Aha Is Creativity Possible in Legal Problem Solving and Teachabl
Aha Is Creativity Possible in Legal Problem Solving and Teachabl
2001
This open-access article is brought to you by the Georgetown Law Library. Posted with permission of the author.
Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarship.law.georgetown.edu/facpub
Part of the Dispute Resolution and Arbitration Commons, and the Legal Education Commons
GEORGETOWN LAW
Faculty Publications
January 2010
Carrie Menkel-Meadow
Professor of Law
Georgetown University Law Center
[email protected]
1. INTRODUCTION
97
7. I want to emphasize the point that many people do not have legal problems,
instead they are engaged in completing a transaction, seeking legal advice about pos-
sible courses of action, or may be engaged in a legal event which mayor may not even
be deemed problematic (home purchase, adoption of child). See CARRIE MENKEL-
MEADOW, THE 59TH STREET LEGAL CLINIC: EVALUATION OF THE EXPERIMENT 40 (1979).
8. See, e.g., TERESA M. AMABILE, CREATIVITY IN CONTEXT (1996); MIHALY CSIK.
SZENTMIHALYl, CREATIVITY: FLOW AND THE PSYCHOLOGY OF DISCOVERY AND INVENTION
(1996); DIMENSIONS OF CREATIVITY (Margaret Boden ed., 1996); HANDBOOK OF CREA.
TIVITY (Robert J. Sternberg ed., 1999).
9. See, e.g., GARY KLEIN, SOURCES OF POWER: How PEOPLE MAKE DECISIONS
(1998); JAMES REASON, HUMAN ERROR (1990).
10. See HOWARD GARDNER, FRAMES OF MIND: THE THEORY OF MULTIPLE INTELLI.
GENCES (1983).
11. See DANIEL KAHNEMAN, PAUL SLOVIC & AMOS TVERSKY, JUDGMENT UNDER
UNCERTAINTY: HEURISTICS AND BIASES (1982); RICHARD NISBETT & LEE Ross, HUMAN
INFERENCE: STRATEGIES AND SHORTCOMINGS OF SOCIAL JUDGMENT (1980).
12. This paper will not discuss the problem solving literature in engineering, ar-
chitecture, psychiatry and social work, to mention a few other domains in which these
issues have been explored.
13. See, e.g., JAMES ADAMS, CONCEPTUAL BLOCKBUSTING: A GUIDE TO BETTER
IDEAS (1986); JAMES ADAMS, THE CARE AND FEEDING OF IDEAS: A GUIDE TO ENCOURAG.
ING CREATIVITY (1986); EDWARD DE BONO, LATERAL THINKING: CREATIVITY STEP BY
STEP (1990); MARTIN GARDNER, AHA! INSIGHT (1978); MARTIN GARDNER, AHA! GOTCHA:
PARADOXES TO PuZZLE AND DELIGHT (1982); FLOYD HURT, ROUSING CREATIVITY: TtnNK
NEW Now! (1999); GERARD 1. NIERENBERG, THE .ART OF CREATIVE THINKING (1982);
DONALD J. NOONE, CREATIVE PROBLEM SOLVING (1998); ROBERT W. OLSON, THE.ART OF
CREATIVE THINKING: A PRACTICAL GUIDE (1978); VINCENT RYAN RUGGIERO, 'fiIE.ART 010'
THINKING: A GUIDE TO CRITICAL AND CREATIVE THOUGHT (5th ed.1998); see also Rich-
ard S. Blackburn, Managing Creativity in Academics, in MANAGING IN ACADEMICS: A
HEALTH CENTER MODEL (Jill Ridky & George F. Sheldon eds., 1993); JENNIFER JAMES,
THINKING IN THE FuTURE TENSE: A WORKOUT FOR THE MIND (1997).
14. For my argument that many, if not most, legal problems today involve more
than two parties and so require a multi-party inventory of needs, goals and objectives,
see Carrie Menkel-Meadow, The Trouble with the Adt'ersary System in a Post-Mod-
em, Multi-Cultural World, 38 WM. & :MARY L. REv. 5 (1996). Lon Fuller recognized
this aspect of some legal problems in Mediation: Its Form and Its Fllnctions, 44 S.
CAL. L. REv. 305 (1971).
15. For a discussion of why I used the term "needs," in addition to the "interests"
or "objectives" used by other negotiatjon theorists, see infra note 52 and accompany-
ing text.
16. See, e.g., RoBERT AxELROD, THE COMPLE.XlTY OF COOPERATION: AGENT BASED
MODELS OF CO!llPETmON AND COLLABORATION (1997); RoBERT AXELROD, TUE EVOLU.
TION OF COOPERATION (1984); DOUGLAS BAIRD ET. AL., GA!lIE THEORY J\ND TIlE L,\w
(1994); ADAM: M. BRANDENBURGER & BARRY NALEBUFF, CO-OPETITION U996); AVlNASU
DIXIT & BARRY NALEBUFF, TmNKING STRATEGICALLY: THE Cor.IPETITIVE EDGE IN BUSI.
NESS, POLmcs AND EVERYDAY LIFE (1991); WILLIA!Il POUNDSTONE, PRISONER'S DI.
LEMMA (1992).
17. See, e.g., MAX BAZERMAN, JUDGMENT IN MANAGERIAL DECISION l\1AKJ.NG (4th
ed. 1998); JOHN HAMIIiOND, RALPH KEENEY & HOWARD RAIFFA, Sr.lARl' CUOICES: A
PRACTICAL GUIDE TO 11AKING BETTER DECISIONS (999); GARY KLEIN, SOURCES OF
POWER: How PEOPLE MAKE DECISIONS (1998); J. EDWARD Russo & PAUL J. H. SHOE.
MAKER, DECISION TRAPs: TEN BARRIERS TO BRILLIANT DECISION-MAKING AND How TO
OVERCOME THEM (1989).
18. See, e.g., KENNETH .ARRow, SOCIAL CHOICE AND INDIVIDUAL VALUES (1951);
RUSSELL HARDIN, COLLECTIVE ACTION (1982); MANcUR OLSON, THE LOGIC OF COLLEC·
TIVE ACTION: PuBLIC GOODS AND THE THEORY OF GROUPS (1965); cf. DONALD GREEN &
!AN SHAPIRO, PATHOLOGIES OF RATIONAL CHOICE THEORY: A CRITIQUE OF APPLICATIONS
IN POLITICAL SCIENCE (1994).
19. See, e.g., JAMES BOHMAN, PuBLIC DELIBERATION: PLURALISM, COMPLEXITY AND
DEMOCRACY (1996); DELIBERATIVE POLITICS: ESSAYS ON DEMOCRACY AND DISAGREE·
MENT (Stephen Macedo ed., 1999); AMY GUTMANN & DENNIS THOMPSON, DEMOCRACY
AND DISAGREEMENT (1996); JURGEN HABERMAS, THE THEORY OF COMMUNICATIVE Ac·
TION (Vol. 1, 1984 & Vol. 2,1987); JANE J. MANSBRIDGE, BEYOND ADVERSARY DEMOC·
RACY (1980).
20. See, e.g., JAMES G. MARCH & HERBERT A. SIMON, ORGANIZATIONS (1958); Di-
ane Vaughan, Rational Choice, Situated Action and the Social Control of Organiza·
tions, 32 L. & SOC'Y REV. 23 (1998); see also DIANE VAUGHAN, THE CHALLENGER
LAUNCH DECISION: RISKY TECHNOLOGY, CULTURE AND DEVIANCE AT NASA (1996).
21. See, e.g., CONFLICT, COOPERATION AND JUSTICE: ESSAYS INSPIRED BY THE WORK
OF MORTON DEUTSCH (Barbara Benedict Bunker et al. eds., 1995); LEWIS A. COSER,
THE FUNCTIONS OF SOCIAL CONFLICT (1956); MORTON DEUTSCH, THE RESOLUTION O~'
CONFLICT: CONSTRUCTIVE AND DESTRUCTIVE PROCESSES (1973); DEAN G. PRUITT & JEI~.
FREY Z. RUBIN, SOCIAL CONFLICT: ESCALATION, STALEMATE, AND SETTLEMENT (1986);
MARc HOWARD Ross, THE MANAGEMENT OF CONFLICT: INTERPRETATIONS AND INTER.
ESTS IN COMPARATIVE PERSPECTIVE (1993); THOMAS C. SCHELLING, THE STRATEGY OF
CONFLICT (1960); GEORG SIMMEL, CONFLICT (1955).
22. See Program On Negotiation, Negotiation Pedagogy: A Research Survey of
Four Disciplines, at http://www.pon.harvard.eduleventslhewlettldayl/shtml (last vis-
ited Feb. 9, 2001).
23. See GEORGE HOMANS, SOCIAL BEHAVIOR: ITS ELEMENTARY FORMS (1974).
24. See, e.g., LEE Ross & RICHARD E. NISBE'IT, THE PERSON AND THE SITt1ATIO~~:
PERSPECTIVES OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY (1991).
25. See Robert l\inookin & Lewis Kornhauser, Bargaining in the Shadow of tlze
Law: The Case of Divorce, 88 YALE L.J. 950 (1979).
26. See ROBERT MNOOKIN, SCOTT R PEPPET, ANDREW S. TULUMELLO, BE,OND
WINNING: NEGOTIATING TO CREATE VALUE IN DEALS AND DISPUTES l2000}; Robert
l\fuookin, Why Negotiations Fail: An Exploration of Barriers to the Resolution of Con-
flict, 8 OHIO ST. J. DISP. REsOL. 235 (1993).
27. See David Luban, Settlements and the Erosion of the Public Realm, 83 GEO.
L.J. 2619 (1995).
28. See, e.g., HERBERT M. KroTZER, LET'S l\L\KE A DEAL: UNDERSTANDING TIlE NE.
GOTIATION PROCESS IN ORDINARY LITIGATION (1991); Herbert M. Kritzer, Fee Arrange-
ments and Negotiations: A Research Note, 21 L. & Soc', REv. 341 (1987) (arguing that
Menkel-Meadow's "creative solutions" to legal negotiation probleIDS will be unlikely
where lawyers are paid on a contingent basis).
29. See, e.g., James J. White, Machiavelli and the Bar: Etlzical Limitations on
Lying in Negotiation, 1980 &.r. B. FOUND. REs. J. 926; cf. Eleanor Holmes Norton,
Bargaining and the Ethic of Process, 64 N.Y.U. L. REv. 493 (1989); Murray Schwartz,
The Professionalism and Accountability of Lawyers, 66 CAL. L. REv. 669 (19781; Ger-
ald Wetlaufer, The Ethics of Lying in Negotiations, 75 IOWA L. REv. 1219 (1990).
30. See, e.g., Robert Condlin, "Cases on Both Sides": Patterns ofArgument in Le-
gal Dispute-Negotiation, 44 MD. L. REv. 65 (1985); Owen M. Fiss, Against Settlement,
93 YALE L.J. 1073 (1984); cf. Carrie Menkel-Meadow. Whose Dispute Is It Anyway? A
Philosophical and Democratic Defense of Settlement an Some Cases), 83 GEO. L.J.
2663 (1995) (arguing that in order to do justice, sometimes legal negotiators must
resolve issues outside the law, with particularized solutions for the parties).
31. Roger Fisher, William Ury & Bruce Patton, GETIING TO YES: NEGOTIATION
AGREEMENT WITHOUT GIVING IN, 4 (1991).
32. See STEVEN J. BRAMS & ALAN D. TAYLOR, THE WIN-WIN SOLUTION: GUARAN.
TEEING FAIR SHARES TO EVERYBODY (1999). I prefer to stay away from the win-win
nomenclature in legal disputes. Often in legal matters someone will not be able to
win anything-a criminal defendant who engages in plea negotiations but who will
serve jail time; a manufacturer of a defective product who will have to take the prod-
uct off the market and perhaps pay millions of dollars in damages. These are not
winning solutions to the legal problem at hand, but the negotiated outcome may still
be better than what might have occurred without negotiation (more transaction costs,
more pay-outs, more embarrassment, adverse publicity or emotional pain).
33. Menkel-Meadow, Toward Another View of Negotiation, supra note 3.
34. In jurisprudential terms, we can see this as the development ofresponsive or
participatory law out of the earlier states of command, repression and individual max-
imization or autonomous law. See PHILLIPPE Nom;T & PHILIP SELZNICK, LAw AND SO.
CIETY IN TRANSITION: TOWARD RESPONSIVE LAw (1978).
35. See Fisher & Ury, supra note 31, at 56·80.
pies before dividing them, or "create value before claiming it,"36 en-
couraged to brainstorm and invent before evaluating, and pushed to
look behind stated preferences or positions to find underlying needs
and interests and search for principles or "objective criteria"37 to re-
solve that which cannot be resolved by mutually acceptable trades,
Negotiation teaching has moved to increasingly demanding planning
exercises that require students to analyze a wide variety of party
needs, preferences, desires, objectives, possible legal outeomes, cost
benefit analysis, decision tree analysis,3S best-caselworst-case scena-
rios, and contingency plans that are designed to force negotiators to
analyze the substance of their dispute before they even sit down to
talk. to the other side, Negotiation analysis, before any behavior \vith
the other side is even enacted, requires rigorous thinking, planning
for problem identification, fact-finding, legal research, option genera-
tion, brainstorming, strategies for communication, information shar-
ing, persuasion of other parties and consideration of agreement
implementation and execution,39
Once the needs, interests, objectives and goals of all interested
parties have been identified, facts have been investigated and agen-
das for information sharing have been developed, where do particular
solutions to legal matters or problems come from? Is there any sys-
tematic way to develop substantive solutions to negotiated legal
problems? How do lawyers generate options, find altelJlative courses
36. See Lax & Sebenius, The MaTUlger as Negotiator: Bargaining for Cooperation
and Competitive Gain, (1986); MNOOKIN ET AL, BEYOND WINNING, supra note 26,
37. See Fisher & Ury, supra note 31, at 81-94.
38. See, e.g., MaIjorie Corman Aaron, Decision Analysis, in l\1EDIATING LEGAL
DISPUTES: EFFECTIVE STRATEGIES FOR LAWYERS AND MEDIATORS IDwight Golann ed"
1996); Hammond, supra note 17. See also Bert Useem et aI" Resolution of Prison
Riots, NIJ REsEARCH IN BRIEF, 1995, at www.soci.niu.eduf-critcrimlescapesipris-
riot.txt Oast visited February 18, 2001) (suggesting difference between "proactive
planning and "reactive problem-solving" in prison riot negotiation).
39. For examples of these planning processes, see Carrie Menkel-Meadow, NEGO-
TIATION PLANS FOR DISPUTE REsOLUTION (LlTIGATION) PROBLEMS AND BusINESS Pl..-\t;.
NING OR TRANSACTIONAL PLANNING (1990); RoGER FISHER & DANNY ERTEL, GE:ITING
READy TO NEGOTIATE: THE GE'ITING TO YES WORKBOOK (995)' Such planning docu-
ments reflect an attention to the stages and phases of most negotiations and thus
represent an operationalization of the theorized structure of negotiations as described
by both negotiation theorists and empiricists. See, e.g., P. H. GULLIVER, DISPUTES MID
NEGOTIATIONS: A CROSS CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE (1979); Carrie Menkel-Meadow,
Stages and Phases of Negotiation, in DAVID BINDER & CARRIE l\fEr..'KEL-MEADOW,
AMERICAN BAR AssOCIATION LAWYERING SKILLS INSTITUTE 119821; LEIGH Tum.tPSo!'-l,
THE MIND AND HEART OF THE NEGOTIATOR (1998); GERALD WILLIAMS, LEGAL NEGOTIA.
TION AND SETTLEMENT (2000).
40. See Marcel Kahan & Michael Klausner, Standardization and Innovation in
Corporate Contracting (Or "The Economics of Boilerplate''), 83 VA. L. REV. 713 (1997).
41. See Menkel-Meadow, Toward Another View of Negotiation, supra note 3, at
789.
42. See Lee Ross, Reactive Devaluation in Negotiation and Conflict Resolution, in
BARRIERS TO CONFLICT REsOLUTION (Kenneth J. Arrow et al. eels., 1995). In addition
to the opposite side of a legal case, this can include one's parent, child, spouse, boss or
employee, or anyone who is not of our point of view.
43. See SHELLEY E. TAYLOR, POSITIVE ILLUSIONS (1989); Shelley Taylor & J.
Brown, Illusion and Well Being: A Social Psychological Perspective on Mental Health,
103 PSYCH. BULL. 193 (1988).
44. See Amabile, supra note 8, at 244-45; R.M. :Milgram & N.A. Milgram, Group
vs. Individual Administration in the Measurement of Creative Thinking in Gifted and
Non-gifted Children, 49 CHILD DEV. 385 (1978).
45. See RICHARD CIALDINI, INFLUENCE: THE PSYCHOLOGY OF PERSUASION (1984);
IRVING JANIS, GROUPTHINK: PSYCHOLOGICAL STUDIES OF POLICY DECISIONS M"D FlAS-
COS (1983).
46. See Wendy M. Williams & Lana T. Yang, Organizational Creativity, in HAND-
BOOK OF CREATIVITY (Robert J. Sternberg ed., 1999); Donald Langevoort, Behavioral
Theories of Judgment and Decision Making in Legal Scholarslzip: A Literature Re-
view, 51 VAND. L. REV. 1499 (1998); David Luban, Milgram Re·visited, 9 REsEARCHlNG
LAw: AN ABF UPDATE, SPRING 1998.
47. See JEFFREY RUBIN & BERT BROWN, THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF BARGAINING
AND NEGOTIATION (1975); Ian Ayres, Fair Driving: Gender and Race Discrimination in
Retail Car Negotiations, 104lIARv. L. REV. 817 (1991); Ian Ayres, Further Evidence of
Discrimination in New Car Negotiations and Estimates of Its Cause. 94 MICH. L. REv
109 (1995); Jeswald Salacuse, Ten Ways That Culture Affects Negotiating Style: Some
Survey Results, 14 NEG. J. 221 (1998); Stephen E. Weiss, Negotiating With Romans,
Part 1, 35 SLOAN MGMT. REV. 51 (1994); Stephen E. Weiss, Negotiating With Romans,
Part 2, 35 SLOAN MGMT. REV. 85 (1994).
48. See SANDRA LIPSITZ BEM, THE LENSES OF GENDER: TRANSFORMING THE DEBA'rE
ON SEXUAL INEQUALITY (1993); HAzEL GENN, HARD BARGAINING: OUT OF COURT SET.
TLEMENT IN PERSONAL INJURY ACTIONS (1987); HAzEL GENN, PATl-IS TO JUSTICE: WHAT
PEOPLE Do AND THINK ABOUT GOING TO LAw (1999); CAROL TAVRIS, THE MISMEASURE
OF Wm.fAN 36-43, 170-207 (1992); Herbert Kritzer et. al., The Aftermath of Injury:
Cultural Factors in Compensation Seeking in Canada and the United States, 25 LAw
& SOC'Y REV. 499 (1991); Herbert Kritzer, Propensity to Sue in England and the
United States of America: Blaming and Claiming in Torts Cases, 18 J. LAw & SOC'Y
400 (1991); Carrie Menkel-Meadow, Teaching About Gender and Negotiation: Sex,
Truths and Videotape, 16 NEG. J. 357 (2000).
49. See Arrow et al., Barriers to Conflict Resolution, supra note 42; Russell
Korobkin & Chris Guthrie, Psychological Barriers to Litigation Settlement: An Experi-
mental Approach, 93 MICH. L. REV. 107 (1994); Mnookin, Why Negotiations Fail,
supra note 26.
50. The study of cognitive biases is a negative one. Researchers derme errors as
departures from rational thinking, see for example, JON ELSTER, ALCHEMIES OF THE
MIND; RATIONALITY AND Tl-IE EMOTIONS, (1999). With a different baseline we could
study decision making descriptively in naturalistic settings (as Gary Klein and others
have done) and perhaps discover that truly rational decision making is the deviance
from the norm that should be explained, not the other way around. Kahneman &
Tversky's work, while path-breaking, has been criticized on this ground and others.
See L. Jonathan Cohen, Can Human Irrationality be Experimentally Demonstrated? 4
BEH. & BRAIN SCIENCES 317 (1981); L. Jonathan Cohen, Can Irrationality be Dis-
cussed Accurately, 7 BEH. & BRAIN SCIENCES 736 (1984) (discussing difficulties of defi-
nitions of "irrationality" or departures from rationality, measurement problems,
cueing problems in such research and philosophical objections to operationalization of
irrationality heuristics); L. Jonathan Cohen, The Controversy About Irrationality, 6
BEH. & BRAIN SCIENCES 510 (1983). Teresa Amabile has commented that most of her
studies of creativity have uncovered the impediments to or suppressors of creativity,
rather than finding empirically valid enhancers. See Amabile, supra note 8, at 243.
For an argument that lawyers may have a particular need to hold on to assumptions
of rationality, both in their own behavior and in the larger rationality of the legal
system, see Langevoort, supra note 46, at 1526 (suggesting that the myth of the ra-
tionality of law allows us to maintain (perhaps incorrectly) the illusion of control over
our world). Langevoort does note that a remarkable amount of productive work is
accomplished by our legal institutions and questions whether the work on biased cog-
nitive processes will prove to be as empirically robust at the organizationaIrmstitu-
tionallevel as it is at the individual level.
51. In reviewing the now vast literature on both the psychology and sociology of
cognition, one cannot help being struck. by the different professional dispositions of
researchers in the field. Some seem almost gleeful in rigorously documenting human
error and intellectual frailty (as well as bell-curve variations) while others have at-
tached themselves to the study of achievement (the rareness of true creativity) and
the acknowledgment of greater human variations. Is this "glass half full or half
empty" phenomenon parallel to the dispositions of traditional and problem solving
negotiation theorists and practitioners?
52. My use of needs rather than interests alone has been quite deliberate and
reflects several important jurisprudential challenges to conventional conceptions of
negotiation in the legal system. First, lawyers should have an obligation to explore
client's needs that may be beneath the articulated surface, both for individuals who
may not be able to express those needs readily and for organizational or entity clients
who may need to be reminded of long-term needs of the organization, rather than
short-term goals of the person representing the entity. Second, my theory of problem
solving negotiation is heavily influenced by work in feminist theory that suggests that
clients have needs that may not be fully recognized by the formal legal system, see for
example, RoBIN WEST, CARING FOR JUSTICE U997); Robin West, The Difference in Wo-
men's Hedonic Lives: A Phenomenological Critique of Feminist Legal Theory, 3 WIS.
WOMEN'S. L. REv. 81 (1987), but which may be appealed to in negotiation settings.
Third, expression of needs may compel an attention to justice in negotiation settings
that is less instrumental than satisfaction of interests and more m·:pnnsive than ap-
peals to legal rights in the formal justice system.
53. See Menkel-Meadow, Toward Another View of Legal Negotiation, supra note
3, at 803. Lawyers need to learn through interviewing and counseling how clients
rate their preferences-not all will conform to Maslovian hierarchies of human need.
54. The question of whether law and legal remedies should set the parameters for
a just or fair solution in legal negotiation is a much debated subject. While law cer-
tainly may set the outer limits of preventing unlawful outcomes, it may be that par·
ties in private negotiations will choose solutions that are not unlawful, but which are
also different than what the law provides for in a particular situation. See Robert
Condlin, Bargaining in the Dark: The Normative Incoherence of Lawyer Dispute Bar·
gaining Role, 51 MD. L. REV. 1 (1992); Carrie Menkel-Meadow, Whose Dispute is it
Anyway? A Philosophical and Democratic Defense of Settlement an Some Cases).
supra note 30; Jacqueline Nolan-Haley, Court Mediation and the Search for Justice
Through Law, 74 WASH. U. L.Q. 47 (1996).
ambiguity and tension, playfulness, outsider family, high el-:pectations and deep com-
mitment. See, e.g., AMABILE, supra note 8, at 203-42; BODEN, supra note 8, at 8;
CSIKZENTI.flHALYI, supra note 8, at 61-76. Such personal characteristics may also be
found among leading legal creative figures-see biographies of Louis Brandeis, Oliver
Wendell Holmes, Thurgood Marshall, and Benjamin Cardozo, to name some of the
prominent judicial examples. See also Mit. JUSTICE (Allison Dunham & Philip B. Kur-
land eds., 1956) (collected essays about several prominent judges throughout history).
62. CSIKSZENThflHALYI, supra note 8, at 8.
63. Id. at 28.
64. JOHN B. WATSON, BEHAVIORISM (19251.
65. AMABILE, supra note 8, at 33.
66. Morris Stein, Creativity and Culture. in EXPLORATIONS IN CREATIVI1Y /Ross L.
Mooney & Taber .A. Razik, eds., 1967).
67. Some creativity scientists suggest that the field was born in reaction to Karl
Popper's assertions that all truly creative activity is irrational or intuitive and there-
fore not amenable to study, despite (perhaps in reaction tol his o...m dissertation on
the psychology of thinking See KARL POPPER, THE LOGIC OF SClEr-.'TIFIC DISCOVERY
(1959), quoted in GERD GIGERENZER, Where do New ideas Come From? in DI?tENSIO~~S
OF CREATIVITY (Margaret .A. Boden ed., 1996).
production, movies, novels and art will be evaluated not only by spe-
cialized critics but by purchasers and the consuming public at large.
And, in some domains, the judgments about creative worth may be
subject to more volatile change than others, for example, the debates
about canons in literature, art and the "classics."74 The law repre-
sents a hybrid domain in which the field is that of fellow eA-perts
Gudges, legal scholars, lawyers) but in fact the lasting creativity of
legal work will be measured by a much broader field-that of clients,
citizens oflegal regimes and all those regulated by law.75 There are
also more creative times than others, when "critical masses" of crea-
tive people are presented with problems to solve or resource-rich com-
munities in which to explore their interests-the Athenian "Golden
Age," the Renaissance, late 19th century Paris (Impressionism), Fin
de Siecle Vienna, Motown, and the"M & A" decade. 76
Students of creativity and cultural evolution speak of memes as
units of analysis. Memes77 are the cultural equivalent of genes-
units of information that are passed on to future generations and
that can be changed. Like successful mutations, if a changed meme
is accepted by enough people in the culture, the culture will change.
While we currently think of Brown v. Board of Education78 as a suc-
cessful meme of the legal culture (in the now accepted idea it ex-
pressed about racial equality; it has been less successful as a meme of
implementation of educational equality), other legal memes are still
trying to gain acceptance (Roe v. Wade 79 ), and still others are trying
to seek recognition and acceptance by the legal field (e.g., same sex
marriage80). While I will more fully explore legal creativity and legal
memes in Part IV below, consider that joint custody for children after
divorce is a legal meme that was produced by the negotiation process,
74. See, e.g., ALLAN BLOOM, The Closing of the American Mind, in DEBATING PC:
THE CONTROVERSY OVER POLITICAL CORRECTNESS ON C,.\MPUSES (Paul Berman ed.,
1992); ROBERT HUGHES, CULTURE OF COlllPLAINT: THE FRAYING OF AMERICA 11993';
MARTHA NUSSBAUM, CULTIVATING HUMANITY (1997).
75. For an illustration of how average citizens creatively mobilize their complex
and sometimes conflicting understandings of law see PATRICIA EWICK & SUSAN
SILBEY, THE COMMON PLACE OF LAw: STORIES FROM EVERYDAY LIFE (1998).
76. See LINCOLN CAPLAN, SKADDEN: THE RISE OF A LEGAL EMPIRE (19931.
77. SUSAN BLACKMORE, THE MEllIE MACHINE (1999); CSIKSZENT?UHALYI, supra
note 8, at 7.
78. 347 U.S. 483 (1954).
79. 410 U.S. 113 (1973).
80. See Baehr v. Miike, 1996 WL 694235 (Haw. Cir. Ct. Dec. 13, 1996J; see also
2000 VT. ACTS & RESOLVES 91 and VT. STAT. ANN. tit. 15, § 3 (2000) (allowing same
sex couples the right to have "civil unions" in Vermont).
when creative lawyers (or perhaps clients) decided to change the pat-
tern of separate physical custody of one parent after divorce and in-
stead use a common negotiation trope of sharing to produce a new
legal result, now codified (and some would say, calcified81 ) in formal
legal presumptions,82 at least in some states.
What makes the study of creativity interesting for legal negotia-
tors and problem solvers is the extent to which debates about
whether creativity depends on internal or external rewards,83 moti-
vations or constraints may inform how we study and teach our own
thinking processes. Modern students of creativity see it as both an
interpersonal and social process, so that mastery ofthe knowledge of
the domain is necessary (but not sufficient) by the individual, but
some degree of acceptability by the relevant group of experts will ulti-
mately be necessary. Some fields may be more social than others, af-
fecting how domain knowledge is transferred as well as evaluated.
Consider the transparency oflaw in which legal arguments are made
publicly and evaluated in writing by lawyers and judges or how
transactional clauses are accepted or rejected by lawyers and clients.
Unlike scientists working in secret labs84 until they are ready to
share their findings for peer review, lawyers and legal scholars must
share their ideas rather quickly, if not in print, then in negotiated
proposals, offers, paper presentations, motions, briefs and
arguments.
Domains may have both specific knowledge bases and domain-
specific processes. Consider the formal art training that Picasso and
many of the Impressionists had before they broke with conventional
forms. In law, we teach the specialized learning process of thinking
like a lawyerB5 by which we mean teaching particular cognitive
processes, including the ability to choose what is relevant, the ability
81. See, e.g., Katharine Bartlett & Carol Stack, Joint Custody, Feminism and the
Dependency Dilemma, 2 BERKELEY WOMEN's L.J. 9 (1986); Carol Bruch & Janet
Bowermaster, The Relocation of Children and Custodial Parents: Public Policy, Past
and Present, 30 FAM. L.Q. 245 (1996).
82. CAL. FAM CODE § 3002 (West 2000).
83. Teresa Amabile's research, for example, suggests that creativity is enhanced
by internal motivations and often destroyed by many, if not all forms of externals
rewards, expectations and constraints. See AMABILE, supra note 8, at 243.
84. For more on the "secret" lab competition in DNA research, see ANNE SAYRE,
ROSALIND FRANKLIN AND DNA (1978); JAMES D. WATSON, THE DOUBLE HELL": A PER-
SONAL ACCOUNT OF THE DISCOVERY OF THE STRUCTURE OF DNA (1969).
85. For several ofthe classic efforts to define what this means, or at least consists
of, see EDWARD H. LEVI,.AN INTRODUCTION TO LEGAL REASONING (1949); KARL LLEWEL-
LYN, THE BRAMBLE BUSH: ON OUR LAw AND ITS STUDY (1930). For a contrary view
that we should be learning other cognitive skills, such as more fact-based analysis,
see JEROME FRANK, COURTS ON TRIAL: MYTH AND REALITY IN AMERICAN JUSTICE (1949).
86. There are an increasing number of arguments that thinking like a lawyer will
no longer be logically linear when we incorporate the logic of computer use and com-
pletely different forms ofless linear thinking, see, for example, Barbara BintlilT, From
Creativity to Computerese: Thinking Like A Lawyer in the Computer Age, 88 LAW.
LmR. J. 338 (1996); Diana R Donahoe, at www.teachinglaw.freewebsites.com Hast
visited February 18, 2001).
87. See GARDNER, FRAMES OF MIND, supra note 10.
88. See Robert J. Sternberg, The Holey Grail of General Intelligence, 289 SCIENCE
399 (2000).
89. GARDNER, INTELLIGENCE REFRAMED, supra note 68, at 33.
process (see Part V infra) and in the practice of legal problem solving
might facilitate the enhancement of certain problem solving capaci-
ties, if not creativity, in the legal domain. Combining this work on
creativity and multiple intelligences for lawyers presents some inter-
esting possibilities for encouraging more positive ways of thinking
about legal reasoning and problem solving, if learning from different
intelligences can be made relevant within one domain (legal
problems).
Gardner originally posited seven intelligences, as follows:
1) linguistic-word and language related;
2) logical-mathematical-use of logic, mathematical operations
and scientific investigation;
3) musical-performance, composition and appreciation of mu-
sical patterns;
4) bodily-kinesthetic-use of body to solve problems (athletes,
dancers, surgeons, craftspeople);
5) spatial-recognition and manipulation of patterns of wide or
confined spaces;
6) interpersonal-capacity to understand the intentions, moti-
vations and desires of other people, and ability to work well with
other people;
7) intrapersonal-capacity to understand oneself;
More recently, Gardner has added several new intelligences: 9o
8) naturalist-expertise in the recognition and classification of
numerous species of the environment;
9) spiritual-relation to supernatural world or larger cosmos;
ability to achieve other states of consciousness;
10) existential-concern with ultimate issues and a capacity to
locate oneself with respect to the furthest reaches of the cosmos
and existence-ability to imagine and contemplate the infinite:
the cognitive side of spiritual intelligence. 91
While some have urged the addition of a moral intelligence,
Gardner declines to recognize that as a separate intelligence and in-
stead suggests that each intelligence can be expressed with positive
90. Id. at 35-41. Gardner specifies the bio-neurological and psychological factors
that set the parameters for recognition of an "intelligence" with both biological and
empirical components. Not all intelligences are clearly mapped in the brain.
91. Id. at 47-66. Because the last two, spiritual and existential intelligences have
not been as fully established by Gardner's own standards, he now speaks of "8 I!J"
intelligences. Howard Gardner, Kenote Address at the Hewlett Center Conference
2000: Focus on Negotation Pedagogy (Mar. 10, 2000). At least one dispute resolution
scholar has been exploring the relationship of spiritual intelligence (in the form of
meditation) to lawyering. See Len Riskin, Project on Meditation and Lawyers, Yale
Meditation Project.
and negative values. He maintains that our choices about how to ex-
ercise our intelligence is a question of values and judgment, not intel-
ligence.92 Those psychologists and others who study altruistic
behavior might disagree; some empirical and socially defined charac-
teristics of rescuers, for example, illustrate some patterns of intelli-
gence, such as capacity for empathy and pro-active moral actions.93
Gardner himself has speculated on the intelligences that might
be marshaled for effective negotiation and legal problem solving: lin-
guistic, logical, interpersonal, intrapersonal, and even spatial, are ob-
vious candidates. 94 Consider how we narrow the domain of legal
problem solving by focusing so much of our education and knowledge
base on only one or two of these intelligences.
Much of the popular literature on learning how to be creative95
uses the approach of making people aware of more ways to think
about particular problems-in essence to access their multiple intelli-
gences, to avoid narrow domain-limited and path-dependent96 ways
of both framing and solving problems and to "think out of the box."
Brainteasers and other brain exercises are used to question assump-
tions, unpack stereotypic thinking, juxtapose elements of a problem,
make "the familiar strange,"97 change levels oftbjnking (aggregation
and disaggregation of problem elements), develop new frames, analo-
gies or metaphors, and to avoid pre-judging or prematurely settling
on particular solutions. Some brainteasers and problems designed to
enhance creativity also teach about the process of thinking, which in-
cludes listening, exploring multiple meanings of language and focus-
ing on less familiar modes of thoughts. A few of these exercises follow
below. As you attempt to solve these problems, pay attention to your
own thinking processes-how are you trying to solve the problem?
What problem or domain space98 are you searching? What do you
call on, from what you already know, to try to solve these problems?
1. In what has become the canonical problem for literally, and
figuratively, demonstrating this point, consider the following: Con-
nect the three rows of dots below with four straight lines without
moving your pen off the page:
• • •
• • •
• • •
Now, try connecting the dots with three lines.
Now, try one. 99
The solution to the first problem involves "thinking out of the
box" and by realizing that most people imagine a closed box or frame
within which they think. the lines must be drawn. Those with both
spatial and some forms of linguistic intelligence may solve this prob-
lem more quickly than others by not feeling so constrained by what
they hear as the instructions. The next two problems involve some
manipulation of the media or problem materials. The dots must be
made a bit larger to use three lines and we must stretch our sense of
"line" to imagine one large line to cover all the dots. Or, we must
change the paper on which the lines are drawn in order to line them
up one by one. lOO Law students, and even some law professors, resist
98. Scholars of decision making and problem solving describe the process ofprob-
lem solving by mapping out a problem space that creates the terrain for cognitive
searching, see, for example, NEWELL & SIMON, supra note 59, at 59. This can also be
considered framing the problem and this first step is quite crucial to how we go about
solving the problem. Recent work on problem solving from several different domains
suggests that many people do not use the rational search for alternatives posited by
rational choice theory, but especially among skilled experts, the "search space" is gen-
erated from past experiences and quick single decisions are made with a trial and
error evaluation process, called "recognition-primed decision making," see KLEIN,
supra note 10, at 7 (describing firefighters' decisions about how to attack fires, with-
out information about cause or specific locations).
99. For solutions, see JAMES A. ADAMS, CONCEPTUAL BLOCKBUSTING 24-32 (3rd
ed.1986).
100. [d. at 26-31.
101. I will not put the solution in print, but will tell you the answer if you contact
me, [email protected], because I do not want my future students to be
able to research the answer.
102. GARDNER, AHA! INSIGHT, supra note 13. You will find this brain teaser, along
with many others used in negotiation classes to teach students to listen carefully and
pay attention to detail, in Martin Gardner's two books of brainteasers and puzzles
which are designed to teach, through problem solving, a number of intelligences, in-
cluding mathematical, quantitative, logical, linguistic, social (breaking of stereotypes)
and spatial. See also, GARDNER, AHA! GoTCHA: PARADOXES TO PUZZLE AND DEUGIlT,
supra note 13.
103. Martin Gardner has a series of brainteasers like this one, see GARD~R, AlIA!
INSIGHT, supra note 13, at 95, that are designed to e.'>."plore the stereotypes we employ
when we visualize a scene from the spoken word and make unnecessary and limiting
assumptions about what the scene is about.
104. See, e.g., Paul Brest & Linda Krieger, Lawyers as Problem Solvers, 72 TEMP
L. REV. 811 (1999).
105. DE BONO, supra note 13.
106. NIERENBERG, supra note 13.
107. See also HURT, surpa note 13; DONALD J. NOONE, supra note 13; ROBERT W.
OLSON, supra note 13; VINCENT RYAN RUGGIERO, supra note 13. For applications of
creativity and problem solving to organizational change and management see, for ex·
ample, Richard S. Blackburn, Managing Creativity in Academics, in MANAGING IN
ACADEMICS (Jill Ridky & George Sheldon eds., 1993); JENNIFER JAMES, THINKING IN
THE FuTURE TENSE: A WORKOUT FOR THE MIND (1997).
108. DE BONO, supra note 13, at 228·30.
outside the usual scope of legal problem solving. So, how can all of
this creativity and multiple intelligences work be harnessed to legal
problem solving?
111. For a law and society scholar this represents a view oflaw from the outside:
how do particular legal ideas, or ideologies, come into being and how are they used,
mobilized or resisted by those affected by legal doctrines and rules? See Alan Hunt,
The Ideology of Law: Advances and Problems in Recent Applications of the Concept of
Ideology to the Analysis of Law, 19 LAw & SOC'Y REV. 11 (1985). Thus, a broader and
more social view oflegal creativity includes not just those who formally make the law
(judges, lawyers, government officials) but those who implement it, are regulated by it
and who translate it into everyday understandings. See e.g., EWICK & SILBEY, supra
note 75; SALLY MERRY, GE'ITING JUSTICE AND GE'ITING EVEN: LEGAL CONSCIOUSNESS
AMONG WORKING CLASS AMERICANS (1990); Regina Austin, Employer Abuse, Worker
Resistance and the Tort of Intentional Infliction of Emotional Distress, 41 STAN. L.
REV. 1 (1988).
112. See John Elster, Strategic Uses ofArgument, in BARRJERS TO CONFUCT REso-
LUTION (Kenneth J. Arrow et al. eds., 1995); Jack Rakove, ORlGlNAL MEANINGS (1996,;
Dana Lansky, Proceeding to a Constitution: A Multi-Party Negotiation Analysis of the
Constitutional Convention of 1787, 5 HARv. NEGOT. L. REv.279 (2000).
113. MNOOKIN ET AL., BEYOND WINNING, supra note 26.
114. Robert Cover, Violence and the Word, 95 Yale L.J. 1601 U9861.
115. Regarded by many as deeply flawed from its inception because of its recogni-
tion of many inequalities (including slavery). This raises the question of the tempo-
rality of measures and assessments of creativity.
116. Ronald Gilson & Robert Mnookin, Foreword: Business Lawyers and Value
Creation for Clients, 74 OR. L. REV. 1,8-9 (1995); MNOOKIN ET AL., BEYOND WINNING,
supra note 26. And in an interesting reversal, social theorists and historians have
recently been applying negotiation theory to constitution fonnation. See Elster, supra
note 60 and Rakove, supra note 60, for examples of the exploration of the differences
in bargaining procedures utilized in different constitutional settings.
117. Despite the recent increase in attention paid to legislation, as Eskridge &
Frickey have done, and regulation, most classic studies oflegal processes still focus on
judicial common law development. See BENJAMIN CARDOZO, THE NATURE OF THE JUDI.
CIAL PROCESS (1921); OLIVER WENDELL HOLMES, THE PATH OF THE LAw (1897); LLEW.
ELLYN, supra note 85; cf. HENRY M. HART, JR. & ALBERT M. SACKS, THE LEGAL
PROCESS: BASIC PROBLEMS IN THE MAKING AND APPLICATION OF LAW (William N. Es-
kridge, Jr. & Philip Frickey eds., 1994). For more nuanced and recent case studies on
how laws are made, see CASE STUDIES IN CONSENSUS BUILDING HANDBOOK (Larry
Susskind et al. eds., 1999); WENDY ESPELUND, THE SmUGGLE FOR WATER: POLITICS,
RATIONALITY AND IDENTITY IN THE AMERICAN SOUTHWEST (1998); Jody Freeman, Col·
laborative Governance in the Administrative State, 45 UCLA L. REV. 1 (1997).
118. Law students are taught analogical reasoning and the "distinguishing" of
cases, to be able to argue for both inclusion or exclusion of particular facts from argua-
bly applicable rules. Interestingly, in recent empirical work on effective modes of
teaching negotiation, Leigh Thompson and her colleagues, of the Northwestern Kel-
logg School of Business, have demonstrated that explicit comparison of cases, drawing
analogies in the characteristics of cases may increase the likelihood of pattern recog-
nition and produce "better" outcomes (in that case, conceptualized as more flexible,
contingent clauses). See Jeffrey Loewenstein et al., Analogical Encoding Facilitates
Knowledge Transfer in Negotiation, 6 PSYCHONOMIC BULL. & REV. 586 (1999); Leigh
Thompson, et al., Avoiding Missed Opportunities in Managerial Life (2000) (manu-
script). Perhaps legal educators are doing something right, after all.
119. For a summary of the new use of behavioral, social and cognitive psychology
in law and economics and legal scholarship more generally see Langevoort. supra note
46; see also Russell B. Korobkin & Thomas S. Ulen, Law and Behavioral Science:
Remouing the Rationality Assumption from Law and Economics, 88 CAL. L. REv. 1051
(2000).
120. For comparisons oflaw and the social sciences other than economics, see Car-
rie Menkel-Meadow, Durkheimian Epiphanies: The Importance of Engaged Social
Science in Legal Studies, 18 FLA. ST. U. L. REV. 91 (1990). For a comparison oflegnl
feminism with law and economics, see Carrie Menkel-Meadow, Mainstreaming Femi·
nist Legal Theory, 23 PAC. L.J. 1493 (1992). Others have focused on the use ofliterary
criticism and humanistic studies, see, for example, MARTHA C. NUSSBAUM, POETIC
JUSTICE: THE LITERARY IMAGINATION AND PUBLIC LIFE (1995); ROBIN WEST, NARRA.
TIVE, AUTHORITY AND LAw (1993); James Boyd White, What Can A Lawyer Learn from
Literature?, 102 HARv. L. REV. 2014 (1989); cf. RICHARD POSNER, LAW AND LITERA.
TURE: A MIsUNDERSTOOD RELATION (rev. ed. 1998).
121. It is sometimes suggested that law and science and law and social science
operate with such different standards of proof that they are hard to reconcile, espe-
cially in courts of law. See, e.g., STEVEN GoLDBERG, CULTURE CL.'\SH: LAw AND SCI.
ENCE IN AMERICA (1994); SHEILA JASANOFF, SCIENCE AT THE B,\R: LAw, SCIENCE AND
TECHNOLOGY IN AMERICA (1995). In some cases, law is more demanding than science
(epidemiological proof in mass torts), requiring degrees of certainty not yet known to
science. In other cases, courts have tolerated what others cnl1 junk science if there is
at least one CAllert to testify to some scientific truth. See Daubert. v. Merrell Dow
Pharmaceuticals, Inc., 509 U.S. 579 (1993).
122. David Cole, Agon at Agora: Creative Misreadings in the First Amendment
Tradition, 95 YALE L.J. 857 (1986).
123. See Id. at 858.
124. See Id. at 860,869-71.
125. Cole, supra note 122, at 875-96.
126. Id. at 879-87.
127. 347 U.S. 483 (1954).
132. Joseph Sax, The Public Trust Doctrine in Natural Resources Law: Effective
Judicial Intervention, 68 MICH. L. REV. 471 (1970). For a recent additional use of this
transfer principle, see Gerald Torres, Who Owns the Sky? Paper presented to Ge-
orgetown Environmental Law Seminar, Nov. 5,1999.
133. See Olin Browder, The Taming of A Duty: The Tort Liability of Landlords. 81
MICH. L. REV. 99 (1982); Joseph Sax, Slumlordism As a Tort, 65 MICH. L. REV. 869
(1967).
134. The military walkie-talkies ofWorId War II were invented by the founder of
Motorola who combined what he knew of car radios with \vireless communication
across battlefields, as he toured Europe on a family trip before the outbreak of the
war. See CSIKSZENTMIHALYI, supra note 8, at 96-97.
135. CATHERINE MAcKiNNON, FEMINISM UNMODIFIED: DISCOURSES ON LIFE AND
LAW (1987).
136. Robin West has suggested that tort and criminal law be extended to recognize
the now unrecognized claims of women's harms. See WEST, CARINO FOR JUSTICE.
supra note 52.
137. The limited hours of women and child labor legislation defended by the Bran-
deis-Goldmark brief in Muller v. Oregon, 208 U.S. 412 (1908), were derived from Eu-
ropean protective labor laws.
138. For a moving, but painful, illustration of how different cultures define
problems and solutions differently, see ANNE FADIMAN, THE SPIRIT CATCHES You AND
You FALL DOWN: A HMONG CHILD, HER AMERICAN DOCTORS, AND THE COLLISION OF
Two CULTURES (1997).
139. This is not unlike the advice given by Fisher & Ury and other negotiation
theorists that looking to other expert systems is another way to look for creative
ideas. How would another discipline characterize or frame a problem and then solve
it? For a phenomenological account of problem solving in several different disciplines,
see DONALD SCHON, THE REFLECTIVE PRACTITIONER: How PROFESSIONALS THINK IN
ACTION (1983).
140. Howard Gardner has solved the mystery of why so many lawyers write mys-
teries (and thrillers and novels and memoirs). We are simply shifting within our lin-
guistic intelligence to another domain in which we can use words and language Inot to
mention logic for mystery writers!)
141. Or rhetoric as the Greeks would have it. For a not so flattering portrait of
how we exercise this form of creativity, see PLATO, GORGIAS (Penguin ed. 1971).
142. See, e.g., GEORGE LAKOFF, WOMEN, FIRE AND DANGEROUS THINGS: WHAT CNfE.
GORIES REVEAL ABoUT THE MIND (1990).
143. James Gleick, Patently Absurd, N.Y. TIMES !liAGAZrNE, March 12,2000, at 44,
47-48. "Judges and lawyers have devoted millions of words to the nuances lofrele-
vant categories for patent and copyright protections). Maybe the trajectories of cul-
ture, economics and technology have reached a point where a distinction between an
idea and machine can no longer be sustained." Id at 48.
144. Lucas v. S.C. Coastal Council, 505 U.S. 1003 (1992); RIcHJ\lU) A. EPSTEIN,
TAKINGS: PRIVATE PROPERTY AND THE POWER OF EMINENT DOMAIN (1985).
145. In re N. Dist. of Cali. Dalkon Shield IUD Prods. Liab. Litig., 693 F.2d 847 (9th
Cir. 1982); Amchem Prods v. Windsor, 117 S. Ct. 2231 (1997).
146. A preemptive strike intended to prevent Skadden, Arps, Meagher & Flom
from being retained in an attempt to take over your company. See CAPLAN, supra note
76.
some, value creation through deals 147 with creative terms and
clauses adapted to meet parties' needs for accumulations of capital
and flexibility in governance. Corporate regulators, it must be said,
may be equally creative. Disclosure laws and securities regulations
were themselves creative responses to the stock market crash of
1929, while current arguments about the importance ofa wider group
of corporate stakeholders to whom the corporation might be responsi-
ble are efforts to create legal relations and liabilities where once
there were none. Negotiators in the transactional setting are often
particularly good at creating solutions with contingent agreements or
gradualist alterations of boilerplate terms. The percentage of gross
or net rent (in commercial leasing) or payment (in the performing
arts and sports) are alterations of standard rent or salary terms when
traditional formulations of fixed amount and distributive bargaining
problems gave way to contingent agreements to allocate risk and un-
certainty. Similar risk allocation in the litigation context changed
conventional monetary judgments to structured settlements and
annuities.
Beyond the case or transaction level, legal professionals demon-
strate creativity when they create whole new institutions like the
"problem solving courts" of drug courts,148 vice courts, and integrated
family courts that are seeking to work interdisciplinarily with both
individuals and families to combine treatment with punishment.
Similarly, creative clinical programs in law school have moved from
totally litigation-oriented strategies to transactional work, develop-
ing venture capital and investments in housing and economic devel-
opment projects, and multi-disciplinary problem solving, such as
legislative advocacy connected to public health professionals for nee-
dle exchange programs. 149
Legal creativity has its own special structure. The adversary
system itself may spark certain forms of creativity by requiring rever-
sals and responses to arguments and characterizations of facts. To
147. Ronald J. Gilson, Value Creation by Business Lawyers: Legal Skills and Asset
Pricing, 94 YALE L.J. 239 (1984).
148. Michael Dorf & Charles Sabel, Drug Treatment Courts and Emergent Experi·
mentalist Government, 53 VAND. L. REV. 831 (2000); Judith Kaye, Changing Courts in
Changing Times: The Need for a Fresh Look at How Courts are Run, 48 HAsTINGS L.J.
851 (1997).
149. Scott Burris, HN Education and the Law: A Critical Review, 20 L. MED. &
HEALTH CARE 377 (1992); Scott Burris et al, Syringe Exchange in Pennsylvania: A
Legal Analysis, 8 TEMP. POL. & Cry. RTS. L. REV. 41 (1998). See also Temple Law
School's "Legal Advocacy for Patients" Clinical Program.
153. Gardner describes the particular kinds of intelligences that political and or-
ganizational leaders are likely to have. GARDNER, INTELLIGENCE REFRAMED, supra
note 68, at 116-34 (linguistic, intrapersonal and interpersonal intelligences). He de-
fines leaders as those "people who can change the thoughts, feelings and/or behaviors
of a significant number of other people." Id. at 126.
154. See, e.g., GERALD GUNTHER, LEARNED HAND: THE MAN AND TIlE JUDGE (1994);
WILLIAM HARBAUGH, LAWYER'S LAWYER: THE LIFE OF JOHN W. DAVIS (1973); ARTHUR
KINOY: RIGHTS ON TRIAL: THE ODYSSEY OF A PEOPLE'S LAWYER (1983); JOHN NOONAN,
PERSONS AND MAsKS OF THE LAw: CARDOZO, HOLMES, JEFFERSON, AND WYTHE AS MAK.
ERS OF MAsKS (1976); MARK TuSHNET, MAKING CIVIL RIGHTS LAW: THURGOOD MAR·
SHALL AND THE SUPREME COURT (1994).
155. See, e.g., JONATHAN HARR, A CIVIL ACTION (1995); ANTHONY LEWIS, GIDEON'S
TRUMPET (1964); ANTHONY LEWIS, MAKE No LAw: THE STORY OF N.Y. TIMES VS. SULLI.
VAN (1991); GERALD STERN, THE BUFFALO CREEK DISASTER (1976) (all chronicling crea-
tive ideas for lawsuits, arguments, and legal strategies).
156. See Center for Public Resources-Institute for Dispute Resolution Problem
Solving and Legal Education Project Report, presented at Symposium on Dispute
Resolution and the Problem Solving Lawyer, Harvard Law School, April 7-8, 2000.
157. "Problem solving" is clearly the educational buzz word of the decade. Every
major university and liberal arts college is touting its goals to create a "problem solv-
ing curriculum," see, for example, Penn Arts & Sciences, University of Pennsylvania
Newsletter, Winter 2000, at http://www.collegeupenn.edulnewsletter/marOO/abc.htm
(Last Visited April 6, 2000). One class offered to undergraduates is GH 210, Seminar
in Faculty-Student Collaborative Learning: Developing an Interdisciplinary Problem-
Solving Curriculum Focused on West Philadelphia.
what creative ideas we can offer for legal education and it is to that
agenda that I now turn.
158. Every modem critique of the profession and legal education seems to begin
with the statement that we just need supplementation, not a totally new system. See
CPR Commission on Lawyering, Problem Solving and Legal Education, at http://
www.cpradr.org Gast visited February 18, 2001); ANTHONY T. KnONMAN, THE LoST
LAWYER: THE FAILING IDEALS OF THE LEGAL PROFESSION (1993); Brest and Krieger,
supra note 5.
159. Why wouldn't the adversarial and competitive economic frame similarly af-
fect business schools? Perhaps because the entrepreneurial culture also values crea-
tivity and the creation of new products?
160. See DEBORAH TANNEN, THE ARGUMENT CULTURE U99S); Carrie Menke!-
Meadow, The Trouble with the Adversary System in a Post-Modern, Multi·cultural
World, 38 WM. & IvlARY L: REv. 5 (1996).
161. Carrie Menkel-Meadow, Taking Problem Solving Pedagogy Seriously: A Re-
sponse to the Attorney General, 49 J. LEGAL Eouc. 14U999); Carrie Menkel-Meadow,
To Solve Problems, Not Make Them: Integrating ADR in the Law School Curriculum,
46 SMU L. REV. 1995 (1993).
165. The processfolio that documents change is in contrast to the portfolio, where
only the best is chosen and fails to document the thinking process itself. For teachers
of negotiation that use journals (l do not), which require students to reflect on what
they have learned, this is an equivalent use of a processfolio idea.
166. Many negotiation courses now use various methods to focus on interpersonal
skills such as use of the Thomas-Kilman Conflict MODE e.xercise, Myers-Briggs and
the Harvard Interpersonal Skills Exercise. Legal interviewing and counseling
courses have also long used insights from Rogerian therapy to teach skills of active
listening. Decision theory has been incorporated in many schools' teaching of legal
counseling, Brest & Krieger, supra note 5. For my argument that the human side of
lawyering is essential to any full conception of legal education, see Carrie Menkel-
Meadow, Narrowing the Gap by Narrowing the Field; What's Missing From the Mac-
Crate Report-Of Skills, Legal Science and Being a Human Being, 69 WASU. L. REv.
593 (1994).
167. See LEONARD RIsKIN & JAMES E. WESTBROOK, DISPUTE REsOLUTION AND 1..,\\,:.
YERS TEACHER's MANUAL (2d ed. 1998).
168. Clinicians, of course, have used conte.xtually rich cases for problem sohing for
close to thirty years. Some problems use real clients or dynamically changing facts to
simulate as much as possible the conditions of the real world, see BINDER, supra note
42.
solved in the real world are not neatly packaged in course-titled cate-
gories, problem solving in context calls on students not only to solve
problems, but to name and frame them as well.
Framing the intellectual and educational problem to be solved is
very important in transforming legal education into a truly problem
solving enterprise. As an example, consider a problem from the
Goldberg, Sander & Rogers DISPUTE RESOLUTION teh-t.174 In Colonial
Confectioners, Inc.,175 an elderly man seeks to sell his candy-making
factory and hopes the business will be continued by the purchaser.
The potential purchaser wants to tear down the factory and build a
more profitable office building. In the problem as written, both sides
have damaging confidential facts that the teacher's manual tells us
will kill the deal if they are revealed. 176 The purpose of the exercise
is for students to confront their truth telling practices and the role of
deception in negotiation. However, when I have used this problem
after class creativity exercises, students carefully reveal the "damag-
ing" information and work around it to create very interesting solu-
tions to the problem. 177 Thus, the curriculum's frame around a set of
problems or courses can strongly influence how students choose to
use the material. Using the framing effects, changing the focus with
the same problems over time illustrates how problems can change or
that they have many different issues. 178
Gardner suggests using different "entry points" for the study of
particular problems, which demonstrate that there are, in fact, many
epistemologies to be used in unpacking legal problems. 179 These in-
clude the use of narrative (a deeply contextualized story of the facts);
a logically based deductive version of the situation; a foundational-
terminological approach (what is at issue here for the parties, for the
larger society?); aesthetic understandings-(organizational charts, a
174. STEPHEN GoLDBERG, FRANK SANDER & NANCY ROGERS, DISPUTE REsOLUTION:
NEGOTIATION, MEDIATION AND OTHER PROCESSES (3d ed. 1999).
175. Id. at 106.
176. Both the fact of the purchaser's true interest and a rumor of an eminent do-
main action on the property are unknown to the respective sides.
177. These have included ESOPs for the candy factory employees, a historically
preserved front of the building, with a candy shop/factory on the first floor of a multi-
use office building, and a host of other solutions that take account of both parties'
preferences.
178. Some business school case studies, for example, are "framed" by the course
title as marketing, finance, accounting, or human relations problems. More comple.x
problems require students to consider many issues simultaneously. Ethics problems,
for e.xample, are almost always embedded in the larger conte.xt and more complicated
facts of the problem.
179. GARDNER, INTELLIGENCE REFRAMED, supra note 68, at 169-72.
183. At the conclusion of several focus groups with practicing lawyers at Ge-
orgetown, which is part of our curriculum review project, we have learned that recent
graduates feel that they are ill prepared for lawyering tasks and decision making that
do not involve conventional litigation choices or matters. Both younger lawyers and
more experienced lawyers want our graduates to be able to make decisions (often in
nano seconds), or draft documents in organizational tboth public interest and en-
trepreneurial) settings for which they are not prepared by our standard curriculum
and its steady diet of litigated cases.
184. This past year a group of Georgetown law students completed the legal and
political process to found a charter school in Washington DC that will focus on law
and legal issues for high school students. The school will open in the next academic
year. The students began work on this project through their own commitment and a
clinical course and completed it with assistance from each other. some professors and
other relevant community advisors.
185. For the purpose of this exercise I have left law school in its current form of
three years but ideally. legal training would take four years; the third year would be a
Northeastern-Antioch like full year of legal internship in the world. with a final re-
turn year in school for students to pursue more academically and with professorial
supervision answers to both substantive and processual issues that arose during the
field work year.