Conditioning and Learning - Noba
Conditioning and Learning - Noba
Basic principles of learning are always operating and always influencing human behavior. This
module discusses the two most fundamental forms of learning -- classical (Pavlovian) and
instrumental (operant) conditioning. Through them, we respectively learn to associate 1) stimuli
in the environment, or 2) our own behaviors, with significant events, such as rewards and
punishments. The two types of learning have been intensively studied because they have
powerful effects on behavior, and because they provide methods that allow scientists to analyze
learning processes rigorously. This module describes some of the most important things you
need to know about classical and instrumental conditioning, and it illustrates some of the many
ways they help us understand normal and disordered behavior in humans. The module
concludes by introducing the concept of observational learning, which is a form of learning that is
largely distinct from classical and operant conditioning.
Tags:
Associative learning, Classical conditioning, Instrumental learning, Learning theory, Operant conditioning,
Pavlovian learning
Learning Objectives
Distinguish between classical (Pavlovian) conditioning and instrumental (operant) conditioning.
Understand some important facts about each that tell us how they work.
Understand how they work separately and together to influence human behavior in the world
outside the laboratory.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 1/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Students will be able to list the four aspects of observational learning according to Social
Learning Theory.
Although Ivan Pavlov won a Nobel Prize for studying digestion, he is much more famous for
something else: working with a dog, a bell, and a bowl of saliva. Many people are familiar with the
classic study of “Pavlov’s dog,” but rarely do they understand the significance of its discovery. In
fact, Pavlov’s work helps explain why some people get anxious just looking at a crowded bus, why
the sound of a morning alarm is so hated, and even why we swear off certain foods we’ve only
tried once. Classical (or Pavlovian) conditioning is one of the fundamental ways we learn about
the world around us. But it is far more than just a theory of learning; it is also arguably a theory of
identity. For, once you understand classical conditioning, you’ll recognize that your favorite music,
clothes, even political candidate, might all be a result of the same process that makes a dog drool
at the sound of bell.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 2/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
The Pavlov in All of Us: Does your dog learn to beg for food because you reinforce her by feeding her from the
table? [Image: David Mease, https://goo.gl/R9cQV7, (https://goo.gl/R9cQV7,) CC BY-NC 2.0, https://goo.gl/FIlc2e]
(https://goo.gl/FIlc2e%5D)
Around the turn of the 20th century, scientists who were interested in understanding the
behavior of animals and humans began to appreciate the importance of two very basic forms of
learning. One, which was first studied by the Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov, is known as
classical, or Pavlovian conditioning. In his famous experiment, Pavlov rang a bell and then gave
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 3/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
a dog some food. After repeating this pairing multiple times, the dog eventually treated the bell
as a signal for food, and began salivating in anticipation of the treat. This kind of result has been
reproduced in the lab using a wide range of signals (e.g., tones, light, tastes, settings) paired with
many different events besides food (e.g., drugs, shocks, illness; see below).
We now believe that this same learning process is engaged, for example, when humans associate
a drug they’ve taken with the environment in which they’ve taken it; when they associate a
stimulus (e.g., a symbol for vacation, like a big beach towel) with an emotional event (like a burst
of happiness); and when they associate the flavor of a food with getting food poisoning. Although
classical conditioning may seem “old” or “too simple” a theory, it is still widely studied today for at
least two reasons: First, it is a straightforward test of associative learning that can be used to
study other, more complex behaviors. Second, because classical conditioning is always occurring
in our lives, its effects on behavior have important implications for understanding normal and
disordered behavior in humans.
In a general way, classical conditioning occurs whenever neutral stimuli are associated with
psychologically significant events. With food poisoning, for example, although having fish for
dinner may not normally be something to be concerned about (i.e., a “neutral stimuli”), if it causes
you to get sick, you will now likely associate that neutral stimuli (the fish) with the psychologically
significant event of getting sick. These paired events are often described using terms that can be
applied to any situation.
The dog food in Pavlov’s experiment is called the unconditioned stimulus (US) because it elicits
an unconditioned response (UR). That is, without any kind of “training” or “teaching,” the
stimulus produces a natural or instinctual reaction. In Pavlov’s case, the food (US) automatically
makes the dog drool (UR). Other examples of unconditioned stimuli include loud noises (US) that
startle us (UR), or a hot shower (US) that produces pleasure (UR).
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 4/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
is almost always the same as the unconditioned response. However, it is called the conditioned
response because it is conditional on (or, depends on) being paired with the conditioned stimulus
(e.g., the bell). To help make this clearer, consider becoming really hungry when you see the logo
for a fast food restaurant. There’s a good chance you’ll start salivating. Although it is the actual
eating of the food (US) that normally produces the salivation (UR), simply seeing the restaurant’s
logo (CS) can trigger the same reaction (CR).
Another example you are probably very familiar with involves your alarm clock. If you’re like most
people, waking up early usually makes you unhappy. In this case, waking up early (US) produces a
natural sensation of grumpiness (UR). Rather than waking up early on your own, though, you
likely have an alarm clock that plays a tone to wake you. Before setting your alarm to that
particular tone, let’s imagine you had neutral feelings about it (i.e., the tone had no prior meaning
for you). However, now that you use it to wake up every morning, you psychologically “pair” that
tone (CS) with your feelings of grumpiness in the morning (UR). After enough pairings, this tone
(CS) will automatically produce your natural response of grumpiness (CR). Thus, this linkage
between the unconditioned stimulus (US; waking up early) and the conditioned stimulus (CS; the
tone) is so strong that the unconditioned response (UR; being grumpy) will become a conditioned
response (CR; e.g., hearing the tone at any point in the day—whether waking up or walking down
the street—will make you grumpy). Modern studies of classical conditioning use a very wide
range of CSs and USs and measure a wide range of conditioned responses.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 5/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Receiving a reward can condition you toward certain behaviors. For example, when you were a child, your
mother may have offered you this deal: "Don't make a fuss when we're in the supermarket and you'll get a treat
on the way out." [Image: Oliver Hammond, https://goo.gl/xFKiZL, (https://goo.gl/xFKiZL,) CC BY-NC-SA 2.0,
https://goo.gl/Toc0ZF] (https://goo.gl/Toc0ZF%5D)
Although classical conditioning is a powerful explanation for how we learn many different things,
there is a second form of conditioning that also helps explain how we learn. First studied by
Edward Thorndike, and later extended by B. F. Skinner, this second type of conditioning is known
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 6/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Now, once the rat recognizes that it receives a piece of food every time it presses the lever, the
behavior of lever-pressing becomes reinforced. That is, the food pellets serve as reinforcers
because they strengthen the rat’s desire to engage with the environment in this particular
manner. In a parallel example, imagine that you’re playing a street-racing video game. As you
drive through one city course multiple times, you try a number of different streets to get to the
finish line. On one of these trials, you discover a shortcut that dramatically improves your overall
time. You have learned this new path through operant conditioning. That is, by engaging with
your environment (operant responses), you performed a sequence of behaviors that that was
positively reinforced (i.e., you found the shortest distance to the finish line). And now that you’ve
learned how to drive this course, you will perform that same sequence of driving behaviors (just
as the rat presses on the lever) to receive your reward of a faster finish.
Operant conditioning research studies how the effects of a behavior influence the probability that
it will occur again. For example, the effects of the rat’s lever-pressing behavior (i.e., receiving a
food pellet) influences the probability that it will keep pressing the lever. For, according to
Thorndike’s law of effect, when a behavior has a positive (satisfying) effect or consequence, it is
likely to be repeated in the future. However, when a behavior has a negative (painful/annoying)
consequence, it is less likely to be repeated in the future. Effects that increase behaviors are
referred to as reinforcers, and effects that decrease them are referred to as punishers.
An everyday example that helps to illustrate operant conditioning is striving for a good grade in
class—which could be considered a reward for students (i.e., it produces a positive emotional
response). In order to get that reward (similar to the rat learning to press the lever), the student
needs to modify his/her behavior. For example, the student may learn that speaking up in class
gets him/her participation points (a reinforcer), so the student speaks up repeatedly. However,
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 7/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
the student also learns that s/he shouldn’t speak up about just anything; talking about topics
unrelated to school actually costs points. Therefore, through the student’s freely chosen
behaviors, s/he learns which behaviors are reinforced and which are punished.
An important distinction of operant conditioning is that it provides a method for studying how
consequences influence “voluntary” behavior. The rat’s decision to press the lever is voluntary, in
the sense that the rat is free to make and repeat that response whenever it wants. Classical
conditioning, on the other hand, is just the opposite—depending instead on “involuntary”
behavior (e.g., the dog doesn’t choose to drool; it just does). So, whereas the rat must actively
participate and perform some kind of behavior to attain its reward, the dog in Pavlov’s
experiment is a passive participant. One of the lessons of operant conditioning research, then, is
that voluntary behavior is strongly influenced by its consequences.
The illustration above summarizes the basic elements of classical and instrumental conditioning.
The two types of learning differ in many ways. However, modern thinkers often emphasize the
fact that they differ—as illustrated here—in what is learned. In classical conditioning, the animal
behaves as if it has learned to associate a stimulus with a significant event. In operant
conditioning, the animal behaves as if it has learned to associate a behavior with a significant
event. Another difference is that the response in the classical situation (e.g., salivation) is elicited
by a stimulus that comes before it, whereas the response in the operant case is not elicited by
any particular stimulus. Instead, operant responses are said to be emitted. The word “emitted”
further conveys the idea that operant behaviors are essentially voluntary in nature.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 8/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Understanding classical and operant conditioning provides psychologists with many tools for
understanding learning and behavior in the world outside the lab. This is in part because the two
types of learning occur continuously throughout our lives. It has been said that “much like the
laws of gravity, the laws of learning are always in effect” (Spreat & Spreat, 1982).
Classical conditioning is also involved in other aspects of eating. Flavors associated with certain
nutrients (such as sugar or fat) can become preferred without arousing any awareness of the
pairing. For example, protein is a US that your body automatically craves more of once you start
to consume it (UR): since proteins are highly concentrated in meat, the flavor of meat becomes a
CS (or cue, that proteins are on the way), which perpetuates the cycle of craving for yet more
meat (this automatic bodily reaction now a CR).
In a similar way, flavors associated with stomach pain or illness become avoided and disliked. For
example, a person who gets sick after drinking too much tequila may acquire a profound dislike
of the taste and odor of tequila—a phenomenon called taste aversion conditioning. The fact that
flavors are often associated with so many consequences of eating is important for animals
(including rats and humans) that are frequently exposed to new foods. And it is clinically relevant.
For example, drugs used in chemotherapy often make cancer patients sick. As a consequence,
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 9/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
patients often acquire aversions to foods eaten just before treatment, or even aversions to such
things as the waiting room of the chemotherapy clinic itself (see Bernstein, 1991; Scalera &
Bavieri, 2009).
Classical conditioning occurs with a variety of significant events. If an experimenter sounds a tone
just before applying a mild shock to a rat’s feet, the tone will elicit fear or anxiety after one or two
pairings. Similar fear conditioning plays a role in creating many anxiety disorders in humans,
such as phobias and panic disorders, where people associate cues (such as closed spaces, or a
shopping mall) with panic or other emotional trauma (see Mineka & Zinbarg, 2006). Here, rather
than a physical response (like drooling), the CS triggers an emotion.
Another interesting effect of classical conditioning can occur when we ingest drugs. That is, when
a drug is taken, it can be associated with the cues that are present at the same time (e.g., rooms,
odors, drug paraphernalia). In this regard, if someone associates a particular smell with the
sensation induced by the drug, whenever that person smells the same odor afterward, it may cue
responses (physical and/or emotional) related to taking the drug itself. But drug cues have an
even more interesting property: They elicit responses that often “compensate” for the upcoming
effect of the drug (see Siegel, 1989). For example, morphine itself suppresses pain; however, if
someone is used to taking morphine, a cue that signals the “drug is coming soon” can actually
make the person more sensitive to pain. Because the person knows a pain suppressant will soon
be administered, the body becomes more sensitive, anticipating that “the drug will soon take care
of it.” Remarkably, such conditioned compensatory responses in turn decrease the impact of the
drug on the body—because the body has become more sensitive to pain.
This conditioned compensatory response has many implications. For instance, a drug user will be
most “tolerant” to the drug in the presence of cues that have been associated with it (because
such cues elicit compensatory responses). As a result, overdose is usually not due to an increase
in dosage, but to taking the drug in a new place without the familiar cues—which would have
otherwise allowed the user to tolerate the drug (see Siegel, Hinson, Krank, & McCully, 1982).
Conditioned compensatory responses (which include heightened pain sensitivity and decreased
body temperature, among others) might also cause discomfort, thus motivating the drug user to
continue usage of the drug to reduce them. This is one of several ways classical conditioning
might be a factor in drug addiction and dependence.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 10/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
A final effect of classical cues is that they motivate ongoing operant behavior (see Balleine, 2005).
For example, if a rat has learned via operant conditioning that pressing a lever will give it a drug,
in the presence of cues that signal the “drug is coming soon” (like the sound of the lever
squeaking), the rat will work harder to press the lever than if those cues weren’t present (i.e.,
there is no squeaking lever sound). Similarly, in the presence of food-associated cues (e.g.,
smells), a rat (or an overeater) will work harder for food. And finally, even in the presence of
negative cues (like something that signals fear), a rat, a human, or any other organism will work
harder to avoid those situations that might lead to trauma. Classical CSs thus have many effects
that can contribute to significant behavioral phenomena.
As mentioned earlier, classical conditioning provides a method for studying basic learning
processes. Somewhat counterintuitively, though, studies show that pairing a CS and a US
together is not sufficient for an association to be learned between them. Consider an effect called
blocking (see Kamin, 1969). In this effect, an animal first learns to associate one CS—call it
stimulus A—with a US. In the illustration above, the sound of a bell (stimulus A) is paired with the
presentation of food. Once this association is learned, in a second phase, a second stimulus—
stimulus B—is presented alongside stimulus A, such that the two stimuli are paired with the US
together. In the illustration, a light is added and turned on at the same time the bell is rung.
However, because the animal has already learned the association between stimulus A (the bell)
and the food, the animal doesn’t learn an association between stimulus B (the light) and the food.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 11/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
That is, the conditioned response only occurs during the presentation of stimulus A, because the
earlier conditioning of A “blocks” the conditioning of B when B is added to A. The reason?
Stimulus A already predicts the US, so the US is not surprising when it occurs with Stimulus B.
Blocking and other related effects indicate that the learning process tends to take in the most
valid predictors of significant events and ignore the less useful ones. This is common in the real
world. For example, imagine that your supermarket puts big star-shaped stickers on products
that are on sale. Quickly, you learn that items with the big star-shaped stickers are cheaper.
However, imagine you go into a similar supermarket that not only uses these stickers, but also
uses bright orange price tags to denote a discount. Because of blocking (i.e., you already know
that the star-shaped stickers indicate a discount), you don’t have to learn the color system, too.
The star-shaped stickers tell you everything you need to know (i.e. there’s no prediction error for
the discount), and thus the color system is irrelevant.
Classical conditioning is strongest if the CS and US are intense or salient. It is also best if the CS
and US are relatively new and the organism hasn’t been frequently exposed to them before. And
it is especially strong if the organism’s biology has prepared it to associate a particular CS and US.
For example, rats and humans are naturally inclined to associate an illness with a flavor, rather
than with a light or tone. Because foods are most commonly experienced by taste, if there is a
particular food that makes us ill, associating the flavor (rather than the appearance—which may
be similar to other foods) with the illness will more greatly ensure we avoid that food in the
future, and thus avoid getting sick. This sorting tendency, which is set up by evolution, is called
preparedness.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 12/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
There are many factors that affect the strength of classical conditioning, and these have been the
subject of much research and theory (see Rescorla & Wagner, 1972; Pearce & Bouton, 2001).
Behavioral neuroscientists have also used classical conditioning to investigate many of the basic
brain processes that are involved in learning (see Fanselow & Poulos, 2005; Thompson &
Steinmetz, 2009).
After conditioning, the response to the CS can be eliminated if the CS is presented repeatedly
without the US. This effect is called extinction, and the response is said to become
“extinguished.” For example, if Pavlov kept ringing the bell but never gave the dog any food
afterward, eventually the dog’s CR (drooling) would no longer happen when it heard the CS (the
bell), because the bell would no longer be a predictor of food. Extinction is important for many
reasons. For one thing, it is the basis for many therapies that clinical psychologists use to
eliminate maladaptive and unwanted behaviors. Take the example of a person who has a
debilitating fear of spiders: one approach might include systematic exposure to spiders. Whereas,
initially the person has a CR (e.g., extreme fear) every time s/he sees the CS (e.g., the spider), after
repeatedly being shown pictures of spiders in neutral conditions, pretty soon the CS no longer
predicts the CR (i.e., the person doesn’t have the fear reaction when seeing spiders, having
learned that spiders no longer serve as a “cue” for that fear). Here, repeated exposure to spiders
without an aversive consequence causes extinction.
Psychologists must accept one important fact about extinction, however: it does not necessarily
destroy the original learning (see Bouton, 2004). For example, imagine you strongly associate the
smell of chalkboards with the agony of middle school detention. Now imagine that, after years of
encountering chalkboards, the smell of them no longer recalls the agony of detention (an
example of extinction). However, one day, after entering a new building for the first time, you
suddenly catch a whiff of a chalkboard and WHAM!, the agony of detention returns. This is called
spontaneous recovery: following a lapse in exposure to the CS after extinction has occurred,
sometimes re-exposure to the CS (e.g., the smell of chalkboards) can evoke the CR again (e.g., the
agony of detention).
Another related phenomenon is the renewal effect: After extinction, if the CS is tested in a new
context, such as a different room or location, the CR can also return. In the chalkboard example,
the action of entering a new building—where you don’t expect to smell chalkboards—suddenly
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 13/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
renews the sensations associated with detention. These effects have been interpreted to suggest
that extinction inhibits rather than erases the learned behavior, and this inhibition is mainly
expressed in the context in which it is learned (see “context” in the Key Vocabulary section below).
This does not mean that extinction is a bad treatment for behavior disorders. Instead, clinicians
can increase its effectiveness by using basic research on learning to help defeat these relapse
effects (see Craske et al., 2008). For example, conducting extinction therapies in contexts where
patients might be most vulnerable to relapsing (e.g., at work), might be a good strategy for
enhancing the therapy’s success.
Most of the things that affect the strength of classical conditioning also affect the strength of
instrumental learning—whereby we learn to associate our actions with their outcomes. As noted
earlier, the “bigger” the reinforcer (or punisher), the stronger the learning. And, if an instrumental
behavior is no longer reinforced, it will also be extinguished. Most of the rules of associative
learning that apply to classical conditioning also apply to instrumental learning, but other facts
about instrumental learning are also worth knowing.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 14/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
The stimulus controlling the operant response is called a discriminative stimulus. It can be
associated directly with the response, or the reinforcer (see below). However, it usually does not
elicit the response the way a classical CS does. Instead, it is said to “set the occasion for” the
operant response. For example, a canvas put in front of an artist does not elicit painting behavior
or compel her to paint. It allows, or sets the occasion for, painting to occur.
Stimulus-control techniques are widely used in the laboratory to study perception and other
psychological processes in animals. For example, the rat would not be able to respond
appropriately to light-on and light-off conditions if it could not see the light. Following this logic,
experiments using stimulus-control methods have tested how well animals see colors, hear
ultrasounds, and detect magnetic fields. That is, researchers pair these discriminative stimuli with
those they know the animals already understand (such as pressing the lever). In this way, the
researchers can test if the animals can learn to press the lever only when an ultrasound is played,
for example.
These methods can also be used to study “higher” cognitive processes. For example, pigeons can
learn to peck at different buttons in a Skinner box when pictures of flowers, cars, chairs, or
people are shown on a miniature TV screen (see Wasserman, 1995). Pecking button 1 (and no
other) is reinforced in the presence of a flower image, button 2 in the presence of a chair image,
and so on. Pigeons can learn the discrimination readily, and, under the right conditions, will even
peck the correct buttons associated with pictures of new flowers, cars, chairs, and people they
have never seen before. The birds have learned to categorize the sets of stimuli. Stimulus-control
methods can be used to study how such categorization is learned.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 15/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Another thing to know about operant conditioning is that the response always requires choosing
one behavior over others. The student who goes to the bar on Thursday night chooses to drink
instead of staying at home and studying. The rat chooses to press the lever instead of sleeping or
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 16/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
scratching its ear in the back of the box. The alternative behaviors are each associated with their
own reinforcers. And the tendency to perform a particular action depends on both the
reinforcers earned for it and the reinforcers earned for its alternatives.
To investigate this idea, choice has been studied in the Skinner box by making two levers
available for the rat (or two buttons available for the pigeon), each of which has its own
reinforcement or payoff rate. A thorough study of choice in situations like this has led to a rule
called the quantitative law of effect (see Herrnstein, 1970), which can be understood without
going into quantitative detail: The law acknowledges the fact that the effects of reinforcing one
behavior depend crucially on how much reinforcement is earned for the behavior’s alternatives.
For example, if a pigeon learns that pecking one light will reward two food pellets, whereas the
other light only rewards one, the pigeon will only peck the first light. However, what happens if
the first light is more strenuous to reach than the second one? Will the cost of energy outweigh
the bonus of food? Or will the extra food be worth the work? In general, a given reinforcer will be
less reinforcing if there are many alternative reinforcers in the environment. For this reason,
alcohol, sex, or drugs may be less powerful reinforcers if the person’s environment is full of other
sources of reinforcement, such as achievement at work or love from family members.
Modern research also indicates that reinforcers do more than merely strengthen or “stamp in”
the behaviors they are a consequence of, as was Thorndike’s original view. Instead, animals learn
about the specific consequences of each behavior, and will perform a behavior depending on
how much they currently want—or “value”—its consequence.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 17/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
This idea is best illustrated by a phenomenon called the reinforcer devaluation effect (see Colwill
& Rescorla, 1986). A rat is first trained to perform two instrumental actions (e.g., pressing a lever
on the left, and on the right), each paired with a different reinforcer (e.g., a sweet sucrose
solution, and a food pellet). At the end of this training, the rat tends to press both levers,
alternating between the sucrose solution and the food pellet. In a second phase, one of the
reinforcers (e.g., the sucrose) is then separately paired with illness. This conditions a taste
aversion to the sucrose. In a final test, the rat is returned to the Skinner box and allowed to press
either lever freely. No reinforcers are presented during this test (i.e., no sucrose or food comes
from pressing the levers), so behavior during testing can only result from the rat’s memory of
what it has learned earlier. Importantly here, the rat chooses not to perform the response that
once produced the reinforcer that it now has an aversion to (e.g., it won’t press the sucrose lever).
This means that the rat has learned and remembered the reinforcer associated with each
response, and can combine that knowledge with the knowledge that the reinforcer is now “bad.”
Reinforcers do not merely stamp in responses; the animal learns much more than that. The
behavior is said to be “goal-directed” (see Dickinson & Balleine, 1994), because it is influenced by
the current value of its associated goal (i.e., how much the rat wants/doesn’t want the reinforcer).
Things can get more complicated, however, if the rat performs the instrumental actions
frequently and repeatedly. That is, if the rat has spent many months learning the value of
pressing each of the levers, the act of pressing them becomes automatic and routine. And here,
this once goal-directed action (i.e., the rat pressing the lever for the goal of getting sucrose/food)
can become a habit. Thus, if a rat spends many months performing the lever-pressing behavior
(turning such behavior into a habit), even when sucrose is again paired with illness, the rat will
continue to press that lever (see Holland, 2004). After all the practice, the instrumental response
(pressing the lever) is no longer sensitive to reinforcer devaluation. The rat continues to respond
automatically, regardless of the fact that the sucrose from this lever makes it sick.
Habits are very common in human experience, and can be useful. You do not need to relearn
each day how to make your coffee in the morning or how to brush your teeth. Instrumental
behaviors can eventually become habitual, letting us get the job done while being free to think
about other things.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 18/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Classical and operant conditioning are usually studied separately. But outside of the laboratory
they almost always occur at the same time. For example, a person who is reinforced for drinking
alcohol or eating excessively learns these behaviors in the presence of certain stimuli—a pub, a
set of friends, a restaurant, or possibly the couch in front of the TV. These stimuli are also
available for association with the reinforcer. In this way, classical and operant conditioning are
always intertwined.
The figure below summarizes this idea, and helps review what we have discussed in this module.
Generally speaking, any reinforced or punished operant response (R) is paired with an outcome
(O) in the presence of some stimulus or set of stimuli (S).
The figure illustrates the types of associations that can be learned in this very general scenario.
For one thing, the organism will learn to associate the response and the outcome (R – O). This is
instrumental conditioning. The learning process here is probably similar to classical conditioning,
with all its emphasis on surprise and prediction error. And, as we discussed while considering the
reinforcer devaluation effect, once R – O is learned, the organism will be ready to perform the
response if the outcome is desired or valued. The value of the reinforcer can also be influenced
by other reinforcers earned for other behaviors in the situation. These factors are at the heart of
instrumental learning.
Second, the organism can also learn to associate the stimulus with the reinforcing outcome (S –
O). This is the classical conditioning component, and as we have seen, it can have many
consequences on behavior. For one thing, the stimulus will come to evoke a system of responses
that help the organism prepare for the reinforcer (not shown in the figure): The drinker may
undergo changes in body temperature; the eater may salivate and have an increase in insulin
secretion. In addition, the stimulus will evoke approach (if the outcome is positive) or retreat (if
the outcome is negative). Presenting the stimulus will also prompt the instrumental response.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 19/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
The third association in the diagram is the one between the stimulus and the response (S – R). As
discussed earlier, after a lot of practice, the stimulus may begin to elicit the response directly.
This is habit learning, whereby the response occurs relatively automatically, without much mental
processing of the relation between the action and the outcome and the outcome’s current value.
The final link in the figure is between the stimulus and the response-outcome association [S – (R –
O)]. More than just entering into a simple association with the R or the O, the stimulus can signal
that the R – O relationship is now in effect. This is what we mean when we say that the stimulus
can “set the occasion” for the operant response: It sets the occasion for the response-reinforcer
relationship. Through this mechanism, the painter might begin to paint when given the right tools
and the opportunity enabled by the canvas. The canvas theoretically signals that the behavior of
painting will now be reinforced by positive consequences.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 20/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
The figure provides a framework that you can use to understand almost any learned behavior
you observe in yourself, your family, or your friends. If you would like to understand it more
deeply, consider taking a course on learning in the future, which will give you a fuller appreciation
of how classical learning, instrumental learning, habit learning, and occasion setting actually work
and interact.
Observational Learning
Not all forms of learning are accounted for entirely by classical and operant conditioning. Imagine
a child walking up to a group of children playing a game on the playground. The game looks fun,
but it is new and unfamiliar. Rather than joining the game immediately, the child opts to sit back
and watch the other children play a round or two. Observing the others, the child takes note of
the ways in which they behave while playing the game. By watching the behavior of the other
kids, the child can figure out the rules of the game and even some strategies for doing well at the
game. This is called observational learning.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 21/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Children observing a social model (an experienced chess player) to learn the rules and strategies of the game of
chess. [Image: David R. Tribble, https://goo.gl/nWsgxI, CC BY-SA 3.0, https://goo.gl/uhHola]
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 22/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
police officers. In the example above, the children who already know how to play the game could
be thought of as being authorities—and are therefore social models—even though they are the
same age as the observer. By observing how the social models behave, an individual is able to
learn how to act in a certain situation. Other examples of observational learning might include a
child learning to place her napkin in her lap by watching her parents at the dinner table, or a
customer learning where to find the ketchup and mustard after observing other customers at a
hot dog stand.
Bandura theorizes that the observational learning process consists of four parts. The first is
attention—as, quite simply, one must pay attention to what s/he is observing in order to learn.
The second part is retention: to learn one must be able to retain the behavior s/he is observing in
memory.The third part of observational learning, initiation, acknowledges that the learner must
be able to execute (or initiate) the learned behavior. Lastly, the observer must possess the
motivation to engage in observational learning. In our vignette, the child must want to learn how
to play the game in order to properly engage in observational learning.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 23/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 24/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Bobo [Image: © Sémhur / Wikimedia Commons / CC-BY-SA-3.0 (or Free Art License), https://goo.gl/uhHola]
In this experiment (Bandura, Ross & Ross 1961), Bandura had children individually observe an
adult social model interact with a clown doll (“Bobo”). For one group of children, the adult
interacted aggressively with Bobo: punching it, kicking it, throwing it, and even hitting it in the
face with a toy mallet. Another group of children watched the adult interact with other toys,
displaying no aggression toward Bobo. In both instances the adult left and the children were
allowed to interact with Bobo on their own. Bandura found that children exposed to the
aggressive social model were significantly more likely to behave aggressively toward Bobo, hitting
and kicking him, compared to those exposed to the non-aggressive model. The researchers
concluded that the children in the aggressive group used their observations of the adult social
model’s behavior to determine that aggressive behavior toward Bobo was acceptable.
While reinforcement was not required to elicit the children’s behavior in Bandura’s first
experiment, it is important to acknowledge that consequences do play a role within observational
learning. A future adaptation of this study (Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963) demonstrated that
children in the aggression group showed less aggressive behavior if they witnessed the adult
model receive punishment for aggressing against Bobo. Bandura referred to this process as
vicarious reinforcement, as the children did not experience the reinforcement or punishment
directly, yet were still influenced by observing it.
Conclusion
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 25/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
We have covered three primary explanations for how we learn to behave and interact with the
world around us. Considering your own experiences, how well do these theories apply to you?
Maybe when reflecting on your personal sense of fashion, you realize that you tend to select
clothes others have complimented you on (operant conditioning). Or maybe, thinking back on a
new restaurant you tried recently, you realize you chose it because its commercials play happy
music (classical conditioning). Or maybe you are now always on time with your assignments,
because you saw how others were punished when they were late (observational learning).
Regardless of the activity, behavior, or response, there’s a good chance your “decision” to do it
can be explained based on one of the theories presented in this module.
Outside Resources
Article: Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not what you think it is. American
Psychologist, 43, 151–160.
Book: Bouton, M. E. (2007). Learning and behavior: A contemporary synthesis. Sunderland, MA:
Sinauer Associates.
Book: Bouton, M. E. (2009). Learning theory. In B. J. Sadock, V. A. Sadock, & P. Ruiz (Eds.), Kaplan &
Sadock’s comprehensive textbook of psychiatry (9th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 647–658). New York, NY:
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Book: Domjan, M. (2010). The principles of learning and behavior (6th ed.). Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 26/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Discussion Questions
1. Describe three examples of Pavlovian (classical) conditioning that you have seen in your own
behavior, or that of your friends or family, in the past few days.
2. Describe three examples of instrumental (operant) conditioning that you have seen in your
own behavior, or that of your friends or family, in the past few days.
3. Drugs can be potent reinforcers. Discuss how Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental
conditioning can work together to influence drug taking.
4. In the modern world, processed foods are highly available and have been engineered to be
highly palatable and reinforcing. Discuss how Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning can
work together to explain why people often eat too much.
5. How does blocking challenge the idea that pairings of a CS and US are sufficient to cause
Pavlovian conditioning? What is important in creating Pavlovian learning?
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 27/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
6. How does the reinforcer devaluation effect challenge the idea that reinforcers merely “stamp
in” the operant response? What does the effect tell us that animals actually learn in operant
conditioning?
7. With regards to social learning do you think people learn violence from observing violence in
movies? Why or why not?
8. What do you think you have learned through social learning? Who are your social models?
Vocabulary
Blocking
In classical conditioning, the finding that no conditioning occurs to a stimulus if it is combined
with a previously conditioned stimulus during conditioning trials. Suggests that information,
surprise value, or prediction error is important in conditioning.
Categorize
To sort or arrange different items into classes or categories.
Classical conditioning
The procedure in which an initially neutral stimulus (the conditioned stimulus, or CS) is paired
with an unconditioned stimulus (or US). The result is that the conditioned stimulus begins to elicit
a conditioned response (CR). Classical conditioning is nowadays considered important as both a
behavioral phenomenon and as a method to study simple associative learning. Same as Pavlovian
conditioning.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 28/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Context
Stimuli that are in the background whenever learning occurs. For instance, the Skinner box or
room in which learning takes place is the classic example of a context. However, “context” can
also be provided by internal stimuli, such as the sensory effects of drugs (e.g., being under the
influence of alcohol has stimulus properties that provide a context) and mood states (e.g., being
happy or sad). It can also be provided by a specific period in time—the passage of time is
sometimes said to change the “temporal context.”
Discriminative stimulus
In operant conditioning, a stimulus that signals whether the response will be reinforced. It is said
to “set the occasion” for the operant response.
Extinction
Decrease in the strength of a learned behavior that occurs when the conditioned stimulus is
presented without the unconditioned stimulus (in classical conditioning) or when the behavior is
no longer reinforced (in instrumental conditioning). The term describes both the procedure (the
US or reinforcer is no longer presented) as well as the result of the procedure (the learned
response declines). Behaviors that have been reduced in strength through extinction are said to
be “extinguished.”
Fear conditioning
A type of classical or Pavlovian conditioning in which the conditioned stimulus (CS) is associated
with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US), such as a foot shock. As a consequence of learning,
the CS comes to evoke fear. The phenomenon is thought to be involved in the development of
anxiety disorders in humans.
Goal-directed behavior
Instrumental behavior that is influenced by the animal’s knowledge of the association between
the behavior and its consequence and the current value of the consequence. Sensitive to the
reinforcer devaluation effect.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 29/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Habit
Instrumental behavior that occurs automatically in the presence of a stimulus and is no longer
influenced by the animal’s knowledge of the value of the reinforcer. Insensitive to the reinforcer
devaluation effect.
Instrumental conditioning
Process in which animals learn about the relationship between their behaviors and their
consequences. Also known as operant conditioning.
Law of effect
The idea that instrumental or operant responses are influenced by their effects. Responses that
are followed by a pleasant state of affairs will be strengthened and those that are followed by
discomfort will be weakened. Nowadays, the term refers to the idea that operant or instrumental
behaviors are lawfully controlled by their consequences.
Observational learning
Learning by observing the behavior of others.
Operant
A behavior that is controlled by its consequences. The simplest example is the rat’s lever-
pressing, which is controlled by the presentation of the reinforcer.
Operant conditioning
See instrumental conditioning.
Pavlovian conditioning
See classical conditioning.
Prediction error
When the outcome of a conditioning trial is different from that which is predicted by the
conditioned stimuli that are present on the trial (i.e., when the US is surprising). Prediction error
is necessary to create Pavlovian conditioning (and associative learning generally). As learning
occurs over repeated conditioning trials, the conditioned stimulus increasingly predicts the
unconditioned stimulus, and prediction error declines. Conditioning works to correct or reduce
prediction error.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 30/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Preparedness
The idea that an organism’s evolutionary history can make it easy to learn a particular
association. Because of preparedness, you are more likely to associate the taste of tequila, and
not the circumstances surrounding drinking it, with getting sick. Similarly, humans are more likely
to associate images of spiders and snakes than flowers and mushrooms with aversive outcomes
like shocks.
Punisher
A stimulus that decreases the strength of an operant behavior when it is made a consequence of
the behavior.
Reinforcer
Any consequence of a behavior that strengthens the behavior or increases the likelihood that it
will be performed it again.
Renewal effect
Recovery of an extinguished response that occurs when the context is changed after extinction.
Especially strong when the change of context involves return to the context in which conditioning
originally occurred. Can occur after extinction in either classical or instrumental conditioning.
Social models
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 31/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Authorities that are the targets for observation and who model behaviors.
Spontaneous recovery
Recovery of an extinguished response that occurs with the passage of time after extinction. Can
occur after extinction in either classical or instrumental conditioning.
Stimulus control
When an operant behavior is controlled by a stimulus that precedes it.
Vicarious reinforcement
Learning that occurs by observing the reinforcement or punishment of another person.
References
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall
Bandura, A., Ross, D., Ross, S (1963). Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology 66(1), 3 - 11.
Bandura, A.; Ross, D.; Ross, S. A. (1961). "Transmission of aggression through the imitation of
aggressive models". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 63(3), 575–582.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 32/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning & Memory, 11,
485–494.
Craske, M. G., Kircanski, K., Zelikowsky, M., Mystkowski, J., Chowdhury, N., & Baker, A. (2008).
Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
46, 5–27.
Dickinson, A., & Balleine, B. W. (1994). Motivational control of goal-directed behavior. Animal
Learning & Behavior, 22, 1–18.
Fanselow, M. S., & Poulos, A. M. (2005). The neuroscience of mammalian associative learning.
Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 207–234.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
13, 243–266.
Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology
of anxiety disorders: It’s not what you thought it was. American Psychologist, 61, 10–26.
Pearce, J. M., & Bouton, M. E. (2001). Theories of associative learning in animals. Annual Review
of Psychology, 52, 111–139.
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the
effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy
(Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). New York, NY:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 33/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Scalera, G., & Bavieri, M. (2009). Role of conditioned taste aversion on the side effects of
chemotherapy in cancer patients. In S. Reilly & T. R. Schachtman (Eds.), Conditioned taste
aversion: Behavioral and neural processes (pp. 513–541). New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Siegel, S. (1989). Pharmacological conditioning and drug effects. In A. J. Goudie & M. Emmett-
Oglesby (Eds.), Psychoactive drugs (pp. 115–180). Clifton, NY: Humana Press.
Siegel, S., Hinson, R. E., Krank, M. D., & McCully, J. (1982). Heroin “overdose” death:
Contribution of drug associated environmental cues. Science, 216, 436–437.
Spreat, S., & Spreat, S. R. (1982). Learning principles. In V. Voith & P. L. Borchelt (Eds.),
Veterinary clinics of North America: Small animal practice (pp. 593–606). Philadelphia, PA: W.
B. Saunders.
Thompson, R. F., & Steinmetz, J. E. (2009). The role of the cerebellum in classical
conditioningof discrete behavioral responses. Neuroscience, 162, 732–755.
Wasserman, E. A. (1995). The conceptual abilities of pigeons. American Scientist, 83, 246–255.
Authors
Mark E. Bouton
Mark E. Bouton is the Lawson Professor of Psychology at the University of
Vermont. His research on learning and extinction is internationally known. He is
a Fellow of several scientific organizations, including APA, APS, and the Society of
Experimental Psychologists. He was recently awarded the Gantt Medal from the
Pavlovian Society.
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 34/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
Bouton, M. E. (2023). Conditioning and learning. In R. Biswas-Diener & E. Diener (Eds), Noba
textbook series: Psychology. Champaign, IL: DEF publishers. Retrieved from http://noba.to/ajxhcqdr
(http://noba.to/ajxhcqdr)
SECTIONS
Abstract
Learning Objectives
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 35/36
8/10/23, 7:19 AM Conditioning and Learning | Noba
https://nobaproject.com/modules/conditioning-and-learning 36/36