Attitude - Introduction and Scope - 2005 Albaracin
Attitude - Introduction and Scope - 2005 Albaracin
net/publication/261796577
CITATIONS READS
245 14,046
4 authors, including:
G. Tarcan Kumkale
Ozyegin University
27 PUBLICATIONS 1,297 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Dolores Albarracin on 29 November 2020.
Human beings react to their environments in an evaluative fashion. They love and protect their
kin and strive to maintain positive evaluations of themselves as well as those around them. They
evaluate others' attractiveness. They also evaluate and select leaders, decide how to spend their
resources, and plan for the futures they envision. Such covert and overt actions often involve
judgments about whether objects, events, oneself, and others are favorable or unfavorable,
likeable or unlikeable, good or bad. Scholars who study attitudes investigate factors involved
in these evaluations: how they are formed, changed, represented in memory, and translated
into cognitions, motivations, and actions.
In this introductory chapter, we first discuss the nature of attitudes and then the organization
of this handbook. Scholars have investigated many different constructs related to attitudes
using many different theoretical frameworks and methods. The constructs that investigators
have studied often concern affect, beliefs, and (overt) behaviors. Affect entails the feelings that
people experience and may or may not concern a particular object or event (Berkowitz, 2000).
Beliefs are cognitions about the probability that an object or event is associated with a given
attribute (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). Behaviors are typically defined as the overt actions of an
individual. Each of these individual phenomena is central to the dynamic forces that form and
transform existing attitudes. Similarly, attitudes have a reciprocal impact on affects, beliefs, and
behaviors. It is this matrix of reciprocal attitudinal forces that constitutes a major portion of this
handbook.
Before providing a more extensive introduction to the matrix of reciprocal attitudinal re-
lations and the rationale for its use, we first discuss definitions of the attitude concept itself
and distinguish attitudes from affects, beliefs, and behaviors. We continue by explaining why
attitudes are not necessarily stable entities. We then discuss the rationale for the volume's orga-
nization and introduce each chapter. The organization of the volume is centered around basic
phenomena that attitudes scholars consider conventional relations rather than on a particular
3
4 ALBARRACIN ET AL.
singular theoretical viewpoint. Nonetheless, theories play a central role within each chapter of
this volume.
Defining Attitude
A handbook is a collective enterprise. Consequently, reaching definitions that satisfy all con-
tributors and readers is as difficult as it is indispensable. It is difficult because hundreds of
definitions exist. It is indispensable because, to develop a handbook of attitudes, contributors
must know the range of phenomena they might cover and precisely conceptualize the processes
at stake. Eagly and Chaiken (1993) provided what may be the most conventional contemporary
definition; specifically, an "attitude is a psychological tendency that is expressed by evaluat-
ing a particular entity with some degree of favor or disfavor" (p. 1, emphasis in original).
The contributors to the current volume have embraced evaluative aspects as central to the
topic, as have prominent other treatises on the subject (e.g., Eagly & Chaiken, 1998; Zanna &
Rempel, 1988). Although definitions may have varied somewhat across time, if one inspects
how scholars have operationalized the concept of attitude across the field's history, evalua-
tive aspects have always played a prominent role (e.g., Bogardus, 1931; Fishbein & Ajzen,
1975; Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953; Katz, 1960; Osgood, Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1957;
Petty & Cacioppo, 1981, 1986; Petty & Wegener, 1998; Sherif & Hovland, 1961; Thurstone,
1928).
The study of attitudes includes both the judgments that individuals form online (Schwarz
& Bohner, 2001) as well as the evaluative representations in memory (Fazio, 1986). If the
term attitude were reserved only to refer to stable structures in memory, excluded would be
all the work in which researchers verify only temporary changes on an attitude scale, as well
as an impressive amount of research on context effects in the study of attitudes. Moreover,
conceptualizing attitudes as memories but not judgments could possibly exclude the literature
on attitude formation and change, because these literatures concern the observation of judg-
mental outcomes much more often than they involve measures of memory. Thus, attitudes can
be judgments, memories, or both.
A good definition of a construct must not only be general but also sufficiently discriminating.
After all, there are multiple levels of generality and almost all definitions could be represented
at an even more abstract level. Consider the definition of beliefs as the perceived likelihood
that an attribute is associated with an object (e.g., Fishbein, 1963). For instance, I may believe
that Coca-Cola is sweet or that my country is now in a state of military alert. An examination
of the deep structure of attitudes makes it clear that one could also define attitudes as beliefs
(see Kruglanski & Stroebe, this volume; Wyer & Albarracin, this volume). Thus, a favorable
attitude toward social psychology might be defined as the perceived probability that the object
social psychology is positive or negative (Wyer, 1974).
Because attitudes and beliefs are at some level both categorizations, one could argue that
treating them as indistinct would make for a more compact definition. Indeed, compactness
was one of our explicit objectives in initiating this handbook. Nonetheless, we also had the
conflicting objective to reach sufficiently discriminating definitions so that one could distin-
guish between categories that have different properties and, often, different outcomes. In this
fashion, the concepts may appear to differ phenomenologically with some consensus. For in-
stance, although a belief and an attitude are both categorizations, and all categorizations can be
conceptualized as a probability assignment, Eagly and Chaiken (1993) noted that at least some
beliefs can be verified or falsified with external, objective criteria, whereas attitudes have more
1. ATTITUDES: INTRODUCTION AND SCOPE 5
difficulty facing such criteria. For instance, the belief that water freezes at 0 degrees Celsius
can be verified by agreement among different individuals. Sampling individuals from different
groups should have little influence on the extent to which this belief is verified in light of
external evidence. Yet few attitudes could withstand the same intersubjective validation. Most
social attitudes, such as political, aesthetic, or consumer preferences, are largely variable across
judges. A prominent exception is people's judgments of targets' physical attractiveness, which
typically show very high reliability across judges (e.g., Bersheid & Walster, 1974). Hence,
some attitudes will exhibit a high degree of social consensus, which some might interpret
as representing social reality. It is important to note that even among the most agreed-upon
attitudes we would find notable exceptions. To take another example, although most human
beings are afraid of snakes or apprehensive about heights, people who have pet snakes enjoy
them as much as sky divers are fond of heights.
Similarly, attitudes can be distinguished from affective reactions in that affective reactions
are not necessarily tied to a particular entity. Of course, it is common to equate how one feels
about an object with one's evaluation of it. Yet, there are several reasons to distinguish attitudes
from affect per se. Perhaps the most important one is that affect is often a powerful basis for
attitudes (see Wyer & Srull, 1989). Defining these two concepts as identical thus creates
logical complications that we and the other contributors hoped to avoid (see Schimmack &
Crites, this volume). In addition, it appears that affect and evaluation are distinct in their actual
phenomenology. For example, one might experience a pleasant sensory affect (see Schimmack
& Crites, this volume) if one walks by a bakery while on a diet, yet still feel apprehensive
toward cookies because of their unfortunate fattening side effects. This example, and many
similar ones that attitudinal ambivalence scholars have long studied (see Fabrigar, MacDonald,
& Wegener, this volume) would be difficult to conceptualize if one equated attitudes and
affect.
Similarly, several positions have emerged that explicate the components of attitudes. Most
notably, scholars have classified different types of attitude responses as well as different types
of information that can serve as bases for attitudes. For instance, Katz and Stotland (1959)
proposed that attitudes encompass cognitive, affective, and behavioral components. Eagly and
Chaiken's (1993, 1998) more contemporary analyses of this literature concluded that these
components best represent the types of responses that allow researchers to diagnose attitudes.
Moreover, people form attitudes on the basis of their cognitive, affective, and behavioral
responses to an entity (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993, 1998; Zanna & Rempel, 1988). Regardless of
the origins of attitudes, the term attitudes is reserved for evaluative tendencies, which can both
be inferred from and have an influence on beliefs, affect, and overt behavior. Treating attitudes
in a similar fashion, the contributors to this volume have analyzed the mutual relations of these
evaluations with beliefs, affect, and behavior. Thus, affect, beliefs, and behaviors are seen as
interacting with attitudes rather than as being their parts.
memory of a prior judgment, people often form judgments on the basis of information that is
temporarily available to them because the information is externally salient and/or momentarily
accessible in memory (see, e.g., Higgins, 1996). To this extent, people's evaluations of an
object can be represented in permanent memory or as judgments that individuals compute in an
online fashion at the time the evaluation becomes relevant. Therefore, although we differentiate
attitudes from affect, beliefs, and behavior, our definition of attitudes is inclusive enough
to encompass both stable, memory-based evaluations, and online, temporarily constructed
ones.
Figure 1.1 depicts the possibility that people's initial judgment about an object may be
stored for later use. The representation of that evaluative judgment in permanent memory,
however, is distinct from the initial judgment performed online and from later judgments that
one can possibly form after recalling the initial judgment. One kind of representation exists in
a latent, stored fashion (see dotted contours), even when people are currently unaware of it (see
Krosnick, Judd, & Wittenbrink, this volume). The other type of representation, the judgment,
only exists in consciousness or working memory (solid contours), either after retrieving an old
judgment or computing a new one on the basis of a prior judgment or other information that
is accessible in memory or externally supplied.
The chapters in this handbook clearly show that the attitudes field is vast and diverse on both
methodological and conceptual grounds, accumulating over 80-plus years. The field is con-
cerned with a variety of phenomena that occur as a result of the interaction between individuals
and the society in which they live. These phenomena take place in the hearts and minds of the
individual members of a society, but also across interpersonal communications and in the con-
text of cultural and social representations that transcend the individual. For example, people's
attitudes are generally the result both of relatively long-term processes such as socialization
and of relatively short-term exposures to information in the environment. Some attitudes may
even be inherited (e.g., Tesser, 1993). These inputs undergo sequential transformations that
give way to individual and social affective reactions, beliefs, attitudes, and overt actions. These
cognitions and behaviors acquire a life of their own and interact dynamically, generating and
1. ATTITUDES: INTRODUCTION AND SCOPE 7
receiving influences in a mutual, ever-changing cycle. This dynamic has different degrees of
consciousness, going from largely deliberate processes to subtle mechanisms of control that
may lie completely outside of awareness.
Theories remain important in contemporary studies of attitude, perhaps even more important
than they have been in the past. Yet because the numerous attitudes theories do not necessarily
make the same predictions about attitudinal phenomena nor even concern the same phenomena
and because there is no one theory with hegemony over the field, it would be misleading to
use any single theoretical approach to organize all knowledge about the topic. Instead, the
contributors to the current volume have kept as a distinct philosophy a fair treatment of the
theoretical diversity relevant to the attitudinal phenomenon under consideration.
Methodological Considerations
Regardless of which theories scholars use to explore attitudinal phenomena, central to the
endeavor is the use of scientific methods to provide observations that may be confirmed and
extended by other scholars. Where relevant, each of the chapters in this volume considers
methods of import. Most centered on methodological aspects is Jon Krosnick, Charles Judd,
and Bernd Wittenbrink's chapter, which thoroughly reviews classic and contemporary mea-
surement methods in the area of attitudes, including an insightful analysis of the advantages and
disadvantages of each procedure. The chapter is unique in its concentration on the processes
by which attitudes are expressed. Exemplifying this focus is their framework describing the
cognitive processes that generate an attitudinal evaluation as well as other response tendencies.
Krosnick and colleagues use this framework to derive various important recommendations for
the optimal measurement of attitudes. Following this chapter are a series of chapters analyzing
reciprocal causal relations of attitudes with affects, beliefs, and behaviors, and the structural
features of each of these four phenomena. We introduce these chapters next, before introducing
the concluding series of chapters that systematically describe ways in which the phenomena
in earlier chapters can be integrated.
Variable
Chapter 6 Chapter 4 The Influence of Behavior on Beliefs The Influence of Behavior on Affect
The Influence of Behavior on Attitudes The Origins and Structure of Behavior
— Cognitive dissonance, biased — Types and structure of behavior
scanning, role-playing, - Relationship between past behavior
self-perception, reactance, impression (habit), current behavior, and future
management, self-affirmation, behavior
selective exposure, automaticity, - Prediction vs. postdiction of behavior
reasoned and automatic influences - Methodological and data-analytic
- The role of individual difference issues in research on behavior
(e.g., preference for consistency, - Distal and proximal determinants of
attributional complexity) behavior
- Paradigms and theories of cognitive
dissonance
Chapter 8 The Influence of Beliefs on Behavior Chapter 7 The Influence of Beliefs on Affect
The Influence of Beliefs on Attitudes The Structure of Beliefs
- Relations among attitudes, beliefs, — Definition, structure, acquisition, and
and goals in the context of attitude change of beliefs
structure, functions, and dynamics - Theories of belief organization and
- Attitudes, goals, and beliefs as change
knowledge structures - Computation and motivational
- Belief-based models of attitudes processes from which beliefs emerge
- Ambivalence, dimensionality, mere - Inference, comprehension, and
exposure, conditioning, conformity memory processes in belief formation
- Current theorizing on persuasion and change
- Majority and minority influence - Heuristic and motivational bases of
- Motivated reasoning belief formation and change
Chapter 1 1 The Influence of Affect on Behavior The Influence of Affect on Beliefs Chapter 10
The Influence of Affect on Attitudes The Structure of Affect
- The role of affect in attitude - Operationalization and
formation and persuasion conceptualization of affect in attitude
- Dimensions of affective experience research
(valence and arousal) and attitudes - Unconscious and conscious affective
- Mood effects on judgment, experiences
affect-as-information, - Types of affective experiences and
affect-as-evidence their origins and implications for
- Role of emotion and mood in styles of attitudes
thinking or processing - Frequency, intensity, and duration of
- Unconscious affective influences on affective experiences
attitudes - Conditioning, mere-exposure,
- Affect and the use of category mood-as-information
information (stereotyping) - Representation of affect in memory
- Affect and evolutionary perspectives - Recentfindingsin affective
neuroscience
Note: Each cell off the diagonal refers to a causal combination of the attitudes, behavior, beliefs, and affect (feelings) variables. Shaded cells indicate phenomena with only indirect coverage
in this handbook.
1O ALBARRACIN ET AL.
trends in recent research. As relevant, each set of authors discuss theories for their attitudinal
phenomena. The dimensions that organize the handbook and the specific interactions they
generate have charted some new territory. For example, as we describe in the following sections,
attitude researchers have conceptualized the interrelations among beliefs, affect, attitudes, and
behavior. Yet researchers have rarely considered the degree to which an extant attitude biases
subsequent affective reactions. Therefore, the challenge of the handbook was sometimes to
identify research outside of the writers' domain, extrapolate findings, generate a relatively
complete line of facts and hypotheses about the issues at stake, and encourage future research
(see, e.g., Marsh & Wallace, this volume). Research conducted in other fields (e.g., political
behavior, intergroup relationships, mental health) and research not surveyed in prior books of
attitudes was also useful in achieving this synthesis (see, e.g., Ottati, Edwards, & Krumdick,
this volume).
activated (Bargh, 1997; Wilson, Lindsey, & Schooler, 2000). Both conscious and nonconscious
attitudes are likely to guide behavior provided external factors allow for implementation of
those actions. Self-efficacy and control beliefs may have similar effects, both because of their
motivational effects and as reflections of environmental obstacles and facilitators (Ajzen, 1991).
In addition, people's self-serving goals are important. For example, people's goals may create
a barrier between their attitudes and behaviors, as when individuals privately disagree with a
given advocacy but publicly comply in order to save face (see, e.g., Kelman, 1961; Nail, 1986).
also Anderson, 1981) theory of reasoned action asserts that the attitude toward the behavior is
a function of subjectively weighting the evaluative implications of each possible outcome /' of
the behavior ( e i , i — 1, . . . , n) by the belief that this outcome i will occur (b i ,,/ = 1, . . . , n),
and then summing these weighted evaluations. Similarly, extrapolating Greenwald's (1968)
cognitive-response framework, Petty and Cacioppo (1986) also argued that thoughts about an
issue can influence people's attitudes, provided that they have the ability and motivation to
think about the issues being considered. Other theories have elaborated on the way in which
different kinds of beliefs influence attitudes. For example, Deutsch and Gerard (1955) argued
that people may form positive attitudes about an issue because they are convinced that there is
evidence in support of the issue or as a result of changes in normative beliefs.
In this chapter, Arie Kruglanski and Wolfgang Stroebe use attitude structure, function, and
dynamics to examine social psychological research on the influences of beliefs and goals on
attitudes. According to Kruglanski and Stroebe, attitude structure, functions, and dynamics
have typically been treated as separate and as though they are concerned with rather different
issues. Given that attitudes, goals, and beliefs are to some extent knowledge structures, their
functions and dynamics are also isomorphic. In this context, the authors review such diverse
past and contemporary work as expectancy-value models, information integration theory,
probabilogical models, mere exposure and conditioning phenomena, the elaboration likelihood
model, and the unimodel.
Chapter IO. The Structure of Affect (Schimmack & Crites). Without a doubt,
people experience affect and this experience guides their cognitions, attitudes, and behavior,
as Ulrich Schimmack and Stephen Crites review in their chapter. Affect concerns the feelings
that people experience and may or may not concern a particular object or event (Berkowitz,
2000). Affect is presumably organized along dimensions of arousal and valence (Watson &
1. ATTITUDES: INTRODUCTION AND SCOPE 13
Tellegen, 1985), although this conceptualization is not without controversy. For example, an
important question in relation to the structure of affect is whether positive and negative affect
are two poles of the same construct or, instead, orthogonal dimensions. Furthermore, to what
extent is it necessary to distinguish among different emotions to understand attitudes? How is
affect represented in memory? How does affect change over time? How can we induce affective
change over time? How and when do people become aware of their affective experience?
Chapter 11. The Influence of Affect on Attitudes (Clore & Schnall). People's
responses to the affect they experience are both reflex-like and voluntary, as Gerald Clore and
Simone Schnall examine in this chapter. For example, sensory inputs like taste or exposure to
heights can trigger visceral reactions, and these reactions can automatically induce avoidance.
Many of these hard-wired responses are the result of evolutionary influences. In addition, affect
arising from any reaction to the environment, including mere exposure to an attitude object
(Zajonc, 1968), can influence attitudes. In this regard, Schwarz and Clore (1983) postulated that
people are inclined to misattribute their mood states to the object they are asked to judge. As
a consequence of this misattribution, people rely on a how-do-I-feel-about-it heuristic to infer
their attitudes toward the other persons, things, and events they encounter. There are, however,
other mechanisms that may underlie the influences of one's affective reactions on one's attitudes
(see, e.g., Festinger, 1957; Forgas, 1995; Hovland et al., 1953; Kaplan & Anderson, 1973). For
example, Hildum and Brown (1956; see also Insko, 1965) were able to condition people to form
positive attitudes toward an issue when the interviewer's nonverbal reactions were positive,
and negative attitudes when the interviewer's subtle feedback was negative. Research on the
potential mechanisms of this effect has accumulated over the years, suggesting that at least
some of these influences do occur outside of awareness. As the chapter describes, however, the
role of awareness in this domain remains controversial. Individuals may scrutinize information
more carefully when they experience negative affect than when they experience positive affect
(Schwarz & Clore, 1996; Worth & Mackie, 1987), an issue that this chapter also examines.
volume). Other such models include McGuire and McGuire's (1991) theory of thought systems,
which describes the complex relations among probability and desirability judgments, as well
as the elaboration likelihood (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986) and the heuristic systematic models
(Chaiken, 1980; Chaiken, Liberman, & Eagly, 1989), which describe effects of beliefs on
attitudes (e.g., central route', systematic and heuristic processing) and of affect or behavior
on attitudes (peripheral route; see Brifiol & Petty, this volume; Fabrigar et al., this volume).
In a similar vein, Fazio (1990) maintained that either elaborative or nonelaborative processes
may trigger behavior depending on the extent to which people think about their behavior
at a given time and the degree of behavior automaticity (see also Ouellette & Wood, 1998,
and Jaccard & Blanton, this volume). This line of theorizing has been extremely influential in
recent decades, as the chapter by Wegener and Carlston reveals across several domains. Finally,
various conceptualizations that have emerged in the last decade (Albarracin, 2002; Albarracin,
Wallace, & Glasman, 2004; Kruglanski & Thompson, 1999; Petty, Brinol, & Tormala, 2002)
promise to illuminate topics that cut across this book.
consistency, self-worth, and social approval. These four motives cut across almost all domains
of social psychology, including the study of the self, identity, and social cognition. Brinol and
Petty first describe these core motives and then discuss the relationship between motives and
attitude change processes and, in conclusion, their implications for attitude strength.
Chapter 16: Social Influence in Attitudes and Attitude Change (Prislin &
Wood). Attitudes are formed and persist in a cultural and social niche. In this chapter,
Radmila Prislin and Wendy Wood review such issues in relation to the matrix in Table 1.1 and
other factors. For example, normative beliefs are important determinants of attitudes as well
as behavior. Such norms most likely reflect the cultural structure of the social environment
and the interactions it contains (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). Thus, individuals' attitudes, affect,
beliefs, and behavior have social correlates, both because they often derive from socially shared
information, and because they collectively influence social representations, rules, and actions.
Therefore, in addition to reviewing classic topics of social influence (e.g., minority and majority
influence, conformity), Prislin and Wood's chapter integrates social scientific knowledge that
is relevant to the handbook matrix.
Chapter 18: Attitude Research in the 21st Century: The Current State of
Knowledge (Eagly & Chaiken). The main objective of the handbook is to review a
tradition of established knowledge in the area of attitudes and attitude change. In this final
chapter, Alice Eagly and Shelly Chaiken summarize this tradition, draw conclusions about the
state of the attitude literature, and point to areas that need further development.
16 ALBARRACIN ET AL.
CONCLUSION
This handbook attests to the mass of scientific knowledge that has accrued about attitudes:
Here is what is now known and may be learned about seemingly all nuances of the attitudinal
phenomena. Yet the chapters also point to areas in which understanding can be improved
through enhancements of method and theory, which can benefit future studies of attitudes. By
casting an attitudes spotlight on human affect, cognition, and behavior, the chapters in this
handbook collectively show that attitudes remain and will continue to be an indispensable
construct with which to understand the human condition.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The writing of this chapter was supported in part by grants KOI-MHO 1861, R01-NR08325,
and R01-MH58563 from the National Institutes of Health.
REFERENCES
Carlson, E. R. (1956). Attitude change through modification of attitude structure. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 52,256-261.
Carlston, D. E., & Smith, E. R. (1996). Principles of mental representation. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski
(Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 184-210). New York: Guilford.
Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in
persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752-766.
Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the
persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212-252). New York: Guilford.
Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual
judgment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 629-636.
Dulany, D. E. (1968). Awareness, rules, and prepositional control: A confrontation with S-R behavior theory. In
T. Dixon & D. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and behavior theory (pp. 340-387). New York: Prentice-Hall.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1998). Attitude structure and function. In D. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindsey (Eds.),
Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 269-322). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Fazio, R. H. (1986). How do attitudes guide behavior? In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of
motivation and cognition (pp. 204-243). New York: Guilford.
Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative
framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75-109). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.
Fishbein, M. (1963). An investigation of the relationships between beliefs about an object and the attitude toward that
object. Human Relations, 16, 233-240.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research.
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39-66.
Frey, D. (1986). Recent research on selective exposure to information. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 41-80). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Greenwald, A. G. (1968). Cognitive learning, cognitive response persuasion, and attitude change. In A. G. Greenwald,
T. C. Brock, & T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological foundations of attitudes (pp. 147-170). New York: Academic
Press.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psycho-
logical Review, 102, 4-27.
Heider, F. (1946). Attitudes and cognitive organization. Journal of Psychology, 21, 107-112.
Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. W.
Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133-168). New York: Guilford.
Hildum, D. C., & Brown, R. W. (1956). Verbal reinforcement and interviewer bias. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 53, 108-111.
Hovland, C. L, Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H. (1953). Communication and persuasion: Psychological studies of opinion
change. New Haven, CT: Yale University.
Insko, C. A. (1965). Verbal reinforcement of attitude. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 621-623.
Janis, I. L., & King, B. T. (1954). The influence of role-playing on opinion change. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 49, 211-218.
Jarvis, W. B. G., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The need to evaluate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,
172-194.
Judd, C. M., & Kulik, J. A. (1980). Schematic effects of social attitudes on information processing and recall. Journal
of Personality & Social Psychology, 38, 569-578.
Kaplan, M. F, & Anderson, N. H. (1973). Information integration theory and reinforcement theory as approaches to
interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 301-312.
Katz, D. (1960). The functional approach to the study of attitudes. Public Opinion Quarterly, 24, 163-204.
Katz, D., & Stotland, E. (1959). A preliminary statement to a theory of attitude structure and change. In S. Koch (Ed.),
Psychology: A study of a science (pp. 423-475). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation
(Vol. 15, pp. 192-238). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Kelman, H. C. (1961). Processes of attitude change. Public Opinion Quarterly, 25, 57-78.
Kerlinger, F. N. (1984). Liberalism and conservatism: The nature and structure of social attitudes. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kruglanski, A. W., & Thompson, E. P. (1999). Persuasion by a single route: A view from the unimodel. Psychological
Inquiry, 10, 83-109.
18 ALBARRACIN ET AL.
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1985). A theory of action identification. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso-
ciates.
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219-235.
Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 67, 1049-1062.
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34-52.
Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus actions: The relationship between verbal and overt behavioral responses to
attitude objects. Journal of Social Issues, 25, 41-78.
Wilson, T. D., Dunn, D. S., Kraft, D., & Lisle, D. J. (1989). Introspection, attitude change, and attitude-behavior
consistency: The disruptive effects of explaining why we feel the way we do. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 287-343). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Wilson, T. D., & Hodges, S. D. (1992). Attitudes as temporary constructions. In L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The
construction of social judgment (pp. 37-65). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101-126.
Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and
decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 181-192.
Worth, L. T., & Mackie, D. M. (1987). Cognitive mediation of positive affect in persuasion. Social Cognition, 5,
76-94.
Wyer, R. S. (1974). Cognitive organization and change: An information-processing approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Wyer, R. S., & Radvansky, G. A. (1999). The comprehension and validation of social information. Psychological
Review, 706,89-118.
Wyer, R. S., & Srull, T. K. (1989). Memory and cognition in its social context. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Zajonc, R. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monographs,
9(2, pt. 2), 1-27.
Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). Dissonance and the pill: An attribution approach to studying the arousal properties
of dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 703-709.
Zanna, M. P., & Rempel, J. K. (1988). Attitudes: A new look at an old concept. In D. Bar-Tal & A. W. Kruglanski
(Eds.), The social psychology of knowledge (pp. 315-334). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.