0% found this document useful (0 votes)
51 views15 pages

Consequentes e Antecedentes Da Regulação Emocional

The document discusses emotion regulation (ER) antecedents based on both traditional research methods and more recent ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies. Some key findings are: 1. Contextual factors like social context, context controllability, and the type/intensity of triggering emotion influence which ER strategies are used. EMA confirms the role of social context and controllability. 2. Momentary positive affect is linked to use of cognitive reappraisal and problem solving, while negative affect predicts suppression, rumination, and use of multiple concurrent strategies. 3. Situation-specific goals also impact ER strategy selection, though this relationship requires more exploration. EMA provides a useful method for understanding dynamic

Uploaded by

Rita
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
51 views15 pages

Consequentes e Antecedentes Da Regulação Emocional

The document discusses emotion regulation (ER) antecedents based on both traditional research methods and more recent ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies. Some key findings are: 1. Contextual factors like social context, context controllability, and the type/intensity of triggering emotion influence which ER strategies are used. EMA confirms the role of social context and controllability. 2. Momentary positive affect is linked to use of cognitive reappraisal and problem solving, while negative affect predicts suppression, rumination, and use of multiple concurrent strategies. 3. Situation-specific goals also impact ER strategy selection, though this relationship requires more exploration. EMA provides a useful method for understanding dynamic

Uploaded by

Rita
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 15

AperTO - Archivio Istituzionale Open Access dell'Università di Torino

The need for change: Understanding emotion regulation antecedents and consequences using
ecological momentary assessment

This is the author's manuscript

Original Citation:

Availability:

This version is available http://hdl.handle.net/2318/1842362 since 2022-02-21T00:11:41Z

Published version:

DOI:10.1037/emo0000671

Terms of use:

Open Access
Anyone can freely access the full text of works made available as "Open Access". Works made available
under a Creative Commons license can be used according to the terms and conditions of said license. Use
of all other works requires consent of the right holder (author or publisher) if not exempted from copyright
protection by the applicable law.

(Article begins on next page)

17 October 2023
The Need for Change: Understanding Emotion Regulation Antecedents and Consequences

using Ecological Momentary Assessment

Desirée Colombo1, Javier Fernandez Alvarez2, Carlos Suso-Ribera1, Pietro Cipresso2,3, Hristo Valev4,5, Tim

Leufkens4, Corina Sas5, Azucena Garcia Palacios1,6, Giuseppe Riva2,3, Cristina Botella1,6

1
Department of Basic Psychology, Clinic and Psychobiology, Universitat Jaume I, Spain
2
Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Italy
3
Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Italy
4
Philips Research, High Tech Campus, Eindhoven, Netherlands
5
School of Computing and Communications, Lancaster University, UK
6
CIBER Fisiopatología Obesidad y Nutrición (CIBERobn), Instituto Salud Carlos III, Spain

ABSTRACT

In recent decades, emotion regulation (ER) has been one of the most widely studied constructs within the

psychological field. Nevertheless, laboratory experiments and retrospective assessments have been the two

most common strands of ER research, thus leaving open several crucial questions about ER antecedents and

consequences in daily life. Beyond traditional methods, ecological momentary assessment (EMA) has the

potential to capture ER dynamics during the flow of daily experiences, in real-life settings and through repeated

measurements. Here, we discuss what we currently know about ER antecedents and consequences. We will

compare findings from previous literature to findings from EMA studies, pointing out both similarities and

differences, as well as questions that can be answered better with the EMA approach.

Keywords: Emotion Regulation, Ecological Momentary Assessment, Emotion Regulation Antecedents,

Emotion Regulation Consequences.


1. Introduction

Emotion regulation (ER) is a process that every individual explicitly or implicitly implements in order to

downregulate or upregulate emotional states and achieve personal goals (Gross, 2015). ER can be

conceptualized as a complex process with physiological underpinnings, behavioral responses, and affective

and cognitive correlates. Adding to this complexity, the preponderant role played by contextual and momentary

factors is increasingly emerging, suggesting the dynamic and situational nature of this process (Aldao, 2013).

To date, research into ER has mostly been based on laboratory experiments, thus leaving situated and

momentary aspects relatively understudied. Additionally, most studies have relied on retrospective

questionnaires, that consider ER to be a trait (i.e. a relatively stable and cross-situational tendency of an

individual). However, trait measures only modestly correlate with ER in daily life (Brockman, Ciarrochi,

Parker, & Kashdan, 2017), which supports the need for a more ecological approach to the investigation of this

process.

An alternative strategy to laboratory experiments and retrospective assessments can be found in ecological

momentary assessment (EMA), a procedure that makes it possible to collect repeated inputs of thoughts,

feelings and behaviors close in time to the experience and in real-life contexts. The past few years have seen

a surge in studies using EMA due to the increased availability of mobile applications (Colombo, Fernandez

Alvarez, Palacios, et al., 2019) for both subjective (Suso-Ribera et al., 2018) and objective (Marzano et al.,

2015) data collection, and this method has already significantly increased our knowledge about ER outside

laboratory settings (Bylsma & Rottenberg, 2011). In fact, a more complex representation of the way people

regulate emotions is emerging that emphasizes the importance of capturing this process in the context where

emotions are regulated.

In the present paper, we will provide an overview of the available literature on ER antecedents and

consequences. To do so, we will relate EMA findings to the broader literature based on laboratory and

retrospective studies, pointing out both similarities and differences, as well as questions that can be better

answered with the EMA approach. We will suggest EMA as a suitable methodology to extend our

understanding of ER, and we will propose a technology-based model to concurrently explore the affective,

physiological, and behavioral concomitants of ER.

2. What are the antecedents of ER?


Beyond individual differences (John & Gross, 2007), ER can be understood as the result of a complex interplay

among contextual and situational factors, momentary affect, and situation-specific goals.

Contextual and situational factors

Existing research has revealed the importance of the social context in ER, showing that both rapid changes in

the social context (McRae, Heller, John, & Gross, 2011) and stable contextual transactions (Srivastava, Tamir,

McGonigal, John, & Gross, 2009) influence this process. Additionally, context controllability has been shown

to be critical for ER. Accordingly, Troy and colleagues demonstrated that higher reappraisal ability (i.e. the

ability to down-regulate negative emotions) is associated with lower levels of depression after the exposure to

an uncontrollable stressor, but it is associated with higher subsequent depression in the context of a controllable

stressor (Troy, Shallcross, & Mauss, 2013). Finally, the general context where the emotions are regulated can

significantly impact ER. For example, one study shows that people are more likely to choose distraction in

high-intensity negative situations, whereas in low-intensity negative situations people are more likely to adopt

cognitive reappraisal, which involves rethinking the meaning of a stimulus in order to change its emotional

impact (Sheppes, Scheibe, Suri, & Gross, 2011).

EMA studies have confirmed the role of both the social context and context controllability in ER. Suppression,

that is the inhibition of ongoing emotion-expressive behaviors, is more frequently adopted when other people

are present in the environment (English, Lee, John, & Gross, 2017) and it is used less frequently in situations

where people feel high in social hierarchy (Catterson, Eldesouky, & John, 2016). Additionally, individuals

with higher well-being report greater reappraisal adoption in situations that are low in controllability (Haines

et al., 2016). Beyond these results, EMA studies also extended the previous literature about contextual

determinants of ER. The identification of the trigger for one’s emotions has been shown to affect ER and, more

specifically, to be tied to the use of certain strategies, such as reappraisal and savoring (i.e. the attempt to

maintain and up-regulate positive emotions) (Heiy & Cheavens, 2014). Furthermore, the type of emotion

targeted also influences this process, so that people concurrently use more strategies in response to certain

emotions, such as excitement and pride (Heiy & Cheavens, 2014).

Momentary affect

According to the broaden-and-build theory, the experience of positive emotions fosters people’s repertoires of

thoughts and actions and encourages the development of new coping skills (Fredrickson, 2001). Evidence
supporting this theory comes, for example, from a longitudinal study, showing that higher PA levels predict

increased positive coping skills eight weeks later (Burns et al., 2008).

Nonetheless, most of the previous literature only relied on retrospective measurements of ER, which made it

difficult to explore real-life relationships between momentary affect and ER. Fortunately, in recent years there

has been a surge in research using the EMA approach for the exploration of affective antecedents. For example,

high levels of momentary self-esteem, psychological adjustment, and PA have been found to trigger the use of

cognitive reappraisal (Nezlek & Kuppens, 2008). High levels of PA also predict greater adoption of problem

solving (Pavani, Le Vigouroux, Kop, Congard, & Dauvier, 2016) and mindfulness (Brockman et al., 2017),

whereas momentary NA has been associated with greater use of suppression (Brockman et al., 2017) and

rumination, which refers to the process of persistently thinking about one’s feelings (Li, Starr, & Hershenberg,

2017; Pavani et al., 2016). Interestingly, momentary affect also appears to influence the number of strategies

adopted, so that the use of different concurrent ER strategies increases in the presence of high levels of

momentary NA compared to PA (Brans, Koval, Verduyn, Lim, & Kuppens, 2013).

Situation-specific goals

Along with the aforementioned contextual and affective factors, situation-specific goals have also been shown

to influence ER. Overall, the previous literature has shown that individuals who frequently reappraise and

distract tend to pursue prosocial and pro-hedonic goals more often, whereas people who habitually suppress

tend to pursue contra-hedonic and impression management goals more frequently (Eldesouky & English,

2018).

These results are consistent with the EMA literature, which has revealed that distraction and cognitive

reappraisal are used more frequently when hedonic goals are pursued, whereas suppression is more likely to

be implemented when achieving instrumental goals (English et al., 2017).

3. What are the consequences of ER?

In the previous section, we explained that ER can be understood as the outcome of a number of antecedents.

However, ER also has multiple consequences on people’s lives. In the following paragraphs, we will discuss

two important outcomes that can be influenced by ER, namely affective and physiological states.

Momentary affect
A large amount of evidence from previous literature shows an influence of ER on subsequent affective states,

and most of the research concludes that cognitive reappraisal has a more adaptive profile in terms of affective

consequences, compared to suppression. Reappraisal is indeed related to experiencing less subsequent NA and

more PA, whereas suppression leads to experiencing less PA and more NA (Gross & John, 2003). Furthermore,

frequent use of savoring has been associated with greater happiness and well-being (Quoidbach, Berry,

Hansenne, & Mikolajczak, 2010), whereas recurrent use of rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, &

Lyubomirsky, 2008) and dampening (i.e. the tendency to decrease positive emotional experiences) (Wood,

Heimpel, & Michela, 2003) have been related to increased and prolonged NA.

Similarly, the body of research on affective consequences employing EMA is also extensive. The EMA

literature has confirmed previous findings regarding savoring (Jose, Lim, & Bryant, 2012) and dampening (Li

et al., 2017). However, contrasting results were observed for other strategies. For instance, cognitive

reappraisal has been found to be related to increased PA (Richardson, 2017), which is consistent with previous

research, but some studies failed to replicate its relationship with decreased NA (Brans et al., 2013; Brockman

et al., 2017; Nezlek & Kuppens, 2008; Pavani et al., 2016). Similarly, many EMA studies confirmed the

association between suppression and subsequent increased NA and decreased PA (Brans et al., 2013;

Brockman et al., 2017). Nevertheless, Richardson and colleagues observed no effects of suppression on NA

(Richardson, 2017), and Heiy et al. found that suppression did not predict subsequent affect (Heiy & Cheavens,

2014). Finally, the EMA literature confirmed the association between rumination and subsequent decreased

PA and increased NA (Brans et al., 2013; Li et al., 2017; Pavani et al., 2016), albeit with mixed results (Genet

& Siemer, 2012; Heiy & Cheavens, 2014).

Physiological responses

In addition to its influence on affective states, ER has also been shown to entail physiological concomitants.

Thus far, rumination and its physiological effects have been widely investigated, showing that high rumination

is associated with heightened activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPAA) and decreased

heart rate variability (HRV) (Ottaviani et al., 2016). Moreover, suppression has been associated with increased

sympathetic activation (Gross, 1998), whereas cognitive reappraisal has been linked to reduced physiological

response (i.e. skin conductance) (Gruber, Hay, & Gross, 2014). Finally, acceptance has been related to

decreased electrodermal reactivity (Dunn, Billotti, Murphy, & Dalgleish, 2009).


A limitation of existent physiological research is that most studies have been conducted in laboratory settings

with very few exceptions, such as the study by Brosschot and colleagues, which confirmed the association

between rumination and lower HRV using a daily ecological experiment (Brosschot, Van Dijk, & Thayer,

2007).

4. Additional contributions of the EMA approach to the ER literature: Reciprocal interplay,

moderation, and ER strategy use over time

In addition to replicating traditional research, the implementation of EMA methodologies has shed light on

important gaps in the previous ER literature, that would have been difficult to address with non-ecological

research.

According to the EMA literature, not only can ER influence outcomes, but outcomes can also affect subsequent

ER processes, thus closing the circle. Momentary affect and ER, for example, have been shown to reciprocally

influence each other at a given point in time (Pavani et al., 2015); similarly, well-being has been shown to be

both a predictor (Haines et al., 2016) and an outcome (Quoidbach, Berry, Hansenne, & Mikolajczak, 2010) of

ER. From a theoretical point of view, these findings provide further support for the need to ecologically and

momentarily evaluate ER and related variables.

The use of EMA has also allowed for the exploration of moderators in the ER-to-outcomes relationship in real-

life settings. The results so far suggest that the effects of ER on subsequent outcomes are not linear, but rather

affected by contextual factors or by ER itself. In relation to the former, research has indicated that, on days

when people experience fewer positive events, momentary savoring (Jose et al., 2012) and positive rumination

(Li et al., 2017) are more relevant in increasing PA levels, whereas suppression is more strongly associated

with subsequent lower PA on more stressful days (Richardson, 2017). Regarding the moderating role of ER

strategies, a study found that rumination moderates the association between negative events and NA levels, so

that unpleasant events predict higher NA when rumination use is high, but not when it is low (Genet & Siemer,

2012).

Another contribution of the EMA approach is the exploration of the concurrent use of ER strategies over time.

Heiy and colleagues showed that individuals use, on average, seven strategies to regulate each experienced

emotion, which suggests that interactions among strategies may produce different outcomes (Heiy &

Cheavens, 2014). In this direction, McMahon and colleagues analyzed the concurrent use of reappraisal,
suppression, and rumination, and they showed that reappraisal compensates for the negative effects of

suppression and rumination on anxiety and depressive symptoms (McMahon & Naragon-Gainey, 2018).

Finally, a key contribution of the EMA approach has been the exploration of the way ER strategy use is

sequenced over time. Emerging evidence suggests that certain strategies are likely to affect subsequent strategy

selection. For example, daily reappraisal has been shown to be associated with increased daily suppression

(Brockman et al., 2017). Furthermore, ER strategies can affect subsequent antecedent-focused and response-

focused ER processes over time. An example of the way ER can affect subsequent situation selection comes

from the study by Farmer and colleagues, showing that suppression of positive emotions leads to higher

engagement in positive social events on the following days (Farmer & Kashdan, 2012). Moreover, there is

evidence suggesting that the adoption of certain strategies affects response modulation (i.e. the attempt to

modify the experiential, physiological, or behavioral components of the emotional response). Weiss et al., for

example, revealed that the adoption of distraction, cognitive reappraisal, and problem-solving predicts a

reduction in marijuana consumption on the following days (Weiss, Bold, Sullivan, Armeli, & Tennen, 2017).

Likewise, the adoption of disengagement (e.g., denial, avoidance, wishful thinking, escape or inaction) and

involuntary engagement strategies (e.g., rumination, impulsive or involuntary action) has been shown to lead

to more problematic behaviors over time, such as lying or stealing (Silk, Steinberg, & Morris, 2003).

5. Conclusions and future directions

Decades of experimental and observational research have resulted in a relatively comprehensive understanding

of ER. Important milestones have already been achieved, such as identification of important antecedents (i.e.,

contextual and situational factors, affect, and personal goals) and consequences (i.e., affect and physiological

states) associated with ER. Although the previous literature has clearly contributed to our understanding of

ER, in this paper we argue that the implementation of EMA is fundamental in order to move the ER research

forward. ER is a dynamic and complex process with elements that are likely to reciprocally interact with each

other, so that antecedents eventually become consequences and vice-versa. Capturing such a complex process

is challenging, especially when using laboratory experiments and retrospective ER measurements.

Additionally, although laboratory studies are and will be important for ER and clearly facilitate the

manipulation of a large number of elements in a controlled setting, the study of ER in real-life settings where
multiple factors occur simultaneously is also necessary, because these scenarios are where people implement

ER in their daily lives.

So far, the EMA literature has confirmed many of the findings achieved by laboratory experiments, again

confirming that the current knowledge about ER is quite robust. Added to the existing literature, EMA research

has provided important insights about the reciprocal nature of the relationship between ER and its antecedents

and consequences, the potential role of ER and situational factors as moderators in the implementation of ER,

and the key role of momentary ER in the study of subsequent ER implementation over time.

In sum, the implementation of EMA allows researchers to explore new dynamics of this process. However,

this method could also make it possible to study ER through a multimodal approach that includes concurrent

behavioral, physiological, and cognitive concomitants. Although interesting findings have already been

achieved with this approach, further advances will require combining more objective and passive data

collection methods with traditional EMA self-reports (Colombo, Fernandez Alvarez, Garcia-Palacios, et al.,

2019). For example, behavioral information (i.e., sleep patterns, physical activity, social interactions, or social

media use) can be obtained with smartphone embedded-sensors, and comfortable wrist-watches or wearable

chest-straps can easily monitor and record physiological parameters in ecological settings, thus achieving a

more comprehensive understanding of ER in daily life could be achieved (see Figure 1).

While acknowledging the aforementioned important contributions of EMA research on ER, this methodology

is not without limitations. For instance, standardized and ad-hoc items to assess ER via EMA are currently not

available, which makes it difficult to compare results across studies. Furthermore, this approach is more time-

consuming for participants (Colombo et al., 2018) and, because the data are collected in naturalistic

environments, it becomes harder to have complete control over the setting (as opposed to laboratory studies).

Considering the strengths and weaknesses of both methods, a combination of approaches should ideally be

pursued in order to fully comprehend the complexity of ER. Several milestones have already been achieved,

but the promising integration of different methods and technologies could provide a more comprehensive

understanding of ER.

Clinical implications of a more ecological, momentary, and technology-assisted approach to ER are also worth

mentioning. EMA could indeed change the way we assess ER in psychopathology by helping us to understand

the dynamics of this process in different emotional disorders, as well as the impact of emotion dysregulation
on patients’ lives. In turn, this could help clinicians to assess patients’ ER directly in daily life in order to

recognize specific strategies that need to be targeted in the therapeutic process and provide therapeutic

feedback to patients, which has been shown to be a valuable practice (Delgadillo et al., 2018). Using sensors

to capture physiological responses or a person’s behavior would further augment EMA’s potential. The

analysis of such multi-modal data can provide some insights into the interplay among emotions, physiology,

and behavior, and systems that utilize these principles can enable the practical application of data-driven

approaches. Particularly, machine learning algorithms are inherently attuned to interpreting complex

interdependent information. Such algorithms have the advantage of continuously learning from observations,

and they can be personalized for a specific user. This ambitious, technology-assisted implementation of EMA

could, for instance, help us to identify person- or population-wide predictors of emotional states which, in turn,

would foster the personalization of treatments and therapy plans.

RECOMMENDATIONS FOR ADDITIONAL READING

1. (Aldao, Sheppes, & Gross, 2015): In this study, Aldao and colleagues discuss the relationship between

ER, goals, and context, and they delve into the concept of ER flexibility, i.e. the ability to regulate

emotions taking into account situational and contextual demands. Interestingly, specific recommendations

to investigate this construct by means of EMA are provided.

2. (Gaggioli et al., 2013): Psychlog is an example of a mobile phone platform that can concurrently collect

psychological (i.e. self-reports), physiological (HR monitoring), and activity (i.e. three-axial

accelerometer) information in naturalistic settings.

3. (Ottaviani et al., 2015): Ottaviani and colleagues concurrently investigated the three dimensions of ER

through an ecological approach. More specifically, cognitive, behavioral, and physiological correlates of

rumination were explored in real-life for 24h, using a smartphone-based electronic diary and an

ambulatory HR device.

4. (Blanke et al., 2019): Using data from four experience-sampling studies, Blanke and colleagues

investigated the importance of the context on ER. According to their results, ER strategies are not adaptive

or maladaptive; instead, adaptiveness is the result of the variability in choosing strategies based on

situational demands.
FUNDING

This work is supported by the Marie Curie EF-ST AffecTech Project, approved at call H2020 – MSCA – ITN

– 2016 (project reference: 722022).

REFERENCES

Aldao, A. (2013). The Future of Emotion Regulation Research: Capturing Context. Perspectives on

Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612459518

Aldao, A., Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion Regulation Flexibility. Cognitive Therapy and

Research, 39(3), 263–278. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-014-9662-4

Blanke, E. S., Brose, A., Kalokerinos, E. K., Erbas, Y., Riediger, M., & Kuppens, P. (2019). Mix It to Fix It:

Emotion Regulation Variability in Daily Life. Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000566

Brans, K., Koval, P., Verduyn, P., Lim, Y. L., & Kuppens, P. (2013). The regulation of negative and positive

affect in daily life. Emotion, 13(5), 926–939. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032400

Brockman, R., Ciarrochi, J., Parker, P., & Kashdan, T. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies in daily life:

mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 46(2), 91–

113. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2016.1218926

Brosschot, J. F., Van Dijk, E., & Thayer, J. F. (2007). Daily worry is related to low heart rate variability

during waking and the subsequent nocturnal sleep period. International Journal of Psychophysiology.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.016

Burns, A. B., Brown, J. S., Sachs-Ericsson, N., Ashby Plant, E., Thomas Curtis, J., Fredrickson, B. L., &

Joiner, T. E. (2008). Upward spirals of positive emotion and coping: Replication, extension, and initial

exploration of neurochemical substrates. Personality and Individual Differences.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.08.015

Bylsma, L. M., & Rottenberg, J. (2011). Uncovering the dynamics of emotion regulation and dysfunction in

daily life with ecological momentary assessment. In Emotion Regulation and Well-Being.

https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-6953-8_14

Catterson, A. D., Eldesouky, L., & John, O. P. (2016). An experience sampling approach to emotion
regulation: Situational suppression use and social hierarchy. Journal of Research in Personality.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.04.004

Colombo, D., Cipresso, P., Fernández Alvarez, J., Garcia Palacios, A., Riva, G., & Botella, C. (2018). An

Overview of Factors Associated with Adherence and Dropout to Ecological Momentary Assessments

in Depression. In B. K. Wiederhold, G. Riva, & S. Bouchard (Eds.), Annual Review of CyberTherapy

and Telemedicine (Vol. 16, pp. 11–17). Interactive Media Institute.

Colombo, D., Fernandez Alvarez, J., Garcia-Palacios, A., Cipresso, P., Botella, C., & Riva, G. (2019). New

Technologies for the Understanding, Assessment and Intervention of ER in Mental Health. Frontiers in

Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01261

Colombo, D., Fernandez Alvarez, J., Palacios, A. G., Patané, A., Semonella, M., Cipresso, P., … Botella, C.

(2019). Current State and Future Directions of Technology- Based Ecological Momentary Assessment

and Intervention for Major Depressive Disorder: A Systematic Review. Journal of Clinical Medicine,

8(4), 465.

Delgadillo, J., de Jong, K., Lucock, M., Lutz, W., Rubel, J., Gilbody, S., … McMillan, D. (2018). Feedback-

informed treatment versus usual psychological treatment for depression and anxiety: a multisite, open-

label, cluster randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-

0366(18)30162-7

Eldesouky, L., & English, T. (2018). Regulating for a reason: Emotion regulation goals are linked to

spontaneous strategy use. Journal of Personality. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12447

English, T., Lee, I. A., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2017). Emotion regulation strategy selection in daily life:

The role of social context and goals. Motivation and Emotion, 41(2), 230–242.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9597-z

Farmer, A. S., & Kashdan, T. B. (2012). Social anxiety and emotion regulation in daily life: Spillover effects

on positive and negative social events. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy.

https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2012.666561

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. The broaden-and-build

theory of positive emotions. The American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-

066X.56.3.218
Gaggioli, A., Pioggia, G., Tartarisco, G., Baldus, G., Corda, D., Cipresso, P., & Riva, G. (2013). A mobile

data collection platform for mental health research. Personal and Ubiquitous Computing, 17(2), 241–

251. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00779-011-0465-2

Genet, J. J., & Siemer, M. (2012). Rumination moderates the effects of daily events on negative mood:

Results from a diary study. Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028070

Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and Emotion Regulation : Divergent Consequences Experience , Expression

, Physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion Regulation: Current Status and Future Prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1),

1–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781

Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual Differences in Two Emotion Regulation Processes:

Implications for Affect, Relationships, and Well-Being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

Gruber, J., Hay, A. C., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Rethinking emotion: Cognitive reappraisal is an effective

positive and negative emotion regulation strategy in bipolar disorder. Emotion.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035249

Haines, S. J., Gleeson, J., Kuppens, P., Hollenstein, T., Ciarrochi, J., Labuschagne, I., … Koval, P. (2016).

The Wisdom to Know the Difference: Strategy-Situation Fit in Emotion Regulation in Daily Life Is

Associated With Well-Being. Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616669086

Heiy, J. E., & Cheavens, J. S. (2014). Back to basics: A naturalistic assessment of the experience and

regulation of emotion. Emotion, 14(5), 878–891. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037231

John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Individual Differences in Emotion Regulation. In J. J. Gross (Ed.),

Handbook of Emotion Regulation (pp. 351–372). New York: The Guilford Press.

Jose, P. E., Lim, B. T., & Bryant, F. B. (2012). Does savoring increase happiness? A daily diary study.

Journal of Positive Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2012.671345

Li, Y. I., Starr, L. R., & Hershenberg, R. (2017). Responses to Positive Affect in Daily Life: Positive

Rumination and Dampening Moderate the Association Between Daily Events and Depressive

Symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-

017-9593-y
Marzano, L., Bardill, A., Fields, B., Herd, K., Veale, D., Grey, N., & Moran, P. (2015). The application of

mHealth to mental health: Opportunities and challenges. The Lancet Psychiatry.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00268-0

McMahon, T. P., & Naragon-Gainey, K. (2018). The Moderating Effect of Maladaptive Emotion Regulation

Strategies on Reappraisal: A Daily Diary Study. Cognitive Therapy and Research.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-018-9913-x

McRae, K., Heller, S. M., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Context-Dependent Emotion Regulation:

Suppression and Reappraisal at the Burning Man Festival. Basic and Applied Social Psychology.

https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2011.614170

Nezlek, J. B., & Kuppens, P. (2008). Regulating positive and negative emotions in daily life. Journal of

Personality. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00496.x

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking Rumination. Perspectives on

Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x

Ottaviani, C., Shahabi, L., Tarvainen, M., Cook, I., Abrams, M., & Shapiro, D. (2015). Cognitive,

behavioral, and autonomic correlates of mind wandering and perseverative cognition in major

depression. Frontiers in Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2014.00433

Ottaviani, C., Thayer, J. F., Verkuil, B., Lonigro, A., Medea, B., Couyoumdjian, A., & Brosschot, J. F.

(2016). Physiological concomitants of perseverative cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis.

Psychological Bulletin. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000036

Pavani, J. B., Le Vigouroux, S., Kop, J. L., Congard, A., & Dauvier, B. (2016). Affect and Affect Regulation

Strategies Reciprocally Influence Each Other in Daily Life: The Case of Positive Reappraisal, Problem-

Focused Coping, Appreciation and Rumination. Journal of Happiness Studies.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9686-9

Quoidbach, J., Berry, E. V., Hansenne, M., & Mikolajczak, M. (2010). Positive emotion regulation and well-

being: Comparing the impact of eight savoring and dampening strategies. Personality and Individual

Differences. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.048

Richardson, C. M. E. (2017). Emotion regulation in the context of daily stress: Impact on daily affect.

Personality and Individual Differences, 112, 150–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.058


Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion-regulation choice. Psychological Science.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611418350

Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ Emotion Regulation in Daily Life: Links to

Depressive Symptoms and Problem Behavior. Child Development, 74(6), 1869–1880.

https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00643.x

Srivastava, S., Tamir, M., McGonigal, K. M., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2009). The Social Costs of

Emotional Suppression: A Prospective Study of the Transition to College. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014755

Suso-Ribera, C., Castilla, D., Zaragozá, I., Ribera-Canudas, M. V., Botella, C., & García-Palacios, A.

(2018). Validity, Reliability, Feasibility, and Usefulness of Pain Monitor, a Multidimensional

Smartphone App for Daily Monitoring of Adults with Heterogeneous Chronic Pain. The Clinical

Journal of Pain, 34(10), 1. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0000000000000618

Troy, A. S., Shallcross, A. J., & Mauss, I. B. (2013). A Person-by-Situation Approach to Emotion

Regulation: Cognitive Reappraisal Can Either Help or Hurt, Depending on the Context. Psychological

Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613496434

Weiss, N. H., Bold, K. W., Sullivan, T. P., Armeli, S., & Tennen, H. (2017). Testing bidirectional

associations among emotion regulation strategies and substance use: a daily diary study. Addiction.

https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13698

Wood, J. V., Heimpel, S. A., & Michela, J. L. (2003). Savoring Versus Dampening: Self-Esteem Differences

in Regulating Positive Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.566

You might also like