively. Lyon (2014a, 2014b) described the process of giving power to a devel
oping person by nurturing his or her ability to operate independently from the
therapist. It seems that power is knowledge and understanding in this context
which leads to availability and extension of choices. This requires each person
to recognise and to make choices that reflect his inner working model of
beliefs, ideas, interests, and standards of living. A professional in early years
care and education may have gained knowledge of mental-health issues
through training, but comprehension and application of a skill set requires
time and experience. However, every newly qualified or experienced early
years practitioner can empower a parent, child, or colleague by generously
sharing expertise, cooperating, collaborating, and nurturing the other person’s
capacity and ability.
Over the past thirty years, there have been several studies conducted that eluci
date the characteristics of a professional and service-user relationship and reflect
the six conditions of Rogers (1990). Smith (1992) conducted a study on the caring
relationship within general nursing, and she clearly identifies the importance of
role-modelling of the therapeutic alliance by a senior staff. The research indicated
that the nursing team followed cues from their ward sister or charge nurse by using
a therapeutic relationship to deliver care to patients. The team also commented on
the benefit to each professional from the positive nurturing atmosphere which was
created throughout the wards.
Stickley and Freshwater (2002) described love with healing potential as
representation of the therapeutic relationship between nurse and patient.
68 The developing child
These authors noted the challenge for nurses in creating a relationship of care
and caring. This two-pronged relationship included delivering intervention
within the parameters of disciplinary role and responsibilities in addition to
responding to a patient’s emotional well-being. Reference is made to Rogers
(1990) and one of his core conditions for a therapeutic relationship: the coun
sellor experiences unconditional positive regard for the client. Nurses encounter
patients who have a broad range of home circumstances, and it is essential that a
non-judgemental professional approach is maintained at all times.
Early years practitioners and parents equate the delivery of care, education,
and nurture with professional love (Whitters, 2019). This is not a replacement for
parental love, which is unconditional between primary carer and child. Profes
sional love is created for a purpose within the specific context of a service. This
purpose broadly relates to holistic development of the child or parent which
includes good mental health. The love projected from the practitioner to the
family is unconditional on an emotional level, but it is formed within conditions
that inherently create boundaries and recognisable parameters to the creation
and maintenance of the relationship. Time, context, environment, and char
acteristics of each person are a few of the factors that define this type of love
between service-provider and service-user. The human emotion of professional
love encompasses a therapeutic connection between two human beings that
portrays respect, and it promotes responsive care in a context of learning and
development (Whitters, 2019). A highly valued and necessary skill is commu
nicating belief in each person that he or she can achieve.
Emotional intelligence was investigated by McQueen (2004) in a literature
review within a context of nursing studies. The use of emotional intelligence was
identified as adoption of strategies to protect the mental health and well-being of
professionals. Strategies included adequate supervision of general, and indivi
dual practice, in addition to periods of reflection and learning time. This author
also promoted the use of emotional intelligence in negotiations within multi
disciplinary teams, in the nursing context. Findings indicated the importance of
emotional labour, but McQueen also warned about the potential for emotional
burnout in a professional if the outlay was prolonged or intensive.
The pedagogical culture of any workplace has an important impact upon
the delivery of a service, which inherently links to the well-being of employees
and their capacity to use emotional labour with emotional intelligence. The
service-provider and service-user relationship is a medium for communica
tion, interaction, promotion of key issues, and transforming knowledge into
understanding regardless of the context being health or care and education in
the early years. Healing of a whole person takes place physically, emotionally,
and socially within a therapeutic alliance. Professional love is demonstrated
within a sensitive and responsive attitude towards an individual’s emotional
needs at a point of time, and it is accompanied by actions that promote
development and minimisation of adversities. An early years worker practises
in a role that encompasses interpersonal and intrapersonal aspects of care
provision. Parents and children may gravitate towards practitioners, ancillary
The developing child 69
staff, or peers who demonstrate skills that match their areas of need. Parents
may also choose to seek support from lay people due to freedom from con
ditions and expectations of these relationships.
Bowles and Jones (2005) discuss the concept of protection time in a context
of mental-health nursing. These authors describe an inbuilt allocation of time
within each shift which could be used to form a therapeutic relationship with
a patient. Early years services are bound by adult–child ratios, but protected
time can be gained through effective deployment of staffing at a local level.
For example, the head of a service or senior practitioner may not be allocated
within these ratios, or absences of children may result in fewer practitioners
required to work directly with a group of children. In my experience, this
protected time between parent and practitioner can also arise unexpectedly
and can be capitalised upon. An example relates to the induction session in
which a new child to the nursery is accompanied by his parent; therefore, he
is not counted in the service adult–child ratios. Parents frequently use this first
contact with a service to seek out emotional support. Information from a
referring agent often provides the focal point of the meeting, and it acts as a
catalyst for a parent to divulge his or her fears, anxieties, adversities, and to
express emotional reactions. An induction session is conducive to a help-
seeking context for a parent. The therapeutic alliance is a significant aspect of
service-delivery, and protected time to support mental health needs should be
given due consideration within health, education, and care services.
Parents may actively reject interaction with professionals or may not be
aware of the route to accessing this support (Rogers & Pilgrim, 1997). In any
service, child protection is paramount, and it is essential that the entire team,
regardless of role, is trained in this area of work and takes responsibility to
share information by following policies and procedures. Parents should
always be aware that information is being shared, to whom, and the reason.
The only caveat to withholding this communication from parents is the
potential for a child to be harmed as a response to data-sharing between
professionals in a child-protection context.
There are many mitigating factors to progress which are unpredictable, for
example, chronic illness of parent or child or a diagnosis of a self-limiting
illness. These barriers to change and development can create undue pressure
on development of parenting skills. Holmes (2014) had promoted acceptance,
courage, and adjustment to daily living as desirable outcomes for parents in
these adverse contexts.
Parenting responses
Holmes (2014) reviewed parenting programmes from around the world and
identified five common approaches within a context of early intervention.
1 Parent and child are supported alongside each other within a programme.
2 Child receives support within a programme.
70 The developing child
3 Parent is the primary participant in a child-focused programme.
4 Parent is primary participant in a generic programme.
5 Parent and child are primary participants in an intervention programme.
daily tasks, students in jeans and college sweatshirts with large, ubiquitous
bags of knowledge slung across their shoulders, and night-shift staff who
were tired and jaded, ready to go home and sleep during daylight hours. As
a childcare worker, I notice children and parents in every context, and my
interest was alerted as a young mother climbed aboard with a buggy.
The time was autumn of 2020 as the COVID-19 pandemic continued to
encompass our world. My fellow travellers wore face-coverings, and we
were separated carefully by definitive signs on each seat that welcomed or
rejected a potential user. We sat quietly, socially distanced, behind, and
diagonally, COVID-safe. This vantage point was useful as I could see the
buggy and occupant clearly. A little girl with masses of dark curls peeked
around her rain cover. I considered her age to be 12 months.
I smiled broadly as a generic introduction to any young child; however,
masks only reveal smiling eyes, and the little girl was too young to interpret
my overture. She frowned and dipped her head back inside the security of
her buggy hood. Disappointed, I glanced out of the dark window and could
only see reflections of streetlights shining against the still water of the river
Clyde. The bus moved smoothly across the bridge, and I returned my gaze
to the bright interior. The scenario had developed. The young mother had
given her child an identity badge. I recognised the health-service colours on
this badge, and I wondered about the mother’s role.
The child was fascinated by this new toy, and it was obvious that her
involvement and well-being were high. I noticed that the little girl had mas
tered the skill of observation and planning. She used two hands, liberating
ten fine motor tools as her fingers felt the webbing on the lanyard. Strangely,
I found myself touching the lanyard of my own identification badge, which
was hidden from view and tucked inside my winter jacket. For a few
moments, I thought about the processes of learning, applicable to all human
beings, young and old. I had followed a basic learning principle of copying
by emulating this little girl’s movements.
The fastening on a lanyard requires opposing forces to release it, although
professionals in public-health fields are usually issued with sophisticated
versions with a quick safety release in case of assault by a client, but the
hard sharp pull required in this context was beyond the capability of a 12
month-old learner.
The girl leant forward, and I had a front-seat row to view the drama. She
rapidly found the plastic clip and momentarily put it into her mouth. I wat
ched the scene unfolding, and I was fascinated by the child’s competent use
of her five senses within this exploratory task. Two hands worked together,
first attempt then one failure, two attempts then success, and the two ends
of the lanyard sprang apart. The child rubbed her curls back and forward
against the buggy in celebration and smiled to herself. Goal achieved!
The developing child 75
Common daily scenes depict the ability and capacity of a child at a point in
time. The motor and coordination skills that this child applied were developed
in situ, and the achievement of the planned outcome demonstrated motiva
tion to learn within the task.
1 Setting goals.
2 Involvement in an activity.
3 Focused attention.
4 High level of intellectual involvement and emotional well-being in current
experience.
Flow theory
Flow theory refers to the influences from two variables upon a child’s experiences.
It is widely accepted that there are three ways in which knowledge is acquired
in the early years. The first approach is an infant observing and copying in
situ. For example, if an adult sticks out his tongue in close proximity to a 6
week-old baby, then he will copy the action as long as the prompt remains
visible. The second approach entails an infant observing and reproducing an
action from memory in the short term. For example, if an adult demonstrates
a practical instruction of putting one brick on top of another before taking
the bricks apart and creating a learning space, then the infant will attempt to
reproduce these actions. The young infant may require reminders of the
actions required to build the tower of bricks if the visual prompt is no longer
available. The final approach is an infant internalising instructions and
applying knowledge, or collaborative learning with peers and adults in a
variety of contexts, and time frames. For example, if an infant attempts to
touch a specific hot surface, the carer will firmly say “No” and actively stop
the child’s hand from completing this action. On subsequent occasions, the
adult will simply say “No” without the practical prompt, and an infant will
internalise representation of this word as cause and effect in the context of the
The developing child 77
aforementioned hot surface. Over time, an infant will acquire the ability to
extrapolate actions and meaning associated with the word “No” to other
similar contexts.
Vygotsky (1978) studied the social nature of learning throughout his career,
and he believed that co-constructivism provided the optimum context in
which potential could be achieved; however, one child may have greater con
trol over environmental change and internal representation than another. An
increase in intrinsic motivation can serve to minimise distractions from
external events or adversities which can be observed in the performance of
some infants. These infants have greater independence and less reliance on the
environment as a prompt for learning. The children require fewer stimuli from
the environment than their peers in order to achieve internal representation of
consciousness in a context of knowledge and understanding. Parents and
practitioners have often commented to me in reference to an independent
young child, “He can find learning opportunities in any situation!”
Alternatively, Csikszentmihalyi (1990) found that greater dependency on
the external environment is required by some individuals to create a repre
sentation of reality. This knowledge of approaches to learning directly
informs practice, and it underlines the importance of planning and of setting
a playroom for infants and young children at different stages of development.
Every practitioner develops skill in presenting choices to children at their
developmental levels. It is always effective practice to have supplementary
items prepared and ready to offer learners who rapidly achieve their goals in
addition to learners who benefit from repetitive interactions with artefacts
relating to lower levels of thinking.
Anxiety and boredom were identified by Csikszentmihalyi (1990) as two main
impediments to enjoyment in life. Anxiety can occur if challenges are perceived as
unachievable, and, conversely, boredom can arise if a child perceives his abilities
as greater than the opportunities that are presented in a particular environment.
Young children may not have an awareness of the reason for feeling boredom but
exhibit unruly behaviour as a response to lack of stimulation from minimal
learning experiences, or restrictive boundaries. The adult should seek to under
stand this medium of communication and to interpret the infant or child’s beha
viour. At times, I have heard carers comment, “There’s no reason for this
disruptive behaviour”, in reference to unruly actions and aggressive tendencies.
Anxiety or boredom often underlie these behavioural communications by a child.
Challenging cognitive tasks or introducing new physical activities are useful
responses. It is not always possible for workers to leave a playroom in order to
bring new artefacts into the play arena, but ingenuity and enthusiasm from a
team can support reconfiguration of resources to stimulate a child. Involving
children in creating a new venture can channel emotions associated with anxiety
or boredom towards emotions linked to achievement of a new task. Actions can
support self-regulation as a child reinterprets his learning environment.
Zhou and Brown (2015) explored three approaches to changing behaviour
which are applicable to all children within the earliest years of childhood: cueing,
78 The developing child
shaping, and modelling. Cueing is the use of verbal or non-verbal prompts that
link specific behaviour to circumstances. Cueing is most effective if used to pre
empt negative behaviours by promotion of a positive change model rather than a
response to a child’s unsociable actions.
A child’s responses can also be shaped by practical scaffolding or verbal
interaction. Shaping entails an educator using strategies to incrementally
support a child to change his behaviour. All the steps may be explained to the
child at the outset of a task and linked to expectations and outcomes. The
child gains awareness of changing his behaviour. Alternatively, the change
process can be determined by an adult introducing each small step in context.
The child may not have an awareness that his behaviour is changing through
these subliminal accumulative steps. This lack of awareness can result in a
child positively embracing a situation of change. If a child perceives that he is
losing control of his environment, then he may rebel against the change
through fear and anxiety associated with an unfamiliar situation.
Modelling is often described as role-modelling within a service in which an
adult or peer actively demonstrates expectations of behaviour to a child
throughout the generic context. Zhou and Brown (2015) make the important
point that modelling can lead to diversion from the original source of learn
ing. For example, a child can produce unique patterns of behaviour as he
attempts to replicate the adult’s actions. The use of child-led pedagogy has
minimised the educator’s focus upon predetermined goals and subsequently
liberated children to demonstrate imaginative and creative skills.
Motivation
The research by Gottfried et al. (1994) referred to intrinsic motivation which
was linked to the content of a task, a child’s personal interests, and the
experience of pleasure in intellectual attainment. The achievement of intel
lectual goals is experienced internally but may be rewarded externally by
teachers or parents which can lead to dependency on external recognition.
This study by Gottfried and his research team had focused upon a child’s
internal reward system and his motivation to seek out higher levels of under
standing. Links were made between intrinsic motivation, giftedness, and
pleasure which were associated with cognitive processing. Findings indicated
that family circumstances, resources, and social status in a community had
positive impact upon cognitive development; however, the study concluded
that familial influences can also hinder or prevent the realisation of genetic
potential.
Twenty-five years later, the same researcher, Gottfried (2019), collected
data on adolescents’ perceptions of their parents’ support during young for
mative years. The adolescent participants expressed that task-intrinsic
approaches by their parents were more effective than task-extrinsic. Findings
indicated that a parent’s affirmation of the child’s internal emotional experi
ence, and the child’s recognition of his goal, supported learning processes.
The developing child 79
The study by Kreppner (2001) had also noted particular responses in rela
tion to mothers of children who had high ability. These mothers adapted their
interactions and information-sharing with their children in accordance with
developing cognitive skills. Additionally, Kreppner found that mothers of
children who had lower abilities tended to interrupt the child’s play and did
not use instructions which reflected his maturation and changing abilities.
This approach restricted the child’s opportunities to access learning experi
ences and to broaden his understanding of the world.
If children gain pleasure and fulfilment during cognitive processing, then
they will develop tendencies to engage with activities and pursuits which fur
ther these outcomes.
Intervention
Stern (2018) reviewed the purpose and effect of interventions which are
delivered in a context of therapeutic support. He described therapeutic inter
vention as building upon a relationship of trust between parent and therapist.
This foundation creates a medium for effective communication and the par
ent’s belief and acceptance in the therapist’s input, which increases the value
of the intervention. Stern also commented that many interventions encompass
a rationale of a strengths-based approach that focuses upon a parent’s current
skill set instead of areas for development. This approach was described by
Stern as superficial interventions of support as opposed to therapies.
Stern (2018) advocated therapies that promote understanding and ultimately
acceptance of past events. One strategy is giving a parent knowledge and a
rationale for using sensitive responding between adult and child. For example,
child–parent psychotherapy assists a family in exposing emotions and reactions
to trauma and gaining resilience to negative impact. Contextualising the nega
tive experiences, in addition to comprehending and accepting the accompanying
80 The developing child
emotions, can support a parent’s ability to change and develop. Over time, par
ents can be supported to reflect upon experiences that change their perceptions
of the parenting role and responsibilities. A therapeutic alliance between thera
pist and parent can contribute to the parent’s capacity to show empathy towards
a child. The therapist’s unconditional positive regard is a key factor in this pro
cess in which the parent’s inner working model is reconfigured, leading to a
change in perceptions, actions, and reactions.
The therapist in child–parent psychotherapy guides these complex processes
by nurturing links between internal beliefs and external behaviours. The term
“port of entry” applied by Lieberman et al. (2019) refers to the use of spon
taneous behaviour, interactions, and free play within the intervention session
to capitalise upon potential for change in parent and child. Personalities,
temperament, and identified clinical issues are often determining factors in
the therapist prioritising a port of entry. Blechman (2016) particularly high
lighted the transition period in which a parent commences the process of
change. During this time, the parent learns to express problems in an abstract
manner and gradually identifies solutions and applies concrete actions within
the circumstances of daily living. Blechman believed that solutions encom
passed overt words and actions, in addition to covert thoughts and feelings of
the individual.
Lieberman et al. (2019) also reviewed family intervention therapy, which
specifically targets areas of change for parents, and children from birth to five
years, by focusing upon negative aspects of family life. Findings indicate that
trauma-informed treatment promotes knowledge and understanding that
supports reconfiguration of the parent’s and child’s inner working models.
Examples of areas for change were given by the researchers as unsafe envir
onmental conditions, mental-health issues that have an adverse impact upon
the parent–child relationship, and maladaptive internal and external parent or
child behaviours. Internal behaviour can include difficulty in recognising and
expressing emotions, which can lead to social withdrawal. Reaction to adver
sities may be demonstrated externally through aggression and a desire for
excessive control of situations.
The rationale of child–parent psychotherapy is presented by the therapist
during play sessions with parent and child. A link between parent’s and
child’s actions and reactions is explored, and the caregiver is supported to
identify negative and ameliorative factors. These factors may be current or
based upon historical experiences. The child and parent learn to put their
trauma experiences into context and to differentiate between remembering
and reliving the events and emotions. The therapist promotes parent and child
engagement with developmental goals and guides the parent to shape a future
positive family life.
Research indicates that the therapeutic alliance is strengthened by the skill
of each therapist in facilitating discussion of traumatic events. This positive
context encompasses change, development, and an increase in mental well
being of child, parent, and extended family (Lieberman et al., 2019). The
The developing child 81
context of an intervention therapy is essential to give the parent and child a
means of controlling the emotional impact of trauma within a safe environ
ment. Families learn to rationalise and to compartmentalise the adversities
within their own time frames. The family members also learn how to create a
new framework for daily living in which negativity is replaced with positivity
and hope for a better future.
I work in a care setting that implements therapeutic pedagogy throughout
all activities and places significance on nurturing practitioner–parent, practi
tioner–child, and parent–child relationships. Families attend the service to
access a nursery placement in addition to parenting support. Referring agents,
for example, social work, addictions, and health professionals, identify the
adversities and the impact upon each member of a family. Formal interven
tions are promoted that support this process of change and development.
However, parents also choose to share trauma experiences informally at times
which are opportune to their emotions, well-being, and understanding of
specific practitioner–parent relationships. These interactions often occur out-
with an allocated intervention session, and it is important that the practi
tioner is equipped to respond sensitively within this moment of personal dis
closure by a parent. In recent years, training in adverse childhood experiences,
neural processes, and trauma-informed practice have become key core com
ponents of continuous professional development for registered practitioners.
Play as therapy
Play that is based upon experiences of trauma provides rich and valid
opportunities to create narratives that respond to emotional issues in a safe
and protected environment. The review by Lieberman et al. (2019) also pro
moted an ongoing narrative by the therapist as an effective strategy to alter
perceptions and interpretation of actions. The play is tracked by the therapist
throughout a session. This specific use of words to express actions and
accompanying emotions has great value in increasing understanding of parent
and child. The therapist encourages the parent to demonstrate nurture and
love through physical contact with a child. I have found that this context can
be challenging for some parents. Role-modelling by a practitioner is a useful
approach to experiential learning for a parent. In every interaction between
adults and children, there are multiple opportunities to demonstrate respect,
care, and love.
In the mid-1960s, a husband-and-wife team of researchers created the filial
therapy model in which recognition is given to parents’ use of play-therapy
principles and skills within adult–child interactive play sessions (Guerney,
1964). This model responded to the mental-health needs of young children
and continues to be implemented today. Adaptation includes the use of a
trained practitioner in the role of facilitator, for example, a key worker within
an early years service (Morrison & Helker, 2010). The therapeutic skills can
be extrapolated beyond the intervention and applied by the facilitator within
82 The developing child
any other environment in which interaction occurs between adult and child
(Post, 2010). This transitional feature of child–teacher relationship training is
supportive for the child’s transference of self-belief, emotional literacy, and
self-regulation within common daily contexts.
Child-centred play therapy is another effective intervention that targets the
mental health of a child and parent (Baggerly, 2010). The child participates in
sessions of interactive play, supported by his parent or trained facilitator. The
conceptual toys stimulate the child’s exploration and increased understanding
of his emotions in relation to trauma. The adult reflects the child’s non-verbal
behaviour, emotions, ability, and capacity to overcome challenges. The ratio
nale of this intervention stems from the belief in the child’s increasing capa
city to gain an understanding of self and to regulate his emotions and actions.
Ray and Edwards (2010) promoted the role of adult as facilitating an envir
onment of permissiveness and freedom to understand and to make choices.
The optimum outcome is the child’s transference of this learning to environ
ments out-with the therapeutic context.
Fall (2010) describes self-efficacy as belief in oneself. It is this belief that
can create reparation links between adversities and a positive fulfilling life.
The reparation element is founded upon an increase in knowledge, under
standing, and resilience, in addition to appointing value and respect to one
self. This belief is necessary for an individual to create internal change and the
confidence to express these changes externally.
Nurturing this belief is a key aspect of trauma-informed practice. Self-belief
can increase dramatically within multiple contexts: formal and informal
intervention and daily living experiences. Short interactions between an
attachment figure and a child can engender self-belief, and the output from an
interaction does not rely upon a specific timescale. Play therapy reinforces
strengths but acknowledges weaknesses. Tracking phrases contribute to the
child’s creation of a trauma and a resilience narrative. Facilitator responses
are built upon principles of acceptance and change.
Fatigue
Professional fatigue is a mental-health issue that has come to the fore in
recent years, and this condition has been highlighted repeatedly by the media
during the current COVID-19 pandemic. However, stress can also be stimu
lating and lead to professionals achieving optimum performance within their
disciplines, as described by Ulrich-Lai and Herman (2009). These authors
identified that stress may represent positive stimuli and the physiological effect
is similar to that of stress that induces negative effects. The research revealed
that delight/elation and fear/terror produced similar biological reactions.
84 The developing child
Additionally, findings indicated that reward behaviours reduced stress
responses by creating physiological and psychological change.
It is well known that healthy eating and regular exercise impact positively
upon long-term stress. During the lockdown periods of 2020 and 2021,
populations throughout many countries were allocated short daily exercise
times outside as a means to improve mental health. There was also a notice
able increase in the public’s engagement with physical activity via indoor
media across all age groups. Throughout our lifespans, the hippocampus
generates neurons. Exercise is linked to this process of neurogenesis (Erikkson
et al., 1998) by generating neurotrophic proteins that are responsible for
growth, maintenance, and survival of neurons. It may be the case that the
increase in society’s focus upon exercise and well-being will continue after the
effects of the pandemic have reduced.
References
Asmussen, K., Fischer, F., Drayton, E., & McBride, T. (2020). Adverse childhood
experiences: What we know, what we don’t know, and what should happen next.
https://members.childlink.co.uk/document/adverse-childhood-experiences-what-we
know-what-we-don%E2%80%99t-know-and-what-should-happen-next?page=3.
Baggerly, J. N. (2010). Preface. In J. N. Baggerly, D. C. Ray, & S. C. Bratton (Eds.),
Child-centred play therapy research: The evidence base for effective practice (pp. xiii–
xviii). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. https://www.idoc.pub/
documents/belsky-1984-determinants-of-parentinga-process-modelpdf-pqn8jpw23141.
Blechman, E. A. (2016). Effective communication: Enabling multi-problem families to
change. In P. A. Cowan & M. Hetherington (Eds.), Family Transitions (pp. 219–
244). Abingdon and New York: Routledge.
Bowles, N., & Jones, A. (2005). Whole systems working and acute inpatient psy
chiatry: An exploratory study. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing 12
(3), 283–289. https://www.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1365-2850.2005.
00834.x.
Bratton, S. C., Landreth, G. L., Kellam, T., & Blackard, S. R. (2006). Child/parent
participation therapy treatment manual. Abingdon and New York: Routledge.
Braun, D., Davis, H., & Mansfield, P. (2006). How helping works: Towards a shared
model of process. London: Centre for Parent and Child Support.
Broadhurst, K. (2003). Engaging parents and carers with family support services:
What can be learned from research on help-seeking? Journal of Child and Family
Social Work, 10. http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1046/j.1365-2206.2003.00289.
x/abstract.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development, 2nd edition. Cam
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York:
HarperCollins.
Department for Education and Skills. (2004). The effective provision of pre-school
education (EPPE) project: The final report. Nottingham: Institute of Education.
The developing child 85
Doyle, C., & Timms, C. (2014). Child neglect and emotional abuse: Understanding,
assessment and response. London: Sage.
Erikkson, P. S., Perfilieva, E., Bjork-Eriksson, T., Alborn, A. M., Nordborg, C.,
Peterson, D. A., & Gage, F. H. (1998). Neurogenesis in the adult human hippo-
campus. Nature Medicine, 4(11), 1313–1317.
Fall, M. (2010). Increased self-efficacy: One reason for play therapy success. In J. N.
Baggerly, D. C. Ray, & S. C. Bratton (Eds.), Child-centred play therapy research:
The evidence base for effective practice (pp. 37–51). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley &
Sons.
Gottfried, A. E. (2019). Academic intrinsic motivation: theory, assessment, and longitudinal
research. Journal of advances in motivation science, (6), 2019, 71–109, https://
www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S221509191830021X
Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (1994). Role of parental motiva
tional practices in children’s academic intrinsic motivation and achievement. Jour
nal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 1040113. https://fosteringintrinsicmotivation.
weebly.com/uploads/2/5/0/3/25031030/role_of_parental_motivational_practices.pdf.
Guerney, B. G. (1964). Filial therapy: Description and rationale. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 28(4), 304–310. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0041340.
Hochschild, A. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialisation of human feeling. Ber
keley, CA: University of California Press.
Hogg, C., & Warne, T. (2010). Ordinary people, extraordinary voices: The emotional
labour of lay people caring for and about people with a mental health problem.
International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 19, 297–306.
Holmes, J. (2014). Where the child is the concern, working therapeutically with par
ents. In P. Holmes & S. Farnfield (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of attachment:
Implications and interventions (pp. 53–64). Hove: Routledge.
Kreppner, K. (2001). Infant education. International Encyclopedia of the Social and
Behavioural Sciences, 7414–7419. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/
B0080430767023809.
Lieberman, A. F., Dimmler, M. H., & Ippen, C. M. G. (2019). Child–parent psy
chotherapy: A trauma-informed treatment for young children and their caregivers.
In C. H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health, 4th edition (pp. 485–499).
New York: The Guilford Press.
Lyon, H. (2014a). Educating the gifted and talented: Freedom to reach your potential.
In C. R. Rogers, H. C. Lyon, & R. Tausch (Eds.), On becoming an effective teacher:
Person-centred teaching, psychology, philosophy, and dialogues with Carl R. Rogers
and Harold Lyon (pp. 47–57). Abingdon: Routledge.
Lyon, H. (2014b). Person-centred management and leadership. In C. R. Rogers, H. C.
Lyon, & R. Tausch (Eds.), On becoming an effective teacher: Person-centred teach
ing, psychology, philosophy, and dialogues with Carl R. Rogers and Harold Lyon
(pp. 95–103). Abingdon: Routledge.
McCoach, D. B., & Flake, J. K. (2018). The role of motivation. In S. I. Pfeiffer (Ed.),
APA handbook of giftedness and talent (pp. 201–213). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
McQueen, A. (2004). Emotional intelligence in nursing work. Journal of Advanced
Nursing, 47(1), 101–108. https://www.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.
1365-2648.2004.03069.x.
Montessori, M. (1964). The Montessori method. New York: Schocken Books. First
published 1912.
86 The developing child
Morrison, M. O., & Helker, W. P. (2010). An early mental health intervention for dis
advantaged preschool children. In J. N. Baggerly, D. C. Ray, & S. C. Bratton (Eds.),
Child-centred play therapy research: The evidence base for effective practice (pp.
427–447). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal
emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Plutchik, R. (1980). A general psycho-evolutionary theory of emotion. In R. Plutchik
& H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, Research, and Experience (pp. 3–33). New
York: Academic Press.
Post, P. (2010). Child-centred kinder training for teachers of preschool children
deemed at risk. In J. N. Baggerly, D. C. Ray, & S. C. Bratton (Eds.), Child-centred
play therapy research: The evidence base for effective practice (pp. 409–427). Hoboken,
NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Ray, D. C., & Edwards, N. A. (2010). Play therapy effect on relationship stress. In J. N.
Baggerly, D. C. Ray, & S. C. Bratton (Eds.), Child-centred play therapy research:
The evidence base for effective practice (pp. 105–125). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley &
Sons.
Rogers, A., & Pilgrim, D. (1997). The contribution of lay knowledge to the under
standing and promotion of mental health. Journal of Mental Health, 6(1). 23–35.
Rogers, C. (1990). Theory and research. In H. Kirschenbaum & V. L. Henderson
(Eds.), The Carl Rogers reader, London: Constable.
Scottish Government. (2020). Early learning and childcare COVID-19 Update, No.
3–9, June. https://www.gov.scot/publications/coronavirus-covid-19-early-learning
childcare-services.
Scottish Government & NHS Scotland. (2021). Trauma-informed practice: A toolkit
for Scotland. https://www.gov.scot/publications/trauma-informed-practice-toolk
it-scotland.
Silverman, L. (2016). Early signs of giftedness. https://www.gifteddevelopment.com.
Siraj-Blatchford, I., Sylva, K., Taggart, B., Melhuish, E., Sammons, P., & Elliot, K.
(2003). The effective provision of pre-school education (EPPE) project (1997–2003).
Nottingham: Department for Education and Skills.
Smith, P. (1992). The emotional labour of nursing. London: Macmillan.
Sroufe, A., Duggal, S., Weinfield, N., & Carlson, E. (2000). Relationships, develop
ment, and psychopathology: Handbook of developmental psychology, 2nd edition.
New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Stern, D. N. (1998). The interpersonal world of the infant. London: H. Karnac.
Stern, D. N. (2018). The motherhood constellation: A unified view of parent–infant
psychotherapy. Abingdon and New York: Routledge.
Stickley, T., & Freshwater, D. (2002). The art of loving and the therapeutic relation
ship. Nursing Inquiry, 9(4), 250–256.
Tausch, R. (2014). Research in Germany on person-centred methods: Teutonic thor
oughness. In C. R. Rogers, H. C. Lyon, & R. Tausch (Eds.), On becoming an effec
tive teacher: Person-centred teaching, psychology, philosophy, and dialogues with
Carl R. Rogers and Harold Lyon (pp. 114–133). Abingdon and New York:
Routledge.
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and
clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(1), 3–48.
Tronick, E., & Beeghly, M. (2011). Infants’ meaning-making and the development of
mental health problems. American Psychologist, 66(2), 107–109.
The developing child 87
Tyndall-Lind, A. (2010). Intensive sibling group play therapy with child witnesses of
domestic violence. In J. N. Baggerly, D. C. Ray, & S. C. Bratton (Eds.), Child-centred
play therapy research: The evidence base for effective practice. Hoboken, NJ: John
Wiley & Sons.
Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., & Herman, J. P. (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and auto
nomic stress responses. Nature Review: Neuroscience 10, 397–409.
Vaivre-Douret, L. (2003). Early developmental characteristics in children with a high
level of potential. Journal français de psychiatrie 1, 1–3. https://www.cairn-int.info/
journal-journal-francais-de-psychiatrie-2003-1-page-I.htm.
Vaivre-Douret, L. (2011). Developmental and cognitive characteristics of “high-level
potentialities” (highly gifted) children. International Journal of Paediatrics, 420297.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/51697095_Developmental_and_Cognitive_
Characteristics_of_High-Level_Potentialities_Highly_Gifted_Children.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: Development of higher psychological processes.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Whitters, H. G. (2018). Family learning to inclusion in the early years. Abingdon and
New York: Routledge.
Whitters, H. G. (2019). Nurture … or is it love? EECERA: European Early Years
Childcare and Education Research Association. https://www.eecera.org/gen/guest
post-nurture-or-love.
Whitters, H. G. (2020). Adverse childhood experiences, attachment, and the early years
learning environment. Abingdon and New York: Routledge.
Zhou, M., & Brown, D. (2015). Educational learning theories, 2nd edition, Education
Open Textbooks, 1. https://oer.galileo.usg.edu/education-textbooks/1.
4 Learning and adversities
Chapter 4 presents the infant and adult mind as a complex system that
encompasses many layers and potential connections (Siegel, 2003). Mental
health is described as a self-organising process that supports an individual to
achieve maximum complexity by use of differentiation and integration.
Uneven adaptation can occur as the mentalising resources are split between
the external world and influences of others and the internal world of the
individual and comprehension of his sense of self (Fonagy, 1999).
Multi-modal approaches are indicated which target different learning styles
of families and can respond sensitively to increasing capacity and ability as
time progresses. Examples of experiences throughout play environments are
linked to activation of the child’s genes. The result is protein production,
which changes the architecture of the brain as new synapses form. Recent
findings of Boyce et al. (2021) referred to individual susceptibility to adversity
and links between unsupportive parenting and increased inflammatory reac
tions throughout the lifespan. Knowledge of the destructive properties of
cortisol is used to inform the rationale for strategic planning and intervention
(National Scientific Council on the Developing Child, 2006).
Targeted interventions
Targeted interventions are available to families who are deemed to be “at
risk” during pregnancy and post-birth. An identified outcome is to support
reconfiguration of the child’s internal working model. Parent–child support
programmes can be effective in enabling the parent to view her child as an
intentional mental agent and promoting the sense of self to the child. Imma
turity in these areas can be exhibited as an unstable sense of self and impul
sivity, which Fonagy (1999) associated with lack of emotional awareness, and
the predominance of physical reactions in response to stress.
These programmes are often implemented through home visiting by health
workers. Indicated interventions can be accessed for women who experience
issues that are detrimental to the mother and unborn baby. Many interven
tions are twofold and support mentalisation and resolving past trauma, in
addition to development of positive parenting, secure attachment, and healthy
lifestyles. A bridge is created between internal and external reality for the
92 Learning and adversities
child, and opportunities for imaginative play, supported by a secure attach
ment figure, can enrich this stage of development.
Researchers from Harvard University recently published an action guide
for policy-makers, in 2020, which indicates three key messages regarding
influences upon child development: adult–child relationships, early experi
ences, and environmental exposure (National Scientific Council on the
Developing Child, 2020). Prior research (Siegel, 2003) highlighted the inter
action between these influences and social-emotional development throughout
the lifespan, specifically from the prenatal period to three years of age. These
influences have a profound impact upon long-term health and well-being as
foundations for healthy functioning systems are established.
A pregnant mother’s stress, lack of nutrition, and environmental exposure can
result in the child having an increased risk of heart disease, obesity, diabetes, and
mental-health conditions. Anxiety, hypervigilance, and depression affect devel
opment and impact upon the emergence of systemic properties, for example,
mobility and communication skills. Chronic negative influences can contribute
to a cycle in which the child’s resilience is reduced and he becomes more sus
ceptible to future adverse influence. Pathological outcomes may result if a child’s
understanding of himself is detrimentally affected (Siegel, 2003).
Siegel (2003) described the mind as a complex system that encompassed
many layers and potential connections which were influenced by genetics, envir
onment, and time. Mental health is regarded as a self-organising process that
supports an individual to achieve maximum complexity. If the system does not
progress towards complexity, then it is regarded as experiencing stress. The
complexity of this system emerges from two fundamental processes, termed
“differentiation” and “integration”. Differentiation relates to specialisation of
components and integration to the forming of a whole system. A functional link
is created by neural integration which occurs as differentiated circuits in the
brain form a coherent information-processing system.
Complexity
Therapeutic relationships, whether in a formal context of psychotherapy or
informal interactions, can enable self-organisation of the mind to progress
towards complexity. Siegel (2003) stated that human minds are created through
neural functioning, and this researcher identified 12 aspects that feature in ther
apeutic relationships and support organisation of the mind: connection, com
passion, contingency, cohesion, continuity, coherence, clarity, co-construction,
complexity, consciousness, creativity, and community. The mind is a sub
jective entity that evolves over the lifespan in accordance with influences from
genetics, environment, relationships, and personality. There are patterns of
flow in the mind that involve energy and information. These patterns can
occur intra-personally and inter-personally. Ultimately, the mind can alter the
brain and the brain alter the mind as a result of information-processing
(Siegel, 2003).
Learning and adversities 93
Neural integration refers to the different elements of the brain, or proces
sing modalities, functioning as a whole entity (Siegel, 2003). As neurons are
activated then patterns of brain activity emerge which are consolidated and
extended during common or repeated experiences. Practitioners will observe
similar actions and emotions in infants during daily drop-off and pick-up
times that become established as routines. Personality and well-being affect
infants to a greater degree than children and adults. As humans mature then
their ability and capacity to overcome personal inclination and to adhere to
social expectations increases.
The young child relies on a consistent secure attachment relationship to direct
his learning. Over a period of time, the child is able to function independently by
referring to memories created by this essential relationship. Informal interactions
occur between parent and child on multiple occasions throughout every day and
night. Practitioners can support parents by highlighting the importance of con
structing communication pathways with a young child to nurture development
and to form secure attachment. Therapeutic progress is often expressed as an
increase in creativity. In nurseries, children may not participate in arts and crafts
activities or imaginative play for several weeks after the induction period. A
child needs time to feel liberated from his perceived emotional and social bar
riers in order to express his inner thoughts and ideas through creative play.
The parent’s own experiences of attachment relationships in early childhood
impact upon the care of subsequent generations. In 2003, a team of researchers
concluded a 23-year longitudinal study, and the findings focused upon a concept
termed “earned secure attachment” (Roisman et al., 2003). This research
explored the potential changes in understanding and demonstration of attach
ment in adults who had described childhood relationships with their parents as
insecure. The descriptor “earned secure” referred to the increase in the adults’
ability and capacity to experience and to express secure attachment with their
own children. This ability was realised for many of the participants, and secure
attachment was established with the second generation.
Brain development includes a process of pruning. This emotive and
descriptive term refers to negative impact upon synaptic growth in which
neural connections may be not form due to minimal or no activation, and
some neurons die. The pruning process is particularly activated during ado
lescent years. Activation is dependent on genetic timing of growth in these
circuits in addition to experience or activity-dependent development.
The infant/child’s interaction with the environment, and relationships, is
influenced by genetic coding and determines developmental pathways.
Memory is a major catalyst to development. For example, a visual/pictorial or
text prompt to the retina activates the optic nerve, and the information tra
verses to the left side of the brain towards the communication-processing
area. Memory is one of the neural processes that benefits from repetition to
increase the likelihood of neurons linking together on more than one occa
sion. Memory can cause prior experiences and accompanying emotions which
are associated with a specific prompt to be recalled and to assimilate with a
94 Learning and adversities
new experience. Siegel (2003) described integration of experiences within one
person’s brain being influenced by the flow from another person’s brain.
Knowledge and understanding of influences upon integration provides a
rationale for implementation of learning being supported by an adult or peer.
Care plans, and regular observation and assessment of each child by a key
worker, facilitate effective curriculum delivery within a service. In addition,
recall is used to support children’s skill in reflecting upon past events, gaining
awareness of memory storage by focusing upon internal thoughts, experien
cing these processes, and expressing personal interpretation. The recall and
discussion of positive experiences creates a useful initial stepping stone to
children accessing memories and understanding their actions and emotions.
One example relates to a child’s memory of nursery activities from the pre
vious day.
Reference to previous experiences provides an opportunity for a carer to
recall the child’s actions in detail and to create a vivid picture by using rele
vant and realistic prompts to support the child’s reflective capacity. Reflection
upon prior experiences which have not been observed by a key worker, for
example a birthday party in the home environment, provide opportunities for
the practitioner to use questions as prompts and to explore the memory
alongside a child. These scenarios appoint control of the situation to a child
and increase his mastery. Conversing with an infant or young child is an art
form and requires skills in empathising with others, accurate observation and
assessment of needs and emotions, and responses based upon reflection and
interpretation.
Neural development
The mind and brain are regulated by the following:
The infant or young child’s mental health has a direct impact on his capacity to
self-regulate emotions and actions. Siegel (2003) used the term “two-person
governed self-regulation” to described support strategies from an adult or peer to
a child. Initially, these strategies can alter external influences and thereafter
contribute to reconfiguration of the child’s inner working model. Relationships
can change the physiological aspect of neural connections within the brain of
each person. Neuroplasticity is significant to development (Doidge, 2008). Con
nections support self-regulation and contribute to achievement of complexity
and, ultimately, good mental health. This optimum outcome from a secure
Learning and adversities 95
attachment relationship in early childhood can continue to be achieved whether
the attachment figure is present in person or within the child’s implicit memories.
During the first three years of life, development of the right hemisphere of
the brain dominates neural growth. This right side is responsible for proces
sing non-verbal communications, for example, tone or gestures, facial expres
sions, emotion, and social cognition, which relates to theory of mind. The
theory of mind entails a child understanding that other people have opinions,
thoughts, and emotions that differ from his own. I have often heard lay terms
used colloquially that demonstrate comprehension of this concept from the
perspective of parents or grandparents, “He is putting his thoughts together.
He is using joined-up thinking. He can sense what other people are thinking.
Both sides of the brain are talking to each other.” These are phrases I reg
ularly hear from families during parenting work, and it is easy to capitalise
upon these thoughts to promote the significance of a parent’s input to a
child’s neural development.
The left side of the brain develops at a later stage than the right side, and it
uses syllogistic reasoning. This type of reasoning encompasses cause and
effect, and it contributes toward the development of self as a teleological
agent (Whitters, 2019). The left side of the brain relies on information from
the right side to create coherent narratives in order to understand sequences
of events and, ultimately, lead to neural integration between the left and right
hemispheres of the brain. The brain follows an innate tendency to seek out
complexity of processes by integration, differentiation, and regulation.
Barriers to these processes may include short-term stress responses or long
term post-traumatic stress disorder. Additionally, research indicates potential for
damage to the corpus callosum which connects the two hemispheres (Teicher
et al., 2002). The corpus callosum consists of bands of neural tissue that support
information transfer. Damaged connections have also been related to shrinking
of the hippocampus which can affect a child’s ability to develop and to use
explicit memory.
Findings from research have linked damage of this neural area to child abuse
and neglect (Teicher et al., 2002) and association with excessive toxic hormone
secretion as a result of stressful experiences. Too much cortisol can cause
death of neuron cells. The hippocampus has cortisol receptors that increase
vulnerability to the impact of stress. This area of the brain does not fully form
until 16–18 months of age, which is a period when the child starts to use
explicit memory (Siegel, 2003). These milestones of development signify the
importance of early intervention by practitioners, parents, society, and
governments.
Therapeutic support delivered within a secure relationship can instigate
connections between the two hemispheres. Emotion influences all neural cir
cuits and, subsequently, mental functioning. The amygdala processes emo
tions, which results in the child’s internal experience of an emotion, his
expression of this sensation to the outside world, and his perception of emo
tions in others. It is known that the face-recognition cells within the amygdala
96 Learning and adversities
are activated by facial representation of emotions in others. Daily encounters
between a parent/carer and infant provide multiple opportunities to support
links between the two hemispheres.
It is important for parents and practitioners to appreciate the power of any
interaction to reduce the impact of stress upon learning and development.
Many services will implement formal therapeutic interventions to families,
but daily play and home/nursery routines also present rich and stimulating
environments. Multi-modal approaches can target different learning styles of
families and can respond sensitively to increasing capacity and ability as time
progresses. Broad experiences throughout play activate the child’s genes which
results in protein production and changes to the architecture of the brain as
new synapses form. Experiences gained from play also change brain function.
Promoting attachment
We continue to wear COVID-19 protection face masks at drop-off and pick
up time, and, contrary to expectations, secure attachment is established
between infants and carers. It is never too late to promote attachment rela
tionships with children. Research by Siegel (2003) outlined five approaches
that contribute to secure attachment if actioned by parents: contingent com
munication, reflective dialogue, repair following rupture of the relationship,
emotional communication, and coherent narratives.
These approaches can change the neural connections as described by the
following examples.
The orbitofrontal cortex is behind the orbit of the eyes. This is a key loca
tion in the brain that facilitates the cortex’s role in neural integration. Coor
dination occurs between the cortex, limbic structures, and the brain stem, and
impacts upon mental health and repairing the effects of trauma. Schore
(1994) identified a function of the orbitofrontal cortex as regulation of the
autonomic nervous system that controls heart rate, respiration, and intestines.
The two branches are termed:
Potential threat is registered within the fast tracts of the limbic system. The
information which is passed to the prefrontal cortex may not be sufficient for
an infant to differentiate between an actual or a perceived threat, and he may
misinterpret stimuli and appear to overreact (Coates, 2010). Carers are
familiar with children overreacting to minor issues, and this explanation by
Coates provides comprehension of these processes which supports interven
tion and responsive practice within daily routines.
The left side of the brain is responsible for perception and expression of
language, and interpretation of circumstances. The right side of the brain has
responsibility for perception and expression of emotions. Research has
Learning and adversities 99
indicated a negative impact upon development of the left side of the brain in
a context of adverse childhood experiences. A research study found that
verbal abuse from parents can be associated with lack of development in areas
of the left hemisphere of the brain that process language (Choi et al., 2008) in
addition to negative impact upon the tracts linked with emotional regulation.
This study accessed data from 1,271 healthy young adults who were initially
screened for exposure to childhood adversity. Fractional anisotropy was
investigated, and three white matter tract regions on the left side of the brain
indicated a significant reduction compared to the norm. Findings associated
the differences in fractional anisotropy with the level of maternal verbal abuse
and potential effect upon language development in addition to evidence of
psychopathology. These findings are informative by publicising the negative
impact of ridicule, humiliation, and disdain upon brain connectivity.
Teicher et al. (2004) conducted a neural study in which child abuse was
shown to affect development of the corpus callosum by limiting growth. The
right and left hemisphere of the brain are connected through this corpus cal
losum and interact with one another in a context of neural processing. Find
ings indicated that neglect had a high association with this reduction in
growth, and sexual abuse was specifically linked with smaller corpus callosum
in girls. Participant group was composed of 25 girls and 26 boys. A smaller
corpus callosum resulted in reduced integration between left and right hemi
sphere and noticeable changes in mood or personality. Intervention was
recommended by Rothschild (2000) as a means to lower stress hormones and
to maintain healthy functioning of the hippocampus.
Memory was investigated in a study by McClelland (1998). Findings indi
cated that explicit memory, based upon stimuli from the environment, had a
series of stages that supported the retention of information. The first stage of
the brain registering an experience lasts for under half a second, and it
involves input to sensory memory. During the second stage, which lasts for
about 30 seconds, the information is deposited within working memory. The
third stage may incur days, months, or years, as the information is retained in
long-term memory. The final stage is the consolidation of long-term mem
ories into permanent memory which may take days or months to be com
pleted. Permanent memory is independent of the hippocampus.
It is informative that the study by McClelland (1998) raised sleep as an
important contributing factor to these memory processes. Health visitors,
midwives, social workers, and early years practitioners have an essential role
to play in educating parents about the reason for establishing healthy sleep
patterns appropriate to an infant’s age, physiological needs, and personality. I
find that parents are keen to understand sleep patterns in their children, and it
is intriguing to learn about our brains working hard during sleep periods to
process and to retain information.
Research has identified evidence of stress responses in the foetus before
birth (Gunnar & Barr, 1998). Cortisol is known to impact physiologically and
psychologically upon the development of the foetus and subsequently upon
100 Learning and adversities
behaviour during childhood. Outcomes associated with antenatal anxiety
include premature delivery, low birthweight (Association of Infant Mental
Health United Kingdom, 2021), and difficulty in creating an attachment
bond. These challenges may stem from a mother’s negative representation of
her baby before the birth which is created within a context of stress and high
anxiety.
The COVID-19 pandemic is a current stress upon infant mental health
through the mother’s experience of direct and indirect adversities. A survey
conducted in 2020 identified two main causes of stress which occurred during
the first wave of the pandemic: participants felt unprepared for the birth and
fearful of infection from COVID-19. Poverty was also cited as an influential
factor to a high level of stress which was experienced by mothers during this
time (Association of Infant Mental Health United Kingdom, 2021). Digital
media were described by parents as useful means of communication which
could be accessed at opportune times. Participants identified positive out
comes from the digital media as choices in response to needs at a point of
time and privacy to share information in this learning context.
Environmental stress factors lead to a fight, flight, or freeze reaction that
can impact upon the following bodily systems.
� The brain and nervous system, which manage and respond to stress
factors.
� The heart and cardiovascular system, which distribute oxygen throughout
the body within the blood.
� The gut and metabolic system, which transform food into energy for the
body.
� The immune system, which defends against disease, and also supports
healing of injuries.
� The neuroendocrine system, which maintains the balance of hormones.
Physical
Rationale: To understand physical and emotional self.
Outcome: Gross motor skills, balance, proprioception, spatial awareness,
social rules, sequencing, action, and reaction.
Key points of implementation: Health and safety for individual and group, risk
assessment in response to children’s maturity, attraction, stimulation, con
solidation and extension, adult space.
104 Learning and adversities
Arts and crafts
Rationale: To understand physical, emotional, and social self.
Outcome: Fine motor skills, hand-to-eye coordination, sensory awareness,
science concepts, numerical concepts, action, and reaction.
Key points of implementation: Variety of choices, variety of media, personal
and group space, adult space.
Imaginative
Rationale: Play that represents own world or imaginary world, social and
emotional self.
Outcome: Conceptual understanding of the world, understanding of personal
impact upon the world.
Key points of implementation: Accessible individually or group, comfortable,
appropriate choices, adult space.
Table-top
Rationale: Cognitive skills, sequencing, solitary/parallel/cooperative play
Outcome: Memory, hand-to-eye coordination, scaffolding learning, seeking
support – peers or adult.
Key points of implementation: Attractive, accessible, stimulating, adult space.
Floor play
Rationale: Sense of physical, social, emotional, and teleological self, cognitive
skills.
Outcome: Fine and gross motor skills, action, and reaction, increase resi
lience, problem-solving.
Key points of implementation: Attractive, accessible, safe, combining several
choices of media, adult space.
Lack of opportunity for physical development can induce passivity, and it
can result in the brain existing in a neurologically neutral state. The passive
child may appear to have a high level of well-being, but his cognitive and
emotional development are limited by the proximal environment. A readiness
to learn leads to behaviour in which the infant actively seeks out knowledge
and identifies opportunities to explore, despite limited stimulation. Good
Learning and adversities 105
mental and physical health are important and include the meeting of basic
needs, for example, food, water, and personal hygiene. The availability of an
attachment figure and opportunities that provide predictability, consistency,
and repeatability contribute to a child’s readiness to learn. Explicit memories
are formed from the environmental experience, and implicit memories are
created by the emotions that accompany the child’s explorations and interac
tions with others.
Self-control is usually high during free play as the young child is using
developing skills associated with his imagination, and he begins to develop
abstract thought. Objects can be transformed for different purposes, initially
within the child’s imagination then followed by actions that specifically relate
to the child’s interests, needs, and knowledge of the world. Vygotsky (1978)
expressed that a preschool child’s actions are led by his ideas as opposed to
his reaction and interactions with concrete objects. A child may previously
have demonstrated subordination to social rules; however, during free-play
episodes, a child can experience pleasure and fulfilment. The child can change
rules and take control of his own actions. The child’s perceptions of his
proximal and distal world change. The child achieves mastery and power,
which can increase his resilience to life’s adversities; however, distraction
within the external environment can rapidly reduce a child’s involvement and
well-being.
Making meaning
The term “distance”, as used by Vygotsky (1978), relates to the transitional
phase of the child’s intellectual capacity. A transition occurs as the child is
supported by an adult or peer, and his knowledge and understanding increa
ses to a higher degree than would have been achieved through independent
play. Zeeshan and his peers transformed plastic pipes into a learning experi
ence that portrayed understanding of height, weight, length, speed, and skill
in eye–hand coordination. Once potential development is achieved then the
new knowledge and understanding is assimilated by the child within his inner
working model. Processes are internalised and thereafter encompassed within
the capacity for independent achievement. This level of functioning is subse
quently maintained without further adult or peer input. Vygotsky’s law of
double formation refers to these two levels of higher psychological function
ing (Whitters, 2019).
The work by Tronick and Beeghly (2011) highlighted the enormity of bio
psychosocial processes which contribute to making meaning for an infant,
and the core bio-psychosocial state of consciousness. Personality affects the
infant’s interactions with his world, and positive feedback can elicit emotions
of joy and well-being. The cycle of learning commences at conception and
continues throughout the entire lifespan.
Tronick and Beeghly (2011) also describe the making of meaning by infants
as limiting engagement to specific areas, in addition to extending the infant’s
awareness of his role and ability within a family context. The infant’s increase
in understanding of his world impacts positively upon development, for
example, physical skills and communication; however, influences may also
affect development adversely. The study by these authors indicated that
meanings that limit growth on a long-term basis increase the potential for
pathological outcomes. An example of adverse childhood experience in
infancy is a mother who is suffering from postnatal depression. The young
baby may develop representation of himself as negative which results in
108 Learning and adversities
withdrawal from active investigation and exploration of the environment or
hypervigilance to perceived dangers. If the infant is exposed to a variety of
stimulating opportunities for learning, then he will acquire resilience to
negative impacts which promotes normative development.
Shonkoff et al. (2021) published research that focused upon the foundations
of health, learning, and behaviour. Findings emphasised the significance of
the perinatal period and infancy to brain development, the immune system,
and metabolic regulation. The discussion includes the use of an interactive
gene–environment–time framework to promote comprehension of the effects
from adversities. Adversity deprives a child of physical, social, emotional, and
intellectual stimulation and may additionally pose a direct threat to develop
ment (Asmussen et al. 2020).
The study by Shonkoff et al. (2021) indicates that health and development are
influenced by interactive adaptations that commence prior to conception and
continue throughout the lifespan. Impact factors that should be considered
within a context of intervention by services include genetic predispositions.
Genetic factors may be activated by physical and social environments, age, and,
inextricably, the time period with regards to an infant or child’s ability to uptake
learning. An additional consideration is the sensitivity of the individual to a
learning context, which often relates to personality, as described in the example
from practice.
Another issue was raised by Schechter et al. (2019) as the stage of devel
opment in which the adversity was experienced. It is known that memory
recall of events is apparent in infants during their first year of life, and by 24
months long-term memories can influence an infant’s reactions and emotions.
If an adversity is experienced prior to language development, then it can be
difficult for a child to gain comprehension of these circumstances. There may
be specific sensory triggers that remind the child of an adversity by influen
cing his body’s reaction in a similar way to the initial source. A child may use
dissociative processes to gain protection from his emotions, and this can affect
memory recall and subsequent understanding of the issues.
One recent research study found that infants who demonstrated the greatest
sensitivity to adversities were also the most responsive to intervention
(Shonkoff et al., 2021). Impact factors can be used positively to reduce
adversity, or the effects minimised in order to promote resilience. Stress is an
example of an impact factor which is commonly categorised into three levels.
Responsive practice
Asmussen et al. (2020) suggested that an alteration in threat, reward, and
memory-processing, as a result of adversity, could cause changes to children’s
social-emotional functioning. Consequently, these authors indicated that the
changes may affect relationships with others, and cause carers and peers to
reduce or to remove their supportive strategies.
Recommendations by Shonkoff et al. (2021) include supportive and
responsive relationships for infants and children that reflect individual needs,
reactions, and interactions within an environment. Additionally, family
centred learning promotes secure attachment between child and caregiver
within a consistently nurturing context. Interventions can have a direct posi
tive impact upon the cycle of negative parenting in response to endemic
adversities and support a parent to improve self-regulation of herself and the
child. Further recommendations advocated reduction of stress through eco
nomic and psychosocial interventions.
The study by Milot et al. (2016) of 33 neglected children and 72 non-
neglected children, focused upon potential links between neglect, complex
trauma, and short-term consequences. Key messages for practice include
application of knowledge by the practitioner. This information is gained
through in-depth assessment of trauma history within a case file and an
understanding of the personal characteristics of child and parent. Trauma-
informed practice encompasses provision of an environment in which each
child feels safe, therapeutic intervention increases responsiveness of parents,
and there is greater comprehension of links between emotions and behaviours
of parent and child.
Interestingly, the previous authors (Shonkoff et al. 2021) also placed
importance on a practitioner’s use of normative values for infants and chil
dren in accordance with age and expected stage of development. Knowledge
and understanding of developmental norms and projected outcomes support
planning and implementation of intervention by services and may increase
understanding of primary caregivers in the context of a home environment.
A public-health report was published in Scotland, in May 2020, just a few
months after the first wave of COVID-19 pandemic, and the writer indicated
110 Learning and adversities
that the long-term impact of coronavirus upon society is unknown at this
stage (Hetherington, 2020). The pandemic is clearly recognised as an
impactful adversity of the 21st century, and Hetherington described that a
response should occur at community, family, and individual level, in addition
to broader society. This author advocates a public-health approach that pre
vents or minimises children’s adverse experiences during formative years.
Public health focuses upon the needs of a population or specific groups, as
opposed to a clinician’s response to an individual. The study identifies social
determinants of health as childhood experiences, housing, education, social
support, family income, employment, community, and access to health
services.
Hetherington (2020) describes three approaches to public health.
Ghosts in a nursery
The concept of ghosts in a nursery was presented in research which was con
ducted half a century ago (Fraiberg et al., 1975), and the understanding of
these issues continues to have current relevance (Scottish Government & NHS
Scotland, 2021). It is well known that parenting patterns are often learned
and repeated between generations. Fraiberg and the research team described
these ghosts as being expressed unconsciously within the parent–infant
relationship.
The descriptor of unresolved parents (Fraiberg et al., 1975) is associated
with the concept of ghosts in a nursery; however, current practice states that
negative issues should not be used to define an individual’s identity. Each
organisation has a duty to review terminology for potential stereotypical bias
in this field. Trauma is regarded as an emotional reaction based upon an
inner working framework which was created in a context of adverse child
hood experiences. The foundation of a pedagogy and the message which is
given to service-users is hope and potential for change. For example, the
Scottish Family Support Strategy, 2020–2023, uses the voice, validation and
hope model (Glasgow City Council, 2020). This model promotes that practi
tioners and families discuss issues as unresolved trauma, which implies
potential for positive change, as opposed to unresolved parents, which impo
ses a negative status upon the parent’s identity.
Unresolved trauma can only be resolved if the parent is supported to
develop his or her sense of self and agency and to take ownership of the
child’s destiny. I practise in a multigenerational context, and I have found that
longevity of change and minimisation of intergenerational transmission of
trauma can be achieved. Children, parents, and grandparents learn to work
Learning and adversities 111
alongside the professional and together as a family unit. Families who use
drugs or alcohol can improve their lifestyles over time, but there are always
peaks and troughs in the change processes. A wide network of available sup
portive adults, out-with the immediate family, can also provide multiple pro
tective factors.
The second and third trimester of pregnancy are often opportune times to
implement intervention. These stages naturally incur the mother developing
maternal representation of her unborn baby. This maternal comprehension of
the forthcoming baby informs the mother’s inner working model. The work
ing model of the child interview (Theran et al., 2005) can be used to cate
gorise the mother’s representation of the forthcoming baby as balanced,
disengaged, or distorted. Negative childhood experiences may result in
inadequate parenting if a pregnant woman develops a disengaged or a dis
torted mental representation of her unborn baby. I have recently learned that
fathers experience physical changes during pregnancy, which includes an
increase in cortisol levels in the early stages and a reduction in the production
of testosterone pre-birth. Physical changes for both parents occur in tandem
with emotional and mental representations of their unborn baby emerging.
Belsky (1984) described three determinants of parenting as the personal
psychological resources of parents, the characteristics of the child, and the
contextual sources of stress and support. Contextual sources which Belsky
regarded as inducing stress, or giving support were marital relationship, social
networks, and employment. The research by this author identified that per
sonal resources of the parent were more effective in enhancing the parent–
child relationship than contextual sources of support. Additionally, contextual
sources had a greater impact upon the parent–child relationship than the
characteristics of the child.
The literature review by O’Hara et al. (2019) accessed 22 publications on
parent–child and family–child interventions to examine the impact of video
feedback upon parental sensitivity. The studies were conducted in Canada,
the Netherlands, the United Kingdom, and the United States of America.
Single studies were also conducted in Italy, Germany, Lithuania, Norway, and
Portugal. Findings indicated that the sensitivity of parents did increase with
the use of feedback from videos of parent–child interactions. Visual media are
excellent tools to use in parenting work and encompass photographs, videos,
narratives, or role-play. It is important that parents are supported to interpret
a photograph or video and to link their increase in comprehension to devel
opment of skills.
References
Asmussen, K., Fischer, F., Drayton, E., & McBride, T. (2020). Adverse childhood
experiences, what we know, what we don’t know, and what should happen next. https://
www.eif.org.uk/report/adverse-childhood-experiences-what-we-know-what-we-dont
know-and-what-should-happen-next.
Association of Infant Mental Health United Kingdom. (2021). The secondary impact
of COVID-19 on infant mental health: What do we know, and what digital methods
of working can improve outcomes. AIMHUK best practice guide number 8. https://a
imh.uk/news-resources/members-only-resources.
Beebe B., & Steele, M. (2013). How does microanalysis of mother–infant commu
nication inform maternal sensitivity and infant attachment? Attachment & Human
Development 15(5–6),583–602.
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development,
55(1), 83–96. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129836.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. Abingdon and New
York: Routledge.
Boyce, W. T., Levitt, P., Martinez, F. D., McEwen, B. S., & Shonkoff, J. P. (2021).
Genes, environments, and time: The biology of adversity and resilience. Pediatrics,
147(2), e20201651. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2020-1651.
Bratton, S. C., Landreth, G. L., Kellam, T., & Blackard, S. R. (2006). Child/parent
participation therapy treatment manual. Abingdon and New York: Routledge.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development, 2nd edition. Cam
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Learning and adversities 119
Chichekian, T., & Shore, B. M. (2014). The international baccalaureate: Contributing
to the use of inquiry in higher education teaching and learning. In J. M. Carfora &
P. Blessinger (Eds.), Inquiry-based learning for faculty and institutional development:
A conceptual and practical resource for educators, vol. I (pp. 73–97). Bingley:
Emerald Group.
Choi, J., Jeong, B., Rohan, M. L., & Polcari, A. (2008). Preliminary evidence for white
matter tract abnormalities in young adults exposed to parental verbal abuse. Biological
Psychiatry, 65(3), 227–234.
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2006). Developmental psychopathology and preventive
intervention. In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. IV
(pp. 511–512). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Coates, D. (2010). Impact of childhood abuse: Biopsychosocial pathways through
which adult mental health is compromised article in Australian social work.
Australian Social Work, 63(4), 391–403.
Doidge, N. (2008). The brain that changes itself. New York: Penguin.
El Nokali, N. E., Bachman, H. J., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2010). Parent involvement
and children’s academic and social development in elementary school. Child Devel
opment, 81(3), 988–1005.
Fonagy, P. (1999). Pathological attachments and therapeutic action. Psyche Matters.
https://www.psychematters.com/papers/fonagy3.htm.
Fraiberg, S., Adelson, E., & Shapiro, V. (1975) Ghosts in the nursery: A psycho
analytic approach to the problems of impaired infant-mother relationships. Journal
of American Academy for Child Development, 14(3), 387–421. https://www.science
direct.com/science/article/pii/S0002713809614424.
Glasgow City Council. (2020). Glasgow’s family support Strategy, 2020–23. https://www.
glasgow.gov.uk/councillorsandcommittees/viewSelectedDocument.asp?c=P62AFQ
DN0GUTT181DX.
Gunnar, M. R., & Barr, R. G. (1998). Stress, early brain development, and behavior.
Infants and Young Children, 11(1), 1–14. https://journals.lww.com/iycjournal/Abstract/
1998/07000/Stress_Early_Brain_Development_and_Behaviour.4.aspx.
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. (2020). Connecting the brain to
the rest of the body: Early childhood development and lifelong health are deeply
intertwined. Working paper no. 15. https://www.developingchild.harvard.edu.
Hetherington, K. (2020). Ending childhood adversity: A public health approach. Edin
burgh: Public Health Scotland.
McClelland, J. L. (1998). Complementary learning systems in the brain: A connec
tionist approach to explicit and implicit cognition and memory. Annals of the New
York Academy of Sciences, 153–178. https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Comp
lementary-Learning-Systems-in-the-Brain%3A-A-to-McClelland/
67d91b41134fd099f962568d77abe235582748ab.
McCrory, E. J., Gerin, M. I., & Viding, E. (2017). Childhood maltreatment, latent
vulnerability and the shift to preventative psychiatry: The contribution of functional
brain imaging. Annual Research Review. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12713.
Milot, T., St-Laurent, D., & Ethier, L. S. (2016). Intervening with severely and
chronically neglected children and their families: The contribution of trauma-
informed approaches. Child Abuse Review, 25, 89–101.
Moore, E., & Churchill, G. (2020). Still here for children: Sharing the experiences of
NSPCC staff who supported children and families during the Covid-2019 pandemic.
120 Learning and adversities
https://learning.nspcc.org.uk/research-resources/2020/still-here-for-children-experiences
of-nspcc-staff-during-coronavirus.
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. (2006). Early exposure to toxic
substances damages brain architecture. Working paper no. 4. https://developingchild.
harvard.edu/resources/early-exposure-to-toxic-substances-damages-brain-architecture.
O’Hara, L., Smith, E. R., Barlow, J., Livingstone, N., Herath, N. I. N. S., Wei, Y.,
Spreckelsen, T., & Macdonald, G. (2019). Video feedback for parental sensitivity
and attachment security in children under five years. Cochrane Database of Sys
tematic Reviews, (11). https://www.cochrane.org/CD012348/BEHAV_video-feedback
parental-sensitivity-and-child-attachment.
Perry, B. D., Pollard, R., Blakely, T., Baker, W., & Vigilante, D. (1995). Childhood
trauma, the neurobiology of the brain: How “states” become “traits”. Infant Mental
Health Journal, 16(4), 271–291.
Roedell, W. C. (1984). Vulnerabilities of highly gifted children. Roeper Review 6(3),
127–130. https://www.positivedisintegration.com/Roedell1984.
Rogers, C. (1990). Toward a modern approach to values: The valuing process in the
mature person. In H. Kirschenbaum & V. L. Henderson (Eds.), The Carl Rogers
Reader. London: Constable.
Roisman, G. I., Padron, E., Sroufe, A., & Egeland, B. (2003). Earned–secure attach
ment status in retrospect and prospect. Child Development. 73(4), 1204–1219. https://
www.srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/1467-8624.00467.
Rothschild, B. (2000). The body remembers: The psychophysiology of trauma and
trauma treatment. London: W. W. Norton & Company.
Schechter, D., Wilheim, E., Suardi, F., & Serpa, S. R. (2019). The effects of violent
experiences on infants and young children. In C. H. Zeanah, Jr (Ed.), Handbook of
infant mental health, 4th edition (pp. 219–238). New York: The Guilford Press.
Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of self: The neurobiology of
emotional development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Scottish Government. (2004). A curriculum for excellence. Edinburgh: Scottish Executive.
Scottish Government & NHS Scotland. (2021). Trauma-informed practice: A toolkit for
Scotland. https://www.gov.scot/publications/trauma-informed-practice-toolkit-scotland.
Scottish Social Services Council. (2003). Codes of practice for social services workers
and employers, code 1. Dundee: Scottish Social Services Council.
Shonkoff, J. P., Boyce, W. T., Levitt, P., & Fernando, D. (2021). Leveraging the biology
of adversity and resilience to transform pediatric practice. Pediatrics, 147(2), 1–9.
Siegel, D. J. (2003). An interpersonal neurobiology of psychotherapy: The developing mind
and the resolution of trauma. In M. F. Solomon & D. J. Siegel (Eds.), Healing trauma,
attachment, mind, body, and brain (pp. 1–56). London: W. W. Norton & Company.
Teicher, M. T., Andersen, S. L., Polcari, A., Andersen, C. M., Navalta, C. P. (2002).
Developmental neurobiology of childhood stress and trauma. Review Psychiatry, 25
(2), 397–426. https://www.pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12136507.
Teicher, M. H., Dumont, N. L., Yutaka, I., Vaituzis, C., Giedd, J. N., & Andersen, L.
(2004). Childhood neglect is associated with reduced corpus callosum area. Journal
of Biopsychiatry, 56(2), 80–85. https://www.pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/?term=teicher
+mh&cauthor_id=15231439.
The Family Nurse Partnership (2011). The evaluation of the Family Nurse Partnership
programme in Scotland: Phase 1 report – intake and early pregnancy. https://www.
gov.scot/publications/evaluation-family-nurse-partnership-programme-scotland
phase-1-report-intake-early-pregnancy.
Learning and adversities 121
Theran, S., Levendosky, A., Bogat, G., & Huth-Bocks, A. (2005). Stability and
change in mothers’ internal representations of their infants over time. Attachment
& Human Development, 7(3), 253–268. https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.
1080/14616730500245609.
Trevarthen, C. (2001). Intrinsic motives for companionship in understanding: Their
origin, development, and significance for infant mental health. Infant Mental Health
Journal, 22 (1–2),95–131. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/247947166_Intrin
sic_motives.
Tronick, E., & Beeghly, M. (2011). Infants’ meaning-making and the development of
mental health problems. American Psychologist, 66(2), 107–109. https://www.pubm
ed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/21142336.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological pro
cesses. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1994). The problem of the cultural development of the child. In R.
Van der Veer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp. 57–71, 308–310).
Oxford: Blackwell.
Weiss, L. (2019). Wechsler intelligence scale for children, fifth edition: Use in societal
context. In L. G. Weiss, D. H. Saklofske, J. A. Holdnack, & A. Prifitera (Eds.),
Practical resources for the mental health professional, 2nd edition (pp. 129–195).
Academic Press.
Whitters, H. G. (2019). Attainment and executive functioning in the early years:
Research for inclusive practice and lifelong learning. Abingdon and New York:
Routledge.
Whitters, H. G. (2020). Adverse childhood experiences, attachment, and the early years
learning environment. Abingdon and New York: Routledge.
Zeanah, C. H., Benoit, D., Hirshberg, L., Barton, M. L., & Regan, C. (1994).
Mothers’ representations of their infants are concordant with infant attachment
classifications. Developmental Issues in Psychiatry and Psychology, 1, 9–18. http://
www.researchgate.net/publications/285330060.
Zhou, M., & Brown, D. (2015). Educational learning theories, 2nd edition, Education
Open Textbooks, 1. https://oer.galileo.usg.edu/education-textbooks/1.
5 Ability, capacity, and creativity
Memory
Memory is formed through integration that inputs to a holistic information
system. It is known that during rapid eye movement sleep experiences are
associated with consolidation and integration of emotions and knowledge
(Siegel, 2003).
Explicit memory has two forms: semantic or factual memory and episodic
or autobiographical memory. Autobiographical memory includes a sense of
self in the memory recall and an awareness of time. There are two known
124 Ability, capacity, and creativity
components to implicit memory: procedural memory, which is associated with
behaviour, and the emotions accompanying an experience. For example, the
actions required to ride a bike are retained in procedural memory, and the
associated emotion which may be positive or negative remain within implicit
memory. Siegel (2003) makes an interesting point as he described the poten
tial for an implicit memory in the earliest years being retained as a sense of
familiarity as opposed to actual remembering of details. Young children may
recall and express emotions and reactions to experiences that occurred before
effective communication skills had been established. This recall is based upon
the sense of familiarity.
It is fascinating that the brain has the ability to process past experiences at
a higher level over time as development and maturity occur. This creates
rationale for the use of counselling in adulthood which targets unresolved
trauma from childhood. Hart and Rubia (2012) highlighted the impact of
childhood maltreatment upon brain architecture and functions. Adversity in
childhood impacts upon infant mental health. The infant experiences internal
issues, for example, anxiety, and demonstrates external behaviours, for example,
poor impulse control. Long-term changes to the infant’s brain architecture and
functioning can occur. These authors indicated links between child abuse and
deficits in intellect, memory, working memory, attention, response inhibition,
and the ability to discriminate between emotions.
The therapeutic process provides verbal media to support resolution of
trauma by enabling an individual to create a coherent narrative of the issues.
Additionally, this process provides non-verbal media in the form of a secure
attachment relationship that supports self-regulation and integration of
experiences. Lack of resolution of trauma is regarded as the mind’s incapacity
to balance differentiation and integration of energy and information flow
(Siegel, 2003).
Stress interferes with integration and differentiation, but a therapeutic
relationship can fulfil the purpose of liberating the mind from these barriers
and by supporting a desire to achieve maximum complexity. Siegel (2003)
studied the mind and brain, and this researcher concluded that the mind
emerges as the brain matures. The development of the mind is influenced by
self, and others, and affected by personality. Interaction occurs between neu
rophysiological and interpersonal relationships. Over time, neural connections
can be made independently and separate from the influences of the ther
apeutic dyad. Sensory input, for example, auditory, visual, or tactile, can
affect perceptions and direct actions. These processes are often associated with
creativity.
Differentiation is influenced by genetics and experiences, particularly
during the earliest years. It is the young child’s involvement with learning
experiences that is key to development, and not solely the environment per se.
It is essential that practitioners and parents appreciate and embrace the value
of their role in supporting a child’s engagement, interaction, and response to
learning.
Ability, capacity, and creativity 125
Siegel (2003) clarified internal and external constraints, and this author
identified self-regulation as essential to good mental health. Internal con
straints refer to the composition of synaptic connections among neurons.
External constraints refer to environmental experiences and include inter
personal communication with others, particularly if a relationship has an
emotional significance. A relationship is an external factor that can influence
the neural system by leading to changes in internal constraints. Positive
changes increase the potential for complexity to be achieved.
Secure attachment is a descriptor of the relationship which a child has cre
ated with another person, for example, a parent, carer, peer, or practitioner.
The child may have different types of relationships with his range of carers.
Fosha (2003) identified skills which are required by a caregiver to nurture a
secure attachment status with a child, and to achieve optimal development.
These skills or characteristics apply to parents and practitioners in early years
settings.
High ability
It has been recognised for many years that identification of children who are
developmentally delayed is an important consideration in implementing early
intervention (Pfeiffer & Petscher, 2008). However, children of high ability
should also be identified and supported in response to individual learning
needs.
A study by Freeman (1998) was conducted over twenty years ago, and the
knowledge and understanding from findings continues to be applied by current
educators. Recommendations were compiled into guidance for teachers regarding
recognition of pupils who had high ability. Freeman focused mainly on assessment
of teacher–pupil interaction in a school setting. Findings included checklists that
contained ability criteria, for example, the use of complicated rules and extensive
imaginative play, alacrity in the use of symbolic communication – talking, reading,
126 Ability, capacity, and creativity
and writing – and extended concentration on tasks from an early age. The
conclusion of this research highlighted circumstances that accompanied problem-
solving and a child’s ability to respond to unexpected circumstances within a
learning context as significant factors.
There are points found on many checklists that provide a useful foundation
for learning about universal traits associated with ability. Examples are
memory and retention of factual knowledge, regulation of oneself, and iden
tification of preference for learning style and medium. Freeman’s research
indicates that the use of checklists can provide knowledge to an educator and
promote professional awareness of the concept of ability, but the author
warns that checklists can be self-limiting (Freeman, 1998). Mental health is
currently regarded as influential to the learning context. This aspect does not
specifically feature on Freeman’s guide to educators.
Intelligence is understood by Freeman (1998) as a way of organising and
using knowledge in relation to a physical and social environment. Findings
did refer to the influence from culture and family upon development, and
the study concluded that every child’s potential can be realised with
encouragement, learning materials, and educational support. Freeman’s
study outlined the use of two intelligence quotient (IQ) tests to determine
ability in a context of chronological age. Findings indicated that testing was
applicable to measuring a child’s academic ability within the early years of a
school environment.
1 Ability which is greater than the norm for a child’s age group.
2 Task commitment.
3 Creativity.
Ability, capacity, and creativity 127
Forty years ago, Renzulli (1978) conducted research that indicated that the
aforementioned three clusters of ability, task commitment, and creativity were
equal partners in contributing to a child’s overall ability. Recent research by
Renzulli and Reis (2018) concluded that gifted behaviour is reflected by
interaction of these three clusters which could be applied to any area of
human development. The researchers emphasised that the three clusters did
not need to be present for a child to exhibit high ability. The constant factor
was demonstration of interaction by two or more clusters at a higher level
than the norm. Findings indicated that task commitment and creativity tend
to be applied together.
It is enlightening to consider research that investigates issues beyond the
immediate scope of a research topic. As a researcher and practitioner in early
years, my focus for this monograph is infant mental health; however, the dis
cussion by Renzulli and Reis (2018), albeit in relation to high ability, certainly
contributes to understanding of generic learning processes. In addition, I refer
to research in children who have additional support for learning needs.
Understanding of the norm can be strengthened by information on outliers.
Ability
Ability is multilayered, and initial subdivisions relate to the use of cognitive
and physical skill that demonstrate the child’s level of prowess. Physical skill
can be further refined by consideration of fine and gross motor skills, spatial
awareness, and proprioception. Ability can be extended through copying
which encompasses a child’s interpretation and reproduction of what he has
observed, including elements of creativity. The skill to copy, and to represent,
requires memory, which is a child’s recall of prior knowledge and application
of his understanding over time. The time period for application may be short
or long term.
Bergen et al. (2018) identified memory potential within every neuron of the
brain, and these researchers indicated that emotional development formed a
necessary foundation for cognitive skills to accumulate and to refine. Implicit
memories are greatly influenced by emotions that accompany an experience,
and from birth these memories are stored in the middle and lower part of the
brain. Also, during this period of growth is the development of cognition.
Enactive cognition entails an infant gaining understanding of the actions of
real objects. Iconic cognition entails comprehension of pictures and symbols
that represent real objects (Bergen et al., 2018).
Explicit memories are based upon experiential learning and emerge
throughout childhood due to dependence on the development of the cerebral
cortex (Bergen et al., 2018). Explicit memories may be expressed in the age
group of 12 months onwards. By 3 years of age, children have the ability to
use their memories voluntarily. Many strategies promote emotional literacy
which often entails prompting a young child’s memory through discussion,
photographs, and actions. Thelen and Smith (2006) conducted research on
128 Ability, capacity, and creativity
tasks that used memory. Findings from their study indicated that the ability of
a young child to reach and achieve a goal through experiential learning cre
ates a significant long-lasting memory. Experiential play which is supported
by an attachment figure is a key response to education in the child’s earliest
years, and it nurtures conditions for attainment.
Child-led pedagogy
It is so interesting that Panksepp (1998) assumed that children’s natural
inclination for a particular type of play was a response to their developmental
needs. Early years practitioners are trained to observe children during free
play, to assess developmental levels and each child’s preponderance to parti
cular activities in comparison to the norm. Results are recorded on care plans
in the categories of personality, interests, and schemas. Daily observations of
children can reveal deficits in development and indicate the body’s inherent
response to these needs. Interpretation of this information provides a valid
baseline for personalised intervention.
Current implementation of curricula is presented within a child-led peda
gogy, and children’s interests lead the planning and implementation of a
learning environment. Greater understanding of these issues can be gained
through the research by Panksepp (1998) and the growing awareness in prac
tice of the human body’s inherent responses to need by young children’s
choices of play activities and behaviour. Behaviour may not adhere to social
boundaries and expectations of a setting. It is important to provide the child
with alternative behavioural choices which continue to respond to needs,
including the impact of trauma and social circumstances.
Many years after Panksepp (1998) published his findings, early years set
tings in many countries provide daily periods of outdoor active play and
learning for all children. The therapeutic benefit upon mental health from
play within green spaces is recognised by educators and parents. Early years
settings provide multiple opportunities for children to develop and to practise
skills in a range of environments. Stress can be reduced over time with a
therapeutic and nurturing approach which is based upon a secure attachment
relationship.
Ability, capacity, and creativity 131
Emotional development and support
Emotions are regarded as responses to external stimuli or internal mental
representations based upon the inner working model of an individual. Ochsner
and Gross (2005) described emotions as incurring changes within experiential,
behavioural, peripheral, and physiological systems. These authors distinguished
emotions from moods by relating a trigger to the onset of an emotion. Emotions
can be based upon an instinctive response to direct stimuli, or a learned response
which may be influenced by prior experiences.
Research studies have shown that infants and young children who have
experienced long-term adversity in the form of toxic stress have difficulty in
identifying emotions through facial expressions. It is also suggested that
emotional discrimination ability is affected by the type of abuse which the
child has suffered. Pollak et al. (2000) indicated that neglected children had
the greatest difficulty in discriminating between facial expressions compared
to children who had experienced physical abuse. The latter group of children
demonstrated a noticeable response to angry expressions compared to other
negative emotions.
Research indicates three significant aspects which have emerged through
studies of emotion regulation and cognitive control. Ochsner and Gross
(2005) describe these aspects as:
1 Defence mechanisms.
2 Management of situations that exceed the resources of an individual.
3 Self-regulation in a context of socio-emotional development.
Green spaces
Early years services use sensory activities as a medium for promoting the
development of children from birth to three years of age (Scottish Govern
ment, 2010). An increase in comprehension of the rationale of this approach
can transform the impetus, motivation, and creativity of practitioners in
teaching and caring for our youngest learners. Once again, research findings
highlight the importance of curriculum planning and implementation of a
rich learning environment that nurtures optimum capacity of the neural areas
of each child.
Robinson and Brown (2016) referred to the use of outdoor spaces to induce
a calm, ambient atmosphere. Green exercise was the term applied by Barton
and Pretty (2010) within research using data from adult participants. The
study compared short- and long-term health benefits that could be gained
from an outdoor environment. Findings indicated that the greatest positive
changes were demonstrated by the youngest participants who had existing
mental-health conditions.
Louv (2005) applied the descriptor “nature-deficit disorder” to indicate the
negative effects that relate to lack of outdoor stimulation in the context of
136 Ability, capacity, and creativity
urban dwellings. Jawer (2005) reviewed the topic from the young child’s per
spective, and he suggested that environmental sensitivity can be nurtured
through the provision of outdoor play opportunities. Outcomes for the child
represent outcomes for nature. The child’s understanding of himself as relevant,
respected, and looked after by society, is gained through his comprehension and
response to the natural world (Whitters, 2019).
Ohly et al. (2016) explain this phenomenon in the context of attention
restoration theory. A literature review of 31 studies had been used to investigate
attention fatigue, which is a condition associated with low self-regulation, poor
decision-making, and physical ill-health. Findings indicated that ability and
capacity to concentrate and to increase executive functioning can be restored
through exposure to natural environments.
Attention restoration theory explains the ability and capacity to refocus
and increase attention to learning in four categories (Kaplan, 1995):
spotted peeping around the edges of the rubber mats, responding to a few
days of mild weather. Papers and sundry refuse items have escaped from
the large industrial metal bins, and the wind encourages these items to fly
across and over the garden, high into the dark sky, and beyond the
rooftops.
Glory and Lewis are best friends. They share a passion for energetic play,
for digging in a mud kitchen, for exploring familiar corners of the garden, and
hiding excitedly in the long bamboo grasses. Lewis always has a goal, and
he uses this green space to further his interests. Currently his play focuses
upon building upwards, and along. The garden offers ample materials for
this constructor of ideas, and he works industriously and individually by
gathering wooden bricks and freeze blocks. The child stands back to survey
his preparations, and he puts a plan into action. It looks like this little boy is
constructing a tower. Lewis could be described as experiencing hard fasci
nation within the outdoor environment. He builds upwards and along the
winding path which is strewn with bark pieces. His plan changes and adapts
to the materials and to his ability in creating a stable structure. As the thin
tower cascades to the ground, Lewis giggles, his plan has changed instan
taneously, and his new goal has been achieved.
Glory is sitting under a bush. The rhododendron creates a tent-like
canopy of dark green foliage above the little girl’s head, and isolated drips of
rain disrupt her play flow, just momentarily. Glory has a worm in the palm of
her hand. The long slender worm is unsure of this new warm environment.
He lifts his head and waves it around to detect familiar territory. Glory tou
ches the worm with a fingertip, and he retracts quickly, curling tightly upon
her palm. The movements feel like gentle tickling to this young explorer. The
child waits and watches – a silent learner. Glory is experiencing soft fasci
nation within her nurturing bush corner in the nursery garden.
A later study by Kaplan and Berman (2010) identified two types of atten
tion: voluntary and involuntary. Voluntary attention is currently termed
“directed attention”, and it relates to a child’s conscious focus upon an
activity. Involuntary attention requires less concerted effort due to a specific
interest of the child being met or attraction to the content through curiosity
or prior knowledge. An outdoor environment is commonly regarded as a
context in which children and adults demonstrate involuntary attention;
however, the restoration effect from interaction within green spaces can also
support directed attention associated with specific plans and goals.
Good health and attainment were linked by Kuo and Taylor (2004) within
a study upon play within green spaces. Findings indicated that symptoms
associated with attention fatigue, which is a periodic condition, were similar
to characteristics of ADHD, which is a long-term condition. Conclusions
reported that direct interaction within green spaces, or indirect pictorial
138 Ability, capacity, and creativity
representations linked to attainment in activities which immediately followed
these experiences. Findings concluded that exercise in green spaces reduced
the characteristics of attention fatigue and increased the capacity for learning
that occurred after the physical activity. Many settings incorporate physical
play outdoors during the initial period of a session. Anecdotal comments by
parents declare that “Children learn better if they run off their energy first!”
Outdoor play stimulates the physical body, and mind, in addition to envel
oping a child in the therapeutic atmosphere of a natural space.
The data of 175 participants, which was collated in a study by Corraliza et
al. (2012), had indicated that children who were exposed to nature on a reg
ular basis had increased coping strategies in response to adversities. Interest
ingly, in this study, the children who were most vulnerable experienced the
greatest positive effects from contact with the natural environment.
Since I have been studying research, I have realised that findings can often
be given clarity by linking to practical experiences in the field. Research
informs practice by presenting rationale, knowledge based upon theory, and
findings gained within a formal context of information retrieval. Practical
experience educates the practitioner by presenting understanding and appli
cation of knowledge in situ. Research and practice should be regarded as
interdependent and equally valuable for professional development.
It is important for early years practitioners to liaise with parents in order to
analyse characteristics that aid attention restoration and to reflect the cultural
context of the area, for example, urban or rural, and needs of service-users.
Strategies can be actioned through effective professional–parent partnerships
and consistent communication links between service and home environment.
Facebook pages are currently popular media for sharing ideas between ser
vice-providers and service-users. An increase in the use of social media has led
to many services using instructional videos to encourage and to support
parent–child interaction within a family home. Videos present a role model
for parents and children to copy. It may be that the parent provides a learning
medium by copying the interactions from the video, and the child copies his
parent. Alternatively, child and parent may jointly use the video as their
source of role-modelling.
Capacity
Capacity relates to emotional well-being, which is founded on a child’s
secure attachment with one or more adults, and it increases alongside a
child’s resilience to adversities. The current term which is being used in
childcare is “homeostasis” (National Scientific Council on the Developing
Child, 2020). This descriptor is representative of a child having optimal
emotional well-being and resilience that leads to demonstration of motiva
tion and curiosity to seek out learning opportunities. Stern (1998) describes
daily social interactions and care routines as major contributions to this
status of a young child.
Ability, capacity, and creativity 139
Increasing capacity includes an infant learning by gaining knowledge and
understanding of the world through the medium of his senses: sight, hearing,
smell, taste, and touch. Emotional well-being is a necessary condition for a child
to liberate his five senses for learning that leads to development. If a child is suf
fering from internal stresses, then he may appear vigilant and alert for danger to
himself, even within the safe environment of a nursery. The receptivity of his five
senses becomes heightened. Being alert is a basic survival instinct, and, although a
child’s senses are attuned and primed to gain understanding of the proximal
environment, it is for the purpose of detecting danger as opposed to development.
Bowlby (1997) recorded different forms of behaviour that linked to attach
ment status:
Play cycle
King and Sturrock (2020) identified four levels of intervention which could be
implemented to support a play cycle:
� Play maintenance in which the adult observes the activity. I would add
that the adult presents a positive attitude and demonstrates an interested
focus upon the child’s play.
� Simple intervention in which the adult can suggest uses for the resources.
The key term indicates the adult should not lead the play cycle.
� Medial intervention in which the adult is invited to participate by the child. I
add that the adult suggests ideas for the play but takes direction from the
child and responds promptly to his play cues, and to his emotional reactions.
� Complex intervention in which the adult becomes intertwined in the
child’s play cycle. Reciprocal interactions can occur in this context, but
the adult should continue to overtly recognise the child as the director of
his play.
140 Ability, capacity, and creativity
Freeman (1998) indicated that negative emotions can inhibit curiosity in
accordance with findings from an extensive German study on children.
Divergent thinking will not occur if a child’s capacity is lowered. Adversities
may have a short-term effect upon capacity to learn, or detrimentally affect a
child’s lifestyle on a long-term basis. Interruption of a play cycle was descri
bed by King and Temple (2020) as resulting in dysplay and this hiatus in
learning was linked directly to negative emotional reactions. During observa
tions in a playroom practitioners should consider whether the play cycle is
interrupted by an internal emotional influence from within the infant which is
based upon memory, or an external factor that leads to this emotional barrier.
An infant who is reacting to an external source will usually turn towards that
source, for example, a loud noise in the vicinity, or deliberately turn away
from a local source, for example, another child screaming.
In early years settings and the home environment, children may demon
strate emotional reactions when their play cycle is curtailed, for example,
crying, shouting, aggressive behaviour, or quiet emotional withdrawal. The
child’s homeostasis becomes uneven. There are many valid reasons for short
ening a play cycle, particularly within a service that is encompassed by the
necessity of routines. However, the key to responsive support by an adult is pre
paring the child for ending a play cycle and by introducing transitional strategies
that lead towards the next event. Preparation is a valuable approach that facil
itates a child’s emotional and physical adaptation to situations. I observe young
children in nurseries completing their play cycle rapidly after being informed that
“tidy-up time” will commence in a few minutes. Practitioners can support the
ending of a play cycle through verbal tracking of the child’s actions and
encouragement to look forward to the next activity or event.
One important component of learning is delivery of knowledge in a manner
and medium that promotes understanding by each recipient. Instruction
needs to be on par with the developmental level of the individual child and to
be accompanied by motivational prompts that match his personality, needs,
and interests at a point in time. The research of McCoach and Flake (2018)
indicated that frequent praise in relation to achievement may impact nega
tively upon a child’s resilience. Zhou and Brown (2015) had referred to a self-
ranking system in which a child may gain a sense of the autobiographical self
as good or bad; thus, the child’s sense of self is inadvertently linked to adult
or peer feedback. Motivation is reliant on an external source. Harter (2006)
commented that abuse in childhood can result in the child viewing himself as
bad, and this writer linked adverse circumstances to depression in the early
years.
Ability may not be demonstrated consistently by a child due to internal
and external influences upon the learner. Hypervigilance can direct a baby or
young child’s attention away from his learning needs, and interests. Mental
health, personality, and environment, which is conducive to the child’s inter
ests, are factors that can strongly impact upon application and, consequently,
demonstration of ability.
Ability, capacity, and creativity 141
Task commitment is represented by motivation to focus upon a specific
problem or area in which understanding is being sought (Renzulli & Reis,
2018). Descriptors which are used in association with this cluster are “ded
ication”, “endurance”, and “self-confidence”. Task commitment is under
pinned by the child’s sense of self, albeit at a rudimentary stage in earliest
childhood, which includes being a physical, social, and teleological agent
(Whitters, 2019).
Harter (2006) presents a parental responsibility as supporting a child to
develop a narrative of self, for example, a positive representation of childhood
as recorded in photographs, memory stories of significant events, and cultural
routines pertaining to each family. This narrative includes perceptions and
interpretation of the world from a young child’s viewpoint in addition to
parental interpretation or other dominant adults. Harter used the term
“impoverished self” to represent the outcome of a parent’s failure to support
the child’s creation of a positive narrative of self. Application that demon
strates commitment by a parent will support a child to understand his fourth
level of self as an intentional mental agent. An elementary level of the auto
biographical self is generally achieved within the first five years; however,
environmental and social adversities may hinder this stage of development.
During parenting work, I find that many parents and children who live in
adversity require sensitive prompting and probing questions in order to recall
positive memories of the early days of their lives. Creating a narrative of self
is a significant stage of childhood that affects early and middle childhood,
adolescence, adulthood, and parental identity. Interventions must support this
foundational aspect of development. Harter (2006) emphasised the impor
tance of creating an autobiographical memory which influences hopes and
dreams for the future. Creativity is linked to the sense of self, and this trait is
only fully liberated when an autobiographical self is understood and
embraced by the individual.
Creativity
Creativity involves devising and following a plan that leads to a unique out
come. There are numerous skill sets associated with planning and imple
mentation of self-appointed goals by children. Plucker and Barab (2005)
describe creativity as interaction between environment, process, and a child’s
aptitude, and, finally the acknowledgement of an end goal. These authors did
comment that creativity may be directed by cultural influences and led by
expectations of adults, in addition to the child’s own goals.
Creativity requires imagination and an ability and desire to extrapolate
beyond the status quo. A point of clarification is made by Renzulli (2019) as
he does not exclusively link creativity to process and production of an end
goal. This author includes descriptors of creativity as originality of thought,
receptivity to new experiences, curiosity within a learning environment, sen
sitivity to detail, and awareness of the emotions of self and others. In the
142 Ability, capacity, and creativity
early years services, these descriptors are commonly used to understand and
to assess children’s progress in a context of curriculum delivery and to guide
practitioners in planning a rich learning environment.
It is interesting that Kayal (2020) mentions links between creativity and the
child’s relationships with teachers and parents. The impact from relationships
upon learning indicates that development can be supported within formal and
informal environments which may have different cultures. Culture can exist
within a local setting, such as a family home or nursery, or it can be asso
ciated with a particular talent or embedded within the expectations of a
nation. Creativity should be embraced and used to forge links between areas
of learning. Diversity among siblings and peers should be celebrated in order
to recognise unique attributes as positive.
Csikszentmihalyi et al. (2018) emphasised the complexity of the psycholo
gical aspects of flow theory and applied the systems model of creativity to
promote understanding. A cyclical process occurs as a child experiences flow
theory. He uses intrinsic motivation to achieve goals which are based upon
creative ideas. This theory indicates that genes may predispose an individual
to a high level of learning, but creativity is regarded as a social construction
that embraces all stages of cognitive development. Creativity is recognised as
a child learning and demonstrating application of existing domain knowledge
which is enriched by his original ideas. Prior knowledge in a particular
domain provides a framework for the scaffolding of creativity. Csikszentmi
halyi et al. (2018) commented that creativity may be identified within a spe
cific context through demonstration of cultural relevance and improvement to
a child’s previous creations.
Opportunities for creativity are regularly incorporated within parenting pro-
grammes and also support primary carers to explore their own identity which is
based upon interests, needs, and spontaneous ideas. The National Scientific
Council on the Developing Child (2020) recently published research that portrays
the importance of brain development in the pre-natal period and postnatal years of
life to long-term success and health in adulthood. The conclusion of this investi
gation declares that it is never too late to reduce risk to development. This research
upholds the implementation of parenting work by services with members of the
extended family, for example, a third generation in the context of grandparents.
Individuality and developmental progress can be observed by the adapta
tions of children and adults to situations and evidence of their maturity and
experience during ongoing interactions with the environment. An opposing
influence upon developmental progress is resistance to change (Rathunde &
Csikszentmihalyi, 2006). Rathunde and Csikszentmihalyi described equilibrium
in human systems as being affected by boredom and anxiety. An individual can
overcome boredom by seeking out challenges, and he can overcome anxiety by
increasing skills and resilience which give him greater choices and a sense of
control. The former entails creativity within a process of seeking novelty. The
latter entails problem-solving and reliance upon current skills or scaffolding
which culminates in an increase in understanding.
Ability, capacity, and creativity 143
The following two studies illustrate the importance of the adult–child rela
tionship to a young child’s interpretation, understanding, and involvement
within a learning environment (Bernier et al., 2012; Nermeen et al., 2010). In
2012, Bernier and a research team identified three distinct dimensions of
parental involvement with their children’s learning in the formative years:
1 Sensitive responding.
2 Mindfulness.
3 Promotion of the child’s autonomy.
The study focused upon children from birth to 2 years. The brain grows
rapidly during the first two years of life, and it attains 90 per cent of the size
of an adult brain during this period. The findings indicated that stimulation
during the first two years directly impacted the infant’s frontal brain devel
opment. Additionally, mindfulness of a parent enhanced the response to an
infant and supported his self-regulation within contexts of emotionally chal
lenging situations. The research concluded that infants, who are securely
attached to a primary carer, are more competent in transferring their skill sets
to circumstances out-with the dyadic relationship.
Nermeen et al. (2010) conducted research on potential links between par
ental involvement and academic and social development in the early stage of
school attendance. Findings indicated that if parents adopted the values and
expectations of a school then there was a direct and positive impact upon
their child’s behaviour. Consistency and continuity of expectations and
boundaries were also key influences that supported children’s autonomy. The
authors suggested that an indirect impact may result from an increase in the
child’s motivation to engage with learning and a subsequent increase in aca
demic achievement.
Divergent thinking
Divergent thinking leads to a new base of knowledge and understanding from
an original foundation termed the child’s inner working model, as described
by Bowlby (1997). As a child explores an environment, or a concept, he uses
information from his current inner working model to interpret, and to
respond. The child’s memory, and imagination, are activated during these
processes, and neural links are made to prior knowledge on similar topics.
Cognitive skill supports the brain to merge these links, and to formulate new
ideas that lead to the child interpreting the concept from a different perspec
tive. This new perspective reconfigures the child’s inner working model, and
his original foundation of knowledge and understanding alters. Rathunde &
Csikszentmihalyi (2006) used the term “creative thinking” to describe the
various steps and “closure” to indicate completion of this cognitive process.
In an early years setting, it is important that children are offered multiple
choices of materials to support divergent thinking, and creativity. Choices
should always include familiar and unfamiliar items.
across the corner to create a hideout. Large soft rubber animals nestle in
amongst a mat of artificial wiry grass: elephants, tigers, giraffes, hippopota
mus, crocodiles, and a solitary sheep which has escaped from the farm box.
The surface has been dotted with round ladybird leaves that act as little
seats for the children.
The two friends peep into the box and open their eyes wide as they
exchange excited glances. Mehwish jumps up and down, and the intrepid
Natalia stretches her hand to retrieve some kitchen-roll holders. In seconds,
she has placed a holder onto the head of each animal, obscuring their faces,
and Natalia declares to her peer, “Putting on their hats.” There is no need for
the girls in this learning partnership to use long sentences as the core
information is shared. Additionally, communication between friends always
contains understanding which is sourced from knowledge of one another
and the focus upon shared interests. Mehwish nods vigorously in agree
ment, and the little girl covers her eyes as she laughs, “They can’t see any
more.” Divergent thinking in an instance.
Convergent thinking
Convergent thinking occurs when the brain identifies similarities in knowl
edge and understanding of an environment or concept in relation to previous
experience. This array of information from the child’s inner working model is
sorted and merged during problem-solving to result in a consensus. A young
child can be observed in a nursery, or home setting using this base of infor
mation to influence his response to a practical problem. Key workers or par
ents can often recall past experiences which have informed a child’s inner
working model and subsequently lead to a demonstration of convergent
thinking. In an early years setting, this understanding of a child’s learning
processes is important to inform curricular planning and implementation
within a playroom or outdoor setting.
During the past two decades, the presentation of educational choices that
respond to children’s interests has been identified as a significant influence
upon engagement and involvement with learning. Reflection on evidence-
gathering and identification of links to progress are essential aspects of pro
fessional development. Planning an environment to support delivery of a
curriculum, and ultimately attainment, is a key skill within the early years
services. Regular records by key workers on each child’s progress should be
one aspect of evidence-gathering that can be linked directly to a curriculum
room planner and evaluation of provision for each child in addition to
assessment of the overall service.
Lloyd and Howe (2003) conducted a study on potential links between dif
ferent types of solitary play and convergent or divergent thinking. There are
146 Ability, capacity, and creativity
many factors that can lead a child to choose solitary play: age and stage of
development, environmental prompts, personality, adversity, or lower and
higher ability than peers. Solitary play was categorised by these authors into
three aspects: active, passive, or reticent, in which the child demonstrated
little emotion. Participants in this study were 72 children aged 4½ years of
age. Findings indicated that active solitary play was positively associated with
divergent thinking. Solitary play that was accompanied by reticent behaviour was
negatively associated with convergent and divergent thinking. This study indicates
that children demonstrate divergent thinking through use of intrinsic motivation,
as portrayed by this research context of active solitary play, and creativity.
Evaluative thinking
Evaluative thinking relates to a child taking time away from practical inter
action, albeit momentarily, to reflect upon newly acquired knowledge and
understanding. The evaluative process supports creation of memories and
retention of information as patterns of exploration and problem-solving.
Neural networks need time and appropriate conditions to make connections,
and busy, noisy playrooms are not always conducive to evaluative thinking.
Breakout areas, for example, tents and quiet corners, and reducing the intru
sion of external noises, can positively influence evaluative thinking in young
children. Specific periods for evaluation are incorporated into education and
care settings and termed thinking and recall times.
Runco (1991) conducted an interesting study in the 1990s, in which he
researched evaluative thinking with over 100 young schoolchildren. This
researcher wanted to determine if the use of creativity was intentional or
unintentional. It seems that creativity is intentional if shown to link with
evaluative thinking by a child. Findings indicated correlation between diver
gent thinking which inherently involved creativity and evaluative ability.
Additionally, it was noted that evaluation by a child was influenced positively
by a teacher’s questions. This finding suggests that open questioning can be
used as a positive strategy to promote evaluation as an aspect of learning.
Reflection spaces, circle time, news time, and nurture groups are current
descriptors of practical responses which are commonly used in early years
settings and primary schools. These strategies encourage divergent thinking,
creativity, and evaluative ability.
References
Barton, J., & Pretty, J. (2010). What is the best dose of nature and green exercise for
improving mental health? A multi-study analysis. Environmental Science Technol
ogy, 44(10), 3947–3955.
Bergen, D., Schroer, J., & Woodin, M. (2018). Brain research in education and the
social sciences: Implications for practice, parenting, and future society. Abingdon and
New York: Routledge.
Ability, capacity, and creativity 147
Bernier, A., Carlson, S., Deschenes, M., & Matt-Gagne, C. (2012). Social factors in
the development of early executive functioning: A closer look at the caregiving
environment. Developmental Science, 15(1), 12–24.
Blumenfeld, P. C., Marx, R., & Harris, C. J. (2006). Learning environments. In W.
Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. IV, Child psy
chology in practice (pp. 297–342). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Bowlby, J. (1997). Attachment and loss. London: Pimlico.
Bremner, J. D., Elzinga, B., Schmahl, C., & Vermetten, E. (2008). Structural and
functional plasticity of the human brain in posttraumatic stress disorder. Progress in
Brain Research, 167, 171–186.
Bremner, J. D., Randall, I. P., Vermetten, E., Staib, L., Bronen, R. A., Mazure, C.,
Capelli, S., McCarthy, G., Innis, R. B., & Charney, D. S. (1997). Magnetic reso
nance imaging-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress
disorder related to childhood physical and sexual abuse: A preliminary report. Bio
logical Psychiatry, 41(1), 23–32.
Corraliza, J. A., Collado, S., & Bethelmy, L. (2012). Nature as a moderator of stress in
urban children. Social and Behavioural Sciences, 38, 253–263.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., Montijo, M. N., & Mouton, A. R. (2018). Flow theory: Opti
mising elite performance in the creative realm. In S. I. Pfeiffer (Ed.), APA handbook
of giftedness and talent (pp. 215–229). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Fidler, D. (2006). The emergence of a syndrome-specific personality profile in young
children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 10(2), 53–60.
Fosha, D. (2003). Dyadic regulation and experiential work with emotion and related
ness in trauma and disorganised attachment. In M. F. Solomon & D. J. Siegel
(Eds.), Healing trauma: Attachment, mind, body, and brain. London: W. W. Norton
& Company.
Freeman, J. (1998). Educating the very able: Current international research. London:
The Stationery Office.
Hart, H., & Rubia, K. (2012). Neuroimaging of child abuse: A critical review. Fron
tiers in human neuroscience. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2012.
00052/full.
Harter, S. (2006). The self. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child
psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 505–570). Hoboken,
NJ: Wiley & Sons.
Jawer, M. (2005). Environmental sensitivity: A neurobiological phenomenon? Seminars
in Integrative Medicine 3(3), 104–109. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/
222409869_Environmental_Sensitivity_A_Neurobiological_Phenomenon.
Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative frame
work. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, 169–182.
Kaplan, S., & Berman, M. (2010). Executive function and self-regulation. Association
for Psychological Science, 5(1), 43–57.
Kayal, N. G. (2020) Supporting of gifted children’s psychosocial developments in pre
school period. Psychology Research on Education and Social Sciences 1(1), 25–30.
King, P., & Sturrock, G. (2020). The play cycle: Theory, research, and application.
Abingdon and New York: Routledge.
King, P., & Temple, S. (2020). A review of the play cycle. In P. King & G. Sturrock
(Eds.), The play cycle: theory, research, and application (pp. 14–38). Abingdon and
New York: Routledge.
148 Ability, capacity, and creativity
Kuo, F. E., & Taylor, A. F. (2004). A potential natural treatment for attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder: Evidence from a national study. American Journal of Public
Health, 94 (9), 1580–1586.
Lloyd, B., & Howe, N. (2003). Solitary play and convergent and divergent thinking
skills in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 18(1), 22–41.
Louv, R. (2005). Last child in the woods: Saving our children from nature-deficit dis
order. New York: Algonquin Books of Chapel Hill.
McCoach, D. B., & Flake, J. K. (2018). The role of motivation. In S. I. Pfeiffer (Ed.),
APA handbook of giftedness and talent (pp. 201–213). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
McCrory, E., De Brito, S. A., & Viding, E. (2011). The impact of child maltreatment:
A review of neurobiological and genetic factors, Frontiers in Psychiatry, 2: 48.
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. (2020). Connecting the brain to
the rest of the body: Early childhood development and lifelong health are deeply
intertwined. Working paper no. 15. http://developingchild.harvard.edu/wp-content/
uploads/2020/06/wp15_health_FINAL_061520.pdf.
Nermeen, E., Nokali, E., Bachman, H. J., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2010). Parent’s
involvement and children’s academic and social development in elementary school.
Child Development, 81(3), 988–1005.
Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion, Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242–249.
Ohly, H., White, M., & Wheeler, B. (2016). Attention Restoration Theory: A sys
tematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural envir
onments. Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health – Part B: Critical
Reviews. https://doi.org/10.1080/10937404.2016.1196155.
Panksepp, J. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorders, psychostimulants, and
intolerance of childhood playfulness: A tragedy in the making? Current Directions
in Psychological Science 7 (3), 91–98.
Perry, B. (1997). Incubated in terror: neurodevelopmental factors in the cycle of vio
lence. In J. Osofsky (Ed.), Children, youth and violence: The search for solutions (pp.
124–148). New York: Guilford Press.
Pfeiffer, S. I., & Petscher, Y. (2008). Identifying young, gifted children using the gifted
rating scales: Preschool/kindergarten form. Gifted Child Quarterly, 52(1). https://
journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0016986207311055.
Plucker, J., & Barab, S. A. (2005). The importance of contexts in theories of giftedness:
learning to embrace the messy joys of subjectivity. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E.
Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness (pp. 201–216). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Hornuny, K., & Reed, A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in
faces: Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology,
36, 679–688.
Rathunde, K., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2006). The developing person: An experiential
perspective. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology:
Theoretical models of human development, vol. I (pp. 465–515). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley
& Sons.
Renzulli, J. S. (1978). What makes giftedness? Re-examining a definition. Phi Delta
Kappan 60(3), 180–184.
Renzulli, J. S. (2019). A practical system for identifying gifted and talented students. In
J. T. Pardeck & J. W. Murphy (Eds.), Young gifted children: Identification,
Ability, capacity, and creativity 149
programming and socio-psychological issues (pp. 9–18). Abingdon and New York:
Routledge.
Renzulli, J. S., & Reis, S. M. (2018). The three-ring conception of giftedness: A devel
opmental approach for promoting creative productivity in young people. In S. I.
Pfeiffer (Ed.), APA handbook of giftedness and talent (pp. 185–200). Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Ritzi, R. M., & Ray, D. C. (2017). Intensive short-term child-centred play therapy and
externalising behaviours in children. International Journal of Play Therapy, 26(1),
33–46.
Robinson, C., & Brown, A. M. (2016). Considering sensory processing issues in
trauma affected children: The physical environment in children’s residential homes.
Scottish Journal of Residential Childcare, 15(1).
Roedell, W. C. (1989). Early development of gifted children. In J. Van Tassel-Basja &
P. Olszewski-Kublius (Eds.), Patterns of influence on gifted learners, the home, the
self and the school (pp. 13–28). New York: Teachers College Press.
Roedell, W. C. (1984). Vulnerabilities of highly gifted children. Roeper Review, 6(3),
127–130.
Roisman, G. I., Padron, E., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2002). Earned–secure
attachment status in retrospect and prospect. Child Development, 73 (4), 1204–1219.
Runco, M. A. (1991). Dimensions of divergent thinkers. In Divergent thinking (pp. 87–
134). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Ruskin, E. M., Kasari, C., Mundy, P. & Sigman, M. (1994). Attention to people and
toys during social and object mastery in children with Down syndrome. American
Journal on Mental Retardation, 99, 103–111.
Scottish Government. (2010). Pre-birth to three: Positive outcomes for Scotland’s chil
dren and families. Edinburgh: Scottish Government.
Siegel, D. (2003). An interpersonal neurobiology of psychotherapy: The developing
mind and the resolution of trauma. In M. F. Solomon & D. J. Siegel (Eds.), Healing
trauma: Attachment, mind, body, and brain. London: W. W. Norton & Company.
Stern, D. N. (1998). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis
and developmental psychology. London: Karnac.
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (2006). Dynamic systems theory. In W. Damon & R.
Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. I (pp. 258–312). Hoboken, NJ:
John Wiley & Sons.
Vaivre-Douret, L. (2011). Developmental and cognitive characteristics of “high-level
potentialities” (highly gifted) children. International Journal of Paediatrics, 1687–
9740, 420297.
Whitters, H. G. (2019). Attainment and executive functioning in the early years.
Abingdon and New York: Routledge.
Zhou, M., & Brown, D. (2015). Educational learning theories, 2nd edition, Education
Open Textbooks, 1. https://oer.galileo.usg.edu/education-textbooks/1.
6 Research, theory, and intervention
1 The baby will use signalling to indicate his desire for positive interaction,
rooting for milk, seeking his mother’s eye contact or being still as he lis
tens and senses his mother in the early stages post-birth.
2 He will use aversive behaviours, for example, crying and screaming, to
prompt his carer to respond to his needs.
3 Over time, the infant will use active intentional behaviours and move
towards his attachment figure.
Person–environment interaction
Bronfenbrenner (2005) explored the theory of human development, and he
proposed that family was the most significant influence in the earliest years
and impactful throughout adult life. According to Bronfenbrenner, family is
defined as a group of people who have unconditional commitment to one
154 Research, theory, and intervention
another’s well-being. It is useful to read this theorist’s views from a practi
tioner’s perspective. Bronfenbrenner focused upon the operational aspects of the
parent–child dyad and influence from a third party. He describes the third party
as composed of friends, community, or organisations that facilitate optimum
effect from the two-person system of parent and child. In working with families
over many years, I have found that each parent–child dyad has a distinct identity
which is steeped in a family culture influenced by historical and current experi
ences. The dyad evolves over time, and the relationship has immense potential to
support development of the two members: parent and child.
In a recent publication, Nagy and Nagy (2022) refer to the skilful adapta
tion of working conditions within a context of the COVID-19 pandemic in
Hungary. During lockdown periods, role-modelling of physical and emotional
nurturing by early years practitioners was replaced by the use of information
technology. This medium provided an aid to maintain communication
between services and home. Key workers used the format of Facebook to
upload videos of activities that promoted parent–child interaction within the
home environment, and it supported the family’s processing of trauma asso
ciated with the pandemic. This study highlighted the ingenuity of practi
tioners in responding to the unprecedented crisis of COVID-19 in addition to
the importance of person-to-person interactions.
Research by Duffy et al. (2021) investigated the use of a screening tool for
the long-term mental-health condition of post-traumatic stress disorder
(PTSD). The participant group was 141 young people who had experienced
maltreatment in childhood. Findings indicated three items which were asso
ciated with PTSD as a history of being on the child-protection register, prior
mental-health issues, and interpersonal trauma. These authors reported an
unexpected finding as data from the young people that identified the death or
loss of a close relationship as a traumatic experience. This research indicates
the importance of interpersonal relationships to reduce the impact of loss.
Tal (2021) recently highlighted four main influential concepts upon
development.
Intervention strategies
In a context of infant mental health, the attachment relationship from child to
adult, and the bonding relationship from adult to child, are key aspects of
156 Research, theory, and intervention
healthy development and resilience to adversities. Barlow (2016) described
several factors of significance to the strengthening of an attachment bond
based upon the principles by Simpson and Belsky (2008): a parent’s sensitivity
to an infant’s needs and emotional reactions, the quality of attunement from
parent to infant, and parental ability to reflect and adapt behaviour to an
infant’s presentation, and interactions within a midrange context. In curri
cular guidance, the term “sensitive responding” is often used to promote the
behaviour, emotion, and intent of an adult’s reflective interaction to an
infant’s needs and preferences.
Berlin et al. (2008) identified three approaches that could accumulatively
respond to attachment needs of infants. The three approaches are inter
dependent and incorporated into many formal and informal intervention
contexts:
Theory of development
Bronfenbrenner (2005) investigated the concept of developmental outcomes
and explored the establishment of mental processes. Developmental outcomes
occur from the joint functioning of a person and his proximal or distal
environment. There are many similarities among the neural workings of
human beings; however, the importance of personal characteristics upon
subjective interpretation, and actions, was consistently promoted by Bronfen
brenner. This theorist described changes that occur in the development of a
person between systems as ecological transitions (Bronfenbrenner, 1979).
Influences that prompt these changes are known as “variable factors”.
Patterns of motivation and actions create developmental trajectories which can
be observed within different settings, for example, home or nursery, indoor or
outdoor play. The trans-contextual dyad of adult and child supports transference
of knowledge and application within different circumstances. Bronfenbrenner
(2005) applied the term “mesosystem phenomenon” as the changes gained
developmental validity by occurring in more than one setting. A secure attach
ment relationship is a key influence upon this event of ecological transitioning.
Bronfenbrenner indicated that momentum gave meaning and perpetuity to short-
term molecular behaviours which were categorised as stimulus-responses or
transitory actions. These behaviours subsequently transformed into longer-term
molar activities if the conditions were conducive.
Bronfenbrenner (2005) researched the concept of variance. He believed that
variance between human beings related to a heritability system. This means
that genetic influences are actualised into observable phenomena. In the con
text of an early years nursery, a practitioner conducts daily observations on
key children, and she rapidly accumulates examples of influences upon the
child’s functioning although evidence of a genetic link is unconfirmed. Varia
tion is demonstrated by joint functioning of proximal processes and char
acteristics. The child’s actions are influenced by his personality and specific
158 Research, theory, and intervention
strengths or weaknesses. Practitioners are well aware of variance through
observing different actions, behaviour, and emotional reactions from siblings
to the same family circumstances, and to their involvement with the learning
environment of a playroom.
Influences may be formative life events or the predispositions which are
based upon personal characteristics of the individual. Examples are genetic
defects, congenital damage, severe and long-term illness, in addition to a high
level of ability, and capacity which is built upon good health at birth, genetic
potential, and learning opportunities. A change in mental health can be the
result of these issues, and this is regarded as a triggering event to alteration of
the child or parent’s interpretation, perceptions, and reaction to a proximal
environment. Bronfenbrenner (2005) termed the outcome of these develop
mental processes as having a social stimulus value, and his work continues to
increase comprehension of negative and positive influences which are relevant
to today’s society.
Bronfenbrenner and Morris (2006) explored indirect impact from a defi
ciency, for example, a low birthweight that could reduce a child’s capacity to
engage with learning. Low birthweight can be associated with many adverse
childhood experiences, including poverty and poor health of the mother.
Research has also indicated that stress exhibited by a baby in adverse circum
stances will result in a stress response by a parent whereas a baby who demon
strates increasing competence and social skills will prompt a parent to provide
stimulation and lead to reciprocal interaction. Parenting work with primary
carers and their children is a relevant and valuable response to these issues.
Clemens et al. (2020) recently conducted research on an extensive sample
group of over 2,000 parents in Germany. The results showed that parents who
had experienced adverse childhood experiences exhibited a higher acceptance
of negative behaviours in their own parenting roles. This study focused speci
fically upon behaviours which had resulted in a head trauma upon the child.
Findings indicated that a mother’s resilience to a crying baby might be
reduced if her personal memory of childhood stress was associated with
negative responses from her own parents. Memories retained from early
childhood experiences shape interactions between adults and children within
each generation.
Therapeutic alliance
Cain (2010) noted that the skill of a third party could facilitate development
of others, and he promoted the therapeutic relationship as an effective
medium for communication. Twenty years previously, Carl Rogers indicated
one such skill as the ability to enter the perceptual world of a child, and he
applied the term “therapeutic alliance” (Rogers, 1990). The Scottish Govern
ment (2009) describe this alliance as contributing to a healing process within
the body and mind of parents and children. Hope and potential for change
are key messages gained from research on these topics.
Research, theory, and intervention 159
Several authors have studied the therapeutic relationship in a context of
parenting support. An extensive literature review was conducted by Moran
and a research team in 2004, and it projected an important message in
response to issues that affected the capacity and motivation of a carer to
engage with parenting support (Moran et al., 2004). The use of relational
skills to engage parents was a common factor that featured in the findings of
this review. A previous study by Benjamin and Karpiak (2001) had indicated
that this type of relationship also had a positive effect upon personality sta
bilisation. Braun et al. (2006) commented that the therapeutic alliance could
be used by a parent to activate support from a professional in response to
current and potential needs which were forecast.
Carl Rogers (1990) studied in the field of psychotherapy, but parallels can
be drawn with his findings to the professional–parent relationship in the early
years sector. Rogers identified six necessary conditions to the creation of an
alliance between professional and a client (Whitters, 2015).
1 Oral questions: The use of open questions, prompts, and probes to further
the child’s knowledge and to support comprehension by tailoring inter
actions to suit current capacity and ability.
2 Feedback: The provision of feedback that responds to each child’s output
can provide scaffolding of knowledge in verbal format, and it builds upon
the current base of information which is held by a child.
3 Linking with prior attainment: This strategy supports a child to recognise, and
to use his current base of knowledge and understanding to advance to the next
level. Learning follows an iterative pathway in response to the child’s ability.
4 Scaffolding: An effective and common practice throughout many different
learning environments that requires an adult or peer to use encourage
ment, specific ideas, role-modelling, prompts, and probes, and to demon
strate an interest in the child’s interests to further development.
164 Research, theory, and intervention
5 Outcome: It may be necessary to have a range of outcomes for children
who present at different stages of development. This approach requires an
educator to consider each task as composed of many aspects and to
determine appropriate goals/outcomes which can reflect every child’s
capacity, ability, and recognise attainment.
A catalyst for learning is the secure relationship between child and a suppor
tive adult. Good mental health empowers the young child to seek out oppor
tunities, to make effective social choices, and to gain satisfaction and
fulfilment from furthering knowledge and understanding. The home environ
ment presents a rich cultural experience for learning within a family unit. It is
a venue in which children can observe and adopt values, attitudes, and beliefs
of parents and gain opportunities for accelerated and enriched learning with
older siblings. Supporting children in the home environment does not need to
entail formal educational experiences, but parents, brothers, and sisters can
utilise everyday resources to activate intrinsic motivation in a child.
� Moving each pencil up and down, little ticks and big ticks, filling each
white page.
� Selecting a sheet of paper, listening, and counting as she follows the
noise of the spiral notebook releasing a page, hole by hole.
� Folding the paper along its faint printed lines, using both hands as tools,
a firm press and creativity is activated in the 3-year-old explorer.
The early stages of literacy are fun to learn in a play environment and
gained easily within the context of a positive sibling relationship on a rainy,
lockdown Saturday afternoon at home.
Learning environments should promote intrinsic rather than extrinsic
reward and activate the child’s curiosity and motivation to learn within a
cultural context. Creativity should be embraced and used to forge links
between areas of learning. Diversity should be celebrated in order to recog
nise unique attributes as positive.
A child requires a broad understanding of the world – not just facts and
information, but many opportunities to experience and to apply what he has
learned in practice. The young learner will use objects in different ways and
gain understanding of concepts such as size, weight, height, and length. Most
nurseries set up defined areas of play which are designed to promote con
textual experiences within a playroom, for example, a home corner, book
corner, an arts and crafts area, a cognitive area, and physical play. A child
can independently extend his learning by linking adjacent toys and materials
in response to interests and needs and to create something new and personal.
Inclusive pedagogy must include opportunities for children of low and high
abilities and many different stages in-between. Responsive staff can nurture
positive relationships and provide rich opportunities to support formation of
a child’s neural connections.
During the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic, children in UK nurseries
were restricted to several small groups called bubbles (Early Learning and
Childcare Directorate, 2020). The rationale for these circumstances was based
upon necessity for an increase in hygiene, reduction of human interaction,
and minimal use of artefacts in order to decrease the risk of virus transfer.
Settings scaled down the range of choices for children and compartmentalised
items for access by each bubble group. The resources and learning environ
ment were sterilised after contact with every group of children. Two years
later, these routines continue to be implemented in relation to the enduring
nature of this pandemic and COVID guidance.
An unexpected outcome of this presentation of learning materials was an
increase in the imaginative and creative play of children. The children used
the limited resources to achieve a deeper level of learning by repeatedly
exploring concepts over time and problem-solving by using materials for dif
ferent functions. It was also observed that children were initiating interactive
play more frequently with their peers, and they developed greater compre
hension of sharing and turn-taking. It appeared that fewer external choices
prompted the children to access implicit memories, leading to an increase in
motivation, creativity, and group interactions. These anecdotal findings are
important to consider, and current research is ongoing to explore the effect of
COVID-19 upon education and care in the early years of childhood (Moore
& Churchill, 2020).
Play cycle
The play cycle refers to the processes that occur during play and include the
play flow (King & Sturrock, 2020). The child expresses play cues externally
which are based upon internal influences in relation to his interests, needs,
and emotions. These play cues may also be given direction or prompts from
another person or the proximal environment. The research findings by King
and Sturrock (2020) indicated that play cues would reduce over time unless
strengthened by interactions with another child or adult or an aspect of the
environment. The distal environment can also affect the direction of play cues
through sensory stimulation which links to familiar concepts. For example,
the distant sound of an emergency vehicle’s siren can induce specific cues, and
it can inform imaginative actions.
The child’s knowledge and understanding that is gained from interaction
between his play cue and another person or environmental stimulus is termed
the “play return”. During observations by key workers, and analysis of each
child’s play, it can be useful to consider the source of the play return from a
behavioural, environmental, or social cue. The play return is processed by the
168 Research, theory, and intervention
child who chooses to extend the play or to introduce another play cue; thus, a
play cycle is completed (King & Sturrock, 2020).
There are six elements to a play cycle as termed by King and Sturrock
(2020): metalude, play cue, play return, play frame, loop and flow, and anni
hilation. The metalude refers to the child’s drive to participate in play. Inter
nal influences result in his external expression through a play cue of actions
and potentially words. If a response is not readily available to activate his play
cue to a subsequent stage of the play cycle, then the child may issue a second
cue, withdraw from play, or react with negative emotion (annihilation). If a
response is available (play return), then a play frame is created. This frame
may encompass physical and psychological elements. The play frame con
tributes to the child’s learning and induces an increase in knowledge, under
standing, and experiential learning. Developmental outcomes are gained
through reciprocal interaction of the child’s play cues and person or environ
mental feedback. These aspects inform the inner working model of the child
and encompass the loop and flow stage of the play cycle. Play cues and
returns will continue unless the child withdraws or this particular cycle is
ended by another person or factor in the environment or social boundary.
The adult’s role and responsibilities within a play cycle should be adapted
to enrich the capacity and abilities of every child, and the ultimate outcome is
a high level of well-being and involvement with a learning environment. For
example, a practitioner may introduce a play cue to a child through role
modelling and support interpretation of an environment, particularly in con
texts which are unfamiliar. During delivery of a therapeutic intervention
(Bratton et al., 2006), I have found that children who have experienced
trauma often require play cues to be introduced by the facilitator before par
ticipation in this context can commence. The important factor is the outcome
of encouraging play and learning.
King and Sturrock (2020) commented upon this phenomenon within an
investigation on cues for playful behaviour in an adult organisational meet
ing. Potential cues were provided on a material level by the use of objects and
sweets which were displayed openly to employees within the environmental
context. Findings indicated that these artefacts were associated with light
hearted behaviour from the adults in the meeting and regarded as cues or
stimulation that led to frivolous behaviour in this particular context.
The play frame in services occurs within contexts that reflect social
boundaries, health, and safety requirements, and respond to additional sup
port for learning needs. It is necessary that the practitioner has a responsi
bility to have a “holding” role in the play frame. King and Sturrock (2020)
applied this term within a discussion of data from a study in the UK. The
research focused upon children’s behaviour and practitioners’ interpretation
of play cues (Nottingham City Council & Russell, 2006). Findings had indi
cated that playworkers often misinterpreted play cues as challenging beha
viour. The study concluded that adults have a role to play in maintaining the
play frame in accordance with social and safety boundaries relevant to each
Research, theory, and intervention 169
setting and to the needs of each child. A description of containment was
given by the authors to represent the adult role in supporting a child to adapt
his play frame as necessary but to maintain the integrity of the child’s original
plan.
Containment involves the use of a broad skill set by a practitioner or parent
which includes observation, interpretation, emotional literacy, assessment of
risk, awareness of the child’s interests and needs, and, ultimately, responsive
intervention. The adult creates a dyad with a child, and he may be allocated an
active or passive role within the play cycle. It is often the case that an adult’s role
fluctuates throughout an interaction and encompasses tentative steps by the
adult and responsive, prompt reaction to the child’s communications.
In 2020, research was conducted that reviewed the understanding of prac
titioners regarding the play cycle (King & Newstead, 2020). Findings indi
cated that comprehension was dependent on the source of practitioner’s
knowledge of this cycle and experience in the field. The use of pre-cue as
explanatory for the term “metalude” has been inserted into the discussion by
these authors, and it supports comprehension of the complex processes for
practitioners in the early years and primary sector.
Play flow
Csikszentmihalyi (1975) defined flow as a holistic sensation and associated
this experience with focused involvement in learning. This researcher com
mented that the term “flow” refers to the consistent nature of a person’s
actions as interactions take place with an environment or other people. This
autotelic experience is led by the person’s motivation in a particular context
which can be related to interests, emotions, curiosity, creativity, and a desire
to gain knowledge and understanding.
There is a significant characteristic of flow which was originally identified
by Csikszentmihalyi (1975) and currently features in practitioner guidance in
the early years and play work. Continuation of the play flow does not need a
reward which is external to the child. The motivation to continue this
experiential learning is driven by the child’s inner working model. Csikszent
mihalyi (1990) believed that the function of consciousness is for the inner
working model to support each person to make sense of the internal and
external world through use of prior knowledge, understanding, and emotional
reactions. Emotions lead interpretation and contribute to the formation of
perceptions which may differ for each person. A lack of consciousness relates
to an immature sense of self and a child leading his life through the use of
instinct and reflexes.
The flow can be interrupted if the child reflects upon the context, reviews
his capacity, and alters his plan. For example, a child may confidently com
mence climbing upwards upon a wall frame driven by his internal motivation
which maintains his physical competence. However, if the child stops and
considers the height, danger, and isolation, then these negative interpretations
170 Research, theory, and intervention
can affect or halt the play flow on a temporary or longer-term basis. A child’s
focus upon negative influences can lead to disorder in his inner working
model which affects interpretation of the environment and reaction to stimuli.
An external prompt in the form of encouragement and empowerment from
another person can support the immersion back into the play flow. Csiks
zentmihalyi (1990) commented that individuals who achieve a flow experi
ence, despite adversities, demonstrate intrinsic motivation and are not easily
distracted by external influences.
A process that follows flow is differentiation, and integration takes place
throughout the learning experience. The infant or young child has an increase
in capability and skill, and his thoughts and actions are integrated by a spe
cific focus. The ability to capitalise upon personal skills, in order to maximise
outcomes, depends on a child’s capacity, which relates to his mental health
and emotional status. Comprehension of flow experience highlights the sig
nificance of infant mental health and the role of adults in supporting well
being.
Csikszentmihalyi (1990) described happiness in terms of a flow experience
in which each person’s skill set matched available learning opportunities. This
researcher described inner happiness as pertaining to harmony with oneself as
opposed to an individual gaining control over the environment or other
people. An activity is autotelic and becomes intrinsically rewarding and
independent of the social environment. This outcome may occur during a
child’s involvement with the environment and alongside his interactions
towards a planned goal.
Several theorists felt that the sensation and outcome of enjoyment was
characterised by the principle of novelty (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Magnusson
& Stattin, 2006). Attention is captured by curiosity, and it is a determining
factor in improving quality of experiences along the continuum of enjoyment.
Csikszentmihalyi (1990) identified major components of enjoyment, and these
aspects are used to evaluate and inform assessment of a child’s well-being and
involvement during intervention. The Leuven Involvement and Well-being
Scales (Laevers, 1994) are commonly used to interpret, to understand, and to
record these issues by practitioners. From the child’s perspective these com
ponents include:
Transitions
The movement from one learning environment to another is recognised as a
significant stage in a young child’s developmental pathway. Transitions are
not solely based upon familiarisation with the new environments but should
primarily consider relationships, the child’s sense of self, and potential change
within a new environment. The relationship is a transitional medium which is
essential to promote good mental health during the integration period. The
rationale for positive transitions is based upon the understanding that a child
should be supported within a secure attachment relationship to leave one area
and to enter a new area. His ability to engage with learning has to be main
tained and to be enhanced by the new circumstances in order that the transi
tion has a positive impact upon development. A positive or negative
transition can affect a child’s learning over a long period of time.
The key internal transition between age groups in a service marks an
important stage of development, and it is managed in accordance with the
personal needs and circumstances of each child. There are several aspects to
consider during the transition period. The child’s current, and receiving, key
worker should commence the process by planning the transition period with
parents, and colleagues, in response to the child’s needs. The child’s referral
details are discussed, stage of development, and child’s ability and capacity to
engage with a learning environment. The care plan is reviewed and updated.
172 Research, theory, and intervention
Each child’s personal interests, preferred medium of learning, and preferences
are shared between the appropriate staff members.
The transition is discussed with a parent in a context of information
exchange and collaboration. It is important to remember that the parent is
also experiencing a transition between relationships and environments. The
child is supported to prepare for leaving his current playroom by the use of
photographs, discussion, and joint interactions with the new key worker. The
child is supported to integrate into the new playroom alongside the new key
worker by using a personal photograph, welcome time, opportunities for
small and large group activities, inside and outside, and experiencing snack
time. Similar processes should be experienced by the transitioning parent. A
key strategy is supporting a child and his parent together and to project a
sense of belonging within the new environment through role identity and clear
expectations. Parents are given regular updates during this time and encour
aged to talk to their child about the transition.
Transitions incur a child coping with the loss of a familiar relationship and
the creation of a new secure attachment relationship. Five areas of develop
ment need to be considered in a context of transitions between, and within
services. Changes for the child are multiple during this period, and expecta
tions within each area of development should be established alongside the
relational support. The following information highlights five areas of devel
opment, expectations of skill sets, and support which is founded on the
attachment relationship.
1 Relationship-based practice.
2 Normal and atypical development.
3 Factors that influence caregiving capacity.
4 Assessment of caregiving.
5 Supporting caregiving.
6 Reflective practice and supervision.
7 Working within relevant legal and professional frameworks.
Final thoughts
I take time to reflect upon learning from my personal perspective as an early
years practitioner and researcher in the twilight of my career. Reflection upon
176 Research, theory, and intervention
individual and collaborative practice is a common strategy which is applied
on a daily basis to review and to develop implementation of a service and to
justify funding for specific projects (Moon, 2004). Reflection on professional
expertise and comprehension of a field are also mandatory aspects of regis
tration by an appointed body, for example, Scottish Social Services Council
(Scottish Social Services Council, 2003).
Reflective assessment is one aspect of a learning process that has to be
judged against a standard in order to gain value and to contribute to a fra
mework for development. Gibbs (1988) promotes a model of reflection which
is used regularly by practitioners in care and education, at times inadvertently.
The model encompasses review of current and past actions, and associated
emotions. This approach is significant to my work context of child protection.
Emotional impact can incentivise educators and support adherence to policy
and procedure, or result in subjective judgement and create a dichotomy
between professional and personal actions. It is essential to gain self-aware
ness during these processes.
Gaining sense and rationale for our actions is often achieved by consideration
of emotions which can be influenced by our own childhood experiences, culture
of a workplace, or mental health at a point in time. I have always used reflection
for myself as a positive tool in recognising professional skill and areas for devel
opment. Each person learns from reflection of self and others. Actions are based
upon teamwork, whether directly or indirectly. I feel that it is essential to widen
the reflective scope beyond the individual and to consider the curtailing influences
from strategic boundaries in addition to the freedom within operational skills of
practice. I have learned over the years that strategy binds, and leads, or inhibits
pedagogy and practice. However, practice without strategy and rationale may
appear to liberate the practitioner but diminishes quality due to lack of bound
aries and direction which can reduce consistency of a desired output.
The parent and infant form an interdependent dyad in the earliest stages of
life which is influential throughout the lifespan. Effective professional–parent
relationships are essential for the development of both generations and to
enable sensitive interpretation and responses to needs, emotions, and mental
health. The COVID-19 pandemic has resulted in many emotive conversations
being conducted outside service buildings in cold, wet, and challenging
environments. I feel humbled by the trust and respect which parents bestow
upon teams. We have all quickly adapted to COVID-19 regulations, and
strategies can be effective if responsive to need. It is important to notice, and
to capitalise upon, help-seeking overtures from parents.
The pandemic has certainly increased professional understanding of
empathic relationships and infant mental health. It is clear that the human
need for relationships continues to be at the forefront of our daily existence. A
therapeutic alliance can be created in any environment and can facilitate the
learning and development of parent and child.
A study published in 2021 reviewed 40 family intervention programmes
from the USA, Australia, UK, Sweden, Netherlands, Canada, Denmark,
Research, theory, and intervention 177
Finland, and France (Lagdon et al., 2021). Findings indicated that 25–50 per
cent of the children whose parents had a mental-health issue experienced a
psychological disorder during childhood or adolescence. Subsequently, 10–14
per cent of these children received a diagnosis of psychotic disorder during
childhood or adulthood. This study also links parental mental-health issues
and child abuse, as published by previous authors (Cleaver et al., 2011;
Finkelhor et al., 2015). The investigation concluded that a universal definition
of family-focused practice would encapsulate key strategies and rationale. Addi
tionally, despite a consistency of components across the 40 studies, and within
each country, greater emphasis on engagement with local community supports
was recommended by the authors.
Change which is embedded within a family and local community encom
passes factors of sustainability, and positive long-term outcomes. A valid
response to infant mental-health issues is joint working between adult
mental-health services and children’s services. Strengthening the links
between research, theory, and practice, in a context of change and develop
ment of family units would upskill the workforce in this field. Researchers,
practitioners, and parents are key contributors to supporting infant mental
health.
References
Association of Infant Mental Health, UK. (2021). The infant mental health compe
tency framework. https://aimh.uk/the-uk-imh-competency-framework.
Barlow, J. (2016). Improving attachment in babies: What works? Association of Infant
Mental Health best practice guidance no.2. https://aimh.uk/news-resources/mem
bers-only-resources.
Benjamin, L. S., & Karpiak, C. P. (2001). Personality disorders. Psychotherapy:
Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 38(4), 487–491. https://www.psycnet.apa.org/
record/2002-01390-023.
Berlin, L. J., Zeanah, C. H., & Lieberman, A. F. (2008). Prevention and intervention
programmes for supporting early attachment security. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver
(Eds.), Handbook of attachment, theory, research, and clinical applications, 2nd edi
tion (pp. 745–761). New York: The Guilford Press.
Bowlby, J. (1997). Attachment and loss. London: Pimlico.
Bratton, S. C., Landreth, G. L., Kellam, T., & Blackard, S. R. (2006). Child/parent
participation therapy treatment manual. New York: Routledge.
Braun, D., Davis, H., & Mansfield, P. (2006). How helping works: Towards a shared
model of process. London: The Centre for Parent and Child Support.
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (2008). Internal working models in attachment
relationships, elaborating a central construct in attachment theory. In J. Cassidy &
P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment, theory, research, and clinical applica
tions, 2nd edition (pp. 102–127). New York: The Guilford Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development, 2nd edition. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (2005). Making human beings human, biological perspectives on
human development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
178 Research, theory, and intervention
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human
development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology,
vol. I. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons.
Cain, D. J. (2010). Person-centred psychotherapies. Washington, DC: American Psy
chological Association.
Cleaver, H., & Freeman, P. (1995). Parental perspectives in cases of suspected child
abuse. London: Her Majesty’s Stationery Office.
Cleaver, H., Unell, I., & Aldgate, J. (2011). Children’s needs – parenting capacity: Child
abuse – parental mental illness, learning disability, substance misuse, and domestic
violence. London: TSO.
Clemens, V., Berthold, O., Witt, A., Brahler, E., Plener, P. L., & Fegert, J. M. (2020).
Childhood adversities and later attitudes towards harmful parenting behaviour
including shaking in a German population-based sample. Child Abuse Review, 29,
269–281.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety: The experience of play in
work and games. San Francisco, CA: Josey-Bass Inc. Publishers.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of experience. New York:
HarperCollins.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., Montijo, M. N., & Mouton, A. R. (2018). Flow theory: opti
mising elite performance in the creative realm. In S. I. Pfeiffer (Ed.), APA handbook
of giftedness and talent (pp. 215–229). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Doyle, C., & Cicchetti, D. (2017). From the cradle to the grave: The effect of adverse
caregiving environments on attachment and relationships throughout the lifespan
institute of child development. Clinical Psychology, Science, and Practice, 24, 203–217.
Duffy, M., Walsh, C., Mulholland, C., Davidson, G., Best, P., Bunting, L., Herron, S.,
Quinn, P., Gillanders, C., Sheehan, C., & Devaney, J. (2021). Screening children
with a history of maltreatment for PTSD in frontline social care organisations: An
explorative study. Child Abuse Review, 30(6), 594–611.
Early Learning and Childcare Directorate. (2020). Phase 1: Easing of lockdown in
Scotland. Edinburgh: Scottish Government.
Education Scotland. (2020). A summary of resources relating to highly able learners.
Livingston: Education Scotland.
Finkelhor, D., Shattuck, A., Turner, H., & Hamby, S. (2015). A revised inventory of
adverse childhood experiences. Child Abuse and Neglect, 48, 13–21. https://www.
unh.edu/ccrc/pdf/CV334.pdf.
Gibbs, G. (1988). Learning by doing: A guide to teaching and learning methods. Oxford:
Further Education Unit, Oxford Polytechnic.
Gross, J. J. (1999). Emotion and emotion regulation. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. Johns.
(Eds.), Handbook of personality: theory and research, 2nd edition (pp. 525–552).
New York: Guilford Press.
Hogg, S. (2019). Rare jewels, specialised parent-infant relationship teams in the UK.
https://parentinfantfoundation.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2019/09/PIPUK-Rare-Je
wels-FINAL.pdf.
King, P., & Newstead, S. (2020). Re-defining the play cycle: An empirical study of
playworkers’ understanding of play-work theory. Journal of Early Childhood
Research, 18(1), 99–111.
King, P., & Sturrock, G. (2020). The play cycle: theory, research, and application.
Abingdon and New York: Routledge.
Research, theory, and intervention 179
Klein, M. H., Kolden, G. G., Michels, J. L., & Chisholm-Stockard, S. (2002). Congruence.
In J. C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Laevers, F. (1994). The project experiential education: Concepts and experiences at level
of context, process, and outcome. Leuven: Leuven University.
Lagdon, S., Grant, A., Davidson, G., Devaney, J., Donaghy, M., Duffy, J., Galway, K., &
McCartan, C. (2021). Families with parental mental health problems: A systematic
narrative review of family-focused practice. Child Abuse Review, 30(5), 400–421.
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning, legitimate peripheral participation.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Magnusson, D., & Stattin, H. (2006). The person in context: A holistic-interactionist
approach. In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. I.
Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2008). Adult attachment and affect regulation. In J.
Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment, theory, research, and clin
ical applications, 2nd edition (pp. 503–531). New York: The Guilford Press.
Moon J. A. (2004). A handbook of reflective and experiential learning: theory and
practice. London: Routledge
Moore, E., & Churchill, G. (2020). Still here for children: Experiences of NSPCC staff
who supported children and families during COVID-19. https://learning.nspcc.org.
uk/research-resources/2020/still-here-for-children-experiences-of-nspcc-staff-during
coronavirus.
Moran, P., Ghate, D., & van der Merwe, A. (2004). What works in parenting support? A
review of the international evidence. London: Department for Education and Skills.
Nagy, A. V. & Nagy, V. (2022). A SARS COVID-19 vírus okozta helyzet a hazai kora
gyermekkori intézményekben [The effect of SARS-COVID-19 virus in Early Child
hood Education in Hungary]. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/357661708_
A_SARS_COVID-19_VIRUS_OKOZTA_HELYZET_A_HAZAI_KORA_GYER
MEKKORI_INTEZMENYEKBEN_The_Effect_of_SARS-COVID-19_VIRUS_
in_Early_Childhood_Education_in_Hungary.
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. (2020). Connecting the brain to
the rest of the body: Early childhood development and lifelong health are deeply
intertwined. Working paper no. 15. https://developingchild.harvard.edu/resources/
connecting-the-brain-to-the-rest-of-the-body-early-childhood-development-and-life
long-health-are-deeply-intertwined.
Nicholson, S. (1971). How not to cheat children: The theory of loose parts. Landscape
Architecture, 62(1), 30–34.
Nottingham City Council & Russell, W. (2006). Play-work impact evaluation: Final
report. Nottingham: Nottingham City Council.
O’Brien, R., & Mitchell, C. (2021). The role of early years practitioners in a perinatal
mental health service. International Journal of Birth and Parent Education 9(1), 33–
36. https://www.ijbpe.com/journals/volume-9/57-vol-9-issue-1
Rogers, C. (1990). Speaking personally. In H. Kirschenbaum & V. L. Henderson
(Eds.), The Carl Rogers reader (p. 16). London: Constable.
Ryecraft, J. R. (2019). Behind the walls of poverty: Economically disadvantaged gifted and
talented children. In J. T. Pardeck & J. W. Murphy (Eds.), Young gifted children, identifi
cation, programming, and socio-psychological issue (pp. 139–147). Abingdon: Routledge.
Scottish Government. (2009). Changing lives. Edinburgh: Scottish Government.
180 Research, theory, and intervention
Scottish Government. (2021). Child neglect in Scotland: Understanding causes and
supporting families, child protection committees, Scotland, October 2021. https://
www.gov.scot/publications/national-guidance-child-protection-scotland-2021.
Scottish Social Services Council. (2003). Codes of practice for social services workers
and employers, code 1. Dundee: Scottish Social Services Council.
Sigel, I. E. (2006). Research to practice redefined. In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Eds.),
Handbook of child psychology, child psychology in practice, vol. IV, 6th edition (pp.
1017–1023). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Simpson, J. A., & Belsky, J. (2008). Attachment theory within a modern evolutionary fra
mework. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment, theory, research,
and clinical applications. 2nd edition (pp. 131–157). New York: The Guilford Press.
Spencer, M. B. (2006). Phenomenology and ecological systems theory: Development of
diverse groups. In R. M. Lerner & W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology:
Theoretical models of human development (pp. 829–893). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley
& Sons.
Tal, C. (2021). An ecosystem perspective of practice and professional development in
early childhood education and care (ECEC). European Early Childhood Education
Research Journal, 29(6), 809–812.
Taylor, I. (2019). We need to talk about differentiation in schools. TES Magazine, 8
November. https://www.tes.com/magazine/article/we-need-talk-about-differentiation
schools.
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (2006). Dynamic systems theory. In W. Damon & R.
Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. I (pp. 258–312). Hoboken, NJ:
John Wiley & Sons.
Thompson, R. A. (2008). Early attachment and later development, familiar questions, new
answers. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment, theory, research,
and clinical applications, 2nd edition (pp. 348–365). New York: The Guilford Press.
Trotter, C. (2002). Worker skill and client outcome in child protection. Child Abuse
Review, 11, pp. 38–50.
UK Trauma Council. (2020). Beyond the pandemic: Strategic priorities for responding
to childhood trauma. A coronavirus pandemic policy briefing. https://uktrauma
council.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/09/Coronavirus-CYP-and-Trauma-UKTC-Policy
Briefing-Sept-2020.pdf.
Walsh, R. L., Hodge, K. A., Bowes, J. M., & Kemp, C. R. (2010). Same age, different
page: overcoming the barriers to catering for young, gifted children in prior-to
school settings. International Journal of Early Childhood, 42(1), 43–58.
Whitters, H. G. (2015). Perceptions of the influences upon the parent-professional
relationship in a context of early intervention and child protection. Ph.D. thesis.
University of Strathclyde.
Whitters, H. G. (2017). Nursery nurse to early years’ practitioner: Role, relationships
and responsibilities. Abingdon: Oxford.
Whitters, H. G. (2020). Adverse childhood experiences, attachment, and the early years’
learning environment, research, and inclusive practice. Abingdon and New York:
Routledge.
Zero to Three (2017). The basics of infant and early childhood mental health: A briefing
paper. https://www.zerotothree.org/policy-and-advocacy/social-and-emotional-health.
Zhou, M., & Brown, D. (2015). Educational learning theories, 2nd edition, Education
Open Textbooks, 1. https://oer.galileo.usg.edu/education-textbooks/1.
Index