Consequentes e Antecedentes Da Regulação Emocional
Consequentes e Antecedentes Da Regulação Emocional
The need for change: Understanding emotion regulation antecedents and consequences using
ecological momentary assessment
Original Citation:
Availability:
Published version:
DOI:10.1037/emo0000671
Terms of use:
Open Access
Anyone can freely access the full text of works made available as "Open Access". Works made available
under a Creative Commons license can be used according to the terms and conditions of said license. Use
of all other works requires consent of the right holder (author or publisher) if not exempted from copyright
protection by the applicable law.
17 October 2023
The Need for Change: Understanding Emotion Regulation Antecedents and Consequences
Desirée Colombo1, Javier Fernandez Alvarez2, Carlos Suso-Ribera1, Pietro Cipresso2,3, Hristo Valev4,5, Tim
Leufkens4, Corina Sas5, Azucena Garcia Palacios1,6, Giuseppe Riva2,3, Cristina Botella1,6
1
Department of Basic Psychology, Clinic and Psychobiology, Universitat Jaume I, Spain
2
Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Italy
3
Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Italy
4
Philips Research, High Tech Campus, Eindhoven, Netherlands
5
School of Computing and Communications, Lancaster University, UK
6
CIBER Fisiopatología Obesidad y Nutrición (CIBERobn), Instituto Salud Carlos III, Spain
ABSTRACT
In recent decades, emotion regulation (ER) has been one of the most widely studied constructs within the
psychological field. Nevertheless, laboratory experiments and retrospective assessments have been the two
most common strands of ER research, thus leaving open several crucial questions about ER antecedents and
consequences in daily life. Beyond traditional methods, ecological momentary assessment (EMA) has the
potential to capture ER dynamics during the flow of daily experiences, in real-life settings and through repeated
measurements. Here, we discuss what we currently know about ER antecedents and consequences. We will
compare findings from previous literature to findings from EMA studies, pointing out both similarities and
differences, as well as questions that can be answered better with the EMA approach.
Emotion regulation (ER) is a process that every individual explicitly or implicitly implements in order to
downregulate or upregulate emotional states and achieve personal goals (Gross, 2015). ER can be
conceptualized as a complex process with physiological underpinnings, behavioral responses, and affective
and cognitive correlates. Adding to this complexity, the preponderant role played by contextual and momentary
factors is increasingly emerging, suggesting the dynamic and situational nature of this process (Aldao, 2013).
To date, research into ER has mostly been based on laboratory experiments, thus leaving situated and
momentary aspects relatively understudied. Additionally, most studies have relied on retrospective
questionnaires, that consider ER to be a trait (i.e. a relatively stable and cross-situational tendency of an
individual). However, trait measures only modestly correlate with ER in daily life (Brockman, Ciarrochi,
Parker, & Kashdan, 2017), which supports the need for a more ecological approach to the investigation of this
process.
An alternative strategy to laboratory experiments and retrospective assessments can be found in ecological
momentary assessment (EMA), a procedure that makes it possible to collect repeated inputs of thoughts,
feelings and behaviors close in time to the experience and in real-life contexts. The past few years have seen
a surge in studies using EMA due to the increased availability of mobile applications (Colombo, Fernandez
Alvarez, Palacios, et al., 2019) for both subjective (Suso-Ribera et al., 2018) and objective (Marzano et al.,
2015) data collection, and this method has already significantly increased our knowledge about ER outside
laboratory settings (Bylsma & Rottenberg, 2011). In fact, a more complex representation of the way people
regulate emotions is emerging that emphasizes the importance of capturing this process in the context where
In the present paper, we will provide an overview of the available literature on ER antecedents and
consequences. To do so, we will relate EMA findings to the broader literature based on laboratory and
retrospective studies, pointing out both similarities and differences, as well as questions that can be better
answered with the EMA approach. We will suggest EMA as a suitable methodology to extend our
understanding of ER, and we will propose a technology-based model to concurrently explore the affective,
among contextual and situational factors, momentary affect, and situation-specific goals.
Existing research has revealed the importance of the social context in ER, showing that both rapid changes in
the social context (McRae, Heller, John, & Gross, 2011) and stable contextual transactions (Srivastava, Tamir,
McGonigal, John, & Gross, 2009) influence this process. Additionally, context controllability has been shown
to be critical for ER. Accordingly, Troy and colleagues demonstrated that higher reappraisal ability (i.e. the
ability to down-regulate negative emotions) is associated with lower levels of depression after the exposure to
an uncontrollable stressor, but it is associated with higher subsequent depression in the context of a controllable
stressor (Troy, Shallcross, & Mauss, 2013). Finally, the general context where the emotions are regulated can
significantly impact ER. For example, one study shows that people are more likely to choose distraction in
high-intensity negative situations, whereas in low-intensity negative situations people are more likely to adopt
cognitive reappraisal, which involves rethinking the meaning of a stimulus in order to change its emotional
EMA studies have confirmed the role of both the social context and context controllability in ER. Suppression,
that is the inhibition of ongoing emotion-expressive behaviors, is more frequently adopted when other people
are present in the environment (English, Lee, John, & Gross, 2017) and it is used less frequently in situations
where people feel high in social hierarchy (Catterson, Eldesouky, & John, 2016). Additionally, individuals
with higher well-being report greater reappraisal adoption in situations that are low in controllability (Haines
et al., 2016). Beyond these results, EMA studies also extended the previous literature about contextual
determinants of ER. The identification of the trigger for one’s emotions has been shown to affect ER and, more
specifically, to be tied to the use of certain strategies, such as reappraisal and savoring (i.e. the attempt to
maintain and up-regulate positive emotions) (Heiy & Cheavens, 2014). Furthermore, the type of emotion
targeted also influences this process, so that people concurrently use more strategies in response to certain
Momentary affect
According to the broaden-and-build theory, the experience of positive emotions fosters people’s repertoires of
thoughts and actions and encourages the development of new coping skills (Fredrickson, 2001). Evidence
supporting this theory comes, for example, from a longitudinal study, showing that higher PA levels predict
increased positive coping skills eight weeks later (Burns et al., 2008).
Nonetheless, most of the previous literature only relied on retrospective measurements of ER, which made it
difficult to explore real-life relationships between momentary affect and ER. Fortunately, in recent years there
has been a surge in research using the EMA approach for the exploration of affective antecedents. For example,
high levels of momentary self-esteem, psychological adjustment, and PA have been found to trigger the use of
cognitive reappraisal (Nezlek & Kuppens, 2008). High levels of PA also predict greater adoption of problem
solving (Pavani, Le Vigouroux, Kop, Congard, & Dauvier, 2016) and mindfulness (Brockman et al., 2017),
whereas momentary NA has been associated with greater use of suppression (Brockman et al., 2017) and
rumination, which refers to the process of persistently thinking about one’s feelings (Li, Starr, & Hershenberg,
2017; Pavani et al., 2016). Interestingly, momentary affect also appears to influence the number of strategies
adopted, so that the use of different concurrent ER strategies increases in the presence of high levels of
Situation-specific goals
Along with the aforementioned contextual and affective factors, situation-specific goals have also been shown
to influence ER. Overall, the previous literature has shown that individuals who frequently reappraise and
distract tend to pursue prosocial and pro-hedonic goals more often, whereas people who habitually suppress
tend to pursue contra-hedonic and impression management goals more frequently (Eldesouky & English,
2018).
These results are consistent with the EMA literature, which has revealed that distraction and cognitive
reappraisal are used more frequently when hedonic goals are pursued, whereas suppression is more likely to
In the previous section, we explained that ER can be understood as the outcome of a number of antecedents.
However, ER also has multiple consequences on people’s lives. In the following paragraphs, we will discuss
two important outcomes that can be influenced by ER, namely affective and physiological states.
Momentary affect
A large amount of evidence from previous literature shows an influence of ER on subsequent affective states,
and most of the research concludes that cognitive reappraisal has a more adaptive profile in terms of affective
consequences, compared to suppression. Reappraisal is indeed related to experiencing less subsequent NA and
more PA, whereas suppression leads to experiencing less PA and more NA (Gross & John, 2003). Furthermore,
frequent use of savoring has been associated with greater happiness and well-being (Quoidbach, Berry,
Hansenne, & Mikolajczak, 2010), whereas recurrent use of rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, &
Lyubomirsky, 2008) and dampening (i.e. the tendency to decrease positive emotional experiences) (Wood,
Heimpel, & Michela, 2003) have been related to increased and prolonged NA.
Similarly, the body of research on affective consequences employing EMA is also extensive. The EMA
literature has confirmed previous findings regarding savoring (Jose, Lim, & Bryant, 2012) and dampening (Li
et al., 2017). However, contrasting results were observed for other strategies. For instance, cognitive
reappraisal has been found to be related to increased PA (Richardson, 2017), which is consistent with previous
research, but some studies failed to replicate its relationship with decreased NA (Brans et al., 2013; Brockman
et al., 2017; Nezlek & Kuppens, 2008; Pavani et al., 2016). Similarly, many EMA studies confirmed the
association between suppression and subsequent increased NA and decreased PA (Brans et al., 2013;
Brockman et al., 2017). Nevertheless, Richardson and colleagues observed no effects of suppression on NA
(Richardson, 2017), and Heiy et al. found that suppression did not predict subsequent affect (Heiy & Cheavens,
2014). Finally, the EMA literature confirmed the association between rumination and subsequent decreased
PA and increased NA (Brans et al., 2013; Li et al., 2017; Pavani et al., 2016), albeit with mixed results (Genet
Physiological responses
In addition to its influence on affective states, ER has also been shown to entail physiological concomitants.
Thus far, rumination and its physiological effects have been widely investigated, showing that high rumination
is associated with heightened activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPAA) and decreased
heart rate variability (HRV) (Ottaviani et al., 2016). Moreover, suppression has been associated with increased
sympathetic activation (Gross, 1998), whereas cognitive reappraisal has been linked to reduced physiological
response (i.e. skin conductance) (Gruber, Hay, & Gross, 2014). Finally, acceptance has been related to
with very few exceptions, such as the study by Brosschot and colleagues, which confirmed the association
between rumination and lower HRV using a daily ecological experiment (Brosschot, Van Dijk, & Thayer,
2007).
In addition to replicating traditional research, the implementation of EMA methodologies has shed light on
important gaps in the previous ER literature, that would have been difficult to address with non-ecological
research.
According to the EMA literature, not only can ER influence outcomes, but outcomes can also affect subsequent
ER processes, thus closing the circle. Momentary affect and ER, for example, have been shown to reciprocally
influence each other at a given point in time (Pavani et al., 2015); similarly, well-being has been shown to be
both a predictor (Haines et al., 2016) and an outcome (Quoidbach, Berry, Hansenne, & Mikolajczak, 2010) of
ER. From a theoretical point of view, these findings provide further support for the need to ecologically and
The use of EMA has also allowed for the exploration of moderators in the ER-to-outcomes relationship in real-
life settings. The results so far suggest that the effects of ER on subsequent outcomes are not linear, but rather
affected by contextual factors or by ER itself. In relation to the former, research has indicated that, on days
when people experience fewer positive events, momentary savoring (Jose et al., 2012) and positive rumination
(Li et al., 2017) are more relevant in increasing PA levels, whereas suppression is more strongly associated
with subsequent lower PA on more stressful days (Richardson, 2017). Regarding the moderating role of ER
strategies, a study found that rumination moderates the association between negative events and NA levels, so
that unpleasant events predict higher NA when rumination use is high, but not when it is low (Genet & Siemer,
2012).
Another contribution of the EMA approach is the exploration of the concurrent use of ER strategies over time.
Heiy and colleagues showed that individuals use, on average, seven strategies to regulate each experienced
emotion, which suggests that interactions among strategies may produce different outcomes (Heiy &
Cheavens, 2014). In this direction, McMahon and colleagues analyzed the concurrent use of reappraisal,
suppression, and rumination, and they showed that reappraisal compensates for the negative effects of
suppression and rumination on anxiety and depressive symptoms (McMahon & Naragon-Gainey, 2018).
Finally, a key contribution of the EMA approach has been the exploration of the way ER strategy use is
sequenced over time. Emerging evidence suggests that certain strategies are likely to affect subsequent strategy
selection. For example, daily reappraisal has been shown to be associated with increased daily suppression
(Brockman et al., 2017). Furthermore, ER strategies can affect subsequent antecedent-focused and response-
focused ER processes over time. An example of the way ER can affect subsequent situation selection comes
from the study by Farmer and colleagues, showing that suppression of positive emotions leads to higher
engagement in positive social events on the following days (Farmer & Kashdan, 2012). Moreover, there is
evidence suggesting that the adoption of certain strategies affects response modulation (i.e. the attempt to
modify the experiential, physiological, or behavioral components of the emotional response). Weiss et al., for
example, revealed that the adoption of distraction, cognitive reappraisal, and problem-solving predicts a
reduction in marijuana consumption on the following days (Weiss, Bold, Sullivan, Armeli, & Tennen, 2017).
Likewise, the adoption of disengagement (e.g., denial, avoidance, wishful thinking, escape or inaction) and
involuntary engagement strategies (e.g., rumination, impulsive or involuntary action) has been shown to lead
to more problematic behaviors over time, such as lying or stealing (Silk, Steinberg, & Morris, 2003).
Decades of experimental and observational research have resulted in a relatively comprehensive understanding
of ER. Important milestones have already been achieved, such as identification of important antecedents (i.e.,
contextual and situational factors, affect, and personal goals) and consequences (i.e., affect and physiological
states) associated with ER. Although the previous literature has clearly contributed to our understanding of
ER, in this paper we argue that the implementation of EMA is fundamental in order to move the ER research
forward. ER is a dynamic and complex process with elements that are likely to reciprocally interact with each
other, so that antecedents eventually become consequences and vice-versa. Capturing such a complex process
Additionally, although laboratory studies are and will be important for ER and clearly facilitate the
manipulation of a large number of elements in a controlled setting, the study of ER in real-life settings where
multiple factors occur simultaneously is also necessary, because these scenarios are where people implement
So far, the EMA literature has confirmed many of the findings achieved by laboratory experiments, again
confirming that the current knowledge about ER is quite robust. Added to the existing literature, EMA research
has provided important insights about the reciprocal nature of the relationship between ER and its antecedents
and consequences, the potential role of ER and situational factors as moderators in the implementation of ER,
and the key role of momentary ER in the study of subsequent ER implementation over time.
In sum, the implementation of EMA allows researchers to explore new dynamics of this process. However,
this method could also make it possible to study ER through a multimodal approach that includes concurrent
behavioral, physiological, and cognitive concomitants. Although interesting findings have already been
achieved with this approach, further advances will require combining more objective and passive data
collection methods with traditional EMA self-reports (Colombo, Fernandez Alvarez, Garcia-Palacios, et al.,
2019). For example, behavioral information (i.e., sleep patterns, physical activity, social interactions, or social
media use) can be obtained with smartphone embedded-sensors, and comfortable wrist-watches or wearable
chest-straps can easily monitor and record physiological parameters in ecological settings, thus achieving a
more comprehensive understanding of ER in daily life could be achieved (see Figure 1).
While acknowledging the aforementioned important contributions of EMA research on ER, this methodology
is not without limitations. For instance, standardized and ad-hoc items to assess ER via EMA are currently not
available, which makes it difficult to compare results across studies. Furthermore, this approach is more time-
consuming for participants (Colombo et al., 2018) and, because the data are collected in naturalistic
environments, it becomes harder to have complete control over the setting (as opposed to laboratory studies).
Considering the strengths and weaknesses of both methods, a combination of approaches should ideally be
pursued in order to fully comprehend the complexity of ER. Several milestones have already been achieved,
but the promising integration of different methods and technologies could provide a more comprehensive
understanding of ER.
Clinical implications of a more ecological, momentary, and technology-assisted approach to ER are also worth
mentioning. EMA could indeed change the way we assess ER in psychopathology by helping us to understand
the dynamics of this process in different emotional disorders, as well as the impact of emotion dysregulation
on patients’ lives. In turn, this could help clinicians to assess patients’ ER directly in daily life in order to
recognize specific strategies that need to be targeted in the therapeutic process and provide therapeutic
feedback to patients, which has been shown to be a valuable practice (Delgadillo et al., 2018). Using sensors
to capture physiological responses or a person’s behavior would further augment EMA’s potential. The
analysis of such multi-modal data can provide some insights into the interplay among emotions, physiology,
and behavior, and systems that utilize these principles can enable the practical application of data-driven
approaches. Particularly, machine learning algorithms are inherently attuned to interpreting complex
interdependent information. Such algorithms have the advantage of continuously learning from observations,
and they can be personalized for a specific user. This ambitious, technology-assisted implementation of EMA
could, for instance, help us to identify person- or population-wide predictors of emotional states which, in turn,
1. (Aldao, Sheppes, & Gross, 2015): In this study, Aldao and colleagues discuss the relationship between
ER, goals, and context, and they delve into the concept of ER flexibility, i.e. the ability to regulate
emotions taking into account situational and contextual demands. Interestingly, specific recommendations
2. (Gaggioli et al., 2013): Psychlog is an example of a mobile phone platform that can concurrently collect
psychological (i.e. self-reports), physiological (HR monitoring), and activity (i.e. three-axial
3. (Ottaviani et al., 2015): Ottaviani and colleagues concurrently investigated the three dimensions of ER
through an ecological approach. More specifically, cognitive, behavioral, and physiological correlates of
rumination were explored in real-life for 24h, using a smartphone-based electronic diary and an
ambulatory HR device.
4. (Blanke et al., 2019): Using data from four experience-sampling studies, Blanke and colleagues
investigated the importance of the context on ER. According to their results, ER strategies are not adaptive
or maladaptive; instead, adaptiveness is the result of the variability in choosing strategies based on
situational demands.
FUNDING
This work is supported by the Marie Curie EF-ST AffecTech Project, approved at call H2020 – MSCA – ITN
REFERENCES
Aldao, A. (2013). The Future of Emotion Regulation Research: Capturing Context. Perspectives on
Aldao, A., Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion Regulation Flexibility. Cognitive Therapy and
Blanke, E. S., Brose, A., Kalokerinos, E. K., Erbas, Y., Riediger, M., & Kuppens, P. (2019). Mix It to Fix It:
Brans, K., Koval, P., Verduyn, P., Lim, Y. L., & Kuppens, P. (2013). The regulation of negative and positive
Brockman, R., Ciarrochi, J., Parker, P., & Kashdan, T. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies in daily life:
mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 46(2), 91–
113. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2016.1218926
Brosschot, J. F., Van Dijk, E., & Thayer, J. F. (2007). Daily worry is related to low heart rate variability
during waking and the subsequent nocturnal sleep period. International Journal of Psychophysiology.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.016
Burns, A. B., Brown, J. S., Sachs-Ericsson, N., Ashby Plant, E., Thomas Curtis, J., Fredrickson, B. L., &
Joiner, T. E. (2008). Upward spirals of positive emotion and coping: Replication, extension, and initial
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.08.015
Bylsma, L. M., & Rottenberg, J. (2011). Uncovering the dynamics of emotion regulation and dysfunction in
daily life with ecological momentary assessment. In Emotion Regulation and Well-Being.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-6953-8_14
Catterson, A. D., Eldesouky, L., & John, O. P. (2016). An experience sampling approach to emotion
regulation: Situational suppression use and social hierarchy. Journal of Research in Personality.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.04.004
Colombo, D., Cipresso, P., Fernández Alvarez, J., Garcia Palacios, A., Riva, G., & Botella, C. (2018). An
Overview of Factors Associated with Adherence and Dropout to Ecological Momentary Assessments
Colombo, D., Fernandez Alvarez, J., Garcia-Palacios, A., Cipresso, P., Botella, C., & Riva, G. (2019). New
Technologies for the Understanding, Assessment and Intervention of ER in Mental Health. Frontiers in
Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01261
Colombo, D., Fernandez Alvarez, J., Palacios, A. G., Patané, A., Semonella, M., Cipresso, P., … Botella, C.
(2019). Current State and Future Directions of Technology- Based Ecological Momentary Assessment
and Intervention for Major Depressive Disorder: A Systematic Review. Journal of Clinical Medicine,
8(4), 465.
Delgadillo, J., de Jong, K., Lucock, M., Lutz, W., Rubel, J., Gilbody, S., … McMillan, D. (2018). Feedback-
informed treatment versus usual psychological treatment for depression and anxiety: a multisite, open-
0366(18)30162-7
Eldesouky, L., & English, T. (2018). Regulating for a reason: Emotion regulation goals are linked to
English, T., Lee, I. A., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2017). Emotion regulation strategy selection in daily life:
The role of social context and goals. Motivation and Emotion, 41(2), 230–242.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9597-z
Farmer, A. S., & Kashdan, T. B. (2012). Social anxiety and emotion regulation in daily life: Spillover effects
https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2012.666561
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. The broaden-and-build
066X.56.3.218
Gaggioli, A., Pioggia, G., Tartarisco, G., Baldus, G., Corda, D., Cipresso, P., & Riva, G. (2013). A mobile
data collection platform for mental health research. Personal and Ubiquitous Computing, 17(2), 241–
251. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00779-011-0465-2
Genet, J. J., & Siemer, M. (2012). Rumination moderates the effects of daily events on negative mood:
Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and Emotion Regulation : Divergent Consequences Experience , Expression
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion Regulation: Current Status and Future Prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1),
1–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual Differences in Two Emotion Regulation Processes:
Implications for Affect, Relationships, and Well-Being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
Gruber, J., Hay, A. C., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Rethinking emotion: Cognitive reappraisal is an effective
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035249
Haines, S. J., Gleeson, J., Kuppens, P., Hollenstein, T., Ciarrochi, J., Labuschagne, I., … Koval, P. (2016).
The Wisdom to Know the Difference: Strategy-Situation Fit in Emotion Regulation in Daily Life Is
Heiy, J. E., & Cheavens, J. S. (2014). Back to basics: A naturalistic assessment of the experience and
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Individual Differences in Emotion Regulation. In J. J. Gross (Ed.),
Handbook of Emotion Regulation (pp. 351–372). New York: The Guilford Press.
Jose, P. E., Lim, B. T., & Bryant, F. B. (2012). Does savoring increase happiness? A daily diary study.
Li, Y. I., Starr, L. R., & Hershenberg, R. (2017). Responses to Positive Affect in Daily Life: Positive
Rumination and Dampening Moderate the Association Between Daily Events and Depressive
017-9593-y
Marzano, L., Bardill, A., Fields, B., Herd, K., Veale, D., Grey, N., & Moran, P. (2015). The application of
https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00268-0
McMahon, T. P., & Naragon-Gainey, K. (2018). The Moderating Effect of Maladaptive Emotion Regulation
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-018-9913-x
McRae, K., Heller, S. M., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Context-Dependent Emotion Regulation:
Suppression and Reappraisal at the Burning Man Festival. Basic and Applied Social Psychology.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2011.614170
Nezlek, J. B., & Kuppens, P. (2008). Regulating positive and negative emotions in daily life. Journal of
Personality. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00496.x
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking Rumination. Perspectives on
Ottaviani, C., Shahabi, L., Tarvainen, M., Cook, I., Abrams, M., & Shapiro, D. (2015). Cognitive,
behavioral, and autonomic correlates of mind wandering and perseverative cognition in major
Ottaviani, C., Thayer, J. F., Verkuil, B., Lonigro, A., Medea, B., Couyoumdjian, A., & Brosschot, J. F.
Pavani, J. B., Le Vigouroux, S., Kop, J. L., Congard, A., & Dauvier, B. (2016). Affect and Affect Regulation
Strategies Reciprocally Influence Each Other in Daily Life: The Case of Positive Reappraisal, Problem-
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9686-9
Quoidbach, J., Berry, E. V., Hansenne, M., & Mikolajczak, M. (2010). Positive emotion regulation and well-
being: Comparing the impact of eight savoring and dampening strategies. Personality and Individual
Differences. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.048
Richardson, C. M. E. (2017). Emotion regulation in the context of daily stress: Impact on daily affect.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611418350
Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ Emotion Regulation in Daily Life: Links to
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00643.x
Srivastava, S., Tamir, M., McGonigal, K. M., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2009). The Social Costs of
Emotional Suppression: A Prospective Study of the Transition to College. Journal of Personality and
Suso-Ribera, C., Castilla, D., Zaragozá, I., Ribera-Canudas, M. V., Botella, C., & García-Palacios, A.
Smartphone App for Daily Monitoring of Adults with Heterogeneous Chronic Pain. The Clinical
Troy, A. S., Shallcross, A. J., & Mauss, I. B. (2013). A Person-by-Situation Approach to Emotion
Regulation: Cognitive Reappraisal Can Either Help or Hurt, Depending on the Context. Psychological
Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613496434
Weiss, N. H., Bold, K. W., Sullivan, T. P., Armeli, S., & Tennen, H. (2017). Testing bidirectional
associations among emotion regulation strategies and substance use: a daily diary study. Addiction.
https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13698
Wood, J. V., Heimpel, S. A., & Michela, J. L. (2003). Savoring Versus Dampening: Self-Esteem Differences
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.566