EDUC 210 - Module 1
EDUC 210 - Module 1
learning, and motivation that takes place in and out of schools. We believe this is the most
important course you will take to prepare for your future as an educator, whether your “students”
are children or adults learning in classrooms or in environments that are outside schools. In fact,
there is evidence that new teachers who have completed coursework in development and
learning are twice as likely to stay in teaching (National Commission on Teaching and America’s
Future, 2003). This may be a required course for you, so let us make the case for educational
psychology by introducing you to classrooms today. 3
facts taken from 2011 and 2016 Census data (Statistics Canada, 2018a-c; Statistics Canada,
2018):
immigrants (62%) come from Asian countries, but people come to Canada from all
over the world. More than 250 ethnic origins were reported.
groups, and approximately 21% report speaking a language other than English or
French at home.
Vancouver School Board speak more than 120 languages and it’s not uncommon to
have more English learners than native speakers in classrooms in some
neighbourhoods.
Indigenous communities across Canada are young and growing. Currently, they
religions other than Christianity is growing, particularly in the Muslim, Hindu, Sikh,
Nearly 1.2 million (approximately 25%) children live in poverty in Canada and
children represent 36% of regular users of food banks (Canadian Institute of Child
Children in classrooms have diverse abilities and disabilities. Our inclusive policies
mean that children with disabilities spend the majority of their school day in general
education classrooms.
Children are surviving diseases as serious as cancer and returning to school, but
sometimes with so-named “late effects” related to the treatment they underwent that
Finally, children’s families are diverse. Some children live with a mom and dad, but
many live with a mom or dad, and some live with two moms or two dads (e.g., in the
case of gay and lesbian parents or blended families where parents are divorced and
remarried). Still others live with grandparents, or with aunts and uncles.
One thing these children have in common is that they are all “digital natives”—they have not
lived without digital technologies and many are better equipped than their teachers to deal with
the changes to learning and living technology brings. Of course, there is also a growing “digital
These statistics are dramatic, but a bit impersonal. As a teacher, counsellor, recreational worker,
speech therapist, or family member, you will encounter real children. You will meet many
individual children in this text, too. Even though students in classrooms are increasingly diverse
in terms of race, ethnicity, language, and economic and technological advantages, the teaching
force remains very homogeneous. Clearly, it is important for all teachers to understand and work
effectively with all their students. Several chapters in this text are devoted to understanding
students. In addition, we will explore the concepts of student diversity and inclusion through the
But teaching and learning in the contexts described above can be challenging for both teachers
and students. This text is about understanding the complex processes of development, learning,
motivation, teaching, and assessment so that you can become a capable and confident teacher.
A teacher’s belief that he or she can reach even difficult students to help them learn.
Much of Anita Woolfolk’s research has focused on teachers’ sense of efficacy, defined as a
teacher’s belief that he or she can reach even difficult students to help them learn. This confident
belief appears to be one of the few personal characteristics of teachers 4 that predict student
achievement (Çakırog˘lu, Aydın, & Woolfolk Hoy, 2012; Tschannen-Moran & Woolfolk Hoy,
2001; Tschannen-Moran, Woolfolk Hoy, & Hoy, 1998; Woolfolk & Hoy, 1990; Woolfolk Hoy,
Hoy, & Davis, 2009). Teachers with a high sense of efficacy work harder and persist longer even
when students are difficult to teach, in part because these teachers believe in themselves and in
their students. Also, teachers are less likely to experience burnout and more likely to be satisfied
with their jobs (Collie, Perry, & Martin, 2017; Fernet, Guay, Senécal, & Austin, 2012; Fives,
Anita Woolfolk (and other researchers) have found that prospective teachers tend to increase
their personal sense of efficacy as a consequence of completing student teaching, but sense of
efficacy may decline after the first year as a teacher, perhaps because the support that was
available during student teaching is gone (Woolfolk Hoy & Burke-Spero, 2005). Teachers’ sense
of efficacy is higher in schools when the other teachers and administrators have high
expectations for students and the teachers receive help from their principals in solving
instructional and management problems (Capa, 2005). Another important conclusion from this
research is that efficacy grows from real success with students, not just from the moral support or
cheerleading of professors and colleagues. Any experience or training that helps you succeed in
the day-to-day tasks of teaching will provide a foundation for developing a sense of efficacy in
your career. This text aims to provide the knowledge and skills that form a solid foundation for
teaching, were the major factors determining who learned in schools (e.g., Coleman, 1966). In
fact, much of the early research on teaching was conducted by educational psychologists who
refused to accept these claims that teachers were powerless in the face of poverty and other
How could you decide if teaching makes a difference in the lives of students? You could look to
your own experience. Did you have teachers who had an impact on your life? Perhaps one of
your teachers influenced your decision to become an educator. Even if you had such a teacher,
and we hope you did, one of the purposes of educational psychology is to go beyond individual
experiences and testimonies, powerful as they are, to systematically examine the impact of
teaching on the lives of students by using carefully designed research studies. Several such
Teacher–Student Relationships.
Bridget Hamre and Robert Pianta (2001) followed 179 children in a small school district from
the time they entered kindergarten through to the end of grade 8. The researchers concluded that
conflict with the child, the child’s dependency on the teacher, and the teacher’s affection for the
child) predicted a number of academic and behavioural outcomes through grade 8, particularly
for students with high levels of behavioural problems. Even when the gender, ethnicity, cognitive
ability, and behaviour ratings of the student were accounted for, the relationship with the teacher
still predicted aspects of school success. So students with significant behaviour problems in the
early years are less likely to have problems later in school if their first teachers are sensitive to
their needs and provide frequent, consistent feedback. In another study that followed children
from grades 3 through 5, Pianta and his colleagues found that two factors helped children with
lower skills in mathematics begin to close the achievement gap. The factors were higher-level
(not just basic skills) instruction and positive relationships with teachers (Crosnoe, et al., 2010).
RELATIONSHIPS MATTER Research has shown that the quality of the teacher–student relationship in
kindergarten predicts a number of academic and behavioural outcomes, particularly for students with
behavioural problems, who are less likely to have problems later in school if their teachers are sensitive to their
needs and provide frequent, consistent feedback.
Monkey Business Images/Shutterstock
5
effective or ineffective teachers in a row (Sanders & Rivers, 1996). They looked at fifth graders
in two large metropolitan school systems in Tennessee. Students who had highly effective
teachers for grades 3, 4, and 5, scored at the 83rd percentile on average on a standardized
mathematics achievement test in one district and at the 96th percentile in the other
(99th percentile is the highest possible score). In contrast, students who had the least effective
teachers 3 years in a row averaged at the 29th percentile in math achievement in one district and
44th percentile in the other—a difference of over 50 percentile points in both cases! Students
who had average teachers or a mixture of teachers with low, average, and high effectiveness for
the 3 years had math scores between these extremes. Sanders and Rivers concluded that the best
teachers encouraged good-to-excellent gains in achievement for all students, but lower-achieving
students were the first to benefit from good teaching. The effects of teaching were cumulative
and residual; that is, better teaching in a later grade could partially make up for less effective
teaching in earlier grades, but could not erase all the deficits. In fact, one study found that at least
7% of the differences in test score gains for students could be traced to their teachers (Hanushek,
Another study about test score gains from the Los Angeles public schools may be especially
interesting to you. Robert Gordon and his colleagues (2006) measured the test performance of
elementary school students in beginning teachers’ classes. Teachers were ranked into quartiles
based on how well their students performed during the teachers’ first 2 years. Then the
researchers looked at the test performance of students in classes with the top 25% of the teachers
and the bottom 25% during their third year of teaching. After controlling for the effects of
students’ prior test scores, their family income, and other factors, the students working with the
top 25% of the teachers gained an average of 5 percentile points more compared to students with
similar beginning-of-the-year test scores, while students in the bottom 25% lost an average of 5
percentile points. So students working with a less effective teacher could be an average of 10
percentile points behind the students working with an effective teacher. If these losses
accumulate, then students working with poorer teachers would fall farther and farther behind. In
fact, the researchers speculated that “. . . having a top-quartile teacher four years in a row would
be enough to close the black-white test score gap” of about 34 percentile points (Gordon,
Effective teachers who establish positive relationships with their students appear to be a
powerful force in those students’ lives. Students who have problems seem to benefit the most
from good teaching. What makes a teacher effective? What is good teaching? We consider those
points next.
scientists, historians, policy-makers, and parents, to name only a few groups, have examined this
question; there are hundreds of answers. And good teaching is not confined to classrooms—it
occurs in homes and hospitals, in museums and sales meetings, and in therapists’ offices and
summer camps. In this text we are primarily concerned with teaching in classrooms, but much of
outstanding teachers. All the situations that follow reflect the conditions in real classrooms
today.
half are English (or additional) language learners. As is true for most linguistically diverse
students in Canada, they spend 100% of 6 their school day using English instead of their native
An English language teacher helps Anne to integrate these students by working in Anne’s
classroom each day. Together Anne and the English language teacher support students in small
groups and make modifications to the curriculum to enable students who are English language
learners to participate in all the activities of the classroom. One strategy the two teachers have
found useful is to make information available through visual materials (e.g., pictures, diagrams,
word or concept maps). Anne also makes use of peer tutors and, whenever possible, offers one-
In addition to supporting students’ acquisition of English, Anne encourages students and their
parents to continue talking, reading, and writing in their first language at home. As well, she
fosters an appreciation for diverse languages and cultures in her classroom by designing tasks
and activities that invite students to draw on their cultural knowledge and by having students
compare and contrast their home or community experiences and practices during classroom
Anne makes a point of learning as much as she can about her students’ linguistic and cultural
heritages. She recognizes how important it is for teachers to understand how issues of language
and culture influence children’s learning, so that they do not misinterpret children’s motivation
and behaviour. This year, five languages are represented in Anne’s classroom: English, Hindi,
have varying racial, ethnic, family income, and language backgrounds. Ken emphasizes “process
writing.” His students complete first drafts, discuss them with others in the class, revise, edit, and
“publish” their work. The students also keep daily journals and often use these to share personal
concerns with Ken. They tell him of problems at home, fights, and fears; he always takes the
time to respond in writing. The study of science is also placed in the context of the real world.
The students use a National Geographic Society computer network to link with other schools in
order to identify acid rain patterns around the world. For social studies, the class plays simulation
games; for example, in two games that focused on the first half of the 1800s, the students “lived”
Throughout the year, Ken is very interested in the social and emotional development of his
students—he wants them to learn about responsibility and fairness as well as science and social
studies. This concern is evident in the way he develops his class rules at the beginning of the
year. Rather than specifying dos and don’ts, Ken and his students generate a list of rights and
responsibilities for their class. This list covers most of the situations that might need a “rule.”
An Inclusive Class.
Eliot was bright and articulate. He easily memorized stories as a child, but he could not read by
himself. His problems stemmed from severe learning difficulties with auditory and visual
integration and long-term visual memory. When he tried to write, everything got jumbled. His
teacher, Mia, and a special education teacher worked together to tailor tasks to take advantage of
Eliot’s strengths as well as to provide explicit and intensive instruction to address his learning
patterns and errors. With his teachers’ help, over the next years, Eliot became an expert on his
own learning and was transformed into an independent learner; he knew which strategies he had
to use and when to use them. According to Eliot, “Learning that stuff is not fun, but it works!”
What do you see in these three classrooms? The teachers are confident and committed to their
students. They must deal with a wide range of student abilities and challenges: different
languages, different home situations, and different abilities and disabilities. They must adapt
instruction and assessment to students’ needs. They must make the most 7 abstract concepts, such
as integrals, real and understandable for their particular students. Then there is the challenge of
how to use new technologies and techniques. The teachers must use them appropriately to
accomplish important goals, not just to entertain the students. And the whole time these experts
are navigating through the academic material, they are also taking care of the emotional needs of
these individuals from the first day of class, we would see that they carefully plan and teach the
basic procedures for living and learning in their classes. These teachers can efficiently collect
and correct homework, regroup students, give directions, distribute materials, collect lunch
money, and deal with disruptions—and they can do all this while also making a mental note to
find out why one of their students is so tired. Finally, these teachers are also reflective—they
constantly think back over situations to analyze what they did and why, and to consider how they
Reflective
Thoughtful and inventive. Reflective teachers think back over situations to analyze what
they did and why, and to consider how they might improve learning for their students.
invention of specific practices? Is a good teacher an expert explainer––“a sage on the stage”—or
a great coach––“a guide by the side”? These debates have raged for years. In your other
education classes, you probably will encounter criticisms of the scientific, teacher-centred sages.
choices. Teachers must be both knowledgeable and inventive. They must be able to use a range
of strategies, and they must also be able to invent new strategies. They must have some basic
research-based routines for managing classes, but they must also be willing and able to break
from the routine when the situation calls for change. They must know the research on student
development, and they also need to know their own particular students with their unique
characteristics of culture, gender, and geography. Personally, we hope you all become teachers
Anne, Ken, and Mia are examples of expert teachers, but they have been teaching for a long
time. What about you? Let’s look at what it is like to be a new teacher.
Imagine walking into class on your first day of teaching. List the concerns, fears, and worries
Beginning teachers everywhere share many concerns, including how to maintain classroom
discipline, motivate students, accommodate differences among students, evaluate students’ work,
deal with parents, and get along with other teachers (Conway & Clark, 2003; Melnick & Meister,
2008; Veenman, 1984). Many teachers also experience what has been called “reality shock”
when they take their first job because they really cannot ease into their responsibilities. On the
first day of their first job, beginning teachers face the same tasks as teachers with years of
experience. Student teaching, while a critical element of becoming a good teacher, does not
really prepare prospective teachers for starting off a school year with a new class. If you listed
any of these concerns in your response to the Stop & Think question, you should not be troubled.
They come with the job of being a beginning teacher (Borko & Putnam, 1996; Cooke & Pang,
1991).
With experience, hard work, and good support, seasoned teachers can focus on students’ needs
and judge their teaching success by the accomplishments of their students (Fuller, 1969; Pigge &
Marso, 1997). One experienced teacher described the shift from concerns about yourself to
concerns about your students: “The difference between a beginning teacher and an experienced
one is that the beginning teacher asks, ‘How am I doing?’ and the experienced teacher asks,
Our goal in writing this text is to give you the foundation to become an expert as you gain
experience. One thing experts do is listen to their students. Table 1.1 shows some advice students
in a grade 1 class gave to their student teacher: It looks like the students know about good
teaching, too.
Table1.1Advice for Student Teachers from Their Students
The students in Ms. Amato’s elementary school class gave this advice as a gift to their student teacher on her
last day.
2. Give us homework.
7. Teach us to read.
have been debates about what it really is. Some people believe educational psychology is simply
knowledge gained from psychology and applied to the activities of the classroom. Others believe
it involves applying the methods of psychology to study classroom and school life (Brophy,
2003; Wittrock, 1992). A look at history shows the close connections between educational
role of the teacher, the relationship between teacher and student, methods of teaching, the nature
and order of learning, the role of affect in learning—are still studied by educational
psychologists today. From its beginning, psychology in North America was linked to teaching. In
1890, William James officially founded the field of psychology and developed a lecture series
for teachers entitled Talks to Teachers on Psychology. These lectures were given in summer
schools for teachers and then published in 1899. James’s student, G. Stanley Hall, founded the
American Psychological Association. His dissertation was about children’s understandings of the
world; teachers helped him collect data. Hall encouraged teachers to make detailed observations
to study their students’ development—as his mother had done when she was a teacher. Hall’s
student, John Dewey, founded the Laboratory School at the University of Chicago and is
considered the father of the progressive education movement (Berliner, 2006; Hilgard,
1996; Pajares, 2003). Another of William James’s students, E. L. Thorndike, wrote the first
educational psychology text in 1903, and founded the Journal of Educational Psychology in
1910.
In the 1940s and 1950s, the study of educational psychology concentrated on individual
differences, assessment, and learning behaviours. In the 1960s and 1970s, the focus of research
shifted to the study of cognitive development and learning, with attention to how students learn
concepts and remember. More recently, educational psychologists have investigated how culture
and social factors affect learning and development (Anderman, 2011; Pressley & Roehrig, 2003).
The discipline concerned with teaching and learning processes; it applies the methods and
theories of psychology and has its own as well.
What is educational psychology today? The view generally accepted is that educational
psychology is a distinct discipline with its own theories, research methods, problems, and
techniques. Educational psychologists study learning and teaching and, at the same time, 9 strive
to improve educational policy and practice (Anderman, 2011; Pintrich, 2000). In order to
understand as much as possible about learning and teaching, educational psychologists examine
team) in some setting (a classroom or theatre or gym) (Berliner, 2006; Schwab, 1973). So
educational psychologists study child and adolescent development; learning and motivation,
including how people learn different academic subjects such as reading or mathematics; social
and cultural influences on learning; teaching and teachers; and assessment including testing
But even with this long history of interest in teaching and learning, are the findings of
educational psychologists really that helpful for teachers? After all, most teaching is just
common sense, is it not? Let’s take a few minutes to examine these questions.
thought, research, and money—sound pathetically obvious. People are tempted to say, and
Helping Students.
When should teachers provide help for lower-achieving students as they do class work?
they need help or they may be too embarrassed to ask for help.
and others watching are more likely to conclude that the helped student does not have the ability
to succeed. The student is more likely to attribute failures to lack of ability instead of lack of
Skipping Grades.
Should a school encourage exceptionally bright students to skip grades or to enter university or
college early?
social misfits. They are neither physically nor emotionally ready for dealing with older students
and would be miserable in the social situations that are so important in school, especially in the
later grades.
Answer Based on Research
Maybe. In A Nation Deceived: How Schools Hold Back America’s Brightest Children (2004),
Nicholas Colangelo, Susan Assouline, and Miraca Gross list the 20 most important points from
their report. The first two are: (1) Acceleration is the most effective curriculum intervention for
gifted children, and (2) for bright students, acceleration has long-term beneficial effects, both
academically and socially. Whether acceleration is the best solution for a student depends on
many specific individual characteristics, including the intelligence and maturity of the student as
well as the other available options. For some students, moving quickly through the material and
working in advanced courses with older students is a very good idea. See Chapter 4 for more on
Obvious Answers?
Many years ago, Lily Wong (1987) demonstrated that just seeing research results in writing can
make them seem obvious. She selected 12 findings from research on teaching. She presented six
of the findings in their correct form and six in exactly the opposite form to university students
and to experienced teachers. Both the college students and teachers rated about half of
the wrong findings as “obviously” correct. In a follow-up study, other participants were shown
the 12 findings and their opposites and were asked to pick which ones were correct. For 8 of the
12 findings, the participants chose the wrong result more often than the right one.
More recently, Paul Kirschner and Joren van Merriënboer (2013) made a similar point when they
challenged several “urban legends” in education about the assertion that learners know best how
to learn. Today society has strongly held beliefs about students being self-educating digital
natives who can multitask, having unique learning styles, and always making good choices about
how to learn; these beliefs have no strong basis in research, but they are embraced nonetheless.
You may have thought that educational psychologists spend their time discovering the obvious,
but the preceding examples point out the danger of this kind of thinking. When a principle is
stated in simple terms, it can sound simplistic. A similar phenomenon takes place when we see a
gifted dancer or athlete perform; the well-trained performer makes it look easy. But we see only
the results of the training, not all the work that went into mastering the individual movements.
And bear in mind that any research finding—or its opposite—may sound like common sense.
The issue is not what sounds sensible, but what is demonstrated when the principle is put to the
Quickly, list all the different research methods you can name. •
Educational psychologists design and conduct many different kinds of research studies. Some of
these are descriptive studies—that is, their purpose is simply to describe events in a particular
Descriptive studies
Studies that collect detailed information about specific situations, often using
Correlational Studies.
Often the results of descriptive studies include reports of correlations. We will take a minute to
examine this concept, because you will encounter many correlations in the coming chapters.
A correlation is a number that indicates both the strength and the direction of a relationship
between two events or measurements. Correlations range from 1.00 to –1.00. The closer the
correlation is to either 1.00 or –1.00, the stronger the relationship. For example, the correlation
between height and weight is about .70 (a strong relationship); the correlation between height
Correlation
The sign of the correlation tells the direction of the relationship. A positive correlation indicates
that the two factors increase or decrease together. As one gets larger, so does the other. Height
and weight are positively correlated because greater height tends to be associated with greater
weight. A negative correlation means that increases in one factor are related to decreases in the
other. For example, the less you pay for a theatre or concert ticket, the greater your distance from
the stage. It is important to note that correlations do not prove cause and effect (see Figure 1.1).
Height and weight are correlated—taller people tend to weigh more than shorter people. But
gaining weight obviously does not cause you to grow taller. Knowing a person’s height simply
allows you to make a general prediction about that person’s weight. Educational psychologists
identify correlations so that they can make predictions about important events in the classroom.
Positive correlation
A relationship between two variables in which the two increase or decrease together.
Negative correlation
A relationship between two variables in which a high value on one is associated with a
low value on the other. Example: height and distance from top of head to the ceiling.
Figure 1.1 Correlations do not show causation
When research shows that broken homes and crime are correlated, it does not show causation. Poverty, a third
variable, may be the cause of both crime and broken homes.
Experimentation
Research method in which variables are manipulated and the effects recorded.
Experimental Studies.
A second type of research—experimentation—allows educational psychologists to go beyond
predictions and actually study cause and effect. Instead of just observing and describing an
existing situation, the investigators introduce changes and note the results. First, a number of
comparable groups of subjects are created. In psychological 11 research, the
term participants (also called subjects) generally refers to the people being studied—such as
teachers or grade 8 students—not to subjects such as math or science. One common way to make
sure that groups of subjects are essentially the same is to assign each subject to a group using a
random procedure. Random means that each subject has an equal chance to be in any
group. Quasi-experimental studies meet most of the criteria for true experiments, with the
important exception that the participants are not assigned to groups at random. Instead, existing
Participants/subjects
People or animals being studied.
Random
Quasi-experimental studies
Studies that fit most of the criteria for true experiments, with the important exception that
the participants are not assigned to groups at random. Instead, existing groups such as
In experiments or quasi-experiments, for one or more of the groups studied, the experimenters
change some aspect of the situation to see if this change or “treatment” has an expected effect.
The results in each group are then compared. Usually, statistical tests are conducted. When
differences are described as statistically significant, it means that they probably did not happen
simply by chance. For example, if you see p < .05 in a study, this indicates that the result
reported could happen by chance less than 5 times out of 100, and p < .01 means less than 1 time
in 100.
Statistically significant
asking questions such as this: If some teachers receive training in how to teach spelling using
morphology, the study of the smallest parts of words that contain meaning—such as “s” or “ies”
for making words plural—(cause), will the trained teachers’ students become better spellers than
students whose teachers did not receive training in morphology (effect)? This was a field
experiment because it took place in real classrooms and not a simulated laboratory situation. In
addition, it was a quasi-experiment because the students were in existing classes and had not
been randomly assigned to teachers, so we cannot be certain the experimental and control groups
were the same before the teachers received their training. The researchers handled this by
looking at improvement in spelling, not just final achievement level (Hurry et al., 2005).
withdrawing a treatment.
teaching method, or other intervention. One common approach is to observe an individual for a
baseline period (A) and assess the behaviour of interest; then try an intervention (B) and note the
results; then remove the intervention and go back to baseline conditions (A); and, finally,
reinstate the intervention (B). This form of single-subject design is called an ABAB experiment.
For example, a teacher might record how much time students are out of their seats without
permission during a week-long baseline period (A), and then try ignoring those who are out of
their seats but praising those who are seated, and record how many are wandering out of their
seats for the week (B). Next, the teacher returns to baseline conditions (A) and records results,
and then reinstates the 12 praise-and-ignore strategy (B) (Landrum & Kauffman, 2006). When
this intervention was first tested, the praise-and-ignore strategy proved effective in increasing the
time students spent in their seats (Madsen, Becker, Thomas, Koser, & Plager, 1968).
The clinical interview uses open-ended questioning to probe responses and to follow up on
answers. Questions go wherever the child’s responses lead. Here is an example of a clinical
interview with a 7-year-old. Piaget is trying to understand the child’s thinking about lies and
What is a lie?— What isn’t true. What they say that they haven’t done. —Guess how old I am.—
Twenty. No, I’m thirty.—Was that a lie you told me?—I didn’t do it on purpose.—I know. But is
it a lie all the same, or not?—Yes, it is the same, because I didn’t say how old you were.—Is it a
lie?—Yes, because I didn’t speak the truth.—Ought you be punished?—No. —Was it naughty or
not naughty?—Not so naughty.—Why?— Because I spoke the truth afterwards! (Piaget, 1965, p.
144).
Researchers also may employ case studies. A case study investigates one person or situation in
depth. For example, Benjamin Bloom and his colleagues conducted in-depth studies of highly
and neurologists to try to understand what factors supported the development of outstanding
talent. The researchers interviewed family members, teachers, friends, and coaches to build an
extensive case study of each of these highly accomplished individuals (Bloom et al., 1985).
Some educators recommend case study methods to identify students for gifted programs because
Ethnography.
Ethnographic methods, borrowed from anthropology, involve studying the naturally occurring
events in the life of a group to understand the meaning of these events to the people involved. In
educational psychology research, ethnographies might study how students in different cultural
groups are viewed by their peers or how teachers’ beliefs about students’ abilities affect
classroom interactions. In some studies the researcher uses participant observation, actually
participating in the group, to understand the actions from the perspectives of the people in the
situation. Teachers can do their own informal ethnographies to understand life in their
classrooms.
Ethnography
A descriptive approach to research that focuses on life within a group and tries to
Participant observation
A method for conducting descriptive research in which the researcher becomes a
happen over several months or years. Ideally, researchers would study the development by
observing their subjects over many years as changes occur. These are called longitudinal
studies. They are informative, but time-consuming, expensive, and not always practical:
Keeping up with participants over a number of years as they grow up and move can be
groups of students at different ages. For example, to study how children’s conceptions of
numbers change from ages 3 to 16, researchers can interview children of several different ages,
Longitudinal studies
Studies that document changes that occur in subjects over time, often many years.
Cross-sectional studies
Studies that focus on groups of subjects at different ages rather than following the same
Microgenetic studies
Longitudinal and cross-sectional research examines change over long periods of time. The goal
while the change is actually occurring. For example, researchers might analyze how children
learn a particular strategy for adding two-digit numbers over the course of several weeks. The
microgenetic approach has three basic characteristics: the researchers (a) observe the entire
period of the change—from when it starts to the time it is relatively stable; (b) make many
observations, often using videotape recordings, interviews, and transcriptions of the exact words
of the individuals being studied; (c) put the observed behaviour “under a microscope,” that is,
they examine it moment by moment or trial by trial. The goal is to explain the underlying
mechanisms of change—for example, what new knowledge or skills are developing to allow
change to take place (Siegler & Crowley, 1991). This kind of research is expensive and time-
like many categories, a bit fuzzy at the edges, but here are some simplified differences.
Qualitative research
Exploratory research that attempts to understand the meaning of events to the participants
Quantitative research
Research that studies many participants in a more formal and controlled way using
observations.
Qualitative Research
Case studies and ethnographies are examples of qualitative research. This type of research uses
words, dialogue, events, themes, and images as data. Interviews and observations are key
procedures. The goal is not to discover general principles, but, rather, to explore specific
situations or people in depth and to understand the meaning of the events to the people involved
in order to tell their story. Qualitative researchers assume that no process of understanding
meaning can be completely objective. They are more interested in interpreting subjective,
Quantitative Research
Both correlational and experimental types of research generally are quantitative because
measurements are taken and computations are made. Quantitative research uses numbers,
remove their own biases from their results. One advantage of good quantitative research is that
results from one study can be generalized or applied to other similar situations or people.
There has been considerable debate about the relative merits of qualitative and quantitative
approaches to research in education, as you will see in the Point/Counterpoint. 14
have emphasized evidence-based practices (McHugh & Barlow, 2010). The American
integration of the best available research with clinical expertise in the context of patient
Practices that integrate the best available research with the insights of expert practitioners
According to Robert Slavin (2002), tremendous progress has occurred in fields such as medicine,
In each of these fields, processes of development, rigorous evaluation, and dissemination have
innovations have transformed the world. Yet education has failed to embrace this dynamic, and
as a result, education moves from fad to fad. Educational practice does change over time, but the
change process more resembles the pendulum swings of taste characteristic of art or fashion
(think hemlines) rather than the progressive improvements characteristic of science and
The major reason for extraordinary advances in medicine and agriculture, according to Slavin, is
that these fields base their practices on scientific evidence. Randomized clinical trials and
In his Presidential Address to the First Conference of the International Mind, Brain, and
What happened to education? If research produces useful knowledge for most of the industries
and businesses of the world, then should not it be serving the same function for education?
Somehow education has been mostly exempt from this grounding in research. Dewey (1896)
combining research with practice in schools, ensuring both formative evaluation and democratic
feedback. Unfortunately, his vision has never been realized. There is no infrastructure in
education that routinely studies learning and teaching to assess effectiveness. If Revlon and
Toyota can spend millions on research to create better products, how can schools continue to use
alleged “best practices” without collecting evidence about what really works? (Fischer, 2009)
Writing in the New York Times, Gina Kolata (2013) claimed that most educational programs
found to be effective in small unscientific studies have not increased student learning when
tested in large scientific studies. For example, a summer institute for math teachers improved the
teachers’ understanding of the math content they taught, but this gain in teacher knowledge did
David Olson (2004) disagrees strongly with Slavin’s position. He claims that we cannot use
medicine as an analogy to education. “Treatments” in education are much more complex and
unpredictable than administering one drug or another in medicine. And every educational
program is changed by classroom conditions and the way it is implemented. Patti Lather, a
colleague of Anita’s at Ohio State, says, “In improving the quality of practice, complexity and
impoverishment rather than improvement. That loss is being borne by the children, teachers, and
administrators in our schools” (Lather, 2004, p. 30). David Berliner (2002) makes a similar
point:
Doing science and implementing scientific findings are so difficult in education because humans
in schools are embedded in complex and changing networks of social interaction. The
participants in those networks have variable power to affect each other from day to day, and the
ordinary events of life (a sick child, a messy divorce, a passionate love affair, migraine
headaches, hot flashes, a birthday party, alcohol abuse, a new principal, a new child in the
classroom, rain that keeps the children from a recess outside the school building) all affect doing
Compared to designing bridges and circuits or splitting either atoms or genes, the science to help
change schools and classrooms is harder to do because context cannot be controlled (p. 19).
Berliner concludes that “a single method is not what the government should be promoting for
tells us specifically what happened in one or a few situations. Conclusions can be applied deeply,
but only to what was studied. Quantitative research can tell us what generally happens under
certain conditions. Conclusions can be applied more broadly. Today many researchers are
questions both broadly and deeply. In the final analysis, the methods used—quantitative,
research can ask different questions and provide different kinds of answers, as you can see
in Table 1.2.
Table1.2What Can We Learn?
Different approaches to research can ask and answer different questions.
METHOD ADDRESSED
METHOD ADDRESSED
Case Studies To understand one or a few How does one boy make the
issues, accomplishments,
METHOD ADDRESSED
they respond?
learning climate by
beginning of school.
15
Teachers as Researchers.
Finally, research can also be a way to improve teaching in one classroom or one school. The
same kind of careful observation, intervention, data gathering, and analysis that occurs in large
research projects can be applied in any classroom to answer questions such as “Which writing
prompts seem to encourage the best descriptive writing in my class?” “When does Kenyon seem
to have the greatest difficulty concentrating on academic tasks?” “Would assigning task roles in
science groups lead to more equitable participation of girls and boys in the work?” This kind of
problem-solving investigation is called action research (or teacher research, or teacher inquiry).
By focusing on a specific problem and making careful observations, teachers can learn a great
Action research
You can find reports of the findings from all types of studies in journals that are referenced in
this text. Table 1.3 provides a list of some of the major journals that publish work in educational
and developmental psychology. As authors, we have published articles in many of these journals
and also have reviewed manuscripts to decide what will be published. For many years, Anita was
the editor of the Theory Into Practice journal, and Nancy sat on the editorial board. The goal of
that journal is just what the title says—to bring the most useful theories into educational practice
and also to bring the wisdom of practice back to researchers who study education. Theory Into
someone teaches something to someone else in some setting (Berliner, 2006; Schwab, 1973).
Reaching this goal is a slow process. There are very few landmark studies that answer a question
once and for all. There are so many different kinds of students, teachers, tasks, and settings; and
besides, human beings are pretty complicated. To deal with this complexity, research in
time or life in one or two classrooms. If enough studies are completed in a certain area and
findings repeatedly point to the same conclusions, we eventually arrive at a principle. This is the
term for an established relationship between two or more factors—between a certain teaching
Principle
Another tool for building a better understanding of the teaching and learning processes is theory.
The commonsense notion of theory (as in “Oh well, it was only a theory”) is “a guess or hunch.”
But the scientific meaning of theory is quite different. “A theory in science is an interrelated set
of concepts that is used to explain a body of data and to make predictions about the results of
educational psychologists have developed explanations for the relationships among many
variables and even whole systems of relationships. There are theories to explain how language
develops, how differences in intelligence occur, and, as noted earlier, how people learn.
Theory
predictions.
You will encounter many theories of development, learning, and motivation in this text. Theories
are based on systematic research and they are the beginning and ending points of the research
cycle. In the beginning, theories provide the research hypotheses to be tested or the questions
examined. A hypothesis is a prediction of what will happen in a research study based on theory
and previous research. For example, two different theories might suggest two competing
predictions that could be tested. Piaget’s theory might suggest that instruction cannot teach
young children to think more abstractly, whereas Vygotsky’s theory might suggest that this is
possible. Of course, at times, psychologists do not know enough to make predictions, so they just
ask research questions. An example question might be, “Is there a difference in internet usage by
male and female adolescents from different ethnic groups?” 16
Hypothesis
A prediction of what will happen in a research study based on theory and previous
research.
17
understandings or theories;
systematic gathering and analyzing of all kinds of information (data) about the
interpreting and analyzing the data gathered using appropriate methods to answer
analyses; and
formulation of new and better questions based on the improved theories . . . and on
and on.
This empirical process of collecting data to test and improve theories is repeated over and over,
as you can see in Figure 1.2. Empirical means “based on data.” When researchers say that
identifying an effective antibiotic or choosing a successful way to teach reading is an “empirical
question,” they mean that you need data and evidence to make the call. Constructing decisions
from empirical analyses protects psychologists from developing theories based on personal
biases, rumours, fears, faulty information, or preferences (Mertler & Charles, 2005). Answering
questions with carefully gathered data means that science is self-correcting. If predictions do not
play out or if answers to carefully formulated questions do not support current best
understandings (theories), then the theories have to be changed. You can use the same kind of
Empirical
Based on systematically collected data.
Figure 1.2 A Research Cycle
Research begins with questions based on current understanding, followed by gathering, analyzing, and
interpreting information to answer the questions. Using these findings, the current theories are refined and
become the basis for new research questions.
Source: Woolfolk, A; Perry, N. E. (2002). Child and Adolescent Development, 1st Edition. Reprinted by permission of Pearson
Education, Inc., Upper Saddle River, NJ.
Few theories explain and predict a phenomenon perfectly. In this text, you will see many
the overall explanations of such issues as learning and motivation. Since no one theory offers all
So why, you may ask, is it necessary to deal with theories? Why not just stick to principles? The
answer is that both are useful. Principles of classroom management, for example, will give you
help with specific problems. A good theory of classroom management, on the other hand, will
give you a new way of thinking about discipline problems; it will give you tools for creating
solutions to many different problems and for predicting what might work in new situations. A
major goal of this text is to provide you with the best and the most useful theories for teaching—
those that have solid evidence behind them. Although you may prefer some theories over others,
We began this chapter by asserting that educational psychology is our favourite topic, as well as
a key source of knowledge and skills for teaching. We end this chapter with one more bit of
evidence for our enthusiasm. Educational psychology will help you support student learning,
Valerie Shute (2010) sifted through thousands of studies of student learning conducted over the
course of 60 years, seeking to identify those that had direct measures of student achievement in
reading and mathematics. Then they narrowed their focus to studies with strong effects. About
150 studies met all their rigorous criteria. Using the results from these studies, Lee and Shute
identified about a dozen variables that were directly linked to K–12 students’ achievement. The
researchers grouped these factors into two categories: student/personal factors and school and
social-contextual factors. These are shown in Table 1.4. It does not surprise us that educational
psychology provides a base for developing knowledge and skills in virtually every area.
Table1.4Research-Based Personal and Social-Contextual Factors That
Support Student Achievement in K–12 Classrooms
PERSONAL
FACTORS
Student
Engagement
activities.
connections, diminish
enjoyment in learning.
Learning
Strategies
processing of valuable
information (e.g.,
summarizing, inferring,
cognitive processes,
strategies.
managing, monitoring,
and evaluating their
skills in
time
management
test taking
help seeking
note taking
homework
management
CONTEXTU
AL
FACTORS
School
Climate
emphasize positive
community.
qualities of collective
efficacy, teacher
of affiliation.
qualities of collegiality,
conveyed goals.
Social-
Familial
Influences
achievement, encourage
Source: Based on Lee, J., & Shute, V. J. (2010). Personal and social-contextual factors in K–12 academic performance:
An integrative perspective on student learning. Educational Psychologist, 45, 185–202.
19
As you can see in Table 1.4, this text should help you become a capable and confident teacher
who can get students engaged in the classroom learning community—a community that respects
its members. This text will guide you toward becoming a teacher who helps students develop
into interested, motivated, self-regulated, and confident learners. As a consequence, you will be
able to set high expectations for your students, rally the support of parents, and build your own