Adolphs - 2006 - Perception and Emotion How We Recognize Facial Expressions
Adolphs - 2006 - Perception and Emotion How We Recognize Facial Expressions
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/
info/about/policies/terms.jsp
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Sage Publications, Inc. and Association for Psychological Science are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to Current Directions in Psychological Science.
http://www.jstor.org
This content downloaded from 131.111.5.130 on Tue, 12 Jan 2016 13:52:01 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
and Emotion
Perception
How We Recognize Facial Expressions
Ralph Adolphs
Division of Humanities and Social Sciences and Division of Biology, California Institute of Technology
and emotion interact, as is borne rarely, directly from the brain in surgical patients). In those
ABSTRACT?Perception
out by studies of how people recognize emotion from facial experiments, the responses to many presentations of emotional
and neurological research has stimuli are across stimuli, and the changes in electrical
expressions. Psychological averaged
elucidated the processes, and the brain structures, that can be timed in relation to stimulus onset.
potential accurately
participate in facial emotion recognition. Studies have Evidence has also come from studies in which viewers were
judge the emotion shown in the face. Recent experiments For instance, subliminal presentation of only the whites of the
have argued that people actively explore facial expressions eyes of fearful faces results in measurable brain activation
that early of a geometric theories of human behavior. For instance, the theory that affect
perception (construction representation
of the face based on its features) led to subsequently separate and are processes, and that affect
cognitive judgment separate
can some
processing of the identity of the face and of the emotional ex precede cognitive judgment, receives support from the
pression of the face (Bruce & Young, 1986). That model has neuroscience findings (Zajonc, 1980). The data also add detail to
received considerable from neuroscience studies theories of visual consciousness. In one set of studies, emotional
support sug
that the are based on facial were shown to neurological who,
gesting separate processes separable expressions patients
In neuroimaging different because of their brain were unable to report the
neuroanatomical systems: studies, damage, seeing
of the brain are activated in response to emotional ex stimuli. An individual with damage to the visual cortex had
parts
pressions
or
identity changes; and brain damage
can result in "blindsight" for emotional faces: He could discriminate the
in but not in emotion shown in faces even he no
impairments recognizing identity recognizing by guessing, though reported
emotional or the reverse. visual experience of seeing the faces. A neuroimaging experi
expressions,
ment in this same individual revealed that the was
amygdala
on conscious awareness of the stimulus, the responses, colliculus),which may be independent of conscious vision
although
in turn, may contribute to conscious awareness. Evidence comes (Johnson, 2005), and cortical processing, which is usually ac
It is interesting to note
from studies event-related measures of the companied by conscious experience. that
using potentials,
more amphibians and reptiles have only subcortical visual process
brain's electrical recorded at the scalp (or, much
activity
since lack a neocortex. One broad of
ing, they interpretation
these observations is thus that the subcortical route for pro
Address correspondence to Ralph Adolphs, HSS 228-77, Caltech,
CA 91125; e-mail: cessing emotional stimuli is the more ancient one, and that in
Pasadena, [email protected].
This content downloaded from 131.111.5.130 on Tue, 12 Jan 2016 13:52:01 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Ralph Adolphs
mammals an additional, cortical route has evolved that probably abstract, and perhaps dimensional rather than categorical
as
allows more flexible behaviors based on learning and conscious pect of emotions, for which fear is but one instance?an issue
shown the reverse: that volitionally allocating attention to the amygdala that participate in perceiving emotion from faces.
stimuli can influence their emotional evaluation. Inhibiting at Brain-imaging studies, in particular, have found evidence for a
tention to visual stimuli that are distractors in a search task, for large number of other brain structures that may come into play,
example, results in devaluation of those stimuli when subjects depending on the emotion shown in the face and on the demands
are asked to rate them on affective dimensions of the experimental task.
(Raymond,
Fenske, & Tavassoli, 2003). Emotions thus represent the value of One framework that summarizes these data runs as follows.
stimuli?what or avoid, or behav Visual areas in the cortex encode the face's features
people approach cognitively temporal
iorally, volitionally
or
automatically. and bind them into a global perceptual representation of the
face. Subcortical visual areas as the colliculus,
(such superior
FEARAND DISGUSTMAYBE PROCESSED SPECIALLY prominent in animals such as
frogs) carry out coarser but faster
processing of the face. Both routes, the cortical and the sub
Several studies have found evidence that the amygdala, the cortical, feed visual information about the face into the amyg
subcortical structure discussed earlier, is disproportionately dala. The amygdala then associates the visual representation of
important for processing facial expressions of fear, although by the face with an emotional response in the body, as well as with
no means so. The other emotion for which a in the operation of other brain structures. For instance,
exclusively only changes
similar neuroanatomical specificity has been reported is disgust. it likely triggers autonomie responses (such
as skin-conductance
A of the cortex called the insula motor and response, the autonomie response of on
region represents sympathetic sweating
sensory aspects ofthat emotional response (Calder, Lawrence, & the palms of the hands) to the face, and it also modulates visual
Young, 2001). In lesion studies, damage to the amygdala is attention to the face.
shown to result in impairment in the ability to recognize fear Tracing the path from initial perception of the face to recog
from facial expressions. Similarly, damage to the insula can nizing the emotion it expresses is complicated by feedback loops
result in impairment in the ability to recognize disgust from fa and by multiple pathways that can be engaged. One class of
cial However, remain as influential as are controversial, argues that the
expressions. questions regarding exactly proposals, they
which emotion category or dimension is being encoded in emotional response elicited by the amygdala can in fact be used
these brain since other emotions are often also the viewer to reconstruct about the emotion shown
regions, variably by knowledge
impaired.
in the face. Roughly: if I experience a pang of fear within myself
SM, who has total damage to the amygdala on both sides of the emotion to infer what the emotional state of the person whose
faces, and she has no difficulty recognizing familiar individuals similar vein do not restrict themselves to the
amygdala
or to fear,
from their faces. She also has little difficulty recognizing most but more generally propose that we make inferences about other
facial with the notable of fear. When emotional states within ourselves aspects
expressions, exception people's by simulating
asked to judge the fearfulness of faces, SM is selectively and of those states (Goldman & Sripada, 2005). Emotional contagion
severely impaired (Adolphs, Tranel, Damasio, & Damasio, and imitation may be the earliest aspects of such a mechanism
However, other with similar are im that can be seen in infants, and and of
1994). patients damage already empathy theory
paired
on a wider array of emotions; in functional neuroimaging mind may be more complex elaborations that develop later in
studies, activation of the amygdala is seen in response to several life. In support of these ideas, a
provocative study found
recently
emotions in addition to fear; and even SM's highly specific im that constricted pupils make sad facial expressions look more
pairment depends
on the questions she is asked. For example, sad, and that this effect is correlated with an empathie pupillary
when asked to faces into of basic emotions, response in the viewer. In other words, at a sad face with
classify categories looking
SM is rather selectively impaired on fear; but when asked to rate more constricted pupils
causes one's own
pupils
to constrict, an
how the emotion shown in the face is, she is impaired on effect activation of brain known to be
arousing accompanied by regions
all emotions of negative valence (Adolphs, Russell, & Tranel, important for emotion and empathy (Harrison, Singer, Rothstein,
1999). These data suggest that, while the amygdala is critical for Dolan, & Critchley, 2006). That finding is in line with prior re
fear in faces, its role encompasses a broader, more ports facial reactions when viewed facial
recognizing documenting subjects
This content downloaded from 131.111.5.130 on Tue, 12 Jan 2016 13:52:01 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Perception and Emotion
1. Impaired recognition of fear in facial expressions, ognition. To obtain a detailed inventory of how different features
Fig. resulting from
within a fearful face are we conducted an
impaired eye gaze onto faces. The figure shows data from a study in subject processed, experiment
SM, who has hilateral damage to the amygdala. Panel a shows images de in which were shown a number of faces in which
subjects large
picting the regions in faces that viewers use to discriminate fear. Normal
most of the face was occluded and only very small portions of the
subjects, on the left, make notable use of the eyes, as well as some use of the
nose and mouth. face were revealed. these called
By contrast, SM, on the right, fails to make use of the eyes. randomly Viewing stimuli,
Panel b shows eye movements (white Unes) and locations of fixation (white was similar to facial a
"bubbles," viewing expressions through
circles) when participants view facial expressions of fear. Whereas normal
of cardboard
piece into which random little holes had been
subjects explore the faces in a particular pattern, concentrating on the eyes
on some viewers see a part of an ear, or
and the mouth (left), SM either does not explore the face at all or does so in a poked. Thus, trials, might
haphazard fashion that fails to make the normal number of fixations onto
part of the mouth or the eyes. A discrimination task asked
the eye region. The brain damage in subject SM is shown in panel c: A
subjects to discriminate whether these "bubbles" faces looked
horizontal magnetic resonance scan through SM's brain shows the front of
her brain at the top of the image, and the back of her brain at the bottom of happy or fearful. After thousands of trials, we then related the
the image. The two symmetrical black circles near the top middle part of of the on a trial to the re
performance accuracy subject given
the brain image are her lesions of the left and right amygdala. The graph in
in matching gions of the face stimulus that had been revealed on that trial.
panel d plots the accuracy expressions of fear with the word
in a task in which 6 different This method a that shows the strength
'Tear," emotions and words were given to generates resulting image
subjects. While SM is impaired (black bars) compared to healthy controls and reliability of the association between features of the face and
(white bars) when viewing the faces without instruction (in which case she discrimination of the emotion. For certain of the
does not fixate on the eyes of the faces normally), the explicit instruction to example, parts
fixate on
the eyes brought face such as the mouth and eyes would be associated with a
her performance to normal levels (gray bar,
arrow). From
"A Mechanism for Impaired Fear Recognition After better discrimination than other such as the
performance parts
by R. Adolphs, F. Gosselin, T.W. Buchanan, D.
Amygdala Damage," chin or ears (which do not provide discriminative information for
Tranel, P. Schyns, & A.R. Damasio, 2005, Nature, 433, pp. 69, 70, 71.
telling emotions
Copyright 2005, MacMillan Press. Reproduced with permission. apart).
Not surprisingly, we found that the eyes and themouth of faces
are most useful to viewers in discriminating the emotion; that is,
Thunberg, & Elmehed, 2000). stimuli. Remarkably, subject SM failed to make normal use of
the eyes in this task: Unlike healthy individuals, she did not
PERCEPTIONOF EMOTIONISACTIVE benefit in her recognition of emotion when shown the eye region
of facial expressions (Fig. la). Importantly, this impairment was
The studies discussed above have provided models of how present for all emotional expressions, not just fear: It just hap
several different brain structures interact, at various
points in pens to be the case that the eyes are the feature that most dis
time and often as a function of context and individual differ fear from those of the other basic
tinguishes expressions
ences, to infer another emotional state from observation emotions. The deficit was even more basic, however. It turned
person's
of their overt behavior. This account
emphasizes the construct out that SM failed to direct her gaze to the eye region of faces
ive role that emotion can
play in generating knowledge about the (Fig. lb). These data suggested that at least part of the reason SM
world, and the inferential, creative nature of the fails to recognize fear is that she fails to make use of the ex
emphasizes
This content downloaded from 131.111.5.130 on Tue, 12 Jan 2016 13:52:01 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Ralph Adolphs
pression of the eyes in faces; moreover her impaired use of the that probe the signaling end as well?ideally in naturalistic
eye of faces was associated with her not at the eyes situations face-to-face interactions between It
region looking involving people.
in the first place. Apparently, the amygdala is involved not only is worth stressing that essentially all studies of visual emotion
in the emotional responses to faces, once but also have used rather artificial stimuli: static black
perceived, perception (often
provides the prerequisite for such perception by guiding gaze and-white) photographs of (often posed) facial expressions?or
and attention to emotionally salient of the face at the in some cases
just drawings of facial expressions. It seems rea
regions
outset. This account was in a further in sonable to suppose that our emotional reactions and eye-gaze
supported experiment,
which SM was explicitly instructed to look at the eyes in facial responses to
photographs of faces would be different than those
expressions; when she did so, she was able to discriminate fear to real faces, and that context effects and individual differences
their at others, an that may contribute Are we more accurate at our own facial ex
gaze normally impairment ages? recognizing
to their impaired social behavior and point to possible strategies pressions than those of others, as some studies suggest (Elfen
for intervention and rehabilitation. Dysfunction in allocating bein & Ambady, 2003)? The future of research on facial emotion
attention to emotionally relevant stimuli is also likely
to con will depend on integrating the powerful tools provided by cog
tribute to other disorders, from post nitive neuroscience with to human
psychiatric ranging ethological approaches
traumatic stress disorder to schizophrenia. behavior.
FUTUREDIRECTIONS
Recommended Reading
One very intriguing approach for the future would be to combine Adolphs, R. (2002). Recognizing emotion from facial expressions:
Psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behavioral and
the "bubbles" technique (cf. Fig. la) with other dependent
Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1, 21-61.
measures. For instance, viewers could be shown randomly
Adolphs, R., Gosselin, E, Buchanan, T.W., Tranel, D., Schyns, P., &
parts of faces while neural (e.g., functional magnetic
sampled Damasio, A.R. (2005). (See References)
resonance imaging, fMRI) or psychophysiological (e.g., skin Gosselin, E, & Schyns, P.G. (2001). Bubbles: A technique to reveal the
conductance measures of emotional response use of information in recognition tasks. Vision Research,
response) 41,2261
are obtained. to what features in faces drive 2271.
Analogous asking
discrimination of we could LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon and Schuster.
behavioral-perceptual emotions,
then ask what features drive response, or
psychophysiological
what features drive brain activation. A state-of-the-art
regional
This content downloaded from 131.111.5.130 on Tue, 12 Jan 2016 13:52:01 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Perception and Emotion
Elfenbein, H.A., & Ambady, N. (2003). When familiarity breeds tation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. Brain, 124,
accuracy: Cultural exposure and facial emotion recogni 1241-1252.
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 276? Ohman, A.,Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention:
290. the snake in the grass. Journal
Detecting of Experimental Psy
Goldman, A.I., & Sripada, C.S. (2005). Simulationist models of face chology: General, 130, 466-478.
based emotion recognition. Cognition, 94, 193-213. J.E., Fenske, M.J., & Tavassoli, N. (2003). Selective attention
Raymond,
Harrison, N.A., T., Rothstein, P., Dolan, R.J., & Critchley, H.D. determines emotional responses to novel visual stimuli.
Singer, Psycho
(2006). Pupillary contagion: Central mechanisms engaged in logical Science, 14, 537-542.
sadness processing. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, Whalen, P.J., Kagan, J., Cook, R.G., Davis, EC, Kim, H., Polis, S.,
5-17. McLaren, D.G., Somerville, L.H., McLean, A.A., Maxwell, J.S., &
Johnson, M.H. (2005). Subcortical face processing. Nature Reviews Johnstone, T. (2004). Human amygdala responsivity to masked
This content downloaded from 131.111.5.130 on Tue, 12 Jan 2016 13:52:01 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions